You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3469-1

ORIGINAL PAPER

A Quantitative Analysis of Mental Health Among Sexual and Gender


Minority Groups in ASD
Rita George1 · Mark A. Stokes1

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
There is increased mental-health adversity among individuals with autism spectrum disorder. At the same time, sexual and
gender minority groups experience poorer mental-health when compared to heteronormative populations. Recent research
suggests that autistic individuals report increased non-heterosexuality and gender-dysphoric traits. The current study aimed
to investigate whether as membership of minority grouping becomes increasingly narrowed, mental health worsened. The
present study compared the rates of depression, anxiety, and stress using the DASS-21 and Personal Well-Being using the
personal well-being index between 261 typically-developing individuals and 309 autistic individuals. As membership to a
minority group became more restrictive, mental health symptoms worsened (p < .01), suggesting stressors added. Specialized
care is recommended for this vulnerable cohort.

Keywords  Autism spectrum disorder · Sexuality · Sexual orientation · Gender · Well-being

Introduction between 5 and 17% (Bijl et al. 1998; Jacobi et al. 2004).
Similarly, when comparing the prevalence rates of anxi-
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a lifelong neurodevel- ety in ASD which is between 50 and 56% (Hofvander et al.
opmental condition that affects, among other things, the way 2009; Lugnegård et al. 2011), these rates are much higher
an individual relates to their environment and their interac- compared to reports in the general population, which range
tion with other people. While some individuals with ASD between 3 and 12% (Bijl et al. 1998; Jacobi et al. 2004).
have meaningful relationships and lead fulfilling lives, the At the same time, minority groups such as non-hetero-
quality of life among many with ASD is lower compared to sexual individuals (Sandfort et al. 2001) and individuals
their typically-developing (TD) peers (van Heijst and Geurts with gender-dysphoria (Hepp et al. 2005; Toomey et al.
2014; Billstedt et al. 2010). Individuals with ASD face a 2010) suffer from a greater burden of depressive and anxi-
range of difficulties owing to challenges from social com- ety disorders, report poorer life-satisfaction (Powdthavee
munication, social comprehension and imagination, which and Wooden 2015; Davey et al. 2014), and a higher rate of
adversely impact their adaptation to a neurotypical world. suicide attempts (Haas et al. 2014) compared to heterosexual
Accordingly, accumulating evidence from clinical practice individuals and individuals without gender-dysphoria. Gen-
and epidemiological research confirms that the rates of psy- der-dysphoria is a clinical condition where individuals expe-
chiatric comorbidity are higher among individuals with ASD rience a persistent discontent over the incongruence between
than the general population (Bolton et al. 1998; Mannion and their experienced and assigned gender, leading to significant
Leader 2013; Piven and Palmer 1999). The prevalence of distress, challenges in social or occupational functioning,
mood disorders in ASD is between 53 and 70% (Lugnegård and a desire to live a cross-gender life (American Psychiatric
et al. 2011; Hofvander et al. 2009) compared to much lower Association 2013).
prevalence rates in the general population, ranging from Research suggests that individuals with ASD report
higher rates of non-heterosexuality than their TD peers
* Mark A. Stokes (Hellemans et al. 2007, 2010; Gilmour et al. 2012; George
mark.stokes@deakin.edu.au and Stokes 2017; George 2016). Additionally, empirical
reports suggest an association between ASD and gender-
1
Faculty of Health, Deakin University, 221 Burwood dysphoric traits (GDT; George and Stokes 2016; DeVries
Highway, Burwood, VIC 3030, Australia

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

et al. 2010; Jones et al. 2012; Pasterski et al. 2013). Given Furthermore, it was hypothesized that all non-heterosexual
increased mental health adversity in the ASD population, individuals (sexual minority group) would have higher rates
and similar outcomes among non-heterosexual individuals, of Depression, Anxiety and Stress and reduced levels of Per-
and individuals with GDT, concerns may be additive for sonal Wellbeing than all heterosexual individuals, and there-
autistic individuals from a sexual and/or gender minority fore, reduced mental health. Lastly, it was hypothesized that
group, relative to their autistic peers who are heterosexual all individuals from a gender minority group (having GDT)
and without GDT. For the purposes of this study, sexual would have higher rates of Depression, Anxiety and Stress
minority group refers to individuals with a non-heterosex- and lower levels of Personal Wellbeing than all individu-
ual orientation, while gender minority group refers to indi- als without GDT, and therefore, reduced mental health. We
viduals with GDT. Moreover, an individual can belong to predict that as of minority grouping membership becomes
more than one minority group, i.e. an individual may be increasingly narrowed (i.e.: an individual is a member of
both non-heterosexual and have GDT. A conceptual model an increasing number of minority groups), so will the rates
is presented in Fig. 1 displaying one minority group within of Depression, Anxiety and Stress increase, while levels of
another minority group, and how stressors of one group Personal Wellbeing will decrease. That is, mental health
could add to that of another group. will worsen as minority group membership becomes more
While there is considerable evidence that individuals restrictive.
from sexual minority groups (Meyer 2003) and gender
minority groups (Toomey et al. 2010) experience poor psy-
chiatric health relative to their heterosexual peers and those Method
without GDT, there is a lack of research investigating the
mental health of individuals who are diagnosed with an ASD Participants
from a sexual and gender minority group. This study aimed
to address this gap using validated psychometric measures The present study compared 261 TD individuals (Mage
in an international sample of individuals with ASD. Mental = 30.20, SD = 11.92; 103 males and 158 females) with
health was evaluated for each participant based upon their 309 individuals with a diagnosis of ASD (Mage = 31.01,
rates of Depression, Anxiety, Stress and Personal well-being. SD = 11.37; 90 males, 219 females). Ethnic background is
This study aimed to assess (a) whether belonging to a sexual reported in Table 1. Additionally, the sample was largely
minority group further reduced the mental health of some- highly educated (see Table 1). There were no significant
one with ASD and (b) whether belonging to a gender minor-
ity group further reduced the mental health of someone with
ASD. Accordingly, it was hypothesized that all individuals Table 1  Demographic characteristics of the study sample
with ASD would have higher rates of Depression, Anxi- ASD (N = 310) TD (N = 261)
ety and Stress and lower levels of Personal Wellbeing than
Race/ethnicity %
all TD individuals, and therefore, reduced mental health.
Caucasian 81.6 71.4
African-American 0.6 1.1
Hispanic 1.0 2.2
Latino 0.3 0.5
Middle-Eastern 0.0 3.8
Asian 1.9 8.1
Pacific Islander 0.0 0.0
Indigenous or Australian Aboriginal 0.3 0.0
Multiracial 8.1 7.0
Other/rather not say 6.2 6.0
Education level %
Primary school 2.7 1.3
High school 17.7 24.2
Trade/Vocational school 7.4 5.8
Some University 29.7 25.5
Bachelor’s degree 23.2 25.8
Master’s degree 10.3 11.9
Fig. 1  Conceptual model of a ‘minority within a minority’. Each level Doctoral degree 3.5 2.9
rests upon the foundation of the prior level and includes all stressors Other/not sure 5.5 2.6
of the prior level. As levels increase, the amount of stress increases

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

differences between the individuals with and without an cohabitation status. This was followed by administration of
ASD on ethnicity or their levels of education. the different psychometric measures.

Demographic Trends
Sell Scale of Sexual Orientation
Chi square tests were conducted to examine cohabitation
rates among different minority groups. Individuals with an The Sell Scale of Sexual Orientation (Gonsiorek et al. 1995)
ASD were less likely to report cohabiting with a romantic/ is a comprehensive measure of Sexual Orientation. It is a
sexual partner than were TD individuals, (χ2(571) = 49.81, well-validated instrument which demonstrates construct
p < .001, φ = 0.29), Relative to their heterosexual peers, non- validity in its strong correlation with the Kinsey scale of
heterosexual individuals were less likely to report cohabiting sexual orientation (r = 0.85), while test–retest reliability is
with a romantic or sexual partner, (χ2(571) = 45.81, p < .001, demonstrated in correlations ranging upward from 0.93 (for
φ = 0.28). Relative to individuals without GDT, individu- further details please refer to George 2016 unpublished doc-
als with GDT were less likely to report cohabiting with a toral thesis).
romantic/sexual partner, (χ2(571) = 44.16, p < .001, φ = 0.28).
Chi square tests were also conducted to assess for group Gender‑Identity/Gender‑Dysphoria Questionnaire
differences on presence of a comorbid psychiatric diagno- for Adolescents and Adults (GIDYQ‑AA)
sis. Individuals with an ASD were more likely to report a
comorbid psychiatric diagnosis than were TD individuals, The GIDYQ-AA (Deogracias et al. 2007) is a standardized
(χ2(571) = 76.59, p < .001, φ = 0.37). Relative to their hetero- 27-item instrument, which assesses cross-gender behavior
sexual peers, non-heterosexual individuals were more likely and different aspects of GD among adolescents and adults,
to have a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis, (χ2(571) = 67.42, with the past 12 months as the time frame. The response
p < .001, φ = 0.34). Relative to individuals without GDT, options are coded along a five-point scale as 1(always), 2
individuals with GDT were more likely to report a comor- (often), 3(sometimes), 4(rarely), or 5(never). A cut-off mean
bid psychiatric diagnosis, (χ2(571) = 63.72, p < .001, φ = 0.34, score of 3.00 is recommended by the authors to discrimi-
see Fig. 2). nate between individuals with and without GD, which has
a sensitivity of 90.4% for clients with GD and a specificity
Procedure and Measures of 99.7% for controls (Deogracias et al. 2007). Gender-dys-
phoric status (No-GDT versus GDT) referred to whether an
Upon receiving ethical approval for the research [Blinded], individual expressed GDT or not. No-GDT (never or rarely
national and international autism organizations were con- experienced gender-dysphoric feelings) versus GDT (some-
tacted, including autism forums. TD participants were times or always experienced gender-dysphoric feelings).
recruited by word of mouth and through social media such
as Facebook. The study was solely conducted in electronic
format. Participants first completed a series of demographic Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale‑21 (DASS‑21)
questions related to their age, country of residence, race,
and level of education, birth-sex, gender-identity, diagnos- The DASS-21 is a short form of Lovibond and Lovibond’s
tic status, comorbid medical/psychiatric conditions, and (1995) 42-item self-report measure of depression, anxiety
and stress. The DASS- 21 has several advantages over the
Non-Heterosexual
full-length version, in being shorter, without sacrificing on
Heterosexual
reliability and having a cleaner latent structure (Henry and
Crawford 2005). Higher scores on each of the three scales
GDT indicate greater severity of the distress. Because the DASS-
No-GDT 21 is a shortened version of the DASS, the final score was
doubled. The DASS had excellent psychometric properties
ASD
with reported internal reliabilities of 0.91, 0.84, and 0.90
TD
for Depression, Anxiety and Stress, respectively (Lovibond
0 20 40 60 80
and Lovibond 1995). In the present study, strong Cronbach
Co-morbid Psychiatric Diagnoses by Group %
alphas of 0.92, 0.87, and 0.89 were obtained for Depression,
Anxiety and Stress respectively. It also has excellent conver-
Fig. 2  Comparison of comorbid psychiatric diagnoses between
gent validity with other scales designed to measure Depres-
minority groups (Black) versus control groups (Striped). Psychiatric
diagnoses included depression, bipolar manic depression, stress, anxi- sion (Beck Depression Inventory: r = .74) and Anxiety (Beck
ety, post-traumatic stress disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder Anxiety Inventory: r = .81; Lovibond and Lovibond 1995).

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Personal Wellbeing Index (PWI) depression, anxiety and stress and lower levels of well-
being as indicated by the Personal Wellbeing Index (PWI)
The Personal Wellbeing of participants was assessed using and therefore, reduced mental health, and that with more
the Personal Wellbeing Index scale (Davern et al. 2007). restricted minority group membership (i.e.: having both
The PWI contains seven items measuring satisfaction cor- GDT and being an individual with ASD, etc.) would be
responding to a quality of life domain as: standard of living, associated with even worse mental health, we undertook
personal health, achieving in life, personal relationships, a repeated measures MANOVA. We assessed diagnosis
safety, community-connectedness, and future security. These (TD, ASD), sexual orientation (heterosexual, non-heter-
domains are summed up to yield an average score. For the osexual), and GDT-Status (No-GDT, GDT) over mental
purposes of the analysis, an individual’s PWI score was health. Mental health was assessed as a repeated meas-
scored following Davern, Cummins and Stokes (2007), and urement, based on scores for depression, anxiety, stress,
then was recoded using the formula: and PWI. When dealing with multiple IVs (diagnosis,
GDT-status, sexual orientation) and a repeated measures
(100 − x) variable as the DV (PWI, depression, anxiety, stress) in
Score = C ∗ (1)
100 a repeated measures analysis, the repeated measurement
where C is the constant, set to 42, required to convert par- interacts with the IVs or predictors. The shape or profile
ticipant scores to the same metric as the DASS, and the of each groups’ responses in the DVs becomes another
negation reverses the direction of the scores. This is a sim- analytic IV, and this complication allows for differences
ple linear transformation of the data for the purposes of the in profile or shape of the DVs to be considered (see
analysis only. By reversing and placing the PWI into the Tabachnick and Fidell 2001).
same metric as the DASS for the analysis, the PWI scores Initially, a single MANOVA was planned, using a single
did not cross the DASS scores simply due to differences in 4-way interaction (mental health by diagnosis by GDT-status
their underlying metrics, which would have given rise to by sexual orientation). This would give rise to four 3-way
a spurious interaction between measures of mental health interactions, six 2-way interactions, and 4 main effects tests,
(e.g.: DASS & PWI) in the profile analysis. (Note for all for a total of 15 planned tests. Following the initial analysis,
tables raw PWI scores are reported; though for figures trans- the single 4-way design was not found significant. Subse-
formed PWI data are presented.) quently, we undertook two 3-way analyses (mental health by
After transformation, higher scores on the PWI repre- diagnosis by sexual orientation; and mental health by diag-
sented lower Personal Wellbeing. This enabled the PWI and nosis by GDT-status) resulting in a further 7 tests each: one
the DASS scales to be interpreted in a linear manner, where 3-way interaction, three 2-way interactions, and three main
higher scores on all scales represented greater mental dis- effects tests. Thereafter, nine tests of simple main effect were
tress. The PWI scale has sound psychometric properties. undertaken involving 30 additional tests. In total, therefore,
Cronbach’s alpha is reported to be 0.85. The index has also there were 15 planned tests and 44 unplanned follow-up tests
demonstrated good test–retest reliability across a 1–2-week used. Additionally, a single Spearman’s Rho correlation was
interval with a correlation coefficient of 0.84 (Lau et al. planned relating mental health status to minority status.
2005). Construct validity is demonstrated through a corre- Consequently, we used a MANOVA Protection approach
lation of 0.78 with the Satisfaction with Life scale (Diener with Sidak correction (Frane 2015) to adjust the type I error
et al. 1985). rate, requiring that we obtain results more significant than
p < .001 to maintain an experimentwise error rate of p < .05.
Data Screening Where Mauchly’s test of sphericity was violated, this was
corrected for using a Huyhh-Feldt corrections (Barcikowski
Data were screened for missing values, outliers, and nor- and Robey 1984; Girden 1992; Huynh and Feldt 1976).
mality. Less than 5% of data was found to be missing on
the variables of interest, and cases with missing data were
excluded from analyses employing pairwise deletion where Results
necessary. No univariate or multivariate outliers were found.
No issues were identified with normality of the data. The means (Table 2) revealed all minority groups (ASD,
non-heterosexual, and those with GDT) reported reduced
Data Analytic Plan mental health (depression, anxiety, stress, and personal
wellbeing) compared to their non-minority group peers.
In order to address the hypotheses that those within the The hypothesis that with increasingly narrow minority group
ASD group, non-heterosexual group, or gender minor- membership (i.e.: having both GDT and being an individ-
ity group (having GDT) would have higher rates of ual with ASD, etc.) mental health would further deteriorate

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 2  Mental health scores by Group Mental health profile indicators


group membership
Depression Anxiety Stress PWI
M SD M SD M SD M SD

Diagnosis***
ASD 19.14 11.82 15.23 10.67 22.45 10.35 49.79 20.15
TD 10.84 10.37 7.62 7.95 13.86 9.56 69.66 21.21
Sexual orientation***
Non-Heterosexual 18.97 11.75 15.26 10.41 21.93 10.26 49.89 20.34
Heterosexual 11.48 10.86 8.04 8.66 14.94 10.35 68.35 21.61
GDT-status***
GDT group 20.02 11.34 15.26 10.36 22.36 9.73 49.34 20.31
No-GDT group 9.00 9.51 6.97 7.87 13.31 10.15 71.84 19.60

***p < .001

non-heterosexuality), those with two or more minority group


memberships (i.e.: ASD & GDT, ASD & non-heterosexual-
ity, etc.) could have a considerably increased or even reduced
mental health burden rather than a burden that was the sim-
ple addition of mental health burden inherited from each
group membership. If this impost on mental health is addi-
tive, there should either be no interaction between groups
with more restrictive minorities, or an ordinal interaction,
where the effect of additional group membership only
increases or reduces the total effect. To test this, we under-
took a repeated measures MANOVA assessing the 4-way
Fig. 3  Mean mental health score by group membership. Scores interaction between diagnosis, sexual orientation, GDT-sta-
include the reversed and re-coded PWI, and the DASS scores. Data is tus and mental health, and all lesser interactions and effects.
sorted by membership of increasingly restrictive minorities The 4-way interaction was not significant, and its effect
size accounted for very little variance (F(2.66, 1465.88) = 0.53,
p = ns, η2 < 0.001). All lower order 3-way and 2-way inter-
suggests that membership of each minority would add or action effects were also not significant (η2 < 0.008). Thus,
increase mental health burden. To evaluate this, we corre- mental health burden did not increase due to an interaction
lated minority status with mental health score. To derive of group membership. This suggests that any increases in
minority status, individuals were classified on the number mental health burden due to increasingly restricted group
of minorities they belonged to. For instance, an individual membership were additive to each other (see Fig. 4).
who did not identify with any minority group was classified Given the lack of significance from all interactions, we
as having no minority status (x = 0), while individuals who examined the main effects, all of which were significant:
classified themselves as non-heterosexual, ASD, or reported diagnosis (F(1, 551) = 28.76, p < .001, ƞ2 = .050); sexuality
GDT, was classified as having one minority representation orientation (F(1, 551) = 11.14, p < .001, ƞ2 = .020); and GDT-
(x = 1), individuals who had two such group memberships status (F(1, 551) = 43.51, p < .001, ƞ2 = .070). Thus, mental
were assigned a value of two (x = 2), and so on. The cor- health depended on group membership, and did not change
relation was significant (Spearman’s r = 0.60, p < .001 two at different levels of the other IVs (diagnosis, sexual orien-
tailed). Thus, as minority status increased, mental health tation, and GDT-status). Individuals with an ASD reported
symptoms worsened (see Fig. 3). poorer mental health than TD individuals, while individuals
As we found that mental heath burden increased with who were non-heterosexual reported poorer mental health
membership of increasingly restrictive minority groups, it than heterosexual individuals, and individuals with GDT
was necessary to establish if group membership was addi- reported poorer mental health than individuals without GDT.
tive or if these interacted with each other. If group mem- It was possible that a 4-way interaction failed because of
berships interact, this would mean that compared to those reduced power. Some design cells (i.e.: TD, non-heterosex-
with a single minority group membership (i.e.: ASD, GDT, ual individuals without GDT) had as few as 16 participants,

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

orientation by mental health (η2 = 0.003), for diagnosis by


mental health (η2 = 0.001), or sexual orientation by men-
tal health (η2 = 0.001; see Table 3). Mental health burden
was not changed by diagnosis and was not changed by
sexual orientation (see Fig. 5). Nonetheless, the interaction
between diagnosis and sexual orientation was significant
(F(1, 555) = 20.65, p < .001, ƞ2 = .036), accounting for 3.6%
of variance. Overall, mental health score depended upon an
individual’s sexual orientation as well as their diagnosis. In
order to explore this interaction further, simple main effects
were undertaken. Initially, sexual orientation separating over
diagnostic status was explored (see Table 4). For TD individ-
uals, those with different sexual orientations (heterosexual
versus non-heterosexual) had a significantly different mental
health burden (F(1, 256) = 92.95, p < .001, ƞ2 = .27), but this
was not found for individuals with ASD (ƞ2 = .012). Being
non-heterosexual added to the mental health burden of TD
individuals (M = 17.14, SD = 6.44) with non-heterosexual
TD individuals having worse mental health than heterosex-
ual TD individuals (M = 8.69, SD = 6.44). Thereafter, we
examined the simple main effect of diagnosis separating over
sexual orientation. For those with heterosexual orientation
there was a significant simple main effect over diagnosis
(F(1, 272) = 115.90, p < .001, ƞ2 = .299), but not those with a
non-heterosexual orientation (ƞ2 = .025). Overall, as may be
seen in Table 4, among heterosexual individuals, a diagnosis
of ASD worsened mental health, while among those with
non-heterosexual sexual orientation, a diagnosis of ASD did
not worsen mental health. In summary, being in one minor-
ity group resulted in increased mental health burden, but this
was not significantly increased by being in two minorities
(ASD and non-heterosexual).
A further 3-way repeated measures MANOVA was under-
taken to assess for the effect of diagnosis and GDT-status
on mental health. The 3-way interaction between diagno-
Fig. 4  Profile of mental health for each of groups evaluated. While
the PWI was rescored for the analysis by reversing and rescaling to sis, GDT-status and mental health profile was not signifi-
42, ensuring all variables were directionally and scale equivalent, cant (ƞ2 = .003; see Table 5), suggesting that individuals
and preventing spurious interactions arising in the analysis, for ease with and without an ASD did not have a different profile of
of interpretation, here the data is presented unadjusted. Thus, here
mental health based on their GDT-status. The 2way interac-
low PWI indicates a poorer outcome, as do higher scores for stress,
depression, and anxiety. a Mental health profiles based on diagnostic tion between diagnosis and mental health profile was also
status b mental health profiles based on sexual orientation c mental not significant (ƞ2 = .001; see Table 5), suggesting that the
health profiles based on GDT status mental health profile ratings did not differ for those with
and without ASD. The 2way interaction between GDT-
status and mental health profile was also not significant
and others had only 40 (i.e.: non-heterosexuals without GDT (ƞ2 = .008; see Fig. 4). However, the interaction between
who had a diagnosis of ASD). As simpler analyses would be diagnosis and GDT-status was significant (F(1, 555) = 13.65,
more sensitive, we undertook two separate 3-way repeated p < .001, ƞ2 = .024; see Table 5), accounting for 2.4% of
measures MANOVA analyses to assess for (a) sexual orien- the variance in mental health and suggests that the mental
tation on diagnosis in mental health, and (b) GDT-status on health profiles of individuals with and without ASD dif-
diagnosis in mental health. fered according to GDTStatus. Simple main effects analysis
The 3-way interaction using a repeated measures was undertaken examining diagnosis while separating GDT-
MANOVA of diagnosis by sexual orientation by mental status. Among those without GDT a significant simple main
health profile was not significant for diagnosis by sexual effect was found for diagnosis (F(1, 237) = 78.99, p < .001,

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 3  Multivariate profile analysis examining effects of diagnosis and sexual orientation on mental health
F Eta-squared
df = (2.65, 1484.26)

Within-subject effects
Mental health 110.30*** 0.170
Mental health × diagnosis 0.74 0.001
Mental health × sexual orientation 0.49 0.001
Mental health × diagnosis × sexual-orientation 1.87 0.003
df = (1, 550)

Between-subject effects
Diagnosis 78.73*** 0.124
Sexual orientation 55.14*** 0.091
Diagnosis × sexual orientation 21.02*** 0.037

Table 4  Simple effects comparing mental health for diagnostic


groups by sexual orientation

Non-Heterosexual Heterosexual
M SD M SD d

ASD
Depression 19.58 12.20 18.11 10.99 0.13
Anxiety 16.20 10.85 13.10 10.01 0.30
Stress 22.86 10.39 21.65 10.28 0.12
PWI 48.56 19.81 53.02 20.22 0.23
TD
Depression 17.32 10.32 8.10 9.11 0.95*
Anxiety 12.72 8.67 5.46 6.53 0.95*
Stress 19.40 9.52 11.52 8.58 0.87*
PWI 54.04 20.69 76.48 17.56 1.17*
Depression d 0.17 0.98†
Anxiety d 0.30 0.92†
Stress d 0.30 1.05†
PWI d 0.26 1.21†

*p < .001 over Sexual Orientation within Diagnosis; †p < .001 over


diagnosis within sexuality

those with GDT, additionally having a diagnosis of ASD did


Fig. 5  Mental Health profile for ASD versus TD by sexual orienta- not significantly affect mental health (M = 20.52, SD = 8.03)
tion and for ASD versus TD for GDT-status. While the PWI was res- compared to the TD group (M = 17.50, SD = 8.03).
cored for the analysis by reversing and rescaling to 42, ensuring all Simple main effects analysis was then undertaken on
variables were directionally and scale equivalent, preventing spurious
interactions arising in the analysis, for ease of interpretation, here the
GDT-status separating on diagnosis. For those with ASD,
data is presented unadjusted. Thus, low PWI indicates a poorer out- there was a significant effect of GDT (F(1, 299) = 14.15,
come, as do higher scores for stress, depression and anxiety. a Profile p < .001, ƞ2 = .045). Those who had both ASD and GDT
analysis of diagnosis by sexual orientation. b Profile analysis of diag- (i.e.: members of increasingly restricted minority groups)
nosis by gender dysphoric trait status
had worse mental health (M = 20.52, SD = 8.44) than those
who only had ASD and no GDT (M = 16.17, SD = 8.43).
ƞ2 = .250), having a diagnosis of ASD worsened mental Specifically, this group had worse stress levels and PWI
health (M = 16.17, SD = 6.48) compared to TD individuals levels (see Table 6). However, things were more complex
(M = 8.00, SD = 6.48). This effect was not evident among for TD individuals. The addition of a minority status to

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 5  Multivariate analysis examining effects of diagnosis and GDT-status on mental health


F Eta-squared
df = (2.65, 1470.86)

Within-subject effects
Mental health 97.46* 0.149
Mental health × diagnosis 0.72 0.001
Mental health × GDT-status 4.62 0.008
Mental health × diagnosis × GDT-status 1.60 0.003
df = (1, 555)

Between-subject effects
Diagnosis 63.88* 0.103
GDT-status 97.57* 0.150
Diagnosis × GDT-status 13.65* 0.024

Table 6  Simple effects comparing mental health for diagnostic p < .001, ƞ2 = .248), and worsened stress (F(1, 258) = 62.31,
groups by GDT-status p < .001, ƞ2 = .195). Thus, for TD individuals adding GDT
GDT No-GDT worsened mental health.
Finally, an examination of main effects was under-
M SD M SD d
taken. The main effect for GDT-status was significant for
ASD TD individuals (F (1, 256) = 147.08, p < .001, ƞ 2 =.365),
Depression 20.33 11.45 15.16 12.26 0.43 and individuals with ASD (F (1, 299) = 14.15, p < .001, ƞ2
Anxiety 16.09 10.41 12.34 11.07 0.35 =.045). While main effects may be interpreted in the pres-
Stress 23.38 9.48 19.37 12.42 0.37 ence of a significant interaction, they must be interpreted
PWI 47.33 19.62 58.98 18.69 0.61* cautiously, recognising that these effects reflect the aver-
TD age GDT trend (Keppel 1991; Kirk, 1995). Those with
Depression 19.19 11.06 6.47 6.65 1.39* GDT (M = 17.50 SD = 8.33) had worse mental health than
Anxiety 13.08 9.97 4.76 4.55 1.07* those without GDT (M = 7.96 SD = 7.39). A significant
Stress 19.68 9.91 10.81 7.81 0.99* main effect for diagnosis was also found accounting for
PWI 55.33 20.55 77.00 17.52 1.13* 10.3% of the variance in mental health (see Table 5). Indi-
Depression d 0.09 1.01† viduals with an ASD reported poorer mental health than
Anxiety d 0.29 1.07† their TD peers (see Table 6). There was also a significant
Stress d 0.39† 0.92† main effect for GDT-status, which accounted for 15.1% of
PWI d 0.40† 1.01† the variance in mental health (see Table 5). Individuals in
the GDT group reported poorer mental health than their
*p < .001 over GDT status within diagnosis
† peers without GDT.
 p < .001 over diagnosis within GDT-status
To summarise, single minority group membership
worsened mental health, adding a second minority group
this group worsened mental health. For TD individuals a membership if it was non-heterosexuality did not worsen
significant interaction of mental health by GDT-status was mental health, but adding a second minority group mem-
found (F(2.42, 619.41) = 6.96, p < .001, ƞ2 = .026). This was bership if it was GDT did worsen mental health. However,
not the case for those with ASD (ƞ2 = .002). Consequently, when compared to a single minority status of GDT, no
this interaction was also followed by further assessments additional minority membership worsened mental health.
of simple main effects, first controlling for GDT-status These results suggest a hierarchy of threats to mental
within TD individuals. Mental health scores differed for health through minority group membership. While ASD
TD individuals with GDT (F(2.19, 371.50) = 49.45, p < .001, diagnosis and non-heterosexuality do not additively
ƞ2 = .226) and without GDT (F(2.46, 213.60) = 19.26, p < .001, worsen mental health (see Table 4), when GDT was added
ƞ2 = .186). Among TD individuals the presence of GDT to ASD diagnosis, the effect was to worsen mental health.
significantly worsened PWI (F(1, 258) = 76.12, p < .001, ƞ2 It would appear the effect of GDT is more extreme than
= .228; see Table 6), worsened depression (F(1, 258) = 132., any other minority membership.
p < .001, ƞ2 = .341), worsened anxiety (F(1, 258) = 84.66,

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Discussion internalization of negative social attitudes (Herek 2015),


efforts to conceal one’s sexual orientation and gender-
The current study assessed whether either sexual minority identity, and pressure to conform to societal expectations.
group membership or gender minority group membership
reduced the mental health of someone with ASD. Indi- Additivity of Minority Stress: A Minority Within
viduals who either had ASD, a non-heterosexual orien- a Minority
tation, or GDT reported poorer mental health than their
peers. These results suggest GDT adds to the poor mental The three minority groups in the current study (ASD, sex-
health associated with an ASD diagnosis. However, non- ual minority group, gender minority group) reported poorer
heterosexual orientation did not add to their mental health mental health outcomes than their respective control groups,
burden of those with ASD. possibly due to additivity of minority group-specific stress-
Participants in any of the three single group minori- ors from different conditions. This cohort of individuals can
ties (ASD, non-heterosexuality, having GDT) reported be conceptualized as a ‘minority within a minority’ and the
lower personal wellbeing compared to controls. Scores mental distress experienced within this cohort may be quan-
on the PWI were about 50 out of 100, and well below titatively and indeed qualitatively different from belonging
the that found for TD populations, which usually aver- to one or the other minority populations alone.
age about 74–77 points out of 100 (Cummins et al. 2014; Interestingly, results suggested that a minority sexual ori-
see Table  2). The range for an individual’s subjective entation did not significantly worsen mental health among
well-being is usually small, with 1 SD being about 5 individuals with ASD. However, the additive effects of GDT
points. Cummins et al. (2014) point out subjective well- to a diagnosis of ASD significantly worsened mental health
being appears to be tightly regulated by a well-defended for this group when compared to non-GDT individuals
homeostatic mechanism. Levels that fall much below 75 with ASD. A non-heterosexual orientation is understood as
(i.e.: more than—2 SD) are only seen in very unusual a ‘concealable stigma’ unlike GDT (Herek and Capitanio
circumstances, as supported by many assessments over 1996). By not disclosing our sexual orientation, some level
many thousands of participants (Cummins et al. 2014). of stigmatization can be avoided. Moreover, non-heterosex-
Prolonged challenges to subjective well-being have been uality is increasingly becoming more acceptable (Kuyper
found to seriously impact upon it, and it is likely that the et al. 2013). Our gender, however, unlike our sexual orienta-
chronic challenges associated with belonging to a minor- tion, permeates every aspect of our lives, and affects how we
ity group may considerably reduce subjective well-being relate to the world and the world relates to us. Gender non-
(Cummins et al. 2014), while the presence of a co-morbid conformity is more salient compared to sexual orientation,
diagnosis may further exacerbate this. The most frequently and might thus induce a stronger negative reaction both from
reported co-morbid conditions were depression, social confrontational external events and internalization of nega-
anxiety, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and obses- tive social attitudes, phobia and guilt due to cognitive disso-
sive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Affective disorders have nance related to the individual’s personal desires and expec-
been linked to reduced well-being (Burns et al. 2011; Gar- tations versus sociocultural expectations. This dissonance
giulo and Stokes 2008). in addition to the socio-communicative and interpersonal
Previous literature indicates higher levels of mental dis- challenges characteristic of ASD, could understandably con-
tress among sexual minorities (Sandfort et al. 2001) and tribute to considerable psychological stress. Victimization
gender minority groups (Hepp et al. 2005). The increased and ostracization by peers of one’s own gender group during
rates of mental health problems in these minority popula- early developmental stages when gender segregation is at its
tions are often a consequence of the stigma and marginali- peak, can increase the likelihood of experiencing significant
zation attached to living outside mainstream sociocultural affective, cognitive and behavioural consequences for gen-
norms (Meyer 2003). This stigma can lead to what Meyer der non-conforming individuals in adulthood (Zucker 2005).
(2003) refers to as ‘minority stress’. This stress could Results from the current study suggested that having GDT
come from external adverse events, which among other had the most extreme adverse effect on mental health. Thus,
forms of victimization could include verbal abuse, acts of adverse mental health consequences may be more severe for
violence, sexual assault by a known or unknown person, autistic individuals from a gender minority group when com-
reduced opportunities for employment and medical care, pared to autistic individuals from a sexual minority group,
and harassment from persons in positions of authority and results from the current study suggested that having
(Sandfort et al. 2007). Minority stress levels are further GDT had the most extreme adverse effect on mental health.
perpetuated by internal stresses, such as the anticipation of However, membership to minority sexual orientation
adverse events, the vigilance this anticipation requires, the among TD individuals worsened mental health symptoms.
In addition to reasons previously mentioned, this difference

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

in adverse mental health effects of non-heterosexuality study, where females are more likely than males to rely on
between ASD and TD groups may be in part due to the rela- the internet for interaction and self-help (Addis and Mahalik
tive insensitivity of autistic individuals to social reputation, 2003; Santor et al. 2007). As such, the study provided an
affording this population some insulation from any ensuing opportunity to study females with an ASD. Socio-cultural
stigmatization (Izuma et al. 2011). Alternatively, perhaps gendered norms might also contribute to this trend (Kite and
individuals with ASD simply do not face the same level of Whitley 1996). Society is less tolerant towards males with
hostility related to sexuality differences as TD individuals GDT than females with GDT (George 2016).
do. TD individuals typically have larger social networks rela-
tive to individuals with ASD, thus facilitating more exposure
to any stigmatization. Many individuals with ASD prefer Conclusions
computer chat-rooms, where most obstacles related to social
cues, body-language and eye-contact are removed. Further- The present study yielded some important findings. The
more, family and friends of individuals with ASD report results tend to suggest that relative to a non-heterosexual
more accepting and liberated attitudes toward sexuality, in orientation, having GDT presents with significant mental
anticipation of reducing any additional stress that could be health adversity among individuals with ASD (see Fig. 3).
caused by disagreement. If a particular disposition and a Findings must be interpreted considering the strengths and
supportive environment provides some level of protection limitations of the study. This was the first study to quantita-
from any minority stigmatization among individuals with tively investigate the effects of non-heterosexuality and GDT
ASD, the protection appears to only reduce the effects of together, on mental health among individuals with an ASD.
stigma, but not negate it, as indicated by overall poorer men- Given ASD-characteristic cognitive and behavioural
tal health among autistic individuals. styles, it is likely that the stresses experienced by sexual
and gender minorities who are on the spectrum are perceived
Limitations differently from neurotypical sexual and gender minority
groups. Further investigations would be useful to undertake,
The demographic profile of the current sample may have particularly a qualitative investigation into the mental health
influenced these results. Individuals on the high-functioning outcomes of individuals who belong to sexual and gender
end of ASD may be particularly vulnerable to psychological minority groups within the ASD population. This study
distress due to a deeper insight into their social challenges would suggest that the design of specialized psycho-sexual
(Settipani et al. 2012; Strang et al. 2012). The cognitive programs for gender-related challenges, tailored to birth-sex,
capacity to recognize any social deficits may increase vul- are necessary.
nerability to stress, provoke anxiety, and feelings of help-
lessness and depression. As the current sample had a large Acknowledgments  No funding was received for this research. This
proportion of participants with college education, it is likely work is original research and forms part of the doctoral dissertation
of the author.
these individuals were more aware of their social difficulties.
This is supported by the elevated scores on the depression, Author Contributions  Both authors contributed to the analytic methods
anxiety and stress scales reported by individuals with ASD. and written work presented in this paper. RG and MAS conceived,
Consequently, it would be useful to see if these results are and participated in the design of the study. RG conducted all system-
replicated in another study utilising a sample more typical atic search and meta-analysis procedures, and drafted the original
manuscript. MAS provided expertise regarding the study, critically
of the population. evaluated all key sections of the review, and assisted in drafting the
The composition of the control group was not typical of manuscript. MAS oversaw all analyses, participated in the interpreta-
the wider population, with a higher number of sexually and tion of data, reviewed, and was involved in the write up of all drafts.
gender-diverse individuals who participated. This in fact Both authors were involved in the final revisions and approval of the
published manuscript.
would make the findings in this study even more remark-
able and group differences may have been underestimated.
The cross-sectional design of the study however, did not
assess for psychological functioning through approaches less References
susceptible to self-report biases, such as through measures
Addis, M. E., & Mahalik, J. R. (2003). Men, masculinity, and the con-
of daily functioning such as occupational status, job satisfac- texts of help seeking. American Psychologist, 58(1), 5.
tion, and relationship success, and it is recommended that American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
future research examine these areas. manual of mental disorders (4th edn.). Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychiatric Association.
Most ASD studies demonstrate the usual male bias and
Barcikowski, R. S., & Robey, R. R. (1984). Decisions in single group
report fewer female participants. The current study did repeated measures analysis: Statistical tests and three computer
not find this. This might be due to the online format of the packages. The American Statistician, 38(2), 148–150.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Bijl, R. V., Ravelli, A., & Van Zessen, G. (1998). Prevalence of Gonsiorek, J. C., Sell, R. L., & Weinrich, J. D. (1995). Definition and
psychiatric disorder in the general population: results of The measurement of sexual orientation. Suicide and Life-Threaten-
Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEM- ing Behavior, 25(1), 40–51.
ESIS). Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33(12), Haas, A. P., Rodgers, P. L., & Herman, J. L. (2014). Suicide attempts
587–595. among transgender and gender non-conforming adults. Work,
Billstedt, E., Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Aspects of 50, 59.
quality of life in adults diagnosed with autism in child- Hellemans, H., Colson, K., Verbraeken, C., Vermeiren, R., &
hood: A population-based study. Autism. https://doi. Deboutte, D. (2007). Sexual behavior in high-functioning male
org/10.1177/1362361309346066. adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder.
Bolton, P. F., Pickles, A., Murphy, M., & Rutter, M. (1998). Autism, Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 260–269.
affective and other psychiatric disorders: patterns of familial https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0159-1.
aggregation. Psychological Medicine, 28(02), 385–395. Hellemans, H., Roeyers, H., Leplae, W., Dewaele, T., & Deboutte, D.
Brown, S. L. (2000). The effect of union type on psychological well- (2010). Sexual behavior in male adolescents and young adults
being: Depression among cohabitors versus marrieds. Journal of with autism spectrum disorder and borderline/mild mental retar-
Health & Social Behavior, 4, 241–255. dation. Sexuality and Disability, 28(2), 93–104.
Burns, R. A., Anstey, K. J., & Windsor, T. D. (2011). Subjective well- Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the
being mediates the effects of resilience and mastery on depression Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct valid-
and anxiety in a large community sample of young and middle- ity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British
aged adults. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, journal of clinical psychology, 44(2), 227–239.
45(3), 240–248. Hepp, U., Kraemer, B., Schnyder, U., Miller, N., & Delsignore, A.
Carroll, L., Gilroy, P. J., & Ryan, J. (2002). Counseling transgendered, (2005). Psychiatric comorbidity in gender identity disorder.
transsexual, and gender-variant clients. Journal of Counseling & Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58(3), 259–261.
Development: JCD, 80(2), 131. Herek, G. M. (2015). Beyond “homophobia”: Thinking more clearly
Cummins, R. A., Li, N., Wooden, M., & Stokes, M. (2014). A demon- about stigma, prejudice, and sexual orientation. American Jour-
stration of set-points for subjective wellbeing. Journal of Happi- nal of Orthopsychiatry, 85(5S), S29.
ness Studies, 15(1), 183–206. Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). " Some of my best friends”:
Davern, M. T., Cummins, R. A., & Stokes, M. A. (2007). Subjective Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’
wellbeing as an affective-cognitive construct. Journal of Happi- attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality & Social
ness Studies, 8(4), 429–449. Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.
Davey, A., Bouman, W. P., Arcelus, J., & Meyer, C. (2014). Social sup- Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nydén, A., Wentz, E.,
port and psychological well-being in gender dysphoria: A com- Ståhlberg, O. et al. (2009). Psychiatric and psychosocial prob-
parison of patients with matched controls. The Journal of Sexual lems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disor-
Medicine, 11(12), 2976–2985. ders. BMC Psychiatry, 9(1), 35.
Deogracias, J. J., Johnson, L. L., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Kessler, Holtmann, M., Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2007). Autism spectrum
S. J., Schober, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (2007). The gender identity/ disorders: Sex differences in autistic behaviour domains and
gender dysphoria questionnaire for adolescents and adults. Jour- coexisting psychopathology. Developmental Medicine & Child
nal of Sex Research, 44, 370–379. Neurology, 49(5), 361–366.
DeVries, A. L. C., Noens, I. L. J., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., van Berck- Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the Box correction
elaer-Onnes, I. A., & Doreleijers, T. (2010). Autism spectrum for degrees of freedom from sample data in randomized block
disorders in gender dysphoric children and adolescents. Journal and split-plot designs. Journal of Educational and Behavioral
of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 930–936. Statistics, 1(1), 69–82.
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011).
satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proceedings of the
49(1), 71–75. National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 17302–17307.
Frane, A. V. (2015). Power and type I error control for univariate com- Jacobi, F., Wittchen, H. U., Hölting, C., Höfler, M., Pfister, H., Mül-
parisons in multivariate two-group designs. Multivariate Behav- ler, N., & Lieb, R. (2004). Prevalence, co-morbidity and corre-
ioral Research, 50(2), 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1080/002731 lates of mental disorders in the general population: results from
71.2014.968836. the German Health Interview and Examination Survey (GHS).
Gargiulo, R. A., & Stokes, M. A. (2009). Subjective well-being as Psychological Medicine, 34(04), 597–611.
an indicator for clinical depression. Social Indicators Research, Jones, R. M., Wheelwright, S., Farrell, K., Martin, E., Green, R., Di
92(3), 517–527. Ceglie, D., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Brief report: Female-to-
George, R. (2016). Sexual Orientation and Gender Identity in High- male transsexual people and autistic traits. Journal of Autism &
Functioning Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. (Unpub- Developmental Disorders, 42(2), 301–306.
lished doctoral dissertation). Deakin University, Melbourne, Keppel, G. (1991). Design and analysis: A researcher’s handbook
Australia. (3rd edn.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
George, R., & Stokes, M. (2016). “Gender is not on my Agenda!”: Kirk, R. E. (1991). Experimental design: Procedures for the behav-
Gender dysphoria and autism spectrum disorder. In Psychiat- ioral sciences (3rd edn.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Pub-
ric symptoms and comorbidities in autism spectrum disorder lishing Company.
(pp. 139–150). Berlin: Springer International Publishing. Kite, M. E., & Whitley Jr, B. E. (1996). Sex differences in atti-
George, R., & Stokes, M. A. (2017). Gender identity and sexual tudes toward homosexual persons, behaviors, and civil rights
orientation in autism spectrum disorder. Autism. https://doi. a meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
org/10.1177/1362361317714587. 22(4), 336–353.
Gilmour, L., Schalomon, P. M., & Smith, V. (2012). Sexuality in a Kuyper, L., Iedema, J., & Keuzenkamp, S. (2013). Towards toler-
community based sample of adults with autism spectrum disorder. ance. Exploring changes and explaining differences in atti-
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 313–318. tudes towards homosexuality in Europe. Sociaal en Cultureel
Girden, E. R. (1992). ANOVA: Repeated measures (No. 84). Sage. Planbureau.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Lau, A. L., Cummins, R. A., & McPherson, W. (2005). An investiga- Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEM-
tion into the cross-cultural equivalence of the Personal Wellbeing ESIS). Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(1), 85–91.
Index. Social Indicators Research, 72(3), 403–430. Sandfort, T. G., Melendez, R. M., & Diaz, R. M. (2007). Gender non-
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative conformity, homophobia, and mental distress in Latino gay and
emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress bisexual men. Journal of Sex Research, 44(2), 181–189.
scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Santor, D. A., Poulin, C., LeBLANC, J. C., & Kusumakar, V. (2007).
Behaviour Research & Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. Online health promotion, early identification of difficulties, and
Lugnegård, T., Hallerbäck, M. U., & Gillberg, C. (2011). Psychiatric help seeking in young people. Journal of the American Academy
comorbidity in young adults with a clinical diagnosis of Asper- of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 50–59.
ger syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), Settipani, C. A., Puleo, C. M., Conner, B. T., & Kendall, P. C. (2012).
1910–1917. Characteristics and anxiety symptom presentation associated with
Mannion, A., & Leader, G. (2013). Comorbidity in autism spectrum autism spectrum traits in youth with anxiety disorders. Journal of
disorder: A literature review. Research in Autism Spectrum Dis- Anxiety Disorders, 26(3), 459–467.
orders, 7(12), 1595–1616. Strang, J. F., Kenworthy, L., Daniolos, P., Case, L., Wills, M. C., Mar-
Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress and mental health in tin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2012). Depression and anxiety symp-
lesbian, gay and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and toms in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders
research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 674–697. without intellectual disability. Research in Autism Spectrum Dis-
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674. orders, 6(1), 406–412.
Pasterski, V., Gilligan, L., & Curtis, R. (2013). Traits of autism Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate analysis.
spectrum disorders in adults with gender dysphoria. Archives New York: Boston University: Harper Collins College Publishers.
of Sexual Behaviour, 43(2), 387–393. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Toomey, R. B., Ryan, C., Diaz, R. M., Card, N. A., & Russell, S.
s10508-013-0154-5. T. (2010). Gender-nonconforming lesbian, gay, bisexual, and
Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1999). Psychiatric disorder and the broad transgender youth: school victimization and young adult psycho-
autism phenotype: evidence from a family study of multiple-inci- social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1580.
dence autism families. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(4), van Heijst, B. F., & Geurts, H. M. (2014). Quality of life in autism
557–563. across the lifespan: A meta-analysis. Autism, 19(2), 158–167.
Powdthavee, N., & Wooden, M. (2015). Life satisfaction and sexual Zimmermann, A. C., & Easterlin, R. A. (2006). Happily ever after?
minorities: Evidence from Australia and the United Kingdom. Cohabitation, marriage, divorce, and happiness in Germany.
Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 107–126. Population & Development Review, 32(3), 511–528.
Sandfort, T. G., de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., & Schnabel, P. (2001). Same- Zucker, K. J. (2005). Gender identity disorder in children and adoles-
sex sexual behavior and psychiatric disorders: Findings from the cents. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 467–492.

13

You might also like