You are on page 1of 3

comment

Cross-discipline evidence principles for


sustainability policy
Evidence-based approaches to sustainability challenges must draw on knowledge from the environment,
development and health communities. To be practicable, this requires an approach to evidence that is broader and
less hierarchical than the standards often applied within disciplines.

Edward T. Game, Heather Tallis, Lydia Olander, Steven M. Alexander, Jonah Busch, Nancy Cartwright,


Elizabeth L. Kalies, Yuta J. Masuda, Anne-Christine Mupepele, Jiangxiao Qiu, Andrew Rooney,
Erin Sills and William J. Sutherland

S
ocial and environmental systems A common interpretation of evidence social–environmental systems embodied in
are linked and, as this relationship among natural sciences and more positivist the Sustainable Development Goals. Using
becomes ever more apparent, social science approaches is, the body of a medical standard to assess evidence on
governments, communities and information relevant to judging whether environment outcomes, for example, would
organizations are increasingly faced a hypothesis is likely to be true or not5. mean excluding much candidate evidence
with, and focused on, problems that are Assessing the strength of evidence that (for instance studies that are observational
complex, wicked and transgress traditional implementing an intervention will result in rather than experimental), thus reducing the
disciplinary boundaries. Indicative of a particular outcome (a causal hypothesis), ability to discriminate the relative strength of
this focus, 12 of the 17 United Nations’ is a critical step in evidence-based decision- evidence supporting different interventions
Sustainable Development Goals directly making about whether, when and where to and likely missing key insights about
reference linkages between human pursue an intervention (or which of many, whether and where an intervention will
development challenges and environmental possibly untested, interventions to pursue). lead to the desired change13. Alternatively,
health, and thus, evidence-based Cross-disciplinary approaches to social– assessing evidence in a manner that
approaches to the Sustainable Development environmental challenges require causal employs a different understanding of
Goals must draw on knowledge from the associations and evidence across various validity than medicine can be a barrier to
environment, development and health domains to be considered6. For example, collaboration across disciplines where health
domains. In response, the environment, tackling the use of fire to clear tropical outcomes are concerned. Even within these
development and health communities are peat forests crosses the environment, disciplines that have a broadly comparable
investing more in shared, cross-disciplinary health and development communities; interpretation of evidence (for example,
approaches to evaluating the effectiveness fire is an important tool for agricultural there is some objective truth out there
of interventions. This effort requires a production, but it leads to significant carbon about whether an intervention causes an
broader, less-hierarchical approach to emissions, the loss of forests and associated outcome), commensurability is not easy
evidence than those often applied biodiversity, and human respiratory illness to achieve. Nevertheless, this is the point.
within disciplines. and mortality linked to smoke. Increasingly we are faced with evaluating
Different kinds of knowledge arise Although there is no universal approach interventions that do not fall neatly into a
from research in disciplines that make to assessing evidence, there is convergence single disciplinary paradigm. To do so,
fundamentally different philosophical within some disciplines, such as in clinical we require an approach to evidence
and methodological assumptions1 or medicine7, toxicology and public health8. assessment that can combine evidence
from knowledge that is entirely outside These discipline-specific approaches have from different disciplines.
the epistemological framework of most grown out of calls since the 1990s for Table 1 describes six different types of
research2. Because different types of evidence-based practice and systematic candidate evidence commonly available
knowledge are useful for different reviews9. Similarly, a causal empiricist within the disciplines of health, development
purposes3 and it is not possible to evaluate approach has become the dominant and the environment. There is no inherent
all knowledge using the same criteria, how paradigm in development economics10. rank order in terms of the strength of
knowledge, including evidence, is defined While calls for evidence-based practice have evidence these six types produce to support
and interpreted has a major impact on also encouraged the growth of systematic a hypothesis. Some forms of evidence are
how understanding complex problems reviews in environmental management and more appropriate for particular questions
and potential interventions is approached. conservation11,12, broader consensus on an than others, for example, understanding
Overcoming the cultural and philosophical approach to evaluating and determining the whether an intervention will lead to a
barriers of working with very different strength and appropriateness of different behaviour change may be better informed
forms of knowledge remains a general types of evidence remains elusive. by qualitative evidence that focuses on
challenge2,4. Here we address the narrower, Discipline-specific approaches to context and perceptions around the
but still difficult, problem of integrating evidence, driven in large part by the types behaviour, than by a quantitative study14.
different types of evidence underpinned by of evidence historically available in different However, within the disciplines that work
the assumption that it is possible to predict disciplines, represent a barrier to the sort with each type of evidence, there is general
outcomes of an intervention. of cross-disciplinary understanding of agreement that some modes of information
Nature Sustainability | www.nature.com/natsustain
comment

of strong evidence, variation in findings


Table 1 | Types of candidate evidence to assess causal associations
across studies does not preclude strong
Evidence type Description evidence because there can be good
Quantitative studies Studies based on inference through numerical data and analysis explanations, often revealed through
that describe the relationship between parts of a system. qualitative studies, for variation resulting
Quantitative studies may be experimental, quasi-experimental or from the basis for comparison. We
observational. intentionally use consistency rather than
Qualitative studies Studies based on inference through a thorough understanding of
size or magnitude as a term because it is
a case (or cases) under investigation, without characterizing an inclusive of evaluation by evidence types
absolute numerical relationship between parts of a system. such as qualitative studies that do not
generally involve magnitude estimations.
Models Representation of how a system (or part of a system) functions.
Potentially a tool for prediction. Models can be conceptual
or mathematical and are typically, but not always, used in
Credible sources. Where candidate evidence
conjunction with the results of quantitative studies, theory or
is available from sources widely seen as
expert knowledge. credible (that is, trusted and believed in),
as judged by the prevailing standards for
Expert knowledge The judgement of those with specialized knowledge obtained
that type of candidate evidence, it provides
through training or experience. This includes local knowledge,
confidence in characterizing evidence as
indigenous knowledge and subject matter expertise.
strong. Confidence is instilled by the fact
Theory A scientifically accepted general principle or body of principles that these sources have standards and
offered to explain phenomena. checks in place to ensure methodologies are
Interpretation of measurement Information gained from measurements that may or may not be appropriately matched to the study question
results part of study, for example, meteorological records. and that the impact of bias on findings is
The typology characterizes underlying sources and important differences between the types of candidate evidence commonly used, both minimized19. Although there is no objective
explicitly and implicitly, in decisions about interventions in social-environmental systems. These types are not mutually exclusive and a rank of sources by credibility and no source
candidate piece of evidence will, in many cases, reflect more than one of these types of evidence. This set of evidence types reflects only provides an unequivocal guarantee of study
forms of knowledge consistent with the view of evidence that is the focus of this manuscript.
quality, the process of publication in peer-
reviewed journals is designed explicitly
collection or generation are more precise greater confidence that the association to improve and support the credibility of
or reliable than others, and that some exists than where evidence is only available findings. It would be remiss not to take
forms of analysis are less biased than from a single type. The basic premise advantage of this process to provide an
others. These ‘methodological standards’ for this principle is that methodological indication of the credibility of different
for each candidate type of evidence can variation between evidence types reduces pieces of candidate evidence. There is, of
be used to determine the quality of the likelihood that a reported relationship course, a spectrum of credibility within the
evidence provided15. is due solely to how a study was conducted, peer-review system, but one that defies easy
and that it is unlikely that the limitations of characterization. Candidate evidence that
Evidence principles different types of evidence would each bias comes from sources outside peer review
We suggest that a reasonable and practicable the findings in the same direction. Evidence can be credible, (for example, reports from
approach to evidence assessment16, which assessment schemes commonly recognize UN organizations). However, where most
recognizes and integrates different types that multiple, unrelated lines of evidence candidate evidence comes from sources
of evidence, is possible. We propose that provide stronger overall evidence17, but with the potential for perceived bias
four characteristics of evidence represent other schemes do not explicitly equate (for example, because of an organization’s
foundational considerations when assessing multiple lines of evidence with multiple agenda or funding), a thorough assessment
a body of evidence for a specific question types of evidence as we do here. of study designs would be a necessary part of
about causal associations (Fig. 1). Here, we evidence assessment.
briefly introduce each of these principles: Consistency of effect. Where the body
of candidate evidence is consistent in its Applicability. Where there is a good fit
• The variety of evidence types that sup- findings, this increases confidence in the between the body of candidate evidence
port an association between a phenom- answer to the question in view. Consistency and the question of interest (for instance,
enon or intervention and observed implicitly places value on having a larger similarity in the populations, interventions,
outcomes amount of evidence but also has multiple and outcomes being considered), it is
• The consistency of the effect found in the dimensions to it. Consistency can be reasonable to assume that this evidence
evidence about the causal association considered in the direction (or sign) of an is relevant and therefore has greater
• The credibility of the evidence sources association, or for quantitative evidence, potential for providing strong support.
being integrated or considered the size and the range or variance of an How applicable candidate evidence is
• The applicability of the evidence to the effect. Consistency of effect across studies is to the question at hand is dependent on
question of interest considered a central tenet of many evidence both the context for the evidence (for
assessment schemes. This is particularly example, the presence or absence of similar
Multiple types of evidence. Each type true of schemes looking to assess general moderator variables and ability to account
of evidence described in Table 1 has its claims of relationships between a treatment for their effect on the outcome) and the
own strengths and weaknesses. Where and outcome for medical interventions7 methods used, such as the implementation
the answer to a question of association or exposure and hazard for public health of the treatment, the measurement of the
is supported by more than one type of questions18. Although it is reasonable to outcome and the basis for comparison
evidence, we conclude that it confers consider a consistent effect as indicative or counterfactual (for example, was the

Nature Sustainability | www.nature.com/natsustain


comment

Applicable Credible sources principles, policymakers and practitioners DH1 3HN, UK. 10University of California San Diego,
should be aware of that lack of certainty La Jolla, CA, USA. 11The Nature Conservancy,
in the cross-disciplinary knowledge base. Durham, NC, USA. 12The Nature Conservancy,
That does not preclude greater certainty Seattle, WA, USA. 13Department of Biometry and
✓ ✓
based on one particular type of evidence, Environmental System Analysis, University of

as judged by the standards of the relevant Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany. 14Nature Conservation
✓ discipline and by its relevance to the and Landscape Ecology, University of Freiburg,
× question at hand. We have focused on Tennenbacher Str. 4, 79106 Freiburg, Germany.
✓ evidence in the context of evaluating 15
School of Forest Resources and Conservation,
✓ interventions but there is need for evidence Fort Lauderdale Research and Education Center,
Research
from a cross-disciplinary knowledge base University of Florida, Florida, FL, USA. 16Division
throughout a policy process20. of the National Toxicology Program, National
✓ Assessing a body of evidence against Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, Research
each of the principles requires individual Triangle Park, NC, USA. 17Department of Forestry
C
judgement and will be implemented and Environmental Resources, NC State University,
A B
variably by different people. This element Raleigh, NC, USA. 18Conservation Science Group,
of subjective judgment is a near universal Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge,
Consistency of effect Multiple types of evidence
feature of evidence assessment schemes. Cambridge, UK.
Fig. 1 | The four principles that underpin a cross-
Our goal is to provide some foundational *e-mail: egame@tnc.org
disciplinary approach to evidence. Where all four
principles of ‘strong evidence’ that will
facilitate understanding and resolution Published: xx xx xxxx
principles are met, the body of candidate evidence
of these differences in judgment in https://doi.org/10.1038/s41893-018-0141-x
would provide strong support for a hypothesized
causal association.
the cross-disciplinary teams that are
required to assess candidate evidence on References
interventions operating across the domains 1. Midgley, G., Nicholson, J. D. & Brennan, R. Ind. Market. Manag.
62, 150–159 (2017).
same outcome of interest measured or of development, environment and health. 2. Fazey, I. et al. Energy Res. Social Sci. 40, 54–70 (2018).
was a related outcome measured). The These principles intentionally move away 3. Cornell, S. et al. Environ. Sci. Pol. 28, 60–70 (2013).
applicability principle is akin to the from the hierarchical view of evidence 4. Tengö, M. et al. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 26,
17–25 (2017).
assessment of evidence directness found in types prevalent in evidence assessment 5. Cartwright, N. Philos. Sci. 73, 981–990 (2006).
other evidence grading schemes7, but with schemes. Such a shift is necessary if we are 6. Munafò, M. R. & Davey Smith, G. Nature 553,
greater flexibility as to what constitutes fit to effectively confront social–environmental 399–401 (2018).
7. Guyatt, G. H. et al. Brit. Med. J. 336, 924 (2008).
with the question and context in focus. sustainability challenges with evidence. 8. Morgan, R. L. et al. Environ. Int. 92, 611–616 (2016).
The body of candidate evidence would Engaging in the full complexity of social– 9. Sackett, D. L. et al. Brit. Med. J. 312, 71 (1996).
provide strong support for a hypothesized environmental systems, however, challenges 10. Panhans, M. T. & Singleton, J. D. Hist. Polit. Econ. 49,
127–157 (2017).
causal association where (i) support comes us to think even more broadly about what 11. Sutherland, W. J. et al. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 305–308 (2004).
from multiple types of evidence, (ii) there counts as knowledge and knowing. ❐ 12. Collaboration for Environmental Evidence, Guidelines for
is consistency in the pattern of association, Systematic Review and Evidence Synthesis in Environmental
(iii) the evidence comes from credible Edward T. Game1,2*, Heather Tallis3, Management Version 4.2 (Collaboration for Environmental
Evidence, 2013).
sources and (iv) the evidence is highly Lydia Olander4, Steven M. Alexander5,6,7, 13. Voβ​, J.-P. et al. J. Environ. Pol. Plan. 9, 193–212 (2007).
applicable to the question of interest. Across Jonah Busch8, Nancy Cartwright9,10, 14. Bennett, N. J. Conserv. Biol. 30, 582–592 (2016).
previously published evidence schemes, a Elizabeth L. Kalies11, Yuta J. Masuda12, 15. Khagram, S. & Thomas, C. W. Public Admin. Rev. 70,
S100–S106 (2010).
large number of factors or criteria have been Anne-Christine Mupepele13,14, 16. Cartwright, N., Goldfinch, A. & Howick, J. J. Child. Serv. 4,
proposed as relevant to an assessment of Jiangxiao Qiu15, Andrew Rooney16, 6–14 (2010).
evidence strength (for example, whether Erin Sills17 and William J. Sutherland18 17. Norris, R. et al. Freshw. Sci. 31, 5–21 (2011).
18. Rooney, A. A. et al. Environ. Health Persp. 122, 711 (2014).
a dose-response relationship can be defined, 1
The Nature Conservancy, South Brisbane, 19. Montibeller, G. & Von Winterfeldt, D. Risk Anal. 35,
or whether the design of individual studies Queensland, Australia. 2University of Queensland, 1230–1251 (2015).
includes randomization). However, we St Lucia, Queensland, Australia. 3The Nature 20. Clark, T. W. The Policy Process: A Practical Guide for Natural
Resources Professionals. (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, CT, 2002).
(authors from a range of disciplines) Conservancy, Santa Cruz, CA, USA. 4Nicholas
consider the four characteristics described Institute for Environmental Policy Solutions,
above as unequivocal indicators of the Duke University, Durham, NC, USA. 5National
Acknowledgements
strength of evidence across disciplines. Socio-Environmental Synthesis Center, University
We thank I. Fazey for extensive input that thoroughly
In making decisions about interventions, of Maryland, Annapolis, MD, USA. 6Stockholm improved the manuscript and Z. Burivalova for input on
it is important to know where there is Resilience Centre, Stockholm University, Stockholm, the design of Fig. 1. This collaboration was supported by
strong support for a hypothesis, and if there Sweden. 7Environmental Change and Governance a grant from the David and Lucile Packard Foundation to
is not strong support, in what ways it is not Group, Faculty of Environment, University of H.T., E.T.G. and L.O. S.M.A. acknowledges support from
the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of
strong (that is, which of the four evidence Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada. 8Center for Global Canada and the National Socio-Environmental Synthesis
principles is not satisfied). Where the Development, Washington DC, USA. 9Philosophy Center through NSF grant no. DBI-1052875. W.J.S. is
body of evidence is inconsistent with these Department, Durham University, Durham funded by Arcadia.

Nature Sustainability | www.nature.com/natsustain

You might also like