You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbusres

An Asia-centric approach to team innovation: Cultural differences in


exploration and exploitation behavior
Sylvia Hubner a, *, Michael Frese b, c, Zhaoli Song d, Neha Tripathi e, Tamara Kaschner f,
Xing Le Kong d
a
Free University of Bozen-Bolzano, Italy
b
Asia School of Business (in Collaboration with MIT Sloan Management), Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
c
Leuphana University of Lüneburg, Germany
d
National University of Singapore, Singapore
e
Indian Institute of Management Ahmedabad, India
f
Altran, Munich, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This paper analyses how cultural differences across China, India, and Singapore influence team exploration,
Team innovation exploitation, and innovativeness. Previous cross-cultural and innovation literature mainly focused on Western
Exploration cultures or East-West comparisons. In this research, we investigate innovation related cultural specifics that
Exploitation
differentiate team behavior in China, India, and Singapore (e.g., guanxi, jugaad, and kiasu), and investigate
National culture
Asia
differences in team exploration, exploitation, and innovativeness across those three cultures. We test our model
in a survey study with matched answers of team members and their supervisors in innovation teams across China,
India, and Singapore. In line with our theorizing, our findings suggest comparably high levels of team exploration
in India, and comparably high levels of team exploitation in China. Additionally, we find team exploration, more
than team exploitation, relates to team innovativeness in China, India, and Singapore.

1. Introduction literature mainly focused on Western cultures and their differences from
Eastern cultures, without detecting or acknowledging differences within
Most countries want to increase the innovativeness of their busi­ Asia (Barkema, Chen, George, Luo, & Tsui, 2015). We believe it is time
nesses. In Asia, most countries have officially stated their will to increase to overcome the Western-centric navel gazing that focuses on the West
national innovative performance (Dutta, Lavin, & Wunsch-Vincent, and seems to overlook the differences across Asia. Therefore, we intro­
2020). Getting to innovativeness, however, is challenging. In Asia, cul­ duce an Asia-centric approach to innovation. Our study focuses on
tural factors are often blamed for limited innovation potential (Chen, China, India, and Singapore because these three countries can be
Sharma, Edinger, Shapiro, & Farh, 2011; House, Hanges, Javidan, considered the most important players in Asia for the global market.
Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004; Rao & Pearce, 2016; Xie & Paik, 2019). Such a China and India have the highest populations and are successfully
general evaluation of Asian culture, however, is not appropriate. Due to increasing their innovation activities, which are also their public priority
the differences among Asian countries, a differentiated picture of (Gupta & Wang, 2009). Singapore is home to many ASEAN headquarters
innovation processes across Asia is needed. Our research contributes to a and has shown an extraordinary journey in growing its innovation level
better understanding of differences within Asia in team innovation (Dutta et al., 2020; Yeung, 2000).
processes. We investigate cultural specifics and differences in team Culture-specific factors might explain why previous findings on
exploration, exploitation, and innovativeness across China, India, and cultural influences on innovation processes in Asia were ambiguous
Singapore, three Asian countries that are among the most important (Frese, 2015; House et al., 2004; Morris & Leung, 2010). We investigate
innovators globally. culture-specific mechanisms to explain differences in exploration and
Our Asia-centric approach to team innovation covers an important exploitation behaviors in innovation teams across China, India, and
gap in the literature; the bulk of previous cross-cultural and innovation Singapore (Bledow, Frese, Anderson, Erez, & Farr, 2009; Lavie, Stettner,

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: sylvia.hubner@unibz.it (S. Hubner).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2021.09.009
Received 27 October 2020; Received in revised form 2 September 2021; Accepted 5 September 2021
Available online 29 September 2021
0148-2963/© 2021 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

& Tushman, 2010; March, 1991; Rosing, Bledow, Frese, Baytalskaya, organization (West & Farr, 1989) and contains conceptual as well as
Johnson Lascano, & Farr, 2018). Team exploration refers to activities financial aspects (Huarng, 2013). Innovation develops in a dialectic
characterized by search, discovery, variation, and experimentation; process combining exploration and exploitation. Exploration generally
team exploitation refers to activities characterized by refinement, vari­ refers to activities characterized by search, discovery, variation, and
ance reduction, efficiency, production, and selection. We do not only experimentation, whereas exploitation refers to activities characterized
test the universal prediction that teams’ exploration and exploitation by refinement, variance reduction, efficiency, production, and selection
behaviors predict innovation, specifically in Asia. We also apply an Asia- activities (Bledow et al., 2009; Lavie et al., 2010; March, 1991). Both
centric perspective to suggest culture-specific factors for explaining types of activities are essential for innovation success, but they entail
different levels of team exploration and exploitation across China, India, contradictions, which need to be managed (Bledow et al., 2009; Lavie
and Singapore. In pilot studies, we identified cultural differences in et al., 2010).
these three countries and tested their relationships with exploration and Innovation is assumed to be particularly successful in teams
exploitation. Then, we tested our model in a survey study with matched (Anderson & West, 1998; Hülsheger, Anderson, & Salgado, 2009). Team
answers of team members and their supervisors in innovation teams innovation refers to the emergence, import, or imposition of new ideas,
across these three countries. which are pursued toward implementation by a group, through inter­
We argue that the national cultures in China, India, and Singapore personal discussions and successive adaptations of the original proposal
influence team exploration and exploitation differently. Previous over time (Anderson & West, 1998). Team innovativeness is dependent
research suggests that both exploration and exploitation are needed for on various team-level factors, such as knowledge integration and team
successful innovation (Benner & Tushman, 2015; Bledow, Frese, & climate (van Knippenberg, 2017). Although innovativeness requires
Mueller, 2011; Lavie et al., 2010; Rosing et al., 2018). The practices that teams to apply both exploration and exploitation behaviors (Rosing
are functional for exploration are different from and even contradictory et al., 2018), teams are likely to focus on either exploration or exploi­
to the practices functional for exploitation (Bledow et al., 2011). We tation, depending on, for example, their emotional states (Døjbak
propose that teams in some Asian cultures are more likely to explore, Håkonsson, Eskildsen, Argote, Mønster, Burton, & Obel, 2016) or team
whereas teams in other Asian cultures are more likely to exploit. In members’ experiences (Beckman, 2006). Exploration and exploitation
India, for example, education valuing leela (playfulness and naughti­ were introduced as concepts at the organizational level (Gupta, Smith, &
ness) and the tendency toward jugaad (a form of creative adaptation for Shalley, 2006; Lavie et al., 2010). Therefore, knowledge on influencing
frugal innovation) might facilitate exploration, whereas in China edu­ factors at the team level, particularly factors explaining why some teams
cation focusing on ting hua (listening to words) and yao mianzi (preser­ emphasize exploration while others emphasize exploitation, is still
ving face) is likely to counteract exploration (Cappelli, Singh, Singh, & limited. One factor determining team innovation activities is national
Useem, 2015; Hossain, 2017). Guanxi (a form of dyadic social re­ culture (Hayton, George, & Zahra, 2002; Hülsheger et al., 2009; Shane,
lationships) might nevertheless facilitate exploitation in China (Tsang, 1992; Stephan & Pathak, 2016; Stephan & Uhlaner, 2010).
1998; Zhang, Li, & Brad Harris, 2015). In Singapore, kiasu (fear of losing
out) might create a tendency to avoid risky attempts and establish a 2.2. Culture’s influence on team innovation
reluctance to explore (Cheng & Hong, 2017; Shin & Eom, 2014). These
cultural differences might explain why teams focus on exploration or To define culture, we draw on the concept of descriptive norms and
exploitation. focus on differences in cultural practices, i.e., differences in shared
Our Asia-centric approach moves beyond previous research on cul­ perceptions of how individuals in a culture routinely behave (Frese,
ture’s influence on innovation. First, we elaborate on innovation-related 2015; Shteynberg, Gelfand, & Kim, 2009; Stephan & Uhlaner, 2010).
cultural differences among China, India, and Singapore. Our research Cultural differences influence the behaviors of individuals, teams, and
extends previous studies, which have mainly analyzed what differenti­ organizations in a society because cultural norms determine what kind
ates Asia from the West and neglected differences within Asia. Second, of behavior is “normal”, expected, and desirable. In this way, culture
we explore culture-specific concepts that differentiate China, India, and influences whether a society considers innovation behaviors such as
Singapore and analyze how they relate to tendencies toward exploration creative thinking and risk-taking to be “normal”, expected, and
vs. exploitation. Thus, our research introduces an Asia-centric perspec­ desirable.
tive on predictors of innovation. Third, by testing differences in team Culture can influence the innovativeness level in a society (e.g.,
exploration and exploitation across China, India, and Singapore on a Hayton et al., 2002; Rinne, Steel, & Fairweather, 2012; Shane, 1993;
behavioral level, we describe pathways explaining how cultural differ­ Williams & McGuire, 2010). For example, tendencies in a culture to shy
ences shape team innovativeness in Asia. We found most team explo­ away from uncertainties may harm innovation because innovation ac­
ration in Indian teams and most team exploitation in Chinese teams tivity requires risk-taking (Hayton & Cacciotti, 2013; Shane, 1992).
when comparing teams across China, India, and Singapore, and team Moreover, the extent to which a culture stresses hierarchy has been
exploration affected team innovativeness. suggested to decrease innovation because hierarchies and related lead­
ership and organizational structures reduce creative activity and op­
2. Literature review: Previous research on innovation and portunities for change (Shane, 1992; Stephan & Pathak, 2016).
cultural differences Additionally, a focus on collective achievements and rewards in a soci­
ety has been suggested to decrease innovation because innovation re­
In the following, we elaborate on previous literature about explo­ quires individual freedom, risk-taking, and the encouragement of
ration and exploitation in innovation teams, outline the impact of cul­ nonconformity (Gelfand, Nishii, & Raver, 2006; Hayton et al., 2002;
ture on team innovation in general, and discuss the influence of cultural Hayton & Cacciotti, 2013; Shane, 1992).
dimensions on team innovation in China, India, and Singapore. Then, Moreover, due to differences in norms, preferences, and routines, the
because the knowledge from previous literature does not provide clarity conceptualizations of creativity, innovation motives, and preferred
regarding differences among these countries, our hypothesis develop­ innovation strategies are likely to differ across different cultures
ment takes a closer look at cultural specifics. (Engelen, 2010; Hayton & Cacciotti, 2013; Mitchell et al., 2002; Nouri,
Erez, Lee, Liang, Bannister, & Chiu, 2015; Shane, Venkataraman, &
2.1. Exploration and exploitation in innovation teams MacMillan, 1995; Tang, Chen, van Knippenberg, & Yu, 2020; Xiong
Chen & Aryee, 2007). Previous research examining such cultural dif­
Innovation refers to the intentional generation, promotion, and ferences mainly focused on East-West differences and indicates, for
realization of new and beneficial ideas within a work role, group, or example, that innovation support in the East is focused on incremental

409
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

innovation and in the West on breakthrough innovation (Morris & high on power distance, with the highest levels in India. These argu­
Leung, 2010). Moreover, recognizing and developing serendipitous ments suggest lower exploration and higher exploitation activity in In­
ideas seems to be facilitated in the West more than in the East , and dian teams than in Chinese and Singaporean teams. It is noteworthy that
Western social norms seem to prioritize the novelty of ideas, whereas this prediction stands in contrast to the prediction following from un­
Eastern norms seem to prioritize the usefulness of ideas (Morris & Leung, certainty avoidance differences, as outlined above.
2010), especially in group situations (Erez & Nouri, 2010). This paper Collectivism has been suggested to facilitate exploitation activities, as
focuses on the idea that cultural differences can influence innovation it facilitates building stable relationships (Rauch et al., 2013), whereas
behavior in the way that cultures tend toward either exploration or individualism has been suggested to facilitate creativity, entrepreneurial
exploitation. We argue that the degree of team exploration vs. exploi­ activity, and exploration (Hayton et al., 2002; Rauch et al., 2013; Xie &
tation may vary across cultures. Paik, 2019). All three countries score high in collectivism, indicating
To explain such differences in innovation behavior, previous litera­ that collectivism may not explain differences in innovation activities
ture connected innovation behaviors with the cultural dimensions of across China, India, and Singapore (House et al., 2004). There are dif­
large-scale cross-cultural research, including dimensions by Hofstede & ferences in two types of collectivism, in-group collectivism (focused on
Hofstede (1991) and the GLOBE study (House et al., 2004). Such cultural the family and groups with close relationships) and institutional
dimensions, for example, power distance and collectivism, describe collectivism (focused on the community or societal level). India scores
differences in values and norms across countries (House et al., 2004). higher on in-group collectivism and lower on institutional collectivism
Those dimensions indicate what kind of behavior is valued (“should be”) than China and Singapore. How these differences may relate to team
and common (“is”) and, in turn, which behaviors might be particularly innovation behavior, however, remains unclear.
effective in a culture. To generate new ideas and overcome resistance to Cultural looseness is suggested to be beneficial for creativity (Gelfand
new ideas, innovation teams are likely to require culture-specific stra­ et al., 2006). Teams in loose societies are likely to have less order,
tegies that are tailored to societal values and norms. Previous cross- accountability, and sanctioning and more tolerance for organizational
cultural research provides comprehensive knowledge about the re­ change, and they have more discretion and a wider range of acceptable
lationships of cultural dimensions with innovation behaviors. In the behavior (Gelfand et al., 2006). A wider range of behaviors enables
following, we first discuss which predictions follow from this cross- higher levels of individuality and the expression of diverse ideas within
cultural research stream for team exploration and exploitation and teams (Cox, Lobel, & McLeod, 1991). In loose cultures, perceptions and
transfer the reasoning to China, India, and Singapore. However, argu­ ideas are openly shared, and organizational practices are not necessarily
ments and results from this research stream, which focuses on general adhered to. Such looseness facilitates flexibility and openness to change
cultural dimensions, on how within-Asia cultural differences influence (Gelfand et al., 2006). China, India, and Singapore are all considered
innovation remain ambiguous. Therefore, we will then discuss in what tight cultures, with the lowest score in China (Gelfand et al., 2011),
way culture-specific factors may provide a more nuanced picture of suggesting more exploration in Chinese teams. This prediction is in line
innovation across China, India, and Singapore. Four cultural di­ with power distance arguments but in contrast to uncertainty avoidance
mensions, i.e., uncertainty avoidance, power distance, collectivism, and arguments.
cultural tightness, have been repeatedly related to innovation. Overall, the extant theoretical knowledge and empirical evidence
based on previous cultural dimensions seem to provide contradictory
2.3. Innovation-related cultural differences: Uncertainty avoidance, predictions about how cultures could influence innovation activity
power distance, collectivism, and tightness/looseness across China, India, and Singapore. Thus, to identify the cultural dif­
ferences in innovation activity across these three countries, a closer look
Uncertainty avoidance (Dwyer, Mesak, & Hsu, 2005), the extent to at the local cultures is required. In the next section, we elaborate on local
which individuals in a society feel uncomfortable with uncertainty and cultural practices in China, India, and Singapore, which may be relevant
ambiguity and shy away from risky attempts (Hofstede & Hofstede, and important to differences in innovation activities.
1991), is considered a key factor for differences in innovation activity
(Efrat, 2014; Hayton & Cacciotti, 2013). Teams’ innovation activity 3. Hypothesis development: A closer look at innovation-related
indeed needs to address uncertainty because innovation increases risk differences among China, India, and Singapore
and involves investments in an unknown future (McMullen & Shepherd,
2006). Uncertainty avoidance, however, produces hesitancy, indeci­ To uncover differences in cultural practices across China, India, and
siveness, and resistance toward such risky attempts (Hayton et al., 2002; Singapore for our hypothesis development, we take an “insider
Mueller & Thomas, 2001). Uncertainty avoidance is likely to hamper perspective” (known as the “emic perspective”)1 in our discussion of
teams’ exploration activity because, as a result of its nonlinear and innovation-relevant cultural attributes. Focusing on culture specifics
experimental nature, exploration requires tolerance for ambiguity and from the insider perspective helps us to understand how team explora­
risks. Thus, teams in cultures with high uncertainty avoidance are less tion, exploitation, and innovation differ across these cultures. We
likely to show exploration activity. Exploitation, in contrast, involves elaborate on specific cultural practices, guanxi, ting hua, and yao mianzi
less uncertainty, risk, and ambiguity, such that uncertainty avoidance (in China), leela and jugaad (in India), and kiasu (in Singapore), and
may hinder only the teams’ exploration, not necessarily their exploita­ propose that these cultural practices could influence the tendency to­
tion. As uncertainty avoidance is higher in China than in India, and in ward either exploration or exploitation activities in innovation teams.
Singapore even higher than in China (House et al., 2004), these argu­ We discuss the cultural practices of each culture in the following
ments may suggest higher exploration activity in Indian teams than in sections.
Chinese and Singaporean teams.
Power distance has been suggested to hinder creativity because high
power distance cultures may not be used to taking personal initiative,
tend to maintain the status quo, and establish barriers to novelty and 1
Our theorizing is not based on an exhaustive emic analysis of the countries’
change (Geletkanycz, 1997; Rauch et al., 2013; Rinne et al., 2012; Xie &
cultures (that would have gone beyond the scope of our study), but we based
Paik, 2019). At the same time, power distance might facilitate team our selection of these practices on a literature analysis of the three countries’
exploitation by making team members adhere to leaders’ innovation innovation literature (in preparation, anonymous for review purpose), on a
strategies. Team members’ adherence enables the leader to implement qualitative pilot study (see Study 1), on exploratory findings from a quantitative
novel ideas and strategies and to overcome resistance associated with analysis of cultural differences (see Study 2), and on frequent discussions within
innovation (Rauch et al., 2013). India, China, and Singapore all score our multinational research team representing all three countries of interest.

410
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

3.1. Chinese culture’s influence on innovativeness Confucian thinking, the importance of guanxi, and valuing ting hua all
emphasize the importance of social harmony.
The literature suggests that Chinese culture is characterized by In combination, the detrimental consequences of violations of the
relationship orientation, often discussed in light of the importance of reciprocity in a guanxi relationship, the cautious communication atti­
‘guanxi’ (Chow, Harrison, McKinnon, & Wu, 1999; Tsang, 1998). Guanxi tude cultivated by the value of ting hua, and the salient concern with yao
refers to developing and maintaining long-term social relationships mianzi suggest that Chinese tend to be cautious in mutual relationships.
based on commitment, loyalty, and obligation (Fu, Tsui, & Dess, 2006). In contrast, coming up with a new idea requires individuals to challenge
Guanxi extends beyond family membership, and guanxi can be devel­ established procedures, to take individual risk (Hayton et al., 2002;
oped via common experiences, preferences, and shared identity-related Shane, 1993), to face challenging positions and opinions of others
characteristics (Chen & Chen, 2004). In Chinese culture, people do not (Gelfand et al., 2006; Morrison & Phelps, 1999; Shane, 1992), and to
quickly establish rapport with ‘outsiders’ (Chen & Chen, 2004). How­ openly discuss conflict (Parker, 2016). Such activities are unlikely in
ever, when established, guanxi is characterized by mutual trust and teams with a highly cautious communication attitude, where chal­
dynamic reciprocity. Therefore, guanxi and the respective network can lenging others is a highly risky attempt that requires tremendous social
be considered a business resource and even a competitive advantage skill (Morrison & Phelps, 1999; Zhang et al., 2015). Thus, we argue that
(Tsang, 1998). valuing ting hua and concerns with yao mianzi (Chow et al., 1999) might
For an innovation task, Chinese will usually evaluate how guanxi inhibit exploration activity, and we expect that exploration tends to be
helps in advancing the innovation process. Guanxi makes implementa­ comparatively low in Chinese teams.
tion fast and efficient because, in a network of established relationships, Exploitation, on the other hand, is enhanced by the ease of
one can immediately and easily get a task done. Guanxi also supports communication and the quickness of being able to get things done in a
interactions with customers, suppliers, and other stakeholders and thus harmonious group. Exploitation is a collective effort. Guanxi networks,
creates numerous opportunities for getting an innovation off the ground combined with the disciplining nature of the value of ting hua and
quickly (Dwyer et al., 2005; Rauch et al., 2013; Wang & Chung, 2013). concerns with yao mianzi, thus might contradict exploration but facili­
Because of its strong reciprocal relationships, guanxi enables the align­ tate exploitation in Chinese teams.
ment of people for opportunity exploitation (Tjosvold, Tang, & West,
2004). Therefore, we argue that guanxi is instrumental for exploitation. 3.2. Indian culture’s influence on innovativeness
Additionally, because guanxi can be developed between business part­
ners, we expect that using guanxi is even more beneficial for exploitation India is a very heterogeneous society in terms of religion and his­
than family-oriented relationships, which are common in India due to its torical influences (Panda & Gupta, 2004). As India is highly religious,
in-group collectivism. religious figures play an important role. Worshipping many different
However, guanxi also constrains the thinking. Guanxi is a system of gods is common to most religious practices. Despite India being a
established relationships and shifts attention toward already established constitutionally secular country, Hinduism is reflected in societal norms
relationships. Therefore, guanxi can create, in innovation processes, a and practices. Education in India is influenced by childhood stories
focus on utilizing those already established relationships (Luo & Hassan, about Krishna, the most worshiped Hindu god in India (Sahi, 2018).
2009). Exploration requires going beyond what is already established Krishna’s leelas are exemplar for the loosely argued systems of thought
and attending to ideas that might be unacceptable in one’s guanxi circle. in India. Krishna’s leelas characterize his playfulness and naughtiness,
Accordingly, guanxi can help speed up processes but also constrain and they refer to breaking social norms and discipline and being a rebel
thinking about new ideas. against social dogma (Sarkar, 2011; Zaveri, 2018; Sahi, 2018). Child­
Additionally, guanxi is reflected in the Confucian system of thoughts, hood is considered a state in which rebellious acts are acceptable; the
which emphasizes hierarchy and status. Chinese culture appreciates child may touch, eat, and do things in a rebellious way (Sarkar, 2011).
status such that Chinese people tend to care about their own status and Such beliefs from cultural-religious norms influence the educational
respect and follow others who are high in status. They tend to care, more value system in India, such that children are allowed and encouraged to
than other cultures, about how they are treated by others, including be playful, loud, naughty, and explorative. Accordingly, the education
friends, coworkers, supervisors, and family (Hwang, 2000). Chinese system in India cultivates competencies related to exploration. Educa­
society practices harmony, particularly in interactions across hierarchy tion in China and Singapore, in contrast, is more strictly focused on
levels (Chuang, Hsu, Wang, & Judge, 2015). Therefore, the ideas of disciplined learning.
guanxi and status are related; guanxi often utilizes harmonious re­ Previous literature has characterized innovation activities in Indian
lationships with hierarchically higher individuals (Hwang, 2000). society as jugaad (Cappelli et al., 2015). Jugaad reflects Indian’s ten­
Importantly, harmonious relationships with hierarchically higher dency toward using the resources at hand to develop and redesign
individuals not only imply cordial relationships but also acceptance of products and processes from scratch at minimum cost (Cappelli et al.,
order, which is emphasized in the concept of ting hua (listening to words) 2015). India has produced numerous ‘frugal innovations’, referring to
(Gao, 1998; Hwang, 1987). Education in China values and focuses on just good-enough, affordable products that meet the needs of resource-
obedience and discipline and thereby cultivates a restrained attitude constrained consumers (Agarwal & Brem, 2012; Hossain, 2017).
(Hildebrandt, 1988). In China, a good child is usually referred to as one Jugaad refers to new uses or new versions of existing technologies. These
who ting hua, i.e., ‘listens to the words’. Additionally, in work relation­ innovations focus on creating social value by providing products that are
ships, the norm is that the superior is the one speaking, whereas the simpler and cheaper and have limited features compared to existing
inferior is supposed to listen (Gao, 1998). Voicing an opinion is not an products (Cappelli et al., 2015). New products are created through
acceptable norm for a person who is not in a power position or a bricolage, reducing time, materials and human resources, and facili­
recognized expert on a subject; voicing an opinion thus requires careful tating self-service options for users (Hossain, 2017).
and skilled communication. Indians’ ability to address region-specific needs in a creative and
Another contributor to this practice is yao mianzi, i.e., preserving frugal way, i.e., their ability to jugaad, is referred to as a particular
face. Mianzi refers to a person’s social standing. Social standing can be ‘brilliance’ (Cappelli et al., 2015). The Indian government facilitates
called into question by criticism voiced by others or by one’s own skills for product development by emphasizing a high priority on state-
behavior that violates social norms (Liao & Bond, 2011). Preserving face of-the-art education in technology (Cappelli et al., 2015). A technology-
is a concern in India and Singapore, as well. However, we expect the focused mindset is likely to facilitate technological advancements and in
inhibiting nature for voicing an opinion, particularly a contradictory turn new product introductions (Ridge, Johnson, Hill, & Bolton, 2017).
opinion, to be particularly strong in China, because the coherence of Because of the limited incomes of the Indian majority and the limited

411
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

availability of public services, it is particularly challenging for Indian 3.4. Influences on innovativeness across China, India, and Singapore
companies to face competitors from other countries (Cappelli et al.,
2015). Additionally, the highly diverse ethnic, religious, and geographic In sum, we suggest that ting hua and yao mianzi may harm explora­
situation, as well as India’s complex system of regulations and licenses tion in Chinese teams, and kiasu may harm exploration in Singaporean
(Tymon, Stumpf, & Doh, 2010), complicate innovation activity in India. teams, whereas the tendency toward leela and jugaad may facilitate
In this challenging situation, Indian companies seem to be able to exploration in Indian teams. Additionally, we expect guanxi, ting hua,
“muddle through” (Javalgi, Todd, Johnston, & Granot, 2012) and find and yao mianzi to facilitate exploitation in Chinese teams. These pre­
ways to come up with innovative, inexpensive, adapted versions of dictions would be in line with arguments following from uncertainty
products and services (Cappelli et al., 2015). Indians seem to have avoidance but in contrast with predictions following from power dis­
developed a particular ability to explore new ideas and opportunities. In tance and cultural tightness. Comparing China, India, and Singapore, we
China and Singapore, concepts similar to jugaad are known under expect team exploitation to be highest in Chinese teams and team
different labels but seem to be less influential than in India. exploration to be highest in Indian teams.
Therefore, we argue that in India the influence of leela in the
educational value system, as well as the tendency toward jugaad, suggest H1: Team exploitation activity is higher in China than in India and
an affinity toward trying out things that are creative and explorative in Singapore.
nature. Because of these creative and explorative aspects in Indian cul­ H2: Team exploration activity is higher in India than in China and
ture, we suggest that there might be particularly high levels of explo­ Singapore.
ration in Indian teams.
We expect that both team exploration and exploitation are positively
3.3. Singaporean culture’s influence on innovativeness related to the innovativeness of teams across cultures. When engaging in
exploration behavior, teams create or import new ideas (Bledow et al.,
Singaporean culture is influenced by cultures in China, Malaysia, and 2009). They take different perspectives, search for new information, and
the West (Li, Ngin, & Teo, 2008). Clan associations, stemming from exchange in networks (Gong, Cheung, Wang, & Huang, 2012; Li, Mag­
Chinese Singaporeans’ ancestors, coordinate and fund schools and gitti, Smith, Tesluk, & Katila, 2013; Zhou, Shin, Brass, Choi, & Zhang,
charitable works and thereby cultivate Chinese culture in Singapore (Li 2009). They do not settle on the first idea but explore a variety of
et al., 2008). Thus, the cultural attributes explained for China above are different approaches and directions and search for a novel solution that
also influential in Singapore. Nevertheless, Singapore’s free-trade is not yet determined (Rosing et al., 2018). When engaging in exploi­
economy, tax incentives, and attractive labor laws also attract many tation behavior, teams pursue their ideas toward implementation
foreign companies and workers. The high percentage of Western for­ (Rosing et al., 2018). They focus on realizing ideas as they evaluate and
eigners in Singapore, the lasting influence of British colonialization, and advance the ideas’ feasibility, convince supporters, and work on over­
the fact that many Singaporeans receive training and education in the coming resistance (Baer, 2012; Janssen, 2003). During the process of
West and in Western companies in Singapore put a Western flavor on the implementation, ideas are refined, reshaped, and adapted (Bledow et al.,
culture (Li et al., 2008; Tan, 2002). The diversity and internationality in 2009; Rosing et al., 2018).
Singapore may support both exploration and exploitation in Singapore. To reach innovativeness, teams need to generate ideas and select,
Specific to the Singaporean culture is the tendency toward kiasu. discuss, adapt, and advance ideas over time. Thus, both team behaviors,
Kiasu refers to being afraid of losing out and relates to selfish, greedy, exploration and exploitation, contribute to innovativeness (Gilson,
and inconsiderate behavior (Bedford & Chua, 2018; Tan, 2003). Being Mathieu, Shalley, & Ruddy, 2005). Zhao, Zong, & Zhang (2020), for
afraid of losing out is likely to drive thrift and a prevention focus in example, found in a Chinese sample that team exploration, via team
Singaporeans (Cheng & Hong, 2017; Low, 2006). Singaporeans’ creativity, and team exploitation, via task completion, contribute to
achievement orientation (House et al., 2004) seems to stem from being team performance. Because of the universal nature of team innovation
afraid to lose rather than from being keen on winning (Low, 2006). processes (van Knippenberg, 2017), we suggest that even when the de­
Singaporeans also have been described as fussy, stingy, and over­ gree of team exploration and exploitation may vary among China, India,
compliant (Tan, 2003), as well as rather low in openness and high in and Singapore, the effect of team exploration and exploitation should be
perseverance (Scherer & Gustafsson, 2015). In Singapore, ‘going by the positive in all three countries. As we expect high exploitation in China
books’ is considered a typical way of working. By rewarding rote and high exploration in India, we hypothesize that team exploitation
learning over creative thinking, the education system has in the past and exploration mediate the effects of country differences on team
contributed to tendencies toward strict rule following and orderliness innovativeness (see Fig. 1).
(Tan, 2003). Although Singapore has made strides to overcome reliance
on rote learning, the overall Singaporean culture is still portrayed as H3: Team exploitation mediates a positive effect of Chinese culture
being left-brained, preferring to work in comfort zones (Low, 2006). on team innovativeness.
Singaporeans seem to assume that the often paternalistic government H4: Team exploration mediates a positive effect of Indian culture on
will care for them, and they can usually rely on the government for team innovativeness.
housing, medical services, and pensions (Li et al., 2008; Low, 2006).
Strong government influence (Wang, 2018) and its authoritarian, 4. Method and results of pilot studies
paternalistic, and comforting policies have been suggested to be related
to kiasu, because relying on comfort can create concerns about losing it. Before we tested our hypotheses in the main study, we conducted
Relying on a paternalistic government may refer to a culture of greedi­ two pilot studies. Pilot 1 built on interviews to uncover innovation-
ness and individual risk aversion (Li et al., 2008; Low, 2006; Tan, 2003). related cultural specifics. Pilot 2 built on a survey study that analyzed
For exploration, risky and courageous investments despite unknown whether the cultural specifics differentiated the cultures across China,
outcomes and without a concrete plan are necessary. As such attempts India, and Singapore and whether the cultural specifics related to
are unlikely in a kiasu society, which is afraid to lose (Cheng & Hong, exploration vs. exploitation.
2017; Low, 2006), kiasu is likely to counteract exploration activity.
Thus, although the international environment and talent in Singapore 4.1. Pilot 1: Innovation-related cultural differences in interviews
may facilitate innovation, we expect a comparatively low level of
exploration activity in Singaporean teams because Singaporeans seem to To explore what differentiates innovation across China, India, and
be rather risk-averse and rely on intensive planning. Singapore, we conducted 15 pilot interviews with senior executives who

412
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Fig. 1. Influences of cultural differences in innovation processes.

had experience in innovation projects in one or more of the three


countries of interest. The interviews combined two different approaches Table 1
Sample quotes on cultural differences in innovation projects (Pilot 1)
(see appendix for details on the interview questions). First, we employed
the critical incident technique. Interviewees described one very suc­ Sample quotes
cessful and one rather unsuccessful innovation project. We deepened our Chinese discipline. Chinese people are very good at implementing what is once
understanding of their narratives with additional open queries. Second, decided. They can go ahead and, you know, replicate their process very, very well.
we asked interviewees whether they perceived anything to be specific And the discipline levels are significantly higher in the work for central China
(Interviewee 12).
for the culture or country where the innovation project took place and, Chinese focus. I think it is a very strong plus that [Chinese] are very hard-working and
in case they had experience in more than one of the countries, whether very focused. They have the target, they have the target in the front of the eye, and
they perceived any differences. Thus, our interview partners shared they leverage if they see, for example, that you they expect a lot, they expect a lot
specific stories and experiences from innovation projects in which they from the boss in the sense that they expect ideas, they expect to show them the way,
but once you have shown them the way and give them all the resources necessary for
were involved and talked about their personal perceptions of the dif­
them to achieve the target, they are extremely focused, extremely hard-working,
ferences among the countries. All interviews were recorded and tran­ and they achieve, normally, they achieve the target (Interviewee 11).
scribed. Two of the authors independently coded the transcripts to Indian flexibility & adapting. The Indians are much more flexible and don‘t work
detect statements that indicated culture specifics and culture differ­ project plans as concrete as Singaporeans do. So they are constantly adapting on the
ences. In an iterative process of reading the transcripts and discussing fly and adjusting (Interviewee 2).
Indians require good quality at low cost. But the Indian consumer has an additional
the specifics, we identified common and reoccurring themes in the requirement, which is that while they expect a really low price, they also expect a
interview data. very good quality, which means that they want the product to last a really long time.
In Table 1, we summarize innovation-related cultural differences and […] Keeping a very fond commitment on the outcome needed by the consumer and
show sample quotes for reoccurring themes. Our interview partners not questioning why it cannot be done in that cost and with that quality, just, it was
not done before. So the willingness to challenge the existing paradigm and believing
reiterated that innovation processes in China, India, and Singapore are
that it can be done in a different way, and then keeping a focus on trying frugal
very different from each other. Interviewees perceived workers in China changes, and attempting frugal in a very frugal manner (Interviewee 12).
to be disciplined, dedicated, and focused, and to be hard-working and fast, Indian creativity. And with the Indian people, I actually appreciate them a lot because
but also lacking in creativity. Singaporeans were also seen as hard- they are quite intelligent. I think they are very intelligent in terms of IQ and also
working and ambitious. Singaporeans were additionally perceived to really creative. I think the problem-solving skills are quite good, but not so focused
(Interviewee 11).
be structured and to usually stick with the plan. Interview partners Indian individuality. Indian engineers and workers bring a certain degree of art to
repeatedly named dealing with failure as a big problem in Singapore. their work. So they believe that they can do it in a different way, which is better than
Singaporeans were perceived to be afraid of failing. Indians were the normal way prescribed. So they apply, they tend to apply a certain amount of
repeatedly described as creative and having exceptional problem-solving individual process or individual thought to what they do (Interviewee 12).
Singaporeans stick to the plan. Whereas my experience with Singapore is that they are
skills. Indians were also perceived to apply more art and individuality in
very quick to go to a plan, then, once they have a plan, all sort of this expectation is
their work. Additionally, Indians were perceived to see, in contrast to ticked to it, you know. And everyone kind of just goes on and does the thing with the
Chinese and Singaporeans, processes as more adaptable and deadlines as expectations in mind, and everyone does exactly what he is briefed (Interviewee 2).
more flexible. Our Indian interview partners referred to setbacks as Singaporean structuredness. As a Singapore company, we expect the processes to be
learning experiences rather than problems. Moreover, our interview very structured (Interviewee 5).

partners repeatedly described Indians as focusing on and valuing


frugality, while also expecting high quality levels. large-scale quantitative analysis of cultural differences. The question­
In summary, in line with our theoretical arguments, Chinese were naire was answered by technology or engineering students across (i.a.)
described as disciplined and focused (which might follow from guanxi, China (N = 261), India (N = 190), and Singapore (N = 191). The items
ting hua, and yao mianzi), Singaporeans as structured, fearing failure, were developed based on cross-cultural and innovation literature,
and sticking to the plan (which is related to kiasu), and Indians as cre­ exploratory findings from our previous analyses of cultural differences,
ative, flexible, and frugal (which is related to leela and jugaad). These and frequent discussions within our multinational research team rep­
observations again suggest high exploitation in China, high exploration resenting the countries of interest. After developing a rough taxonomy of
in India, and low exploration in Singapore. diverse constructs that had been suggested to be relevant to Asian cul­
ture and innovation, items were developed in two brainstorming and
4.2. Pilot 2: Cultural differences and their relation with exploration and discussion sessions. As recommended for cross-cultural analyses, the
exploitation wording of the items reflects concrete cultural practices or descriptive
norms (i.e., how people of a cultural background commonly think or
In the next step, we investigated whether the identified cultural behave) (Frese, 2015). We used semantic differentials (Friborg, Marti­
differences differentiate the countries and whether the differences relate nussen, & Rosenvinge, 2006; Heine, Lehman, Peng, & Greenholtz, 2002)
to exploration vs. exploitation. To do so, we analyzed data from ques­ because responses to semantic differentials are less affected by ten­
tionnaires that were collected as part of preparations for developing a dencies toward extremes or the middle, which vary across countries. The

413
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

developed items were refined based on independent ratings by re­ Thus, these findings support the notion that guanxi, ting hua, yao
searchers and cognitive interviews (on a representative set of 26 items) mianzi, leela, jugaad, and kiasu differentiate the three cultures. Jugaad
with individuals from the Asian countries. Following recommendations and leela appear to relate to exploration more than exploitation,
outlined in Brislin (1970), we engaged in back-translation procedures. whereas yao mianzi, ting hua, and kiasu seem to relate to exploitation
For our pilot study, we analyzed differences on items that exemplify more than exploration. As India scores high on the cultural attributes
differences in the culture-specific concepts that we propose contrast the that are related to exploration, these results again suggest a tendency
three countries of interest and relate to exploration and exploitation toward exploration in India and, vice versa, a tendency toward exploi­
(guanxi, ting hua, yao mianzi, leela, jugaad, and kiasu). tation in China and Singapore.
The items contain responses to situation descriptions, in which par­
ticipants indicate on a semantic differential which of two practices is 5. Method and results of the main study
more likely in their own society (see Table 2). We analyzed country
mean differences on each item and calculated correlations with two 5.1. Participants and procedures
items measuring tendencies toward exploration vs. exploitation, i.e., one
measuring preferences for applying current vs. coming up with new Our main study was a survey study with innovation teams in China,
solutions, and the other measuring preferences for mature vs. new India, and Singapore. We collected multisourced team data in a one-time
projects. In the following, we report mean differences (MDs) and their online survey from team members and their formal supervisors. All
significance in ANOVA post hoc tests to compare the cultural tendencies contacted teams were working on an R&D project in an engineering- or
in the three countries. Additionally, we report correlations with the two technology-related field. Those teams were working on innovation, here
measures of tendencies toward exploration vs. exploitation (rnew solution defined as embarking on anything new, including but not limited to a new
and rnew project). As shown in Table 2, the results suggest that tendencies project, new product, or new restructuring process. Our aim was to contact
toward exploration vs. exploitation might indeed differentiate the cul­ teams working for companies that are “typical” for each country. We
tures in China, India, and Singapore. In line with our hypotheses, Chi­ included teams consisting of at least one supervisor and two or more
nese participants indicated stronger tendencies toward exploitation in team member responses. Each team was given a unique team code to
their society (H1) than Indian participants, who indicated stronger match the data. A total of 624 individuals across 138 valid teams
tendencies toward exploration (H2) on both measures (MDnew solution = participated in the research2. The supervisor questionnaire took 5–10
− 0.53, p < .01; MDnew project = − 0.55, p < .01). Indians (MDnew solution = min, and the team member questionnaire took 25–30 min to fill out.
1.05, p < .01) and Chinese (MDnew solution = 0.52, p < .01) also indicated Team innovation can be assessed either by self or supervisor ratings
stronger tendencies toward exploration than Singaporeans (H2) on one (Hülsheger et al., 2009). In our main analysis, we evaluate the model
measure. with multisource data (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003),
We found the assumed country differences in cultural tendencies i.e., team members’ ratings of team exploration and exploitation and
regarding the concepts of our theorizing: Chinese (MD = 0.79, p < .01) supervisors’ ratings of team innovativeness. In robustness checks, we
and Singaporeans (MD = 0.52, p < .01) indicated a stronger tendency additionally evaluate the validity of the findings by recalculating the
toward keeping opinions to themselves (vs. pointing to problems) than model with (a) team members’ ratings of team innovativeness and (b)
Indians (this tendency is related to practicing harmonious relationships supervisors’ ratings of team exploration and exploitation.
as suggested by guanxi and ting hua), and this tendency was negatively In China, we had 49 teams from six large companies in raw materials,
correlated with exploration (rnew solution = − 0.18, p < .01; rnew project = pharmaceuticals or research-related fields. The average team tenure was
− 0.13, p < .01), as suggested by our theorizing. Being afraid of loss of 3.75 years. The mean team size was 10.82 members. A significant
face (mianzi) also negatively correlated with tendencies toward explo­ portion of team members (38%) had a bachelor’s degree, 14% had a
ration (rnew solution = − 0.11, p < .01; rnew project = − 0.14, p < .01), as 12th grade education, and 16% had a master’s degree. All participants
expected, and this tendency was higher for the Chinese than for the were born in China. The participants completed the survey in Chinese.
Indian society (MD = 0.52, p < .01) and higher for the Singaporean than For the Chinese version of the questionnaire, the English items were
for the Indian society (MD = 0.86, p < .01). Singaporeans indicated translated into Chinese and retranslated back into English by a different
being afraid of loss of face as a stronger tendency in their society than person, based on back-translation procedures (Brislin, 1970).
the Chinese (MD = 0.33, p < .01). Feeling insecure (vs. challenged) In India, we had 40 teams from 34 companies of small to large en­
when facing uncertainty (related to kiasu) was also negatively corre­ terprises, to reflect the diverse market in India. The average team tenure
lated with exploration tendencies (rnew solution = − 0.13, p < .01; rnew project was 3.27 years. The mean team size was 10.62 members. The majority of
= − 0.12, p < .01), as expected. Singaporeans indicated a stronger ten­ the respondents in the Indian teams had university education, 45% had a
dency toward feeling insecure in their society compared to both Chinese bachelor’s degree, 26% had a master’s degree, and 1.8% had a doctoral
(MD = 1.35, p < .01) and Indians (MD = 0.58, p < .01); Chinese also degree. Except for one participant born in Nepal and another born in
indicated a stronger tendency toward feeling insecure than Indians (MD Singapore, all other participants were born in India. The participants
= 0.77, p < .01). The preference for being viewed as creative and completed the survey in English because English is the usual language to
adaptive (vs. precise, reliable, and efficient) was, as expected, positively be used in education and in most innovation companies in India.
correlated with exploration, at least one measure was significant (rnew In Singapore, we had 40 teams from 27 companies of small to large
solution = 0.20, p < .01; rnew project = n.s.). This preference for creative and size, including government agencies and global companies, which are
adaptive characteristics (which is related to valuing jugaad and leela) typical for Singapore. The average team tenure was 3.56 years. The
was indicated to be stronger for the Indian society than for both the mean team size was 6.23 members. More than half of the respondents
Chinese (MD = 0.40, p < .05) and the Singaporean society (MD = 1.23, had a university education, with 41% holding a bachelor’s degree, 16%
p < .01) and stronger for the Chinese than for the Singaporean society holding a master’s degree, and 4.1% holding a doctoral degree. Out of
(MD = 0.83, p < .01). Valuing orderliness and organization (related to 196 individuals, their ethnicity included 162 Singaporeans, 11 Malay­
kiasu) compared to imaginativeness and creativity (related to leela) was, sians, seven Chinese, four Indians, and 10 participants from 10 other
as expected, negatively correlated with exploration tendencies (rnew so­ countries. In comparison to teams in China and India, Singapore had the
lution = − 0.11, p < .01; rnew project = − 0.10, p < .05). Tendencies toward highest proportion of cross-cultural teams, which reflects the city’s
orderliness and organization were indicated to be stronger for the Sin­
gaporean than for both the Chinese (MD = 0.95, p < .01) and the Indian
society (MD = 0.58; p < .01) and stronger for the Indian than for the 2
The data was collected as part of a larger research project, with other
Chinese society (MD = 0.37, p < .05). subprojects focusing additional research questions.

414
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Table 2
Cultural differences and their relation with exploration vs. exploitation (Pilot 2)
Culture specific Measure M M M MD MD MD Correlation with
concepts, assumed China India SG China China India exploration vs.
country differences –India –SG –SG exploitation

new new
solution project

1 Preference for applying current solutions vs. coming up 2.25 2.78 1.72 − 0.53** 0.52** 1.05**
exploitation vs. with new solutions. (new solution)
2 exploration Preference for a mature project vs. a new project. (new 1.45 2.00 1.69 − 0.55** − 0.23 0.33
project)
3 guanxi/ting hua In case of disagreement, directly pointing out the 3.26 2.46 3.01 0.79** 0.27 − 0.52** − 0.18** − 0.13**
high in China problems vs. keeping their opinion to oneself.
4 mianzi People are not afraid of loss of face vs. afraid of loss of 3.71 3.18 4.05 0.52** − 0.33** − 0.86** − 0.11** − 0.14**
high in China face.
5 kiasu Uncertainty feels challenging vs. insecure. 3.08 3.86 4.44 − 0.77** − 1.35** − 0.58** − 0.13** − 0.12**
high in SG
6 jugaad/leela Preference for being viewed as precise, reliable, efficient 3.17 3.57 2.36 − 0.40* 0.83** 1.23** 0.20**
high in India vs. creative, adaptive, approaching tasks from
unsuspected angles.
7 leela vs. kiasu Most important characteristics are believed to be 2.59 2.96 3.53 − 0.37* − 0.95** − 0.58** − 0.11** − 0.10*
India < SG imaginativeness and creativity vs. orderliness and
organization.

Note. N = 642; *p < .05; **p < .01; M = Mean; MD = Mean Difference; SG = Singapore; for each item, lower values are closer to the option mentioned first (e.g.,
current solution/mature project), higher values are closer to the option mentioned second (e.g., new solution/new project).

cosmopolitan outlook. The participants completed the survey in English, together in an innovation process. The educational level can be related
which is an official language in Singapore. to team members’ innovation ability (Fasko, 2001). Therefore, we ran
the analyses again while controlling for team size (team members re­
5.2. Measurements ported the number of members in their team), team conflict (reported by
team members on a 5-point Likert scale), and education level (whether
Team exploration/exploitation activity. Team members reported the majority of team members had a university degree, as reported by
team exploration and exploitation for the main model. We used five team members). We do not discuss the details of those effects because we
items to measure team exploration, e.g., “We tested new methods and assume that company size, team size, conflict, and education similarly
ways of working”, and six items to measure team exploitation, e.g., “We influence innovation processes across cultures, such that they are
relied on well-established routines”, based on scales by He and Wong beyond the scope of our analysis. The results of our model that tests
(2004). Participants rated whether they agreed to each item, thinking country differences remained stable in the analyses with additional
about the last month, on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) controls.
to 5 (absolutely), with an internal consistency of α = 0.85 for exploration
and α = 0.82 for exploitation. We aggregated team members’ ratings to 5.3. Analyses and results
obtain team measures based on rwg (both variables reported rwg >
0.80). We aggregated the individual scores to team level (exploration Table 3 presents the correlations among the main study variables.
ICC(1) = 0.54, ICC(2) = 0.85; exploitation ICC(1) = 0.43, ICC(2) = Team exploration was positively correlated with team exploitation (r =
0.82). 0.22, p < .05) and team innovativeness (r = 0.62, p < .001) among the
Team innovativeness. Innovativeness requires creating something team ratings, and among the supervisor ratings also positively correlated
that is new, has practical use, and is of quality. To produce ideas and with team exploitation (r = 0.28, p < .01) and team innovativeness (r =
solutions that are novel, useful, and of high quality, both the team’s 0.37, p < .001). Company size correlated negatively with team explo­
creativity and its ability to implement is needed. In line with innova­ ration (r = -0.31, p < .001). Team ratings correlated positively with
tiveness conceptualizations (Amabile, 1983; Anderson, Potočnik, & supervisor ratings for team exploration (r = 0.18, p < .05), team
Zhou, 2014; Oldham & Cummings, 1996; West & Farr, 1989), we exploitation (r = 0.25, p < .01), and team innovativeness (r = 0.45, p <
assessed the supervisors’ perception of the novelty, usefulness, and .001), indicating measurement validity.
quality of the team’s innovation output. Using Rosing et al.’s (2018) In the first step, we evaluated country differences by calculating
items with a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 5, the team’s direct super­ country means and conducting an ANOVA. Table 4 presents the means
visors rated the innovation’s novelty from “a usual and conventional and standard deviations for the variables in our model by country. The
solution” to “completely novel and hardly relies on conventional and means provide a first indication for country differences in team explo­
usual solutions”, its usefulness from “a useless solution” to “a completely ration and exploitation, as suggested by Hypotheses 1 and 2. China has
useful solution”, and its quality from “an insufficient solution” to “a the highest mean for team exploitation, and India has the highest mean
much better than standard solution”. The alpha coefficient for team for team exploration. Table 5 shows the results of an ANOVA that tests
innovativeness was 0.86. country mean differences. The results show that there are significant
Control Variables. In the main analysis, we calculated the model country-level differences in the predicted directions. Team exploitation
controlling for company size (reported by team members on a 5-point (H1) was significantly higher in China than in Singapore (MD = − 0.36,
scale with size categories) because the size of the company might in­ p < .01) and India (MD = − 0.24, p < .05). Team exploration (H2) was
fluence how important creativity, efficiency, and innovativeness is, and significantly higher in India than in Singapore (MD = − 0.30, p < .05).
how much influence a team has on a particular innovation, and mean These differences also indicate that in Singaporean teams, both team
company size varied between the countries. In robustness checks, we exploration and team exploitation were significantly lower than those in
calculated the model without controls and with other controls. Team Chinese and Indian teams. Team innovativeness was significantly higher
size and team conflict determine the communication and motivation in a in India than in China (0.33, p < .05).
team, which can influence whether a team can productively work Next, we tested the direct and indirect effects on team exploration,

415
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Table 3
Correlations (main study)
Team ratings Supervisor ratings

Team exploration Team exploitation Team innovativeness Team exploration Team exploitation Team innovativeness Company size Team size

Team ratings
Team exploration —
Team exploitation 0.22 * —
Team innovativeness 0.62 *** 0.13 —

Supervisor ratings
Team exploration 0.18 * -0.12 0.23 * —
Team exploitation 0.22 * 0.25 ** 0.16 0.28 ** —
Team innovativeness 0.34 *** 0.01 0.45 *** 0.37 *** —
0.16

Controls
Company size -0.06 0.08 -0.14 -0.31 *** -0.15 —
0.06
Team size 0.09 0.08 0.09 0.01 0.13 —
0.09 0.05
Conflict 0.03 -0.04 -0.15 0.19 * 0.01 -0.04
-0.00 -0.16

Note: N = 138 teams; * p < .5, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Table 4 India, indicating that higher levels of team exploration in India may feed
Descriptives by country (main study) forward into team innovativeness (β = 0.13; p = .045). Thus, the data
Country Mean SD SE support Hypothesis 4, indicating that team exploration activities, which
were highest in India, positively relate to team innovativeness.
Team exploration Singapore 3.09 0.55 0.09
India 3.38 0.59 0.09 Robustness checks and additional analyses. As previous research
China 3.19 0.51 0.07 suggested an interaction effect between exploration and exploitation
Team exploitation Singapore 3.06 0.50 0.08 (ambidexterity), we ran an additional analysis that included the inter­
India 3.18 0.45 0.07 action; however, the interaction was nonsignificant. Moreover, we reran
China 3.42 0.46 0.07
Team innovativeness Singapore 3.76 0.67 0.11
our model without controls and reran it with other control variables, i.e.,
India 3.95 0.69 0.11 team size, team conflict, and team education level. In those additional
China 3.62 0.58 0.08 models, the pattern remained stable. As a validity check, we also
recalculated the mediation model with the team members’ ratings for all
variables of the model (team exploration, exploitation, and innova­
exploitation, and innovativeness in a regression analysis (Table 6).
tiveness) instead of using the supervisors’ ratings for team innovative­
Dummy coded variables for the country effects were the independent
ness; in this analysis, the relationships had the same directions as in the
variables, team member-rated team exploration and exploitation were
main analysis and were even stronger. In an analysis with the supervi­
the mediators, and supervisor-rated team innovativeness was the
sors’ ratings for all variables, we found positive effects on team explo­
outcome variable. In this analysis, we controlled for company size (see
ration and exploitation for India and a negative effect on exploration for
also our robustness checks in which we report the results without this
China. These findings are also in line with our prediction that China is
control and with other controls). Mediation effects were calculated with
more focused on exploitation than exploration.
1000 bias-corrected bootstraps. As we tested whether we would find
higher levels of team exploitation in China and higher levels of team
6. Discussion
exploration in India, we used Singapore as the “baseline”.
Supporting Hypothesis 1, we found a positive effect for China on
We investigated differences in team exploration and exploitation
team exploitation (β = 0.35; p < .001), indicating more exploitation
across China, India, and Singapore and analyzed whether those differ­
activities in Chinese teams. Supporting Hypothesis 2, we also found an
ences explained differences in team innovativeness among the three
effect for India on team exploration (β = 0.33; p = .013), indicating more
countries. Our findings indicate that teams focus more strongly on either
exploration in Indian teams. Hypothesis 3 predicted that team exploi­
exploration or exploitation, depending on national culture. We elabo­
tation mediates the effect of Chinese culture on team innovativeness.
rated on innovation-related culture specifics in China, India, and
Hypothesis 4 predicted that team exploration mediates the effect of In­
Singapore and suggest that those culture specifics explain differences in
dian culture on team innovativeness. In our data, team exploration was
team exploration, exploitation, and innovativeness. Our Asia-centric
significantly positively related to team innovativeness (β = 0.38; p <
perspective on innovation expands the theoretical scope of factors that
.001), but team exploitation was not. Therefore, the indirect effect for
may influence innovativeness.
China on team innovativeness via team exploitation was not significant,
We found in an interview pilot study that Chinese were described as
and Hypothesis 3 was not supported. We did find an indirect effect for

Table 5
ANOVA post hoc results (main study)
Team exploration Team exploitation Team innovativeness

India China India China India China

Singapore (MD) -0.30* -0.10 − 0.13 -0.36** -0.18 0.14


India (MD) — 0.19 — -0.24* — 0.33*

Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001; MD = Mean Difference.

416
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Table 6
Regression analysis (main study).
DV: Team Exploration

95% C.I.

Estimate SE Lower Upper β df t p

India a 0.33 0.13 0.07 0.60 0.28 125 2.52 0.013


China a 0.05 0.12 − 0.19 0.29 0.04 125 0.40 0.693

DV: Team Exploitation

95% C.I.

Estimate SE Lower Upper β df t p

India a 0.13 0.11 − 0.09 0.35 0.13 125 1.16 0.249


China a 0.35 0.10 0.15 0.55 0.36 125 3.41 < 0.001

Mediation model 95% C.I.

Estimate SE Lower Upper β z p

Components
India a ⇒ exploration 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.33
India a ⇒ exploitation 0.13 0.12 − 0.12 0.40 0.13 1.08 0.282
China a ⇒ exploration 0.05 0.12 − 0.17 0.28 0.04 0.42 0.677
China a ⇒ exploitation 0.35 0.10 0.15 0.54 0.36 3.44 < 0.001
exploration ⇒ innovativeness 0.38 0.10 0.21 0.58 0.33 3.91 < 0.001
exploitation ⇒ innovativeness − 0.01 0.13 − 0.28 0.23 − 0.01 − 0.11 0.914

Indirect effect

India a ⇒ exploration ⇒ innovativeness 0.13 0.06 0.04 0.30 0.09 2.00 0.045

Note. a In this model, we included two dummy variables to differentiate the countries. The two dummies, “India” and “China”, compare to Singapore as a baseline.
Confidence intervals are calculated with bias corrected bootstrap. In this model, we control for company size.

disciplined and focused in innovation projects, Singaporeans as struc­ Second, we elaborated on culture-specific concepts to explain why
tured, fearing failure, and sticking to the plan, and Indians as creative, Indian teams may tend toward exploration and why Chinese teams may
flexible, and frugal. These differences relate to the concepts of guanxi, tend toward exploitation. We explored how far culture-specific factors
ting hua, yao mianzi, leela, jugaad, and kiasu, which we suggest influ­ differentiate cultural tendencies in China, India, and Singapore and how
ence innovation in China, India, and Singapore. An additional survey such culture-specific factors relate to tendencies toward exploration vs.
pilot indicated that jugaad and leela, which we found to be particularly exploitation. We suggest considering guanxi, ting hua, yao mianzi, leela,
influential in India, related to exploration more than exploitation. In jugaad, and kiasu when analyzing team innovation in China, India, and
contrast, yao mianzi, ting hua, and kiasu, which we found to be Singapore. Our findings support our reasoning and are in line with un­
particularly influential in China and Singapore, related to exploitation certainty avoidance arguments, which would also suggest higher
more than exploration. In our main study, which analyzed survey data exploration in India because of lower uncertainty avoidance in India
from innovation teams and their supervisors, we found that Indian teams than in China and Singapore (House et al., 2004). However, our findings
showed higher team exploration, and Chinese teams higher team are in contrast to power distance and cultural looseness arguments,
exploitation, when comparing China, India, and Singapore. Moreover, which would suggest lower exploration in India because of higher power
we found that the differences in exploration shaped the teams’ inno­ distance and cultural tightness (Gelfand et al., 2011; 2006; House et al.,
vativeness. Our findings hold contributions to the innovation and cross- 2004). Our research addresses calls for a nuanced investigation of cul­
cultural literature. tural differences by innovation research (Lavie et al., 2010; van Knip­
penberg, 2017), and complements previous cross-cultural research
(Gelfand et al., 2011; House et al., 2004) by introducing Asian culture-
6.1. Contribution specific factors as predictors of innovation.
Third, we tested differences in team exploration and exploitation as
First, elaborating on the cultural differences among China, India, and pathways explaining how cultural differences shape innovativeness in
Singapore, three Asian countries that are among the most important innovation teams across China, India, and Singapore. Previous research
innovators globally, goes beyond previous research, which mainly suggested that both exploration and exploitation are needed for suc­
focused on Western cultures or East-West differences. Such traditional cessful innovation, but most of this research focused on the organiza­
East-West comparisons have strongly influenced cross-cultural innova­ tional level. Our study investigated exploration and exploitation at the
tion research. East-West comparisons suggest lower exploration in the team level and suggests that team exploration relates to team innova­
East, because of the importance of status and higher power distance tiveness in China, India, and Singapore. We did not find an influence of
levels (House et al., 2004; Shane, 1992; Xie & Paik, 2019). This previous exploitation and did not find an influence of ambidexterity, the inter­
research predominantly drew on established cultural dimensions (Gel­ action between exploration and exploitation (Bledow et al., 2011). Thus,
fand et al., 2011; House et al., 2004), which may neglect differences future research is needed to better understand the process of combining
across Asia. Our Asia-centric perspective provides a more nuanced pic­ exploration and exploitation at the team level in different cultures
ture, which is needed to understand innovation in Asia (Gupta & Wang, (Yang, Zhou, & Zhang, 2015). Our findings suggest that the assumption
2009). Our research demonstrates the necessity for more research that that the interaction of exploration and exploitation predicts innovation
considers the variability of cultures within Asia and opens up new av­ across countries needs to be reconsidered.
enues for future research.

417
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

6.2. Limitations and avenues for future research future research:


In China, innovation is considered a national priority, and the gov­
Although our multisource data collection holds advantages, our ernment intends to push a transition from a ‘catch-up’ to an ‘innovation
study design also has limitations. The teams in our sample differed not leadership’ orientation (Vinig & Bossink, 2015). Although the govern­
only in national culture but also in other criteria, e.g., gender and age ment’s extensive influence and preference for state-owned enterprises
composition, education level of team members, team size, tenure, and might harm innovation efficiency (Chung & Tan, 2017), several factors,
team climate, which might also influence team innovativeness. For such as an advanced financial market, facilitate innovation in China (Li,
example, there could be differences in exploration behavior due to ed­ Lee, & Ko, 2017). Chinese companies seem to effectively iterate between
ucation level because education could determine the creative ability of external and internal knowledge generation, which helps them reduce
team members (Fasko, 2001). Although we did not find an influence of costs and speed up product development (Williamson & Yin, 2014). The
these controls in our data, future research should investigate additional Chinese innovation system seems to gradually move from a public-
influence mechanisms in more detail. Moreover, we did not investigate centered approach to a firm-centered approach (Vinig & Bossink,
differences among different regional areas within each country, which 2015). Future research is needed for a better understanding of how
would provide an even more accurate picture. much this situation might also contribute to China’s innovation success
More research is needed to better understand the mechanisms of the (Dutta et al., 2020).
influence of culture-specific aspects on innovation. Our findings are in In India, the highly diverse ethnic, religious, and geographic situa­
line with our prediction that exploration is particularly high in India and tion, resource constraints, and India’s complex system of regulations and
exploitation is particularly high in China; however, future research is licenses (Tymon et al., 2010) complicate innovation activities. Despite
needed to test the mediating role of the culture-specific factors under­ these difficulties, India has been classified as an innovation achiever in
lying our theorizing. recent innovation rankings and ranks among the best for quality in
Our measures of team exploration and exploitation as well as team innovation in middle-income economies (Dutta et al., 2020). Our find­
innovativeness were based on perceptions. However, our findings of ings suggest cultural tendencies to be drivers of exploration activity in
country differences could be influenced by differences in perceptions. India. Future research could aim for a better understanding of how
For example, Singaporeans could be particularly critical with them­ cultural tendencies and market dynamics work together.
selves, and being critical could negatively influence their perceptions of In Singapore, economic relationships are largely coordinated by
team innovation processes and outcomes; then, exploration, exploita­ market competition with strong government interventions (Wang,
tion, and innovativeness might actually not be lower but rather 2018). The government has (indirect) control over all major local banks
perceived lower than in the other countries. Moreover, tendencies to­ and runs powerful organizations, boards, and companies in most in­
ward higher values, lower values, or the middle might differ across dustries. The government fosters entrepreneurship, and innovation in
countries and thereby bias our analyses of cross-cultural differences. Singapore is often policy-driven. Additionally, large foreign multina­
These limitations are common in cross-cultural research. Our design has tional companies (MNCs) influence the innovation system (Wang,
some advantages in this regard, because of reduced common source bias, 2018). Thus, most innovation activities in Singapore are coordinated
because team members reported team activities and team supervisors and focused on only a few fields selected by the government or MNCs.
reported team innovativeness. Nevertheless, international talent and multinational headquarters in
We analyzed differences among China, India, and Singapore. This Singapore are likely to facilitate innovation. Rankings report lively
comparison is of course just a first step toward an Asia-centric innovation and entrepreneurship activities in Singapore, and Singapore
perspective. Future research is needed that includes other Asian coun­ continues to rank among the leading innovation-driven nations (Dutta
tries and additional comparisons. Nevertheless, several of the cultural et al., 2020). More research is needed to show how Singapore overcomes
specifics, which we considered in our conceptual model, also are influ­ the cultural tendencies that we found to counteract exploration activity.
ential in other Asian countries. For example, guanxi, which we consid­
ered for China, is related to inhwa in Korea, and yao mianzi, also 6.3. Conclusion
considered for China, is also a concern in Japan, translated as tuk muk.
Knowledge on how cultural specifics are distributed across Asia, Our research indicates that, because of differences in national cul­
particularly how they shape innovation across different Asian countries, ture, some innovation teams may focus on exploration and others on
is limited. We call for more research on cultural specifics and differences exploitation. Previous literature primarily examined innovation in
across all Asian countries and their influence on innovation processes. Western cultures and East-West comparisons. With an Asian-centric
In this study, we did not focus on one particular kind of innovation. approach, we found culture to differentiate team innovation behaviors
Although all teams in our sample were working in R&D projects in a across three Asian countries. While cultural factors are often blamed for
technology- or engineering-related field, the innovations included limited innovation potential in Asia (Chen et al., 2011; House et al.,
different kinds of innovations. Innovations can focus on particular 2004; Rao & Pearce, 2016), a differentiated picture of innovation pro­
knowledge or technology (Huarng, Mas-Tur, & Moreno, 2018) and can cesses across Asia is missing. Previous research at the intersection of
include product, service, and process innovations. Some cultural aspects culture and innovation focused on cultural dimensions such as uncer­
might be tailored to a specific kind of innovation. For example, jugaad tainty avoidance, collectivism, power distance, and cultural tightness,
might be particularly helpful for product innovation, whereas kiasu, but the findings from this research remain mainly inconclusive (Hayton
with its attention to detail, could be critical for process innovations. et al., 2002; Hayton & Cacciotti, 2013). Our research contributes to a
Since different countries tend to attract different industries, it is difficult better understanding of cultural influences on innovation, particularly
to disentangle national and subject matter effects. Such differences are regarding team innovation processes across Asia.
not covered in our analyses and need attention in future research. Our Asia-centric approach to innovation introduces culture-specific
Our study focused on the influence of national culture on team factors that can explain culture-specific mechanisms in China, India,
innovation. However, also the innovation situation in the local market, and Singapore. We suggest that the relevance of ting hua (‘listening to
and support from industry and government, influence innovation ac­ words’) and the need for yao mianzi (preserving face) may harm
tivity. Additionally, the economic situation in a country influences exploration in Chinese teams. Nevertheless, ting hua and yao mianzi, as
innovation processes (Wang, Yu, & Liu, 2013). China, India, and well as guanxi (strong dyadic social relationships), may facilitate
Singapore all have become increasingly successful in recent years but exploitation activity in Chinese teams. In Singaporean teams, high levels
are very different from one another, not only in terms of culture. of kiasu (fear of losing out) may harm exploration. In Indian teams,
Considering the market situations in the countries opens up avenues for tendencies stemming from valuing leela (playfulness and naughtiness)

418
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

and jugaad (creative adaptation for frugal innovation) may facilitate managers better understand how to manage innovation across Asia.
exploration. Thus, going beyond the factors that were considered in
previous literature, we suggest additional predictors of innovation ac­
tivities, which may explain why teams may focus on exploration in some Declaration of Competing Interest
cultures and on exploitation in others.
Drawing on empirical analyses across China, India, and Singapore, The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
our research suggests that managing innovation in teams requires interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
considering culture specifics. Knowledge about cultural tendencies can the work reported in this paper.
help organizations and managers leverage the strengths of their inno­
vation teams and compensate for weaknesses (Bledow et al., 2011). For Acknowledgement
example, considering the tendencies toward exploration in India, mak­
ing sure that the project moves toward implementation appears to be This research was supported by funding of the Singapore Ministry of
particularly important. In China and Singapore, in contrast, it appears to Education Social Science Research Thematic Grant under MOE2017-
be more important to create a trustful atmosphere in which team SSRTG-022. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommenda­
members dare to bring up their own ideas. Moreover, cross-border tions expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not
innovation projects could aim to leverage the strengths of each cul­ reflect the views of the Singapore Ministry of Education or the Singapore
ture. We believe that continuous research efforts may help innovation Government.

Appendix A

Additional information about pilot 1 (interview study)

Interview partners

In a first step, we contacted potential interviewees via e-mail, based on the network of the authors. Then, we applied a snowball system, i.e., we asked the managers whom we
interviewed to introduce us to additional managers. All interviewees had several years of experience in innovation projects, in one or more of the countries of interest.

Interview questions for the positive critical incident

Situation Please think of one recent situation at your workplace in China/India/Singapore in which one of your team innovations went particularly well (i.e., was
successfully implemented).
Try to remember as many details as possible – what you were thinking and feeling; when it happened; where it took place. Try to place yourself back in that
moment.
Innovation Tell me about that innovation project from your point of view. What was the innovation project about?
Innovation Please describe the innovation process in more detail. How was the innovation achieved? How were ideas generated? How were ideas implemented?
process
Success factors What was most essential to why it went well? Which factors were beneficial for the process?
Challenges Which challenges did you face? How did you deal with these challenges?
Culture In what way do you think the described innovation process was typical for China/India/Singapore? Which cultural factors could have influenced the innovation
process?
Ending question Is there anything else that we didn‘t talk about that appears relevant to you?

Interview questions for the negative critical incident

Situation Now, think of one situation at your workplace in China/India/Singapore in which one of your team innovations did not work out (i.e., was not successfully
implemented). Again, try to remember as many details as possible – what you were thinking and feeling; when it happened; where it took place. Try to place
yourself back in that moment.
Innovation Tell me about that innovation project from your point of view. What was the innovation project about?
Innovation Please describe the innovation process in more detail. Why did the innovation not work out? How were ideas generated? How were ideas implemented?
process
Detrimental What was most essential to why it went wrong? Which factors were detrimental for the innovation process?
factors
Positive aspects Despite the failed implementation, what went well in that innovation process?
Culture In what way do you think the described innovation process was typical for China/India/Singapore? Which cultural factors could have influenced the innovation
process?
Ending question Is there anything else that we didn‘t talk about that appears relevant to you?

References Barkema, H. G., Chen, X.-P., George, G., Luo, Y., & Tsui, A. S. (2015). West meets east:
New concepts and theories. Academy of Management Journal, 58(2), 460–479.
Beckman, C. M. (2006). The influence of founding team company affiliations on firm
Agarwal, N., & Brem, A. 2012. Frugal and reverse innovation - Literature overview and
behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 49(4), 741–758.
case study insights from a German MNC in India and China. Conference proceedings
Bedford, O., & Chua, S. H. (2018). Everything also I want: An exploratory study of
of the 18th International ICE Conference on Engineering, Technology and
Singaporean Kiasuism (fear of losing out). Culture & Psychology, 24(4), 491–511.
Innovation, Munich, Germany, publisher: IEEE.
Benner, M. J., & Tushman, M. L. (2015). Reflections on the 2013 Decade
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential
Award—“Exploitation, Exploration, and Process Management: The Productivity
conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(2), 357–376.
Dilemma Revisited” Ten Years Later. Academy of Management Review, 40(4),
Anderson, N. R., Potočnik, K., & Zhou, J. (2014). Innovation and creativity in
497–514.
organizations: A state-of-the-science review, prospective commentary, and guiding
Bledow, R., Frese, M., & Mueller, V. (2011). Ambidextrous leadership for innovation: The
framework. Journal of Management, 40(5), 1297–1333.
influence of culture. In W. H. Mobley, M. Li, & Y. Wang (Eds.), Advances in Global
Anderson, N. R., & West, M. A. (1998). Measuring climate for work group innovation:
Leadership (Vol. 6, pp. 41–69). Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Development and validation of the team climate inventory. Journal of Organizational
Bledow, R., Frese, M., Anderson, N., Erez, M., & Farr, J. (2009). A dialectic perspective
Behavior, 19(3), 235–258.
on innovation: Conflicting demands, multiple pathways, and ambidexterity.
Baer, M. (2012). Putting creativity to work: The implementation of creative ideas in
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2(3), 305–337.
organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 55(5), 1102–1119.

419
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross- House, R. J., Hanges, P., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P., & Gupta, V. (Eds.). (2004). Culture,
Cultural Psychology, 1(3), 185–216. leadership, and organizations: the GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, Calif:
Cappelli, P., Singh, H., Singh, J., & Useem, M. (2015). Indian business leadership: Broad Sage Publications.
mission and creative value. Leadership Quarterly, 26(1), 7–12. Huarng, K. H. (2013). A two-tier business model and its realization for entrepreneurship.
Chen, G., Sharma, P. N., Edinger, S. K., Shapiro, D. L., & Farh, J.-L. (2011). Motivating Journal of Business Research, 66(10), 2102–2105.
and demotivating forces in teams: Cross-level influences of empowering leadership Huarng, K. H., Mas-Tur, A., & Moreno, F. C. (2018). Innovation, knowledge, judgment,
and relationship conflict. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(3), 541–557. and decision-making as virtuous cycles. Journal of Business Research, 88, 278–281.
Chen, X.-P., & Chen, C. C. (2004). On the intricacies of the Chinese Guanxi: a process Hülsheger, U. R., Anderson, N., & Salgado, J. F. (2009). Team-level predictors of
model of Guanxi development. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 21(3), 305–324. innovation at work: A comprehensive meta-analysis spanning three decades of
Cheng, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2017). Kiasu and creativity in Singapore: An empirical test research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(5), 1128–1145.
of the situated dynamics framework. Management and Organization Review, 13(4), Hwang, K. (1987). Face and Favor: The Chinese Power Game. American Journal of
871–894. Sociology, 92(4), 944–974.
Chow, C. W., Harrison, G. L., McKinnon, J. L., & Wu, A. (1999). Cultural influences on Hwang, K. (2000). Chinese Relationalism: Theoretical construction and methodological
informal information sharing in Chinese and Anglo-American organizations: An considerations. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 30(2), 155–178.
exploratory study. Accounting. Organizations and Society, 24(7), 561–582. Janssen, O. (2003). Innovative behaviour and job involvement at the price of conflict and
Chuang, A., Hsu, R. S., Wang, A.-C., & Judge, T. A. (2015). Does west “Fit” with east? In less satisfactory relations with co-workers. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Search of a Chinese model of person-environment fit. Academy of Management Psychology, 76(3), 347–364.
Journal, 58(2), 480–510. Javalgi, R. (Raj) G., Todd, P. R., Johnston, W. J., & Granot, E (2012). Entrepreneurship,
Chung, L., & Tan, K. H. (2017). The unique Chinese innovation pathways: Lessons from muddling through, and Indian Internet-enabled SMEs. Journal of Business Research,
Chinese small and mediuem sized manufacturing firms. International Journal of 65(6), 740–744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2010.12.010.
Production Economics, 190, 80–87. Lavie, D., Stettner, U., & Tushman, M. L. (2010). Exploration and exploitation within and
Cox, T. H., Lobel, S. A., & McLeod, P. L. (1991). Effects of ethnic group cultural across organizations. Academy of Management Annals, 4(1), 109–155.
differences on cooperative and competitive behavior on a group task. Academy of Li, H.-C., Lee, W.-C., & Ko, B.-T. (2017). What determines misallocation in innovation? A
Management Journal, 34(4), 827–847. study of regional innovation in China. Journal of Macroeconomics, 52, 221–237.
Dutta, S., Lavin, B., & Wunsch-Vincent, S., 2020. The Global Innovation Index 2020: Who Li, J., Ngin, P. M., & Teo, A. C. Y. 2008. Culture and leadership in Singapore:
Will Finance Innovation? Ithaca, Fontainebleau, and Geneva: Cornell University, Combination of the East and the West. In J. S. Chhokar, F. C. Brodbeck, & R. J. House
INSEAD, and WIPO. (Eds.), LEA’s organization and management series. Culture and leadership across the
Døjbak Håkonsson, D., Eskildsen, J. K., Argote, L., Mønster, D., Burton, R. M., & Obel, B. world: The GLOBE book of in-depth studies of 25 societies (pp. 947–968). Lawrence
(2016). Exploration versus exploitation: Emotions and performance as antecedents Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
and consequences of team decisions. Strategic Management Journal, 37(6), 985–1001. Li, Q., Maggitti, P. G., Smith, K. G., Tesluk, P. E., & Katila, R. (2013). Top management
Dwyer, S., Mesak, H., & Hsu, M. (2005). An exploratory examination of the influence of attention to innovation: The role of search selection and intensity in new product
national culture on cross-national product diffusion. Journal of International introductions. Academy of Management Journal, 56(3), 893–916.
Marketing, 13(2), 1–27. Li, J., Ngin, P. M., & Teo, A. C. (2007). Culture and leadership in Singapore: Combination
Efrat, K. (2014). The direct and indirect impact of culture on innovation. Technovation, of the East and the West. In In Culture and Leadership Across the World (pp.
34(1), 12–20. 981–1002). Psychology Press.
Engelen, A. (2010). Entrepreneurial orientation as a function of national cultural Liao, Y., & Bond, M. H. (2011). The dynamics of face loss following interpersonal harm
variations in two countries. Journal of International Management, 16(4), 354–368. for Chinese and Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(1), 25–38.
Erez, M., & Nouri, R. (2010). Creativity: The influence of cultural, social, and work Low, K. C. P. (2006). Cultural obstacles in growing entrepreneurship: A study in
contexts. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 351–370. Singapore. Journal of Management Development, 25(2), 169–182.
Fasko, D. (2001). Education and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), Luo, X., & Hassan, M. (2009). The role of top management networks for market
317–327. knowledge creation and sharing in China. Journal of Business Research, 62(10),
Frese, M. (2015). Cultural practices, norms, and values. Journal of Cross-Cultural 1020–1026.
Psychology, 46(10), 1327–1330. March, J. G. (1991). Exploration and exploitation in organizational learning.
Friborg, O., Martinussen, M., & Rosenvinge, J. H. (2006). Likert-based vs. semantic Organization Science, 2(1), 71–87.
differential-based scorings of positive psychological constructs: A psychometric McMullen, J. S., & Shepherd, D. A. (2006). Entrepreneurial action and the role of
comparison of two versions of a scale measuring resilience. Personality and Individual uncertainty in the theory of the entrepreneur. Academy of Management Review, 31(1),
Differences, 40(5), 873–884. 132–152.
Fu, P. P., Tsui, A. S., & Dess, G. G. (2006). The dynamics of guanxi in Chinese hightech Mitchell, R. K., Smith, J. B., Morse, E. A., Seawright, K. W., Peredo, A. M., & McKenzie, B.
firms: Implications for knowledge management and decision making. Management (2002). Are entrepreneurial cognitions universal? Assessing entrepreneurial
International Review, 46(3), 277–305. cognitions across cultures. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 26(4), 9–32.
Gao, G. (1998). ‘“Don’t take my word for it”.’—understanding Chinese speaking Morris, M. W., & Leung, K. (2010). Creativity East and West: Perspectives and parallels.
practices. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 22(2), 163–186. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 313–327.
Geletkanycz, M. A. (1997). The salience of ‘culture’s consequences’: The effects of Morrison, E. W., & Phelps, C. C. (1999). Taking charge at work: Extrarole efforts to
cultural values on top executive commitment to the status quo. Strategic Management initiate workplace change. Academy of Management Journal, 42(4), 403–419.
Journal, 18(8), 615–634. Mueller, S. L., & Thomas, A. S. (2001). Culture and entrepreneurial potential: A nine
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., & Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and importance of country study of locus of control and innovativeness. Journal of Business Venturing, 16
cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6), 1225–1244. (1), 51–75.
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., et al. (2011). Nouri, R., Erez, M., Lee, C., Liang, J., Bannister, B. D., & Chiu, W. (2015). Social context:
Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), Key to understanding culture’s effects on creativity. Journal of Organizational
1100–1104. Behavior, 36(7), 899–918.
Gilson, L. L., Mathieu, J. E., Shalley, C. E., & Ruddy, T. M. (2005). Creativity and Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (1996). Employee creativity: Personal and contextual
standardization: Complementary or conflicting drivers of team effectiveness? factors at work. Academy of Management Journal, 39(3), 607–634.
Academy of Management Journal, 48(3), 521–531. Panda, A., & Gupta, R. K. (2004). Mapping cultural diversity within India: A meta-
Gong, Y., Cheung, S. Y., Wang, M., & Huang, J. C. (2012). Unfolding the proactive analysis of some recent studies. Global Business Review, 5(1), 27–49.
process for creativity: Integration of the employee proactivity, information Parker, H. (2016). Team effectiveness and open discussion of conflict in collaborative
exchange, and psychological safety perspectives. Journal of Management, 38(5), new product development: A cross-national study. Journal of Business Research, 69
1611–1633. (11), 4757–4762.
Gupta, A. K., Smith, K. G., & Shalley, C. E. (2006). The interplay between exploration and Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method
exploitation. Academy of Management Journal, 49(4), 693–706. biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended
Gupta, A. K., & Wang, H. 2009. Getting China and India right: Strategies for leveraging remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903.
the world’s fastest growing economies for global advantage. John Wiley & Sons. Rao, A. N., & Pearce, J. L. (2016). Should management practice adapt to cultural values?
Hayton, J. C., & Cacciotti, G. (2013). Is there an entrepreneurial culture? A review of The evidence against power distance adaptation. Cross Cultural & Strategic
empirical research. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 25(9–10), 708–731. Management, 23(2), 257–286.
Hayton, J. C., George, G., & Zahra, S. A. (2002). National culture and entrepreneurship: A Rauch, A., Frese, M., Wang, Z.-M., Unger, J., Lozada, M., Kupcha, V., et al. (2013).
review of behavioral research. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 26(4), 33–52. National culture and cultural orientations of owners affecting the innovation–growth
He, Z. L., & Wong, P. K. (2004). Exploration vs. exploitation: An empirical test of the relationship in five countries. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 25(9–10),
ambidexterity hypothesis. Organization Science, 15(4), 481–494. 732–755.
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross- Ridge, J. W., Johnson, S., Hill, A. D., & Bolton, J. (2017). The role of top management
cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales?: The reference-group effect. Journal team attention in new product introductions. Journal of Business Research, 70, 17–24.
of personality and social psychology, 82(6), 903. Rinne, T., Steel, G. D., & Fairweather, J. (2012). Hofstede and Shane revisited: The role of
Hildebrandt, H. W. (1988). A Chinese managerial view of business communication. power distance and individualism in national-level innovation success. Cross-Cultural
Management Communication Quarterly, 2(2), 217–234. Research, 46(2), 91–108.
Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, J. (1991). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. USA: Rosing, K., Bledow, R., Frese, M., Baytalskaya, N., Johnson Lascano, J., & Farr, J. L.
McGrawHill. (2018). The temporal pattern of creativity and implementation in teams. Journal of
Hossain, M. (2017). Mapping the frugal innovation phenomenon. Technology in Society, Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 91(4), 798–822.
51, 199–208.

420
S. Hubner et al. Journal of Business Research 138 (2022) 408–421

Sahi, J. H. (2018). Bala Krishna: A paradigm of childhood relevant to the present time. In Wang, J. (2018). Innovation and government intervention: A comparison of Singapore
T. S. Saraswathi, S. Menon, & A. Madan (Eds.), Childhoods in India (chapter 6). New and Hong Kong. Research Policy, 47(2), 399–412.
York: Routledge. West, M. A., & Farr, J. L. (1989). Innovation at work: Psychological perspectives. Social
Sarkar, T. (2011). Holy infancy: Love and power in a ‘low caste’ sect in Bengal. South behaviour, 4(1), 15–30.
Asian History and Culture, 2(3), 337–351. Williams, L. K., & McGuire, S. J. (2010). Economic creativity and innovation
Scherer, R., & Gustafsson, J. E. (2015). The relations among openness, perseverance, and implementation: The entrepreneurial drivers of growth? Evidence from 63
performance in creative problem solving: A substantive-methodological approach. Countries. Small Business Economics, 34, 391–412.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 18, 4–17. Williamson, P. J., & Yin, E. (2014). Accelerated innovation: The new challenge from
Shane, S. (1992). Why do some societies invent more than others? Journal of Business China. MIT Sloan Management Review, 55(4), 27.
Venturing, 7(1), 29–46. Xie, G., & Paik, Y. (2019). Cultural differences in creativity and innovation: Are Asian
Shane, S. (1993). Cultural influences on national rates of innovation. Journal of Business employees truly less creative than western employees? Asia Pacific Business Review,
Venturing, 8(1), 59–73. 25(1), 123–147.
Shane, S., Venkataraman, S., & MacMillan, I. (1995). Cultural differences in innovation Xiong Chen, Z., & Aryee, S. (2007). Delegation and employee work outcomes: An
championing strategies. Journal of Management, 21(5), 931–952. examination of the cultural context of mediating processes in China. Academy of
Shin, Y., & Eom, C. (2014). Team proactivity as a linking mechanism between team Management Journal, 50(1), 226–238.
creative efficacy, transformational leadership, and risk-taking norms and team Yang, Z., Zhou, X., & Zhang, P. (2015). Discipline versus passion: Collectivism,
creative performance. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(2), 89–114. centralization, and ambidextrous innovation. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 32
Shteynberg, G., Gelfand, M. J., & Kim, K. (2009). Peering into the “magnum mysterium” (3), 745–769.
of culture: The explanatory power of descriptive norms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Yeung, H. W. C. (2000). State intervention and neoliberalism in the globalizing world
Psychology, 40(1), 46–69. economy: Lessons from Singapore’s regionalization programme. The Pacific Review,
Stephan, U., & Pathak, S. (2016). Beyond cultural values? Cultural leadership ideals and 13(1), 133–162.
entrepreneurship. Journal of Business Venturing, 31(5), 505–523. Zaveri, P. (2018). The Legend of Krishna. In Wall Paintings of Gujarat and Rajasthan ((2018
Stephan, U., & Uhlaner, L. M. (2010). Performance-based vs socially supportive culture: edition.).). Bhopal: New Delhi, India: Niyogi Books.
A cross-national study of descriptive norms and entrepreneurship. Journal of Zhang, X., Li, N., & Brad Harris, T. (2015). Putting non-work ties to work: The case of
International Business Studies, 41(8), 1347–1364. guanxi in supervisor–subordinate relationships. Leadership Quarterly, 26(1), 37–54.
Tan, B. L. B. (2002). Researching managerial values: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal Zhao, K., Zong, B., & Zhang, L. (2020). Explorative and exploitative learning in teams:
of Business Research, 55(10), 815–821. Unpacking the antecedents and consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2041.
Tan, K. P. (2003). Sexing up Singapore. International Journal of Cultural Studies, 6(4), Zhou, J., Shin, S. J., Brass, D. J., Choi, J., & Zhang, Z. X. (2009). Social networks,
403–423. personal values, and creativity: Evidence for curvilinear and interaction effects.
Tang, G., Chen, Y., van Knippenberg, D., & Yu, B. (2020). Antecedents and consequences Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1544.
of empowering leadership: Leader power distance, leader perception of team
capability, and team innovation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41(6), 551–566.
Sylvia Hubner is an Assistant Professor at Free University of Bozen-Bolzano, Italy.
Tjosvold, D., Tang, M. M. L., & West, M. (2004). Reflexivity for team innovation in China:
The contribution of goal interdependence. Group & Organization Management, 29(5),
540–559. Michael Frese is a Professor at Asia School of Business (in collaboration with MIT Sloan
Tsang, E. W. K. (1998). Can guanxi be a source of sustained competitive advantage for Management), Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia, and at Leuphana University of Lüneburg,
doing business in China? Academy of Management Perspectives, 12(2), 64–73. Germany.
Tymon, W. G., Stumpf, S. A., & Doh, J. P. (2010). Exploring talent management in India:
The neglected role of intrinsic rewards. Journal of World Business, 45(2), 109–121.
Zhaoli Song is an Associate Professor at National University of Singapore, Singapore.
van Knippenberg, D. (2017). Team Innovation. Annual Review of Organizational
Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 4(1), 211–233.
Vinig, T., & Bossink, B. (2015). China’s indigenous innovation approach: The emergence Neha Tripathi is an Assistant Professor at Indian Institute of Management Ahmedabad,
of Chinese innovation theory? Technology Analysis & Strategic Management, 27(6), India.
621–627.
Wang, C. L., & Chung, H. F. L. (2013). The moderating role of managerial ties in market Tamara Kaschner conducted her BA and MA studies at Ludwig-Maximilians-University
orientation and innovation: An Asian perspective. Journal of Business Research, 66 Munich and she is now a HRM professional at Altran, Munich, Germany
(12), 2431–2437.
Wang, D. H. M., Yu, T. H. K., & Liu, H. Q. (2013). Heterogeneous effect of high-tech
industrial R&D spending on economic growth. Journal of Business Research, 66(10), Xing Le Kong conducted her BA studies at National University of Singapore, Singapore.
1990–1993.

421

You might also like