You are on page 1of 5

Blackwell Publishing IncMalden, USATRFTransfusion0041-11322006 American Association of Blood BanksJune 200646610061010Original ArticleANXIETY, SYMPTOMS, AND DONOR RETURNDITTO AND

FRANCE

BLOOD DONORS AND BLOOD COLLECTION

Vasovagal symptoms mediate the relationship between


predonation anxiety and subsequent blood donation in
female volunteers

Blaine Ditto and Christopher R. France

F
rom the perspective of the inexperienced donor,
BACKGROUND: Although not universal, a certain the blood donation procedure involves a number
amount of predonation anxiety is common and not of potentially anxiety-eliciting features. In addi-
surprising among inexperienced blood donors. Variations tion to the possibility of physical “injury” in the
in predonation anxiety, however, may influence the form of a fingerprick, venipuncture, blood loss, and symp-
donor’s experience in several respects and might be toms such as dizziness and nausea, there is a risk of social
related to the likelihood of subsequent donation even stress or embarrassment. Reasonable or not, some people
among those who do not report particularly high levels of feel awkward about the possibility of displaying physical
anxiety. symptoms or answering personal screening interview
STUDY DESIGN AND METHODS: A total of 671 questions.1 All of these issues are probably accentuated
inexperienced blood donors (zero or one prior donation) by the uncertainty inexperienced donors have about the
enrolled in a treatment study and completed exact nature of the procedure. Fortunately, such concerns
questionnaires assessing anxiety before and after giving are outweighed (or else the person would not be in the
blood. Ratings of pain and blood donation–related clinic at all) by various altruistic motives, and most donors
symptoms were obtained after giving blood, as well as discover that the procedure is typically uneventful and not
information from the nurses about the blood donation unpleasant. Nonetheless, it may be premature to dismiss
procedure. Follow-up information on number of additional the issue and to assume that anxiety will largely habituate
visits to a blood collection clinic during the subsequent with experience, except for those who are extremely anx-
year was also obtained for most participants. ious and probably not well-suited to be blood donors any-
RESULTS: Predonation anxiety was positively related to way. While a certain degree of donor loss related to anxiety
ratings of blood donation–related symptoms and chair and donation-related symptoms is almost inevitable,
reclining by the nurses and negatively related to the there is reason to believe that there is not a clear “cutoff”
donor’s rating, obtained at the end of the procedure, of beyond which these issues do not affect donor return. For
the likelihood that he or she would give blood again.
Women, but not men, with higher predonation anxiety
were significantly less likely to return to a blood clinic in
the following year. The results of mediation analyses ABBREVIATION: BDRI = Blood Donation Reactions Inventory.
suggest that this was due to the association between From the Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal,
anxiety and donation-related symptoms. Quebec, Canada; and the Department of Psychology, Ohio
CONCLUSION: The results do not indicate whether or University, Athens, Ohio.
not it is feasible or useful to modify predonation anxiety, Address reprint requests to: Blaine Ditto, PhD, Department
but highlight the relationships between the emotional of Psychology, McGill University, 1205 Dr Penfield Avenue,
state of inexperienced donors and several aspects of the Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1, Canada; e-mail: blaine.ditto@
donation experience, including subsequent donor return. mcgill.ca.
Further study of the psychosocial aspects of blood This research was supported by grants from the Canadian
donation and examination of related research (e.g., Blood Services/Canadian Institutes of Health Research
treatment of dental anxieties) may be worthwhile. Partnership in Transfusion Science and the Fonds de la
Recherche en Santé du Québec.
Received for publication August 4, 2005; revision received
October 12, 2005, and accepted November 1, 2005.
doi: 10.1111/j.1537-2995.2006.00835.x
TRANSFUSION 2006;46:1006-1010.

1006 TRANSFUSION Volume 46, June 2006


ANXIETY, SYMPTOMS, AND DONOR RETURN

example, France and coworkers2 recently examined the return to give blood in the future. The nurses who col-
relationship between blood donation–related symptoms lected the blood reported whether any needle adjust-
assessed with the Blood Donation Reactions Inventory ments were required, a full unit of blood was obtained,
(BDRI)3 and subsequent trips to a blood collection clinic. and the donor’s chair had to be reclined to treat symptoms
The relationship was found to be continuous, with each and rated the difficulty of needle insertion. Finally, the
point increase on this 0 to 55 scale (11 symptoms, each number of subsequent trips to a blood collection clinic
rated 0-5) related to a 4 percent reduction in the donor’s was obtained, with the donor’s written permission, from
likelihood of returning to give blood within 1 year. In large the provincial blood collection agency, Héma-Québec.
part, such findings are responsible for recent interest in The analyses of donor return focused on whether or not
various techniques aimed at reducing blood donation– the donor returned to a blood clinic in the following year.
related symptoms.4-7 Similarly, it is possible that lower lev- This research was approved by the Research Ethics Boards
els of blood donation–related anxiety may be somewhat of McGill University and Héma-Québec.
more problematic than widely appreciated. This study
examined the relations between predonation anxiety and
various aspects of the blood donation procedure, symp- Statistical analysis
toms, and donor return in inexperienced donors. It was Initial analyses revealed that none of the effects of anxiety
predicted that predonation anxiety in inexperienced was mediated by treatment group. This is not surprising
donors would be negatively related to the likelihood of because applied tension is a physical muscle tensing
returning to give blood again and that this association intervention aimed at stabilizing blood pressure and facil-
would be mediated primarily by the impact of anxiety on itating blood return to the heart. Nevertheless, treatment
risk for blood donation–related symptoms. group was entered as a covariate in the analyses along
with age, body mass index, donation experience (first vs.
second time giving blood), and mother tongue (English vs.
MATERIALS AND METHODS French). Predonation anxiety score and sex were the inde-
Participants pendent variables. Analyses were conducted with the gen-
Participants were part of a trial of the effects of a behav- eral linear model because this can accommodate both
ioral technique called applied tension on blood donation– continuous (anxiety) and categorical (sex) independent
related symptoms.7,8 Although applied tension is primarily variables.
a physical, muscle tensing technique, the predonation Baron and Kenny11 described a procedure to test the
and postdonation questionnaires included an abbreviated possible role of a psychosocial variable as a mediator of
version of the widely used, well-validated Spielberger the effects of another variable. That is, if a researcher
State Anxiety Questionnaire.9 A total of 1254 donors at believes that Variable B mediates the relationship between
mobile clinics held in colleges and universities partici- Variables A and C, then A should predict B and C, B should
pated in the trial, which included 671 individuals who predict C, and the regression coefficient reflecting the
have given blood on no more than one previous occasion relationship between A and C should become nonsignifi-
(the median value). The sample was composed of 245 cant if B is entered in the same equation. The possibility
men, 426 women (461 first-time donors and 210 second- that the predicted relationship between predonation anx-
time donors) and had a mean age of 20.1 (SD 3.9) years. iety and the return of inexperienced donors is mediated
by blood donation–related symptoms was evaluated in
this manner.
Measures The distributions of the variables were examined
To minimize the impact of the study on the blood collec- before analysis. To reduce skewness, BDRI scores were
tion procedure, the predonation questionnaire included log-transformed. The criterion for statistical significance
an abbreviated version of the Spielberger State Anxiety was p < 0.05.
Scale.9 With data from a pilot study for this project,10 factor
analysis was used to identify the five highest loading items RESULTS
from the full 20-item scale on the principal factor. Each of
these five items is rated on a 0 to 3 scale, creating a maxi- Predonation anxiety
mum possible score of 15 on the abbreviated version. Par- The blood donation procedure began similarly for anxious
ticipants completed the anxiety scale a second time, on and nonanxious donors. Predonation anxiety was not
the postdonation questionnaire. The postdonation ques- associated with the nurse’s rating of the ease of needle
tionnaire also included the BDRI3 and several 167-mm insertion or the need for a needle adjustment. Predonation
visual analog scales. Ratings of pain produced by the pre- anxiety, however, was significantly associated with blood
donation fingerprick and venipuncture were requested donation–related symptoms assessed by the BDRI
and an overall judgment of the likelihood that they would (F1,629 = 22.12, p < 0.001; (Fig. 1)). This does not seem to

Volume 46, June 2006 TRANSFUSION 1007


DITTO AND FRANCE

0.7 30
BDRI score (log units)

Chair reclined (%)


0.6
20

0.5

10
0.4

0.3 0
0,1 2,3 4,5 >5 0,1 2,3 4,5 >5

Predonation anxiety score Predonation anxiety score


Fig. 1. Anxiety quartiles and self-reported donation-related Fig. 2. Anxiety quartiles and chair reclining by nurses.
symptoms.

have been related simply to a general 70


tendency to complain because predona- 60
tion anxiety was also positively related
Returned (%)

50
to the frequency of chair reclining by the
nurses (F1,533 = 5.38, p = 0.021; (Fig. 2)). 40

Consistent with the literature indicating 30


a positive association between pain and 20
“relevant” anxiety (anxiety related to the
10
pain-producing procedure or environ-
12
ment), anxiety was associated with 0
0 3 6 9 12 15
ratings of both fingerprick (F1,626 = 4.93,
p = 0.027) and venipuncture (F1,624 = Predonation anxiety score
5.99, p = 0.015) pain.
Fig. 3. Female blood donor return in relation to predonation anxiety score.
At the end of their donation experi-
ence, there was a significant main effect
of anxiety on the donors’ rating of the likelihood they likelihood of return (OR, 0.51; p = 0.005). That is, inexperi-
would give blood again (F1,619 = 14.78, p < 0.001). Those enced female donors reporting more dizziness, etc., were
with higher predonation anxiety rated their chances of less likely to return, independent of age, body mass index,
returning as significantly lower than those who were more and treatment group. When both predonation anxiety and
at ease. These judgments appear to have been relatively BDRI scores were included in the equation, the effect of
accurate, at least for women. Follow-up data revealed a predonation anxiety on return was not significant (OR,
significant interaction between predonation anxiety and 0.95; p = 0.203), whereas the effect of BDRI score on return
sex in whether or not the individual volunteered to give remained significant (OR, 0.53; p = 0.011). Following
blood again during the year (F1,482 = 4.22, p = 0.040). Sepa- Baron and Kenny,11 this suggests that the impact of predo-
rate analyses of data from men and women indicated that nation anxiety on the return of inexperienced female
predonation anxiety was not related to whether or not the donors is mediated by increased likelihood of blood dona-
donor returned among men. A separate GLM of data from tion–related symptoms.
women, however, revealed a significant effect of predona-
tion anxiety (F1,310 = 4.06, p = 0.045; (Fig. 3)). When ana-
lyzed with logistic regression, the odds ratio (OR) was 0.93 Postdonation anxiety
(p = 0.045). Similar and generally stronger relations were observed
between these measures and ratings of postdonation anx-
iety. For example, postdonation anxiety was strongly with
Mediation analyses associated with BDRI score (F1,631 = 62.82, p < 0.001). A sig-
When BDRI score replaced predonation anxiety in this nificant negative association between postdonation anxi-
regression equation predicting donor return in women, ety and whether or not a full unit of blood was obtained
BDRI score was significantly associated with a decreased was also observed (F1,538 = 22.01, p < 0.001; in contrast to a

1008 TRANSFUSION Volume 46, June 2006


ANXIETY, SYMPTOMS, AND DONOR RETURN

more modest association with predonation anxiety, which tain euphoria, which increases the likelihood of subse-
only approached significance [F1,535 = 2.82, p = 0.094]). quent donation. Although it is clear that people often feel
Although noteworthy, these ratings of anxiety are more good about themselves after giving blood,24 these results
difficult to interpret because they could reflect distress indicate little positive benefit to predonation anxiety and
that was the result of an unpleasant experience produced suggest that only mild elevations may have negative
by some factor other than predonation anxiety. effects on the experience of inexperienced donors. For
example, the second quartile of predonation anxiety in
Figs. 1 and 2 corresponds to a predonation anxiety score
DISCUSSION
of 2 or 3. Because the anxiety questions were rated on a 0
This study provides several important pieces of informa- to 3 scale, this would require only two symptoms rated as
tion. Most important, the results tie together several dis- mild. Yet these individuals reported significantly more
tinct findings and point to the possible importance of blood donation–related symptoms and had their chairs
searching for ways to reduce anxiety in inexperienced vol- reclined significantly more than donors who were more at
unteers. Donor anxiety has been related to risk for vasova- ease when entering the clinic.
gal symptoms in a number of studies,3,13-16 and blood That said, there were several limitations of the study.
donation–related symptoms have been linked to reduced Despite the prospective nature of the associations, the
donor return.2,17-19 The results of this study indicate that present results do not demonstrate causal effects of pre-
apprehensive inexperienced female blood donors are donation anxiety on symptoms and donor return. For
more likely to have unpleasant symptoms and, as a result, example, it is possible that people who anticipate symp-
less likely to return to give blood again. The chain of rea- toms based on previous experiences with injections,
soning is unsurprising, but evidence for this belief has blood draws, etc., enter the blood donation clinic with
been largely anecdotal to date. somewhat greater anxiety, but the relationship between
The relationship between predonation anxiety and an anxiety and subsequent symptoms is an artifact of the
objective characteristic of the blood donation procedure, donor’s accurate assessment of their susceptibility to
that is, whether the donor’s chair is reclined by the nurse, symptoms. Related to this idea, the results do not neces-
suggests that the effects of anxiety on reports of symptoms sarily imply that strategies aimed at reducing predonation
such as dizziness and nausea on the BDRI were not simply anxiety (e.g., distraction with movies) will lead to fewer
the product of reporting bias or hypochondriasis. The symptoms and improved donor retention. On the other
nurses did not view donor questionnaires and presumably hand, this neglects extensive literature on anxiety about
decided whether or not to recline the chair based on their medical procedures, much of which is relevant to the topic
objective judgment of the donor’s need for this interven- of blood donation. For example, behavioral interventions
tion. Although it is possible that some nurses may have for dental anxieties have been found to affect both self-
been influenced by anxious donors who did not really report anxiety and dental attendance25 even with auto-
need their chairs to be reclined but who expressed distress mated interventions.26 One particularly interesting feature
verbally or nonverbally, this seems unlikely for at least two about the literature on dental anxieties is the fact that
reasons. First, most of the nurses working in these clinics dental attendance has been increased among individuals
had considerable experience in blood collection and the with dental phobias, that is, people who avoid dental
recognition of blood donation–related symptoms. Second, exams, as well as those with less pronounced fears. This
we recently observed no relationship between chair reclin- raises the idea of possible programs for those who might
ing and a personality characteristic (alexithymia) strongly otherwise be interested in giving blood but who have
related to emotional expressivity.20 In sum, it appears that never done so before, in part out of fear about the proce-
anxious donors were more likely to experience real blood dure.27,28 Whether the focus is on retaining or recruiting
donation–related symptoms. As a result, anxious female new donors, the study of psychosocial aspects of blood
donors were less likely to return to give blood again. donation is a useful area that may contribute to increased
Another useful aspect of this study is the observation blood donor satisfaction and, perhaps, blood supplies.
of a linear relationship between predonation anxiety and
blood donation–related symptoms (Fig. 1). The relation-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
ship between predonation anxiety and syncope has been
appreciated for many years.13,14 The relationship between The assistance of the staff and organization of Héma-Québec is
anxiety and less dramatic symptoms, however, has acknowledged.
received much less attention and concern, although sev-
eral studies have examined personality correlates of dona-
REFERENCES
tion anxiety.3,16,21,22 Zillmer and colleagues23 suggested that
the reduction of anxiety after blood donation and feeling 1. Mair M, Barrett SP, Campbell T, Ditto B. Prevalence,
that one has done a good thing might even lead to a cer- disclosure and interpretations of sexual activities in a

Volume 46, June 2006 TRANSFUSION 1009


DITTO AND FRANCE

sample of Canadian college-aged blood donors. Int J STD Questionnaire as a predictor of blood/injury fear and
AIDS 2003;14:399-403. fainting. Behav Res Ther 1990;28:429-37.
2. France CR, France JL, Roussos M, Ditto B. Mild reactions to 16. Labus J, France CR, Taylor BK. Vasovagal reactions in
blood donation predict a decreased likelihood of donor volunteer blood donors: analyzing the predictive power of
return. Transfus Apheresis Sci 2004;30:17-22. the Medical Fears Survey. Int J Behav Med 2000;7:62-72.
3. Meade MA, France CR, Peterson LM. Predicting vasovagal 17. Thomson RA, Bethel J, Lo AY, et al. Retention of “safe” blood
reactions in volunteer blood donors. J Psychosom Res donors. The Retrovirus Epidemiology Donor Study.
1996;40:495-501. Transfusion 1998;38:359-67.
4. Hanson SA, France CR. Predonation water ingestion 18. Staallekker LA, Stammeijer RN, Dudok de Wit C. A Dutch
attenuates negative reactions to blood donation. blood bank and its donors. Transfusion 1980;20:66-70.
Transfusion 2004;44:924-8. 19. Piliavin JA. Why do they give the gift of life? A review of
5. Bonk VA, France CR, Taylor BK. Distraction reduces self- research on blood donors since 1977. Transfusion 1990;
reported physiological reactions to blood donation in novice 30:444-59.
donors with a blunting coping style. Psychosom Med 20. Byrne N, Ditto B. Alexithymia, cardiovascular reactivity, and
2001;63:447-52. symptom reporting during blood donation. Psychosom Med
6. Sauer LA, France CR. Caffeine attenuates vasovagal 2005;67:471-5.
reactions in female first-time blood donors. Health Psychol 21. Kaloupek DG, Scott JR, Khatami V. Assessment of coping
1999;18:403-9. strategies associated with syncope in blood donors.
7. Ditto B, France CR, Lavoie P, et al. Reducing reactions to J Psychosom Res 1985;29:207-14.
blood donation with applied muscle tension: a randomized 22. Kaloupek DG, White H, Wong M. Multiple assessment
controlled trial. Transfusion 2003;43:1269-75. of coping strategies used by volunteer blood donors:
8. Ditto B, France CR. The effects of applied tension on implications for preparatory training. J Behav Med
symptoms in French-speaking blood donors: a randomized 1984;7:35-60.
trial. Health Psychol 2006;in press. 23. Zillmer EA, Glidden RA, Honaker LM, Meyer JD. Mood states
9. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. State-trait anxiety in the volunteer blood donor. Transfusion 1989;29:27-30.
inventory manual. Palo Alto (CA): Consulting Psychologists 24. Sojka B, Sojka P. The blood-donation experience: perceived
Press; 1970. physical, psychological and social impact of blood donation
10. Ditto B, Wilkins JA, France CR, et al. On-site training in on the donor. Vox Sang 2003;84:120-8.
applied muscle tension to reduce vasovagal reactions to 25. Kvale G, Berggren U, Milgrom P. Dental fear in adults: a
blood donation. J Behav Med 2003;26:53-65. meta-analysis of behavioral interventions. Community Dent
11. Baron R, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable Oral Epidemiol 2004;32:250-64.
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, 26. Coldwell SE, Getz T, Milgrom P, et al. CARL: a LabVIEW 3
strategic, and statistical considerations. J Personality Social computer program for conducting exposure therapy for the
Psychol 1986;51:1173-82. treatment of dental injection fear. Behav Res Ther 1998;
12. Janssen SA, Arntz A. Anxiety and pain: attentional and 36:429-41.
endorphinergic influences. Pain 1996;66:145-50. 27. Hupfer ME, Taylor DW, Letwin JA. Understanding Canadian
13. Callahan R, Edelman EB, Smith MS, Smith JJ. Study of the student motivations and beliefs about giving blood.
incidence and characteristics of blood donor “reactors.” Transfusion 2005;45:149-61.
Transfusion 1963;3:76-82. 28. Boulware LE, Ratner LE, Ness PM, et al. The contribution of
14. Graham DT. Prediction of fainting in blood donors. sociodemographic, medical, and attitudinal factors to blood
Circulation 1961;23:901-6. donation among the general public. Transfusion 2002;42:
15. Kleinknecht RA, Thorndike RM. The Mutilation 669-78.

1010 TRANSFUSION Volume 46, June 2006

You might also like