You are on page 1of 6

Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century)

Spencer A McWilliams, California State University San Marcos, San Marcos, CA, USA
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
This article is a revision of the previous edition article by M.G. Ash, volume 18, pp. 12399–12405, Ó 2001, Elsevier Ltd.

Abstract

Twentieth-century academic psychology emerged formally in 1879, originally defined as the science of mental life, with the
goal of uncovering the facts of consciousness. Evolutionary theory expanded its emphasis on how mental processes and
behavior function to help organisms adapt to environments. The study of learning dominated the early half of the twentieth
century, with the goal of predicting and influencing behavior. Various subfields and a positivist emphasis on experimental
methods led to consolidation of the discipline by midcentury. Technological developments stimulated resurgence of the
study of cognition and neuropsychology and increased disciplinary fragmentation later in the century.

Nineteenth-Century Foundations Further research by Gustav Fechner (1801–87) confirmed


and refined Weber’s law, generalized it to auditory and
Modern academic psychology emerged during the latter part of visual stimuli, and, in 1860, stated it mathematically that
the nineteenth century as a way of addressing age-old questions sensations increase arithmetically as physical stimuli increase
regarding human nature and built upon work in biology, physi- geometrically (Weber’s law), indicating that humans perceive
ology, medicine, and philosophy – particularly philosophical sensations relative to the background context in which they
debates over explanations for how humans develop knowledge, occur. Building on Fechner’s work, Hermann Ebbinghaus
how humans represent environmental objects in conscious (1850–1909) studied the processes of learning and memory as
awareness, and how best to account for human behavior they occurred and his findings, still regarded as valid today,
(Hergenhahn and Henley, 2014; also see article on the Psychology, demonstrated that forgetting occurs rapidly during the first few
History of (Early Period)). Twentieth-century psychology built hours following learning and then declines more slowly. He
upon these and other topics and branched out into a wide variety also found that learning distributed over a period of time led to
of issues, theories, and methodologies. This article addresses better retention than massed repetition at a single time and that
psychology as an academic undertaking, primarily in the form of overlearning already memorized material reduced the rate of
an experiment science, thus (unfortunately) omitting discussion forgetting.
of the many applied areas of psychology that are covered in the Charles Darwin’s (1809–82) theory of evolution stimu-
encyclopedia article on the history of clinical psychology, lated an interest in individual differences among people in
including the important influences of psychoanalytic theory, other terms of intelligence and personal characteristics, based on
theories of personality and psychotherapy, and psychological the assumption that such differences might account for
testing and measurement, which also had a large impact on the success in adaptation and survival. Darwin’s English cousin
work of academic psychologists. Francis Galton (1822–1911) began attempts to measure
Hermann von Helmholtz’s (1821–94) mechanistic intelligence, following the British empiricist tradition, by
approach to physiology and his contributions to under- emphasizing innate sensory acuity as the primary way
standing the physiology of visual and auditory perception, humans come to know and adapt to the world. His work led
along with such findings as Paul Broca’s (1824–80) and Carl to statistical concepts such as the mean and correlation, which
Wernicke’s (1848–1905) identification of localized brain became central to the twentieth-century psychology emphasis
functions, stimulated research into the relationship among on quantitative analysis and the search for general principles.
sensations, brain processes, and conscious experience, and in Charles Spearman (1863–1945), also in England, studied the
particular, the measurement of these relations. Ernst Weber relation between sensory acuity and children’s success in
(1795–1878) pioneered the study of psychophysics – the school and suggested that an overall inherited factor, called
relationship between physical characteristics of objects and general intelligence, accounted for most of intelligence, with
the psychological perception of them. He asked people to specific factors reflecting learning and experience. In France,
discriminate the weight differences of two objects by Alfred Binet (1857–1911) pursued the measurement of
comparing a target weight with a standard and determined the intelligence from the Continental rationalist perspective by
relationship between the physical weight of the object and the measuring a variety of complex mental operations that re-
participant’s perception of whether the object was heavier than, flected his view of intelligence as active and complex, rather
lighter than, or equal to the standard. From these data, he than a single ability, and that could be nurtured rather than
derived the just noticeable difference (jnd) – the smallest being fixed by inheritance. While a revised version of Binet’s
weight that the participant could discriminate – and found that intelligence test became the most popular method for
the jnd did not reflect an absolute weight difference, but rather assessing children’s intelligence in the USA, most psycholo-
a ratio that is relative to the weight of the standard. This finding, gists adopted Spearman’s general factor understanding of
in 1834, Weber’s law, is regarded as the first ‘law’ in psychology. intelligence.

412 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 19 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.03046-4
Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century) 413

Beginnings of Experimental Psychology functionalist psychology emphasized how mental processes


serve to aid adaptation to the environment and included the
Voluntarism, Structuralism, and Phenomenology
study of individual differences as well as general principles, the
Scholars typically date Wilhelm Wundt’s (1832–1920) found- application of psychology to improving the human situation,
ing of a laboratory for psychological research at the University and the study of behavior as well as consciousness. James
of Leipzig in 1879 as the formal beginning of modern likened consciousness to an unbroken stream rather than
psychology. Wundt’s voluntarism, which focused on the active a chain of independent elements, emphasizing its personal,
processes involved in selective attention, incorporated the work continuous, and impermanent qualities and its function in
of Helmholtz, Weber, and Fechner into an organized research enhancing adaptation to the environment. His views on habits,
program that employed scientifically controlled introspection the self, emotion, volition, and cognition, along with the
to determine the underlying processes involved in human importance of useful practical applied psychology, elaborated
conscious awareness of environmental objects. He viewed the by other functionalist colleagues such as John Dewey
goal of psychology as identifying general principles of percep- (1859–1952), James Angell (1869–1949), and Harvey Carr
tion and learning that applied to all normal, adult humans by (1873–1954) established a foundation for the eclecticism of
identifying how higher-order concepts derived from raw twentieth-century psychology. The functionalists also
sensory elements. Wundt built on the European Continental embraced a pragmatist philosophical perspective that rejected
rationalist tradition’s view of a structured, active mind that the positivist search for independent truths and instead
transforms sensations and particularly focused his research on proposed that the validity of ideas and theories lay in their
selective perception, the question of what determines how, usefulness in solving human-defined problems and antici-
among the entire perceptual field, people attended to specific pating future events.
sensations and objects.
Following his graduation from Oxford, Edward Titchener
Behaviorism
(1867–1927) studied with Wundt but followed his native
British empiricist tradition, which emphasized a blank mind The desire for a more objective approach to psychology, which
that passively builds up perceptions through association of would avoid the subjectivity of studying conscious experience
sensations. He came to the USA in 1892 and developed and using introspection, led Russian physiologists Ivan
a successful doctoral program in psychology at Cornell Sechenov (1829–1905), Vladimir Bekhterev (1857–1927), and
University. His structuralist approach pursued the search for Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) to propose that psychology studies
mental elements further by attempting to identify the imme- the relationship between environmental stimuli and behavior,
diate experience of raw sensations and determine the laws both physiological and overt responses. The American
through which they combine to make perceptions and more psychologist John Watson (1878–1958) adopted Pavlov’s
complex mental phenomena, attempting to create a chart of respondent conditioning approach and elaborated on this view
psychological elements analogous to chemistry’s period chart with his Behaviorist Manifesto, in 1913, which proposed that
of elements. Although Titchener trained many doctoral grad- the goal of psychology should be the prediction and control of
uates who became highly productive and well-known behavior. These approaches renewed interest in the empiricist
psychologists, his structuralist approach declined, primarily tradition that emphasized learning experiences as the founda-
because even highly trained introspectionists could not agree tion of knowledge, rather than rationalist view that emphasized
on how to describe a raw sensation, and the retrospective the importance of innate characteristics. Although initially
nature of their descriptions hampered the attempt to study resisted by most contemporaries, the behaviorist view gradu-
sensory events as they occurred. ally came to dominate American psychology for half of the
European phenomenologists, such as Franz Brentano twentieth century, changing the definition of the discipline
(1838–1917) and Carl Stumpf (1848–1936), opposed the from “the science of mental life” (James, 1890: p. 1) to “the
emphasis on raw sensory elements and championed science of human and animal behavior.”
a phenomenological approach that attempted to understand Within this behaviorist emphasis researchers differed in the
consciousness in terms of whole, meaningful, intact percep- type of behavior they investigated. Some pursued Watson’s
tions, mental processes rather than contents, and the functional Pavlovian emphasis on associative, reflexive, respondent
nature of mental acts. Edmund Husserl (1859–1938) further (Classical) conditioning. However, the emphasis eventually
proposed that scientific psychology should examine the pure shifted to the study of purposive, goal-directed behaviors,
phenomenology of personal subjective experience, and he which organisms voluntarily emit and which lead to conse-
searched for mental essences and meaning. These phenome- quences that influence future behavior. Edward Thorndike
nological approaches influenced Gestalt psychology and exis- (1874–1949), a pioneer in the study of goal-directed behavior,
tential approaches to psychology during the twentieth century. used the term ‘law of effect’ in 1898 to emphasize the role of
the consequences of behavior to explain its purposive aspects.
In 1912, William McDougall (1871–1938) emphasized
Functionalism
a behavioral perspective in studying purposive behavior, which
The publication of William James’ (1842–1910) Principles of he defined as spontaneous, rather than a response to a known
Psychology in 1890 incorporated and elaborated on each of stimulus, and which persists until the attainment of a goal. This
these traditions and expanded the field to embrace any useful emphasis on purposive, goal-directed behavior came to char-
topic or methodology that would shed light on the human acterize the era of the learning theories in the USA, from the
condition. Influenced by Darwin’s theory of evolution, James’ 1930s through the 1960s, which used nonhuman animals to
414 Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century)

study learning, the primary mechanism by which organisms verifiability, at least in principle; therefore, logical positivism
adapt to environmental change. itself cannot be meaningful, by its own criterion). However,
Three major approaches vied for dominance during this despite strong criticism from within the field (Bakan, 1973), its
period, erecting theories of learning following the logical adoption by psychologists, in conjunction with the positivist
positivist approach to theoretical concepts (see below). Edwin methodology of Newtonian physics and psychology’s self-
Guthrie (1886–1959) remained close to Watson’s respondent conscious focus on gaining scientific respectability, led to the
learning approach by attempting to explain purposive behavior primacy of research methodology, more than content, as the
in terms of the laws of associative learning; he depicted defining common identity of academic psychologists.
purposive learning as a response to the most recent stimuli
associated with that response. Clark Hull (1884–1952) applied
Subfields and Subspecialties
the model of Newton’s physics to psychology, developing
a psychological theory explaining purposive behavior in Gestalt Psychology
mechanistic terms. He emphasized the importance of biolog- Although empirically based behaviorism dominated
ical drive as motivating behavior, with reduction of the drive psychology research in America for the first half of the twen-
serving to reinforce, or increase the probability of, the behavior tieth century, the Continental rationalist tradition remained
that led to it. Edward Tolman (1886–1959) attempted to active in Europe, particularly in the work of the German Gestalt
explain purposive behavior in behavioral terms, but he oper- psychologists, led by Max Wertheimer (1880–1943). Gestalt
ationally defined concepts like purpose and cognition as psychology continued to elaborate on the rationalist view of
intervening variables in goal-directed behavior. His theory perception as an active process in which mental or brain
sustained a cognitive emphasis that supported the resurgence processes structure and transform sensory data. Along with
of cognitive psychology in the 1960s. colleagues Wolfgang Köhler (1887–1967) and Kurt Koffka
The behaviorist perspective reached its zenith in the 1960s (1886–1941), Wertheimer provided persuasive support for the
with B.F. Skinner’s (1904–90) operant conditioning approach, perspective that the tendency to perceive forms, structure, and
which rejected the need for theories of learning and use of patterns dominated the perception process more than partic-
cognitive, or mentalistic, explanations of behavior. Instead, he ular sensory inputs, that the whole (perception) is greater than
analyzed the functional relationships among environmental the sum of the parts (sensations) that generate it. Many Gestalt
situations, overt behaviors, and consequences, and how these psychologists found a welcome home in America immediately
three factors influenced the future probability of behavior. before and after World War II, increasing interest in Gestalt
Using evolutionary theory as a metaphor, he described how, principles, perception, and the active role of mental processes
during an organism’s lifetime, the environment selected its in America.
behaviors through the process of conditioning. Reinforcing
environmental consequences strengthened (increased the Social Psychology
probability of) behaviors, while other consequences decreased Gestalt psychology also had an impact on the emerging
behavioral probability through punishment or extinction. subfield of social psychology. Several of the Gestalt psycholo-
Skinner’s radical behaviorism became widely embraced in gists who immigrated to the USA focused their work on
experimental psychology and also led to application in many applying the perspective to the influence of social factors,
fields of human services. defined as the real or imagined presence of other people, on
psychological processes. Kurt Lewin’s (1890–1947) field theory
pioneered the Gestalt approach to the study of personality and
Influence of Logical Positivism
social behaviors and its application to motivation, conflict, and
Throughout the behaviorist heyday, many psychologists in the group dynamics. Lewin’s and Koffka’s student Tamara Dembo
USA, Europe, and elsewhere continued to pursue research on (1902–93) contributed to the study of frustration and aspira-
psychology’s original topics such as sensation, perception, tion, and Lewin’s student Leon Festinger (1919–89) became
memory, problem-solving, and other cognitive topics. well known for his work on social comparison and cognitive
Although many investigators on these topics did not self- dissonance. Solomon Asch (1907–96) studied Gestalt
identify as behaviorists, most came to accept observable psychology and taught at Swarthmore College with Köhler. He
behaviors as the proper valid evidence for psychological gained recognition for his work on how humans form
processes, even if these behaviors consisted of responses to self- impressions of others and for his research in conformity that
report questionnaires or scales. Based on the success of New- demonstrated that groups can influence even obvious percep-
tonian physics, the methods of the natural sciences, grounded tions. Social psychology evolved into an active field that
in the positivist belief in an absolute independent external employed creative methodologies to study a variety of
reality accessible to human knowing through empirical contemporary social topics that interested the psychologists of
research, emerged as psychology’s dominant methodological the time (Gergen, 1973).
approach. Twentieth-century psychologists reaffirmed natural
sciences methodology and embraced the logical positivist Developmental Psychology
argument of the Vienna Circle in the 1920s that legitimated the Darwin’s theory also influenced the emerging field of devel-
use of theoretical concepts as long as they were operationally opmental psychology by suggesting that functional psycho-
defined. Eventually, logical positivism failed as a philosophy of logical processes evolve in individuals across the life span,
science due to its self-referential incoherence (logical posi- particularly during infancy, childhood, and adolescence.
tivism states that meaningful statements require empirical G. Stanley Hall (1844–1924), founder of the American
Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century) 415

Psychological Association (APA), proposed that human various perspectives described above establishing the field’s
development proceeds in a manner that mirrors the increasing topics and defining its content. Methodologically, the emphasis
complexity of organisms on the phylogenic scale, that on experimentation, quantification, and laboratory-based
‘ontology recapitulates phylogeny.’ He built a strong develop- research unified the prominence of experimental method-
mental psychology program at Clark University that included ology as the discipline’s standard. By reaffirming the logical
Heinz Werner’s (1890–1964) pivotal role in bringing the positivist perspective, the methodology supported operational
European, Kantian, intellectual traditions to America. Werner definitions of theoretical concepts, the quest for causal rela-
elaborated the orderly directionality of developmental changes tionships, and the ‘discovery’ of universal laws and principles
in terms of a succession of structures that succeed each other of human and animal cognition and behavior.
(Glick, 1992). The Continental rationalist tradition also guided Graduate students in psychology during this period typi-
the work of the Swiss genetic epistemologist, Jean Piaget cally received broad exposure to psychological theory, research,
(1896–1980), whose constructivist cognitive approach greatly and content that led to their identification with the discipline as
influenced developmental psychology and also sustained a whole. In sum, “psychology during the 1900–60 period dis-
interest in cognitive processes during the behaviorist era. Piaget played a general pattern of centripetality in research and theory,
showed how a child’s experience of the environment becomes philosophy of science, graduate education, and organizational
more complex and adapted as the child articulates a cognitive infrastructure. This centripetal pattern resulted in consolidation
schema in the process of biological maturation. These pio- and unification of the discipline” (Altman, 1987: p. 1061). This
neering developmental themes of Werner and Piaget appear in sense of unification existed even though psychology had not
a variety of subsequent developmental psychology models and generated an agreed-upon core lexicon, or shared paradigm
approaches (Kegan, 1982). (Kuhn, 1962), as was the case in other established scientific
disciplines.
Neuropsychology
Early-twentieth-century advances in studying the relationship
between psychological and neurological processes continued Late-Twentieth-Century Developments
to contribute to the evolution of psychological research. Karl
Lashley (1890–1958) sought to identify the neurological bases In the latter half of the twentieth century, two interrelated
of conditioned reflexes as studied by the Russian physiologists themes influenced research into the questions and topics that
and championed by Watson’s behaviorism. He reasoned that characterized this unified discipline of psychology. The waning
innate, instinctive reflexive responses to certain stimuli require popularity of behaviorism and its emphasis on learned
an innate neurological structure, and thus conditioned associ- behavior influenced a resurgence of interest in cognition and
ated responses to these stimuli might also come to have invigorated the importance of nativist, biological explanations
a specific neurological location. The term ‘engram’ referred to of psychological phenomena. Additionally, rapid technological
this hypothetical location of learning and memory, and Lashley improvements, which routinely stimulate advances in many
devoted his career to attempting to find it. Ultimately, he failed scientific fields (Pickering, 1984, 1995), had an increasing
to find a single location in the brain for specific learning and impact on academic research on psychology’s core questions
proposed that the brain operates as a unified whole rather than and topics.
with specific physical links, a principle he called mass action.
Donald Hebb (1904–85) saw the newborn’s brain as
Resurgence of Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Science
unstructured and undifferentiated, with organized connections
among neurons occurring through experience. He surmised A symposium at MIT on information theory in 1956 sparked
that sensory organs respond to environmental objects by firing a revival of research in cognition within the context of the
a complex of neurons, which he called cell assemblies, as development of sophisticated computer technology, cyber-
a result of reverberating neural activity that enabled separate netics, and information processing models. George A. Miller
neurons to establish connections while stimulated. He further (1920–2012) demonstrated that humans can discriminate
suggested that the assemblies of neurons representing an seven aspects of events and retain seven units of experience,
environmental object become connected in a series of activities and published his landmark text Psychology: The Science of
that he called phase sequences. These sequences come to Mental Life in 1962, reclaiming James’ original definition of
represent complex trains of memories and thoughts. Hebb the discipline. Jerome Bruner (b. 1915) studied abstract
articulated the neurological level of connection by positing thinking, concept learning, and the use of active cognitive
what came to be known as ‘Hebb’s rule’ that neurons that fire strategies. Ulric Neisser (1928–2012) defined cognition as
together become wired together. These principles of mass any processes that transform, reduce, elaborate, store, recover,
action and cell assemblies influenced developments in cogni- and use sensory input, further clarifying this renewed
tive science and neuroscience in the latter part of the twentieth approach to the topics that occupied early experimental
century. psychology.
Subsequent developments in cognitive psychology focused
on computer programs as a metaphor for the processes of the
Psychology in Midcentury human mind, leading to the field of artificial intelligence,
a branch of computer science. Englishman Alan Turing
Altman (1987) described the period of 1900–60 as a major era (1912–54) founded this field by asking whether a machine can
of consolidation of the discipline of psychology, within the actually think, creating his famous Turing test in which
416 Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century)

participants attempt to consistently differentiate a human a complex system of specialized pathways that evolved through
responder from that of a computer. The question of whether natural selection to assist in solving problems that the humans
computers actually think like a human or only follow rules encountered over the course of evolutionary history. Both
of program received further discussion by John Searle sociobiology and evolutionary psychology base their work on
(b. 1932), who argued against the likelihood of valid the assumption that we can understand psychological
artificial intelligence. By the mid-1970s, Neisser criticized the processes as selected for by their benefit in perpetuation of
computer and information models of cognition as too sterile copies of genes into subsequent generations.
and argued that cognitive psychologists should study
cognition as it naturally occurs in people’s ordinary lives and
how it functions in their purposeful activities. However, Psychology at the End of the Twentieth Century
computer modeling remained the major methodology.
Toward the end of the twentieth century, an interdisci- Irwin Altman (1987) in the USA, Theo Herrmann (2009) in
plinary field of cognitive science emerged, combining the work Germany, and Tommy MacKay (2001) in the United Kingdom,
of psychologists with anthropologists, linguists, computer each in their own way, characterized the latter half of psy-
scientists, neuroscientists, and philosophers. An approach chology’s twentieth century, in contrast to the first half, as
called connectionism surfaced as an active model in this field, a time of centrifugal trends and discontinuity. They portray the
attempting to provide a computer-based virtual model of how field as a proliferation of specialty areas, loss of a shared
the cognitive and neurological system learns by studying the common history (Fuchs and Viney, 2002; Samelson, 1999) and
relationship between input and output in terms of Hebb’s core of mutual beliefs, and a weaker identification of its
rule and Lashley’s principle of mass action. members with the field as a whole. Graduate programs came to
focus more on specialized areas, with each field developing its
own curriculum and cohort of students, organizational struc-
Neuropsychology, Neuroscience, and Evolutionary Psychology
tures, and separated physical quarters. Applied and profes-
Late-twentieth-century development of sophisticated neuro- sional psychology programs became distanced from the basic
imaging measurement techniques such as functional magnetic science of the field, and professional schools and psycho-
resonance imaging and computed axial tomography scanning, therapy training programs proliferated independently of
along with advances in human genetics and electrophysiology, academic research psychology.
enhanced the progression of mapping human emotions and Much of what had become the traditional content of
cognitions to specific neural areas, rapidly extending the earlier psychology has been absorbed into the newly emerging inter-
work of Lashley and Hebb. These developments led to emer- disciplinary specialties of cognitive science and neuroscience
gence of the interdisciplinary field of neuroscience, including (Gardner, 2009; Gentner, 2010), decreasing psychology’s
disciplines such as biology, linguistics, mathematics, computer absolute claim to this territory (MacKay, 2001). The author’s
science, and philosophy, as well as psychology, which studies own graduate education exemplifies this trend. I completed my
neural circuitry and its relationship to psychological functions. PhD in the Department of Psychology at the University of
As we have seen, Darwin’s theory of evolution influenced Rochester in 1971, with a major in clinical psychology and
many, if not most, areas of psychology. Toward the latter part a minor in perception. In 1995, the Department of Psychology
of the twentieth century, however, we see the development of ceased to exist, as it split into two departments: the Department
a specific approach to psychology called evolutionary of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology (locus of my
psychology. This perspective evolved from sociobiology, major field) and the Department of Brain and Cognitive
founded by Edward O. Wilson (b. 1929), which proposed that Sciences (locus of my minor field).
evolution has selected for humans with innate tendencies We can also see this fragmentation reflected in the organi-
toward social behavior, language, creating cultures, social zation structure of the discipline in the United States. The APA
conventions, phobias, etc. The question of the relative strength originally primarily represented those involved in academic
of these innate tendencies versus the influence of culture and and basic research and only slowly admitted professional
learning experiences generated a lively debate, with Wilson psychologists over its early developing years. By the 1980s,
suggesting that nature gives us a ‘long leash,’ enabling flexible however, professional psychologists came to outnumber
modes of adapting to changing environments, rather than academics in APA, and it currently functions as an organization
a ‘short leash’ that strictly determined specific behaviors. serving the interests of professional practitioners. This trend led
Evolutionary psychology regards human psychological to the establishment of an alternative organization in 1988,
processes as a collection of functionally specialized regulatory The Association for Psychological Science (APS), which focuses
structures or circuits that organize how humans interpret on research and the scientific aspects of the field. APS has
experiences and create concepts and frameworks of meaning. embraced an international perspective and the inclusion of the
This evolutionary perspective recognizes the universality of interdisciplinary fields described above, sponsoring an inter-
‘normal’ human behavior through the study of natural national convention of psychological science in the Nether-
competencies that all humans share. Leda Cosmides and John lands in 2015.
Tooby (1997) thus define psychology as a branch of biology Although we might feel compelled to do so, we need not
that studies how brains develop programs to process infor- regard fragmentation, discontinuity, and centrifugal trends as
mation, emphasizing the brain as a physical system that func- harmful to the discipline. Rather, we can view them as reflect-
tions as a computer and generates behavior appropriate to the ing an energetic period of change and reformulation that can
environment. They regard the brain’s neural circuitry as imbue the discipline with new directions in methods and
Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century) 417

theories. By regarding psychology as a relatively young disci- Bibliography


pline, even characterizing it as in a medieval period (Cromwell,
2010), we can see the state of the discipline in the beginning of Altman, I., 1987. Centripetal and centrifugal trends in psychology. American
the twenty-first century as providing intriguing opportunities Psychologist 42 (12), 1058–1069.
Bakan, D., 1973. On Method: Toward a Reconstruction of Psychological Method.
for new ideas and formulations.
Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA.
Bandura, A., 2006. Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on
Psychological Science 1, 164–180.
Challenges to Twentieth-Century Conventions Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., 1997. Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer. http://www.cep.
Although most mainstream academic and research psycholo- ucsb.edu/primer.html (accessed 17.03.14.).
Cromwell, R.L., 2010. Being Human: Human Being. Manifesto for a New Psychology.
gists maintain a commitment to the scientific goal of estab- iUniverse, New York.
lishing general laws of human nature through empirical Fishman, D.B., 1999. The Case for Pragmatic Psychology. New York University Press,
research and logical positivist theorizing, alternative New York.
approaches began to receive growing attention by the end of Fuchs, A.J., Viney, W., 2002. The course in the history of psychology: present status
and future concerns. History of Psychology 5 (1), 3–15.
the twentieth century. Sometimes viewed as part of a broader
Gardner, H., 2009. Scientific psychology: should we bury it or praise it?
postmodern movement and reflecting challenges to scientific Humana.Mente 11, 11–23.
absolutism spawned by Kuhn’s (1962) critique of the corre- Gentner, D., 2010. Psychology in cognitive science: 1978–2038. Topics in Cognitive
spondence theory of scientific truth, many psychologists have Science 2, 328–344.
begun to question the prevailing view that scientific methods Gergen, K.J., 1973. Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 26 (2), 309–320.
uncover objective, permanent truths. Instead, they suggest that Gergen, K.J., 2009. An Invitation to Social Constructionism, second ed. Sage, London.
humans play a central role in the construction of knowledge. A Glick, J.A., 1992. Werner’s relevance for contemporary developmental psychology.
variety of approaches to psychology, including personal Developmental Psychology 28 (4), 558–565.
constructivism (Raskin, 2002), social constructionism (Gergen, Hayes, S.C., Barnes-Holmes, Roche, B. (Eds.), 2001. Relational Frame Theory: A Post-
Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition. Kluwer Academic/Plenum,
2009), contextualism (Hayes et al., 2001), hermeneutics
New York.
(Martin and Sugarman, 1999; Slife and Christensen, 2013), Held, B.S., 2007. Psychology’s Interpretive Turn: The Search for Truth and Agency in
Buddhism (McWilliams, 2014), and pragmatism (Fishman, Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. American Psychological Association,
1999), emphasize the active responsibility of human psycho- Washington, DC.
logical and social processes in generating ever-changing Hergenhahn, B.R., Henley, T.B., 2014. An Introduction to the History of Psychology,
seventh ed. Wadsworth, Belmont, CA.
knowledge and the temporal embeddedness of these Herrmann, T., 2009. What’s next?: continuity and discontinuity in psychology. Zeits-
processes in historical, cultural, and language contexts. Albert chrift für Psychologie 217 (2), 95–102.
Bandura’s (2006) social cognitive approach also came to James, W., 1890. The Principles of Psychology, vols. I & II. Holt, New York.
strongly embrace the active role of human agency in Kegan, R., 1982. The Evolving Self: Problem and Process in Human Development.
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
coevolution and creating unanticipated futures in a world of
Kuhn, T., 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. University of Chicago Press,
flux and change. Although not without criticism from those Chicago.
committed to conventional psychological models (Held, MacKay, T., 2001. The future belongs to psychology. The Psychologist 14 (9),
2007), these perspectives suggest alternative models that may 466–469.
have promise for a new view of the field. Martin, J., Sugarman, J., 1999. The Psychology of Human Possibility and Constraint.
SUNY Press, Albany, NY.
We might anticipate that as many of the topics that origi- McWilliams, S.A., 2014. Foundations of mindfulness and contemplation: traditional
nally fell within psychology’s twentieth-century domain and contemporary perspectives. International Journal of Mental Health and
migrate to interdisciplinary, biologically based fields, Addiction. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11469-014-9486-1.
psychology may come to focus more on the processes by which Pickering, A., 1984. Constructing Quarks: A Sociological History of Particle Physics.
University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
people, personally and collectively, actively create knowledge
Pickering, A., 1995. The Mangle of Practice: Time, Agency, and Science. University of
and meaning, including the approaches to knowledge and Chicago Press, Chicago.
meaning that we call science. Perhaps then, rather than viewing Raskin, J.D., 2002. Constructivism in psychology: personal construct psychology,
psychology as a branch of the natural sciences, we may come to radical constructivism, and social constructionism. American Communication
see the natural sciences, the humanities, the arts, religion, Journal 5 (3), 1–24.
Samelson, F., 1999. Assessing research in the history of psychology: past,
philosophy, and other human undertakings as the subject present, and future. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 35 (3),
matter of psychology. 247–255.
Slife, B.D., Christensen, T.R., 2013. Hermeneutic realism: toward a truly meaningful
See also: Clinical Psychology in Europe, History of; Clinical psychology. Review of General Psychology 17 (2), 230–236.
Psychology in North America, History of; Cognitive
Psychology: History; Industrial–Organizational Psychology:
Science and Practice; Psychology, History of (Early Period);
Social Psychological Theory, History of; Social Psychology.

You might also like