You are on page 1of 14

Sampling Context Affects MLU in the

Language of Adolescents With


Down Syndrome
RESEARCH NOTE

Sally Miles
Robin Chapman Purpose: The authors describe the procedures used to explain an unexpected finding
Heidi Sindberg that adolescents with Down syndrome (DS) had a lower mean length of utterance
Waisman Center, (MLU) than typically developing (TD) children in interviews without picture support, but
University of Wisconsin—Madison not in narratives supported by wordless picture books. They hypothesize that the
picture support of the narrative context increased the MLU for the group with DS alone.
Method: Adolescents with DS (n = 14) and TD children (n = 14) matched for receptive
syntax narrated picture storybooks and participated in interviews. Transcription
reliability, intelligibility/fluency, grammatical errors, discourse and sampling contexts,
and discourse characteristics were examined for their effects on MLU.
Results: The DS group showed a greater responsiveness to adult questions than the TD
group; an alternate MLU without yes/no responses showed the same interaction of
group and context as the original finding. An additional comparison of MLUs, obtained
from narratives present in the interview and narratives elicited using picture books,
showed that picture support in narrative increased MLUs only for the group with DS.
Conclusion: Picture support, rather than narrative context alone, increased MLUs for the
group with DS. Clinical use of narratives and picture support in assessment and
intervention with individuals with DS is discussed.
KEY WORDS: MLU, Down syndrome, language samples,
child language disorders, language assessment

D
own syndrome (DS) has been shown to have a specific behavioral
phenotype in language that includes delays in expressive
syntax, errors of grammatical morpheme omission and use,
and deficits in intelligibility (Chapman & Hesketh, 2000; Miller, Leddy,
& Leavitt, 1999). In individuals with DS, mean length of utterance
(MLU) has typically been used as a measure of expressive language
in conversational (Fowler, 1995) or narrative (Chapman, Seung,
Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1998) speech samples, which have
been shown to differ in the opportunities for complex syntax that they
provide (Abbeduto, Benson, Short, & Dolish, 1995; Chapman et al.
1998; Leadholm & Miller, 1992; Thordardottir, Chapman, & Wagner,
2002). MLU differences have been shown to correspond to differences
in complex syntax use (Thordardottir et al., 2002). Variation among nar-
rative contexts has received less study in the DS population, although
MLU has been shown to be influenced by discourse contexts.
This research note first reports our comparison of MLUs computed
from language samples obtained in two contexts: narratives elicited using
wordless picture books and an interview that allowed varied forms of

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006  AAmerican Speech-Language-Hearing Association 325
1092-4388/06/4902-0325
extended discourse including narration but used no a developmental measure for TD children beyond the age
picture materials to elicit any content. Two groups were of 5 years and for children with language disorders with
tested: adolescents with DS and a comparison group of MLUs greater than 5.0 (Miller, Freiberg, Rolland, &
typically developing (TD) children matched for syntax Reeves, 1992; Leadholm & Miller, 1992). Interview sam-
comprehension. Findings from previous research led us ples have also been used with older children with lan-
to make two predictions: (a) that narrative MLUs, if guage disorders (Evans & Craig, 1992). Here we use both
higher than those from the interview, would be so for both narrative and interview samples.
groups (Abbeduto et al., 1995) and (b) that MLUs derived Discourse characteristics. Length of utterance may
from the same sampling context would be higher for the reflect the pragmatic requirements of different types of
syntax-comprehension-matched TD group than those of interactions rather than syntactic development (Brown,
the group with DS (Chapman et al., 1998). Instead we 1973). In sampling the language of children with lan-
found that the group with DS had an MLU in the nar- guage and cognitive disorders, MLU may be affected by
rative context that was significantly higher than in the differing levels of skill in the pragmatic domain inde-
interview, but the TD group’s MLUs did not differ from pendent of, or in addition to, syntax; for example, higher
one another or from the DS group’s narrative MLU. That rates of response to questions or acknowledgments of
the two groups differed was not surprising, but that they clarifications can cause a reduction in MLU by increasing
differed only in one context was. The unexpected inter- the proportion of one-word or constituent phrase utter-
action between group and context was difficult to explain ances (Johnston et al., 1993).
without further analyses.
Adult discourse characteristics can affect the amount
We next report the series of post hoc exploratory and type of the child’s talk (Yoder, Davies, Bishop, &
analyses we used to determine the source of the differ- Munson, 1994) and the child’s overall length of utterance
ences. We suspected that the picture support in the (Chapman, 1981; Johnston, 2001; Klee, 1992). In lan-
narrative condition might be responsible for the high guage sampling with children with language disorders,
narrative MLU in the group with DS for two possible question asking, although a naturally occurring part of
reasons. The pictures may make apparent or highlight conversational exchange, must be taken into account to
visual detail of characters or events and relationships determine whether it differs from that of the control
between events, or pictures may free up cognitive or group (e.g., because of intelligibility difficulties) or from
information-processing resources to be allocated to the sampling procedure used to create a language sam-
expression. First, however, we evaluated other variables ple database (Johnston et al., 1993). In this study, we
to rule out their contribution. These variables were designed examiner scripts to minimize question asking
participant intelligibility and fluency and grammatical in both contexts; our interest in adult discourse charac-
errors and adult and participant discourse character- teristics was whether it differed for the two groups.
istics. Additional analyses evaluated the effects of alter-
A greater understanding of discourse characteris-
nate methods of calculating MLU that would reduce the
tics in individuals with DS might assist in the design of
influence of pragmatic variables and picture support in
the sampling procedures that could effectively reduce
the two language samples.
the influence of pragmatic variables and help determine
whether MLU would be more representative with yes/no
responses omitted. We considered pragmatic differences
Factors Affecting MLU from TD participants as potential contributors to the
MLU has been shown to be sensitive to the character- MLU differences we found (Chapman, 1981; Johnston,
istics of the sampling context, to the discourse character- 2001; Klee, 1992) and investigated using an alternate
istics of the conversational partner, and to the criteria analysis set, excluding yes/no question answers, for the
used to calculate it (Abbeduto et al., 1995; Chapman, calculation of MLU.
1981; Johnston, 2001; Stalnaker & Creaghead, 1982; Transcription reliability. In addition to discourse
Wagner, Nettelbladt, Sahlen, & Nilholm, 2000). The ef- characteristics and contexts, the accuracy and reliabil-
fects of variations in the sampling context or procedures ity of transcription have far-reaching consequences
may be different for children with cognitive or language (Johnson, 2000; Klee, 1992) for MLU and other mea-
impairment than for the TD children they are compared sures of spontaneous expressive language. Reliability,
with (see Johnston, Miller, Curtiss, & Tallal, 1993). in turn, may differ as a consequence of speaker char-
Discourse context. Sources of variation in the MLU acteristics such as intelligibility. Here we examined the
measure have been found in aspects of sampling and dis- reliability of our transcriptions in detail.
course contexts. Spontaneous play tends to elicit shorter Intelligibility. Individuals with DS are known to
utterances (Leadholm & Miller, 1992). Unlike conversa- have intelligibility problems, with some individuals
tional samples, narrative samples have shown MLU to be tending to be unintelligible throughout their lives (see

326 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
Table 1. Characteristics of participants (n = 14, each group).

DS TD

Characteristic M SD M SD

Chronological agea 16.36 2.14 4.06 0.75


Syntax comprehensionb 5.29 1.11 5.23 1.01
Maternal education levelc 15.21 2.26 16.93 1.49
Hearing statusd n=5 n = 13
Gender 6 female, 8 male 8 female, 6 male

Note. DS = Down syndrome; TD = typically developing.


a
Age is in years. bAge-equivalent score from the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language—Third
Edition; mean of Subtests II, Grammatical Morphemes, and III, Elaborated Sentences. cMother’s years of
education. dPassed hearing screening at 20 dB bilaterally at 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz. Minimum
criterion for participation: e40 dB for at least one ear.

the review by Stoel-Gammon, 1997). In addition to prob- of errors, occurred more frequently in one context and
lems with accurate articulation of specific sounds and contributed to MLU differences in this study. Never-
a greater range and variation of substitution types, chil- theless, it was difficult to predict which context might be
dren with DS display atypical prosodic patterns at the associated with higher omissions; the expression of more
suprasegmental level in phrasing, speech rate, and place- narrative content supported by pictures might lead to
ment of sentence stress. For some individuals with Down more omitted grammatical morphemes, but less support
syndrome, dysfluency is also a problem (Kumin, 1994) might lead to more omissions in the interview.
and could affect intelligibility. For this research note, we investigated the roles of
These intelligibility difficulties may affect the calcu- multiple factors as they affected MLU measures obtained
lation of MLU in a number of ways. First, when a word from interview and narrative contexts in adolescents with
is unintelligible, the probability of understanding other DS compared with TD children matched for syntax com-
words in the utterance decreases; longer utterances are prehension. We evaluated the effect of discourse context
thereby differentially excluded from the MLU calculation. (narrative vs. interview), examiner questions, participant
Intelligibility problems affect the utterances excluded responsiveness to questions, transcription reliability, par-
from the analysis set (the complete and intelligible set of ticipant intelligibility, and participant grammatical errors
utterances used to calculate MLU), reducing both the on the calculation of MLU. We evaluated the effect of
total number of utterances and the number of longer picture support by developing an alternate analysis set
utterances available for analysis. Second, in discourse asking whether it was narrative context alone or narra-
with the examiner, intelligibility problems may result tive context plus picture support that led to differential
in frequent adult questions to clarify or confirm content increases in MLU in the DS group.
of the child’s utterances, skewing the number of single-
word utterances in the analysis set. Third, the amount of
shared knowledge between child and examiner will affect Method
the number of communication breakdowns in the sample.
Of the two contexts examined in this study, the context
Participants
in which no shared knowledge was provided by pictures Twenty-eight children, adolescents, and young
would be expected to have a larger number of adult clar- adults participated in this study: 14 individuals with
ification requests, resulting in a disproportionately larger DS age 12 years, 10 months, to 21 years and 14 TD
number of shorter child utterances than the context with children, age 2 years, 11 months, to 5 years, 8 months (see
picture support. For these reasons, we compared intelli- Table 1 for participant characteristics). Originally, the
gibility rates in the two groups. study had 30 participants, but 1 of the TD participants
Grammatical errors. Adolescents with DS show a was unable to participate in one of the language-
greater number of bound morpheme omissions than TD sampling procedures used in this study and so was
children matched for MLU (Chapman et al., 1998). There excluded, as was her DS match.
may be contexts (e.g., narrative samples, picture-sup- All participants were part of a seven-session study of
ported samples) that support improved grammatical narrative development and word learning in adolescents
production in groups with language impairment. We and young adults with DS. The TD participants were
wanted to know if higher rates of omission, or other types recruited from the Madison, WI, area; the group with DS

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 327


was recruited from the state of Wisconsin. Potential elicitation. These participants were excluded. All families
participants were screened initially by telephone to were paid for their participation in the study.
determine that (a) individuals with DS were Trisomy
21 and had no other developmental disorder or history
of severe behavior, attentional problem, or psychiatric Language Samples
disorder; (b) TD children had no history of behavioral, Narrative Samples
attentional, or psychiatric disorder; and (c) oral language
Materials. The first author elicited two narratives
was the primary means of communication (no partic-
using the wordless picture stories, One Frog Too Many
ipants were augmentative and alternative communica-
(Mayer, 1975) and April Fools (Krahn, 1974). In One Frog
tion or sign language users); and (d) no participants were
Too Many, a boy’s pet frog is unhappy about the arrival
bilingual speakers.
of a new, smaller pet frog and attempts to get rid of it.
All participants met the following additional cri- The plot line of April Fools follows two boys’ adventures
teria for participation in the study, determined on the as they surprise and scare people with a dragon they
1st day of testing: (a) MLU was at least 2.0, calculated construct. The two stories were modified so they were
from the narrative language sample, elicited using equivalent in story structure, total number of pages,
wordless picture storybooks. This MLU level allowed story plot line (Berman, 1988), and number and length
us to capture the beginnings of the developmental of episodes.
course of narrative skills (Nelson, 1996) as well as the
Elicitation procedure. The narratives were counter-
range of expressive language skills in individuals with
balanced for order of presentation and for previewing
DS, both in the initial narrative samples and in the
condition. (The previewing conditions, i.e., one story was
longitudinal sessions. (b) Hearing loss, if present, was
previewed before telling and the other was not, were
not greater than a mild loss, as indicated by a bilateral
established for a separate study to evaluate the effect of
hearing screening at 20 dB, performed at 500, 1000,
previewing on narrative content and linguistic complexity.)
2000, and 4000 Hz. Threshold testing was conducted
for any frequency a participant did not respond to at During the narrative elicitation, the examiner pre-
20 dB. Participants proceeded with the language and sented each story picture, stated the page number, and
cognitive assessment protocol only if the screening re- waited for the participant’s spontaneous utterance about
vealed a hearing loss of 40 dB pure-tone average (500, the picture. Nonspecific prompts were provided to elicit
1000, 2000 Hz) or less. This criterion allowed inclu- story content only when the participant produced no
sion of a participant sample that is representative of spontaneous utterances about the picture (e.g., ‘‘What’s
the DS population (Brooks, Wooley, & Kanjilal, 1972; happening?’’ If this prompt was not effective, ‘‘Tell me
Dahle & McCollister, 1986). about the picture ’’ was used). The examiner corrected
mislabeling or incorrect interpretation of pictures. She
TD children were matched for syntax comprehension
repeated participants’ utterances for each page; the
on Subtests II and III of the Test for Auditory Compre-
repetitions were produced to aid transcription but were
hension of Language—Third Edition (TACL–3; Carrow-
incorporated into the adult–child interaction as inter-
Woolfolk, 1999; see Table 1). The entire TACL–3 was
ested encouragements (Peterson & McCabe, 1994) or as
administered (three subtests: I—Vocabulary, II—Gram-
statements with rising intonation that sought confir-
matical Morphemes, and III—Elaborated Sentences and
mation of the child’s story content. No time limit was
Phrases). However, the score used for matching was
imposed on narration; total time elapsed in producing
based only on the mean of the age-equivalent scores from
narratives ranged between 3 and 13 min.
Subtests II and III. Subtest I, composed of single-word
items, was not used in the calculation of syntax compre-
hension for matching purposes. This decision was made Interview Language Sample
because of the concern that syntax rather than recep- An interview format was used to elicit spontaneous
tive vocabulary be matched. The syntax comprehension language samples (adapted from Evans & Craig, 1992) by
match group was developed for the study as a whole, in introducing topics of personal experience. This format
which we expected different levels of expressive syntax in was preferable to sampling in free-play interactions
the two groups in the narratives (Chapman et al., 1998) because it was more appropriate for adolescents and
but similar levels of narrative content (i.e., plot line and because free-play samples tend to elicit shorter MLUs
theme; Miles & Chapman, 2002). (Leadholm & Miller, 1992). Depending on an individual
Three individuals with Down syndrome had chal- participant’s skill level, interview topics permit narration
lenging behaviors that precluded completing individual or extended discourse of varying elaboration.
tasks or the full protocol. Three of the younger TD Elicitation procedure. The interview was timed to
children refused to participate during the narrative last approximately 10 min, allotting about 2 to 3 min per

328 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
topic. No props or pictures were used to elicit this sample. memory, phonological memory, visual problem solving,
Participants were informed that they would be talking to fast mapping), with counterbalancing, selected for other
the examiner, the first author, who wanted to get to know aspects of the larger study. Because the protocol for
more about them. The examiner introduced a sequence of the initial session was both long and challenging, it was
four topics: family, school (preschool or other regularly broken into two sessions for the youngest TD partici-
attended activities were substituted as necessary), activi- pants (n = 2), taking into consideration developmental
ties done outside of school (or its substitute), and favorite limitations in attention span and stamina. The possi-
movies, videos, or TV shows. Topics were introduced in ble confound favoring these individuals’ performance
the same order for each participant and were initiated was preferable to the loss of data or the participants’
with the phrases ‘‘ Let’s talk aboutI ’’ or ‘‘ Tell me discomfort.
aboutI ’’ Examiner prompts were formulated similarly
(e.g., ‘‘That’s great; tell me more about that’’; ‘‘Tell me
about a movie you saw’’). Topic-extending prompts were
Transcription
provided to extend the participant’s own content within The narratives and interviews were transcribed
the adult-initiated topic (‘‘Tell me more about ___’’). More and entered into the Systematic Analysis of Lan-
specific prompts (e.g., ‘‘Tell me about who lives with you’’ guage Transcripts; SALT; Miller & Chapman, 2003).
or ‘‘Tell me about your mom’’) within the more general Transcribers, who were undergraduate and graduate
topic (e.g., family) were used when a participant showed students in communicative disorders, typed the tran-
difficulty in getting started on the topic. The examiner scripts from audiotapes, occasionally from video when
repeated participants’ utterances to aid transcription mechanical failure occurred in audiotaping. Tapes were
only when they were particularly difficult to understand. replayed on Sanyo Memo-scriber TRC-8080 transcrip-
Although this made the two contexts slightly different in tion machines with foot pedal and speed control, us-
this respect, this decision was made because it was not ing Radio Shack headphones. Transcribers had access
possible to incorporate repetition of all utterances into to the SALT manual (Miller, 1997), a list of SALT
the interaction without being disruptive. A pause time spelling conventions, the decision rules for intelligi-
was imposed on the first utterance of two topics to inves- bility and segmentation, and the picture storybooks
tigate, in a separate study, its effect on productive lan- used in the narrative language samples. They listened
guage (Ellis Weismer & Schraeder, 1992; Evans, Viele, & to an utterance up to three times, using speed control
Kass, 1997). The pause procedure was presented to the as necessary.
participant as a brief time to think about what she or he SALT conventions for segmentation are based on
wanted to say. Pause times were counterbalanced across capturing thought completion. Two factors are used to
the topics. determine the completion of a thought, intonational
Audiotapes of the narratives and interviews were contour, and the presence of a pause. However, children
made on a Marantz portable cassette recorder PMD222, may produce more than two independent clauses joined
using a Crown PZM-185 microphone located on the by conjunctions without pausing or changing intonation
table between the participant and the experimenter. contour. When this occurs, utterances are segmented af-
Videotapes were made with a Panasonic WV-3260 video- ter the second conjoined clause so that run-on clauses
camera, located in a corner of the room, with a Crown do not unduly inflate the MLU measure. For example, an
PZM-185 microphone located on the table between par- utterance without pauses, such as ‘‘And then the frog
ticipant and experimenter. Videotapes were recorded jumped and he fell on the boat/and the boat is sinking’’
and replayed on a Panasonic videocassette recorder would be segmented at the slash mark preceding the
AG-1970. third independent clause. (All SALT transcription con-
ventions were based on Brown, 1973, with the exception
of the segmentation convention; also see Leadholm &
Procedure Miller, 1992.)
The language samples were obtained on the initial Reliability for segmentation and morpheme tran-
day of testing, the first of seven sessions, which lasted scription. All of the transcripts were submitted to
approximately 3 hr. Each participant was given a break reliability checks of segmentation and morpheme tran-
approximately midway through each session; other scription (100% of the data) to assure quality control
breaks were inserted as needed. Most participants across numerous transcribers and throughout the dura-
separated from parents; a parent was present in the tion of data collection (3 years). For the narratives, mean
testing room, as needed by some of the younger partic- percentage of agreement in morpheme transcription was
ipants. The initial session included additional language 98% for the group with DS and 99% for the TD group;
and cognitive measures and experimental tasks (non- agreement on segmentation and maze identification was
verbal cognition, auditory and visual memory, working 98% and 99% respectively. For the interviews, mean

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 329


percentage of agreement in morpheme transcription and the TD group’s MLU–Narrative. The TD children’s
segmentation was 99% for both groups. MLUs did not differ in the two contexts.

Scoring for the Initial Analysis


MLU was computed from complete and intelligible
Discussion
(C & I) utterances in the SALT program. To compare The finding of an interaction between group and
MLUs for the two groups in the two contexts, we context led us to formulate hypotheses about the possible
computed narrative and interview MLUs, as follows. source of this difference and to perform a series of
The analysis set for MLU–Narrative–All consisted of exploratory analyses to identify it. We speculated that
the full set of participant utterances in the two the picture support provided in the narrative task’s
narrative transcripts, which included yes/no responses wordless storybooks might have aided the group with
to adult questions and to adult utterances with rising DS in producing longer utterances expressing complex
intonation and off-task conversation about topics other relations shown in the pictures (Abbeduto et al., 1995).
than the narrative or storytelling procedure. MLU– However, the source of the MLU differences might be
Interview–All was calculated in the SALT program found in any one of a number of other factors, described in
from all participant utterances in the interview tran- the introduction, or might be the result of a combination
script, which included yes/no responses to adult ques- of factors. In the first phase of our follow-up analyses,
tions and to adult utterances with rising intonation (a) we ruled out interactions between group and context
and participant-initiated topics that differed from the in transcription reliability and participant intelligibility/
four initiated by the examiner. fluency; (b) taking into consideration the characteristic
expressive grammatical deficits in conversational lan-
guage samples of individuals with DS, we asked whether
Results the narrative context contributed to a higher MLU by
reducing errors for that group; and (c) we examined adult
MLUs in the Two Groups and Contexts
and participant discourse characteristics to determine
This analysis was conducted to determine the whether they differed by group or context, thus differ-
effects of group and context on MLU. MLUs were entially affecting the length of participant utterances.
analyzed in a mixed-design two-way 2 (Group)  2
(Context) analysis of variance (ANOVA), with repeated
Follow-Up Analysis 1: Measures of
measures on the second factor, narrative versus inter-
view context. A Type I error rate of .05 was used for Factors Affecting MLU
statistical tests. In addition to transcription reliability, the following
The ANOVA calculated for MLU–All (all C & I variables were computed, using the SALT program, for
utterances; see Table 2 for means and standard devia- participants’ C & I utterances, unless noted. These vari-
tions) revealed a main effect of context, F(1, 26) = 10.11, ables include measures of the participants’ intelligibility/
p G .01, hp2 = .28, and a significant Group  Context fluency and grammatical errors and adult discourse char-
interaction, F(1, 26) = 5.14, p = .03, hp2 = .17. There was acteristics (the extent to which the experimenter asked
no significant effect of group. For the group with DS, questions) and participant discourse characteristics (re-
MLU–Narrative was significantly higher than the sponsiveness to questions), to be considered as contribu-
MLU–Interview and did not differ significantly from tors to MLU differences.
Intelligibility and fluency. To evaluate possible effects
Table 2. MLU and number of utterances, both contexts: All C & I of intelligibility and fluency on the calculation of MLU, we
utterances. computed percentage intelligibility and percentage mazed
words. Percentage intelligible utterances was the percent-
DS TD age of the participant’s total utterances that were complete
and intelligible. Percentage mazed words was the percent-
Context Measure M SD M SD age of the participant’s total words that were in mazes.
Narrative MLU 5.69 1.89 6.19 1.79 This proportional measure of fluency was chosen over
Number of utterancesa 68.2 33.0 48.1 22.7 others available in the SALT program because it controls
Interview MLU 4.38 1.56 5.98 1.58 for the number of opportunities to maze at the word level.
Number of utterances 128.4 34.4 88.5 18.1 Grammatical errors. To evaluate the effect of gram-
matical errors on MLU in the two contexts and groups,
Note. MLU = mean length of utterance; C & I = complete and intelligible.
a we computed percentage omitted bound morphemes,
Mean number of utterances per narrative.
percentage omitted words, percentage errors at the word

330 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
level, and percentage errors at the utterance level. program computes the number of answers to wh- and
Percentage omitted bound morphemes was of interest yes/no questions, including their variants, such as yeah,
because in a previous study, adolescents with DS showed um-hum, and uh-uh.
a greater number of bound morpheme omissions than
TD children matched for MLU (Chapman et al., 1998). Results
We wanted to know if higher rates of omission in one
The dependent variables that might affect the
context contributed to MLU differences in this study.
calculation of MLU were each analyzed in a mixed-design
This variable was calculated by dividing the number of
2 (group)  2 (context) ANOVA, with repeated measures
omitted bound morphemes by the total of omitted and
on the second factor of narrative versus interview con-
produced bound morphemes. Percentage omitted words
text. A Type I error rate of .05 was used for all statistical
was calculated by dividing the total number of omitted
tests. All calculations were on C & I utterances except
words by the sum of omitted words and words produced.
for the calculation of percentage intelligibility, propor-
Total omitted words was the total word tokens omitted
tions of examiner questions, and percentage participant
in the main body of the utterance; words with omitted
response to questions. Arcsine transformations were per-
bound morphemes and those within mazes were not
formed on proportional data.
counted as omitted words. Percentage of errors at the
word level was calculated by dividing the number of Transcription reliability. We analyzed transcription
words coded as errors by the total number of words. reliabilities to be certain that reliabilities were not poorer
Errors included overgeneralization errors (e.g., goed/ for the narrative transcripts in the group with DS. The
went), pronoun errors (e.g., him/he), and semantic errors ANOVAs for morpheme transcription reliability and
(He does/causes lots of trouble). Percentage of errors at segmentation reliability revealed no significant main
the utterance level was calculated by dividing the effects or interaction.
number of errors by the total number of utterances. This Intelligibility. The ANOVA calculated for percentage
type of error occurred when there were errors in word intelligibility revealed no significant main effects or inter-
order, when several words were omitted, or when the actions. Intelligibility was high for both groups (96%–98%),
utterance did not make sense and it was not possible to and the groups were not significantly different. This was
determine which words were in error. surprising given the phonological characteristics of the
Experimenter discourse characteristics. To evaluate population with DS in general (Stoel-Gammon, 1997) and
this variable, we computed the proportion of experimenter the characteristics of this particular cohort. Two factors
questions and the proportion of experimenter requests for may have contributed to the high intelligibility found
information as a function of the number of participant in this study. First, the content of the two sampling con-
utterances. Experimenter questions were defined by texts was constrained, the narrative by the wordless
SALT as utterances ending with a question mark. These picture storybooks and the interview by a set number of
included all experimenter utterances with rising intona- topics presented in the same order for all participants.
tion: requests for clarification, confirmation and repeti- Transcribers were thus able to anticipate, to some ex-
tion, tag questions and other forms of yes/no questions, tent, the content of the participants’ utterances in both
and wh-questions. Proportion of experimenter questions contexts. Second, the transcription practice established
was calculated by dividing the total number of experi- in our lab allowed transcribers to listen to an utterance
menter questions by the total number of participant three times, and the examiner aided transcription by
utterances. Experimenter requests for information were glossing all narrative utterances and by glossing difficult
coded as a subset of experimenter questions; these were utterances in the interview.
defined as wh-questions asking for new information, not Fluency. The ANOVA calculated for percentage of
to confirm or clarify information already stated. Experi- maze words revealed a significant main effect of context,
menter scripts were designed to eliminate or reduce this F(1, 26) = 7.85, p G .01, hp2 = .23, but no significant
type of question in the sampling procedure; we expected interaction between context and group. There was not a
to find few of these. Proportion of experimenter requests significant main effect of group. Both groups produced a
for information was calculated by dividing the total slightly higher percentage of mazed words in the inter-
number of requests by the total number of participant view context (TD: 11% interview, 9% narrative; DS: 9%,
utterances. 6%, respectively). The picture support of the narrative
Participant discourse characteristics. For this vari- context may have given both groups a slight edge in
able, we computed the percentage of response to exam- fluency by reducing demands on the formulation of
iner questions. This measure was calculated by utterance content.
comparing the number of responses with the number In summary, our transcription procedures allowed
of opportunities to respond. It is a measurement of the us to capture a considerable proportion of both groups’
participant’s responsiveness to questions. The SALT language for further analysis and resulted in similarly

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 331


high intelligibility rates, and low mazing rates, for both than single-word yes/no responses, would not have as
groups and contexts. Differences in MLU cannot be great an impact on MLU. In these samples, the most
attributed to differing rates of intelligibility or fluency common type of response to adult questions was single-
or to differing transcription practices or reliability. word yes/no responses; however if one group answered
Grammatical errors. The ANOVA calculated for the more questions than the other, or in one context over
arcsine transformation of the percentage omitted bound another, MLU would be disproportionately affected. Our
morphemes revealed no interactions or main effect of last variable was examined for this possibility.
context, but the main effect of group was significant, Participant discourse characteristics. The ANOVA
F(1, 26) = 5.47, p = .03, hp2 = .17. The group with DS calculated for percentage response to examiner questions
omitted a greater percentage of bound morphemes than revealed a significant main effect of group, F(1, 26) = 1.26,
the TD group in both contexts. While the TD group omit- p G .01, hp2 = .28, and a significant interaction between
ted 6% in each context, the group with DS omitted group and context, F(1, 26) = 5.47, p = .03, hp2 = .17. There
slightly more in the narrative context (18%) than in the was no main effect of context. Percentage response to
interview context (12%) although this difference was not experimenter questions reveals the number of times the
statistically significant. Despite the greater bound mor- participant responded to questions relative to the num-
pheme omission in the narratives, the group with DS had ber of opportunities to do so. The group with DS
a higher MLU in the narratives than in the interview. responded more frequently than the TD group in both
The current cohort with DS shows a deficit in the pro- contexts (DS group: 82% narrative, 77% interview; TD
duction of bound grammatical morphemes consistent group: 53%, 63%, respectively), even though no signifi-
with that reported in Chapman et al. (1998); the error cant differences were found in the amount of experi-
rates, however, do not explain the MLU difference we menter questioning for the two groups and contexts (see
found here. To achieve an MLU not significantly differ- above). The group with DS responded to examiner
ent from the TD children who make fewer errors, the questions more frequently in the narrative context than
group with DS may be adding greater length by pro- in the interview context, whereas the TD group
ducing more content words. responded to questions less frequently in the narrative
context than the interview context. This result, the
We also computed three other error types: percent-
interaction between group and context, would lead to a
age omitted words (where omissions were not optional, as
prediction that the DS group’s MLU would be lower than
in ellipsis), percentage errors at the word level (incorrect
that of the TD group in the narrative, the opposite of the
use of a word), and errors at the utterance level (typically,
results of our first analysis.
this type of error meant that the utterance was incompre-
hensible). In absolute numbers, the group with DS had This finding indicated a higher responsiveness to
adult questions in the DS group. It did not, however,
more errors (2%–3% for the group with DS; 1%–2% for
explain the DS group’s narrative MLU, although it did
the TD group), but for both groups, these error types were
indicate that the group would have a relatively higher
so infrequent overall that subsequent analyses were not
proportion of single-word acknowledgments and back-
undertaken. The small percentage of these errors in both
channel responses in the transcripts, reducing their
contexts and both groups rules them out as sources of
MLUs disproportionately more than those of the compar-
difference in the DS group’s MLUs in the two contexts.
ison group.
Adult discourse characteristics. The mean proportion
Summary. When we examined variables with a
of experimenter questions ranged between .26 and .28
possible effect on MLU, none of the analyses appeared
for the group with DS and between .28 and .35 for the
to explain the different MLUs for the group with DS.
TD group. The ANOVA calculated for the arcsine trans-
However there were two interesting findings. (a) The
formation of this variable revealed no significant main
group with DS had a higher MLU only in the narrative
effects or interaction.
sample despite a higher frequency of omitted bound
The proportion of experimenter questions that were morphemes than the TD group in both contexts. (b) The
true requests for information was minimal for both groups group with DS showed a higher percentage of response to
and contexts. The ANOVA calculated for this variable questions, suggesting a greater degree of skill in this
revealed no significant main effects or interaction. We aspect of discourse than the control group and the
expected this type of question to be rare, and it was (.004 likelihood of more frequent single-word utterances affect-
to .01 for the group with DS; .01–.02 for the TD group). ing MLU. The second finding was a reason to consider an
This type of question was a minute proportion of the alternate calculation of MLU. The use of an alternate
number of questions overall, indicating that the type of calculation of MLU has been suggested when there
examiner question that predominated was yes/no ques- is a high rate of adult questioning (Chapman, 1981;
tions. Requests for information, even if frequent, would Johnston, 2001). In this study, we found that our rates
be likely to elicit constituent phrases and, being longer of questioning did not differ between the groups, but

332 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
differences in participant discourse characteristics pro- Results and Discussion
duced a similar result: a higher proportion of single-word
For the group with DS, the mean MLU–Interview–
utterances in one group. On the basis of this finding, we
Subset was 5.35 (SD = 1.51), and the mean MLU–
recalculated MLU omitting yes/no responses.
Narrative–Subset was 7.01 (SD = 2.26). The TD children’s
mean MLU–Interview–Subset was 6.64 (SD = 1.64) and
Follow-Up Analysis 2: Alternate Means their MLU–Narrative–Subset was 6.73 (SD = 1.90). The
ANOVA calculated for MLU–Subsets revealed a main
of MLU Calculation effect of context, F(1, 26) = 12.37, p = .00, hp2 = .32, and a
For this analysis, in addition to eliminating the effect significant interaction between group and context, F(1,
of one group’s greater rate of answering questions, we 26) = 10.04, p = .00, hp2 = .28. There was no significant
wanted to address our initial hypothesis, that the picture effect of group.
support of the narrative condition was a factor in the For the group with DS, MLU–Interview–Subset
higher MLU for the DS group. We created new analysis was significantly lower than MLU–Narrative–Subset.
sets for alternate MLU calculations, by excluding yes/no The difference between MLU–Narrative and MLU–
responses in both contexts and by omitting any utter- Interview was greater when the analysis set was the
ances from the narratives that were not supported by the subset of all C & I utterances omitting yes/no utterances
picture storybook. This created a narrative analysis set of in both contexts and using relevant-only utterances in
strictly picture-supported utterances to compare with the the narrative calculation. The TD children’s MLUs did
interview. Thus, we were able to compare a narrative- not differ in the two contexts, using either method of
relevant MLU scaffolded with pictures to an interview calculation. Thus, for the group with DS, the higher
MLU without such support. narrative MLU continued to appear using this method of
calculation, and the gap between receptive and expres-
Creation of New Analysis Sets From sive language again showed only in the interview lan-
Subsets of the Interview and Narrative guage sample. The gap between receptive and expressive
language was greater when we used the alternate means
MLU–Narrative–Subset (narrative-relevant utter-
of calculating MLU; the difference between the two DS
ances only; yes/no responses excluded). The analysis set
MLUs was also greater.
for this calculation was a subset of all participant utter-
ances, consisting of story-relevant utterances, that is, These results however led to another and final
participant utterances referring to the story as shown in question. Was the increased MLU related to narrative
the picture book, excluding off-task remarks, remarks content (e.g., complex relations among events) alone or to
about the act of story telling, or conversation about topics the picture support of complex relations shown in the
other than the narrative. (Nonrelevant utterances con- storybook? To answer this, we had to compare narrative
sisted of 8% of the transcripts for the group with DS and sample MLUs to MLUs derived from interview utter-
13% for the TD group. The difference was not statistically ances identified as narratives.
significant.) Utterances in the narrative transcripts were
coded for story relevance; a reliability check for this Follow-Up Analysis 3: Comparison
scoring was performed on 10% of the transcripts and was of Narratives With and Without
95%. Disagreements were discussed, and changes were
made by the original coder when necessary. This MLU
Picture Support
calculation excluded yes/no responses to adult questions To evaluate the effect of picture support in narrative
and to adult utterances with rising intonation and yes/no alone, we calculated an MLU–Narrative within the in-
utterances acknowledging examiner statements. terview (receiving no picture support), to contrast with
MLU–Interview–Subset (yes/no responses excluded). the MLU–Narrative–Subset calculated from picture-
The analysis set for this calculation was a subset of all supported fictional narratives.
participant utterances, excluding yes/no responses to Narrative utterances in the interview. For both
adult questions or rising intonation and yes/no utter- groups, we found four types of discourse in the interview
ances acknowledging examiner statements. Unlike the transcripts for both groups: lists of family members and
narrative context, all utterances were considered rele- school classes/activities, descriptions of people and
vant in the interview because the main focus there was events, explanations of various situations, and narra-
the interests and activities of the participants, so no tives. For each participant’s interview, we identified the
utterances were excluded as off-topic. Examination of a utterances expressing narratives within the interview.
random sample of 10% of the transcripts showed that We established minimal criteria for both fictional and
only 6% of C & I interview utterances were off the topics personal experience narratives that were developmen-
introduced by the examiner. tally appropriate for the lowest age range of TD

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 333


participants and for the least experienced narrators in MLU was calculated for these (interview) narrative
the group with DS. We defined narratives of past event or utterances; 5.90 for the group with DS, 7.64 for the TD
personal experience as a reference to a past experience, or group. Figure 1 compares MLUs for picture book narra-
an anticipated future event, containing a minimum of two tives and narratives in interview. A paired-samples t test
nonroutine actions or events (modified after Peterson & was used to compare the MLUs for each group. For the
McCabe, 1994, 1983, and Labov & Waletsky, 1967/1997). DS group only, the difference between the MLU–Narra-
This definition allowed narratives of any length, includ- tive and the MLU–Narrative in Interview was statisti-
ing single-utterance narratives (e.g., ‘‘I threw up at cally significant. Narrative increased MLU in both
school so my dad got me home’’); however, there was contexts, but MLU was the highest in the language
only one instance of a single-utterance narrative. Fic- samples that were narratives supported by the picture
tional narratives were scored as any attempt to tell storybook. For the TD group, differences between the two
content recalled from TV shows and movies. Utterances narrative MLUs were not statistically significant.
were scored as fictional narrative if they included story
action or event or a listing or description of characters.
Reliability for identifying narratives within the inter- Summary and Conclusions
view. Reliability was calculated on 20% of the interview
The group with DS had a higher MLU in the
transcripts because the proportion of narrative utter-
narrative context than the interview, and the TD group
ances was relatively small. The mean MLU for the
showed no difference. Our analyses ruled out tran-
narratives in the six interview transcripts used in the
scription reliability, participant intelligibility, and
reliability check was 7.84 and differed only .04 of a
experimenter discourse characteristics as the source of
morpheme from the checker’s MLU. Agreement on the
this interaction. The DS group did differ from the TD
number of narrative utterances was 81%; agreement on
group in higher grammatical morpheme error rates, but
the number of utterances identified as nonnarrative was
this did not explain the MLU interaction. The DS group
95%. These percentages are not the same because agree-
showed a greater responsiveness to adult yes/no ques-
ment was on the total number of utterances, not point-to-
tions, elevating disproportionately the number of single-
point agreement on individual utterances.
word utterances in their samples and justifying the use of
an alternate calculation of MLU. The greater response to
Results questions may show greater sensitivity to the needs of the
In the group with DS, 93% of the participants (13/14) listener than the TD group, or it may reflect more
produced at least one narrative during the interview. The experience in repairing communicative breakdown due
range for the group was 1–6 narratives. All TD partic- to intelligibility difficulties. Experimenter questions
ipants produced at least one narrative, the range being were predominately requests for clarification and con-
1–8 narratives. For the group with DS, 19% of the inter- firmation of the participant’s production.
view utterances were in narratives (M = 26.3 utterances); The higher narrative MLU in the group with DS was
for the TD group, 18% (M = 17.3 utterances). A separate indeed partially attributable to narrative, rather than
interview, context, but as we had hypothesized, an even
Figure 1. Mean lengths of utterance in narrative and narratives in greater increase in MLU was seen with picture support of
interview. DS = Down syndrome; TD = typically developing. the narrative. We believe that the pictures in the
wordless storybooks were a form of scaffolding for the
group with DS that allowed access to and expression of
knowledge that they have but would not express inde-
pendently. This resulted, for the DS group alone, in a
statistically significant increase in MLU over the sam-
pling context without pictures.1
MLU’s sensitivity to context and to the discourse
characteristics of conversational participants is not new

1
In a prior study, by contrast, adolescents with DS (Chapman et al., 1998)
showed a greater expressive language deficit relative to syntactic
comprehension when MLU was obtained from narrative samples. We believe
the differences in the two studies can be traced to differences in sampling
procedure. In Chapman et al. (1998), the sample was elicited using a frog story
(Mayer, 1969) and mixed sources from other topics, with and without picture
support, to create a standard length in minutes. In the current study,
two wordless storybooks were used; the resulting MLUs were averaged,
regardless of sample length.

334 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
in the literature. Because of this sensitivity, the MLU assume that no other characteristics of expressive
measure’s reliability and validity have been examined in language differ.
a variety of studies. Measures of children’s language and A limitation of our study is that the narrative
cognitive abilities are generally recognized to be task sampling contexts we compared differed in number of
sensitive (e.g., Berman’s, 1995, review). However, the utterances as well as picture support. Future research
idea that features of the task may affect a language could alter the interview procedure, for example, by using
measure such as MLU differentially in a clinical pop- explicit instructions, ‘‘Tell me about a time youI ’’
ulation and the comparison group has only recently been (Hudson, Gebelt, Haviland, & Bentivegna, 1992), or by
explored (e.g., Johnston, 2001; Miolo, Chapman, & using Peterson and McCabe’s (1983) method of providing
Sindberg, 2005). Interactionist models of language devel- a model of the desired type of experience and asking, ‘‘Did
opment (e.g., Bock, 1982; Chapman et al., 1992; Elman anything like that happen to you?’’ Multiple narratives
et al., 1996; Snow & Pan, 1993) propose that multiple can be elicited using these or similar procedures, permit-
domains contribute to the language that gets produced at ting equal numbers of narrative utterances to be obtained.
any one time and that the domains drawn on may vary
However, the interview narratives and wordless
depending on the context. These models would predict
picture book narratives may not provide equivalent
that MLU would vary with sources contributing to
opportunities for complex syntax. We observed that our
language production, including variations in sampling
participants reported differing types of personal experi-
procedures. Contextual support may affect individuals
ence, varying in the types of relations expressed (e.g.,
with deficits according to their differing strengths in
some only temporal, others with complex causal relation-
linguistic, cognitive, and social domains. Contextual
ships, some not explicitly expressed with conjunctions). A
variations can have different effects on different popula-
study in which participants have common experiences to
tions, playing a greater role in one type of group than
report (experienced in experimental conditions), with
another. In the current study, our data show that
opportunity for the expression of varied relationships
sampling context had an influence on the group with
among events and internal states, would be desirable.
DS that did not affect the TD group.
Creating two contexts that are equivalent in complexity
is a research challenge yet to be solved.
Implications for Future Research
Decisions about what sampling context to use will
affect both the measure of expressive language and the
Clinical Implications
creation of a TD language comparison group matched Narrative, rather than conversational, samples for
for expressive language. A sampling context eliciting a expressive language assessment (cf. Abbeduto et al., 1995)
higher MLU, such as the picture-supported narratives for individuals with DS make it possible to capture, and
used here, will match the DS group to TD children determine the extent of, an individual’s skill in the use
with higher language skill and be a more conservative of longer utterances to express complex relations among
match. A context eliciting a lower MLU, such as play events. Other measures of language characteristics, such
or conversation, will result in a match to TD children as omitted bound morphemes, will complete the clinical
functioning at a lower developmental level. Generaliza- picture of an individual’s grammatical skill. Computer-
tions can be made to other individuals in the popula- ized language analysis programs are excellent tools for
tion from the data, but with a cautious eye toward the clinicians in that respect, to save time in counts of specific
nature of the sampling context and its effects on out- grammatical elements and to hone the analysis for any
comes in the language measure and construction of the individual client.
control group. This study highlights the importance of careful
Investigators must also choose language measures. observation of both adult and child behavior in clinical
MLU does not capture all features of children’s expres- interactions and their bidirectional effects. Intelligibility,
sive language. In Chapman et al. (1998) and the current for example, is a child discourse characteristic that has an
study, the group with DS shows a specific deficit in the effect on adult behavior. Where there are intelligibility
omission of bound morphemes, a rate of omission greater problems, it is difficult to avoid questioning the child to
than the control group matched for MLU and than the confirm or clarify an understanding of the child’s utter-
group matched on the TACL–3 here. Language sampling ances. Questioning in turn may influence the length of
done in a context eliciting longer utterances, as we see utterances, creating shorter or longer utterances than
here for the DS group in the picture-supported context, the child’s typical production. Depending on the amount
cannot necessarily be interpreted as a reduction of and type of adult questioning, and how responsive the
grammatical errors. When MLU does not statistically child is, the clinician may want to consider an alternate
differ from that of the control group, it is not safe to calculation of MLU, omitting question responses. As in

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 335


research, sampling context decisions in clinical practice deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
will affect the MLU outcome. Research, 41, 861–885.

In intervention with adolescents with DS, we believe Chapman, R. S., Streim, N. W., Crais, E. R., Salmon, D.,
Strand, E. A., & Negri, N. A. (1992). Child talk: Assump-
our results indicate that the use of pictures will support tions of a developmental model for early language learning.
access to knowledge not expressed without such support In R. S. Chapman (Ed.), Processes in language acquisition
and hence practice of more advanced syntax. Pictures sup- and disorders (pp. 3–19). Chicago: Mosby/Yearbook.
porting narrative production may reduce the demands on Dahle, A. J., & McCollister, F. P. (1986). Hearing and
resources for sentence formulation and memory for story otologic disorders in children with Down syndrome.
events and may aid individuals in operating at the outer American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 79, 636–642.
bound of their zone of proximal development. Given prac- Ellis Weismer, S., & Schraeder, T. (1992). Discourse
tice at this level, the higher level of expression provided characteristics and verbal reasoning: Wait time effects on
by such support may become established in the repertoire the performance of children with language learning dis-
abilities. Exceptionality Education Canada, 3, 71–92.
of individuals with DS (Vygotsky, 1978).
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-
Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking
Acknowledgments innateness: A connectionist perspective on development.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
This research was supported by National Institutes of Evans, J. L., & Craig, H. K. (1992). Language sample
Health Grant R01 HD23353 to Robin Chapman, with additional collection and analysis: Interview compared to free play
funding from the National Down Syndrome Society. We thank assessment contexts. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Cynthia Renk for her diligent work in transcription and Research, 35, 343–353.
reliability. Evans, J., Viele, K., & Kass, R. (1997). Response latency
and verbal complexity: Stochastic models of individual
differences in children with specific language impairments.
References Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40,
754–764.
Abbeduto, L., Benson, G., Short, K., & Dolish, J. (1995). Fowler, A. (1995). Linguistic variability in persons with
Effect of sampling context on the expressive language of Down syndrome: Research and implications. In L. Nadel, &
children and adolescents with mental retardation. Mental D. Rosenthal (Eds.), Down syndrome: Living and learning
Retardation, 33, 279–288. in the community (pp. 121–131). New York: Wiley-Liss.
Berman, R. A. (1988). On the ability to relate events in Hudson, J., Gebelt, J., Haviland, J., & Bentivegna, C.
narrative. Discourse Process, 11, 469–497. (1992). Emotion and narrative structure in young child-
Berman, R. A. (1995). Narrative competence and storytell- ren’s personal accounts. Journal of Narrative and Life
ing performance: How children tell stories in different History, 2, 129–150.
contexts. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 5, 283–313. Johnson, C. E. (2000). What you see is what you get: The
Bock, J. K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: importance of transcription for interpreting children’s
Information processing contributions to sentence formula- morphosyntactic development. In L. Menn & N. Bernstein
tion. Psychological Review, 89, 1–47. Ratner (Eds.), Methods for studying language production
Brooks, D. N., Wooley, H., & Kanjilal, G. C. (1972). (pp. 181–204). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hearing loss and middle ear disorders in patients with Johnston, J. R. (2001). An alternate MLU calculation:
Down’s syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, Magnitude and variability of effects. Journal of Speech,
16, 21–28. Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 156–164.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Johnston, J. R., Miller, J. F., Curtiss, S., & Tallal, P.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (1993). Conversations with children who are language
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Test for Auditory Comprehen- impaired: Asking questions. Journal of Speech and Hearing
sion of Language, Third Edition (TACL–3). Austin, TX: Research, 36, 973–978.
Pro-Ed. Klee, T. (1992). Measuring children’s conversational lan-
Chapman, R. S. (1981). Computing mean length of utter- guage. In S. F. Warren, & J. E. Reichle (Eds.), Communi-
ance in morphemes. In J. Miller (Ed.), Assessing language cation and language intervention: Vol. 1. Causes and
production in children (pp. 22–25). Baltimore: University effects in communication and language intervention
Park Press. (pp. 315–330). Baltimore: Brookes.

Chapman, R. S., & Hesketh, L. J. (2000). Behavioral Krahn, F. (1974). April fools. New York: Dutton.
phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Mental Kumin, L. (1994). Intelligibility of speech in children with
Retardation and Developmental Disability Research Down syndrome in natural settings: Parents’ perspective.
Reviews, 6, 84–95. Perception and Motor Skills, 78, 307–316.
Chapman, R., Seung, H.-K., Schwartz, S., & Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1997). Narrative analysis: Oral
Kay-Raining Bird, E. (1998). Language skills of children versions of personal experience. Journal of Narrative and
and adolescents with Down syndrome: II. Production Life History, 7, 3–38. (Reprinted from Essays on the Verbal

336 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research  Vol. 49  325–337  April 2006
and Visual Arts: Proceedings of the 1966 Annual Spring Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist
Meeting of the American Ethnological Society, pp. 12–44, account of developing decontextualized narrative skill.
by J. Helm, Ed., 1967, Seattle: University of Washington Developmental Psychology, 30, 937–948.
Press.) Snow, C. E., & Pan, B. A. (1993). Ways of analyzing the
Leadholm, B., & Miller, J. (1992). Language sample spontaneous speech of children with mental retardation:
analysis: The Wisconsin guide. Madison: Wisconsin The value of cross-domain analyses. International Review
Department of Public Instruction. of Research in Mental Retardation, 19, 163–192.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial. Stalnaker, L. D., & Creaghead, N. A. (1982). An exami-
Mayer, M. (1975). One frog too many. New York: Dial. nation of language samples obtained under three exper-
imental conditions. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Miles, S., & Chapman, R. S. (2002). Narrative content as Services in Schools, 13, 121–128.
described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically
developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Stoel-Gammon, C. (1997). Phonological development in
Hearing Research, 45, 175–189. Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Development
Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 300–306.
Miller, J. F. (1997). SALT database programs. Madison:
Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, Univer- Thordardottir, E., Chapman, R., & Wagner, L. (2002).
sity of Wisconsin. Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down
syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 163–183.
Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S. (2003). Systematic Analysis
of Language Transcripts [Computer software]. Madison: Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of
University of Wisconsin—Madison, Waisman Center, higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner,
Language Analysis Laboratory. S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Miller, J. F., Freiberg, C., Rolland, M., & Reeves, M. A.
(1992). Implementing computerized language sample Wagner, C. R., Nettlebladt, U., Sahlen, B., & Nilholm, C.
analysis in the public school. Topics in Language Disorders, (2000). Conversation versus narration in pre-school chil-
12, 69–82. dren with language impairment. International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 83–93.
Miller, J. F., Leddy, M., & Leavitt, L. A. (1999). Improving
the communication of people with Down syndrome (pp. 241– Yoder, P. J., Davies, B., Bishop, K., & Munson, L. (1994).
262). Baltimore: Brookes. Effect of adult continuing wh-questions on conversational
participation in children with developmental disabilities.
Miolo, G., Chapman, R. S., & Sindberg, H. (2005). Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 193–204.
Sentence comprehension in adolescents with Down syn-
drome and typically developing children: Role of sentence
voice, visual context, and auditory–verbal short-term
memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing, 48, Received September 30, 2003
172–188. Revision received March 19, 2004
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The Accepted September 6, 2005
emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge, United
Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/026)

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psy- Contact author: Sally Miles, 540 Waisman Center, University
cholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53705.
New York: Plenum. E-mail: miles@wisc.edu

Miles et al.: Sampling Context Affects MLU 337

You might also like