You are on page 1of 13

Motivating Civil Engineering Students:

Self-Determinacy Perspective
Hector Martin, Ph.D., M.ASCE 1; Sapphire Vital 2; Leighton Ellis, Ph.D. 3;
and Charmaine Obrien-Delpesh 4

Abstract: Motivation is a psychological construct shown to influence an individual’s success. Researchers have focused on factors affecting
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

student motivation but have neglected to address the temporal changes in student motivation and the effects of these changes on academic
performance. The purpose of this paper is to answer three main questions: What variables motivate students? Do the motivational variables
change over time? And is there a relationship between the motivational variables that influence students and their academic performance as
measured in grade point average (GPA)? Using a questionnaire survey approach, this study explores self-determinacy theory to evaluate
students’ progression over a three-year enrollment in an undergraduate civil and environmental engineering degree. The evaluation showed
that internal factors are the primary motivators of students with high GPA. Also, students’ quality and the quantity of motivation decreased
with time in pursuit of a constant goal. Gender differences highlighted that females had superior self-regulatory practices to males. By setting
intermediate report deadlines with performance feedback, educators would improve male students’ self-regulatory practices through an im-
provement of their organization and goal setting skills. These findings provide a better understanding of student attrition than previously
acknowledged in the literature. DOI: 10.1061/(ASCE)EI.1943-5541.0000374. © 2018 American Society of Civil Engineers.
Author keywords: Academic performance; Learning motivation; Cognitive theory; Educational philosophy; Student motivation;
Self-determinacy theory.

Introduction involvement and commitment of the students to learn and acquire


excellent academic grades that can aid them in their future occupa-
Martin et al. (2014) sought to address an alarming trend observed tional career (Ullah et al. 2013, p. 91). Motivation research in
about students enrolled in an undergraduate civil engineering pro- engineering education has focused on the identification of motiva-
gram. The pattern, although abstractly recognized, was more pre- tional factors (Alpay et al. 2008; Law and Chuah 2009; Law et al.
cisely identified via the following statistics. Sixty percent of the 2009); disparities based on gender (Kolmos et al. 2013); and
students enrolled in the graduating class of 2012–2013 failed to correlations among motivational factors, problem-solving skills,
complete the degree in the allotted three years. This trend was again and knowledge transfer (Benson et al. 2013; Husman and Lens
repeated in the academic years 2013–2014 and 2014–2015 with 1999). Despite these proposed interventions, there is an absence
approximately 68% of the students in each year graduating at least of studies focusing on differences in motivators as an individual
in their fourth year of study. As explained by Martin et al. (2014) develops and the effects of these differences on academic perfor-
this trend of low performance raises numerous questions. Why are mance. Further, the literature has been inconclusive regarding the
some students performing well while others do not? Is it that the link between these two facets of need for achievement and perfor-
material to be covered is too difficult for the majority of students? mance in engineering education (Law and Chuah 2009).
Alpay et al. (2008), Law and Chuah (2009), and Kolmos et al. In motivation research, context is significant (Pintrich 2003).
(2013) believed that when innate academic ability fails to explain
Context encompasses classroom interactions, tasks, activities, prac-
the vast differences in academic performance amongst students,
tices, and culture. Context can create, shape, facilitate, or constrain
motivation is often the defining quality. Specifically, since all
the development of student motivation (Pintrich 2003). The study
the students enrolled met the entry requirements, all were thought
by Benson et al. (2013) on bioengineering and mechanical engi-
to have the basic knowledge, skills, and ability required to succeed
neering indicates differences between the engineering disciplines
in their program of choice. Motivation to learn is defined as the
on several motivational factors, reflecting the probability that stu-
1 dents with similar motivational profiles have similar long-term
Lecturer, Construction Management, Univ. of the West Indies,
St. Augustine, Trinidad and Tobago (corresponding author). Email: goals and therefore tend to choose the same degree. Different
hector.martin@sta.uwi.edu knowledge characterizes each engineering discipline, which alters
2
Civil Engineer, Engineering Dept., CEP Ltd., P.O. Box 663, General the educational context. Hence, motivational research on one type
Post Office, Roseau, Dominica. Email: sapphirevital@outlook.com of engineering cannot necessarily be assumed universal to all
3
Lecturer, Construction Management, Univ. of the West Indies, kinds of engineering fields, in the case of the current scope, civil
St. Augustine, Trinidad and Tobago. Email: Leighton.ellis@sta.uwi.edu engineering.
4
Lecturer, Coastal Engineering, Univ. of the West Indies, St. Augustine, In light of the motivation construct, further questions come to
Trinidad and Tobago. Email: Charmaine.delpesh-obrien@sta.uwi.edu
mind. Are the students who complete their degree at an exception-
Note. This manuscript was submitted on April 28, 2017; approved on
January 29, 2018; published online on May 25, 2018. Discussion period ally high standard motivated differently from their other class-
open until October 25, 2018; separate discussions must be submitted for mates? Is this motivation consistent throughout their studies or
individual papers. This paper is part of the Journal of Professional Issues does it change? Can educators influence motivation in its stagnant
in Engineering Education and Practice, © ASCE, ISSN 1052-3928. or dynamic form? Although directional behavior was advocated by

© ASCE 04018005-1 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Schunk and Zimmerman (2008) almost a decade ago, there are lim- unmet, as students may only focus on an outcome without openness
ited studies on directional behavior, which accesses magnitude and to exploration, growth, or understanding, and thus inhibits implicit
dynamic changes in motivators and reasons supporting these learning (Shalley 1995). Therefore, on their own, these theories
changes. Martin et al. (2014) explored motivation as a contributor might not be sufficient to explain possible changes in student
to academic performance by determining the motivational factors motivation, which occurs during a program of study.
influencing students in the school year 2013–2014. This study was Specific to education, need/motive-based theories belong to an
cross-sectional and focused on the possible links between motiva- older pool of research, which attempted to use the need to achieve
tion and academic performance as well as establishing the basis for and the need to avoid failure to explain academic motivation and
the changing nature of motivation. The results indicated that intrin- achievement. As time passed, research showed that these motives
sic motivational factors had a more positive effect on motivation merely acted as antecedents to the social-cognitive achievement
compared to that of extrinsic motivational factors. The results, how- goals, which then directly influenced academic motivation and
ever, did not yield a significant factor linking motivation to aca- achievement (Elliot and Church 1997). Such findings resulted in
demic performance, which would be indicative of the probable the recognition that needs/motive-based theories and social-
impact of educators. Additionally, inferences on changing motiva-
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

cognitive theories were not dichotomous but complementary to each


tion were made based on year-level differences rather than continu- other. Cognitive theories seek to reveal the conscious thought proc-
ous data on the same group of students. Mapping changes in esses, which lead to behavior. Social-cognitive theories reflect the
motivation are done best via microgenetic studies, which scrutinize use of conscious thought, making it suited for the educational con-
motivation as students develop task expertise over time (Pintrich text, i.e., educators can positively influence students’ thought proc-
2003). Understanding motivational changes should aid researchers esses. Accordingly, current motivational research focuses on using
and educators to design systems and programs that seek to reverse theories that at least account for social-cognitive constructs as me-
any negative aspects of changing motivation. If motivation is diators of desired outcomes such as motivation (Pintrich 2003).
assumed to be constant over a period, changes in teaching styles Building on the work of Vroom’s (1964) expectancy-valence
and course content aimed to propel motivation would be shot theory of motivation, Porter and Lawler (1968) advocated structur-
off target. Consequently, using a longitudinal design and self- ing the educational environment so that effective performance
determination theory, this research aims to provide a better under- would lead to both intrinsic and extrinsic rewards, which would,
standing of student performance. More specifically, in line with in turn, produce academic satisfaction. As such, intrinsic motiva-
Pintrich’s (2003) recommendations for future exploration in moti- tion would occur when an individual is moved to perform a task due
vational science, the objective of this paper is firstly to determine to it being implicitly enjoyable. In contrast, extrinsic motivation is
what motivates one continuous group of engineering students to fuelled by external forces such as rewards and recognition and
perform during three consecutive academic years. The second presents itself as a means to an end (Singh et al. 2004). Tradition-
objective is to evaluate the link between the changing nature of ally, intrinsic and extrinsic motivational factors have been viewed
motivation and academic performance. These analyses will greatly as mutually exclusive (Hayenga and Corpus 2010). The simple
help the engineering education literature address the present chal- dichotomy between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation made the
lenges of student attrition described by Harris and Moll (2015). cognitive theory difficult to apply to educational settings (Gagné
A further benefit derived from this study involves an understanding and Deci 2005) because there is a focus on one or the other, that
of the risk factors that promote individual motivational develop- is, either on promoting intrinsic motivation through participation
ment. The findings, therefore, assist educators in ensuring civil and empowerment while minimizing the use of extrinsic factors
engineering students graduate as well-educated, driven, and inno- or, alternatively, on using rewards and other extrinsic contingencies
vative professionals. to maximize extrinsic motivation while ignoring the importance of
intrinsic motivation. Within this context, intrinsic motivation has
been linked to adaptive learning and high academic achievement
Exploring Self-Determination Theory whereas extrinsic motivation has been correlated to negative emo-
tions, maladaptive coping strategies, and poor academic achieve-
Motivational theories are used to provide explanations and insight ment (Corpus et al. 2009). A study by Ryan and Deci (2000)
into the direction and energization of human behavior (Pintrich has shown, however, that levels of extrinsic motivation do produce
2003). The many motivational theories that exist can usually be positive outcomes. Ryan and Deci (2000) establish this view in
placed in one of two categories: needs/motive-based theories light of the self-determination theory (SDT). SDT proposes that,
and cognitive theories. Needs/motive-based theories approach mo- under optimal conditions, people can, at any time, fully integrate
tivation by investigating the implicit human necessities that drive a new regulation, or can integrate an existing regulation that
behavioral patterns. They suggest that individuals are motivated by had been only partially internalized. That is, SDT posits a self-
the desire to achieve or maintain the various conditions upon which determination continuum (Gagné and Deci 2005).
basic satisfactions reside (Maslow 1968). The degree of satisfaction Central to SDT is the distinction between autonomous motiva-
is goal specific, and the underlying assumption as it pertains to ed- tion and controlled motivation (Gagné and Deci 2005). Autonomy
ucation is that as one changes the goal from bachelors to masters, involves acting with a sense of volition and having the experience
and ultimately a doctorate, needs are being progressively satisfied of choice. Autonomy means endorsing one’s actions at the highest
from lower levels to higher levels. However, at the micro level, for level of reflection (Dworkin 1988). In contrast, being controlled
example, while pursuing a bachelor’s program, students do not involves acting with a sense of pressure, a sense of having to en-
strive to satisfy a need for a lower mark in a course, and then gage in the actions. The use of extrinsic rewards in the early experi-
work toward a higher score in subsequent examinations. At all ments was found to induce controlled motivation (e.g., Deci 1971).
times, good students strive toward the highest achievement level. SDT postulates that autonomous and controlled motivations
Achievement goal theory, which provides a framework for under- differ regarding both their underlying regulatory processes and
standing goals students might have for pursuing particular their accompanying experiences, and it further suggests that behav-
academic tasks, supports this concept of motivation (Ciani et al. iors can be characterized in terms of the degree to which they
2011). However, this theory has its limitations when goals are are autonomous versus controlled. Autonomous motivation and

© ASCE 04018005-2 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


controlled motivation are both intentional, and together they stand with time, good quality and high quantity motivation decreases
in contrast to amotivation, which involves a lack of intention and while low quantity and poor quality motivation increases (Corpus
motivation. Social-cognitive constructs, as previously stated, medi- and Wormington 2014; Hayenga and Corpus 2010; Otis et al. 2005;
ate between need and behavior and can be grouped into the five Spinath and Steinmayr 2008). The quality of motivation identifies
main subsets of self-efficacy, control beliefs, interests, values, both productive and counter-productive behaviors and creates a
and goals. Self-efficacy, competence, and expectancy relate to stu- snapshot of an individual’s effectiveness and efficiency toward mo-
dents believing they have the ability to and the will to achieve a tivational regulation strategies used, whereas the quantity of moti-
particular goal. Control constructs reflect students’ beliefs about vation addresses the extent of motivational regulation strategies
their ability to be an influential part of the learning process. Addi- used (Engelschalk et al. 2017).
tionally, in pursuit of a specific goal, students’ interest and appre- Performance goal adoption leads to nonadaptive, helpless mo-
ciation of the value of the information are important. Each concept tivational patterns of achievement behavior (Valentini and Rudisill
can be intrinsic or extrinsic in nature, and research has shown 2006). Individuals oriented toward performance goals are moti-
that adaptive quantities lead to more cognitive learning via self- vated to seek public recognition, to obtain positive and avoid
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

regulatory practices, persistence, cognitive engagement, and thus negative judgments about their performance, and to compare their
higher levels of motivation and academic performance (Bandura performance and effort with others (Valentini and Rudisill 2006).
1993; Benson et al. 2013; Pintrich 2003; Pintrich and Schunk Thus, the relationship highlighted by Martin et al. (2014) in which
1996). Motivational factors are also seen as precursors to intrinsic academic achievement relates to social acceptance may be con-
and extrinsic motivation (Alpay et al. 2008; Kolmos et al. 2013; nected to the formation of performance-related goals in higher
Law et al. 2009). As such, motivational factors can be considered achieving students. The cross-sectional nature of the research by
an alias of sorts reflecting combinations of students’ self-efficacy Martin et al. (2014) limits the drawing of a full conclusion. A lon-
beliefs, control beliefs, interests, values, and goals. gitudinal study, mapping the motivational factors associated with
In the work of Ryan and Deci (2000), motivation is presented as the same group of students, could more accurately identify relation-
a spectrum from amotivation (lack of motivation) through extrinsic ships between changing motivational patterns and academic
motivation to intrinsic motivation. Behavior is shown to become achievement. The present study fills this gap. Results are expected
progressively internal and self-determined. The first established to show that over time students are motivated by a combination of
point after amotivation is external regulation. External rewards intrinsic and extrinsic motivational factors. Additionally, extrinsic
and recognition drive students who fall into this category. At the motivational factors are expected to increase while intrinsic motiva-
next level, labelled introjection, students engage in learning based tional factors are expected to decrease with time. Also, higher qual-
on a sense of responsibility rooted in the desire to gain approval ity motivation is anticipated to lead to higher cognitive learning,
from self and others. Thirdly, when students participate in tasks adaptive skills, and academic achievement.
to achieve goals that are important to them identification comes
into play. In its final and most autonomous state of extrinsic mo-
tivation, integration would occur when students aim toward a goal, Research Method and Design
which via internalization has become congruent with their values
and needs. More recent research has confirmed this theory and also This study consisted of two parts: in the first part a cross-sectional
revealed that the quality of motivation is more essential, and thus evaluation (Cohort 1, Cohort 2, and Cohort 3) of the motivation
more directly associated with positive outcomes than the quantity profile for the students in a three-year program was undertaken,
of motivation (Corpus and Wormington 2014; Lin et al. 2003; and the second part, being longitudinal, maps the movement of
Vansteenkiste et al. 2009). The quality of motivation refers to Cohort 1 as they develop from Year 1 to Year 3. This work reports
the ratio of intrinsic to extrinsic motivation versus the quantity the findings of the longitudinal section of the survey. To achieve the
of motivation, which is merely concerned with the amount of objectives, a quantitative survey was executed via structured writ-
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. ten questionnaires. Neither Law and Chuah (2009), Law et al.
Martin et al. (2014), via a cross-sectional study, determined the (2009), Kolmos et al. (2013), nor Alpay et al. (2008) evaluated
motivational factors affecting students enrolled in an undergraduate the five key motivational constructs of SDT as expressed by Ryan
civil and environmental engineering degree in 2013–2014. and Deci (2000). The work of Martin et al. (2014) makes such a
Although the findings did not provide conclusive evidence relating consideration by adopting the three instruments (Table 1); this
intrinsic motivation to academic performance, they illustrate that approach increased the criterion validity of the questionnaire used.
different levels of extrinsic motivation do play a salient part in pre- They further tested the clarity of the final instrument using the
dicting academic achievement. The factors job/career and perfec- comments of two lecturers and three students of the Civil and
tionist motivation were found to change from one academic level to Environmental Engineering department. This study also uses this
another, providing a basis for further exploration of the varying questionnaire.
nature of motivation. The perfectionist factor associated with the The questionnaire consists of two main sections. Section 1
more autonomous intrinsic constructs was found to decrease from called “Demographic Information and Academic Level” inquired
Level 1 to Level 2 and Level 2 to Level 3. Research has revealed about students’ gender, age, year of study, nationality, and current
that motivation, since it is based on cognitive processes, can change GPA. Section 2 consists of nine questions seeking to determine
with time (Pintrich and Schunk 1996). Pintrich and Schunk (1996) why the students chose engineering and 22 motivational attributes.
stated that working toward a goal, which has both intrinsic and ex- The motivational attributes posed the central question of “What do
trinsic value, causes intrinsic motivation to decrease over time. That you think is driving your desire to complete your degree?” Ques-
is, once an activity becomes a means to an end, intrinsic motivation tions were geared to evaluate the following motivational factors:
automatically diminishes. This behavior should be the case in individual aspirations/goals, clear direction, individual attitude, re-
educational contexts in which all activities lead to achieving the ward and recognition, parental or mentor motivation, punishment,
external rewards of grades and certificates of accomplishments. and social/group motivation. A five-point Likert scale was designed
In agreement with results found by Martin et al. (2014), the to answer each statement in which 1 meant strongly disagree and 5
trend emerging from studies done on changing motivation is that, meant strongly agree.

© ASCE 04018005-3 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Table 1. Motivational factors assessed based on literature
Category Question
Why did you choose Engineering?
Intrinsic I am interested in the field of engineering
Extrinsic I want to get a good job and/or make money
Intrinsic I want to be able to design and build things
Extrinsic This program has a good reputation
Extrinsic Family/teachers/careers adviser recommended this program
Extrinsic I want to be like my mother or father
Extrinsic All my friends chose engineering
Intrinsic I enjoy learning about new things
Intrinsic I want to further my education
What do you think is driving your desire to complete your degree?
Reward and recognitiona,b,c
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

Studying, learning and achieving something makes my parents/family proud


Clear directiona,d,c I am keen to learn about new aspects of my subject and to explore new ideas
Reward and recognitiona,b,c Completing this degree will help me get a good, well-paying and respectable job
Clear directiona,d,c I get satisfaction from meeting intellectual challenges and pushing my limits
Clear directiona,d,c Studying, understanding and learning increases my knowledge and makes me feel accomplished
Punishmenta,c I don’t want to disappoint my family for fear of the consequences
Social or group motivationa,b,c I enjoy working with my study group/friends and they encourage me
Individual aspirations/goalsa,d,b,c I want to be the top student
Punishmenta,c I want to do well to avoid mistakes (losing scholarship, sponsorship, failing)
Individual attitudea,d,b,c I want to accomplish my own learning goals
Punishmenta If I don’t do well I will be humiliated
Social or group motivationa,b,c All my friends are doing well so I need to do well also
Individual aspirations/goalsa,d,b,c I want to make a difference or contribution to engineering in the world
Group or social pressure/competitiona,b,c I want to outperform my classmates and friends
Individual aspirations/goalsa,d,b,c I want to get good grades and graduate with high honors
Reward and recognitiona,c I just want to get a degree at the end of the day
Parental or mentorb I want to be like my mother or father
Individual attitudea,d,b,c I don’t want to let myself down
Individual attitudea,d,b,c I want to prove something to myself
Individual aspirations/goalsa,d,b,c I believe that what I am learning now will be beneficial to my future job
Parental or mentorb The lecturers are informative, encouraging and good role models
Parental or mentorb My parents/guardians/family encourage me
a
Law and Chuah (2009).
b
Kolmos et al. (2013).
c
Alpay et al. (2008).
d
Law et al. (2009).

Procedure scale items. Separating the variables into positive motivational


and amotivational variables and testing the reliability of the positive
Since the nature of the survey was longitudinal, the questionnaires
were administered in three academic years, 2013–2014, 2014– motivational variables only increases the unidimensional nature of
2015, and 2015–2016, targeting the same group of students as the Motivational Attribute scale (Martin et al. 2016; Schmitt 1996).
they moved through each year level. To increase the probability A measure of 0.7 for Cronbach’s alpha was used to indicate
of maximum response, participants were asked to volunteer to take whether acceptable results were obtained (George and Mallery
the survey during class time. Overall 134 responses were secured. 2011). Reliability in longitudinal data collection can be addressed
Of those, 55 students were from Year 1 of the academic year 2013– through triangulation, which refers to the verification of data by
2014, 36 were from Year 2 of the academic year 2014–2015, and 43 multiple reference points (Street and Ward 2012). Theoretically,
were from Year 3 of the academic year 2015–2016. Ipsative nor- triangulation of data sources increases reliability because each type
malization is essential for longitudinal (repeated-measures) studies, of data provides a unique but overlapping perspective on the same
particularly, to transform the data for individual observations and phenomenon (Lustick 1996). As such, alpha was determined for
remove baseline differences between years (Yarnold 1988). All data each cohort and the entire study population in the cross-sectional
were therefore normalized before further analysis. To evaluate what study, as well as each year group in the longitudinal study. The
motivates students the approach used by Law and Chuah (2009) former evaluation, as separate students do it, confirms the consis-
was used to identify positive motivational variables (mean response tency in the interpretation of the instrument, whereas the latter com-
greater than three) and the amotivational variables (mean response parison confirms the reliability of the measure of change (Salthouse
less than three). Microsoft Excel 2016 and SPSS v19.0 were used and Tucker-Drob 2008).
for further analysis. Some longitudinal case studies do not explicitly recognize time
as a variable within the research design, and it is not uncommon
for time to be only implicitly reflected in research (Schonfeld
Reliability and Validity Analysis and Rindskopf 2007; Saunders and Kim (2007). Reasons for such
Cronbach’s alpha is a widely used construct to show the reliability assertions by some researchers (Stengers and Gille 1997;
of survey instruments. It measures the inter-item correlation of Nandhakumar 2002) involve the regard for time as a social mental

© ASCE 04018005-4 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


construct. Within this social domain, time is used to keep track of analyses was p ≤ 0.05. Subsequently, testing was conducted for
what we are doing and where we are, and therefore, time passes at each academic year to assess possible differences and explanations
different rates and with different salience for each individual (Das for those differences.
1993; Street and Ward 2012). Notwithstanding these views, validity To minimize the likelihood that the cohort under investigation
in this work was improved through consideration of the time unit has a unique experience from another cohort of the same age, cross-
and time boundary. sectional data for the second year and final year students from the
Time unit validity deals with the question of how to segment the (2013–2014) academic year are used as a comparative reference for
timeline (Street and Ward 2012), be it months, semesters, or years. the longitudinal study. The Spearman coefficient was thus deter-
To minimize measurement error and inaccurate conclusions when mined for Year 3 as well as Year 2 of the longitudinal study com-
the time unit is too large or too small, we examined the issue of pared to Year 3 and Year 2 of the cross-sectional study.
whether the selected time unit (years) is appropriately sensitive
for capturing changes in the measured motivational variables.
The unit of one year was deemed suitable because the anticipated Differences between Groups
cognitive changes should be recognizable over this period.
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

Damci (2015) and Kolmos et al. (2013) have established the im-
Time boundaries validity deals with the question of how long of portance of demographics on perception to motivation. However,
a timeline. Time boundaries validity addresses the issue of whether there is no conclusive evidence on motivators toward the high per-
the beginning and ending points of the total observation window formance of engineering students. Particularly, with the growing
have been set in such a way as to capture all significant events number of female students in engineering, it becomes critical to
or effects that occurred in the process under study. Time boundaries examine any gender differences that may exist in students’ moti-
validity is a form of content validity indicating the degree to which vation (Marra et al. 2009).
the domain is accurately reflected in the measure. Time boundaries To understand whether there is an interaction between the inde-
validity was assessed by considering the use of a formal protocol pendent motivational variables on year group, gender, nationality,
for defining the start and end conditions of the degree. and GPA, a two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
was undertaken. To appraise which motivational variables signifi-
cantly changed with progression and how those changes occur,
Relative Importance Index and Spearman’s Coefficient
a one-way ANOVA was carried out on the variables Age (≤18,
Analysis
19–20, 21–22, 23–24, >24), and academic performance [Low
The relative importance index (RII) and Spearman’s coefficient GPA (≤1.49), Intermediate GPA (1.50–2.99), High GPA (≥3.00)].
(rho) were used to conduct a preliminary evaluation to determine Subsequently, independent sample t-test on gender (male and
if the responses from the various academic years differed. The RII female), and nationality (Trinidadian and non-Trinidadian) was
was used to rank the motivational variables for each academic year. performed.
Higher values of RII indicate higher levels of importance/ For all the tests, the null hypothesis (H0 ) states that there is no
motivation of the incident variable. RII was evaluated in Microsoft significant difference between the means of groups. The alternative
Excel 2016 using the following equation: hypothesis (H1 ) states that there is a significant difference between
P5 the means of groups. If a significant difference is observed based on
w i xi
RII ¼ Pi¼1 5
ð1Þ academic performance, the source of these differences for each aca-
i¼1 xi demic year was explored. One-way ANOVA and t-tests were car-
ried out to decipher any significant differences between the GPA
where wi = weighting to the ith response (wi ¼ 0, 1, 2, 3, 4 for means of the following groups:
i ¼ 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, respectively); and xi = frequency of the ith • Gender: male and female (independent sample t-test)
response; i = response category index = 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 ranging from • Nationality: Trinidadian and non-Trinidadian (independent
strongly disagree to strongly agree. sample t-test)
Spearman’s coefficient is a nonparametric measure of correla- • Age: ≤18, 19–20, 21–22, 23–24, >24 (one-way ANOVA)
tion between two sets of data that can be ranked. Assaf and Al-Hejji
(2006) used the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient to deter-
mine whether there is a degree of agreement between parties
concerning their ranking of factors. For this study, Spearman cor- Results
relation is used to evaluate the agreement of ranking for motiva-
tional variables between academic years. Spearman’s coefficient Students’ Motivation
(rho) was calculated using the following formula (Zar 1972) Table 2 highlights the population characteristics for each academic
P year. Motivation in students before the start of an engineering
6 d2
rho ¼ 1 − 3 ð2Þ undergraduate degree can also affect the extent of motivation dur-
n −n
ing the degree and might be a possible indicator of academic per-
where d = difference between the RII of a motivational variable for formance (Dias 2011). Thus, Fig. 1 illustrates the mean score as to
two academic years; and n = number of variables in each of the two why persons chose engineering as a career.
groups being compared. The reliability of the estimates for the cross-sectional and
Spearman’s coefficient (rho) values range from −1 to þ1, where longitudinal studies is shown in Table 3. In each year of study,
the magnitude of the value indicates the strength of the relationship to address the objective of determining what motivates students,
and the direction (negative or positive) reflects the nature of the only the positive motivational variables were considered to contrib-
relationship. The null hypothesis (H0 ) states that the motivational ute toward students’ motivation. Twenty-one positive motivational
variables’ ranking from one academic level to another does not variables contribute toward students’ motivation in Year 1,
change. The alternative hypothesis (H1 ) states that the motivational whereas in Years 2 and 3, 18 positive motivational variables
variables’ ranking from one academic level to another does change. defined students’ motivation. The remaining the variables were
The significance level used for this test and all other statistical amotivational.

© ASCE 04018005-5 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Table 2. Years 1, 2, and 3 demographic information The results of the one-way ANOVA showed that there were sig-
Variable Category Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 nificant differences between the means for the motivational variable
“I want to be the top student” (F ½2; 131 ¼ 4.536; p ¼ 0.012) and
Age <18 5 2 0
the year groups. Least-square-difference (LSD) analysis (equal var-
19–20 36 15 4
21–22 8 15 18
iances assumed) indicated that there was a significant difference
23–24 4 2 17 between the means for the variable when comparing Year 1 and
>24 2 2 4 Year 3 students, as well as for Year 2 and Year 3 students. A sig-
GPA <1.49 6 3 1 nificant difference also occurred for the motivational variable “I
1.50–2.99 31 23 30 want to do well to avoid mistakes (losing scholarship, sponsorship,
>3 18 10 12 failing, etc.)” (F ½2; 130 ¼ 3.857; p ¼ 0.024). Tamhane analysis
Country of origin Trinidadian 30 21 19 (equal variances not assumed) also showed a significant difference
Other 25 15 24 between the means of this variable for Year 1 and Year 3 students.
Gender Male 38 20 28
Female 17 16 15
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

Investigating Positive Motivational Variables versus


Gender for Each Academic Year
For the two-way MANOVA analysis, gender differed significantly
with p ≤ 0.003 when tested with the Wilks’s lambda. As such, sig-
nificant differences between the means for male and female in Year
1 were found for a number of the motivational variables. Firstly, for
the variable “I get satisfaction from meeting intellectual challenges
and pushing my limits” the mean score for males was 4.05
(SD ¼ 0.868), whereas the average score for the females was
4.53 (SD ¼ 0.624). The independent t-test revealed t ¼ −2.036,
degree of fredom ðdfÞ ¼ 53, and p ¼ 0.047. For the variable
“Studying, understanding and learning increases my knowledge
and makes me feel accomplished” the analysis produced a mean
male score of 4.32 (SD ¼ 0.574) and a mean female score of
4.76 (SD ¼ 0.437). The independent sample t-test revealed
t ¼ −2.866, df ¼ 53, and p ¼ 0.006. Similarly, for the variable
“I want to accomplish my learning goals” the mean male score was
found to be 4.08 (SD ¼ 0.749), whereas the mean female score
was 4.53 (SD ¼ 0.800). The t-test results revealed significance
at t ¼ −2.019, df ¼ 53, p ¼ 0.049. For the variable “I want to
make a difference or contribution to engineering in the world”
the mean score for males was 4.13 (SD ¼ 0.875), whereas the
Fig. 1. Why students choose engineering. mean female score was 4.71 (SD ¼ 0.470). The independent t-test
results showed that t ¼ −3.155, df ¼ 51.05, and p ¼ 0.003. Lastly,
the variable “I want to get good grades and graduate with high hon-
Investigating Motivational Differences between ors” the male, mean score was shown to be 4.11 (SD ¼ 1.008),
Academic Year of Study whereas the female score was 4.88 (SD ¼ 0.332). The t-test results
indicated that t ¼ −4.264, df ¼ 50.29, and p < 0.00005.
Table 4 provides the relative importance and rank of each motiva- Four of the five variables that showed a significant difference for
tional variable by year. Spearman’s correlation analysis revealed the students in Year 1 also showed a significant difference when
that the motivational variables’ ranking did significantly change they moved on to Year 2. For the clear direction variable “I get
from Year 1 to Year 2 (rho ¼ 0.881) as well as from Year 2 to satisfaction from meeting intellectual challenges and pushing my
Year 3 (rho ¼ 0.876). Additionally, the motivational variables’ limits” the mean male score was 3.95 (SD ¼ 0.510), whereas
ranking between Years 1 and 3 also displayed significant change the mean female score was 4.50 (SD ¼ 0.516). The independent
(rho ¼ 0.878). For the three analyses conducted the null hypothesis t-test showed the following results: t ¼ −3.192, df ¼ 32.12, and
(H0 ) was rejected at the 5% significance level. All the rho values p ¼ 0.003. Similarly, for the rewards and recognition variable
obtained indicate a very strong positive relationship. A Spearman “Studying, understanding and learning increases my knowledge
coefficient between Year 3 (cross-sectional) and Year 3 (longitudi- and makes me feel accomplished” the mean male score was found
nal) showed strong positive correlation (rho ¼ 0.8184). Similarly, a to be 4.25 (SD ¼ 0.550), whereas the mean female score was found
Spearman coefficient between Year 2 (cross-sectional) and Year 2 to be 4.63 (SD ¼ 0.500). The independent t-test indicates that
(longitudinal) showed strong positive correlation (rho ¼ 0.902). t ¼ −2.115, df ¼ 34, and p ¼ 0.042. For the individual attitude
Both comparisons nullify the effect of differences existing between variable “I want to accomplish my learning goals” the mean male
this cohort under investigation and similar cohorts. score was 4.21 (SD ¼ 0.535), and the mean female score was 4.63

Table 3. Reliability for the respective studies by year


Study type Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 Overall
Cross-sectional 0.761 (n ¼ 55) 0.817 (n ¼ 46) 0.778 (n ¼ 43) 0.805 (n ¼ 144)
Longitudinal 0.761 (n ¼ 55) 0.794 (n ¼ 36) 0.787 (n ¼ 43) 0.779 (n ¼ 134)

© ASCE 04018005-6 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Table 4. Relative importance index and Spearman rank
Year 1 Year 2 Year 3
Motivational attributes of the students RII Rank RII Rank RII Rank
Studying, learning and achieving something makes my parents/family proud. 3.27 9 3.22 8.0 3.07 11.0
I am keen to learn about new aspects of my subject area and explore new ideas. 3.25 10 3.36 5.0 3.26 7.0
Completing this degree will help me get a good, well-paying and respectable job. 3.40 4 3.19 9.5 3.28 6.0
I get satisfaction from meeting intellectual challenges and pushing my limits. 3.20 12 3.19 9.5 3.14 9.0
Studying, understanding and learning increases my knowledge and makes me feel accomplished. 3.45 2 3.42 2.0 3.35 4.5
I don’t want to disappoint my family. 3.16 13 2.94 14.0 2.81 13.0
I enjoy working with my study group/friends and they encourage me. 2.58 16 2.81 15.0 2.53 16.0
I want to be top student.a 2.64 15 2.64 16.0 1.98 19.0
I want to do well to avoid mistakes (losing scholarship, sponsorship, failing etc.). 3.13 14 3.08 12.0 2.60 14.5
I want to accomplish my own learning goals. 3.22 11 3.40 4.0 3.19 8.0
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

If I don’t do well I will be humiliated. 2.13 19.5 2.22 18.0 2.05 18.0
All my friends are doing well so I need to do well also.a,b 2.15 18 1.83 20.0 1.79 20.0
I want to make a difference or contribution to engineering in the world. 3.31 7 3.31 6 3.35 4.5
I want to outperform my classmates and friends.a,b 2.13 19.5 2.00 19 1.56 21
I want to get good grades and graduate with high honors. 3.43 3.0 3.25 7 2.91 12
I just want to get a degree at the end of the day. 2.35 17.0 2.36 17 2.60 14.5
I want to be like my mother or father.a,b,c 1.41 22.0 1.39 22 1.42 22
I don’t want to let myself down. 3.58 1.0 3.42 2 3.37 3
I want to prove something to myself. 3.30 8.0 2.97 13 3.12 10
I believe that what I am learning now will be beneficial to my future job. 3.36 5.5 3.17 11 3.42 1.5
The lecturers are informative, encouraging and good role models.b 2.07 21 1.72 21 2.14 17
My parents/guardians/family encourage me. 3.36 5.5 3.42 2 3.42 1.5
a,b,c
Amotivational variables for each year. Cronbach’s alpha for amotivational variables was found to be 0.782; 0.818; 0.729 for overall, Year 2, and Year 3.
Year 1 represents a single item; as such, the reliability is not determined.

(SD ¼ 0.619). The t-test conducted showed that t ¼ −2.125, get a degree at the end of the day” was also observed to be
df ¼ 33, and p ¼ 0.041. Evaluation of the individual attitude var- (F ½2; 52 ¼ 4.348; p ¼ 0.018). Tamhane analysis illustrated that
iable “I want to make a difference or contribution to engineering in significant mean difference occurred between students with low
the world” resulted in a male mean score of 3.90 (SD ¼ 0.553) and and intermediate GPA as well as between students with low and
a female, mean score of 4.81 (0.403). The independent t-test done high GPA. Thirdly, a significant difference was also found between
showed that t ¼ −5.527, df ¼ 34, and p ¼ 0.000. the mean scores for the individual attitude variable “I want to prove
When the students moved to Year 3, only one variable showed something to myself” (F ½2; 52 ¼ 3.383; p ¼ 0.042). Tamhane
significant difference based on the gender of the students. For the analysis revealed that significant difference occurred between stu-
clear direction variable “I am keen to learn about the new aspects of dents with low and intermediate GPA scores as well as between
my subject area and to explore new ideas” the mean male score was those with low and high GPA scores.
found to be 4.04 (SD ¼ 0.693), whereas the female score was No significant difference was found for the means of the vari-
found to be 4.67 (SD ¼ 0.488). The independent t-test yielded ables for Year 2 students based on GPA.
the following results: t ¼ −3.128, df ¼ 41, and p ¼ 0.003. Only one student had a low GPA in Year 3, which deemed one-
Two-way MANOVA indicates a significance of p <¼ 0.002 for way ANOVA inappropriate. As such, this student was removed
Wilks’s lambda when both age and gender are compared on the from the analysis and an independent t-test used to investigate pos-
motivational variables. sible differences between the mean responses of the remaining two
groups and the motivational variables. As expressed by the Year 3
students, there are significant differences observed between the
Positive Motivational Variable versus Nationality
mean GPA groups for the motivational variables. Analysis on
or Age
personal attitude variable “I want to get good grades and graduate
No significant differences were observed for the mean values given with high honors” produced the following results: t ¼ −2.335,
for the motivational variables based on respondent’s nationality or df ¼ 41, and p ¼ 0.025. The mean value obtained for students with
age when they were in Year 1, Year 2, or Year 3. intermediate GPA was found to be 3.67 (SD ¼ 1.155), whereas
the mean for students with high GPA was found to be 4.50
(SD ¼ 0.674). The results indicate that students in Year 3 with high
Investigating Positive Motivational Differences Against GPA scores were more motivated by attaining high grades than stu-
GPA dents with intermediate GPA scores. For the rewards and recogni-
A significant difference was observed between the mean scores tion variable “I just want to get a degree at the end of the day” a
for three motivational variables based on GPA grouping for Year significant difference was also observed between the mean scores
1 students. Firstly, analysis on the punishment variable “If I do not of the students with high and intermediate GPA scores (t ¼ 2.672,
do well I will be humiliated” produced the following results: df ¼ 40, and p ¼ 0.011). The mean score obtained for students
F ½2; 52 ¼ 3.200 with p ¼ 0.049. LSD analysis showed there with intermediate GPA scores was 3.90 (SD ¼ 1.145), whereas
was a significant difference between students with low and inter- the mean score for students with high GPA scores was 2.75
mediate GPA scores as well as between those with low and high (SD ¼ 1.485). The results show that students in Year 3 with inter-
GPA scores for this variable. A significant difference between the mediate GPA scores are more motivated by the desire for a degree
mean scores for the rewards and recognition variable “I just want to compared to students with high GPA scores.

© ASCE 04018005-7 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Academic Performance (GPA) versus Gender, Age, Cognitive processes progress through the following stages: remem-
and Nationality bering, understanding, application, analyzing, evaluation, and
creation. Students’ motivation may be decreasing due to various
A significant difference was obtained between the mean GPA
reasons such as disappointing outcomes after applying effort to
scores for the different ages (F ½4; 50 ¼ 3.049; p ¼ 0.025). LSD
analysis revealed that significant differences occurred between the increasingly challenging workload. Longitudinal motivational
students of the following age ranges: 19–20 and 21–22 years studies have shown that over time the quantity of motivation de-
old, 21–22 and 23–24 years old, and 21–22 and >24 years old. creases. For instance, Hayenga and Corpus (2010) revealed that
There were no significant differences found between the over time students with good quantity motivational profiles moved
mean GPA for students of different genders, nationalities or ages toward a lower quantity of motivational profile. Motivation levels
in Years 2 and 3. may decrease over the course of a degree program because of in-
stitutions, in the beginning, raising student expectations only to be
dampened when the student finds out that the reality is not what
Discussion they expected.
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

Secondly, differences were observed that have implications for


the quality of the students’ motivation. Although the decreased
What Variables Motivate Students? quantity of motivation suggests cause for concern, research has
A combination of intrinsic and extrinsic motivational variables was shown that the quality of motivation more strongly predicts cogni-
used to test students’ motivation. Utilizing the self-determination tive learning and engagement (Corpus and Wormington 2014; Lin
theory’s segregation of extrinsic motivation via level of autonomy et al. 2003). In Year 1 the most important motivational variable for
(Ryan and Deci 2000), it can also be said that the presented var- the students was “I don’t want to let myself down.” In Year 2 “Study-
iables test the external regulation, introjection, identification, and ing, understanding and learning increases my knowledge and makes
integration forms of extrinsic motivation. The numerous positive me feel accomplished” (intrinsic), “I don’t want to let myself down,”
motivational variables illustrate that a mixture of intrinsic and ex- and “My parents/guardians/family encourage me” (introjection)
trinsic factors motivates students. Other research (Law and Chuah tied for the first position. Alternatively, in Year 3 the top variables
2009; Law et al. 2009; Husman and Lens 1999) on students’ mo- with the same relative importance emerged to be “I believe that what
tivational factors also found that both intrinsic and extrinsic moti- I am learning now will be beneficial to my future job” (identifica-
vation contributed to students’ motivation. tion) and “My parents/guardians/family encourage me.” Each year
Utilizing the relative importance index (RII), the most important the most important variable(s) became more extrinsic. In Year 2,
positive motivational variable for the students throughout their although an intrinsic variable was introduced, so was an introjection
three years of study was found to be “I don’t want to let myself motivational variable. In Year 3, an identification variable (some-
down.” This variable suggests that the goal of attaining their degree what internal) replaced the intrinsic and integration variables asso-
has become an internal part of who the students’ consider them- ciated with Year 2. Additionally, the amotivational variables in Year
selves to be. This variable is the most intrinsic form of extrinsic 2 were all introjection (parental/mentor and social/group) motiva-
motivation (Ryan and Deci 2000). A variable that has a mean score tional variables. In Year 3, however, one of the variables “I want
less than three is not considered to motivate the students. A concept to be top student” stood out. Unlike the previous observations, this
that does not have motivational power does not necessarily have variable belongs to the individual aspirations and goals group. In
demotivating power. Hence, the variables with a mean score less agreement with self-determination theory, this variable displays
than three are deemed amotivational variables. The four amotiva- identification characteristics. In light of achievement goal theory,
tional variables from the overall analysis were introjection varia- “I want to be the top student” illustrates performance approach
bles. The driving force tested was either due to parental/mentor behavior. The achievement goal theory postulates two reasons
motivation or social/group motivation focusing on gaining others’ for student engagement: mastery and performance goals. According
approval. This result may indicate that throughout the students’ to Meece et al. (2006), mastery goals reflect perfectionism. Students
studies motivational factors, which are rooted outside of the stu- focus on mastering a skill, advancing to more challenging levels.
dents, do not substantially contribute to their motivation to com- With performance goals, on the other hand, students engage in
plete their degree. Law and Chuah (2009) and Law et al. (2009) learning activities to demonstrate higher abilities when compared
concluded that intrinsic motivational factors contribute more to stu- to other students. Both goals can be viewed from approach and
dents’ motivation on an individual level compared to that of extrin- avoidance perspectives. Approach goals involve positive attitudes,
sic motivation. whereas avoidance goals involve negative attitudes. Commonly,
mastery goals have been attributed to positive outcomes such as in-
trinsic motivation, whereas performance goals have been concomi-
How Do the Positive and Amotivational Variables tant to the opposite. Recently, however, performance approach
Change over Time? behavior has been found to contribute to intrinsic motivation leading
A similar evaluation of each academic year indicated variations in to more adaptive behavior such as cognitive learning and the devel-
the positive and amotivational variables that affected the students. opment of metacognitive and self-regulatory practices (Husman and
Firstly, the number of amotivational variables for each year level Lens 1999; Meece et al. 2006). The more internal motivation be-
differed. While the students were in Year 1, only one introjection comes, the higher its quality and positive outcomes. As such, the
variable (parental/mentor) did not contribute to their motivation. In fact that this variable is not positively contributing to students’ mo-
Year 2 and Year 3, this value increased to four variables, possibly tivation, coupled with the changes observed for the most important
signifying that the quantity of the students’ motivation is decreas- variables in each year of study, demonstrates that the internal nature
ing. As students progress in their program of choice, the difficulty of their motivation is decreasing throughout the course. As students
level of the material increases. Educational programs are often de- get nearer to their goal, the desire to attain the goal in and of
signed in this way to cause students to grow and develop cogni- itself may be increasing, thus shifting the focus from performing
tively. Bloom’s taxonomy illustrates the hierarchy of learning at the highest level to merely passing to reach the target. The
that numerous academic programs are built on (Seaman 2011). overjustification theory (Pintrich and Schunk 1996) supports this

© ASCE 04018005-8 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


interpretation because it hypothesizes that once both intrinsic and men do (Kolmos et al. 2013). Consequently, females who choose
extrinsic motivations exist in the same arena: as time passes extrinsic to pursue core engineering disciplines such as civil engineering
motivation undermines intrinsic motivation. would be thought to integrate their personal values to their
academic/career goals. Some of the significant variables such as
“I want to make a difference or contribution to the engineering
Do the Motivational Variables Change Significantly world” and “I am keen to learn about the new aspects of my subject
Over Time? area and to explore new ideas” illustrate this. The same trend was
Statistically, the Spearman’s correlation coefficients indicated that observed in Alpay et al. (2008) for similar testing on the motiva-
there were significant changes in the motivational variables as the tional variable called “Make a difference in the world.” The vari-
students went through each academic level. Results from one-way ables “I want to get good grades and graduate with high honors,” “I
ANOVA solidify the correlation evaluation. The motivational want to accomplish my learning goals,” and “I get satisfaction from
strength of the variable “I want to be top student” significantly de- meeting intellectual challenges and pushing my limits” suggest
creased from Year 1 to Year 3 and from Year 2 to Year 3, supporting self-regulatory behavior. Self-regulation speaks to a student’s abil-
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

the previous observation and reasoning. Based on the various the- ity to organize the way they prepare or move toward accomplishing
ories, this result, along with previous observations made, seems to their goals. Meece and Painter (2008) reported on various works
demonstrate that the degree of internal motivation is decreasing. done on motivation and self-regulatory practices with gender in
The second variable found to be significant, “I want to avoid mind. Although conflicted results were obtained based on student
making mistakes (losing a scholarship, sponsorship, failing, motivation, the studies on self-regulation indicated that often fe-
etc.),” showed that the students were more motivated by this fear males display better cognitive and metacognitive practices such
when they were in Year 1 as opposed to Year 3. In the first year of as intermediate goal setting, planning, and environmental structur-
the undergraduate civil engineering degree programs, there are ing than males do. Good self-regulatory practices are associated
several crucial courses that are commonly known to have high fail- with intrinsic motivation.
ure rates. Since these courses are prerequisites to Level 2 courses,
failure would usually lead to an additional year of study. Year-1
students are made aware of this, and the fear associated with such Is There a Significant Change in Motivational Variables
failure can be high. Conversely, in Year 3 during the second semes- over Time Based on Gender?
ter (when the questionnaire was administered) students have com- As we moved from one academic year to the next, the number
pleted the majority of their courses. At that point in their studies,
of significantly different motivational variables due to gender de-
fear of making mistakes may be less since the probability and sub-
creased. The number of significant variables dropped from five var-
sequent risk of failure attached to their remaining time in university
iables in Year 1 to four in Year 2 to one in Year 3. Evaluation of the
is low. The self-determination theory describes the motivation that
male and female means of each variable for each academic level
occurs due to fear of punishment from an external source as exter-
showed that both genders mostly decreased in succession. This
nally regulated. Furthermore, regarding the achievement goal
behavior may indicate that students’ overall intrinsic motivation
theory, this variable shows characteristics of performance avoid-
decreased as time passed. When Alpay et al. (2008) investigated
ance, in which students strive to do well to avoid failure. Studies
have demonstrated that both external regulation extrinsic and per- the motivation of male and female students as they passed through
formance avoidance behavior lead to the most maladaptive forms of each academic level, the overall motivation (male and female) was
educational practices (Eccles and Wigfield 2002; Elliot and Church found to decrease as time passed. Additionally, female motivation
1997; Wolters 2004). These results may indicate that by the end of declined at a slower rate and even increased slightly from their
their course of study students’ extrinsic motivation has decreased. third to the fourth (final) year of study. Other researchers also
show this trend; as time passes, once an extrinsic goal is present,
intrinsic motivation tends to decrease (Corpus and Wormington
Do Trends Exist between the Motivational Variable for 2014; Hayenga and Corpus 2010; Otis et al. 2005; Spinath and
Each Academic Year by Gender? Steinmayr 2008).
Results revealed that throughout the program female students were
significantly more motivated compared to their male classmates Do Trends Exist between the Motivational Variable
based on six variables. The variables all displayed high internal Scores for Each Academic Year by Nationality
qualities, showing intrinsic and integration features. Consequently, and Age?
it can be deduced that females may be more intrinsically motivated
compared to their male classmates. Engineering is a traditionally The results of the statistical analysis showed no significant differ-
male-dominated field, and as such gender disparities exist (Alpay ences for the motivational variables when compared to nationality
et al. 2008; Kolmos et al. 2013). Improvement in the presence of and age in all the academic years. Benson et al. (2013) carried out
females in engineering has occurred in recent times, although a bias motivational tests on Year 2 and Year 3 engineering students. The
is sometimes still evident; in the survey conducted in each year, work evaluated the possible differences in the students’ motivation
there was a greater percentage of male students, although the differ- as a result of ethnicity. However, no significant differences were
ence was not substantial. Even in the advent of a more substantial observed. Ethnicity, similar to nationality and age, proposes stu-
proportion of female participation in the field, research has shown dents from different contexts. Ethnicity and nationality speak to
that females gravitate toward the newly established engineering cultural differences whereas age speaks to developmental differen-
fields, which display broader contextual approaches (Kolmos ces. Benson et al. (2013), attributed the lack of differences to simi-
et al. 2013). Values hold high importance when students choose larities in students’ motivational profiles when they choose to enter
a particular career path (Matusovich et al. 2010). Research has a degree program. The same deduction may apply in the current
shown that female and male values differ. Women are known to circumstance. The vast majority of the students were Caribbean na-
gravitate toward engineering fields that utilize contextual ap- tionals. As such, the results may reflect similar cultural experiences
proaches because they value social context more highly than and values across the region.

© ASCE 04018005-9 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Is There a Relationship between the Motivational If a student wants to get good grades, he/she is more likely to work
Variables, Which Influence Students, and Their toward that goal and therefore is more likely to attain it. The results
Academic Performance (GPA)? show a split in the type of motivation the students observe in their
last semester of their degrees. The group of students with inter-
In Year 1, evaluation showed that for the variable “If I don’t do well
mediate GPA is more extrinsically motivated, focusing on simply
I will be humiliated” students with low GPA scores adhered to this
finishing the program. This variable belongs to the external regu-
variable more than students with high GPA scores. The same trend
lation segment of extrinsic motivation. In the second group of stu-
was noticed for students with intermediate GPA and high GPA. As
dents, however, students with high GPA display more intrinsic
previously discussed, performance avoidance behavior such as that
forms of extrinsic motivation. Needing to obtain good grades
portrayed by this variable is known to precede maladaptive forms
may signify that attaining those grades is important to the students,
of learning and consequently poor academic achievement (Wigfield
resulting in the identification form of extrinsic motivation. A more
and Cambria 2010). Students with higher GPA are in good internal form of motivation is therefore shown to more positively
academic standing and would most likely not be overly concerned predict academic achievement. A review of achievement values,
with humiliation from poor academic performance. Their goals goal orientations, and interests by Wigfield and Cambria (2010)
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

have already been met, and their perceived level of competence indicated that numerous research endeavors support the postulated
and resulting self-efficacy may be higher (Pintrich 2003). In the correlation.
study carried out by Benson et al. (2013), the authors also found
that students with higher GPA had significantly higher problem-
solving self-efficacy. Evaluation of the second significant variable, What Influences the GPA Differences?
“I just want a degree at the end of the day,” showed that students
To find out why the significant differences occurred in Year 1 and
with low GPA scores were more motivated by this variable in com-
Year 3 further statistical tests were carried out based on the dem-
parison to students in both the intermediate and high GPA groups.
ographics of the students. In Year 1 mean GPA scores were found to
If a student’s motivation is primarily to obtain a degree, merely
differ based on age groups significantly. The 21–22-year-old stu-
passing their courses would be satisfactory for them. Therefore, this dents outperformed students in all the other age groups. Descriptive
reflects that students who are motivated by this variable do not see statistics revealed that there were eight students in this age range.
the need to do the work to attain high academic standards. This No significant difference was found based on age or any other dem-
thought pattern would reflect in their academic grades. Extrinsic ographic dissimilarities for GPA in Year 3. The results do not allow
motivation, as embodied here, is linked in the literature to poor for solid interpretation of the observed phenomenon. This observa-
academic performance. The same trends were observed for the tion may simply reflect the specific situation, especially since the
third significant variable, “I want to prove something to myself.” number of students aged 21–22 in Year 1 was small and by Year 2
Students with low GPA would have more to prove as compared to any demarcation based on GPA dissipated.
students with intermediate and high GPA. That is, students with
higher GPA have already proven to themselves that they can attain
satisfactory and good grades. Overall, in Year 1 students with Implication to Educational Practice
higher GPA scores seem to be more intrinsically motivated.
Over time, the motivation of the students decreased in both quantity
No statistical differences were highlighted in Year 2’s GPA
and quality. The study revealed that as students progressed, their
evaluation. Hence, students’ motivational profiles do not differ motivation became more extrinsic—deteriorating the quality of
based on GPA groups in Year 2. There is not enough evidence their motivation. This information reinforces the fact that as time
to extract conclusions from this data explaining this phenomenon, passes students would greatly benefit from an increasingly greater
leaving questions and room for further research. The motivational intensity of intervention and guidance that pushes them toward in-
variables used in this study test the social cognitive constructs of trinsic motivation because the quality of motivation is of highest
values, goals, and interests directly. However, they do not directly concern. Specifically, the variables like “I want to be top student”
test control and self-efficacy. Research has shown that self-efficacy that differ significantly over time highlight the loss of aspirations
and control could also predict academic performance (Pintrich and goals that intrinsically motivate students. This observation
2003). Previous results suggested that motivation decreased from translates to educators implementing strategies to help students
the first through to the third year. However, do the students’ self- identify and internalize their goals because in the classroom goals
efficacy or control beliefs drop dramatically in Year 2? What are the are considered as the measures that regulate an individual’s actions
implications of such a case on the motivational profiles of students (Schunk 2001). Goals can be progressive from the short-term goal
in the various GPA groups? For instance, in Year 1 the students’ of just earning a good grade on an exam and advancing toward the
attitude may be “I can accomplish academic excellence.” If self- long-term achievement of being the top student in a program. Thus,
efficacy drops in Year 2, their attitude may become “Can I accom- encouraging students to set short-term goals such as studying for a
plish this?” After successfully completing the first semester of Year fixed amount of time for their learning can be a useful way to help
3, self-efficacy may again increase. As the goal draws nearer, the students track their progress and assist in ensuring success on
students’ attitudes may then become “I must accomplish this.” exams (Zimmerman 2004).
Future work should address this behavior. The results also indicated that throughout the three-year study
The results from the students’ third year of study, in fact, support females were more intrinsically motivated than males and as such
this change in attitude hypothesis. The variable “I just want to get a demonstrated more self-regulatory practices compared to men.
degree at the end of the day” showed a significant difference be- Self-regulated learning (SRL) is recognized as an essential predic-
tween students with intermediate GPA and high GPA. Similar to the tor of student academic motivation and achievement (Zimmerman
observations made in Year 1, the students with lower GPA were 2011). This process requires students to in advance plan to accom-
more motivated by this variable when compared to students in plish both their short- and long-term goals for their learning,
the higher GPA group. The second significant variable, “I want self-motivate themselves, monitor their progress, and assess their
to get good grades,” revealed that students with higher GPA scores learning independently. However, few students naturally do this
desire good grades more than students with intermediate scores do. well, and so course assessments must be designed with this in mind

© ASCE 04018005-10 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


so students may use multiple learning strategies and adjust those This study, being longitudinal, seeks to provide deductions
strategies and seek help from others as needed. The males need about changes in motivation with time, and the link between stu-
to be motivated to internalize their goals more and learn to incor- dents’ motivation and academic performance. Evaluation of the
porate better organization and self-evaluation skills. Lecturers can motivational variables revealed that the students were motivated
use the aforementioned strategies to promote self-regulation in their by a combination of intrinsic and extrinsic variables, which were
classrooms. However, they should understand that learners develop observed to decrease in quantity and quality over time. This obser-
at various paces and a strategy that works best for one student may vation was supported by the temporary decline of the two signifi-
not always work with the next. cantly different variables found with respect to academic level: “I
Students in the lower GPA range are also in need of direct at- want to be the top student” and “I want to avoid making mistakes
tention. Overall, students with higher GPAs were more intrinsically (losing a scholarship, sponsorship, failing, etc.). Assessment of sig-
motivated than the rest of the students. For instance, in Year 1 lower nificant disparities in gender revealed that in each academic year
performing students were found to adhere to the variable “If I don’t females were more internally motivated compared to their male
do well I will be humiliated.” This finding indicates a possible de- classmates. However, as time progressed, the trend again emerged
that the internal nature of motivation decreased. A better under-
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

ficiency in perceived competence and self-efficacy. Work should be


done to empower the lower achievers to see their value and believe standing of the relationship between demographics and motivators
in their abilities. should allow lecturers to motivate students individually rather than
The findings strongly support the importance of the social- using a blanket approach.
cognitive constructs to the development of student motivation. Testing of the motivational variables on academic performance
Specific factors in the social context, such as choice and meaning- (GPA) showed that when the students were in Year 1 and Year 3
ful positive feedback, can be thought of as being analogous to spe- those with higher GPA were more internally motivated. An
cific aspects of work contents and settings. There is considerable anomaly characterizes Year 2; no significant differences occurred
similarity between the social-contextual factors that maintain in- between academic performance levels (GPA). This anomaly raises
trinsic motivation and those that facilitate the integration of extrin- questions about what may be influencing this observation. Further
sic motivation. Specifically, autonomy-supportive climates, as well research into this area would be beneficial, providing continuity
as such specific factors as choice and acknowledgement, promote and further insight into the link between motivation and academic
both types of autonomous motivation (Gagné and Deci 2005). achievement. At entry, students had the skills and belief that they
Social support from peers and teachers can serve as a significant will complete the program; as a result, this investigation did not
role as students are learning to be more self-regulative. Findings directly test the social cognitive constructs of self-efficacy; how-
from a study by Patrick et al. (2007) showed that task engagement ever, its importance relating to other motivational variables is dis-
and the use of self-regulated learning strategies were more preva- cussed. Exploring the applicability of this construct and its links to
lent in students that regularly received support from their teachers academic achievement especially for Year 2 students needs to be
and peers. Feedback is the most common form of social support. done. Consequent findings combined with the present results
Research indicates that effective feedback includes information would inform the Civil and Environmental Engineering department
about what students need to improve, what they did well (Labuhn and educators with directions for aiding students’ motivation.
et al. 2010), and steps they can take to enhance their work (Hattie The results and subsequent interpretations illustrate that mea-
and Timperley 2007). Progress feedback assists students in raising sures need to be put in place to assist students as they move from
their academic achievement (Brookhart 2011) and can promote stu- one year level to the next to maintain their internal motivation. The
dent motivation (Wigfield and Cambria 2010) and self-regulation. same sentiments need to be applied to students who are at lower
Students who received feedback from their teachers were more academic performance levels. Further research is required to deter-
likely to use self-regulated learning strategies to improve their tests mine the factors that are causing the students to be demotivated.
scores (Labuhn et al. 2010). For instance, the results of this survey indicated that the variable
“The lecturers are informative, encouraging and good role models”
was rated low in each academic year. Evaluating the effect of teach-
ing styles may fully uncover practical measures that can be taken to
Conclusions and Recommendations
improve the students’ motivation.
Motivation is a concept that elucidates the magnitude and direction
of human behavior (Pintrich 2003). Often educators and research-
ers look to motivation to resolve inconsistencies in students’ aca- References
demic performance. At the Department of Civil and Environmental
Alpay, E., A. L. Ahearn, R. H. Graham, and A. M. J. Bull. 2008. “Student
Engineering, over the past four years, more than half of each under-
enthusiasm for engineering: Charting changes in student aspirations and
graduate graduating class has failed to complete the programs of- motivation.” Eur. J. Eng. Educ. 33 (5–6): 573–585. https://doi.org/10
fered in the allotted timeframe. Students, although entering the .1080/03043790802585454.
program with similar abilities, have not performed, on average, Assaf, S. A., and S. Al-Hejji. 2006. “Causes of delay in large construction
at a high academic level. The disparity between the minority projects.” Int. J. Project Manage. 24 (4): 349–357. https://doi.org/10
who perform exceptionally well and the remaining students is stark. .1016/j.ijproman.2005.11.010.
Martin et al. (2014) examined this outcome in the academic year Bandura, A. 1993. “Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and
2013–2014 to determine possible links between motivation and functioning.” Educ. Psychol. 28 (2): 117–148. https://doi.org/10.1207
academic performance. The results of the cross-sectional survey /s15326985ep2802_3.
Benson, L., A. Kirn, and C. Faber. 2013. “CAREER: Student motivation
conducted suggested that intrinsic as opposed to extrinsic motiva-
and learning in engineering.” In Proc., 120th ASEE Annual Conf. and
tional factors had a more positive effect on students’ motivation. Exposition. Paper No. 6363. Washington, DC: American Society for
The study, however, only revealed a significant difference in aca- Engineering Education.
demic performance based on the social acceptance motivational Brookhart, S. 2011. “Educational assessment knowledge and skills for
factor and provided inferences about the changing nature of teachers.” Educ. Meas.: Issues Pract. 30 (1): 3–12. https://doi.org/10
motivation. .1111/j.1745-3992.2010.00195.x.

© ASCE 04018005-11 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Ciani, K. D., K. M. Sheldon, J. C. Hilpert, and M. A. Easter. 2011. “Ante- Lustick, L. S. 1996. “History, historiography, and political science: Multi-
cedents and trajectories of achievement goals: A self-determination ple historical records and the problem of selection bias.” Am. Political
theory perspective.” Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 81 (2): 223–243. https://doi Sci. Rev. 90 (3): 605–618. https://doi.org/10.2307/2082612.
.org/10.1348/000709910X517399. Marra, R. M., K. A. Rodgers, D. Shen, and B. Bogue. 2009. “Women
Corpus, J. H., M. S. McClintic-Gilbert, and A. O. Hayenga. 2009. “Within- engineering students and self-efficacy: A multi-year, multi-institution
year changes in children’s intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orienta- study of women engineering student self-efficacy.” J. Eng. Educ.
tions: Contextual predictors and academic outcomes.” Contemp. Educ. 98 (1): 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2168-9830.2009.tb01003.x.
Psychol. 34 (2): 154–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.01 Martin, H., T. M. Lewis, and A. Petersen. 2016. “Factors affecting the
.001. choice of construction project delivery in developing oil and gas econo-
Corpus, J. H., and S. V. Wormington. 2014. “Profiles of intrinsic and mies.” Archit. Eng. Des. Manage. 12 (3): 170–188. https://doi.org/10
extrinsic motivations in elementary school: A longitudinal analysis.” .1080/17452007.2016.1151762.
J. Exp. Educ. 82 (4): 480–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973 Martin, H., C. Sorhaindo, and F. Welch. 2014. “Motivation of undergradu-
.2013.876225. ate civil engineering students for higher levels of academic success.”
Damci, A. 2015. “Impact of personal demographics on civil engineers’ In Proc., 30th Ann. Int. Conf. by the Association of Researchers in
motivators: Case study of Turkey.” J. Manage. Eng. 32 (2): 1–7. Construction Management (ARCOM), 1205–1224. Reading, UK:
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

https://doi.org/10.1061/(ASCE)ME.1943-5479.0000406. Association of Researchers in Construction Management.


Das, T. K. 1993. “Time in management and organizational studies.” Time Maslow, A. 1968. Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand
Soc. 2 (2): 267–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/0961463X93002002008. Reinhold.
Deci, E. L. 1971. “Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic mo- Matusovich, H. M., R. A. Streveler, and R. L. Miller. 2010. “Why do stu-
tivation.” J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 18 (1): 105–115. https://doi.org/10 dents choose engineering? A qualitative, longitudinal investigation of
.1037/h0030644. students’ motivational values.” J. Eng. Educ. 99 (4): 289–303.
Dias, D. 2011. “Reasons and motivations for the option of an engineering https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2168-9830.2010.tb01064.x.
career in Portugal.” Eur. J. Eng. Educ. 36 (4): 367–376. https://doi.org Meece, J. L., E. M. Anderman, and L. H. Anderman. 2006. “Classroom
/10.1080/03043797.2011.593096. goal structure, student motivation, and academic achievement.” Annu.
Dworkin, G. 1988. The theory and practice of autonomy. New York: Rev. Psychol. 57 (1): 487–503. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych
Cambridge University Press. .56.091103.070258.
Eccles, J. S., and A. Wigfield. 2002. “Motivational beliefs, values, and Meece, J. L., and J. Painter. 2008. “Gender, self-regulation, and motiva-
tion.” In Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, research,
goals.” Ann. Rev. Psychol. 53 (1): 109–132. https://doi.org/10.1146
and applications, 339–367. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153.
Nandhakumar, J. 2002. “Managing time in a software factory: Temporal
Elliot, A. J., and M. A. Church. 1997. “A hierarchical model of approach
and spatial organization of IS development activities.” Inf. Soc.
and avoidance achievement motivation.” J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72 (1):
18 (4): 251–262. https://doi.org/10.1080/01972240290075101.
218–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.218.
Otis, N., F. M. Grouzet, and L. G. Pelletier. 2005. “Latent motivational
Engelschalk, T., G. Steuer, and M. Dresel. 2017. “Quantity and quality of
change in an academic setting: A 3-year longitudinal study.” J. Educ.
motivational regulation among university students.” Educ. Psychol.
Psychol. 97 (2): 170–183. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.170.
37 (9): 1154–1170. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2017.1322177.
Patrick, H., A. Ryan, and A. Kaplan. 2007. “Early adolescents’ perceptions
Gagné, M., and E. L. Deci. 2005. “Self-determination theory and work mo-
of the classroom social environment, motivational beliefs, and engage-
tivation.” J. Organiz. Behav. 26 (4): 331–362. https://doi.org/10.1002
ment.” J. Educ. Psychol. 99 (1): 83–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022
/job.322. -0663.99.1.83.
George, D., and P. Mallery. 2011. SPSS for windows step by step: A simple Pintrich, P. R. 2003. “A motivational science perspective on the role of stu-
study guide and reference, 17.0 update, 10/e. Needham Heights, MA: dent motivation in learning and teaching contexts.” J. Educ. Psychol.
Allyn & Bacon, Inc. 95 (4): 667–686. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.667.
Harris, F., and C. Moll. 2015. “The impact of quality management practices Pintrich, P. R., and D. H. Schunk. 1996. Motivation in education: Theory,
on the extended curriculum programme at a university of technology.” research, and applications. Englewood Cliffs: NJ: Merrill.
Int. J. Innov. Educ. Res. 3 (2): 143–152. Porter, L. W., and E. E. Lawler. 1968. Managerial attitudes and perfor-
Hattie, J., and H. Timperley. 2007. “The power of feedback.” Rev. Educ. mance. Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Res. 77 (1): 81–112. https://doi.org/10.3102/003465430298487. Ryan, R. M., and E. L. Deci. 2000. “Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations:
Hayenga, A. O., and J. H. Corpus. 2010. “Profiles of intrinsic and extrinsic Classic definitions and new directions.” Contemp. Educ. Psychol.
motivations: A person-centered approach to motivation and achieve- 25 (1): 54–67. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1020.
ment in middle school.” Motivation Emotion 34 (4): 371–383. Salthouse, T. A., and E. M. Tucker-Drob. 2008. “Implications of short-term
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9181-x. retest effects for the interpretation of longitudinal change.” Neuropsy-
Husman, J., and W. Lens. 1999. “The role of the future in student moti- chology 22 (6): 800–811.
vation.” Educ. Psychol. 34 (2): 113–125. https://doi.org/10.1207 Saunders, C., and J. Kim. 2007. “Editor’s comments: Perspectives on time.”
/s15326985ep3402_4. MIS Q. 31 (4): iii–xi. https://doi.org/10.2307/25148813.
Kolmos, A., N. Mejlgaard, S. Haase, and J. E. Holgaard. 2013. “Motiva- Schmitt, N. 1996. “Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha.” Psychological
tional factors, gender and engineering education.” Eur. J. Eng. Educ. Assessment 8 (4): 350–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.8.4
38 (3): 340–358. https://doi.org/10.1080/03043797.2013.794198. .350.
Labuhn, A. S., B. J. Zimmerman, and M. Hasselhorn. 2010. “Enhancing Schonfeld, I. S., and D. Rindskopf. 2007. “Hierarchical linear modeling in
students’ self-regulation and mathematics performance: The influence organizational research: Longitudinal data outside the context of growth
of feedback and self-evaluative standards.” Metacognit. Learn. 5 (2): modeling.” Organ. Res. Methods 10 (3): 417–429. https://doi.org/10
173–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-010-9056-2. .1177/1094428107300229.
Law, K. M., and K. Chuah. 2009. “What motivates engineering students? A Schunk, D. H. 2001. “Social cognitive theory and self-regulated learning.”
study in Taiwan.” Int. J. Eng. Educ. 25 (5): 1068–1074. In Self-regulated learning and academic achievement. 2nd ed.,
Law, K. M., F. E. Sandnes, H.-L. Jian, and Y.-P. Huang. 2009. “A compar- 125–152. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
ative study of learning motivation among engineering students in South Schunk, D. H., and B. J. Zimmerman. 2008. “Motivation: An essential di-
East Asia and beyond.” Int. J. Eng. Educ. 25 (1): 144–151. mension of self-regulated learning.” In Motivation and self-regulated
Lin, Y.-G., W. J. McKeachie, and Y. C. Kim. 2003. “College student in- learning: Theory, research, and applications, 1–30. Mahwah, NJ:
trinsic and/or extrinsic motivation and learning.” Learn. Individual Lawrence Erlbaum.
Differences 13 (3): 251–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(02) Seaman, M. 2011. “Bloom’s taxonomy: Its evolution, revision, and use in
00092-4. the field of education.” Curriculum Teach. Dialogue 13 (1–2): 29–43.

© ASCE 04018005-12 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005


Shalley, C. E. 1995. “Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal quality of motivation matters.” J. Educ. Psychol. 101 (3): 671–688.
setting on creativity and productivity.” Acad. Manage. J. 38 (2): https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015083.
483–503. https://doi.org/10.2307/256689. Vroom, V. H. 1964. Work and motivation. New York, NY: Wiley.
Singh, S. P., A. G. Barto, and N. Chentanez. 2004. “Intrinsically motivated Wigfield, A., and J. Cambria. 2010. “Students’ achievement values, goal
reinforcement learning.” In Proc., Advances in Neural Information orientations, and interest: Definitions, development, and relations to
Processing Systems 17 (NIPS 2004). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. achievement outcomes.” Develop. Rev. 30 (1): 1–35. https://doi.org/10
Spinath, B., and R. Steinmayr. 2008. “Longitudinal analysis of intrinsic .1016/j.dr.2009.12.001.
motivation and competence beliefs: Is there a relation over time?” Child Wolters, C. A. 2004. “Advancing achievement goal theory: Using goal
Develop. 79 (5): 1555–1569. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008 structures and goal orientations to predict students’ motivation, cogni-
.01205.x. tion, and achievement.” J. Educ. Psychol. 96 (2): 236–250. https://doi
Stengers, I., and D. Gille. 1997. “Time and representation.” In Power and .org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.2.236.
invention: Situating science. Vol. 10 of Theory out of bounds, edited by
Yarnold, P. R. 1988. “Classical test theory methods for repeated-measures
I. Stengers, 177–214. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
N = 1 research designs.” Educ. Psychol. Meas. 48 (4): 913–919. https://
Street, C. T., and K. W. Ward. 2012. “Improving validity and reliability in
doi.org/10.1177/0013164488484006.
longitudinal case study timelines.” Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 21 (2): 160–175.
Downloaded from ascelibrary.org by University of Liverpool on 06/16/18. Copyright ASCE. For personal use only; all rights reserved.

https://doi.org/10.1057/ejis.2011.53. Zar, J. H. 1972. “Significance testing of the Spearman rank correlation co-
Ullah, M. I., A. Sagheer, T. Sattar, and S. Khan. 2013. “Factors influencing efficient.” J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 67 (339): 578–580. https://doi.org/10
students motivation to learn in Bahauddin Zakariya University, Multan .1080/01621459.1972.10481251.
(Pakistan).” Int. J. Human Resour. Stud. 3 (2): 90–108. https://doi.org Zimmerman, B. J. 2004. “Sociocultural influences and students’ develop-
/10.5296/ijhrs.v3i2.4135. ment of academic regulation: A social-cognitive approach.” In Big the-
Valentini, N. C., and M. E. Rudisill. 2006. “Goal orientation and mastery ories revisited, edited by D. M. McInerney and S. V. Etten, 139–164.
climate: a review of contemporary research and insights to interven- Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
tion.” Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas) 23 (2): 159–171. https://doi Zimmerman, B. J. 2011. “Motivational sources and outcomes of self-
.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2006000200006. regulated learning and performance.” In Handbook of self-regulation
Vansteenkiste, M., E. Sierens, B. Soenens, K. Luyckx, and W. Lens. 2009. of learning and performance, edited by D. H. Schunk and B. J.
“Motivational profiles from a self-determination perspective: The Zimmerman, 49–64. New York, NY: Routledge.

© ASCE 04018005-13 J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract.

J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract., 2018, 144(4): 04018005

You might also like