You are on page 1of 26

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1999, 32, 393–418 NUMBER 3 (FALL 1999)

ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS: IMPLICATIONS FOR


THE ASSESSMENT, TREATMENT, AND PREVENTION OF
PROBLEM BEHAVIOR
PETER MCGILL
UNIVERSITY OF KENT AT CANTERBURY

This paper seeks to integrate Michael’s (1982, 1993) discussion of the concept of the
establishing operation (EO) with existing conceptual and empirical analyses of problem
behavior in people with developmental disabilities. The paper begins with a summary of
Michael (1993), which seeks to describe his concept of the EO and place it briefly in
historical context. The role of EOs in evoking and establishing motivation for problem
behavior is considered in some detail. A case is made for the greater consideration of
EOs in the functional analysis of problem behavior, and specific suggestions for detecting
the operation of conditioned establishing operations are offered. Turning to treatment, the
paper considers the role played by EOs in existing procedures and discusses the devel-
opment of treatment strategies that seek to modify EOs, extinguish EOs, and modify the
responses evoked by EOs. Finally, consideration is given to the implications of EOs for
the more systemic treatment and prevention of problem behavior.
DESCRIPTORS: problem behavior, establishing operations, assessment, prevention

Smith and Iwata (1997) have recently re- Smith and Iwata proposed directions for fu-
viewed existing knowledge of the influence ture research, emphasizing particularly the
of antecedent events on problem behavior. experimental manipulation of antecedents
They outlined the approaches of Skinner, while holding constant known response–re-
Kantor, and Michael to the conceptualiza- inforcer contingencies.
tion of antecedent events, noting the partic- The current paper focuses exclusively on
ular potential of Michael’s (1982, 1993) the relevance of EOs (rather than anteced-
concept of the establishing operation (EO) to ent events more generally) to the assessment
aid understanding of events that have pre- and treatment of problem behavior. This
viously been poorly understood. Smith and narrower focus allows the extension of
Iwata went on to review studies of the as- Smith and Iwata’s (1997) discussion in a
sessment and treatment of problem behavior number of ways. First, more attention is
in which antecedent events were recorded or given to Michael’s elaboration of condi-
manipulated, drawing attention, when pos- tioned establishing operations (CEOs). Sec-
sible, to the apparent discriminative or mo- ond, the evidence for the relevance of spe-
tivative function of such events. Noting the cific EOs (e.g., deprivation of attention) to
conceptual and methodological limitations problem behavior is considered. Third, a
of many studies and the degree to which an- number of specific treatments (e.g., extinc-
tecedent influences have been understudied, tion) are conceptually analyzed from an EO
perspective. Fourth, the implications of an
I am grateful to the reviewers, and to Joseph E. Spradlin EO conceptualization for the treatment and
and Jim Mansell for their very helpful comments on earlier prevention of problem behavior are dis-
drafts of this manuscript. cussed. And finally, previous analyses of
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- EOs are extended through discussion of
dressed to Peter McGill, Tizard Centre, University of Kent
at Canterbury, Canterbury, Kent, United Kingdom CT2 EOs associated with biological circumstanc-
7LZ (E-mail: P.McGill@tizard.ukc.ac.uk). es, and the extinction of EOs.

393
394 PETER MCGILL

ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS cape from aversive stimulation, it similarly


The variables incorporated in the concept depends on the prior presentation of aversive
of the EO played a significant part in early stimulation. Such effects have been dealt
elaborations of behavior analysis. Skinner with piecemeal and sometimes have been
(1953) took the concept of drive as a starting confused with discriminative stimulus con-
point to consider the effects of deprivation trol. Michael recommends a more systematic
and satiation at some length. Keller and approach to the analysis of motivation and
Schoenfeld (1950) used the term establishing presents the EO as an appropriate unifying
concept.
operation to refer to the ‘‘operations . . . of
Michael defines the EO as an environ-
deprivation . . . or stimulation’’ that establish
mental event, operation, or stimulus condi-
drives (p. 274). Michael has argued that this
tion having two conjoint functions. First, it
early interest in motivation declined to the
alters the effectiveness of certain other events
point at which it was necessary to ‘‘reintro-
as reinforcers or punishers. Second, it alters
duce the concept of the establishing opera-
the frequency of behaviors associated with
tion’’ (Michael, 1993, p. 191). In the inter-
these reinforcing or punishing events. Thus,
vening years, motivation was by no means
to use Michael’s example, food deprivation
completely neglected, but more often was is an EO that increases the reinforcing effect
incorporated in proposals for the conceptual of food and evokes behaviors that have a his-
expansion of behavior analysis (see, e.g., Bi- tory of leading to food. Michael also uses
jou & Baer, 1961; Kantor, 1959; Morris, the term abolishing operation (in passing) to
1988; Wahler & Fox, 1981) than treated in refer to those EOs that reduce the effective-
its own right. In reviving the concept of the ness of certain other events as reinforcers and
EO, Michael has elaborated a treatment of reduce the frequency of behaviors associated
motivation that requires little such concep- with those reinforcing events. Michael iden-
tual expansion, because it is systematically tifies food satiation as an example of an abol-
related to the basic three-term contingency ishing operation.
and many of its tenets were present in early The most immediately visible effect of the
discussions. Michael has, however, elaborat- EO is the altered rate of behaviors associated
ed the concept, most especially in its dis- with the relevant reinforcer. This is mani-
tinction from discriminative stimulus con- fested in three ways, as illustrated by the ef-
trol, and in his description of CEOs. fects of food deprivation in the following ex-
Michael (1993) begins his account of EOs amples. First, operant responses may be
by noting that, in commonsense under- evoked directly by the relevant EOs. For ex-
standing, the occurrence of a behavior re- ample, the person deprived of food is more
flects both the ability (skill or knowledge) likely to eat. Second, discriminative stimuli
and the motivation (want) to produce it. He (SDs) are more likely to evoke responses that
notes that motivation has recently been have resulted in the relevant reinforcer in the
largely equated to reinforcement. Motivation past. For example, the person is more likely
(or its lack) has come to mean the presence to respond to a restaurant sign by entering.
(or the absence) of sufficient, appropriately Third, responses maintained by related con-
scheduled reinforcement. However, the ef- ditioned reinforcers will be more probable.
fectiveness of such reinforcement depends For example, the person is more likely to
on the extent of deprivation or satiation (and start preparing a meal. The evocative effect
other variables having similar effects) with of the EO may be confused with the simi-
respect to the reinforcer. In the case of es- larly evocative effect of the SD, and Michael
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 395

pays careful attention to their distinction. warmth) for specific responses was more
The SD evokes behavior as a result of a his- available in the presence of coldness than in
its absence. However, in the absence of cold-
tory of correlation with greater availability of
the relevant reinforcer. If you are hungry, a ness, reduced coldness is not less available
sign saying ‘‘Eat here’’ may evoke behaviors but is, for the moment, not a reinforcer. My
such as entering and asking for something behavior of turning on the heater in my of-
to eat. Such a sign does not make you hun- fice is evoked, therefore, by coldness as an
grier. Long periods without food are likely EO. Of course, such escape behaviors may
to evoke behaviors that may lead to food, also come under discriminative stimulus
but their effect is not discriminative—food control. The heater in my office is on a time
is no more likely to be available when you switch and will not switch on after a certain
are hungry than when you are not. In short, hour in the evening. When I enter my office
EOs change how much people want some- late at night, therefore, I do not switch on
thing; SDs change their chances of getting it the heater (the behavior has been extin-
(Michael, 1982). guished) but, instead, engage in other be-
haviors evoked by the still-operative EO for
Unconditioned Establishing Operations which reinforcement (albeit less effective) re-
Many EOs are unconditioned or un- mains available, such as keeping my coat on.
learned, even though the behaviors that they The notion of the UEO is closely related
evoke are usually learned. Without any con- to the notion of unconditioned reinforce-
ditioning history, food will reinforce the be- ment. The former represents the environ-
havior of the organism in a state of food mental event or operation that alters the mo-
deprivation even if the behaviors that reli- mentary effectiveness of the latter (e.g., food
ably obtain food have not yet been learned. deprivation regulates the effectiveness of
Deprivation of food, water, activity, sleep, food as a reinforcer). In principle, therefore,
and variables related to sexual reinforcement there should be parallel lists of UEOs and
is likely to act as an unconditioned estab- unconditioned reinforcers.
lishing operation (UEO). Satiation of the
same events is likely to function as an un- Conditioned Establishing Operations
conditioned abolishing operation. Michael describes three types of condi-
Using the example of pain arising from tioned establishing operations (CEOs). Sur-
electric shock, Michael also argues that such rogate CEOs (so called because they act in
aversive stimulation should be considered a the same way as another event) are previ-
UEO and seeks to distinguish this interpre- ously neutral events that, through correla-
tation from that of aversive stimulation as tion in time with a UEO or an already-es-
an SD. Electric shock (and other sources of tablished CEO, acquire similar motivative
aversive stimulation often used in animal ex- effects. Reflexive CEOs (so called because
perimentation) is not a frequent event in ev- they alter their own function) are previously
eryday human environments. Let us consid- neutral events whose termination comes to
er, therefore, the more common example of be reinforcing (or punishing) through sys-
the aversive stimulation arising from enter- tematically preceding ‘‘worsening’’ (or ‘‘im-
ing a cold place. In the winter my office is provement’’) when not terminated. Transi-
typically cold when I enter it first thing in tive CEOs (so called because they alter not
the morning. For the ‘‘coldness’’ to be con- their own function but that of another
sidered an SD it would be necessary that the event) are previously neutral stimuli whose
reinforcer (reduced coldness or increased occurrence alters the reinforcing (or punish-
396 PETER MCGILL

ing) effectiveness of another event and cally, such a CEO is generated by correlation
evokes responses that produce (or suppress) with a UEO (such as painful stimulation)
that event. The different types of CEO are that can be avoided if a response (terminat-
now described in more detail. ing the warning stimulus) is made after the
A surrogate CEO involves the simple onset of the warning stimulus but prior to
pairing of a previously neutral stimulus with the onset of the UEO. Although often seen
a UEO, with the former developing the mo- as an SD, Michael argues that the warning
tivative effects of the latter. For example, stimulus is a CEO because of the lack of
stimuli correlated with a reduction in tem- correlation between its occurrence or non-
perature (the UEO), such as the sight of occurrence and the availability or nonavail-
snow out of a window, may evoke behaviors ability of reinforcement. To be an SD the
associated with an increase in temperature, presence of a stimulus must be accompanied
such as putting on a sweater, independently by the availability of effective reinforcement
of (or at least beyond that expected by) the for a specific response or set of responses and
current ambient temperature. Adelinis, Pi- its absence must be accompanied by the lack
azza, Fisher, and Hanley (1997) reported ev- of (or reduced) availability of effective rein-
idence of client location (being in a wheel- forcement for the same response. The warn-
chair) apparently functioning as an EO for ing stimulus fails on the second component
attention-maintained self-injury. Being in a of this correlation because, in the absence of
wheelchair may be correlated with depriva- the warning stimulus, its offset (even were it
tion of attention; indeed, the authors re- available) would not be effective reinforce-
ported less noncontingent attention being ment. That is, the response (that would oth-
provided while the person was in a wheel- erwise remove the warning stimulus) does
chair. Although described as an ‘‘establishing not occur, not because it no longer produces
stimulus’’ by the authors, the wheelchair, reinforcement but because there is no rein-
therefore, meets the definition of a surrogate forcement to produce. Michael has also re-
CEO. Despite these examples the inconclu- ferred to this type of CEO as a ‘‘threat
sive results of research conducted on the CEO.’’ The second type involves stimuli
evocative effects of stimuli correlated with whose onset is correlated with later rein-
food deprivation should be noted (for a forcement. Analogously, such stimuli estab-
summary, see Michael, 1993, pp. 199–202). lish motivation to prevent their termination
The existence of such CEOs thus remains and suppress behaviors associated with their
putative, although they might be expected to termination. Michael has also referred to this
be more prevalent and easier to detect with as a ‘‘promise CEO.’’ For example, stimuli
UEOs that change faster than food depri- (such as those associated with being tired)
vation. whose onset is correlated with my later fall-
Reflexive CEOs are stimuli that acquire ing asleep (reinforcement) may suppress be-
their motivative effect through correlation haviors (such as drinking coffee) associated
with worsening (e.g., the presentation of an with their termination. In the absence of
aversive stimulus) or improvement (e.g., the such stimuli, the response (drinking coffee)
presentation of a reinforcing stimulus). The is more likely to occur, not because it no
first type is exemplified by the warning stim- longer produces punishment (avoidance of
ulus in a shock-avoidance procedure. The falling asleep) but because there is no pun-
warning stimulus establishes its termination ishment to produce (not falling asleep is
as reinforcing and evokes behaviors previ- only punishing if I am tired).
ously associated with its termination. Typi- Transitive CEOs are stimuli in whose con-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 397

text the reinforcing or punishing effective- 23% of a series of 152 cases of self-injurious
ness of existing conditioned reinforcers or behavior were maintained by attention. Sim-
punishers is altered. In Michael’s example, ilarly, in a series of 79 cases of self-injurious
the sight of a slotted screw evokes a worker’s or aggressive behavior, attention was hypoth-
request to his assistant for the appropriate esized as the maintaining condition in 24%
screwdriver. The screwdriver is just as avail- (Derby et al., 1992). It has been suggested
able with or without the presence of a slot- (e.g., Durand & Crimmins, 1988b; McGill,
ted screw (so the relation is not discrimina- 1993) that problem behavior maintained by
tive) but it is more reinforcing in the pres- attention is more likely to occur in environ-
ence of the screw (so the relation is moti- ments characterized by low levels of social
vative). In the analogous situation of contact. In the design of experimental (an-
conditioned punishment, the effect of the alogue) conditions for the functional analysis
CEO would be to increase the effectiveness of self-injurious behavior, Iwata et al. (1982/
of the punisher and suppress (rather than 1994) systematically varied the level and dis-
evoke) behaviors associated with its occur- tribution of available attention and later
rence. If, in the course of telling my daugh- (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994) explicitly
ter not to run around without shoes, I notice referred to this as an EO as well as making
that she has cut her foot, my admonishing adjustments to the methodology to maxi-
behavior is likely to be suppressed. The im- mize presession deprivation of attention.
mediate consequences of my reprimanding More direct evidence for the role of depri-
her (perhaps her appearing upset) are just as vation of attention comes from a number of
available with or without the presence of the sources. Taylor and Carr (1992b) found that
cut (so the relation is not discriminative) butchildren’s problem behaviors followed by
are more punishing to me in the presence of high levels of teacher attention were more
the cut (so the relation is motivative). likely to be preceded by the absence of at-
tention. Hall and Oliver (1992) showed that
bursts of self-injurious behavior were preced-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS ed by a reduced probability of social contact
AND FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS from staff and led to increases in the prob-
Analyses of problem behavior have iden- ability of social contact. A number of studies
tified a range of reinforcers that have been (Hagopian, Fisher, & Legacy, 1994; Iwata,
commonly grouped into three categories: so- Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994; Mace & Lalli,
cial-positive reinforcement, social-negative 1991; Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, Smith, &
reinforcement, and automatic reinforce- Mazaleski, 1993) have demonstrated that
ment. The events that may establish moti- eliminating deprivation through noncontin-
vation for these categories of reinforcement gent access to attention substantially reduces
are now considered. a variety of problem behaviors.
Given Michael’s account, deprivation of
Social-Positive Reinforcement attention is recognizable as an EO that both
Considerable evidence exists that many increases the reinforcing value of attention
different topographies of problem behavior and evokes behaviors previously associated
are maintained by attention (self-injurious with attention. We may conclude that dep-
behavior: e.g., Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, rivation of attention establishes motivation
& Richman, 1982/1994; aggression: e.g., for some attention-maintained problem be-
Mace, Page, Ivancic, & O’Brien, 1986). Iwa- havior. Motivation for attention, however,
ta, Pace, Dorsey, et al. (1994) found that may be established or abolished by EOs oth-
398 PETER MCGILL

er than deprivation or satiation of attention EO evoking tangibly maintained problem


(Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski, 1997; Hanley, behaviors.
Piazza, & Fisher, 1997), and deprivation of
attention may, perhaps, establish motivation Social-Negative Reinforcement
for other ‘‘substitutable’’ (Iwata & Michael, Considerable evidence also exists that
1994) reinforcers (e.g., children may be many different topographies of problem be-
more likely to seek toys used in independent havior are maintained by escape from task
play when deprived of attention and atten- demands (disruptive behavior: e.g., Carr &
tion-seeking responses produce no effect). Durand, 1985a; stereotyped behavior: e.g.,
We cannot conclude, therefore, that depri- Durand & Carr, 1987; self-injurious behav-
vation of attention is a specific or a universal ior: e.g., Iwata et al., 1982/1994). Epide-
EO for attention-maintained problem be- miological studies suggest escape to be the
havior. It seems reasonable, however, to sug- most common reinforcer for problem behav-
gest that EOs that establish motivation for ior, representing 35% of cases of self-injuri-
attention are likely to be particularly in- ous behavior (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al.,
volved in attention-maintained (rather than, 1994) and 48% of cases of self-injurious or
say, escape-maintained) problem behavior, aggressive behavior (Derby et al., 1992).
given the growing literature showing that in- Many authors (e.g., Smith & Iwata, 1997)
terventions relevant to the identified conse- have now pointed out that demands proba-
quences of problem behavior are more likely bly function as EOs (rather than SDs) evok-
to be effective than irrelevant interventions ing escape-maintained problem behavior.
(see, e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985a; Iwata, Consistent with this account, problem be-
Pace, Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994; havior is more likely to occur following de-
Repp, Felce, & Barton, 1988). mands (Edelson, Taubman, & Lovaas, 1983;
Although the supporting evidence is more Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981) and is elim-
limited, similar arguments can be construct- inated or substantially reduced by the re-
ed for other events considered to be social- moval of demands (Pace, Iwata, Edwards, &
positive reinforcers because their availability McCosh, 1986) or the provision of noncon-
is frequently confounded with attention tingent access to escape (Vollmer, Marcus, &
(Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994). Problem Ringdahl, 1995). Investigations of the prop-
behavior has been found to be maintained erties of demands that establish escape mo-
by food or access to materials in a number tivation are limited. However, investigators
of studies (e.g., Durand & Crimmins, have identified a number of dimensions that
1988a; Marcus & Vollmer, 1996), repre- are salient in one or more cases, including
senting 3.3% and 12%, respectively, of the task difficulty (Carr & Durand, 1985a;
series reported by Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al. Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981), type of re-
(1994) and Derby et al. (1992). Wacker et quired motor responses (Dunlap, Kern-
al. (1996) found direct evidence of a relation Dunlap, Clarke, & Robbins, 1991), number
between food deprivation and problem be- of required responses (Mace, Browder, &
havior in a single case study. Reductions in Lin, 1987), task novelty (Mace et al., 1987),
problem behavior have been reported when duration of instructional sessions (Dunlap et
noncontingent access to tangible reinforce- al., 1991), rate of task presentation (Smith,
ment was provided (e.g., Ayllon & Michael, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995), unpredictabil-
1959; Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994; Mar- ity of events (Flannery & Horner, 1994),
cus & Vollmer, 1996). Deprivation of tan- and task preference (Dunlap et al., 1991;
gible reinforcement may then be seen as an Foster-Johnson, Ferro, & Dunlap, 1994). In
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 399

Table 1
Reinforcers and Establishing Operations for Problem Behavior

Class of maintaining consequences Specific consequence Establishing operation

Social-positive reinforcement Attention Deprivation of attention


Tangible items Deprivation of tangible items
Social-negative reinforcement Escape Aversive events (e.g., new, difficult,
high-rate, or nonpreferred demands)
Automatic reinforcement Sensory stimulation Deprivation of stimulation

addition, the probability of problem behav- as the relevant EO. Evidence consistent with
ior following a particular demand may be this view comes from studies reporting more
reduced by prior or interspersed events such problem behavior in some individuals in
as storytelling (Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, stimulation-deprived environments (e.g.,
1976), social comments (Kennedy, Itkonen, Berkson & Mason, 1964), the removal or
& Lindquist, 1995), demands highly likely substantial reduction of problem behavior by
to be complied with (Mace & Belfiore, the provision of noncontingent stimulation
1990), and advance notification (Tustin, (e.g., Berkson & Mason, 1964; Iwata, Pace,
1995). Dorsey, et al., 1994; Kennedy & Souza,
Much of the literature on social-negative 1995; Wells & Smith, 1983), and the greater
reinforcement has focused on escape from reduction of problem behavior by noncon-
demands. It is clear, however, that escape tingent access to matched than to alternative
from other kinds of aversive stimulation may stimulation (Piazza et al., 1998).
also maintain problem behavior. Examples
include attention (Taylor & Carr, 1992a, Including the Analysis of EOs in
1992b) and ambient noise (O’Reilly, 1997). Functional Analysis
Conceptual issues. A summary of the
Automatic Reinforcement events known to establish motivation for
Studies have also suggested the mainte- problem behavior is shown in Table 1 and
nance of problem behavior by automatic re- illustrates, in line with Michael’s suggestion,
inforcement (self-injurious behavior: e.g., parallel lists of reinforcers and EOs. Al-
Iwata et al., 1982/1994; stereotyped behav- though parallel, it should be noted that the
ior: e.g., Sturmey, Carlsen, Crisp, & New- two lists address different questions. The
ton, 1988). Epidemiological studies found identification of the consequences of prob-
automatic reinforcement in 26% of cases of lem behavior answers the question typically
self-injurious behavior (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, asked by functional analysis: What main-
et al., 1994) and 34% of cases of self-inju- tains this behavior? The identification of
rious or aggressive behavior (Derby et al., EOs addresses a different question: Why
1992). Although it is often difficult to iden- does motivation for this consequence exist,
tify the specific reinforcers involved (Iwata, or, why does the person ‘‘want’’ this conse-
Vollmer, Zarcone, & Rodgers, 1993), some quence?
kind of specific (e.g., oral stimulation: Piazza It has been argued (see, e.g., Neef & Iwa-
et al., 1998) or general (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, ta, 1994) that the first of these questions
et al., 1994) sensory stimulation has typi- should be answered or at least considered be-
cally been reported. The absence or depri- fore intervention. Thus, intervention should
vation of such stimulation has been inferred be directed by the results of a prior func-
400 PETER MCGILL

tional analysis that either establishes clearly shown reductions in such variability when
the reinforcer maintaining the behavior EOs are detected and manipulated (Horner,
(Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994) or allows Day, & Day, 1997; Kennedy & Meyer,
the development of an educated and testable 1996; O’Reilly, 1995, 1997; Smith et al.,
hypothesis about the reinforcer (Carr & 1995).
Carlson, 1993). Although knowledge of the Third, knowledge of EOs may have dif-
relevant EO may accumulate during pre- ferential implications for intervention. For
treatment analysis, this has not generally each function of problem behavior derived
been seen as a requirement. However, there from functional analysis, there are several
are good reasons for knowing about such possible treatments that have been demon-
EOs. strated to be effective with some individuals
First, in the situation in which the EO is in some circumstances. Given the identifi-
found to represent an unacceptable state of cation of such sets of treatments, it is not
affairs, treatment that does not include its clear how selection from within these sets
alteration might be contraindicated (Iwata, should proceed. This decision may be facil-
Vollmer, & Zarcone, 1990; Sturmey, 1995). itated by a knowledge of the relevant EOs.
Examples include levels of deprivation of at- For example, attention-maintained behavior
tention or tangible reinforcers of an unac- may occur in a context of generally low lev-
ceptable degree (Emerson & Hatton, 1994; els of attention, in which case increasing the
Hile & Walbran, 1991); levels of demand density of attention (noncontingent rein-
aversiveness that are symptomatic of abusive, forcement) may be indicated. Alternatively,
unskilled, or ineffective programming (Iwa- attention-maintained behavior may occur in
ta, 1987; Whittington & Wykes, 1996); a context of generally high levels of attention
other aversive events evoking escape behav- distributed rather more to problem behavior
ior that are open to remediation (Horner, than to adaptive behavior, suggesting the use
Vaughn, Day, & Ard, 1996); and levels of of extinction.
deprivation of general sensory stimulation Approaches to analysis. Approaches to the
that are incompatible with health or devel- detection and discrimination of the environ-
opment (Lovaas, Newsom, & Hickman, mental events influencing problem behavior
1987). have, broadly, been of two kinds: descriptive
Second, knowledge of EOs may help to and experimental analysis (Emerson, 1995).
explain the frequently reported variability in Methods of descriptive analysis have includ-
the occurrence of problem behavior under, ed interviews with informants (e.g., O’Neil,
apparently, the same three-term contingen- Horner, Albin, Storey, & Sprague, 1990)
cies (Carr, Reeve, & Magito-McLaughlin, and observations of the correlation of envi-
1996; Gardner, Cole, Davidson, & Karan, ronmental events and problem behavior us-
1986). That is, ‘‘third variables’’ (Skinner, ing either relatively simple approaches such
1931) may be required to explain variability as records of inappropriate behavior (Pyles
in responses to the same stimulus (see Mor- & Bailey, 1990) and scatter plots (Touchette,
ris, 1992). If a behavior is maintained by a MacDonald, & Langer, 1985) or more com-
particular reinforcement contingency and, plex procedures for the detailed recording
with the contingency operative and in the and analysis of event sequences (e.g., Emer-
presence of relevant SDs, the behavior some- son, Thompson, Reeves, Henderson, &
times occurs at high rates and sometimes at Robertson, 1995; Lerman & Iwata, 1993;
low rates, the operation of an EO might be Mace, Lalli, Pinter Lalli, & Shea, 1993).
suspected. A number of studies have now Methods of experimental analysis have typ-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 401

ically investigated sensitivity to various re- analysis should be used when possible to
inforcement contingencies (e.g., Iwata et al., identify the functional properties of putative
1982/1994) or the evocative effects of an- EOs.
tecedent events (Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, Michael’s (1993) elaboration paid partic-
1980). ular attention to the differentiation of UEOs
Although these methods have been used and three types of CEOs. It might be asked,
primarily to detect reinforcement contingen- therefore, if developments in functional
cies, they all, in principle, can be modified analysis methods could be identified that
to detect EOs and, in many cases, have al- would enable a similar differentiation of the
ready been so used. Horner et al. (1997) in- EOs that evoke problem behavior. It should
terviewed the staff working with 3 individ- be noted, first, that the distinction between
uals to identify putative EOs for their prob- UEOs and CEOs is not always easy to apply
lem behavior and subsequently used an al- in practice. This can be illustrated by further
ternating treatments design to demonstrate consideration of Table 1. Deprivation of tan-
their evocation of problem behavior. Al- gible items and deprivation of stimulation
though some approaches to the observation would appear to be UEOs, but it is difficult
of correlations between environmental to describe deprivation of attention and
events and problem behavior (e.g., ABC aversive events such as demands in this way.
charts) have limitations resulting from their Demands can be seen as reflexive CEOs be-
focus on the immediate antecedents of be- cause they may historically have been cor-
havior, they do typically enable the detection related with worsening. Deprivation of at-
of EOs that immediately precede problem tention, however, does not seem to fit any
behavior (e.g., demands). Bodfish and Kon- of the CEO categories and, in many ways,
arski (1992) used a scatter plot to identify appears to function very similarly to a UEO
periods when problem behavior occurred at in that its effects do not appear to be de-
high rates in a residential setting. The results pendent on the simultaneous operation of
suggested the hypothesis that lack of activity other, more obviously unlearned EOs (Ge-
evoked problem behavior and led to the suc- wirtz & Baer, 1958a, 1958b).
cessful provision of structured activities. Em- Different types of CEO may be more
erson et al. (1995) conducted detailed direct amenable to differentiation. Illustrative sug-
observations of problem behavior in the nat- gestions for the detection of reflexive and
ural environment and showed patterns of transitive CEOs follow.
variation across different social contexts Reflexive CEOs are stimuli that acquire
(e.g., when the person was alone or was sub- their motivative effect through correlation
ject to high-rate demands) suggestive of the with worsening (e.g., the presentation of an
operation of EOs. Smith et al. (1995) adapt- aversive stimulus) or improvement (e.g., the
ed their experimental analysis technique to presentation of a reinforcing stimulus). As-
identify the specific aspects of demands that sume that problem behavior is maintained
functioned as EOs and suggested this ap- by the termination of demands and, further,
proach as a useful strategy for the investi- that the presence or behavior of staff acts as
gation of EOs more generally. Given the a reflexive CEO. A test condition for social-
range of EOs that might be operating in the negative reinforcement (escape and avoid-
natural environment and their sometimes ance) could be conducted repeatedly using a
temporal distance from problem behavior, it design similar to that described by Smith et
seems likely that all approaches will be useful al. (1995, Study 3). The escape contingency
in some situations, although experimental would be standard (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et
402 PETER MCGILL

al., 1994) but would be preceded, in each this condition could be modified so that the
trial, by the presentation of stimuli thought person is required to interact with materials
to function as reflexive CEOs (e.g., ap- (as in the standard demand test condition),
proaching with materials, saying ‘‘let’s start with the attention contingency remaining
to work in a minute’’). Problem behavior the same. If a transitive CEO relation exists,
would be reinforced by the withdrawal of problem behavior should occur in this mod-
the stimulus (presence, materials, demand, ified condition at a higher rate than in the
etc.) most recently presented. The presence standard attention condition. Because de-
of a reflexive CEO would be shown by high mands would not be withdrawn or delayed,
rates of problem behavior occurring prior to no escape contingency would be in effect
the presentation of demands, that is, evi- and the occurrence of persistent problem be-
dence that one function of problem behavior havior would distinguish the transitive CEO
is to escape the stimuli preceding demands. effect from the burst of extinction respond-
In a variant of this procedure, different pu- ing characteristic of escape-maintained be-
tative reflexive CEOs could be compared havior under these circumstances. In the
across conditions. In the natural environ- natural environment, higher rates of atten-
ment, problem behavior would be expected tion-gaining problem behavior (as measured,
to be evoked both by demands and by staff e.g., by conditional probabilities) would be
presence or features of staff behavior that expected in situations of high-rate demands.
warn of imminent demands. These threat Although the above proposals remain to
CEOs would have an establishing rather be tested empirically, they suggest that in-
than discriminative function because their cluding the analysis of EOs in functional
onset establishes their offset as reinforcing analysis does not require major changes to
(through a history of their correlation with existing assessment methods. As a result, it
later demands) rather than being correlated might be hoped that, just as it has become
with the availability of reinforcement. That routine to assess and report the function of
is, threats of imminent demands make es- problem behavior in both clinical practice
cape from the threats more valuable rather and research, future studies will also report
than more likely. Such descriptive analysis EOs. In addition to the benefits likely to
would be an important way of identifying arise directly from this practice, cumulative
warning behavior that may be idiosyncratic information about the relation of EOs to
in nature prior to formal experimental anal- problem behavior would then become avail-
ysis. able in the same way that it has in respect
Transitive CEOs are stimuli in whose con- of reinforcement contingencies (Derby et al.,
text the reinforcing or punishing effective- 1992; Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al., 1994), and
ness of existing conditioned reinforcers or (as illustrated by Horner et al., 1997) basic
punishers is altered. Demands may some- knowledge and conceptions of EOs and
times act as transitive CEOs establishing their relations to behavior would be consid-
motivation for attention. In the standard at- erably extended.
tention test condition (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey,
et al., 1994), attention is provided contin-
gent on the occurrence of problem behavior ESTABLISHING
and at no other time while the person is OPERATIONS AND TREATMENT
allowed (but not required) to interact with What significance might EOs have in suc-
recreational materials. If demands are sus- cessful treatment? To address this question,
pected to increase motivation for attention, a number of existing treatments will be an-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 403

alyzed from an EO perspective, and the im- 1995). Extinction has been shown to reduce
plications of EOs for the development of the occurrence of problem behavior, al-
new ideas about treatment will be consid- though reduction is sometimes preceded by
ered. an extinction burst in which the problem
behavior temporarily occurs at rates higher
Analyzing Existing Treatments from than in baseline (Lerman & Iwata, 1995).
an EO Perspective The problems associated with this burst
In what follows, no attempt is made to (health risks to self or others, threats to pro-
comprehensively review existing treatments cedural integrity) have been seen as signifi-
from an EO perspective. Rather, selected cantly limiting the potential of extinction for
treatments, commonly used and discussed in more widespread use (LaVigna & Donellan,
the recent literature, are considered. Treat- 1986). This has led to its being combined
ments have been selected for consideration with other techniques (such as instructional
partly on the basis of having been little dis- fading, e.g., Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree,
cussed from an EO perspective. Thus, non- & McIntyre, 1993; Zarcone, Iwata, Vollmer,
contingent reinforcement, the effectiveness et al., 1993) designed to reduce the occur-
of which has been interpreted as at least rence of an extinction burst, and to a call
partly the result of the modification of EOs for programmatic research addressing the
(Hagopian et al., 1994; Marcus & Vollmer, factors associated with its most effective use
1996; Vollmer et al., 1993, 1995; Wilder & (Lerman & Iwata, 1996).
Carr, 1998), has not been considered. It From an EO perspective, the most inter-
should be noted that, in routine clinical esting feature (and main limitation) of ex-
practice, treatment procedures are often tinction is its lack of explicit attention to the
combined into a multicomponent treatment EOs that evoke problem behavior. This can
package (Carr, Robinson, Taylor, & Carlson, be seen with the admittedly stark and un-
1990; LaVigna, Willis, & Donellan, 1989; usual example of the extinction of self-inju-
Meyer & Evans, 1989) in which they may rious behavior maintained by food. When
interact in complex ways. For heuristic rea- self-injury previously resulted (probably in-
sons, they are discussed below as single-com- termittently) in food, it will (during extinc-
ponent independent treatments. tion) no longer lead at all to food. Self-in-
Extinction. Extinction involves the termi- jury, however, is likely to be evoked by food
nation of a previously existing contingency deprivation, and the nondelivery of food will
between problem behavior and its reinforc- leave the EO still in effect, at least until the
ing consequence. In practice, this amounts next scheduled snack or meal. Under these
to no longer delivering the reinforcer that circumstances, the likely extinction burst can
previously maintained the behavior (Lerman be seen as a reflection of the modification of
& Iwata, 1996). Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, and the response–reinforcer relation without an
Miltenberger (1994) have recently demon- accompanying modification of the relevant
strated that extinction’s success depends EO. The person is still hungry but is no
upon the correct identification of the rein- longer able to obtain food through self-in-
forcer and have suggested that extinction jury.
likely plays a role in a range of other treat- It would be very unlikely, of course, that
ments such as differential reinforcement of extinction would be used as a sole treatment
other behavior (Mazaleski, Iwata, Vollmer, under these circumstances. It has, however,
Zarcone, & Smith, 1993) and noncontin- been used frequently in this way in other,
gent reinforcement (Vollmer et al., 1993, not conceptually different, circumstances.
404 PETER MCGILL

For example, escape extinction (Iwata, Pace, be important predictors of its effectiveness
Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990) typi- in modifying the EO and, therefore, of the
cally involves preventing escape from de- extent and strength of any extinction burst.
mands and, thus, maintaining in place the Behavioral momentum. High-probability
EO (demands) that evokes escape responses. command sequences (Mace & Belfiore,
When such a state of aversive stimulation 1990; Mace et al., 1988; Singer, Singer, &
exists and reinforcement (escape from the Horner, 1987) involve the presentation of a
aversive stimulation) no longer results, we sequence of commands with which the per-
might expect to see severe, long-lasting ex- son is likely to comply (high p) immediately
tinction bursts coupled with other problem prior to the presentation of a command with
behaviors that have a history of producing which he or she has typically not complied
escape because, as originally suggested by (low p). Mace and his colleagues have shown
Skinner (1953, p. 150), the number of re- that this procedure can result in both in-
sponses that occur during extinction may re- creases in compliance to the low-p command
flect the current strength of the relevant EO. and reductions in problem behaviors previ-
Indeed, extended bursts during escape ex- ously evoked by this command. Increases in
tinction have been reported in the literature compliance have been discussed in terms of
(Goh & Iwata, 1994; Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, behavioral persistence or momentum (Nev-
Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990). Nonetheless in, 1996; Nevin, Mandell, & Atak, 1983),
such extinction bursts are often transitory under which a response persists as a function
(Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, of recently high response and reinforcement
1990) or are not reported (Lerman & Iwata, rates. The high-p command sequence then
1995). increases both rate of compliance to com-
In practice, escape extinction may work mands and obtained reinforcement for com-
not just because of its directly suppressive pliance, with compliance persisting despite
effect on previously reinforced responses but the change in conditions resulting from the
because it accidentally modifies the relevant presentation of the low-p command. Various
EO. In the example of escape from de- mechanisms for the accompanying reduction
mands, extinction exposes the person to the in problem behavior have been proposed, in-
demands from which he or she previously cluding topographical incompatibility (Mace
escaped successfully by displaying problem & Belfiore, 1990), the interaction of con-
behavior. Frequent contact with the de- current operants (Parrish, Cataldo, Kolko,
mands is likely to directly change those as- Neef, & Egel, 1986), functional incompati-
pects of the demands that make them aver- bility (Mace & Belfiore, 1990), and extinc-
sive to the person. For example, novel de- tion (Zarcone, Iwata, Hughes, & Vollmer,
mands will become familiar; difficult de- 1993).
mands (with practice and help) will become In analyzing this procedure from an EO
easier. This argument leads to the conclusion perspective, it is important to remember the
that extinction, as commonly practiced, is differentiation made between discriminative
both a treatment that changes the conse- and motivative effects. High-p and low-p
quences of problem behavior and a proce- command sequences constitute EOs rather
dure that may create conditions under which than SDs, because it makes no sense to talk
(in this example) the aversiveness of de- of compliance (or escape-related problem
mands is reduced (i.e., the EO is modified). behavior) in the absence of commands. In
As such it would be expected that the pro- the initial situation, the low-p command
cedural details of the use of extinction would may be seen as an EO evoking escape,
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 405

whereas high-p sequences constitute EOs function of their being repeatedly followed
evoking compliance. In the circumstance in by low-p commands rather than a result of
which a series of high-p commands precedes other variables such as simple repeated pre-
a low-p command, the high-p commands sentation. An EO interpretation would also
may act as a reflexive CEO ‘‘promising’’ re- predict that similar changes in compliance
inforcement for continued compliance and to high-p commands could be produced by
suppression of escape-producing behavior. procedures (e.g., satiation with social praise)
Further, the pairing of these CEOs will re- that alter the reinforcing effectiveness of the
sult, over time, in the transfer of the moti- typically scheduled consequences for com-
vative properties of one stimulus to the oth- pliance, and that changes in response to
er, although it is not a priori clear in which high-p commands, however produced, could
direction the transfer will occur. Thus, be- be reversed by procedures that restore rein-
sides evidence of increased compliance to forcing effectiveness to these consequences.
low-p commands in the absence of the high- Functional communication training (FCT).
p sequence (Davis, Brady, Williams, & This treatment (Carr & Durand, 1985a) in-
Hamilton, 1992; Ducharme & Worling, volves the replacement of problem behavior
1994), there is also evidence of reduced with a functionally equivalent communica-
compliance to high-p commands (Davis & tive behavior. FCT has been shown to lead
Reichle, 1996; Zarcone, Iwata, Hughes, & to rapid, durable, and generalized reductions
Vollmer, 1993; Zarcone, Iwata, Mazaleski, in problem behavior without the occurrence
& Smith, 1994). The EO perspective, there- of an extinction burst (Durand & Carr,
fore, offers the possibility of an explanation 1991, 1992; Sprague & Horner, 1992). The
for all outcomes of the high-p/low-p proce- success of FCT has generally been attributed
dure in a relatively parsimonious way, and to the notion of functional equivalence (Carr
may also facilitate a coherent account of a & Durand, 1985a) in which the strength-
wide range of antecedent manipulations sim- ening (through FCT) of a socially desirable
ilar in their effects to behavioral momentum response leads to the weakening or elimina-
but with widely differing origins and expla- tion of problem behavior responses having
nations (Kennedy, 1994). the same function. It has been pointed out,
Similar observations have been made by however (Fisher et al., 1993), that FCT usu-
Smith and Iwata (1997) and Houlihan and ally has been combined with other treatment
Brandon (1996). It should be noted that procedures such as extinction, punishment,
these (and the current) accounts of the role and antecedent assistance, and when FCT
that may be played by EOs in high-p/low-p has been used without such accompanying
procedures rely heavily on inference from procedures its effectiveness appears to be
observed changes in the probability of com- markedly reduced (Fisher et al., 1993). The
pliance to high-p and low-p commands. The lack of evidence for the general effectiveness
results of studies reporting such observations of FCT as a single-component treatment has
have generally been interpreted in terms of been explained variously as an issue of re-
behavioral momentum theory (Nevin, sponse efficiency (Horner, Sprague, O’Brien,
1996), although no explanation of the re- & Heathfield, 1990), the effects of reinforce-
duction in compliance to high-p commands ment of functional communication respons-
appears to have been offered. Further studies es on other (problem behavior) members of
are clearly required. In particular, such stud- the same response class, and the chaining of
ies might seek to confirm that the reduction communicative responses to problem behav-
in compliance to high-p commands is a ior (Fisher et al., 1993).
406 PETER MCGILL

From an EO perspective, three points are sitive CEO that makes the conditioned re-
noteworthy. First, when communication re- inforcer of help more reinforcing. Under
sponses are emitted and reinforced (with the these circumstances, help is reinforcing be-
stimulus found to reinforce problem behav- cause (like Michael’s screwdriver) it changes
ior), the EO that evokes both problem be- the task into an easier one that does not es-
havior and communication responses is only tablish motivation to escape. Help, of
temporarily modified, although it may be course, is one kind of attention so that, if
more permanently modified by some of the such a transitive relation already exists, prob-
procedures (such as antecedent assistance) lem behavior in the context of demands may
typically used in parallel. That is, FCT seeks be attention rather than escape maintained
to modify the response evoked by the EO (Iwata, 1994; Repp & Karsh, 1994; Rort-
rather than the EO directly. Insofar as the vedt & Miltenberger, 1994).
EO is a reflection of aberrant environmental
characteristics (such as inappropriate de- Developing New Ideas About Treatment
mands) and parallel procedures to directly The preceding discussion suggests the use-
modify such characteristics are not in place, fulness of the EO concept in the analysis of
FCT may raise ethical concerns (because it a number of commonly used treatments.
leaves a counterhabilitative environment in The role of EOs may be more clearly un-
place) and may be limited in its effectiveness derstood, however, through a more thor-
(because the circumstances evoking problem oughgoing analysis of their implications for
behavior still exist). treatment. The intention in the following
Second, the focus on the development of discussion is to consider the implications of
communication responses reflects FCT’s the- a coherent EO-based approach to treatment.
oretical origins in the communication hy- Inevitably, such a discussion will pay less at-
pothesis of problem behavior (Carr & Du- tention to the importance of already well-
rand, 1985b). A focus on communication established concepts such as reinforcement
responses is unnecessary from an EO per- and discriminative stimulus control.
spective. If FCT is conceptualized as modi- EOs and the treatment of problem behavior.
fying the response evoked by the EO, then It is already clear that much problem behav-
a more general notion of the development ior can be seen as discriminated responses
of functionally equivalent responses includ- maintained by social-positive, social-nega-
ing, for example, environmental control tive, and automatic reinforcement. This no-
skills, appears tenable (Carr, 1988; Horner tion has led to analysis and treatment strat-
& Day, 1991; Steege, Wacker, Berg, Cig- egies that focus on the reduction of problem
rand, & Cooper, 1989). behavior through the manipulation of the
Third, a functional equivalence perspec- relevant three-term contingencies. The con-
tive is difficult to maintain regarding those cept of the EO potentially extends this anal-
instances of FCT that involve the develop- ysis because it can help to explain both the
ment of assistance-seeking responses in the evocation of problem behavior and the re-
face of demands such as difficult tasks. The inforcing effectiveness of the maintaining
function of problem behavior in this context consequences—why problem behavior is
would usually be identified as escape, but happening now and why the person ‘‘needs’’
getting help is not functionally equivalent to or ‘‘wants’’ the reinforcer. It seems likely that
escape and may in fact lead to longer rather clinically significant, long-term, generalized
than shorter contact with the task. In this change in problem behavior can occur only
case, the difficult task may constitute a tran- if the EOs that evoke problem behavior are,
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 407

directly or indirectly, addressed in treatment. tention), the person will display the problem
Problem behavior should then establish mo- behaviors that treatment has failed to extin-
tivation in behavior analysts to identify the guish. If such behaviors are dangerous to
relevant EOs and the classes of responses person, property, or reputation, their recur-
evoked by these EOs. Following such inves- rence is clearly problematic. To the extent,
tigation, treatment can focus on the modi- however, that such reemergence indicates the
fication of the EOs, the extinguishing of the likely breakdown or decay of the ‘‘life ar-
EOs, or the modification of the class of re- rangements’’ (Risley, 1996) established in
sponses evoked. treatment, the preserved functionality of
Modifying EOs. As listed in Table 1, the problem behavior may be a useful counter-
EOs most often implicated in the motiva- control that will lead to action that reinstates
tion of problem behavior are deprivation of the previously effective arrangements.
attention, deprivation of tangible items, dep- Second, a focus on the modification of
rivation of stimulation, and demands. Treat- EOs clearly produced by external environ-
ment based on modification of EOs will mental arrangements (e.g., absence of atten-
then involve the provision of higher levels of tion) should not avert consideration of the
attention, tangible items, and stimulation, role played by biological factors and the in-
and the modification of demands. In one ternal environment in the evocation and
sense this is noncontroversial and helps to motivation of problem behavior (Carr,
make treatments such as noncontingent re- 1994). This issue appears to have been little
inforcement and those based around the ma- addressed in the literature of applied behav-
nipulation of antecedents more conceptually ior analysis, perhaps partly because of a lack
systematic. In another sense, however, it may of conceptually systematic approaches to its
extend considerably the purview of applied analysis (Romanczyk & Matthews, 1998).
behavior analysis. Treatment involving the However, it is clear that problem behavior
modification of EOs may not focus on the may arise from or be increased as a function
manipulation of contingencies at all but of various internal or biological circumstanc-
rather on the provision of a better quality of es of a temporary or more permanent nature
life, ‘‘reduced in stress, deprivation, and fear; (Bailey & Pyles, 1989; Carr et al., 1996; Ca-
enriched in those things that attract and en- taldo & Harris, 1982; Clements, 1987;
gage the person’s interest and repertoire’’ Guess & Carr, 1991; Kennedy & Meyer,
(Risley, 1996, p. 428). In short, treatment 1998; Lovaas et al., 1987; McGill, Clare, &
would focus on modifying the challenging Murphy, 1996; Murphy, 1997; Oliver,
environment (McGill, 1993; McGill & Too- 1995).
good, 1994), which the literature on the At least some of these circumstances may
quality of life of people with developmental be considered as EOs because they alter the
disabilities tells us is still commonly found effectiveness of certain other events as rein-
(Emerson & Hatton, 1994; Holburn, forcers or punishers and the frequency of be-
1997). haviors associated with these events. For ex-
Two cautions should be noted. First, it ample, the reinforcing effectiveness of teach-
has been suggested that the modification of er attention may be temporarily increased by
EOs carries the danger of failing to remove the administration of methylphenidate
the functionality of problem behaviors (Iwa- (Northup, Fusilier, Swanson, Roane, & Bor-
ta et al., 1993). In this perspective, when rero, 1997) but enduringly lowered in peo-
confronted with the same circumstances as ple labeled autistic (e.g., Taylor & Carr,
previously prevailed (e.g., deprivation of at- 1992a), the reinforcing effectiveness of food
408 PETER MCGILL

may be enduringly raised in Prader-Willi the relation of density and satiation point
syndrome (Clarke, Boer, & Webb, 1995), should determine the focus given to the
and the aversiveness of demands may be modification of environmental EOs in inter-
temporarily increased by physical illness vention.
(Horner et al., 1996) or sleep deprivation Extinguishing CEOs. The possibility of ex-
(Kennedy & Meyer, 1996; O’Reilly, 1995). tinction of CEOs is not one directly ad-
Events such as autism and Prader-Willi syn- dressed by Michael’s analysis. However, be-
drome do not entirely fit the definition of cause Michael argues that some EOs are un-
the EO, in that their impact on reinforcing conditioned or unlearned and some are con-
effectiveness is enduring rather than mo- ditioned or learned, it seems appropriate to
mentary. Although it is an extension of the consider examples of the extinction of the
EO concept, consideration of the enduring latter. The term extinction usually refers to
motivative effects of genetic or biological cir- the termination of the contingency between
cumstances is not new. Skinner (1989, pp. response and reinforcer with a resulting de-
50–51), for example, discussed the degree to cline in the frequency of the previously re-
which genetic susceptibilities to reinforce- inforced response. In the following discus-
ment have evolved, noting, as an illustration, sion, extinction refers instead to the reduc-
the following of large moving objects by tion and removal of the motivative effects of
ducklings with reductions in the distance to stimuli that have previously functioned as
the object being reinforcing. Deprivation re- CEOs.
sulting from biological impairment was also Reflexive CEOs are stimuli that acquire
described as an EO by Epling and Pierce their motivative effect through correlation
(1990). The probable relevance of such fac- with worsening (e.g., the presentation of an
tors and our lack of knowledge of their ef- aversive stimulus) or improvement (e.g., the
fects suggest caution about the degree to presentation of a reinforcing stimulus) and
which the modification of EOs associated that establish their termination as reinforc-
with external environmental arrangements ing or punishing with consequent evocative
provides a universally applicable treatment or suppressive effects on behaviors associated
for problem behavior. with that termination. CEOs correlated with
Although clearly diverse, the putative EOs worsening may be common occurrences in
associated with biological circumstances escape- or avoidance-motivated problem be-
share the characteristic of modifying (on a havior (Sundberg, 1993). The person who
temporary or more enduring basis) the in- presents a difficult demand, the setting in
dividual’s satiation point for the relevant re- which difficult demands are made, the ma-
inforcer independently of its more typical terials used, and so on, may become CEOs
modification through variation in the avail- that evoke problem behavior, successful both
ability of reinforcement. This suggests that in escaping the conditioned aversive stimuli
the occurrence of problem behavior will, constituting the CEO and in avoiding the
other aspects of the prevailing contingencies worsening (difficult demand). Of course this
being equal, reflect the density of reinforce- usually means that the person may also be
ment available in the relevant environment avoiding demands with which they ordinar-
and the current operation of other variables ily would comply, or even all demands. Ex-
that affect the satiation point of the individ- tinction of such CEOs must involve the pre-
ual for (or sensitivity to, cf. Mulick & Mein- sentation of the same stimuli without the
hold, 1991; Oliver, 1993; Reiss & Haver- worsening that has typically followed in the
camp, 1997) the relevant reinforcer. Further, past. In the example of the person associated
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 409

with difficult demands, extinction might be ‘‘No!’’ It is transitive in that it operates di-
accomplished in a number of ways; for ex- rectly on an independent event (the rein-
ample, by the person remaining (but not forcing effectiveness of escape from the ac-
presenting difficult demands) despite any oc- tivity). Because error correction may be im-
currence of problem behavior (analogous to portant to the development of independence
escape extinction); by associating the person in the activity, it might well be a reasonable
with the provision of positive reinforcement; goal to seek to extinguish such a CEO by
or by the person’s presence (also without dif- methods analogous to those that extinguish
ficult demands) being faded in (analogous to reflexive CEOs. In this example, error-cor-
instructional fading). The key difference rection procedures might be withdrawn and
from more typical use of such procedures is gradually reintroduced, attending to their
that their purpose is to extinguish the CEO, rate, intrusiveness, or other relevant features
not just the specific escape or avoidance re- until procedures that previously evoked
sponse. Note also that the existence of such problem behavior can be used without such
responses should occasion investigation of an effect.
the demands made on the person because (as Two general points about the extinction
a result of content or presentation) they ap- of CEOs are noteworthy. First, despite the
pear to act as EOs motivating escape. If, relevant treatment procedures being similar
having extinguished a reflexive CEO of the to or the same as those involved in the ex-
kind described above, it is again paired with tinction of problem behaviors, their ratio-
subsequent worsening (e.g., through the pre- nale is quite different and their effects would
sentation of unchanged demands), rapid re- also be expected to be different. In particu-
establishment of the reflexive CEO would be lar, successful extinction of a CEO will lead
expected. to the nonoccurrence of all responses (i.e.,
Transitive CEOs are stimuli that alter the the operant class) evoked by the CEO (al-
effectiveness of existing conditioned rein- though topographically similar or identical
forcers or punishers. Assume that problem responses may still be evoked by other EOs).
behavior sometimes (but not always) occurs Second, the extinction of CEOs should not
during a particular activity and is main- proceed lightly because of the danger of ad-
tained by escape from the activity. Events verse side effects. This is particularly the case
that alter the momentary reinforcing effec- when the CEO includes stimuli that would
tiveness of escape from the activity and normally be regarded as aversive, when ex-
evoke escape-maintained problem behavior tinction may involve the person ‘‘putting up
may be acting as transitive CEOs. For ex- with’’ more aversive events than before.
ample, the instructor’s saying ‘‘No!’’ during Modifying the responses evoked by EOs. In
the activity may momentarily increase the some cases it may be seen as unreasonable
reinforcing effectiveness of escape from the to seek to extinguish the motivative effects
activity and evoke problem behavior that re- of EOs, such as severe deprivation of atten-
sults in escape. If it can be shown that the tion, that evoke problem behavior. Because
problem behavior is reinforced by escape such stimulus conditions are not always de-
from the activity (rather than just from the tected or easy to alter, the immediate prob-
instructor saying ‘‘No!’’), then the correction lem for the person is the damaging or self-
procedure has the properties of a transitive damaging nature of the behavior that is
CEO. It is a CEO (rather than an SD) be- evoked. Treatment should, therefore, include
cause escape from the activity is equally the development of functional responses that
available whether or not the instructor says allow the person to temporarily modify the
410 PETER MCGILL

EO other than through behaviors with dam- (with respect to self-injurious behavior) that
aging side effects. This may involve devel- ‘‘many individuals have not acquired socially
oping skills to communicate what he or she appropriate means for gaining access to re-
wants or to obtain it directly. However, the inforcement through others or, alternatively,
environment may not respond to commu- that the social environments of many indi-
nicative attempts (Carr et al., 1996), or the viduals are not responsive to less aberrant
control skills required may be too complex forms of attention-seeking or escape behav-
to establish. Treatment may also consider, ior’’ (p. 235). Similarly, Durand (1990, p. 6)
therefore, teaching the person to substitute noted that ‘‘behavior problems . . . are rea-
one attainable reinforcer for another, tem- sonable behavioral adaptations necessitated
porarily unattainable reinforcer (cf. Smith & by the abilities of our students and the lim-
Iwata, 1997) and to cope (cf. Gardner et al., itations of their environments.’’ An EO per-
1986) with the temporary continuation of spective suggests that, in addition, the dep-
what has historically constituted a state of rivation and aversiveness found in some of
deprivation or aversive stimulation. When these environments evoke problem behavior
coupled with the other treatment compo- by establishing motivation for reinforcers that
nents discussed above, this may allow the are not easily obtained in any other way, and
person to avoid problem behavior even un- that biological circumstances may serve a
der conditions of ‘‘challenge’’ or ‘‘relapse’’ similar function. These ‘‘causes’’ of problem
(Reiss & Havercamp, 1997) in environmen- behavior suggest a multicomponent agenda
tal or biological conditions (see also Horner (cf. Carr & Smith, 1995) for efforts that seek
et al., 1996). On its own, however, such a to reduce its prevalence through prevention.
treatment strategy would be fraught with the
dangers associated with attempts to help Developing Adaptive Behaviors That
‘‘people adjust to a system that in itself is in Allow Access to Reinforcement
need of change’’ (Winnett & Winkler, 1972,
pp. 501–502). This is a common course of action in in-
dividual treatment when, as noted above, def-
icits in adaptive, reinforcement-accessing
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS skills are commonly found, and their devel-
AND THE PREVENTION OF opment may lead to successfully ‘‘replacing’’
PROBLEM BEHAVIOR problem behavior in the person’s repertoire.
Although the vast majority of behavior- As an agenda for prevention, it is consistent
analytic studies of problem behavior have fo- with programs (Dunlap et al., 1990; Mc-
cused on individual assessment and treat- Eachin et al., 1993) that have sought to re-
ment, a small number of studies (e.g., Dun- duce the prevalence of problem behavior.
lap, Foster Johnson, & Robbins, 1990; Note that in neither individual nor systemic
McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993) have treatment does this approach require an
documented reductions in its prevalence as awareness of EOs. Rather it can be derived
an apparent result of prophylactic interven- entirely from analysis of the reinforcement
tion within relatively small, defined groups. contingencies commonly found to maintain
The agenda for such efforts has been in- problem behavior. As seen in the earlier dis-
formed by the knowledge gained from studies cussion of functional communication train-
of the events evoking and maintaining prob- ing, however, an EO perspective may draw
lem behavior in individuals. For example, attention to possible limitations and exten-
Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al. (1994) concluded sions of such an approach.
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 411

Supporting Environments’ Responsiveness to maintain problem behavior is established by


Adaptive Behavior conditions of deprivation of attention, tan-
In individual treatment, the simple devel- gible items, and stimulation, and the occur-
opment of functionally equivalent alterna- rence of aversive events (see Table 1). The
tives to problem behavior reinforced by oth- modification of such EOs has become an
ers may not be sufficient when existing re- important part of conceptions of both in-
inforcement contingencies for problem be- dividual treatment (e.g., Iwata et al., 1993)
havior remain in place (Horner & Day, and a variety of specific treatments (e.g.,
1991; Shirley, Iwata, Kahng, Mazaleski, & Horner et al., 1997; Kennedy, 1994; Voll-
Lerman, 1997). Reduction in problem be- mer et al., 1993). Given that such EOs are
havior requires that staff or caregivers are at commonly found in the environments of
least as responsive (i.e., provide equivalently people at risk for problem behavior, their
immediate, frequent, and high-quality rein- systemic modification holds the promise of
forcement; Fisher & Mazur, 1997) for the reducing both the occurrence of problem be-
alternative as for the problem behavior. havior in individuals for whom it is already
Studies of the apparent escalation of prob-
part of their repertoire and preventing its
lem behavior (Lalli, Mace, Wohn, & Livezey,
initial onset in others. Inevitably, such a
1995) or the apparent differential reinforce-
project also has likely limitations. In partic-
ment of longer durations (Hall & Oliver,
1992) are consistent with a lack of such re- ular, the general modification of EOs in this
sponsiveness prior to individual treatment. way is likely to interact with other influences
More systemic studies of caretaker–client in- on motivation (e.g., biological variables)
teraction (Felce et al., 1987; Warren & with, in individual cases, an increase rather
Mondy, 1971) suggest that it is not uncom- than a decrease in motivation for the rein-
mon to find higher rates of presumed rein- forcers that maintain problem behavior. For
forcement available for problem behavior example, an individual whose problem be-
than for appropriate behavior. It has been havior is maintained by escape from social
suggested that such patterns of interaction interaction may, in the presence of generally
may help to shape the frequency or severity increased levels of social contact, display
of problem behavior over time (Warren & higher rates of problem behavior (Taylor &
Mondy, 1971) so that systemic interventions Carr, 1992a). Also, the impact of such EO
that seek to alter such patterns are of poten- modification on other behaviors and on the
tial value (Felce, 1991; McGill & Toogood, availability of other reinforcers should be
1994). Notably, for this paper’s context, considered. The frequent and noncontingent
problem behavior has been hypothesized to availability of the reinforcers that typically
be an aversive stimulus that either punishes maintain problem behavior may lead to re-
caregiver behavior (Taylor & Carr, 1992b) ductions in the rate of functionally equiva-
or acts as an EO evoking caregiver responses lent, adaptive alternatives to problem behav-
(such as the provision of attention or the ior such as communication (Kahng, Iwata,
withdrawal of demands) that successfully, al- DeLeon, & Worsdell, 1997; but see Marcus
beit temporarily, remove the problem behav-
& Vollmer, 1996) and a reduction in the
ior (Oliver, 1995).
degree to which the individual controls the
Providing Reinforcing, Nonaversive environment (Carr, McConnachie, Levin, &
Environments Kemp, 1993). The seriousness and preva-
It has been argued above that motivation lence of such side effects require empirical
for the reinforcers commonly found to study.
412 PETER MCGILL

Modifying Biological Circumstances vention through the changing of cultural


It has been argued above that temporary practices (Biglan, 1995; Mayer, 1995).
or more enduring biological circumstances
may act as EOs that increase satiation points CONCLUDING COMMENTS
for the typical consequences of problem be-
havior. Although the importance of screen- Although EOs have been the focus of this
ing for biological and genetic abnormalities paper, they should be considered in a more
has long been recognized (Bijou, 1966; Carr, general behavior-analytic context. Much
progress has been made in accounting for
1977), they have sometimes been seen as in-
and treating problem behavior by primary
fluencing behavior in a quite different way
reference to the reinforcing and discrimina-
to the events typically investigated through
tive functions of the stimuli to which it is
functional analysis. This extension of the
related. The EO concept extends this ac-
EO concept provides a way of considering
count both by drawing attention to an ad-
the behavioral function of such states or
ditional function of previously recognized
events in a conceptually systematic way that
stimuli and by allowing additional stimuli
allows their incorporation in individual anal-
(previously unrecognized or of unclear func-
ysis and intervention. Studies reporting such tion) to be included in assessment and treat-
approaches in work with individuals have in- ment. Considerable attention has been given
evitably focused on those EOs that are rel- in recent years to the importance of consid-
atively easy to define, measure, and directly ering and analyzing the context in which
modify; for example, premenstrual difficul- three-term contingencies develop and are
ties (Carr & Smith, 1995), otitis media maintained (e.g., Carr, 1994; Morris, 1992).
(O’Reilly, 1997), and sleep deprivation The EO allows the more conceptually sys-
(Kennedy & Meyer, 1996). Although indi- tematic consideration of some aspects of this
vidual treatment is clearly still developing in context but should not be seen as equivalent
this area, the systemic treatment and preven- to all of the aspects discussed in the litera-
tion agenda is to begin to map the biological ture. For example, the concepts of setting
circumstances found to be implicated in in- factors (Kantor, 1959) and setting events
stances of problem behavior, consider their (Bijou & Baer, 1961) include references to
possible prevalence, and, when practical, tar- analogous operations but also refer to stim-
get intervention (the modification of EOs) ulus functions that do not appear to relate
at high-risk groups. directly to EOs (Kennedy & Meyer, 1998).
This analysis suggests that we are now at In summary, the notions revived in Mi-
the point at which we can begin to think chael’s (1982, 1993) treatment of the EO
about the systemic treatment and prevention have been shown to have considerable rele-
of problem behavior with the aim of sub- vance to conceptions of problem behavior
stantially reducing its prevalence. Our un- and its assessment, treatment, and preven-
derstanding of what is needed in this task tion. They enable a more complete account
appears to be enhanced by the application of problem behavior to be given, both gen-
of the EO concept and is consistent with erally and with respect to particular individ-
developments in the behavior analysis of uals. The pretreatment assessment of EOs
other applied problems in which the agenda seems likely to have important implications
is shifting from an exclusive concern with for treatment. Further, existing treatments
individual contingency management to a can be usefully analyzed from an EO per-
concern with systemic intervention and pre- spective, and the concept can guide the de-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 413

velopment of frameworks for both treatment in children. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), The-
oretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 219–254).
and prevention. New York: Academic Press.
Carr, E. G., McConnachie, G., Levin, L., & Kemp,
D. C. (1993). Communication-based treatment
REFERENCES of severe behavior problems. In R. Van Houten
& S. Axelrod (Eds.), Behavior analysis and treat-
Adelinis, J. D., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Han- ment (pp. 231–267). New York: Plenum.
ley, G. P. (1997). The establishing effects of client Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976).
location on self-injurious behavior. Research in De- Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a
velopmental Disabilities, 18, 383–391.
psychotic child. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. (1959). The psychiatric
chology, 4, 139–153.
nurse as behavioral engineer. Journal of the Exper-
imental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 323–334. Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980).
Bailey, J. S., & Pyles, D. A. M. (1989). Behavioral Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior of
diagnostics. In E. Cipani (Ed.), The treatment of two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior
severe behavior disorders (pp. 85–107). Washing- Analysis, 13, 101–117.
ton, DC: American Association on Mental Retar- Carr, E. G., Reeve, C. E., & Magito-McLaughlin, D.
dation. (1996). Contextual influences on problem behav-
Berkson, G., & Mason, W. A. (1964). Stereotyped ior in people with developmental disabilities. In
movements of mental defectives: IV. The effects L. K. Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.),
of toys and the character of acts. American Journal Positive behavioral support: Including people with
of Mental Deficiency, 68, 511–524. difficult behavior in the community (pp. 403–423).
Biglan, A. (1995). Changing cultural practices: A con- Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
textualist framework for intervention. Reno, NV: Carr, E. G., Robinson, S., Taylor, J. C., & Carlson,
Context Press. J. I. (1990). Positive approaches to the treatment of
Bijou, S. W. (1966). A functional analysis of retarded severe behavior problems in persons with develop-
development. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International mental disabilities: A review and analysis of rein-
review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 1, pp. forcement and stimulus-based procedures. Seattle:
1–19). New York: Academic Press. The Association for Persons with Severe Handi-
Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1961). Child develop- caps.
ment: Vol. 1. A systematic and empirical theory. En- Carr, E. G., & Smith, C. E. (1995). Biological setting
glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. events for self-injury. Mental Retardation and De-
Bodfish, J. W., & Konarski, E. A. (1992). Reducing velopmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 1, 94–
problem behaviors in a residential unit using 98.
structural analysis and staff management proce- Cataldo, M. F., & Harris, J. (1982). The biological
dures: A preliminary study. Behavioral Residential basis for self-injury in the mentally retarded. Anal-
Treatment, 7, 225–234. ysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities,
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious 2, 21–39.
behavior: A review of some hypotheses. Psycholog- Clarke, D. J., Boer, H., & Webb, T. (1995). Genetic
ical Bulletin, 84, 800–816. and behavioural aspects of Prader-Willi syndrome.
Carr, E. G. (1988). Functional equivalence as a Mental Handicap Research, 8, 38–53.
mechanism of response generalization. In R. H. Clements, J. (1987). Severe learning disability and psy-
Horner, R. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Gener- chological handicap. Chichester, England: Wiley.
alization and maintenance: Life-style changes in ap-
Davis, C. A., Brady, M. P., Williams, R. E., & Ham-
plied settings (pp. 221–241). Baltimore: Paul H.
ilton, R. (1992). Effects of high-probability re-
Brookes.
Carr, E. G. (1994). Emerging themes in the func- quests on the acquisition and generalization of re-
tional analysis of problem behavior. Journal of Ap- sponses to requests in young children with behav-
plied Behavior Analysis, 27, 393–399. ior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Carr, E. G., & Carlson, J. I. (1993). Reduction of 25, 905–916.
severe behavior problems in the community using Davis, C. A., & Reichle, J. (1996). Variant and in-
a multicomponent treatment approach. Journal of variant high-probability requests: Increasing ap-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 157–172. propriate behaviors in children with emotional-
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985a). Reducing behavioral disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior
behavior problems through functional communi- Analysis, 29, 471–482.
cation training. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal- Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M.,
ysis, 18, 111–126. Northup, J., Cigrand, K., & Asmus, J. (1992).
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985b). The social- Brief functional assessment techniques to evaluate
communicative basis of severe behavior problems aberrant behavior in an outpatient setting: A sum-
414 PETER MCGILL

mary of 79 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal- to application: An experimental analysis of severe
ysis, 25, 713–721. problem behaviors. In A. C. Repp & N. N. Singh
Ducharme, J. M., & Worling, D. E. (1994). Behav- (Eds.), Perspectives on the use of nonaversive and
ioral momentum and stimulus fading in the ac- aversive interventions for persons with developmental
quisition and maintenance of child compliance in disabilities (pp. 451–464). Sycamore, IL: Syca-
the home. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, more.
639–647. Felce, D. (1991). Using behavioural principles in the
Dunlap, G., Foster Johnson, L., & Robbins, F. R. development of effective housing services for
(1990). Preventing serious behavior problems adults with severe or profound mental handicap.
through skill development and early intervention. In B. Remington (Ed.), The challenge of severe
In A. C. Repp & N. N. Singh (Eds.), Perspectives mental handicap: A behaviour analytic approach
on the use of nonaversive and aversive interventions (pp. 285–316). Chichester, England: Wiley.
for persons with developmental disabilities (pp. 273– Felce, D., Saxby, H., deKock, U., Repp, A., Ager, A.,
286). Sycamore, IL: Sycamore. & Blunden, R. (1987). To what behaviors do
Dunlap, G., Kern-Dunlap, L., Clarke, S., & Robbins, attending adults respond?: A replication. American
F. R. (1991). Functional assessment, curricular Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 496–504.
revision, and severe behavior problems. Journal of Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 387–397. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary re-
Durand, V. M. (1990). Severe behavior problems: A inforcers as treatment for self-injurious behavior.
functional communication training approach. New Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.
York: Guilford. Fisher, W. W., & Mazur, J. E. (1997). Basic and ap-
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influ- plied research on choice responding. Journal of Ap-
ences on ‘‘self-stimulatory’’ behavior: Analysis and plied Behavior Analysis, 30, 387–410.
treatment application. Journal of Applied Behavior Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jef-
Analysis, 20, 119–132. ferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional communication training with and without extinc-
communication training to reduce challenging be- tion and punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior
havior: Maintenance and application in new set- Analysis, 26, 23–36.
tings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, Flannery, K. B., & Horner, R. H. (1994). The rela-
251–264. tionship between predictability and problem be-
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1992). An analysis of havior for students with severe disabilities. Journal
maintenance following functional communication of Behavioral Education, 4, 157–176.
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, Foster-Johnson, L., Ferro, J., & Dunlap, G. (1994).
777–794. Preferred curricular activities and reduced prob-
Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988a). Iden- lem behaviors in students with intellectual dis-
tifying the variables maintaining self-injurious be- abilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
havior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- 493–504.
orders, 18, 99–117. Gardner, W. I., Cole, C. L., Davidson, D. P., & Kar-
Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988b). The an, O. C. (1986). Reducing aggression in indi-
motivation assessment scale: An administration man- viduals with developmental disabilities: An ex-
ual. New York: State University of New York at panded stimulus control, assessment, and inter-
Albany. vention model. Education and Training in Mental
Edelson, S. M., Taubman, M. T., & Lovaas, O. I. Retardation, 21, 3–12.
(1983). Some social contexts of self-destructive Gewirtz, J. L., & Baer, D. M. (1958a). Deprivation
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, and satiation of social reinforcers as drive condi-
11, 299–312. tions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
Emerson, E. (1995). Challenging behaviour: Analysis 57, 165–172.
and intervention in people with learning disabilities. Gewirtz, J. L., & Baer, D. M. (1958b). The effect of
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. brief social deprivation on behaviors for a social
Emerson, E., & Hatton, C. (1994). Moving out: The reinforcer. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
impact of relocation from hospital to community on ogy, 56, 49–56.
the quality of life of people with learning disabilities. Goh, H. L., & Iwata, B. A. (1994). Behavioral per-
London: HMSO. sistence and variability during extinction of self-
Emerson, E., Thompson, S., Reeves, D., Henderson, injury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied
D., & Robertson, J. (1995). Descriptive analysis Behavior Analysis, 27, 173–174.
of multiple response topographies of challenging Guess, D., & Carr, E. (1991). Emergence and main-
behavior across two settings. Research in Develop- tenance of stereotypy and self-injury. American
mental Disabilities, 16, 301–329. Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, 299–319.
Epling, W. F., & Pierce, W. D. (1990). Laboratory Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., & Legacy, S. M.
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 415

(1994). Schedule effects of noncontingent rein- Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Mil-
forcement on attention-maintained destructive be- tenberger, R. G. (1994). What makes extinction
havior in identical quadruplets. Journal of Applied work: An analysis of procedural form and func-
Behavior Analysis, 27, 317–325. tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (1992). Differential effects of 131–144.
severe self-injurious behaviour on the behaviour Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J.
of others. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 20, 355–365. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., Rodgers, T. A.,
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). Lerman, D. C., Shore, B. A., Mazaleski, J. L.,
Noncontingent presentation of attention and al- Goh, H. L., Cowdery, G. E., Kalsher, M. J.,
ternative stimuli in the treatment of attention- McCosh, K. C., & Willis, K. D. (1994). The
maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied functions of self-injurious behavior: An experi-
Behavior Analysis, 30, 229–237. mental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied
Hile, M. G., & Walbran, B. B. (1991). Observing Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240.
staff-resident interactions: What staff do, what Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery,
residents receive. Mental Retardation, 29, 35–41. G. E., & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental
Holburn, S. (1997). A renaissance in residential be- analysis and extinction of self-injurious escape be-
havior analysis? A historical perspective and a bet- havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23,
ter way to help people with challenging behavior. 11–27.
The Behavior Analyst, 20, 61–85. Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1990).
Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of The experimental (functional) analysis of behavior
response efficiency on functionally equivalent disorders: Methodology, applications and limita-
competing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior tions. In A. C. Repp & N. N. Singh (Eds.), Per-
Analysis, 24, 719–732. spectives on the use of nonaversive and aversive in-
Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., & Day, J. R. (1997). terventions for persons with developmental disabili-
Using neutralizing routines to reduce problem be-
ties (pp. 301–330). Sycamore, IL: Sycamore.
havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30,
Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Rod-
601–614.
gers, T. A. (1993). Treatment classification and
Horner, R. H., Sprague, J. R., O’Brien, M., & Heath-
selection based on behavioral function. In R. Van
field, T. H. (1990). The role of response efficien-
cy in the reduction of problem behaviors through Houten & S. Axelrod (Eds.), Behavior analysis and
functional equivalence training: A case study. Jour- treatment (pp. 101–125). New York: Plenum
nal of the Association for Persons with Severe Hand- Press.
icaps, 15, 91–97. Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Worsdell,
Horner, R. H., Vaughn, B. J., Day, H. M., & Ard, A. S. (1997). Evaluation of the ‘‘control over re-
W. R., Jr. (1996). The relationship between set- inforcement’’ component in functional commu-
ting events and problem behavior. In L. K. Koe- nication training. Journal of Applied Behavior
gel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive Analysis, 30, 262–277.
behavioral support: Including people with difficult Kantor, J. R. (1959). Interbehavioral psychology. Gran-
behavior in the community (pp. 381–402). Balti- ville, OH: Principia Press.
more: Paul H. Brookes. Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles
Houlihan, D., & Brandon, P. K. (1996). Compliant of psycholog y. New York: Appleton-Century-
in a moment: A commentary on Nevin. Journal Crofts.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 549–555. Kennedy, C. H. (1994). Manipulating antecedent
Iwata, B. (1987). Negative reinforcement in applied conditions to alter the stimulus control of prob-
behavior analysis: An emerging technology. Jour- lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 361–378. 27, 161–170.
Iwata, B. A. (1994). Functional analysis methodolo- Kennedy, C. H., Itkonen, T., & Lindquist, K. (1995).
gy: Some closing comments. Journal of Applied Be- Comparing interspersed requests and social com-
havior Analysis, 27, 413–418. ments as antecedents for increasing student com-
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. pliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a func- 97–98.
tional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Be- Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1996). Sleep dep-
havior Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from rivation, allergy symptoms and negatively rein-
Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Dis- forced behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
abilities, 2, 3–20, 1982) ysis, 29, 133–135.
Iwata, B. A., & Michael, J. L. (1994). Applied im- Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1998). Establishing
plications of theory and research on the nature of operations and the motivation of challenging be-
reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy- havior. In J. K. Luiselli & M. J. Cameron (Eds.),
sis, 27, 183–193. Antecedent control: Innovative approaches to behav-
416 PETER MCGILL

ioral support (pp. 329–346). Baltimore: Paul H. nants of aggression and disruption in mentally re-
Brookes. tarded children. Applied Research in Mental Retar-
Kennedy, C. H., & Souza, G. (1995). Functional dation, 7, 203–221.
analysis and treatment of eye poking. Journal of Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R. (1996). Combining
Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 27–37. noncontingent reinforcement and differential re-
Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livezey, K. inforcement schedules as treatment for aberrant
(1995). Identification and modification of a re- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,
sponse-class hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior 43–51.
Analysis, 28, 551–559. Mayer, G. R. (1995). Preventing antisocial behavior
LaVigna, G. W., & Donellan, A. M. (1986). Alter- in the schools. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
natives to punishment: Solving behavior problems 28, 467–478.
with non-aversive strategies. New York: Irvington. Mazaleski, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone,
LaVigna, G. W., Willis, T. J., & Donellan, A. M. J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the
(1989). The role of positive programming in be- reinforcement and extinction components of
havioral treatment. In E. Cipani (Ed.), The treat- DRO contingencies with self-injury. Journal of Ap-
ment of severe behavior disorders (pp. 59–83). plied Behavior Analysis, 26, 143–156.
Washington, DC: American Association on Men- McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993).
tal Retardation. Long-term outcome for children with autism who
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive received early intensive behavioral interventions.
and experimental analyses of variables maintaining American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 359–
self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior 372.
Analysis, 26, 293–319. McGill, P. (1993). Challenging behaviour, challeng-
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1995). Prevalence of ing environments, and challenging needs. Clinical
the extinction burst and its attenuation during Psychology Forum, 56, 14–18.
treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, McGill, P., Clare, I. C. H., & Murphy, G. H. (1996).
93–94. Understanding and responding to challenging be-
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing haviour: From theory to practice. Tizard Learning
a technology for the use of operant extinction in Disability Review, 1, 9–17.
clinical settings: An examination of basic and ap- McGill, P., & Toogood, A. (1994). Organizing com-
plied research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, munity placements. In E. Emerson, P. McGill, &
29, 345–382. J. Mansell (Eds.), Severe learning disabilities and
Lovaas, O., Newsom, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). challenging behaviours: Designing high quality ser-
Self-stimulatory behavior and perceptual rein- vices (pp. 232–259). London: Chapman & Hall.
forcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Meyer, L., & Evans, I. M. (1989). Nonaversive inter-
20, 45–68. vention for behavior problems: A manual for home
Mace, F. C., & Belfiore, P. (1990). Behavioral mo- and community. New York: Teachers College Press.
mentum in the treatment of escape-motivated ste- Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discrim-
reotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, inative and motivational functions of stimuli.
507–514. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
Mace, F. C., Browder, D. M., & Lin, Y. (1987). Anal- 37, 149–155.
ysis of demand conditions associated with stereo- Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Be-
typy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental havior Analyst, 16, 191–206.
Psychiatry, 18, 25–31. Morris, E. K. (1988). Contextualism: The world view
Mace, F. C., Hock, M. L., Lalli, J. S., West, B. J., of behavior analysis. Journal of Experimental Child
Belfiore, P., Pinter, E., & Brown, D. K. (1988). Psychology, 46, 289–323.
Behavioral momentum in the treatment of non- Morris, E. K. (1992). The aim, progress, and evolu-
compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, tion of behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst,
21, 123–141. 15, 3–29.
Mace, F. C., & Lalli, J. S. (1991). Linking descriptive Mulick, J. A., & Meinhold, P. M. (1991). Evaluating
and experimental analyses in the treatment of bi- models for the emergence and maintenance of ste-
zarre speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, reotypy and self-injury. American Journal on Men-
24, 553–562. tal Retardation, 96, 327–333.
Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., Pinter Lalli, E., & Shea, M. Murphy, G. H. (1997). Understanding aggression in
C. (1993). Functional analysis and treatment of people with intellectual disabilities: Lessons from
aberrant behavior. In R. Van Houten & S. Axel- other populations. In N. Bray (Ed.) International
rod (Eds.), Behavior analysis and treatment (pp. review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 21,
75–99). New York: Plenum. pp. 33–67). New York: Academic Press.
Mace, F. C., Page, T. J., Ivancic, M. T., & O’Brien, Neef, N. A., & Iwata, B. A. (1994). Current research
S. (1986). Analysis of environmental determi- on functional analysis methodologies: An intro-
ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS AND PROBLEM BEHAVIOR 417

duction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1997). Sensitivity
211–214. theory and mental retardation: Why functional
Nevin, J. A. (1996). The momentum of compliance. analysis is not enough. American Journal on Men-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 535–547. tal Retardation, 101, 553–566.
Nevin, J. A., Mandell, C., & Atak, J. R. (1983). The Repp, A. C., Felce, D., & Barton, L. E. (1988). Bas-
analysis of behavior momentum. Journal of the Ex- ing the treatment of stereotypic and self-injurious
perimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 49–59. behaviors on hypotheses of their causes. Journal of
Northup, J., Fusilier, I., Swanson, V., Roane, H., & Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 281–289.
Borrero, J. (1997). An evaluation of methylphe- Repp, A. C., & Karsh, K. G. (1994). Hypothesis-
nidate as a potential establishing operation for based interventions for tantrum behaviors of per-
some common classroom reinforcers. Journal of sons with developmental disabilities in school set-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 615–625. tings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 21–
Oliver, C. (1993). Self-injurious behaviour: From re- 31.
sponse to strategy. In C. Kiernan (Ed.), Research Risley, T. (1996). Get a life! Positive behavioral in-
to practice? Implications of research on the challeng- tervention for challenging behavior through life
ing behaviour of people with learning disability (pp. arrangement and life coaching. In L. K. Koegel,
135–188). Clevedon, England: British Institute of R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive be-
Learning Disabilities. havioral support: Including people with difficult be-
Oliver, C. (1995). Annotation: Self-injurious behav- havior in the community (pp. 425–437). Balti-
iour in children with learning disabilities: Recent more: Paul H. Brookes.
advances in assessment and intervention. Journal Romanczyk, R. G., & Matthews, A. L. (1998). Phys-
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 909–927. iological state as antecedent: Utilization in func-
O’Neil, R., Horner, R., Albin, R., Storey, K., & tional analysis. In J. K. Luiselli & M. J. Cameron
Sprague, J. (1990). Functional analysis: A practical (Eds.), Antecedent control: Innovative approaches to
assessment guide. Sycamore, IL: Sycamore.
behavioral support (pp. 115–138). Baltimore: Paul
O’Reilly, M. F. (1995). Functional analysis and treat-
H. Brookes.
ment of escape-maintained aggression correlated
Rortvedt, A. K., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1994). Anal-
with sleep deprivation. Journal of Applied Behavior
ysis of a high-probability instructional sequence
Analysis, 28, 225–226.
O’Reilly, M. F. (1997). Functional analysis of episod- and time-out in the treatment of child noncom-
ic self-injury correlated with recurrent otitis me- pliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
dia. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 165– 327–330.
167. Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S., Mazaleski, J.
Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Cowdery, G. E., Andree, P. L., & Lerman, D. C. (1997). Does functional
J., & McIntyre, T. (1993). Stimulus (instruction- communication training compete with ongoing
al) fading during extinction of self-injurious es- contingencies of reinforcement? An analysis dur-
cape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, ing response acquisition and maintenance. Journal
26, 205–212. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 93–104.
Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Edwards, G. L., & McCosh, Singer, G. H. S., Singer, J., & Horner, R. H. (1987).
K. C. (1986). Stimulus fading and transfer in the Using pretask requests to increase the probability
treatment of self-restraint and self-injurious be- of compliance for students with severe disabilities.
havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
381–389. Handicaps, 12, 287–291.
Parrish, J. M., Cataldo, M. F., Kolko, D. J., Neef, N. Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in
A., & Egel, A. L. (1986). Experimental analysis the description of behavior. Journal of General Psy-
of response covariation among compliant and in- chology, 5, 427–458.
appropriate behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
Analysis, 19, 241–254. New York: Macmillan.
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, Skinner, B. F. (1989). Recent issues in the analysis of
L. A., Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., & Keeney, behavior. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
K. M. (1998). Treatment of pica through mul- Smith, R. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1997). Antecedent
tiple analyses of its reinforcing functions. Journal influences on behavior disorders. Journal of Ap-
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 165–189. plied Behavior Analysis, 30, 343–375.
Pyles, D. A. M., & Bailey, J. S. (1990). Diagnosing Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Goh, H. L., & Shore, B.
severe behavior problems. In A. C. Repp & N. A. (1995). Analysis of establishing operations for
N. Singh (Eds.), Perspectives on the use of nonav- self-injury maintained by escape. Journal of Ap-
ersive and aversive interventions for persons with de- plied Behavior Analysis, 28, 515–535.
velopmental disabilities (pp. 381–401). Sycamore, Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. (1992). Covariation
IL: Sycamore. within functional response classes: Implications
418 PETER MCGILL

for treatment of severe problem behavior. Journal quantity on problematic behavior. Journal of Ap-
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 735–745. plied Behavior Analysis, 29, 79–87.
Steege, M. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Cigrand, Wahler, R. G., & Fox, J. J. (1981). Setting events in
K. K., & Cooper, L. J. (1989). The use of be- applied behavior analysis: Toward a conceptual
havioral assessment to prescribe and evaluate treat- and methodological expansion. Journal of Applied
ments for severely handicapped children. Journal Behavior Analysis, 14, 327–338.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 23–33. Warren, S. A., & Mondy, L. W. (1971). To what
Sturmey, P. (1995). Analog baselines: A critical review behaviors do attending adults respond? American
of the methodology. Research in Developmental Journal of Mental Deficiency, 75, 449–455.
Disabilities, 16, 269–284. Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task diffi-
Sturmey, P., Carlsen, A., Crisp, A. G., & Newton, J. culty and aberrant behavior in severely handi-
capped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
T. (1988). A functional analysis of multiple ab-
ysis, 14, 449–463.
errant responses: A refinement and extension of Wells, M. E., & Smith, D. W. (1983). Reduction of
Iwata et al.’s (1982) methodology. Journal of Men- self-injurious behavior of mentally retarded per-
tal Deficiency Research, 32, 31–46. sons using sensory-integrative techniques. Ameri-
Sundberg, M. L. (1993). The application of estab- can Journal of Mental Deficiency, 87, 664–666.
lishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 211– Whittington, R., & Wykes, T. (1996). Aversive stim-
214. ulation by staff and violence by psychiatric pa-
Taylor, J. C., & Carr, E. G. (1992a). Severe problem tients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35,
behaviors related to social interaction 1: Attention 11–20.
seeking and social avoidance. Behavior Modifica- Wilder, D. A., & Carr, J. E. (1998). Recent advances
tion, 16, 305–335. in the modification of establishing operations to
Taylor, J. C., & Carr, E. G. (1992b). Severe problem reduce aberrant behavior. Behavioral Interventions,
behaviors related to social interaction 2: A systems 13, 43–59.
analysis. Behavior Modification, 16, 336–371. Winnett, R. A., & Winkler, R. C. (1972). Current
Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N. behavior modification in the classroom: Be still,
(1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus be quiet, be docile. Journal of Applied Behavior
control of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Analysis, 5, 499–504.
Behavior Analysis, 18, 343–351. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Hughes, C. E., & Voll-
Tustin, R. (1995). The effects of advance notice of mer, T. R. (1993). Momentum versus extinction
activity transitions on stereotypic behavior. Journal effects in the treatment of self-injurious escape be-
havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26,
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 91–92.
135–136.
Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Mazaleski, J. L., & Smith,
G., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of atten- R. G. (1994). Momentum and extinction effects
tion in the treatment of attention-maintained self- on self-injurious escape behavior and noncompli-
injurious behavior: Noncontingent reinforcement ance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
and differential reinforcement of other behavior. 649–658.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Jagtiani,
Vollmer, T., Marcus, B., & Ringdahl, J. (1995). Non- S., Smith, G., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). Extinc-
contingent escape as treatment for self-injurious tion of self-injurious behavior with and without
behavior maintained by negative reinforcement. instructional fading. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15–26. Analysis, 26, 353–360.
Wacker, D. P., Harding, J., Cooper, L. J., Derby, K. Received November 18, 1997
M., Peck, S., Asmus, J., Berg, W. K., & Brown, Final acceptance April 30, 1999
K. A. (1996). The effects of meal schedule and Action Editor, Joseph E. Spradlin

You might also like