You are on page 1of 3

Journal 2

The Problem of Comprehension in Psycholinguistics

Introduction

Psycholinguists are people who study the psychology of language processing.


Individual sentence processing is emphasized in the study of sentence comprehension. We
argue that our comprehension theories are not as informative about the products of processing
as they could be. Although research on discourse processing has demonstrated the importance
of comprehension measures such as question-answering accuracy and text recall, these
findings have not been applied to studies on sentence comprehension. Psychologists
investigate how people interpret a sentence that contains a specific type of linguistic
expression, such as a quantified noun phrase. The standard method entails writing a number
of distinct sentences with and without the quantified expression. Subjects are then shown
stimuli on a computer screen, and their reading times are recorded.

Competing theories are primarily tested using results from online measures, despite
the fact that we have no clear understanding of what those reading times or reaction times
reveal. Decisions about the number and types of items to include are mostly intuitive and
based on rules of thumb, with little empirical support. Traditionally, psycholinguistic research
has focused on outcome measures such as reading comprehension. Our goal is to encourage
further research into sentence comprehension along the lines of discourse processing. We
have a poor understanding of how these concepts interact, emphasizing the need for
additional research.

People often fail to comprehend or comprehend superficially

Ben Stiller purposefully designed misunderstandings in his film There's Something


About Mary to highlight misinterpretations. In classic experiments, these kinds of
misunderstandings have also been elicited in the lab. Some of these sentences are referred to
as Plausible Angloid Gibberish by Pullum (Language Log). Misinterpretations are
widespread and systematic. Subjects who make these mistakes are certain that their
interpretations are correct. Once a proposition or set of propositions has been stored in
memory, it is tenacious and difficult to erase. Infrequent forms, such as the passive, are
frequently misunderstood. These findings demonstrate that researchers cannot simply assume
the meaning that subjects in experiments will obtain based solely on the words and structure
of the sentence.

What causes misinterpretations?


The proclivity for interpretations to deviate from the meaning specified by the
sentence can be attributed to two broad categories of causes. Misinterpretations, it is
assumed, occur because the language system occasionally fails to function properly. The
other is based on the idea that the system was designed to handle certain forms well at the
expense of other linguistic forms. Comprehension frequently occurs in settings other than our
quiet testing rooms. Comprehension has some built-in "auto-correct" functions that repair
erroneous input. We all know that producers are human and make mistakes when they speak
and write. The producer who normalized a speaker's intended form may be to blame. One
explanation for misinterpretations holds that certain linguistic forms are similar to visual
illusions. The missing-verb phrase effect is one example (Gibson & Thomas, 1999). Another
factor is that subjects prefer the illegible. The ancient manuscript to the grammatical version,
which includes the obligatory verb phrase A similar explanation has been proposed for
linguistic illusions such as "More people have visited Russia than I have" (Wellwood et al.,
2018). But shouldn't comprehenders recognize that their interpretations can't be correct
because, while parts of the sentence appear to be correct, the sentence as a whole is
grammatically incorrect? A post-sentence comprehension task would encourage subjects to
process the sentences more carefully, removing some of the local-over-global effects. To
answer this question, readers or listeners will most likely need to be given comprehension
questions or a memory task. Passive, resonance-based memory processes help to support
global coherence.

Why the bias against offline measures?

Standard psycholinguistic research does not assess the content of people's


interpretations more directly. One reason for avoiding the use of question-answering accuracy
or recall is the risk that the results will be dismissed as the result of comprehension.
Reviewers may even advise against publishing an article until the reported effect is detectable
in an online measure. Combining the two sorts of metrics would be the optimum research
technique. Online measurements such as reading times or regression likelihood would reveal
how representations are constructed in real time. The field of discourse comprehension offers
a useful model for this type of research. One reason comprehension tasks are unpopular as
primary dependent measures is that they are "metalinguistic" in the sense that they indicate
what readers or listeners know about their own comprehension processes. However,
answering questions regarding a line or text is not out of the ordinary for most people.
Researchers may predict that comprehension tasks will influence and alter how subjects
approach the process of understanding sentences. This possibly biasing component of
comprehension tasks could be viewed as a bonus rather than a flaw. Some components of
language understanding are automatic, while others are complex and need attention.

Engagement

Subjects' attention regularly wanders away from what they are meant to be reading,
and these mindwandering episodes are most likely connected with shallower understanding.
If a variable like word frequency is subject to task demands, then higher level linguistic
aspects like syntactic qualities are likely to be even more so. Subjects are likely to get
distracted and sensitive to mind-wandering if they are forced to read a succession of
disjointed sentences in a darkened, quiet room for nearly an hour. It is unusual for
psycholinguistic studies to include comprehension items that provide individuals with
feedback on whether their answers are correct or incorrect. We would suggest that our
theories of language processing should be concerned with the interpretation of materials in
which our subjects are engaged, rather than stimuli that they are only skimming. The use of
more lifelike materials in psycholinguistic study is encouraged as a good result of stressing
engagement in comprehension.

Interconnections among task, comprehension level, and online measures

Our goal should be to create a theory of language processing that connects the three
principles we've discussed. Researchers are starting to look at this process, which is thought
to go from engagement to understanding depth to consequences on dependent measures,
among other factors. Previous study has arguably addressed these concepts in pairs. When
deciding how to test comprehension, for example, we could construct research more
intelligently, led by data and theoretically grounded hypotheses.

Disclosure statement

The authors reported no potential conflicts of interest.

Strengthness of Journal 2

 The title used is in accordance with the overall content of the study
 Abstract and introduction are good enough
 Use easy-to-understand language
 The identity of this journal is written completely and clearly and the reference source
is written down
 In this journal, several expert opinions are given in order to increase the reader's
understanding or understanding.

Weaknesses of Journal 2

 The writing space is irregular so it makes it difficult for readers to read it and does not
provide a complete description of the formula written in the journal.
 This journal has no conclusions

You might also like