You are on page 1of 12

LSHSS

Research Article

Morphological Errors in Monolingual


Spanish-Speaking Children
With and Without Developmental
Language Disorders
Anny Castilla-Earls,a Alejandra Auza,b Ana Teresa Pérez-Leroux,c
Katrina Fulcher-Rood,d and Christopher Barre

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to identify adjectives, plurals, verb conjugations, and the subjunctive
which morphological markers have the best diagnostic in Spanish.
accuracy to identify developmental language disorders Results: Statistically significant group differences between
(DLD) in monolingual Spanish-speaking children. children with and without DLD were found for all morphological
Method: The participants in this study included 50 Spanish- structures examined but plurals. Logistic regression analyses
speaking monolingual children with (n = 25) and without suggested that a model that included clitic and verbs was
(n = 25) DLD. Data collection took place in Mexico. Children the best model to uniquely predict group membership. This
were administered a comprehensive elicitation task that model showed sensitivity of 96% and specificity of 80%.
set up felicitous contexts to produce morphological Conclusion: Clitics and verbs should be considered
structures previously identified as problematic for Spanish- morphological markers of DLD in monolingual Spanish-
speaking children with DLD: articles, direct object pronouns, speaking children.

C
hildren with developmental language disorders children with DLD make errors that appear to be lan-
(DLD), also known as “specific language impair- guage specific. For example, children with DLD who speak
ment” or “primary language impairment,” experi- Spanish are known to make morphological errors at the
ence unexplained limitations in language ability that are noun phrase level, while English-speaking children with
not attributable to sensory or neurological deficits (Bishop DLD tend to make more errors in the verbal domain
et al., 2016; Leonard, 1998). These limitations include diffi- (see Leonard, 2014, for a review of morphological errors
culties in lexical learning, phonology, pragmatics, and across languages). This seeming variation in the specific
particularly morphosyntax. At the morphosyntactic level, morphosyntactic markers across languages has driven
interest in the study of cross-linguistic manifestations of
DLD (Leonard, 2013).
a
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University In general, Spanish-speaking children with DLD
of Houston, TX produce shorter, less complex, and more ungrammatical
b
Hospital General Dr. Manuel Gea González, Mexico City, Distrito sentences when compared to typically developing (TD)
Federal, Mexico children (Bedore & Leonard, 2005; Restrepo, 1998). Com-
c
Department of Linguistics and Spanish, University of Toronto,
mon grammatical errors seen in Spanish-speaking children
Canada
d
Department of Speech-Language Pathology, SUNY Buffalo State
with DLD include morphological and omission errors with
College, NY articles, direct object pronouns, verb inflections, auxiliary
e
Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics, verbs, adjective agreement, plural inflections, and the sub-
University of Houston junctive mood (Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005). One im-
Correspondence to Anny Castilla-Earls: annycastilla@uh.edu portant limitation in the Spanish literature is that a lot of
Editor-in-Chief: Holly L Storkel what is known about DLD in Spanish-speaking children
Received April 18, 2019 originates from studies on Spanish-speaking children living in
Revision received August 1, 2019 the United States. This is problematic since Spanish-speaking
Accepted October 21, 2019
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-19-00022
Publisher Note: This article is part of the Forum: Morphosyntax Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the
Assessment and Intervention for Children. time of publication.

270 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020 • Copyright © 2020 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
children living in the United States are either active dual significant differences for elements of the noun phrases be-
language learners of Spanish and English or are growing up tween children with and without DLD in both spontaneous
with English as an additional ambient language. Since lan- and elicited language (Anderson & Souto, 2005; Bedore &
guage development in bilingual contexts differs from mono- Leonard, 2001, 2005; Castilla-Earls et al., 2016; Jacobson &
lingual language development (e.g., Morgan et al., 2013), Schwartz, 2002; Morgan et al., 2013; Restrepo & Gutierrez-
it is difficult to know to what extent the characteristics de- Clellen, 2001; Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen, 2007).
scribed in these previous studies are specific to either DLD An exception is the study of Jackson-Maldonado and
or the bilingual context. Thus, it is crucial to specifically Maldonado (2017), who did not find statistically significant
examine the unique grammatical profiles of Spanish-speaking differences in direct object production using a spontaneous
children with DLD growing up in monolingual contexts. language task. Effect sizes range between 1.20 and 2.29 for
The differentiation between typical and atypical language articles and between 0.88 and 2.96 for direct object pronouns,
development remains at the forefront of our current priorities. indicating large effect sizes (Cohen, 1998). Other nominal
Refining grammatical markers (grammatical characteristic elements, such as plurals and noun–adjective agreement, show
typical of DLD; Rice & Wexler, 1996) that are useful to statistically significant differences with large effect sizes, with
identify DLD would improve diagnostic accuracy in Spanish- the exception of the study of Grinstead et al. (2008), who
speaking children with DLD and advance diagnostic prac- found no statistically significant differences for plurals, but
tices for clinicians working with Spanish-speaking children these findings need to be replicated to ensure their validity.
globally. This study has a clinical focus and aims to find
the clinical markers with the best diagnostic accuracy to Verbal Morphology
identify DLD in monolingual Spanish-speaking children. Children with and without DLD showed over 90%
verb accuracy in spontaneous language samples. However,
Grammatical Markers of DLD the accuracy rates are lower in elicited language showing
children with DLD performing around 60% accuracy while
in Spanish-Speaking Children TD children are over 80%. Effect sizes for elicited language
The last 20 years have seen a rise in the study of ranged between 1.08 and 1.69, which are considered large
grammatical markers of DLD in Spanish-speaking children. effect sizes (Cohen, 1998). The subjunctive mood was in-
Our review of the literature identifies 16 studies between vestigated in two studies with large effect sizes (Castilla-
2001 and 2017 investigating grammatical markers in Spanish- Earls et al., 2016; Morgan et al., 2013).
speaking children with DLD. Most studies describe the The results summarized here reflect average group
grammatical profiles in terms of accuracy rates and provide differences between children with and without DLD. How-
a description of error patterns (e.g., Bedore & Leonard, 2001). ever, group differences alone are not enough to establish
In some cases, the emphasis of the studies is on grammatical whether a grammatical structure can differentiate between
errors (Jackson-Maldonado & Maldonado, 2017; Jacobson the two groups. Instead, to determine the viability of a
& Schwartz, 2002). The findings of these 16 studies are sum- diagnostic procedure for the identification of a disorder,
marized by grammatical structure and elicitation method it is crucial to study diagnostic accuracy. In these types of
in terms of accuracy rates in Table 1. We calculated effect studies, the results of a gold standard measure (reference
sizes of the magnitude of the difference using Cohen’s measure) are compared to the diagnostic data of a measure
(1998) d between children with and without DLD for each of interest (index measure). The results of the index measure
grammatical structure to predict the grammatical structures are compared against the reference measure to calculate
with the best potential to differentiate the groups. the amount of agreement between them. This comparison
As can be seen in Table 1, studies that employed elic- yields measures of sensitivity (ability to detect a language
ited language tend to obtain larger effect sizes than studies disorder when it is truly present) and specificity (ability to
employing spontaneous language measures. Some of the exclude a language disorder when it was truly not present).
studies focus on a particular grammatical structure in detail These measures are vital when determining the clinical use-
(e.g., verbs in Grinstead et al., 2013; articles in Restrepo & fulness of a diagnostic test. A test with poor specificity
Gutierrez-Clellen, 2001), while other studies employ a more incorrectly classifies children with typical development as
comprehensive approach testing multiple structures at a having a language impairment. Conversely, a test with poor
time (Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005; Morgan et al., 2013). sensitivity incorrectly classifies children with language im-
Seven of the studies included in Table 1 were conducted pairment as typical. A good measure of diagnostic accuracy
with monolingual children living in monolingual countries, should have a balance between sensitivity and specificity
while the other eight studies included children living in bi- and be at least 90% accurate (10% margin of error). Diag-
lingual contexts. In what follows, we summarize observa- nostic measures with 80%–90% accuracy are considered
tions for nominal and verbal morphology. fair and with less than 80% accuracy are considered unac-
ceptable for diagnostic purposes (Plante & Vance, 1994).
Nominal Morphology From the 16 studies summarized in Table 1, only three
Among the grammatical markers of DLD, direct object studies investigated the diagnostic accuracy of grammati-
pronouns and articles emerge as robust identifiers of DLD cal structures (Grinstead et al., 2013; Morgan et al., 2013;
in Spanish-speaking children. Most studies found statistically Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen, 2007). Simon-Cereijido

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 271


Table 1. Summary of percentage of correct production across various studies.

Age
Form Study Elicitation Context n (years;months) DLD vs. TD d

Articles Anderson & Souto (2005) Spontaneous Monolingual 11 4;10 85 vs. 98 NSD
Jackson-Maldonado & Maldonado (2017) Spontaneous Monolingual 17 7;3 92 vs. 99 NSD
Bedore & Leonard (2005) Spontaneous Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 84 vs. 96 1.05
Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) Spontaneous Bilingual 24 4;6 79 vs. 96 1.05
Restrepo & Gutierrez-Clellen (2001) Spontaneous Bilingual 15 5;9 78 vs. 97 NSD
Anderson & Souto (2005) Elicited Monolingual 11 4;10 64 vs. 95 NSD
Bedore & Leonard (2005) Elicited Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 47 vs. 83 1.70
Morgan et al. (2013) Elicited Monolingual 7 5;7 54 vs. 81 1.20
Castilla-Earls et al. (2016) Elicited Bilingual 16 6;3 38 vs. 86 2.29
Clitics Bedore & Leonard (2005) Spontaneous Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 79 vs. 95 0.91
Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) Spontaneous Bilingual 24 4;6 80 vs. 92 0.09
De la Mora (2004) Elicited Monolingual 10 45 vs. 85 NSD
Morgan et al. (2013) Elicited Monolingual 7 5;7 51 vs. 74 0.88
Plurals Bedore & Leonard (2005) Elicited Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 38 vs. 80 2.96
Jacobson & Schwartz (2002)a Elicited Bilingual 12 8;1 65 vs. 84 1.33
Castilla-Earls et al. (2016) Elicited Bilingual 16 6;3 34 vs. 64 1.27
Bedore & Leonard (2005) Spontaneous Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 93 vs. 97 1.13
Bedore & Leonard (2001) Elicited Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 56 vs. 96 1.96
Grinstead et al. (2008) Elicited Monolingual 9 4;9 89 vs. 94 1.56
Adjectives Bedore & Leonard (2001) Elicited Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 72 vs. 94 1.27
Subjunctive Morgan et al. (2013) Elicited Monolingual 7 5;7 52 vs. 72 0.78
Castilla-Earls et al. (2016) Elicited Bilingual 16 6;3 45 vs. 75 1.15
Verbs Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) Spontaneous Bilingual 24 4;6 90 vs. 97 1.20
Sanz-Torrent et al. (2008) Spontaneous Bilinguala 6 3;7–4;1 93 vs. 97 NSD
Jackson-Maldonado & Maldonado (2017) Spontaneous Monolingual 17 7;3 95 vs. 98 NSD
Grinstead et al. (2013) Spontaneous Monolingual 26 5;8 95 vs. 99 NSD
Grinstead et al. (2009) Elicited Monolingual 21 Unknown 82 vs. 95 1.08
Jacobson (2012) Elicited Bilingual 20 7;0 62 vs. 96 1.69
Contreras González & Soriano Ferrer (2007) Elicited Monolingual 27 8;4 54 vs. 84 1.60
Bedore & Leonard (2001) Elicited Bilingual 15 4;0–5;6 65 vs. 89 1.13

Note. Bilinguals here refer to children in the United States who are either bilingual (Spanish–English) or in language contact situation with
English. n = sample size for children with DLD; Age = average age is provided with some exception where only age range was available;
DLD = children with developmental language disorders; TD = children with typical development; d = Cohen’s effect size; NSD = no standard
deviations provided in the original article, so d was not calculated.
a
Bilingual Spanish and Catalan.

and Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) examined spontaneous language a single grammatical structure suggesting that verb conjuga-
and reported that articles, verbs, and direct object pronouns tion is particularly important for the identification of Spanish-
in isolation showed fair specificity but poor sensitivity to speaking children with DLD. In summary, the results of
identify Spanish-speaking children with DLD living in the the studies of diagnostic accuracy of grammatical markers
United States (articles: sensitivity 74%, specificity 89%; direct suggest that the best potential markers of DLD with good
object pronouns: sensitivity 68%, specificity 84%; verbs: diagnostic accuracy are a combination of grammatical struc-
sensitivity 68%, specificity 89%). However, all three gram- tures (Morgan et al., 2013; Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-
matical structures combined yielded a sensitivity of 79% Clellen, 2007) or verbs in isolation (Grinstead et al., 2013).
and a specificity of 100%, indicating that a pooled measure
might have better diagnostic accuracy than isolated struc-
tures. Morgan et al. (2013) investigated the diagnostic ac- Current Study
curacy of a combined elicited measure that included articles, Among the potential problematic grammatical struc-
clitics, subjunctive mood, and derivational morphemes. tures, direct object pronouns, articles, and verbs have been
They reported that this combined measure had a sensitivity identified as potentially good identifiers of DLD in Spanish-
of 100% and a specificity of 72%. However, Morgan et al. speaking children because (a) the differences on accuracy
did not conduct diagnostic accuracy calculations for gram- rates between TD children and children with DLD are ro-
matical structures in isolation. Lastly, Grinstead et al. (2013) bust and consistent among studies and (b) there is previous
investigated the diagnostic accuracy of elicited verb produc- evidence that suggest that these measures have good diag-
tion. They reported that targeting specific verb forms in nostic accuracy. Other grammatical structures, such as plu-
isolation achieved sensitivity and specificity of 89%. Their rals, noun–adjective agreement, and the subjunctive mood,
findings are very promising considering that they employed had previously shown statistically significant differences

272 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020
between children with and without DLD but need to be includes a subtest of noun phrase morphology and a
replicated and tested for potential diagnostic accuracy. sentence repetition subtest. We used the prespecified
The research questions and hypotheses guiding this cutoff scores in the TPL by age group. The sensitiv-
investigation are as follows: ity and specificity of the TPL for children between 4
and 6 years of age are over 80%, which is considered
1. Are there differences in the accurate production of acceptable for studies of diagnostic accuracy (Plante &
articles, direct object pronouns, verb agreements, noun–
Vance, 1994; sensitivity: age of 4 years 85%, age of 5
adjective agreement, plurals, and the subjunctive
years 83%, age of 6 years 96%; specificity: age of 4
mood between Spanish-speaking monolingual chil-
dren with and without DLD? years 83%, age of 5 years 81%, age of 6 years 81%).
Hypothesis: We predict that all grammatical struc- 2. Showed evidence of low grammatical skills in naturalis-
tures included in this study will show group differences tic language (percentage of grammatical utterances
between monolingual Spanish-speaking children with and below 80% during a story retell task; Auza & Castilla-
without DLD. Earls, 2017; Restrepo, 1998). There were a small num-
ber of children who failed the TPL but had a percentage
2. Which grammatical structures are the best markers of of grammatical utterances of above 80%. In this case,
DLD in Spanish monolingual children? their utterance length (e.g., mean length of utterance in
Hypothesis: We predict that a combination of articles, words) in a story retell task was examined to determine
direct object pronouns, and verbs will have the best diag- whether the high grammaticality was an effect of little
nostic accuracy to identify monolingual Spanish-speaking elaboration at the utterance level as suggested by
children with language disorders. Simon-Cereijido and Gutierrez-Clellen (2007). If
their sentence length was below 1 SD from the mean
in comparison with the Systematic Analysis of Lan-
Method guage Transcripts samples reference database (J. Miller
Participants & Iglesias, 2018) for monolingual children, then
these children were considered to have DLD.
The participants in this prospective study included
50 monolingual Spanish-speaking children living in Mexico To further describe the language profiles of these
City. There were 25 children with TD language skills children, we also administered the Spanish Morphosyntax
(TD children) and 25 children with DLD. Fifty-eight subtest of the Bilingual English–Spanish Assessment (BESA;
percent of the participants were boys. All children passed Peña et al., 2014), the core language subtests of the Clinical
an otoacoustic emission test and obtained a score of ≥ 70 Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool–Second
on the Nonverbal Scale of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Edition–Spanish Edition (CELF Preschool-2 Spanish;
Test–Second Edition (KBIT-2; Kaufman & Kaufman, Semel et al., 2009), and the Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes
2004; see Table 2 for descriptive information). This was (TVIP; Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The TVIP was normed with
a convenience sample of monolingual children with and monolingual children in Mexico, while the CELF Preschool-2
without DLD. Spanish and the BESA were normed with bilingual children
Children with DLD met both of the following criteria: in the United States. When language tests normed in bilin-
1. Failed the Tamiz de Problemas del Lenguaje (TPL; gual children are used with monolingual children, there is
Auza et al., 2018), which is a screening test standard- an inflation effect on standard scores: Monolinguals often
ized with monolingual Mexican children. The TPL score higher than bilinguals in tests standardized with

Table 2. Descriptive information.

Variable Group n M SD Min Max t df `p

Age in months TD 25 68.2 10.5 48 81 1.185 48 .242


DLD 25 65.0 8.7 50 83.
KBIT-2 TD 25 97.5 9.4 84 121 1.278 48 .207
DLD 25 93.9 10.7 78 118.
TVIPs TD 22 117.7 11.4 102 145 5.035 39 > .001
DLD 19 93.8 18.6 58 135.
BESA TD 25 111.2 5.5 93 118 7.917 48 > .001
DLD 25 89.0 12.9 58 115.
CELF Preschool-2 TD 25 110.0 7.9 95 123 5.918 48 > .001
Spanish DLD 25 91.1 13.6 58 125.

Note. TD = typically developing; DLD = developmental language disorder; KBIT-2 = Kaufman Brief Intelligence
Test–Second Edition; TVIP = Test de Vocabulario en Imagenes; BESA = Bilingual English–Spanish Assessment–
Morphosyntax Scale; CELF Preschool-2 Spanish = Core Language Score of the Clinical Evaluation of Language
Fundamentals Preschool–Second Edition–Spanish Edition.

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 273


bilingual children (Castilla-Earls et al., 2019; Morgan Due to a test administration error, there was vocabulary
et al., 2013), although group differences between the TD data available for 22 children in the TD group and 19 chil-
and DLD groups are still evident (Morgan et al., 2013). There- dren in the DLD group. All TD children had scores that
fore, traditional clinical scores for the identification of chil- were equal or higher than 100 on the TVIP, suggesting
dren with DLD in bilingual children are not applicable that their vocabulary skills were within normal limits. In
for these measures, but group differences are expected. In the DLD group, most children performed within normal
this study, we used measures standardized with bilingual limits (above 85), although only eight children with DLD
children to further describe the language performance of the had scores above 100 on the TVIP. In addition, there
participants and not for identification purposes. were five children with DLD who had scores below 85 on
Descriptive information is presented in Table 2. There the TVIP. These results suggest that some of the children
were no significant statistical differences on scores of the in the DLD group had additional deficits in vocabulary
KBIT-2 or on age between the TD and DLD groups. As development.
expected, the group of TD children outperformed the group
of children with DLD on the Spanish BESA Morphosyntax
Measures
subtest, the CELF Preschool-2 Spanish, and the TVIP.
Figure 1 shows the performance on the BESA Morphosyntax The experimental measure used in this study consisted
and CELF Preschool-2 Spanish by group. At the individ- of an elicitation task that set up felicitous contexts to pro-
ual level, all children in the TD group had relatively duce grammatical structures that had been previously identi-
high scores (above 93 on the BESA and 95 on the CELF fied as problematic for Spanish-speaking children with
Preschool-2 Spanish). Children in the DLD group showed DLD: articles, direct object pronouns, adjectives, plurals,
more variable performance on these measures as suggested verb conjugations, and the subjunctive in Spanish. The elicita-
by the dispersion of their scores. However, most of the chil- tion task consisted of 48 items that were elicited by asking
dren in the DLD group (93%) scored below 100 on both questions about a picture (direct object pronouns, articles,
the CELF Preschool-2 Spanish and the BESA. These scores adjectives, and the subjunctive mood) or asking the child to
are in alignment with studies reporting a need to increase the complete a sentence related to a picture (verbs and plurals).
cutoff score of bilingual tests by 1 SD to identify monolin- Children were provided three training items per task.
gual children with DLD (Morgan et al., 2013), although
our purpose in using these bilingual test is purely descriptive. Direct Object Pronouns
In terms of vocabulary, children with DLD, as a The elicitation of direct object pronouns follows the
group, performed significantly poorer than TD children. classic elicitation method described in the study of Schaeffer

Figure 1. Plot of individual scores in the Bilingual English–Spanish Assessment (BESA) and the Clinical Evaluation of Language
Fundamentals Preschool–Second Edition–Spanish Edition (CELF) by group. TD = typically developing group; DLD =
developmental language disorder group.

274 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020
(1997) and used previously in the studies of Castilla (2008) the second-person plural and in the present tense bailamos
and Castilla-Earls and Pérez-Leroux (2010). To elicit direct (“dance-plural”) and caminamos (“walk-plural”). Incorrect
object pronouns, we presented a picture and asked a ques- responses included tense or person errors such as bailar
tion that obliged the use of a direct object pronoun. For (“to dance”) and bailó (“danced-sing”). Unscorable responses,
example, Qué le hace la mamá a la niña? (“What does the as described previously, were also considered incorrect. This
mom do to the girl?”), with an accompanying picture of an task included five items in the first person (two singular, three
adult female combing the hair of a girl. In this case, the plural), four items in the third person (two singular, two
target/correct response is the pronoun la (SgFem) with any plural), and one item in the second/singular person. In terms
accompanying verb in either the pronominal or the post- of tense, seven items were presented in the preterite, and
nominal position. Correct responses for this item included three were presented in the present tense.
la peina (“he/she combs her”), la arregla (“he/she gets her
ready”), and peinándola (“combing-her”). Incorrect responses Subjunctive
included object omissions peinando (“combing”) and The elicitation of the subjunctive mood included very
arreglando (“getting ready”), gender error Esta peinándolo short stories to force the production of the subjunctive in
(“he/she is combing him”), and number error Está peinándolas object complement and temporal clauses. For example, there
(“he/she is combing them-fem”). Other incorrect responses is a picture of a mother visibly upset with a girl in front of
included unscorable responses such as unintelligible state- a mirror with a toothbrush in her hand. Qué quiere la mamá
ments, no responses, and other unrelated responses. que haga la niña? (“What does the mom want the girl to
do?”). Correct responses included any use of the subjunctive
Articles and Adjectives mood (Que se lave sus dientes/“that she washes-subj her
To elicit noun phrases that included both articles teeth”). Incorrect responses included any response using the
and adjectives, we presented pictures with two alternatives indicative mood lava (“washes-ind”), person/tense errors
(e.g. red birds and yellow birds) and asked a question that lavar (“to wash”), and unscorable responses.
forced the child to choose one of the alternatives. For ex-
ample, Estos animales iban volando. Mira, estos se le pararon Plurals
al niño. Cuáles se le pararon al niño? (“These animals were To elicit plurals, we showed pictures with single and
flying towards the child. Look, these ones landed on the multiple objects and asked children to complete a sentence.
child. Which ones landed on the child?”). If the child an- For example, we showed a single train picture while saying
swered los pájaros (“the birds”), a follow-up question was aquí hay un tren (“here there is a train”) and a picture of
introduced Cuáles? (“which ones?”) to facilitate the pro- three trains with y aquí hay tres… (“and here there are
duction of an adjective. Correct responses to articles included three…”). The target response was trenes (“trains”). Incorrect
any article in agreement with the noun produced by the child responses included trens (“trains-incorrect plural”), tren
los pájaros rojos (“the red birds”), los pájaros (“the birds”), (“train”), and unscorable responses.
el pájaro (“the bird”), and los rojos (“the red ones”). Incorrect
responses for articles included article omissions pájaros
(“birds”), gender errors la pájaro (“the-Sgfem bird”), and Procedure
number errors el pájaros (“the-MasSg birds”). For adjectives, This study was approved by institutional review
correct responses included any adjective in agreement with boards in both the United States (State University of New
the noun los pájaros rojos (“the red birds”) and los rojos (“the York–Fredonia) and Mexico (Hospital General Dr. Manuel
red ones”), and incorrect responses included omissions el Gea González). Parents provided consent for their child’s
pájaro (“the bird”) and gender and/or number substitution participation, and children provided assent. Data collection
el pájaro roja (“the red-fem bird”) and los pájaros rojo (“the took place between 2016 and 2017. Testing consisted of two
red-Sg birds”). Unscorable responses (unintelligible utter- sessions of approximately 50 min each, within a 3-week
ances, no responses, and other unrelated responses) were period. Testing took place in the children’s school or at a
also considered incorrect. convenient location for the parents. The first session included
the administration of the hearing screening, the KBIT-2 Non-
Verbs verbal Scale, the CELF Preschool-2 Spanish, the Morpho-
To elicit verbs in this study, we employed a sentence syntax subtest of the BESA, and the TVIP. The second
completion task using pictures. In this task, we presented session consisted of the Spanish retelling task and the experi-
a coordinated sentence in which the second clause needed mental measures. The order of tasks within a session were
to be completed by adding a verb. In the first clause of the administered randomly, with the exception of the hearing
sentence, we provided a subject and a verb to set the tense, screening and the KBIT-2, which were always administered
and in the second clause, we provide a noun to be conju- first. Native Spanish-speaking research assistants adminis-
gated to complete the sentence. For example, we showed tered all the testing in Spanish, and responses to the elicita-
a picture of a boy running and two girls dancing with the tion and story retell tasks were audio-recorded.
following sentence: Nos estamos divirtiendo mucho. Juan Research assistants who were blind to the language
corre y nosotras… (“We are having fun. Juan runs and status of the children transcribed and coded all data to
we…”). Correct responses included any verb conjugated in minimize subjective bias (Dollaghan & Horner, 2011).

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 275


Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (J. Miller & articles, clitics, subjunctive, and adjectives; and (j) verbs,
Iglesias, 2018) was used to transcribe and code the language articles, clitics, subjunctive, adjectives, and plurals. There
samples. Regarding the elicitation task, all responses were were no missing data. To determine the best fitting model,
audio-recorded during assessment and later transcribed by we considered two criteria: statistical and clinical. Because
a research assistant. We calculated transcription reliability Models 1–10 involved the inclusion or exclusion of predictor
for the transcription task at the item level. Two different re- variables, all models could be considered to evaluate the
search assistants transcribed the responses for 10 randomly same model, but certain models constrain a parameter esti-
selected audios (20%). Transcription agreement per item of mate to be zero. As such, these models are nested. There-
the elicitation task was established if both transcriptions fore, the statistical criteria for best model selection included
had the same coding. This was considered a stricter proto- the significance of the predictors, the χ2 test of model fit,
col than traditional transcription at the word level, which and the Akaike information criterion (AIC). The clini-
might underestimate errors related to the grammatical cal criteria included the best possible sensitivity and
structure of interest. The transcription reliability for the specificity with the simpler/fewer grammatical elements
elicitation task was 96%. The coding of all responses was possible.
done in two steps. First, a research assistant coded all re- The results of these models can be seen in Table 5.
sponses as correct or incorrect. Incorrect responses were In Models 1–5, we found verbs, articles, and clitics all to
classified by type of error. Second, the first author of this be significant. However, when articles and clitics were en-
study reviewed all coding in its entirety. Disagreements be- tered together (Model 6), articles were no longer significant,
tween the two raters were discussed and solved. suggesting that articles did not uniquely predict group
membership. This pattern of article nonsignificance was
consistent in Models 7–10. The addition of subjunctives in
Results Model 8, adjectives in Model 9, and plurals in Model 10
To examine group differences, we compared the correct did not make further changes in the significance of the pre-
production of articles, direct object pronouns, verb agree- dictors: The only two predictors that uniquely predicted
ment, noun–adjective agreement, plurals, and the subjunctive group membership were verbs and clitics. In conducting
mood in monolingual children with and without DLD. the nested model comparisons, we found significant χ2 test
Accuracy percentages are presented in Table 3. Table 3 when comparing Model 4 to Models 1–3. These significant
also includes omissions, substitutions, and other responses χ2 tests indicate that the additional parameters estimated
for informative purposes. However, all analyses in this study in Model 4 improve model fit. Additionally, we found all
were based on accuracy. Children without DLD outper- χ2 tests for Model 4 with Models 7–10 to be nonsignificant,
formed those with DLD on all grammatical structures but indicating that the additional predictor variables in Models
plurals. We used an adjusted p value of .0083 to control for 7–10 did not significantly improve model fit. Models 5 and
Type 1 error (see Table 4). In examining the effect sizes as 6 could not be compared to Model 4 because they estimated
indexed by Cohen’s d, we found large mean differences, with the same number of parameters; therefore, we compared
the smallest mean difference being 1.23 for the subjunctive the −2 log likelihood of models: Model 4 had the lowest
mood and the largest being 2.41 for direct object pronouns. −2 log likelihood indicating better fit. Thus, based on this cri-
To find the best grammatical markers of DLD in terion, we found Model 4 to be the best fit to the data. Next,
Spanish monolingual children, we fit a series of logistic re- we examined the AIC, which is an index of absolute fit.
gression models to find a predictor set that most accurately Again, Model 4 had the lowest AIC value indicating that,
predicted group membership. Therefore, our analyses were according to this index, Model 4 was the best fit to the
exploratory. The models tested included (a) clitics only; data.
(b) articles only; (c) verbs only; (d) verbs and clitics; (e) verbs We also considered the clinical significance of the
and articles; (f ) article and clitics; (g) verbs, articles, and various models examined. Models 4, 6, 7, 8, and 9 all
clitics; (h) verbs, articles, clitics, and subjunctive; (i) verbs, had the same diagnostic accuracy (sensitivity = 96% and

Table 3. Response categories by group.

TD DLD
Morphological
structure Correct Substitutions Omission Unscorable Correct Substitution Omission Unscorable

Clitics 84.4 (14.5) 6.0 (9.6) 1.2 (4.4) 2.0 (4.1) 32.5 (26.9) 12.0 (13.4) 20.4 (24.4) 22.4 (28.7)
Articles 95.7 (11.4) 2.0 (5.9) 0.5 (2.5) 2.5 (8.1) 69.6 (22.0) 8.5 (11.8) 8.6 (10.1) 13.3 (14.6)
Adjectives 90.5 (13.6) 0.5 (2.5) 5.5 (9.6) 3.5 (8.5) 66.2 (22.4) 3.0 (5.4) 12.5 (13.5) 18.3 (15.6)
Plural 83.2 (17.9) 8.0 (10.0) 6.4 (11.1) 2.4 (8.8) 63.2 (33.5) 10.4 (13.1) 19.2 (26.8) 7.2 (14.0)
Subjunctive 72.0 (31.1 16.0 (23.8) NA 12 (16.3) 33.6 (31.5) 27.2 (23.0) NA 39.2 (31.3)
Verbs 78.4 (14.9) 8.8 (15.1) NA 12.8 (14.0) 49.6 (21.5) 12 (31.7) NA 38.4 (42.8)

Note. Standard deviation in parentheses. TD = typically developing; DLD = developmental language disorder; NA = not applicable.

276 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020
Table 4. Group comparisons. model following our clinical criteria. Thus, taken together,
both statistical and clinical criteria for selecting the best fit-
Variable Difference t df p Cohen’s d ting model pointed to Model 4, which included clitics and
verbs as the best set of grammatical markers to predict
Verbs 28.80 5.50 48 < .0001 1.56
Articles 25.36 5.12 48 < .0001 1.45 DLD in Spanish monolingual children.
Clitics 51.47 8.53 48 < .0001 2.41 Model 4 had a positive likelihood ratio of 4.8 and a
Subjunctive 38.40 4.34 48 < .0001 1.23 negative likelihood ratio of 0.05. Dollaghan’s (2007) rule
Adjective 24.29 4.63 48 < .0001 1.31 of thumb to interpret likelihood ratios is that a diagnostic
Plurals 20.00 2.63 48 .0114 0.74
measure with a positive likelihood ratio over 10 is clinically
informative to identify a child with a language disorder and
a measure with a negative likelihood ratio of 1 or below is
specificity = 80%). However, Model 4 had the fewest pre- clinically informative to rule out the presence of a language
dictors (i.e., would have the lowest testing burden for chil- disorder. Therefore, the likelihood ratios found in this study
dren); therefore, we determined that Model 4 was the best suggest that a combination of clitics and verbs is clinically

Table 5. Model comparisons.

Model Parameter Estimate SE χ2 p Sensitivity Specificity 2LL LL ratio AIC Prob χ2

1 Age –0.03 0.05 0.27 .603 100 76 28.92 40.39 34.92 .990
Clitics 0.10 0.03 12.75 .000
2 Age –0.01 0.04 0.04 .847 80 80 48.4 20.91 54.4 .999
Articles 0.08 0.02 11.94 .001
3 Age 0.00 0.04 0.01 .919 92 64 44.4 24.91 50.4 .999
Verbs 0.09 0.03 13.02 .000
4 Age –0.11 0.09 1.56 .211 96 80 22.32 47.0 30.32 NA
Verbs 0.09 0.04 4.77 .029
5 Clitics 0.10 0.04 7.28 .007
Age –0.03 0.05 0.52 .472 96 68 37.24 32.08 45.24 NA
Verbs 0.08 0.03 8.17 .004
Articles 0.07 0.03 5.65 .018
6 Age –0.03 0.05 0.40 .529 96 80 27.94 41.38 35.94 NA
Articles 0.03 0.03 0.93 .334
Clitics 0.09 0.03 9.00 .003
7 Age –0.11 0.09 1.56 .212 96 80 22.22 47.1 32.22 .248
Verbs 0.09 0.04 4.16 .041
Articles 0.01 0.05 0.10 .757
Clitics 0.09 0.04 5.24 .022
8 Age –0.14 0.12 1.44 .230 96 80 22.02 47.3 34.02 .139
Verbs 0.08 0.04 4.10 .043
Articles 0.01 0.05 0.09 .759
Clitics 0.09 0.04 4.80 .028
Subjunctive 0.01 0.03 0.21 .651
9 Age –0.18 0.14 1.63 .201 96 80 21.67 47.65 35.67 .115
Verbs 0.09 0.04 4.12 .042
Articles 0.00 0.05 0.00 .969
Clitics 0.08 0.04 4.39 .036
Subjunctive 0.02 0.03 0.23 .634
Adjective 0.04 0.06 0.33 .565
10 Age –0.22 0.16 1.96 .161 96 76 20.85 48.46 36.85 .168
Verbs 0.10 0.05 4.43 .035
Articles 0.00 0.06 0.00 .988
Clitics 0.09 0.04 4.29 .038
Subjunctive 0.02 0.03 0.24 .624
Adjective 0.05 0.08 0.42 .515
Plurals –0.03 0.03 0.72 .395

Note. χ2 are for all models versus Model 4. Degrees of freedom for all models = 1. 2LL = log-likelihood ratio; LL = likelihood ratio; AIC =
Akaike information criterion; NA = not applicable.

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 277


informative to rule out the presence of DLD and suggestive differences between Spanish-speaking children with and
to identify a Spanish monolingual child with DLD. without DLD.
As Model 4 was deemed the best predictive model, we Also, at the noun phrase level, our results supported
employed the results of Model 4 to identify cutoff scores for our hypothesis for group differences for adjectives but not
clitics and verbs. Number of correct combined items (clitics + for plurals. These two morphological structures had been
verbs) were used for these cutoff scores. The maximum total studied considerably less than article and direct object pro-
number of items was 20 (10 clitic items and 10 verb items). nouns, so replication of findings was of importance. These
Children between the ages of 48 and 54 months were classi- results are in agreement with the results for adjectives but
fied as having DLD if they had at nine items correct or less in disagreement with the results for plurals in the study of
(i.e., cutoff score is 9 items), whereas children in the same Bedore and Leonard (2001), who studied bilingual children.
age range were classified as TD if they had 10 or more The current results about plurals are in agreement with
items correct. Cutoff scores for the remaining ages were Grinstead et al.’s (2008) data on monolingual children, in
10 items at 55–60 months, 11 items at 61–72 months, and that we did not find statistically significant differences. It
12 items at 78–82 months. is important to note that Grinstead et al. (2008) found an
effect size of 1.56 for plurals, but their sample size was small
(n = 9), which probably affected the statistical significance
Discussion of their results. It is also possible that plurals are not as
problematic for Spanish-speaking monolingual children in
The goal of this study was to comprehensively exam- comparison to bilingual children.
ine the various morphological structures that have been At the verb phrase level, this study found significant
previously identified to be problematic for Spanish-speaking differences for both the subjunctive and verb agreement.
children in a group of monolingual children with and with- These results are in agreement with previous studies report-
out DLD to find the grammatical markers with the best ing group differences between children with and without
diagnostic accuracy. We found group differences with large DLD for the subjunctive mood (Castilla-Earls et al., 2016;
effect sizes for articles, clitics, adjective agreement, verb Morgan et al., 2013) and verb agreement (Bedore & Leon-
agreement, and the subjunctive mood with monolingual ard, 2001; Grinstead et al., 2009, 2013; Jacobson & Schwartz,
Spanish-speaking children without DLD demonstrating 2002). The effect sizes found in this study (subjunctive: 1.23;
higher accuracy rates than children with DLD. Although verbs: 1.56) were slightly higher than the effect sizes reported
most of the grammatical structures revealed group differ- for the subjunctive in previous studies and within the range
ences, a combination of clitics and verbs uniquely predicted of previously reported studies for verb agreement. The
group membership with acceptable sensitivity and specificity. difference in the subjunctive might be due to the type of sub-
These findings will be discussed in terms of group differ- junctive used in the various studies. In general, the results
ences, diagnostic accuracy, and the relevance of these find- for the verb phrase replicated previous studies. These results
ings for the identification of morphological markers of DLD are of particular importance because the errors seen in DLD
for Spanish-speaking children. in Spanish have been traditionally described more in terms
of noun phrase errors than verb phrase errors. This is the
first study investigating various elicited morphological struc-
Group Differences tures, including verbs, in monolingual Spanish-speaking
Article and direct object pronouns are the most studied children. Therefore, the differences in verbs at the group
morphological structures for Spanish-speaking children level seen in this study highlight the importance of examin-
with DLD (see review in Table 1). As predicted, this study ing verb production in Spanish-speaking children who are
found statistically significant group differences for both suspected to have DLD.
articles and direct object pronouns. These results are in agree- Although error patterns were not the emphasis on
ment with previous studies investigating articles (Anderson this investigation, the results in Table 3 provide some inter-
& Souto, 2005; Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005; Castilla- esting insights about potential differences between the groups.
Earls et al., 2016; Morgan et al., 2013; Restrepo & Gutierrez- In general, children with DLD produce a high percentage
Clellen, 2001; Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen, 2007) of unscorable responses (no responses, unintelligible re-
and direct object pronouns (Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005; sponses, and unrelated responses). In addition, the type of
Castilla-Earls et al., 2016; Morgan et al., 2013; Simon- error varied by structure. For example, clitic omission
Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen, 2007). In terms of the seem to be the most prevalent error for children with DLD
magnitude of the group difference, both articles and direct (about 20%), while it is almost not existent in TD children
object pronouns had effect sizes within the range of previ- (1%). These results warrant further investigation to explore
ously reported studies (articles: 1.45, range: 1.2–2.3; clitics: the possibility that the type of error might add information
2.41, range: 0.88–2.96), which suggests that the group dif- about the clinical status of a child. Current efforts in our
ferences in articles and clitics observed across the various research laboratories are examining this possibility in de-
studies are relatively stable in terms of size. These results tail using a complex error coding that is distinct to each
confirm the results of previous studies suggesting that grammatical structure to truly tap into its morphological
both articles and direct object pronouns show large group complexity.

278 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020
Diagnostic Accuracy of Morphological Structures Morphological Markers of DLD in Monolingual
Our results suggest that, in isolation, articles were Spanish-Speaking Children
the only morphological structure with acceptable diagnos- Rice and Wexler (1996) defined a clinical marker
tic accuracy (sensitivity and specificity of 80%). Clitics in as a morphological characteristic that is typical of DLD.
isolation showed excellent sensitivity but poor specificity. These morphological markers in Spanish have been tradi-
Similarly, verbs showed good sensitivity but poor specific- tionally identified using group comparisons in terms of ac-
ity. These results indicate that performance on both articles curacy (Bedore & Leonard, 2001, 2005). An evidence-based
and verbs works better to identify children with a language assessment approach should rely on the use of analysis of
disorder than to exclude those with typical language skills diagnostic accuracy (Simon-Cereijido & Gutierrez-Clellen,
at this age when children are still acquiring verbal and 2007). Each of these approaches offer related but different
nominal morphology. These results are similar to the results results. Group differences show that, on average, a group
of Simon-Cereijido and Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) in that arti- of TD children perform better than children with DLD.
cles, clitics, and verbs showed lower diagnostic accuracy when However, variation among the profiles of the children in
tested separately. However, Simon-Cereijido and Gutierrez- a group could indeed exist. Although group differences are
Clellen found that when articles, verbs, and clitics were of importance for general description of language abilities,
tested separately, they had higher specificity but poor sensitiv- they are not enough to identify whether performance on a
ity. The difference in these results might be due to the fact that specific marker is typical of a group. Examination of diag-
Simon-Cereijido and Gutierrez-Clellen examined spontaneous nostic accuracy reveals whether performance on a morpho-
language while we examined elicited language. However, our
logical structure can differentiate groups of children with
results differed from the results of Grinstead et al. (2013) in
and without language disorders. This approach seems to
that verb accuracy in isolation did not have good diagnostic
be more appropriate for the identification of morphological
accuracy. The examination of morphological structures
markers of DLD disorders in Spanish-speaking children.
separately shows that no single morphological structure is
We believe that both clitics and verbs should be con-
an ideal marker of DLD in monolingual children, and
sidered markers of DLD in Spanish-speaking monolingual
therefore, pooling morphological structures might be a bet-
children for two reasons. First, these two morphological
ter approach for assessment and identification of DLD.
markers taken together accurately identified 96% of children
Pooling morphological structures also reveals impor-
with language disorders and 80% of children with typical
tant traits about the morphological structures. In this study,
development, which is considered acceptable in terms of
when articles and clitics were entered together in Model 6,
diagnostic accuracy (Plante & Vance, 1994). The high accu-
articles were no longer statistically significant in their predic-
racy found in this study is not trivial, considering that per-
tion of group membership. In fact, in all models where ar-
formance on two morphological structures identified 88% of
ticles and clitics were entered together, articles were not
the cases correctly. Second, the results of this study replicate
significant. This was due to the fact that articles and clitics
previous finding in the literature that identify these mor-
shared a significant amount of variance (r = .645), and
phological structures as markers of DLD in Spanish-speaking
when entered together, clitics explain variability above and
children.
beyond the variability explained by articles. This is consistent
One might argue that the results seen in this study
with previous work showing the similarities between articles
are somewhat circular since children were included in this
and clitics in terms of developmental timing and phonolog-
study because they either had or lack morphosyntactic errors
ical characteristics, but also the differences in terms of their
using global measures of morphosyntax, and therefore, the
developmental trajectories (Pérez-Leroux et al., 2012).
fact that morphological errors differentiate the groups is not
A combination of verbs and clitics was the best ap-
surprising. However, it is important to note that the goal of
proach to improve diagnostic accuracy in this study with
monolingual Spanish-speaking children (sensitivity 96% and this study was to identify the best morphological structures
specificity 80%). Although other models produced the same to differentiate the groups since this has clinical and theo-
diagnostic accuracy levels (Models 7, 8, and 9), the model retical implications that are discussed below. Nonetheless,
with clitics and verbs fitted the data better and was the most this points to a crucial limitation of this study because our
parsimonious. Therefore, it was deemed the best model to results do not generalize to children with DLD whose deficits
differentiate the groups. This study replicated previous are not specific to the morphosyntactic domain. It is critical
findings in Simon-Cereijido and Gutierrez-Clellen (2007) to consider that children with DLD are a heterogeneous
in that articles, clitics, and verbs showed better diagnostic group with difficulties in various language domains, not only
accuracy when pooled together than individually, although morphosyntax (for a thorough review of current definitions
they used spontaneous language and studied bilingual chil- and views of DLD, see Bishop et al., 2016). For example,
dren. The other morphological structures investigated in the results of this study would not be applicable to children
this study, such as adjectives, plurals, and subjunctive, did with semantic and word-finding difficulties. Although there
not further improve the diagnostic accuracy of the models is no current consensus on the possibility of subtypes of
examined in this study. These results are of clinical rele- DLD (Bishop, 2017), it is essential that the results of this
vance because it is critical to distinguish the nuances of study are interpreted in light of a profile of DLD morpho-
results at the group level versus diagnostic accuracy. syntactic deficit.

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 279


The results of this study can be applied to the identifi- Auza, A., Murata, M., Márquez, M. E., & Morgan, G. P. (2018).
cation of monolingual Spanish-speaking children with DLD. Tamiz para detectar problemas del lenguaje (TPL) [Screening
The data in this study could be used by speech-language for language difficulties]. Manual Moderno.
pathologists for the assessment of language skills, more spe- Bedore, L., & Leonard, L. (2001). Grammatical morphology defi-
cits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language im-
cifically for morphological development. Therefore, these pairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
data have the potential to advance diagnostic practices for 44(4), 905–924. https://pubs.asha.org/doi/10.1044/1092-4388
clinicians working with monolingual Spanish-speaking chil- (2001/072)
dren globally. However, careful considerations should be in Bedore, L., & Leonard, L. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase
place in regard to the application of these results to bilingual morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking
populations. Morgan et al. (2013) have shown that it is not children with specific language impairment. Applied Linguistics,
appropriate to use monolingual norms for the assessment of 26(2), 195–225.
bilingual children. Our current research efforts are focused Bishop, D. V. M. (2017). Why is it so hard to reach agreement
on the comparisons of the diagnostic accuracy of Spanish on terminology? The case of developmental language disorder
(DLD). International Journal of Language & Communication
morphological structures between monolingual and bilingual
Disorders, 52(6), 671–680.
children to directly contribute to the identification of DLD Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh,
in children growing up in bilingual contexts. Another limita- T., & The CATALISE Consortium. (2016). CATALISE: A
tion of the clinical application of these results is the potential multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study.
for dialectal differences. There are data that suggest that Identifying language impairments in children. PLOS ONE,
other Spanish dialects show different patterns in some of the 11(12), e0158753. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158753
grammatical structures studied here. For example, children Castilla, A. P. (2008). Developmental measures of morphosyntactic
who use /s/-leniting dialects of Spanish might show different acquisition in monolingual 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old Spanish-speaking
results for plurals (K. Miller, 2014). Similarly, children who children (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of
Toronto, Canada.
use Andean Spanish might show a normal pattern of clitic
Castilla, A. P., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2010). Omissions and sub-
omission (Stewart, 1999). Clinicians should be aware of stitutions in early Spanish clitics. Journal of Language Acquisi-
dialectal differences when examining the applicability of these tion, 17(1), 2–25.
results to children in their caseloads. Castilla-Earls, A., Perez-Leroux, A. T., Nieto, L., Restrepo, M. A.,
& Barr, C. (2019). Vulnerability of clitics and articles in typi-
cally developing bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and
Conclusion Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000610
Spanish monolingual children with DLD differed Castilla-Earls, A., Restrepo, M. A., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Gray, S.,
from Spanish monolingual children with typical language Holmes, P., Gail, D., & Chen, Z. (2016). Interactions between
bilingual effects and language impairment: Exploring grammati-
skills in their accurate elicited production of articles, clitics,
cal markers in Spanish-speaking bilingual children. Applied
adjectives, verbs, and the subjunctive mood. The accurate Psycholinguistics, 37(5), 1147–1173.
production of clitics and verbs discriminated Spanish mono- Cohen, J. (1998). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
lingual children with DLD from their counterparts with (2nd ed.). Routledge.
typical language skills with a sensitivity of 96% and a spec- Contreras González, M., & Soriano Ferrer, M. (2007). La morfo-
ificity of 80%. The likelihood ratios found in this study logía flexiva en el trastorno específico del lenguaje y en la
suggest that a combination of clitics and verbs is clinically deprivación sociocultural [Reflexive morphology in specific
informative to rule out the presence of DLD in a Spanish language impairment and cultural deprivation]. Revista de
monolingual child and is suggestive to identify a Spanish Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 27(3), 110–117.
monolingual child with DLD. De la Mora, J. (2004). Direct object clitics and determiners in
Spanish specific language impairment (Unpublished master’s
thesis). University of Alberta, Alberta, Canada.
Acknowledgments Dollaghan, C. A. (2007). The handbook of evidence-based practice
in communication disorders. Brookes.
Research reported in this publication was supported by Na- Dollaghan, C. A., & Horner, E. A. (2011). Bilingual language as-
tional Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders sessment: A meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy. Journal of
Award R15DC013670 granted to Anny Castilla-Earls. The content Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(4), 1077–1088.
is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0093)
represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test–Third Edition: Manual. AGS.
Grinstead, J., Baron, A., Vega-Mendoza, M., De la Mora, J.,
References Cantú-Sánchez, M., & Flores, B. (2013). Tense marking and
Anderson, R., & Souto, S. (2005). The use of articles by monolin- spontaneous speech measures in Spanish specific language
gual Puerto Rican Spanish–speaking children with specific impairment: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech,
language impairment. Applied Linguistics, 26(4), 621–647. Language, and Hearing Research, 56(1), 352–363. https://doi.
Auza, A. & Castilla-Earls, A. (2017). Percentage of grammatical org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0289)
utterances in Spanish-speaking children with and without specific Grinstead, J., Cantú-Sánchez, M., & Flores, B. (2008). Canonical and
language impairment. Presented at the Symposium of Research epenthetic plural marking in Spanish-speaking children with spe-
in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI, United States. cific language impairment. Language Acquisition, 15(4), 329–349.

280 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 270–281 • April 2020
Grinstead, J., De la Mora, J., Vega Mendoza, M., & Flores, B. (2009). Peña, E. D., Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Iglesias, A., Goldstein, B. A.,
An elicited production test of the optional infinitive stage in & Bedore, L. M. (2014). BESA: Bilingual English–Spanish
child Spanish. In J. Crawford, K. Otaki, & M. Takahashi (Eds.), Assessment. AR-Clinical Publications.
Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Generative Approaches Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earls, A. P., & Bruner, J. (2012).
to Language acquisition North America (GALANA 2008) General and specific effects of lexicon in grammar: Determiner
(pp. 36–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and object pronoun omission in child Spanish. Journal of Speech,
Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Maldonado, R. (2017). Grammaticality Language, and Hearing Research, 55(2), 313–327. https://doi.
differences between Spanish-speaking children with specific org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0004)
language impairment and their typically developing peers. Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, tests. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25(1),
52(6), 750–765. 15–24. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.2501.15
Jacobson, P. F. (2012). The effects of language impairment on the use Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-
of direct object pronouns and verb inflections in heritage Spanish speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech,
speakers: A look at attrition, incomplete acquisition and mainte- Language, and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1398–1411. https://doi.
nance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(1), 22–38. org/10.1044/jslhr.4106.1398
Jacobson, P. F., & Schwartz, R. G. (2002). Morphology in incipi- Restrepo, M. A., & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article produc-
ent bilingual Spanish-speaking preschool children with specific tion in bilingual children with specific language impairment.
language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(1), 23–41. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 433–452.
Kaufman, A. & Kaufman, S. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker
for Children–Second Edition. Pearson. of specific language impairment in English-speaking children.
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1239–1257.
MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239
Leonard, L. B. (2013). Specific language impairment across lan- Sanz-Torrent, M., Serrat, E., Andreu, L., & Serra, M. (2008).
guages. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 1–5. Verb morphology in Catalan and Spanish in children with
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. specific language impairment: A developmental study. Clinical
In Language, speech, and communication (2nd ed.). MIT Press. Linguistics & Phonetics, 22(6), 459–474. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Miller, K. (2014). Assessing plural morphology in children acquiring 02699200801892959
/S/-leniting dialects of Spanish. Language, Speech, and Hearing Schaeffer, J. (1997). Direct object scrambling and clitic placement
Services in Schools, 45(3), 173–184. https://doi.org/10.1044/ in Dutch and Italian child language (Dissertation). University
2014_LSHSS-13-0032 of California, Los Angeles.
Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2018). Systematic Analysis of Language Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2009). Clinical Evalua-
Transcripts (SALT), Research Version 2018 [Computer software]. tion of Language Fundamentals Preschool–Second Edition–
SALT Software. Spanish Edition (CELF Preschool-2 Spanish). Pearson.
Morgan, G. P., Restrepo, M., & Auza, A. (2013). Comparison of Simon-Cereijido, G., & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. (2007). Spontaneous
Spanish morphology in monolingual and Spanish–English bilin- language markers of Spanish language impairment. Applied
gual children with and without language impairment. Bilingualism, Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 317–339.
16(3), 578–596. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000697 Stewart, M. (1999). The Spanish language today. Routledge.

Castilla-Earls et al.: Spanish-Speaking Children With DLD 281

You might also like