You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

Reduce Content or Raise Price? The Impact of Persuasion Knowledge and


Unit Price Increase Tactics on Retailer and Product Brand Attitudes
Luke Kachersky ∗
Fordham University Schools of Business Administration, 441 East Fordham Road, Bronx, NY 10458, United States

Abstract
This research investigates consumer reactions to the practice of increasing unit prices of products by either reducing product content or increasing
total prices. Using pricing tactic persuasion knowledge (PTPK) as a lens for understanding, I predict that total price increases garner less favorable
attitudes toward retailers for relatively low PTPK consumers, while content reductions garner less favorable attitudes toward product brands for
relatively high PTPK consumers. The results of an experiment with a nationally representative sample of U.S. consumers support these asymmetric
predictions about PTPK consumer segments as well as expectations about the underlying psychological processes.
© 2011 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Retail pricing; Unit prices; Persuasion knowledge; Retail consumer behavior; Attitudes

Regardless of the marketing channel member who first insti- does, it is imperative for retailers and other channel members to
gates a unit price increase, retailers are at the front line executing understand consumer reactions to these tactics.
those changes. For decades unit price increases have been Consumers have become more educated and given a more
implemented beyond consumer awareness by hiding beneath far-reaching voice with the ubiquity of the Internet. Combin-
differential thresholds, or the point at which a stimulus changes ing these two trends, it is not surprising that today’s consumers
enough to where a person will consciously notice it. Unit price are quick to notice even minute changes and to share that news
increases reflect an increase in the currency per unit of prod- with the rest of the world. For example, the Consumers’ Union
uct obtained. This can be done by adding only a few pennies to website, The Consumerist, has dubbed the practice of reducing
a price without altering its leftmost digit (e.g., changing from content “The Grocery Shrink Ray,” (Consumerist.com, 2010).
$2.95 to $2.99; Thomas and Morwitz, 2005), a unit price increase Its readers from the general population regularly submit photo-
tactic called a total price increase, or by keeping the price of a graphic examples of the phenomenon that they observe on their
product constant while reducing content in barely perceptible local retail shelves. Even major national media outlets such as
ways (e.g., not reducing the physical package size despite the NBC News and The New York Times have been publicizing the
reduction in content, or reducing the package size proportionate practice; a recent article on the topic reached number three on
to the original dimensions; Chandon and Ordabayeva, 2009), a NYTimes.com’s “Most E-Mailed” list and appeared on the front
unit price increase tactic called a content reduction. For example, page of the print edition (Clifford and Rampell, 2011). Prior
Folgers coffee recently reduced a popular package of its coffee research has explored if consumers notice differential thresh-
from thirteen ounces to eleven ounces, yet the product still retails olds in pricing (cf. the controversy on the psychophysics of
for the same price under the same stock keeping unit number. prices, Kamen and Toman, 1970), and largely concluded that
Implementing unit price changes in these ways has worked for consumers did not. Thus, prior work had not questioned what
so long because, as intended, most consumers did not notice the happens when consumers do notice such changes; if few con-
changes, and if they did, word of the change would not spread sumers were noticing the changes, it was not a practical question
far. This lack of consumer awareness seems to be fading. As it to ask. Yet today, it is clear that greater numbers of consumers
are noticing, and are spreading the information to others. There-
fore, the purpose of this research is to address this newly relevant
question: what happens when consumers do notice unit price
∗ Tel.: +1 718 817 5921. increases? Moreover, does the outcome depend on the manner
E-mail address: kachersky@fordham.edu of price increase? Finally, do outcomes vary across segments of

0022-4359/$ – see front matter © 2011 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2011.08.001
480 L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

Fig. 1. The influence of unit price increase tactic on product brand and retailer attitudes.

consumers? While answers to these questions are of theoretical Unit price increases and persuasion knowledge
importance in understanding differential thresholds in pricing, in
today’s marketing environment they are of increasing practical Persuasion is the act of causing someone to do or believe
importance as well. something. According to the persuasion knowledge model
In this paper, the persuasion knowledge model (PKM; (PKM; Friestad and Wright, 1994), regardless of an action’s
Friestad and Wright, 1994) is used to explain and predict con- objective persuasive intent, when people perceive an action to
sumer reactions to unit price changes. The PKM is a normative be an attempt to persuade, they tend to think about that action
account of how people construe and react to persuasion attempts. differently. In the parlance of the PKM, subjective belief of
A recent extension of the PKM demonstrated that consumers persuasive intent causes a change of meaning. For example, a
have varying levels of knowledge about the intent underly- consumer’s perception of a price can range from a construal as
ing marketers’ pricing tactics (Hardesty et al., 2007), and that a simple presentation of information – the cost of access to a
consumers having relatively higher levels of this pricing tactic product – to a construal as a tactic intended to directly influ-
persuasion knowledge (PTPK) tend to react more negatively to ence beliefs or behavior, undermining the consumer’s agency.
persuasive pricing tactics. The present paper extends this work For example, Schindler et al. (2005) found that some perceive
to the domain of unit price increases, also adding important a price of “$190 + $10 shipping” as a simple presentation of
insights about the cognitive reactions to unit price changes and information, while, to varying degrees, others view it as a tac-
their resulting implications for attitudes toward two potential tic designed to make people believe they are getting a lower
persuasion agents—product brands and retailers. price (i.e., in the $190 range) than is actually offered (i.e.,
In an experiment with a nationally representative consumer $200). In other words, perceived persuasive intent changes the
panel this research makes three major contributions. First, I meaning of “$190 + $10 shipping” from a simple presentation
show that higher levels of PTPK lead consumers to infer dif- of the cost of product access to a more detailed, subjective
ferent motives behind the two types of unit price increases, with understanding of the communication including the intent of the
content reductions being attributed to firm motives to increase communicator.
profit margins and total price increases being attributed to firm Unit price increases may also be subject to such variation in
motives to maintain profit margins in the face of situational fac- perceptions of persuasive intent. Namely, people may perceive
tors such as cost inflation. Second, I show that higher levels of unit price increases as tactics intended to alter consumer behav-
PTPK lead consumers to look less favorably on product brands ior such that consumers continue to purchase the product despite
when the product content is reduced compared to when the total the change. If such persuasive intent is perceived, then con-
price is increased, and that this outcome is driven by inferred sumers’ beliefs about the extent to which the action is designed
motives. Third, in contrast to high PTPK consumers, lower lev- to persuade should cause them to make additional inferences
els of PTPK lead consumers to alter their evaluations not of about it. If consumers perceive unit price increases as persua-
the product brand but of the retailer. In similar contrast, low sion attempts, then what additional cognitive content should be
PTPK consumers look less favorably on total price increases added to consumer processing of the tactic? In general, when
than on content reductions, and this outcome is not based faced with price increases consumers tend to infer that firms are
on inferred motives. The predicted processes and outcomes trying to increase their profit margins (Bolton et al., 2003). Rel-
for each PTPK consumer segment are depicted graphically in ative to total price increases, content reductions tend to be less
Fig. 1. transparent because they can be implemented without chang-
The rest of this article is organized as follows: first, I discuss ing the packaging at all or by changing the packaging in a way
how persuasion knowledge, particularly pricing tactic persua- consumers are less likely to notice (Chandon and Ordabayeva,
sion knowledge, may influence both the psychological process 2009). This type of subversion should lead consumers that notice
and the outcomes of consumer reactions to unit price changes. content reductions to perceive them as having greater persuasive
Second, the article presents the details and results of a study that intent than total price increases have. In turn, content reductions
tests and supports theoretical predictions about the influence of should result in a stronger tendency of consumers to infer that a
content reductions versus total price increases across consumers firm’s motive for the unit price increase is to increase profit mar-
with varying levels of PTPK. Third, I discuss the theoretical and gins. However, this tendency is not likely to be universal among
managerial implications of this work while also taking stock of consumers; some consumers should be more apt than others
its limitations. to make such inferences about the motives underlying the two
L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488 481

types of unit price increase tactics. Hardesty et al. (2007) identi- that is, they should react less favorably to total price increase
fied an individual difference variable, pricing tactic persuasion than to a content reduction because the latter does not affect the
knowledge (PTPK), which moderates consumers’ reactions to affordability of the good.
pricing tactics. PTPK reflects the degree of accurate knowledge A key question remains, though. What should these reactions
a consumer has about myriad pricing practices in the market- entail? In their formulation of the PKM, Friestad and Wright
place. Notably, they found that consumers with high levels of (1994) held that one of consumers’ primary goals in reacting to
PTPK generated more knowledge-related thoughts in response persuasion attempts is to form valid attitudes about persuasion
to pricing tactics. For example, in response to tensile discount agents (i.e., the responsible party for the persuasion attempt). In
claims (i.e., discounts with ambiguous ranges, such as, “Up to construing persuasion attempts, they note that consumers should
$10 off”) high PTPK participants were more likely to believe recognize agents as those who are responsible for designing
that the discount might be less than the upper end of the claim, the persuasion attempt, not necessarily those who follow said
regardless of the objective truth (i.e., without knowing the factual designs. In their words, “We presume that consumers seek valid
levels of the discount). Analogously, I expect that higher levels attitudes toward the puppet masters, not their puppets” (p. 8).
of PTPK should be associated with greater differences in the Therefore, I expect that the negative reaction toward content
likelihood of inferring increased profit margin motives from a reductions exhibited by high PTPK consumers will be directed
content reduction compared with a total price increase, regard- toward the upper level channel partner, or the product brand, and
less of the objective reason for the change’s implementation. not the lower level channel partner, or the retailer.
Specifically: Lacking the prerequisite knowledge, low PTPK consumers
should react differently than high PTPK consumers. Namely,
Hypothesis 1. Content reductions (vs. total price increases)
in the absence of different inferences for content reductions
increase the likelihood of inferred profit margin increase motives
compared to total price increases, their reaction should not be
for individuals with relatively higher levels of PTPK.
influenced by cognitive and affective reactions to persuasion
An important part of the PKM is that once the consumer per- attempts, but by more practical concerns. A total price increase
ceives a persuasion attempt, he must cope with this persuasion more directly impacts the affordability of the product than does
knowledge, often by discounting or refuting the content of the a content reduction. In line with classical economic theory then,
attempt. Friestad and Wright (1994) contended that the idea that, a total price increase should cause a commensurately negative
“someone is using a tactic of influence ‘on me’ is fundamen- reaction, but toward whom? It has been shown that lower levels
tally ‘off-putting,”’ (p. 13) because it undermines one’s sense of of domain specific knowledge are associated with more simple
control. Thus, persuasion attempts may even invoke emotional attributions (Fletcher et al., 1986). Therefore, low PTPK con-
reactions in direct contradiction to message content, or reactance sumers should react toward more proximal (i.e., the retailer)
(Brehm, 1966), as they can be conceptualized as restrictions on rather than distal (i.e., the product brand) agents. Taken all
behavioral freedoms (Petty and Cacioppo, 1979). together, the prediction for consumer responses to content reduc-
Prior research has shown that when consumers perceive per- tions compared with total price increases across levels of PTPK
suasive intent in marketers’ pricing tactics, they tend to react is as follows:
negatively. Schindler et al. (2005) identified a segment of con-
Hypothesis 2. Content reductions (vs. total price increases)
sumers who believe that shipping surcharges have persuasive
lead to less favorable product brand attitudes for individuals
intent by way of making the total price of a product seem smaller
with relatively higher levels of PTPK.
than it actually is. As predicted by the PKM, they demonstrated
that these consumers react against shipping surcharges by choos- Hypothesis 3. Total price increases (vs. content reductions)
ing not to shop from catalogs or on the Internet. More recently, lead to less favorable retailer attitudes for individuals with rela-
Kachersky and Kim (2011) found that consumer preference for tively lower levels of PTPK.
partitioned prices (e.g., $189 camera + $10 shipping) compared
Finally, because these differences are proposed to be driven
with inclusive prices (e.g., $199 camera, including shipping) is
by practical concerns for relatively low PTPK consumers and
driven in part by perceived persuasive intent, such that, all other
by cognitive and affective reactions to inferred motives for rel-
things equal, consumers prefer the price format they perceive as
atively high PTPK consumers, it is hypothesized that:
having less persuasive intent. Additionally, Brown and Krishna
(2004) showed that greater levels of perceived persuasive intent Hypothesis 4. For individuals with relatively higher levels of
caused consumers to react against higher-priced default options. PTPK, the influence of unit price increases by way of content
Hardesty et al. (2007) found that consumers with high PTPK reductions (vs. total price increases) on product brand attitudes
reacted negatively to tensile discount claims and to quantity sur- is mediated by inferred motives.
charges. In a similar vein, if hypothesis one proves true, that high
PTPK consumers infer greater profit margin increase motives Study
from content reductions than from total price increases, then high
PTPK consumers should also be more likely to react negatively This study aimed to comprehensively test the proposed
to content reductions than to total price increases. By contrast, framework using online survey software and a nationally rep-
low PTPK consumers, not having generated such inferences, resentative sample of U.S. consumers. Unit price change tactic
should react more akin to a standard economic explanation; was manipulated between two experimental conditions, while all
482 L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

trait constructs and outcomes were measured. Participants were Measures


posed with a hypothetical situation in which a product that they As a filter question, after being exposed to the stimuli par-
routinely purchased increased its unit price. In one experimental ticipants were asked if they noticed a change between sets of
condition the content amount remained constant while the total potato chip bags and supermarket shelf cards. No participants
product price increased (referred to as “total price increase”); were screened out as a result of this question. As a manipu-
in the other condition the product content was reduced while lation check, participants were subsequently asked to describe
the total product price remained constant (referred to as “con- the change they noticed; all participants described the change
tent reduction”). Participants then wrote about the motives they commensurate with their experimental condition. Importantly,
inferred for the change and evaluated both the retailer and the they were asked to describe the change on the screen following
product brand. exposure to the stimuli, meaning they had to accurately recall
the information to successfully answer that question. This is a
Method good indication that participants were paying attention in the
study, a potential drawback of using online experiments.
Participants and design An assumption in the theory is that content reductions are
155 U.S. consumers drawn from a nationally representative perceived as having greater persuasive intent than total price
panel participated in exchange for points that could be redeemed increases. Participants indicated their perceptions of persuasive
for prizes. The experiment employed a single factor design in intent on a Likert scale adapted from Campbell and Kirmani
which unit price change tactic was manipulated at two levels (2000). In the total price increase condition it read, “The total
between experimental conditions, and the continuous form of price increased while the package size remained the same in
the moderator, pricing tactic persuasion knowledge (PTPK), was order to ensure my purchase decision would not change;” in the
measured. content reduction condition it read, “The package size decreased
while the total price remained the same in order to ensure my
purchase decision would not change.” While all pricing tactics
Stimuli
can be considered persuasive, the relative levels of agreement
Stimuli are replicated in the appendix. Participants were
between these two statements describe the difference in the
asked to imagine a scenario in which they purchase a bag of
perceived strength of the persuasive intent of each tactic. As
name brand potato chips for their household each week at their
expected, participants exposed to the content reduction per-
regular supermarket. They were shown a picture of an 11.5-
ceived greater persuasive intent than those exposed to the total
ounce bag of the potato chips along with its supermarket shelf
price increase (MCR = 5.36 vs. MTPI = 3.53, p < .001).
card including the retail price, $2.50, and its unit price, 21.8 cents
Inferred motives were assessed in the responses to the open-
per ounce. They were then asked, “Now imagine that on your
ended question, “Why do you think the change(s) were made?”
next trip to the supermarket you see the following,” and shown
Two independent judges blind to the purpose of the study
a picture of a bag of the same name brand potato chips with its
coded these responses into one of the following categories:
supermarket shelf card. The pictures were visually identical to
increase profit margin, passing on cost inflation, general state
the ones participants first saw, with the following exceptions: (1)
of the economy, “I don’t know,” and other. Interrater reliabil-
In both experimental conditions, the unit price increased to 23.8
ity was .72, and conflicts were resolved through discussion.
cents per ounce, (2) in the “total price increase” condition, the
Forty-seven percent of respondents were classified into “increase
retail price displayed on the supermarket shelf card was $2.73,
profit margin motive,” 39% were classified into “passing on
not $2.50, and (3) in the “content reduction” condition, the fine
cost inflation,” and 14% were classified into “general state
print on the potato chip bag and the supermarket shelf card was
of the economy;” notably, no participants responded, “I don’t
10.5 ounces, not 11.5 ounces.
know.” Sample increase profit margin motive responses were,
“Everybody is cutting back to make a profit,” and, “The need to
Procedure meet a new profit margin.” Sample passing on cost inflation
Participants were first exposed to the hypothetical scenario responses were, “The cost of supplies – gas and transporta-
and unit price change. They were then asked if they noticed a tion – has gone up,” and, “Some cost went up so the price
change between the sets of stimuli. Participants that noticed the of the product had to go up as well.” Sample general state of
change proceeded to the next screen, in which they were asked the economy responses were, “Inflation,” and, “Because of the
three-open ended questions: (1) to articulate the change they economy.”
noticed, (2) to explain why they thought the change occurred, and Product brand and retailer attitudes were measured on
(3) to describe how the change made them feel. They then indi- two-item, seven-point semantic differential scales including
cated their perceptions of the persuasive intent of the unit price unfavorable/favorable and bad/good. The scales demonstrated
increase. The next two screens of the survey asked participants high reliability (rPRODUCT BRAND = .973 and rRETAILER = .972),
about their attitudes toward the potential actors in the situa- and were each averaged to indices for product brand attitude
tion: the retailer and the product brand. Finally, they responded (M = 3.97) and retailer attitude (M = 4.32).
to a battery of true/false questions to measure PTPK; PTPK Pricing tactic persuasion knowledge was measured using the
was measured at the end to minimize the potential for demand 17-item scale developed by Hardesty et al. (2007). Participants
bias. responded to 17 true/false questions about a variety of pricing
L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488 483

Table 1
Coefficientsa ,b for effects of unit price change tactic and pricing tactic persuasion
knowledge (PTPK).
Predictors Inferred motive Product brand attitude Retailer attitude

Constant −.317 (1.60) 3.34 (1.09)* 1.74 (1.14)


Tactic −2.70 (2.09) 2.47 (1.41) 3.55 (1.48)*
PTPK −.090 (.15) .085 (.10) .214 (.10)*
Tactic × PTPK .449 (.19)* −.264 (.13)* −.285 (.13)*
R2 a .27c .06 .05

n = 155.
a Standard error in parentheses.
b Each column represents a separate estimated model, with column headings Fig. 2. Percentage of participants who inferred a profit increase motive as a
representing dependent variables, and the predictors as independent variables. function of PTPK and unit price change tactic.
c Cox & Snell R2 .
* p < .05. inferred a profit motive across the lowest-third and highest-third
of PTPK and across each unit price change tactic.
tactics, receiving one point for each correct response, zero oth- Moreover, Aiken and West (1991) prescribe the Johnson and
erwise. The measure of pricing tactic persuasion knowledge was Neyman approach to detect and explain the conditional effect of
the sum of accumulated points (M = 11.10). a focal variable (unit price change tactic, in this case) at varying
values of a moderator variable (PTPK, in this case). This anal-
Results ysis revealed a stronger impact of content reduction (vs. total
price increase) on inferred motives as PTPK levels went up, a
Inferred motives pattern depicted in the first column of Table 2. An extension
Logistic regression was used to test Hypothesis 1. Inferred of this technique is used to describe the region of the modera-
motive for the price change was the key dependent variable, tor variable in which the effect of the independent variable on
coded as “0” for a profit margin maintenance motive (i.e., the the dependent variable will be statistically significant, called the
economy and cost inflation were collapsed into a single category) Johnson-Neyman significance region. Essentially, the inverse
and “1” for a profit margin increase motive. The independent of a z-test is used, setting z equal to the critical value for the
variables were (i) unit price change tactic, coded as “0” for total desired ␣ and solving for the levels of the moderator that dis-
price increase and “1” for content reduction, (ii) pricing tactic tinguish significant effects of the independent variable on the
persuasion knowledge, and (iii) their interaction. Results reveal a dependent variable from insignificant effects. Specifically, the
significant two-way interaction between unit price change tactic Johnson-Neyman significance region for PTPK was 8.53 and
and pricing tactic persuasion knowledge (β = .449, p < .05). Full above, meaning the effect of unit price change tactic on inferred
regression results are presented in the first column of Table 1. motives becomes significant (p < .05) when PTPK is at least
Per Irwin and McClelland (2001), the nature of that inter- 8.53. That is, as PTPK increases beyond 8.53, the likelihood of
action is best articulated by examining the coefficients of a content reduction (vs. a total price increase) resulting in an
PTPK when substituting in commensurate values for total price inference of a profit margin increase motive also increases. A
increase (i.e., 0) or content reduction (i.e., 1) into the estimated recent explanation of this analysis and a PASW (formerly SPSS)
regression. With a content reduction, the slope of PTPK is signif- macro for conducting it are available in Preacher et al. (2007).
icant and positive (β = .359, p < .005). With a total price increase,
the slope of PTPK is insignificant and negative (β = −.090, Attitudes toward product brand and retailer
p = .54). To facilitate understanding of the interaction, Fig. 2 Linear regression was used to test Hypotheses 2 and 3. For
shows the tabulation of the percentage of participants who Hypothesis 2, attitude toward the product brand was used as

Table 2
Coefficientsa ,b for effect of unit price change tactic across levels of PTPK.
PTPK Inferred motive Product brand attitude Retailer attitude Product brand attitude,
controlling for inferred
motive

One s.d. below mean (PTPK = 8.99) 1.33 (.51)** .096 (.37) .99 (.39)* −.390 (.15)*
Mean (PTPK = 11.10) 2.27 (.39)*** −.461 (.26) .39 (.27) −.613 (.17)**
One s.d. above mean (PTPK = 13.21) 3.22 (.61)*** −1.02 (.37)** −.21 (.39) −.837 (.23)**
Johnson–Neyman significance region PTPK > 8.53 PTPK > 11.30 PTPK < 10.52 PTPK > 8.36
a Standard error in parentheses.
b Coefficients computed using regression models reported in Table 1 and inserting the commensurate value of PTPK.
* p < .05.
** p < .01.
*** p < .001.
484 L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

the dependent variable, with the independent variables being (i) prediction that low PTPK consumers would be driven by afford-
unit price change tactic, (ii) pricing tactic persuasion knowl- ability concerns.
edge, and (iii) their interaction. Results revealed a significant To illustrate the interactions from Hypotheses 2 and 3, and
two-way interaction between unit price change tactic and pric- because product brand attitudes and retailer attitudes were corre-
ing tactic persuasion knowledge (β = −.264, p < .05). Regression lated (r = .58, p < .001), a 2 (PTPK: high/low) × 2 (tactic: content
results are presented in the second column of Table 1. Using reduction/total price increase) multivariate analysis of variance
the Johnson–Neyman approach revealed an increasingly nega- was used to make an additional assessment of the influence of
tive influence of content reductions (vs. total price increases) pricing tactic persuasion knowledge and unit price change tactic
on product brand attitudes across increasing levels of PTPK. on the outcome variables, attitude toward the retailer and attitude
As portrayed in the second column of Table 2, at one standard toward the product brand. To perform this test an ordinal form
deviation above the PTPK mean there is a negative, statistically of the individual difference variable, pricing tactic persuasion
significant influence of unit price change tactic on product brand knowledge, was used, with the top third of PTPK scores com-
attitude (β = −1.02, p < .01), while at the PTPK mean and one prising the high PTPK group and the bottom third comprising
standard deviation below the influence is not statistically sig- the low PTPK group. As expected, a significant interaction of
nificant (β = −.461, p = .08 and β = .096, p = .80, respectively). PTPK and unit price change tactic emerged (Wilks’ lambda F(2,
More precisely, the effect of unit price change tactic on prod- 79) = 3.37, p < .05). The means of the attitude measures across
uct brand attitudes is statistically significant at PTPK scores of conditions are presented in Fig. 3.
11.30 and above. Hypothesis 2 is supported. For Hypothesis 4, a regression analysis of moderated medi-
For Hypothesis 3, attitude toward the retailer was used as ation (Preacher et al., 2007) was used to assess the indirect
the dependent variable, with the independent variables being (i) effect of unit price change tactic on product brand attitudes, via
unit price change tactic, (ii) pricing tactic persuasion knowledge, inferred motives and conditional upon PTPK levels. In testing
and (iii) their interaction. A significant two-way interaction hypothesis 1, the interactive effect of the independent variable,
between unit price change tactic and pricing tactic persuasion unit price change tactic, and the moderator variable, PTPK, on
knowledge emerged (β = −.285, p < .05). Regression results are the mediator variable, inferred motives, was established. Per
presented in the third column of Table 1. Using the Johnson- Preacher et al. (2007), a second regression was run with attitude
Neyman approach revealed an increasingly negative impact of toward the product brand as the dependent variable and the fol-
total price increases (vs. content reductions) on retailer attitudes lowing independent variables: inferred motive (the mediator),
across decreasing levels of PTPK. As shown in the third col- unit price change tactic, pricing tactic persuasion knowledge,
umn of Table 2, at one standard deviation below the PTPK and the interaction of the latter two. As expected, the media-
mean there is a positive, statistically significant influence of unit tor, inferred motive, was significant (β = 3.82, p < .001) while
price change tactic on retailer attitude (β = .99, p < .05); in other all other predictors were not (p’s > .16). The conditional indi-
words, total price increases (tactic = 0) garner less favor than rect effect of unit price change tactic on product brand attitudes
content reductions (tactic = 1). Meanwhile, at the PTPK mean through inferred motives was estimated at varying levels of
and one standard deviation above, the influence of unit price the moderator, PTPK, using the Johnson–Neyman approach. At
increase tactic is not statistically significant (β = .39, p = .15 and specified intervals of PTPK, the conditional indirect effect was
β = −.21, p = .60). Hypothesis 3 is supported. In particular, the statistically significant, but importantly, as expected, the condi-
Johnson–Neyman significance region indicates that the effect tional indirect effect is stronger for higher levels of PTPK; this
becomes statistically significant at PTPK scores of 10.52 and pattern, in which the coefficient increased in strength from −.390
below. to −.837, is depicted in the last column of Table 2. That (1) the
In the regression used to test Hypothesis 3, presented in tactic-by-PTPK interaction was significant in predicting inferred
the third column of Table 1, a positive main effect of PTPK motives; (2) it became insignificant in predicting product brand
emerged. It was predicted that as PTPK decreases, total price attitude when controlling for inferred motives; and (3) the pre-
increases (vs. content reductions), would produce less favor- dicted pattern of conditional indirect effects emerged suggest
able retailer attitudes. Because unit price increase tactic was moderated mediation (Preacher et al., 2007; Zhao et al., 2010);
coded as “0” for total price increase, the total effect of PTPK Hypothesis 4 is supported. Moreover, the Johnson–Neyman sig-
on retailer attitudes becomes .214 in that circumstance; mean- nificance region of PTPK is 8.36 and above. Therefore, the effect
ing that when total price is increased, for each unit decrease in of unit price change tactics on product brand attitudes through
PTPK attitude toward the retailer goes down by .214. By con- inferred motives is statistically significant when PTPK is at least
trast, when content is reduced (i.e., tactic = 1), the total effect 8.36.
of PTPK on retailer attitudes becomes −.071; meaning that
when content is reduced, for each unit decrease in PTPK atti- Discussion
tude toward the retailer goes up by .071. This indicates that
for relatively low PTPK consumers, the difference in retailer This article makes important contributions to research on the
attitudes between content reductions and total price increases psychology of pricing and how consumers react to persuasion.
is driven more by a preference against total price increases Foremost, research on unit prices never garnered favor among
than by a preference for content reductions. While the main marketing academics except for a short flurry of activity after it
effect was unexpected, this pattern bolsters support for the became mandatory to post unit price information in the 1970s
L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488 485

Fig. 3. Mean attitudes across PTPK and unit price change tactic.

(cf. Edward, 1977). These studies largely focused on the influ- this direct approach may not be practical, our findings can still
ence of the presence (vs. absence) or the presentation format be useful if managers look to more accessible information that
of unit price information on purchasing behavior. Heretofore, may be correlated with PTPK. For example, Hardesty et al.
changes in unit prices and the corresponding cognitive and eval- (2007) demonstrated that younger consumers have lower lev-
uative consumer reactions to them had not been studied. With els of PTPK than older consumers, so managers could use age
renewed interest from consumers on unit price changes, specifi- as a proxy for PTPK. Once there is some understanding of the
cally content reductions, this work sets a foundation upon which market, the product brand and the retailer should decide in con-
a more complete understanding of consumer responses to unit cert the specific unit price increase tactic to be used. If the target
price changes can be built. market has a low level of PTPK, then content should be reduced
This work also extends Hardesty et al.’s (2007) findings in since these consumers will not penalize product brands for it and
their development of the PTPK scale. They found that high levels will appreciate retailers for the additional measure of affordabil-
of PTPK were associated with increased likelihood of rejecting ity. If the target market has a high level of PTPK, the total price
offers with tensile price claims or with quantity surcharges. I should be increased since consumers will not penalize retail-
observed a similar result for content reductions, but also find ers for it and will appreciate the lack of subversiveness from
that inferred motive mediates such effects. Additionally, I found the product brand. Such coordinated efforts will give consumers
that high PTPK consumers reacted toward the more distal per- what they want while also avoiding potential vertical channel
suasion agent, the product brand, demonstrating Friestad and conflicts between retailers and producers.
Wright’s (1994) heretofore-untested contention that consumers From a policy perspective, this work highlights one of the
aim to hold higher level persuasion agents responsible for per- ways in which unit prices, which were enforced by most U.S.
suasive tactics. On the other hand, low PTPK consumers reacted states in the 1970s and largely ignored by consumers in subse-
toward the more proximal agent, the retailer, extending prior quent decades (e.g., Aaker and Ford, 1983), are finally having
work in which people with lower levels of domain specific an impact on consumers. In the 1970s, public policy officials
knowledge made more simple attributions (Fletcher et al., 1986). and consumer advocates lauded unit price information for its
This reveals additional levels of specificity to the pioneering ability to enable consumers to more easily and accurately com-
work on the existence and influence of PTPK by Hardesty and pare package sizes within the same product category. While true,
colleagues. up until recently evidence showed that consumers did not pay
attention to unit prices and missed out on the potential savings
Implications (Koenenn, 1996). Today the Internet abounds with examples
that consumers are paying attention to this information, and this
In total the results indicate that managers should not approach research highlights the attitudinal outcomes of the policy, sug-
unit price increases as a “one tactic fits all” decision. Rather, gesting that efforts in the 1970s to mandate the display of unit
they must understand their markets first, then coordinate with price information may not have been in vain after all.
their fellow channel partners to implement the tactic that is
most beneficial to all involved—consumers, retailers, and prod- Limitations and future research
uct brands. To start with an understanding of the market, it would
behoove both product brands and retailers to understand the The results of this research should be used with caveats in
level of PTPK among consumers at the retail location. While mind. First, the experiment used only one product category.
486 L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

Future work should test these propositions across different Appendix B. Appendix 2: Measures
product categories. Second, this work is focused specifically on
the question of what happens once consumers notice the unit Open-ended
price increase. Our assumption, backed by anecdotal evidence,
is that ever more consumers notice such changes. However, If you noticed any changes, please describe them in the space
future work should explore the factors that influence consumers’ below:
motivation and ability to detect and react to such changes. For If you noticed any changes, why do you think the change(s)
example, perhaps in a completely natural environment high were made?
PTPK consumers are more likely than low PTPK consumers If you noticed any changes, in the space below please describe
to notice unit price increases. If this were the case, then the how the changes make you feel:
results presented here for relatively low PTPK consumers might
not hold as much weight with retailers, to which low PTPK Perceived persuasive intent
individuals directed their reactions. By extension, though, if
low PTPK consumers do not notice the tactic at the point of Total price increase condition: The total price increased while
purchase, it is possible they may have especially harsh reactions the package size remained the same in order to ensure my
if they discover the tactic via a fellow consumer or the media. purchase decision would not change (1 = SD/7 = SA).
Third, this research focused on using content amount and total Content reduction condition: The package size decreased while
price to minimize the negative impact of a unit price increase. the total price remained the same in order to ensure my purchase
It would be fruitful for future work to understand how these decision would not change.
tactics vis-à-vis each other can best be used to maximize the
positive impact of unit price decreases. Attitudes

Toward the retailer: Please indicate your feelings about your


Appendix A. Appendix 1: Study stimuli regular supermarket by circling the appropriate number from
L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488 487

1 to 7 on the following scales: [1 = unfavorable/7 = favorable], A four-pack of a new brand of batteries—$2.00.


[1 = bad/7 = good]. Penetration pricing is used by marketers so that, by setting prices low,
Toward the product brand: Please indicate your feelings about consumers will be encouraged to try the product.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
the producer of the potato chips, Lay’s, by circling the
appropriate number from 1 to 7 on the following scales: Just $1.00 per issue for a 1-year subscription to sports magazine XYZ.
[1 = unfavorable/7 = favorable], [1 = bad/7 = good]. Pennies-a-day or XXX-per-day pricing is used by marketers to provide price
information in the most understandable format to consumers.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Pricing Tactic Persuasion Knowledge (Hardesty, Bearden,
and Carlson, J. Retailing, 2007) A Computer having a 2 GHz processor and 1 GB RAM and laser printer
for $700.
Price bundling is used by marketers in order to increase revenue over what
$3.00 for a non-disposable, easy grip razor, and $10.00 for a package of
would have been obtained had the products been priced separately.
eight replacement razor blades.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Captive pricing is used by marketers in order to take advantage of the fact that,
eventually, consumers will need to purchase the high-priced replacement
A new pair of running shoes—$140.00.
components if they want to continue using the product.
Price signaling is used by marketers since consumers may make quality
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
judgments for products or services based on price (i.e., high price = high
quality, low price = low quality).
$0.79 for a hamburger on the value menu at a fast-food restaurant chain.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Customer value pricing is used by marketers to attract consumers who seek
low prices to the marketer’s store.
Brand new product—videophone: $500.00.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Price skimming is used by marketers to appeal to consumers who are willing to
pay a high price for a new product.
Always low prices at store XYZ.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Everyday-low-pricing is used by marketers so that they will be perceived as
having really low prices on some items and higher prices on others.
A brand of orange juice’s 1/2 gallon price over a 4-week time period was
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
as follows: Week 1 $2.50, Week 2 $2.50, Week 3 $1.50, Week 4 $2.50.
Random discounting is used to obtain sales from both consumers who carefully
$100.00 for a brand of wine (‘Brand X’). The same wine is currently sold
search for low prices and consumers who do not check prices carefully.
for $20.00 a bottle under the name ‘Brand Z’.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
Image pricing is used by marketers in order to have a higher priced version of a
product available for consumers who view higher-priced goods as having
Products X, Y, and Z: Up to 50 percent off.
higher quality, and a lower-priced version of the same product available for
Tensile price claims are used by marketers in order to take advantage of
consumers who strongly desire lower prices.
consumers who may inadvertantly perceive most or all products to be
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
discounted by the stated amount (i.e., 50 percent off).
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
2009 automobile—$500.00 over invoice.
Invoice external reference prices are used by marketers to persuade consumers
to seek out complete price information for a product.
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW
References

Box of a dozen ‘Grade A’ eggs for $0.80. Aaker, David A. and Gary T. Ford (1983), “Unit Pricing Ten Years Later: A
Loss leader pricing is used by marketers to get consumers to not only purchase Replication,” Journal of Marketing, 47 (1), 118–22.
the low-priced item but also other regularly priced items within the store. Aiken, Leona S. and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW Interpreting Interactions, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Bolton, Lisa E., Warlop Luk and Joseph W. Alba (2003), “Consumer Perceptions
A new 19 in. color television (with remote control): MSRP $300, Sale Price of Price (Un)Fairness,” Journal of Consumer Research, (March), 474–91.
$200. Brehm, Jack W. (1966), A Theory of Psychological Reactance, New York:
MSRP’s are used by marketers in efforts to cause consumers to perceive that Academic Press.
the sale price looks attractive. Brown, Christina L. and Aradhna Krishna (2004), “The Skeptical Shopper:
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW A Metacognitive Account for the Effects of Default Options on Choice,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (December), 529–34.
All automobiles for sale at the lowest price possible—no haggling! Campbell, Margaret C. and Amna Kirmani (2000), “Consumer’s Use of Per-
No haggle pricing is used by marketers in order to convince buyers that suasion Knowledge: The Effects of Accessibility and Cognitive Capacity
negotiations will be fair. on Perceptions of an Influence Agent,” Journal of Consumer Research, 27
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW (June), 69–83.
Chandon, Pierre and Nailya Ordabayeva (2009), “Supersize in 1D, Downsize in
Buy a dining room set today and pay no interest for twelve months. 3D: Effects of Spatial Dimensionality on Size Perceptions and Preferences,”
No interest pricing offers are used by marketers to persuade consumers that the Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (6), 739–53.
price has been reduced. Clifford, Stephanie and Catherine Rampell (March 29, 2011), “Food Inflation
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW Kept Hidden in Tinier Bags,” The New York Times (Section A, 1).
Consumerist.com (2010), Grocery Shrink Ray, The Consumerist.
30.00 for a button-up, 100 percent long-sleeve shirt, plus $5.00 shipping http://consumerist.com/tag/grocery-shrink-ray/
and handling. Edward, Russo J. (1977), “The Value of Unit Price Information,” Journal of
Partitioned pricing is used by marketers to persuade consumers that the Marketing Research, 14 (May), 193–201.
marketer is offering an attractive shipping and handling rate. Fletcher, Garth J.O., Paula Danilovics, Guadalupe Fernandez, Dena Peterson
(circle one) TRUE FALSE DO NOT KNOW and G.D. Reeder (1986), “Attributional Complexity: An Individual Differ-
488 L. Kachersky / Journal of Retailing 87 (4, 2011) 479–488

ences Measure,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (October), Petty, Richard E. and John T. Cacioppo (1979), “Effect of Forewarn-
875–84. ing of Persuasive Intent and Involvement on Cognitive Responses
Friestad, Marian and Peter Wright (1994), “The Persuasion Knowledge Model: and Persuasion,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5 (2),
How People Cope with Persuasion Attempts,” Journal of Consumer 173–6.
Research, 21 (June), 1–31. Preacher, Kristopher J., Derek D. Rucker and Andrew F. Hayes (2007), “Address-
Hardesty, David M., William O. Bearden and Jay P. Carlson (2007), “Persua- ing Moderated Mediation Hypotheses: Theory, Methods, and Prescriptions,”
sion Knowledge and Consumer Reactions to Pricing Tactics,” Journal of Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42 (1), 185–227.
Retailing, 83 (2), 199–210. Schindler, Robert M, Maureen Morrin and Nada Nasr Bechwati (2005), “Ship-
Irwin, Julie R. and Gary H. McClelland (2001), “Misleading Heuristics and ping Charges and Shipping-charge Skepticism: Implications for Direct
Moderated Multiple Regression Models,” Journal of Marketing Research, Marketers’ Pricing Formats,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 19 (1),
38 (1), 100–9. 41–53.
Kachersky, Luke and Hyeong-Min (Christian) Kim (2011), “When Consumers Thomas, Manoj and Vicki G. Morwitz (2005), “Penny Wise and Pound Foolish:
Cope with Price Persuasion Knowledge: The Role of Topic Knowledge,” The Left Digit Effect in Price Cognition,” Journal of Consumer Research,
Journal of Marketing Management, 27 (1), 28–40. 22 (June), 54–64.
Kamen, Joseph M. and Robert J. Toman (1970), “Psychophysics of Prices,” Zhao, Xinshu, John G. Lynch Jr. and Qimei Chen (2010), “Reconsidering Baron
Journal of Marketing Research, 7 (February), 27–35. and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis,” Journal of Con-
Koenenn, Connie (October 4, 1996), “Consummate Consumer: Shop Smart, sumer Research, 37 (August), 197–206.
Save Big,” Washington Post (Section F, 5).

You might also like