You are on page 1of 8

Driver Behavior in Road Tunnels

Association with Driver Stress and Performance

Erika E. Miller and Linda Ng Boyle

Complex traffic environments can affect driver stress and driving per- stress was shown to foster negative emotions (e.g., anger) or more
formance negatively. Driving through tunnels can be particularly stress- risky driving performance outcomes (7). Others have also noted the
ful, and these segments have been associated with higher crash rates. association between workload and driving performance (8–10). For
However, the constraints of the natural environment (e.g., mountains, example, Clapp and associates showed that stressful driving is asso-
waterways) often restrict the flexibility to make major modifications in ciated with anxiety-based performance deficits or hostile–aggressive
tunnels. The purpose of this study was to evaluate stress as drivers tra- behaviors (8, 11). Driver stress is also associated with specific road-
versed along an interstate route that included tunnel and nontunnel seg- way design and operations and could be influenced by the driver’s age,
ments, as well as a 75-m transition period before the tunnels. Data from gender, personality, impairment, and even stressful life events (8, 12).
50 drivers, including information from electrocardiogram recordings In road tunnels, these feelings of anxiety can lead to reduced
such as heart rate and standard deviation of interbeat intervals (SDNN), vehicle speed just before the entrance and at the exit of tunnel open-
were collected. Driving performance measures included vehicle speed ings (1, 13). Akamatsu et al. noticed that speed changes occurred
and braking. Multivariate and univariate analyses of variance were used 50 m before a tunnel and again at the exit of a tunnel; this finding
to identify increases in drivers’ stress during several road segments: the suggests that drivers were hesitant or stressed when entering the
transition to a tunnel entrance, within a tunnel, and open road segments. tunnel and relieved when exiting (1). Amundsen et al. found that
The largest variations in performance measures were observed in tunnels, crash rates were four times higher 50 m before and after tunnel
followed by the periods in transition segments. Evaluation of continuous openings compared with the middle segment of the tunnel (14).
speed profiles along the route showed that drivers tended to decrease their Calvi et al. found that drivers in tunnels tend to move away from
speed before entering a tunnel and increase speed just before exiting a the lane edge that is nearest the tunnel wall (15). This increase in
tunnel. The highest level of stress (denoted by the largest positive change driver stress at the onset of the tunnel has also been associated with
in heart rate and lowest variability in SDNN) was observed along the decreased cognitive processing ability for critical information.
transition segments, followed by the tunnel segments. Identifying situa- Some tunnel designs may reduce the anxiety associated with driv-
tions in which drivers may experience higher levels of stress and the cor- ing through a tunnel. Shimojo et al. suggested that the shape of a
responding impact on driving performance is important for future road tunnel opening can help alleviate the perceptions of tunnel nar-
and tunnel designs. rowness and meager tunnel lighting (16). The contrast in lighting
between the tunnel and the open road can also impact the driver’s
ability to perceive the inside of the tunnel and make the tunnel look
Driver–road–vehicle interactions can be influenced by several internal like a dark hole (1). Although tunnels are often constrained by the
and external factors that are compounded in complex traffic environ- limitations of the roadway and natural environment, possible solu-
ments. Tunnels are one such complex environment shown to increase tions to ease tunnel anxiety may be found by adjusting illumina-
driver stress and crash rates (1, 2). These enclosed environments can tion and longitudinal ventilation, and increasing height and width
decrease the driver’s attention toward important traffic information clearances (13).
as early as 150 m before a tunnel opening (3), and this decreased Several methods have been commonly accepted for quantifying
attention has resulted in lane selection errors when entering the driver workload. Coughlin et al. suggested several metrics in detect-
tunnel (4). ing driver state, including quantifying driving behavior, vehicle per-
Measures of physiology (e.g., cardiac and other muscle activity) formance, environmental factors, and biometrics (17). Akamatsu et al.
and decision-making tasks (e.g., working memory tests) have been used information from the accelerator pedal to examine fluctuations
used to show that stress is associated with workloads. In a model of in speed (1), and Tornros looked at deviations in lateral position as
driver stress by Matthews (5), exposure to stressful situations has indicators of driver response to tunneled versus nontunneled segments
been surmised to induce driving-related behaviors, where stress is (18). Ceci et al. compared physiological measures and considered
based on the interaction of persons and environment (6). This situation higher variability in the physiological responses to indicate increases
was demonstrated by Oz et al. in a survey study, in which cognitive in cognitive workload in response to tunnel driving (4). Driver heart
rate and variability are often used as an indicator of driver workload
E. E. Miller, Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, and L. N. Boyle, and have been shown to relate to driver performance (19, 20). Simi-
Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering and Department of Civil larly, changes in vehicle speed and braking have been observed when
and Environmental Engineering, University of Washington, Box 352650, Seattle, drivers appear to be more stressed (1, 21).
WA 98195-2650. Corresponding author: L. N. Boyle, linda@uw.edu. The purpose of this study was to identify the relationship of tun-
Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board,
nels and driver stress, and whether the stress may be correlated with
No. 2518, Transportation Research Board, Washington, D.C., 2015, pp. 60–67. driver performance. The study used an on-road study that included
DOI: 10.3141/2518-08 tunnels and nontunnel segments. Driver performance was quantified

60
Miller and Boyle 61

by using braking events, speed variability, and driver stress (heart the consent form and familiarized with the study route and instru-
activity measured through an electrocardiogram, ECG). mented vehicle. Participants were connected to the ECG device right
before the drive began. A 3-min resting heart rate was recorded as a
baseline. The original drive began and ended at the university. The
Methodology route consisted of a 25-mi (40-km) circular route. Half the partici-
pants completed the drive in the clockwise direction, while the other
The analysis built on previous work by Miller and Boyle by further half completed it in the counterclockwise direction. Participants were
investigating the variations in physiological and performance mea- randomly assigned to a direction of travel. For the clockwise direc-
sures collected in an on-road study along various roads in the Puget tion, the route began with State Route 520 (eastbound), to rural roads
Sound region of Washington State (22). in Bellevue, Washington (southeast), to Interstate 405 (I-405) (south),
to I-90 (west), and finally on I-5 (north). The counterclockwise direc-
tion encountered the same roads, but in reverse order. Immediately
Subjects following the completion of the drive, the participants were given a
four-page questionnaire that solicited data on their driving behaviors,
The study recruited 60 subjects, and 54 completed the entire experi- study drive experience, and demographics. The study took about 2 h
mental drive. Six were removed because of screening complications. for each participant to complete.
Of the 54 who completed the entire drive, four drivers had miss- All drives were conducted during July through September 2012
ing data and were excluded from further analyses. In total, 50 par- between 10:30 and 14:30. This permitted each driver to experience
ticipants (26 male, 24 female) were examined. All the subjects had similar conditions of daylight, clear weather, and nonpeak traffic.
Washington State driver licenses for at least 2 years and were from Furthermore, each driver was asked to obey the posted speed limit
one of three age groups: younger (25 years old and younger), middle and keep the radio turned off.
age (35 to 55 years), and older (65 years and older). Participants were
compensated $25 per hour for their time.
Road Segment

Equipment The analyses in this paper focus on the I-90 segment of the study,
which was located from Mile Post (MP) 2 to MP 10 in Washington
The study was conducted in an instrumented 2002 Ford Taurus with State. Previous work suggested that differences within this segment
automatic transmission. The data recorded included vehicle kine- might differ from the other road segments for heart rate variability
matics, GPS location, and images from video recordings. Vehicle data (22). Of particular interest is that this corridor also encompassed
were collected at a rate of five samples per second (5 Hz) with the road segments with tunnels. Therefore, this portion of the data was
program LabView, version 6. extracted for further analysis. Approximately 12 km of I-90 were
Continuous ECG data were collected for each participant before included in the drive, which were partitioned into seven segments
and during the drive with a Biopac MP150 and ECG 100 C amplifier. based on location; for the westbound (clockwise) approach, the route
The ECG readings were recorded with a Lead 2 configuration with was segmented into open road (4.9 km), transition (75 m), tunnel
shielded leads for the positive and negative channels and unshielded (0.8 km), open road (2.8 km), transition (75 m), tunnel (1 km), and
leads for the ground. ECG data were collected at a sampling rate of open road (1.4 km). The eastbound (counterclockwise) drive included
1,000 Hz with the program AcqKnowledge, version 4.2. The vehicle the same segments in reverse order.
and ECG data were recorded with the same data acquisition com- Data from the Washington State Department of Transportation
puter and synchronized with the computer’s timestamp. Reduction showed that all open road lanes on this section, including the transition
on the raw ECG data was done with the same program as acquisition, segments, were 12 ft wide, while the lanes in the tunnels were 11 ft wide
AcqKnowledge (version 4.2). To ensure that all the data were accu- (Washington State Department of Transportation, personal communi-
rately processed, a tachogram of each data set was used to highlight cation, 2014). The shoulder widths ranged from 6 to 10 ft along the
artifacts, and subsequent manual reduction was performed to adjust open road and transition segments, and 1 to 4 ft widths in the tunnels.
peaks and eliminate artifacts. This section of I-90 connects the city of Seattle to Bellevue (Fig-
ure 1). There are minimal grade changes and only slight horizontal
curves. Although this interstate section has a variable speed zone, the
Procedure posted speed limit for all drivers is 60 mph. The highway includes
three lanes in each direction and an additional two-lane reversible
Participants met the researcher at the University of Washington in expressway. Drivers were advised not to take the express lanes during
Seattle, Washington, at which point the participants were provided the study.

Open Road 1 Open Road 2 Open Road 3

Tunnel A Tunnel B
Seattle Bellevue

FIGURE 1   Map of road segments in study. (Source: modified from Google
Maps.)
62 Transportation Research Record 2518

I-90 Tunnels gender, and all corresponding interaction terms were also considered
in the model.
I-90 has two tunnels (labeled A and B) for each direction of travel
(Figure 2). The two tunnels have similar lane configurations and ver-
tical and horizontal alignments. However, Tunnel A (Figure 2) has
Dependent Variables
narrower lanes and tighter shoulders for the eastbound direction of
travel, as it splits into two tunnels; the left tunnel encompasses two The dependent variables comprised vehicle kinematics and physio­
lanes and the right tunnel has only one lane. logical measurements. The vehicle kinematics included means and
standard deviations of speed and deceleration calculated for each
participant at each of the seven road segments. Deceleration was
Independent Variables measured as a change in the voltage of the brake pedal load cell or
foot pedal application. The change could range from zero (for no
All participants were randomly assigned to a designated direction change) to 1.5 (hard braking event) and was recorded at 5 Hz. Speed
of travel, but the roadway segments were all the same. That is, par- was defined by the rate at which the vehicle was traveling in miles
ticipants were placed in either the eastbound or westbound direction per hour and was also recorded at 5 Hz.
of travel to minimize any variations that may exist between the two Physiological measures were extracted from the ECG data and
travel directions. The seven road segments were categorized as: Open included heart rate and the standard deviation of interbeat intervals
Roads 1, 2, and 3; Tunnels A and B; and Transitions A and B (Fig- (SDNN). Heart rate was defined as the number of R-wave peaks over
ure 1). Tunnel A is the west most tunnel (nearer Seattle) and Tunnel B a given interval measured in beats per minute (bpm) and standard-
is closest to the east side (Bellevue). The transition segment was ized for comparison across participants by subtracting each partici-
defined as the 75 m of open road before the entrance of the tunnel. pant’s mean resting heart rate from their continuous ECG recordings.
This segment was included in the analyses since studies have indi- As a result, a positive value for heart rate would be associated with
cated that increased stress and performance errors can occur in the increased stress.
transition segment (3, 14). The geometrics were comparable between Heart rate variability was also accounted for by the SDNN; lower
the two directions. Driver age groups [younger (25 years or younger), values of SDNN are associated with increasing stress (23). Each
middle aged (35 to 55 years old), and older (65 years old or older)], participant’s SDNN was standardized similarly to his or her heart rate

(a) (b)

(c) (d)

FIGURE 2   Views of Tunnel A (a) eastbound and (b) westbound, and Tunnel B (c) eastbound and (d) westbound. (Source: Google Street View.)
Miller and Boyle 63

to compare across participants; the participants’ resting SDNN was sion 2.15.2). Pairwise comparison with Tukey’s honest significance
subtracted from their SDNN along each segment. difference (HSD) test was conducted to examine the differences in
the significant effects.

Data Analysis
Results
A Pearson correlation analysis showed that the standard deviation
(SD) of speed and the SD of deceleration were correlated (r = .27, Summary Statistics
p < .001). Therefore, a 7 (road segment) × 2 (direction) × 2 (gender)
× 3 (age group) multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was The study included 19 younger (9 male, 10 female), 19 middle-aged
performed to examine these two dependent measures. A univari- (9 male, 10 female), and 12 older (8 male, 4 female) drivers. The age
ate ANOVA was conducted for SDNN, as this was not correlated distributions by direction of travel were: younger (10 east, 9 west),
with the other driver performance measures. The same independent middle (10 east, 9 west), and older (7 east, 5 west). The self-reported
variables were examined. annual driving distance ranged from 1,600 to 70,000 km, with a mean
A second univariate ANOVA was also performed on mean speeds; of 16,400 km per year.
however, this analysis focused on the two tunnels only, rather than all The speeding profiles in each travel direction were examined
seven segments. For this analysis, a 2 (Tunnels A and B) × 2 (direc- graphically (Figure 3a in the eastbound direction and Figure 3b in
tion) × 2 (gender) × 3 (age group) ANOVA was performed with the the westbound direction). The data used for the plots were based
intent to examine performance differences with the two tunnels. on the mean speeds of each driver aggregated to 15-m bins. The
All the analyses used the linear mixed effects model function posted speed limit of 60 mph is represented as a horizontal dashed
in the nlme package of the statistical software program R (ver- line and the transition periods as a vertical dashed line. There was

Speed (km/h)
Speed (mph)

Mean
SD
Direction of travel

Seattle Bellevue
Distance Driven
(a)
Speed (km/h)
Speed (mph)

Mean
SD
Direction of travel

Seattle Bellevue
Distance Driven
(b)

FIGURE 3   Mean speed profiles along I-90: (a) eastbound, Seattle (Open Road 1) to Bellevue (Open
Road 3), and (b) westbound, Bellevue (Open Road 3) to Seattle (Open Road 1) (SD 5 standard deviation).
64 Transportation Research Record 2518

no observed car following in the study, given that all the drives took TABLE 2   Summary Statistics for SDNN by Road Segments
place during nonpeak hours and under free-flow traffic conditions
specifically to minimize any potential confounding effects. SDNN (ms), by Direction and Gender
For both directions of travel, three of the four tunnel segments Eastbound Westbound
(Tunnel A eastbound, Tunnels B and A westbound) had notably lower Road
speeds than the adjacent open road segments. The traveling speeds Segment Age Group Male Female Male Female
within these three tunnel segments were notably lower than the posted
speed limit. Furthermore, the lowest observable speed occurred in Open road Younger −0.05 −0.11 −0.02 −0.02
Middle aged −0.01 −0.13 −0.01 −0.01
the tunnels for both directions. For the four tunnel segments, relative Older −0.01 0.00 0.03 0.00
spikes in speed were observed at tunnel openings; there were notice- Transition Younger −0.06 −0.13 −0.05 −0.04
able drops in speed before each tunnel and increases in speed near the Middle aged −0.01 −0.16 −0.02 −0.02
tunnel exits. Eastbound Tunnel A is considerably narrower. The driver Older −0.02 −0.02 0.02 0.00
response to this tighter enclosure is reflected where eastbound Tunnel Tunnel Younger −0.05 −0.11 −0.01 −0.02
A has a lower mean speed compared with the rest of the route. These Middle aged −0.03 −0.13 −0.01 −0.01
Older −0.01 0.00 0.03 0.00
differences in speed are further analyzed in the section on inferential
statistics.
Tables 1, 2, and 3 show the summary statistics for open road, tran-
sition, and tunnel, by age, gender, and travel direction, for heart rate, Furthermore, SD speed and SD braking within each tunnel was
SDNN, and speed, respectively. The standardized heart rate revealed greater than the SD within the respective transition segment (both
that there was less variability in the tunnels in the eastbound direc- significant at p < .001).
tion of travel (Figure 4). Table 1 shows that all middle-aged drivers Significant differences in age groups were also observed [F(2, 43)
and older females in the westbound direction experienced the largest = 7.152, p = .002]. More specifically, the SDs of the younger drivers
(positive) incremental changes in heart rate compared with the rest- were greater than those of the middle-aged drivers, and this was
ing state. This was most prominent through the transition segments. significant at p = .004. No other significant outcomes were observed:
The SDNN shows that the highest workloads for all age and gender gender [F(1, 43) = 0.260, p = .613], interaction of gender and age
groups were experienced in the transition segments (Figure 5). The [F(2, 43) = 1.373, p = .264], and direction [F(1, 43) = 2.218, p = .144].
lowest speeds were observed through the tunnel segments (Table 3).
When aggregated to level of direction of travel (Figure 6), variations
in mean speeds were highest in the transitions and tunnels. Entire Segment: SDNN

The results of this test showed that road segment was significant with
Inferential Statistics F(6, 288) = 5.587, p < .001. A trend toward significance was identi-
fied for direction with F(1, 43) = 3.528, p = .067 and gender with
Entire Segment: Standard Deviation F(1, 43) = 3.236, p = .079. However, age, interaction of age and gen-
in Speed and Deceleration der, and interaction of road segment and direction were not significant
on SDNN. A Tukey HSD test on this model revealed a trend that
Road segment had a significant impact on speed and braking females experienced more stress than males (p = .09). The SDNN
[F(6, 288) = 98.923, p < .0001] as did the interaction of road seg- values were lower in the transition segment before Tunnel B com-
ment and travel direction [F(6, 288) = 4.045, p = .001]. Both tran- pared with the proceeding open road (p < .01) and compared with
sition segments (A and B) had higher SD speed and braking than being in Tunnel B (p = .10), corresponding to higher stress in the tran-
the respective open-road segment immediately before ( p < .001). sition segment. Furthermore, this transition into Tunnel B had higher

TABLE 1   Summary Statistics for Mean Standardized Heart Rate


by Road Segments TABLE 3   Summary Statistics for Mean Speed by Road Segments

Heart Rate (bpm), by Direction Mean Speed (mph), by Direction


and Gender and Gender

Eastbound Westbound Eastbound Westbound


Road Road
Segment Age Group Male Female Male Female Segment Age Group Male Female Male Female

Open road Younger −3.6 4.2 2.7 1.6 Open road Younger 59.8 54.4 58.2 56.3
Middle aged 3.3 1.4 12.3 6.4 Middle aged 59.5 56.5 56.3 56.3
Older 2.4 −3.8 −1.3 9.7 Older 57.1 52.8 55.7 52.1
Transition Younger −8.5 4.1 0.9 5.5 Transition Younger 56.7 57.3 57.0 54.6
Middle aged 6.5 3.3 14.1 11.2 Middle aged 59.9 56.2 54.5 56.0
Older 1.5 −6.5 −3.6 10.6 Older 57.7 56.3 55.7 51.6
Tunnel Younger 2.7 −1.2 1.8 −2.9 Tunnel Younger 55.8 52.7 56.8 53.6
Middle aged 1.7 0.9 4.9 0.7 Middle aged 58.4 53.0 54.8 54.8
Older 1.5 1.0 1.9 5.3 Older 56.0 53.0 55.0 49.1
Miller and Boyle 65

Open road Transition Tunnel

Standardized Heart Rate (bpm)

Seattle Bellevue Seattle Bellevue


Segment Segment

FIGURE 4   Standardized heart rate by travel direction.

Open road Transition Tunnel


Standardized SDNN (ms)

Seattle Bellevue Seattle Bellevue


Segment Segment

FIGURE 5   Standardized SDNN by travel direction.

Open road Transition Tunnel


Mean Speed (km/h)
Mean Speed (mph)

Seattle Bellevue Seattle Bellevue


Segment Segment

FIGURE 6   Mean speed by travel direction.


66 Transportation Research Record 2518

The lowest speeds were observed in Tunnel A in the eastbound


direction of travel, which is the tunnel closest to Seattle. This tunnel

Mean Speed (km/h)


Mean Speed (mph)

was the first the participant observed if the participant was traveling
eastbound. This difference in vehicle control could be associated
with the difference in configuration between travel directions. Spe-
cifically, eastbound Tunnel A split into two tunnels with narrower
shoulders (1 to 2 ft wide) and lanes (11 ft wide), whereas Tunnel A
in the westbound direction and Tunnel B in both directions had lanes
that were 11 ft wide, but shoulders that were 2 to 4 ft wide.
In general, the tunnel lanes were narrower than the open and transi-
tion segments (width 11 ft compared with 12 ft) and had less shoulder
FIGURE 7   Interaction plot of tunnel and direction on width (1 to 4 ft compared with 6 to 10 ft in the open segments). These
mean speed. dimensional differences may impact workload and vehicle control.
Differences were observed in the study participants’ heart rate and
SDNN between the tunnel and nontunnel segments. Stress was high-
est in the transition segments before tunnel entrances. Furthermore,
observed stress than the segment following Tunnel B ( p < .01). How- stress was higher within a tunnel compared with the open road, as
ever, for α = .05, the Tukey contrasts for SDNN specifically evaluating verified by SDNN. Variations for each driver’s speed and braking
the segments around Tunnel A did not expose significant differences. were greatest within tunnels and also elevated during the transition
The aggregation of westbound and eastbound in this Tukey test may segments. The differences in gender and age noted in this study have
explain this ambiguity of Tunnel A, as the tunnel design differs between also been observed in past studies that have shown age and gender
directions. differences in stress and vehicle control (24, 25). In the current study,
females had significantly higher stress compared with males across
the entire study drive, which was confirmed on the basis of their lower
Tunnel A and Tunnel B: Mean Speed values of SDNN. Younger drivers had the largest variations in speed,
as was observed by comparing participant-level standard deviations
The mean speeds were further examined between tunnels and of vehicle speed.
directions because differences were observed in this measure in Fig­ In summary, the transition segments, which have the same wide
ure 3. The results suggest that there was a significant difference lanes and shoulders as the open segments, still presented workload
between the two tunnels [F(1, 48) = 53.730, p < .0001]. The inter­ issues. Similar findings have been noted in previous studies (1, 3,
action of tunnel and direction was also significant [F(1, 48) = 4.372, 14). This finding suggests that driver perceptions of tunnels at the
p = .042]. The Tukey HSD test showed that Tunnel A had lower mean approach can still have an impact on driver stress and attention.
speeds than Tunnel B ( p < .001) and that speeds for eastbound were For design purposes, the conspicuity of relevant information and
significantly slower than westbound ( p < .01). There was also a differ- the visual cues that need to be provided before entering tunnels are
ence in gender [F(1, 43) = 7.452, p < .009]; males had faster observed important considerations within the transition segment.
speeds. The interaction of age and gender was not significant. Some studies have suggested that modifying the face of a tunnel
The interaction plot of mean speed between these two tunnels (16) or the entrance and exit lighting (13) could be designed to allevi-
for each direction of travel is provided in Figure 7. This plot further ate such stress. These suggestions would need to be examined further
clarifies the divergence in vehicle speeds between the different tun- for this particular segment, as differences may exist across different
nel designs. The change in mean speeds between Tunnels A and B geographical regions and driver populations. Further, the road sec-
is greatest for the eastbound drivers, as portrayed by the slope of the tion being examined was relatively flat with minimal vertical grade
dotted line. Conversely, the gap in observed mean speeds between changes. Dramatic changes in the vertical alignment may also increase
Tunnels A and B for the westbound drivers is smaller (slope of the the level of stress that drivers experience as they approach a tunnel.
solid line). However, in both cases (direction of travel), speeds are An examination of historical crash records along this corridor could
lowest in Tunnel A and higher in Tunnel B (line end points). also be useful for further understanding the safety implications across
each segment.
This study evaluated the impact of tunnels on driver stress, which
Discussion of Results was measured with physiological and driver performance measures.
Survey data were also collected to assess drivers’ perceptions of
The objective of this study was to examine driver performance and stress, but the outcomes did not provide additional insights on behav-
stress within tunnels. A 12-km section of I-90 between Seattle and ior in the tunnel, as this specific segment was not part of the original
Bellevue, Washington, was used for the study. This section includes study hypothesis. However, this additional information could have
two tunnels in each direction and was partitioned into seven unique been useful to compare actual performance with perceptions. The
segments of open roads, tunnels, and transitions to the tunnels. Vehi- data clearly suggest that there are driver differences and that the
cle kinematics data and physiological data (heart rate and SDNN) approach to a tunnel could be as stressful as the tunnel itself. Varia-
were used to examine performance and stress. tions in tunnel design appear to be associated with higher stress lev-
Lower traveling speeds were observed through the tunnel segments els, specifically the horizontal clearances. Although environmental
compared with the nontunnel segments. There were also notable drops constraints often impact the design of tunneled roadways, it is impor-
in speeds just before entering the tunnel and an increase on exiting tant to understand how these designs will affect drivers. Engineers
the tunnel, which suggests that drivers were uncomfortable with the should consider the internal design of the tunnels as well as the exter-
tunnel segments and this is supported by past research (1, 13). nal construction and surrounding areas. The potential to reduce crash
Miller and Boyle 67

risk can be achievable by designing tunnels with consideration of the Scale Construction and Validation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
drivers, their stress levels, and the implications of stress for safety. Vol. 25, No. 1, 2011b, pp. 96–105.
12. Ba, Y., and W. Zhang. An Empirical Approach for Driver-Driver Inter-
action Study: Attributes, Influence Factors and Framework. In Advances
in Usability Evaluation Part II (F. Rebelo and M. M. Soares, eds.). CRC
Acknowledgments Press, Boca Raton, Fla., 2013, pp. 404–413.
13. Amundsen, F. H. Studies of Driver Behaviour in Norwegian Road Tun-
This project was supported by funds provided to the second author nels. Tunneling and Underground Space Technology, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1994,
from the University of Washington. The authors acknowledge the pp. 9–17.
14. Amundsen, F. H., P. O. Roald, A. Engebretsen, and A. Ragnoy. Traffic
thoughtful comments of three anonymous reviewers. Accidents in Norwegian Subsea Tunnels. Publication 98. Directorate of
Public Roads, Norwegian Public Roads Administration, Oslo, Norway,
2005.
References 15. Calvi, A., M. R. De Blasiis, and C. Guattari. An Empirical Study of the
Effects of Road Tunnel on Driving Performance. Procedia Social and
  1. Akamatsu, M., N. Imachou, Y. Sasaki, H. Ushiro-Oka, T. Hamanaka, Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 53, 2012, pp. 1100–1110.
Y. Sakauchi, and M. Onuki. Simulator Study on Driver’s Behavior While 16. Shimojo, A., H. Takagi, and H. Onuma. A Simulation Study of Driving
Driving Through a Tunnel in a Rolling Area. Proc., Driving Simulator Performance in Long Tunnel. Proc., IEEE 6th International Conference
Conference—North America, Dearborn, Mich., National Advanced of Vehicle Navigation and Information Systems, Seattle, Wash., IEEE,
Driving Simulator, University of Iowa, Iowa City, 2003. New York, 1995, pp. 96–103.
  2. Lemke, K. Road Safety in Tunnels. In Transportation Research Record: 17. Coughlin, J. F., B. Reimer, and B. Mehler. Monitoring, Managing, and
Journal of the Transportation Research Board, No. 1740, TRB, National Motivating Driver Safety and Well-Being. IEEE Pervasive Computing,
Research Council, Washington, D.C., 2000, pp. 170–174. Vol. 10, No. 3, 2011, pp. 14–21.
 3. Verwey, W. B. Effects of Tunnel Entrances on Driver’s Physiological 18. Tornros, J. Driving Behavior in a Real and a Simulated Road Tunnel—
Condition and Performance. Report TM 1995 C-19. Human Factors A Validation Study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol. 30, No. 4,
Research Institute, Netherlands Organization for Applied Research, 1998, pp. 497–503.
Soesterberg, Netherlands, 1995. 19. Liu, Y. C., and M. H. Wen. Comparison of Head-Up Display (HUD)
  4. Ceci, R., L. Hogman, and C. Patten. Measures of Driver Behavior vs. Head-Down Display (HDD): Driving Performance of Commercial
and Cognitive Workload in a Driving Simulator and in Real Traffic Vehicle Operators in Taiwan. International Journal of Human-Computer
Environment—Experiences from Two Experimental Studies in Sweden. Studies, Vol. 61, No. 5, 2004, pp. 679–697.
Proc., 1st International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver 20. Healey, J., and R. Picard. Detecting Stress During Real-World Driving
Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, Aspen, Colo., University of Tasks Using Physiological Sensors. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent
Iowa, Iowa City, 2001. Transportation Systems, Vol. 6, No. 2, 2005, pp. 156–166.
  5. Matthews, G. Towards a Transactional Ergonomics for Driver Stress and 21. Apparies, R., T. Riniolo, and S. Porges. A Psychophysical Investigation
Fatigue. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, Vol. 3, No. 2, 2002, of the Effects of Driving Longer-Combination Vehicles. Ergonomics,
pp. 195–211. Vol. 41, No. 5, 1998, pp. 581–592.
  6. Lazarus, R. S. Stress and Emotion: A New Synthesis. Springer, New York, 22. Miller, E. E., and L. N. Boyle. Variations in Road Conditions on Driver
1999. Stress: Insights from an On-Road Study. Proceedings of the Human
  7. Oz, B., T. Ozkan, and T. Lajunen. Professional and Non-Professional Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 57, No. 1, 2013,
Drivers’ Stress Reactions and Risky Driving. Transportation Research pp. 1864–1868.
Part F, Vol. 13, 2010, pp. 32–40. 23. Lee, H., J. Kim, Y. Kim, J. Baek, M. Ryu, and K. Park. The Relationship
  8. Clapp, J. D., S. A. Olsen, S. Danoff-Burg, J. H. Hagewood, E. J. Hickling, Between HRV Parameters and Stressful Driving Situation in the Real
V. S. Hwang, and J. G. Beck. Factors Contributing to Anxious Driving Road. Proc., 6th International Special Topic Conference on Information
Behavior: The Role of Stress History and Accident Severity. Journal of Technology Applications in Biomedicine, Tokyo, Japan, IEEE, New York,
Anxiety Disorders, Vol. 25, 2011a, pp. 592–598. 2007, pp. 198–200.
  9. Taylor, J., F. P. Deane, and J. Podd. Driving Fear and Driving Skills: 24. Matthews, G., A. Tsuda, G. Xin, and Y. Ozeki. Individual Differences in
Comparison Between Fearful and Control Samples Using Standardized Driver Stress Vulnerability in a Japanese Sample. Ergonomics, Vol. 42,
On-Road Assessment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 45, 2007, No. 3, 1999, pp. 401–415.
pp. 805–818. 25. Westerman, S. J., and D. Haigney. Individual Differences in Driver
10. Matthews, G., L. Dorn, T. W. Hoyes, D. R. Davies, A. I. Glendon, and Stress, Error, and Violation. Personality and Individual Differences,
R. G. Taylor. Driver Stress and Performance on a Driving Simulator. Vol. 29, No. 5, 2000, pp. 981–998.
Human Factors, Vol. 40, 1998, pp. 136–149.
11. Clapp, J. D., S. A . Olsen, J. G. Beck, S. A . Palyo, D. M. Grant, The Standing Committee on Vehicle User Characteristics peer-reviewed this
B. Gudmundsdottir, and L. Marques. The Driving Behavior Survey: paper.

You might also like