You are on page 1of 74

This article was downloaded by: 10.3.98.

104
On: 15 Mar 2022
Access details: subscription number
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG, UK

Handbook of Parenting
Volume 3: Being and Becoming a Parent
Marc H. Bornstein

Fathers and Families

Publication details
https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9780429433214-3
Ross D. Parke, Jeffrey T. Cookston
Published online on: 20 Feb 2019

How to cite :- Ross D. Parke, Jeffrey T. Cookston. 20 Feb 2019, Fathers and Families from:
Handbook of Parenting, Volume 3: Being and Becoming a Parent Routledge
Accessed on: 15 Mar 2022
https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9780429433214-3

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR DOCUMENT

Full terms and conditions of use: https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/legal-notices/terms

This Document PDF may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproductions,
re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or
accurate or up to date. The publisher shall not be liable for an loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
3
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

FATHERS AND FAMILIES


Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Introduction
Theoretical assumptions that guide research on fathers and families both explain the choice of topics
and provide an organizational structure for this chapter. We draw attention to 11 themes of impor-
tance. First, to understand fully the nature of father-child relationships, it is necessary to recognize
the interdependence among the roles and functions of all family members. Families are best viewed
as social systems. Consequently, to understand the behavior of one member of a family, the comple-
mentary behaviors of other members also need to be recognized and assessed. For example, as men’s
roles in families shift, changes in women’s roles in families are also in flux (Parke, 1996, 2013).
Second, family members—mothers, fathers, and children—influence each other both directly
and indirectly (Cox and Paley, 2003; Parke, Power, and Gottman, 1979). Examples of fathers’ indirect
impact include various ways in which fathers modify and mediate mother-child relationships. In
turn, women affect their children indirectly through their husbands by modifying both the quantity
and the quality of father-child interaction. Children may indirectly influence the husband-wife rela-
tionship by altering the behavior of either parent that consequently changes the interaction between
spouses.
Third, different levels of analysis are necessary to understand fathers. The individual level—child,
mother, and father—remains a useful and necessary level of analysis, but recognition of relationships
among family members as levels or units of analysis is also necessary. The marital, the mother-child,
and the father-child relationships require separate analyses. The family as a unit that is independent
of the individual or dyads within the family requires recognition (McHale and Lindahl, 2011).
Fourth, families are embedded within a variety of other social systems, including both formal
and informal support systems as well as the cultures in which they exist. These include a wide range
of extrafamilial influences such as extended families, informal community ties such as friends and
neighbors, work sites, and social, educational, and medical institutions (Leventhal, Dupéré, and Shuey,
2015; Parke, 2013).
Fifth, the role of biology in shaping father roles has become more widely recognized. Fathers and
families are embedded in a network of biological and neurological systems, and we will examine the
biological basis of fathering. This perspective suggests that fathers (as well as mothers) are biologi-
cally prepared for the parenting role due to hormonal changes as well as neurological predispositions
to respond to infants as part of their preparation for the caregiving role (Feldman, Gordon, Influs,
Gutbir, and Ebstein, 2013; Swain et al., 2014).

64
Fathers and Families

A sixth assumption concerns the importance of considering father-child relationships from a vari-
ety of developmental perspectives. Developmental changes in child perceptual-cognitive and soci-
oemotional capacities represent the most commonly investigated types of development. In addition,
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

a life-span perspective (Elder, Shanahan, and Jennings, 2015; Parke and Elder, in press) suggests the
importance of examining developmental changes in the adult because parents continue to change
and develop during adult years. For example, age at the time of the onset of parenthood can have
important implications for how females and males manage their maternal and paternal roles. Adult
age involves an exploration of the tasks faced by adults such as self-identity, education, and career and
examination of relations between these tasks and the demands of parenting.
A seventh assumption involves recognition of the impact of secular shifts on families. In recent
years, a variety of social changes in the United States has had a profound impact on families. These
include the decline in fertility and family size, changes in the timing of the onset of parenthood,
increased participation of women in the workforce, an initial rise in rates of divorce that has become
more stable, and subsequent increase in the number of single-parent families (Golombok, 2015;
Parke, 2013). The ways in which these society-wide changes impact on interaction patterns between
parents and children merit examination.
Another closely related eighth assumption involves recognition of the importance of the his-
torical time period in which the family interaction is taking place (Parke and Elder, in press). His-
torical time periods provide the social conditions for individual and family transitions: Examples
include the 1930s (the Great Depression), the 1960s (the Vietnam War Era), or the 1980s (Farm
Belt Depression). Across these historical time periods, family interactions may be quite different due
to the peculiar conditions of the particular era (Elder et al., 2015). These distinctions among differ-
ent developmental trajectories, as well as social change and historical period effects, are important
because these different forms of change do not always harmonize (Elder et al., 2015; Parke, 1988).
For example, a family event such as the birth of a child—the transition to parenthood—may have
very profound effects on a man who has just begun a career in contrast to the effects on one who has
advanced to a stable occupational position. Moreover, individual and family developmental trajecto-
ries are embedded within both the social conditions and the values of the historical time in which
they exist (Elder et al., 2015). The role of parents, as is the case with any social role, is responsive to
such fluctuations (Cabrera, Fitzgerald, Bradley, and Roggman, 2014).
A ninth assumption addresses the major challenge concerning the universality of theories about
fathers and families and the error of assuming that generalizations from a single culture (e.g., United
States) to other cultures are appropriate (Rogoff, 2003). Cultures vary enormously in how they
organize their families, how they allocate family roles to different individuals, what outcomes they
value in their children, and even what childrearing tactics they choose to achieve their socialization
goals. Although there are many similarities across and within cultural groups around the globe, we
need to recognize the variations and how to learn from these variations. Closely related to the issue
of cross-cultural variation in fathering is recognition that intracultural variations in ethnicity need to
be considered as well. It is important not only to examine the diversity of familial organization, roles,
goals, and strategies across ethnic groups but also it is equally critical to explore variations within
different ethnic groups (Parke, 2013). Recognizing the ethnic diversity within our own culture is
necessary both for understanding variations in fatherhood and for guiding social policy.
A tenth assumption concerns the role of cognitive factors in understanding father-child relation-
ships. Specifically, we assume that the ways in which parents perceive, organize, and understand both
their children and their roles as parents will affect the nature of father-child interaction (Cookston
et al., 2012; Goodnow and Collins, 1990).
The eleventh and final assumption observes that research on fathering benefits from an inter-
sectional framework that operationalizes both identity and social position simultaneously (Shields,
2008). Because fathers have authority over their children, they are imbued with power in the lives

65
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

of others that nonfathers lack. Similarly, when men enact the role of father, they assume a gendered
identity that differs from mothering in the types of experiences a man might provide his child, his
expectations for the child’s behavior when they are together, and his expectations for how the child
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

should act when alone with the father versus when in public. Themes of identity and social position
abound when fathers are the focus of study, such as the father’s sex assignment at birth, ethnicity,
socioeconomic opportunities and disadvantage in childhood, and availability of male role models
during childhood, among others.
To understand the nature of father-child relationships within families, a multilevel and dynamic
approach is required. Multiple levels of analysis are necessary to capture the individual, dyadic, and
family unit aspects of operation within the family itself as well as to reflect the embeddedness of
families within a variety of extrafamilial social systems. The dynamic quality reflects the multiple
developmental trajectories that warrant consideration in understanding the nature of families in
children’s development.
Figure 3.1 serves as a theoretical guide for the chapter and aims to capture the transactional nature
of the linkages among predictors and father and child outcomes.
The substantive portion of this chapter begins with a discussion of the nature of father-child rela-
tionships and how these shift across development of the child. Next, the chapter moves to an exami-
nation of the determinants of father involvement to examine the impact of the marital relationship
on the parent-child relationship. The effect of historical changes, namely, shifts in work patterns of
family members and changes in the timing of the onset of parenthood on father-child relationships,
will be reviewed. Finally, the implications of fathering for men themselves, their wives, and their
children are examined.
This chapter is a review of recent work on fatherhood and devotes less attention to the historical
aspects of the topic. However, several reviews caution against any simple and linear set of histori-
cal trends that lead clearly from the past to the present (LaRossa, 1997; 2012; Parke and Stearns,
1993; Pleck, 2004; Rotundo, 1993). Perhaps most striking is the continued tension and variability in
fathering behavior—a set of characteristics that have long marked definitions of fatherhood. There
have always been counteracting forces that have both promoted and limited father involvement with
their children and families. There have been “good dads and bad dads” (Furstenberg, 1988) through-
out the course of the history of fatherhoods, and this theme of variability in the amount, type, and
consistency of fathering across time and context continues (Furstenberg, 2014). It is encouraging to
note that there has been an increase in research attention to fathers from diverse family structures as
reflected in journal articles focused on fathers (Goldberg, Tan, and Thorsen, 2009). Stearns (1991,
p. 50) characterized the shifts over the last century as follows:

The most pressing context for fatherhood over the past century has been the change in
work—family relationship. . . . An 18th Century father would not recognize the distance
contemporary men face between work and home or the importance of sports in father-
child relationships or the parental leadership granted to mothers or indeed the number
of bad fathers. An 18th Century father would, however, recognize certain contemporary
tensions such as a balance between seeking and giving love on the one hand and defining
proper authority and he might feel kinship to present-day fathers who sense some tension
between responses they regard as male and special restraints required for proper family life.

In summary, many of the themes that characterize contemporary thinking about fatherhood have
clearer antecedents over the last century than we often assume. There has been a tendency to confuse
the resurgence of interest in fathering as a research topic with the assumption that the changes in
fathering activities have been only recent as well.

66
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Biological
Paternal hormones Men
Temperament Self-identy
Neurological influences Psychological adjustment
Emoonal processes Generavity
Encoding and decoding
Physical health
Individual Emoonal regulaon
Relaonship with partner
Relaonship with family of origin Display rule skills
Work pa‡erns
Preparaon for parenng
Paternal mental health
Child (e.g., temperament, age, sex) A‡enon regulaon
Ability to a‡end to relevant cues,
Coparental sustain a‡enon, and refocus

Father
Couple relaonship a‡enon

involvement
Maternal gatekeeping
Coresidenal status
Timing of parenthood Cognive processes
Cognive working models Children
Representaons of relaonships Emoonal development
Contextual Perceived ma‡ering Social development
Socioeconomic status Language development
Employment opportunies Cognive development
Ethnicity
Governmental policies
Other social influences

Societal condions changing over me


Figure 3.1 Theoretical overview of the predictors and consequences of father involvement
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Paternal Versus Maternal Involvement With Children


There are overall differences in the quantity of involvement for mothers and fathers, and there are
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

important stylistic or qualitative differences as well. Not all forms of father involvement are concep-
tually equivalent (Lamb, Pleck, and Levine, 1985; Palkovitz, 1997; Parke, 2000). The most influential
scheme was offered by Lamb and his colleagues (Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, and Levine, 1987; Lamb
et al., 1985), who suggested three components: Interaction, availability, and responsibility.
Interaction refers to the father’s direct contact with his child through caregiving and shared activi-
ties. Availability is a related concept concerning the father’s potential availability for interaction, by
virtue of being present or accessible to the child whether direct interaction is occurring. Responsi-
bility refers to the role the father takes in ascertaining that the child is taken care of and arranging
for resources to be available for the child (Lamb et al., 1987, p. 125).
As several previous authors (e.g., Palkovitz, 1997; Parke, 2000; Pleck, 2010) found, the focus of
research on fathers has been primarily on face-to-face parent-child interaction. To a large degree,
this emphasis reflects the common assumption that parental influence takes place directly through
face-to-face contact or indirectly through the impact of the interaction on another family member.
Pleck (2010) modified this conceptual scheme to further distinguish specific qualitative aspects of
interaction, namely, warmth, responsiveness, and control. This has encouraged researchers to meas-
ure not only time with the child but also the quality of the interactions as well. In turn, the goal
was to increase the strength of the links between aspects of father engagement and child outcomes.
However, it is still useful for cross-time comparisons to continue to track changes in the relative
level of father and mother overall involvement as part of the goal of monitoring shifting family roles
by mothers and fathers. If the goal is to assess the effects of parental behavior on child outcomes,
qualitative measures of the type of involvement are generally better than simply the amount of time
in parental activities (Maccoby and Martin, 1983). Other updates on the paternal involvement meas-
ure have been suggested. Absolute and relative involvement need to be distinguished because these
two indices are independent and may affect both children’s and adults’ views of role distributions
in different ways (Pleck, 2010; Pleck and Masciadrelli, 2004). It is important to distinguish among
domains of involvement, because fathers and mothers vary in their distribution of time across dif-
ferent child and household activities (Doucet, 2013). Several distinctions have been made including
personal care activities, involvement in play, leisure, and affiliative activities with children (Beitel
and Parke, 1998; Palkovitz, 2002; Pleck, 2010). Others (Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, and Hofferth,
2001) have expanded the domain list to include not just personal care and play but also achievement-
related activities (homework, reading), household activities (housework, shopping), social activities
(conversation, social events), and other activities (time in school, sleep). As noted ahead, both the
amount of time that fathers spend on these different activities and the determinants of involvement
in these domains vary across fathers. Finally, estimates of father involvement have usefully distin-
guished between weekdays and weekends because both the types of activities and levels of father
involvement vary as a function of the time period being assessed (Yeung et al., 2001).
Although the availability issue has been addressed largely through the research on father absence as
a consequence of either divorce or unwed parenthood (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010; ­McLanahan
and Sandefer, 1994; Stevenson et al., 2014), studies of the quantity and quality of fathers’ nonfamily
work have begun to shed light on variations in father availability (O’Brien and Moss, 2010; Williams,
2010). Finally, researchers and theorists have recognized the “managerial” function of parents (Lamb
et al.’s “responsibility” notion) and have examined the impact of variations in how this managerial
function influences child development (Furstenberg, Cook, Eccles, Elder, and Sameroff, 1999; Parke
et al., 2003). By managerial, we refer to the ways in which parents organize and arrange the child’s
home environment and set limits on the range of the home setting to which the child has access and
the opportunities for social contact with playmates and socializing agents outside the family. The

68
Fathers and Families

managerial role may be just as important as the parent’s role as stimulator, because the amount of
time that children spend interacting with the inanimate environment far exceeds their social interac-
tion time (White, Kaban, Shapiro, and Attonucci, 1976). Nor are fathers always mere helpers; in over
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

20% of married employed mother families, fathers served as the primary care provider during the
hours that the mother was working (U.S. Census Bureau, 2008). So fathers are active contributors to
childcare not only in U.S. culture but in other places as well. Mothers still do more than fathers of
the child caregiving, such as feeding and diapering in infancy and in providing meals, school lunches,
and clothing, as the child develops (Gray and Anderson, 2010; Pleck, 2010). And mothers continue
to assume more managerial responsibility than fathers, such as arranging social contacts, organizing
schedules, taking the child for medical checkups, and monitoring homework and school-related
tasks. However, these patterns change as a function of the time of the week and the age of the child.
For example, fathers are more involved in household activities (shopping) and social activities on
weekends than weekdays, and play as the focus of fathering decreases as the child develops (Yeung
et al., 2001).
Mothers and fathers differ in their degree of responsibility for management of family tasks. From
infancy through middle childhood, mothers are more likely to assume the managerial role than
fathers. In infancy, this means setting boundaries for play, taking the child to the doctor, or arranging
day care (Doucet, 2013; Power and Parke, 1982). In middle childhood, mothers continue to assume
more managerial responsibility (e.g., directing the child to have a bath, to eat a meal, to put away toys)
(Russell and Russell, 1987). Nor is the managerial role restricted to family activities but includes initi-
ating and arranging children’s access to peers and playmates (Ladd, 2005; McDowell and Parke, 2009),
and maintaining contact with teachers and schools (Kim and Hill, 2015), tasks which fathers are less
likely to undertake than mothers. In addition, parents function as supervisors or overseers of especially
younger children’s interactions with age mates. Both mothers and fathers are equally capable of this
type of supervisory behavior as shown in laboratory studies (Bhavnagri and Parke, 1991) and in home
contexts, but fathers are less likely than mothers to perform this supervisory role in everyday settings
(Bhavnagri and Parke, 1991; Ladd, Profilet, and Hart, 1992). As Doucet (2013) observed, this dispar-
ity between men and women’s managerial/responsibility roles has persisted: “The responsibility for
childcare and domestic life has remained overwhelmingly in women’s hands” (pp. 302–303).
The extent to which fathers in intact families participate in childcare needs to be distinguished
from the level of involvement of fathers who are not coresident with their children for a variety of
reasons including divorce, out-of-wedlock births, economic hardship, or incarceration. In fact, this
conceptual distinction reflects the contradictory trends in the fathering literature that Furstenberg
(2014) characterized as the “two-tier family system.” On the one hand, well-educated and economi-
cally viable fathers seem to be increasing their involvement and moving slowly toward more equal
participation with their wives in the care and rearing of children. On the other hand, due to lack
of education and limited employment opportunities, many men forgo marriage and, in many cases,
coresidency due to lack of education and limited employment opportunities and develop different pat-
terns of involvement with their children than fathers in intact families. Similarly, other groups of fathers
who are separated from their children due to military deployment, incarceration, or immigration are
receiving attention. By recognizing these various groups of fathers beyond traditional intact, residential
family-based fathers, the variability in contemporary fathering becomes evident and reminds us that
change across time in fathering is not linear and straightforward but is contradictory and inconsistent
and open to influence by changing societal conditions.

Father Involvement in Intact, Majority Families in the United States


Most attention in the research literature has been devoted to the study of fathers in two-parent intact
families. According to a survey of over 1,100 journal articles from 1930 to 2006, 76% of the fathers

69
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

were from two-parent families and the rest were focused on divorced, single or nonresident father
families (Goldberg, Tan, and Thorsen, 2009). In spite of shifts in cultural attitudes concerning the
appropriateness and desirability of shared roles and equal levels of participation in routine caregiving
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and interaction for mothers and fathers, the shifts toward parity are small, but nonetheless real (Pleck,
2010). More mothers are entering the workforce, but current occupational arrangements still mean
that the vast majority of fathers have less opportunity for interaction with their children than mothers.
Bianchi, Robinson, and Milkie (2006) using a national survey of parents, found that in the year 2000,
resident American fathers with children under 18 spent an average of 2.4 hours per week in interactive
activities with their children in comparison to 3.3 hours for mothers. Fathers spent 72% of the time as
mothers. Across time there is a marked increase. In comparison with 1965, the interactive engagement
time in 2000 increased 94% while mothers’ time increased even more from 1.5 hours to 3.3 hours.
However, the gap is closing, as data from 2003 to 2004 indicate that fathers’ engagement had increased
to 3.0 hours (Wang and Bianchi, 2009). To place these trends in perspective, fathers also increased their
time in routine care as well as direct interactive activities. Fathers spent 4.1 hours per week in routine
childcare activities and were present and accessible to their children another 26.5 hours per week. This
was an impressive 33 hours per week in total time with their children.
Compared with mothers, fathers’ total time in 2000 was 64.7% of time as mothers compared with
44.7% in 1965 (Bianchi et al., 2006). Others report similar trends. Yeung et al. (2001) found that the rel-
ative time fathers in intact families were directly engaged with children was 67% of the time that moth-
ers were involved on weekdays and 87% of mothers’ engagement on weekends. Accessibility showed
similar shifts across time and in accord with earlier estimates were higher than levels of engagement.

Involvement Among Ethnic Minority Fathers in the United States


The historical view of the African American father as absent or uninvolved in the lives of their
children is no longer valid, and recent work has corrected the scholarly neglect of this large ethnic
minority group (12% of U.S. population). Comparisons across ethnic groups (African and European
American) revealed either few differences in level of father involvement (Yeung et al., 2001) or in
some studies African American fathers are higher in their levels of caregiving and play than Euro-
pean American fathers (Cabrera, Hoffreth, and Schae, 2011; Hofferth, 2003). The greater level of
caregiving involvement is consistent with the less traditional attitudes of African American fathers
toward gender roles than European American fathers (Gadsden, 1999). However, studies of African
American families confirm the pattern found for European Americans, namely, that fathers were
less involved in caregiving their infants than mothers (Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013). In a study
of Head Start fathers, African American fathers of sons reported more involvement in physical play
and social activities (taking children out with friends and visiting relatives) than European American
fathers (Leavell et al., 2012). In terms of responsibility, African American fathers are higher in moni-
toring than European American fathers, in part possibly due to their residence in more dangerous
neighborhoods (Deater-Deckard and Dodge, 1997). As we note ahead, even nonresidential African
American fathers are more involved with their children than is commonly assumed.
Another ethnic group that has received increased attention is Latin American fathers. Today,
Latino Americans constitute 18% of the U.S. population, and it is projected that by 2065, Latin
American families will be the most prominent minority in the U.S. culture at 24% (Pew Charitable
Trust, 2015). Historically, it was assumed that Latino fathers are “traditional patriarchs, authoritar-
ian, and emotionally uninvolved, and not very engaged in the daily care of their children” (Cabrera,
Aldoney, and Tamis-Lemonda, 2013, p. 248). Recent research suggests that this portrait is outdated
and misleading. In contrast to this characterization, Latino fathers typically rear their children in
two-parent families where fathers are often involved in their children’s lives from an early age, even
beginning in the prenatal period (Cabrera et al., 2009). The importance of focusing on Latino fathers

70
Fathers and Families

stems from Latin American fathers having been found to be more involved in household and per-
sonal care activities than European American fathers (Yeung et al., 2001) and spending more time
in caregiving activities than fathers of other ethnic groups (Cabrera et al., 2011; Hofferth, 2003).
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Finally, Latino fathers show similar levels of affection and warmth to their children as fathers of
other ethnicities (Hofferth, 2003; Toth and Xu, 1999). Studies of responsibility among Latino fathers
have focused on monitoring and restrictiveness of their children’s behavior. Some reports increased
restriction of their children’s behavior (Toth and Xu, 1999), especially for daughters (Wilson, Dal-
berth, and Koo, 2010). At the same time, as in the case of other groups, fathers spend less time than
mothers in routine caregiving and more time in play (Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013). These find-
ings are consistent with Latino values of familism, which stress the centrality of family life in Latino
culture. Acculturation is important as well. More acculturated Latino fathers are more involved with
their children than less acculturated men (Glass and Owen, 2010)
Asian Americans are another rapidly growing ethnic minority. Although Asian Americans cur-
rently constitute 6% of the U.S. population, this will increase to 14% by 2065 (Pew Charitable Trust,
2015). Asian American fathers in comparison with mothers have typically been viewed as aloof and
uninvolved with their children (Ishii-Kuntz, 2009). Again, this stereotype has some historical basis,
but the portrait of the modern Asian father is changing due to both patterns of immigration and
acculturation as well as shifting economic conditions in some Asian countries (Chao and Tseng,
2002; Ishii-Kuntz, 2009; Li and Lamb, 2013). In both Asian American and Asian families, there are
some general findings that characterize maternal and paternal roles in the family. The strong com-
mitment to the breadwinner role among Asian fathers has resulted in men’s limited involvement
with the care of their children (Ishii-Kuntz, 2009; Li and Lamb, 2013) compared with mothers as
well as to men in other cultures. For example, Japanese fathers are less involved with their children
compared with mothers and less than fathers in other countries (Ishii-Kuntz, 2009). Similar lower
levels of father involvement in childcare relative to mothers have been reported for Chinese families
(Chuang and Zhu, 2018; Jankowiak, 2010; Lau, 2016) as well as Taiwanese families (Sun and Roo-
pnarine, 1996). In China, fathers were not viewed as sufficiently competent or trustworthy to care for
infants. Instead, in Chinese families, a father’s role was focused on children’s education when children
entered middle school (Li and Lamb, 2013). However, the centrality of the men’s breadwinning role
among Asian fathers is changing (Li and Lamb, 2013; Nakazawa and Shwalb, 2013). In one study,
81% of Japanese fathers ranked the paternal role as first or second in importance among five paternal
roles—evidence that the importance of the worker role is lessening (Shwalb et al., 2010). An increase
in the labor force participation of women in both China and Japan has contributed to this decline. In
Japan, fathers are more likely to be involved in childcare when their wives were employed full time
(Ishii-Kuntz, 2009). In China, a similar pattern has emerged with a shift away from the focus only on
the provider role for father to a more multifaceted set of roles including a more active role in child-
care (Chuang and Su, 2008; Chuang and Zhu, 2018), although some evidence suggests that paternal
involvement is more evident among preschoolers and older children than with infants ( Jankowiak,
2010). There are also rural/urban and regional differences. Urban Chinese fathers are less oriented
to traditional gender roles, more likely to be more involved with their children, and tend to have
closer relationships with their children than their rural counterparts, in part due to higher employ-
ment rates among urban mothers as well as more exposure to contemporary cultural messages con-
cerning parental roles (Song, 2004). In terms of regional variations, fathers in Asian Indian families
are less likely to participate in household and childcare work even when their wives are employed,
which suggests that patriarchal values may persist in some Asian regions more than others (Suppal
and Roopnarine, 1999). As in the case in other cultures, Asian fathers are more likely to be play-
mates or as children enter school to act as an educational guide than as caregivers, a reminder that
involvement still often follows gender-based scripts (Chuang and Su, 2008; Song, 2004). Similarly, in
the United States and Canada as Asian families become more acculturated, there is a trend toward

71
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

greater equality in the division of household labor including more equity in childcare responsibili-
ties among Japanese Americans (Ishii-Kuntz, 2009), Chinese Canadian (Costigan and Su, 2008), and
Asian Indian immigrant fathers in the United States ( Jain and Belsky, 1997). However, the patterns
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

are not uniform across Asian subgroups that vary in terms of immigration patterns and levels of
economic participation. It is clear from these brief examples that cultural factors will either enhance
or diminish differences between mothers and fathers and underscore the plasticity of parental roles.
These findings are important in light of past negative characterizations of low-income African
American and Latin American fathers as uninvolved. Clearly, the stereotypes surrounding fathers of
different ethnic backgrounds are inaccurate and outdated and needs to be revised in light of recent
evidence (Cabrera et al. 2013; Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013). Much of the earlier work concern-
ing fathering among ethnic minority fathers was based on single-parent families and/or on young
unwed fathers and failed to recognize differences within ethnic groups.

Father Involvement in International Perspective


An examination of non-U.S. countries reveals a pattern similar to the one observed in the United
States. From 1965 to 2000, fathers in Canada, Australia, the Netherlands, France, and the United
Kingdom showed a similar increase over time in the amount of “child care time” (Bianchi et al.
2006). However, with the exception of France, “childcare time” for U.S. fathers is lower than fathers
in many industrial countries, with British fathers being highest (Bianchi et al., 2006). The most com-
prehensive cross-national examination of this issue comes from the Multinational Time Use Study
in which time use diary surveys across 16 countries were utilized to assess cross-time shifts from the
1960s to the 2000s in men’s and women’s household roles (Kan, Sullivan, and Gershuny, 2011). As
expected, women continue to devote more time to routine housework (e.g., cooking, cleaning, other
care) than men who, in turn, have increased their contribution to nonroutine household tasks (e.g.,
shopping, gardening, house repairs). Over time, the gender gap is closing, in part, due to a reduction
in routine household activities by mothers and an increase in men’s contributions to these tasks.
Small-scale studies in several countries including Sweden, Israel, and Japan confirm this general
pattern (see Shwalb, Shwalb, and Lamb, 2013). These findings are consistent with the more general
proposition that pregnancy and birth of a first child, in particular, are occasions for a shift toward a
more traditional division of roles regardless of whether their initial role division between husbands
and wives was traditional or equalitarian (Cowan and Cowan, 2010).

Developmental changes in father involvement


The overall pattern of contact time between mothers and fathers and their infants and toddlers contin-
ues into middle childhood and adolescence. In a study of middle childhood (6- to 7-year-olds), Russell
and Russell (1987) found that Australian mothers were available to children 54.7 hours/week compared
with 34.6 hours/week for fathers. Mothers also spent more time alone with children (22.6 hours/week)
than did fathers (2.4 hours/week). However, when both parents and child were together, mothers and
fathers initiated interactions with children with equal frequency and children’s initiations toward each
parent were similar. Adolescents spend less time with their parents than younger children and less time
alone with their father than with their mother (Larson and Richards, 1994).
Part of the explanation for the greater involvement of mothers versus fathers is because the
maternal parenting role is more mandatory and more clearly scripted by our culture, whereas pater-
nal parenting is less clearly scripted and more discretionary. The gendered nature of parenting roles
is illustrated by custody decisions which, in spite of shifts toward more equal custody arrangements
across mothers and fathers, continue to favor mothers. In a study with a national probability sample,
Kelly, Redenbach, and Rinaman (2005) found that in 80% of the cases, the mother received sole

72
Fathers and Families

physical custody of the minor child or children. However, the number of fathers with sole custody
has increased over the last several decades as a result of the growing national fatherhood movement,
the growing recognition of fathers as significant parental figures and perhaps father-friendlier courts
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

(Stevenson et al., 2014).


A caveat is in order. The lower level of father involvement in caregiving and other forms of inter-
action does not imply that fathers are less competent than mothers to care for infants and children.
Competence can be measured in a variety of ways; one approach is to measure the parent’s sensitivity
to infant cues in the feeding context. Success in caregiving, to a large degree, depends on the parent’s
ability to correctly “read” or interpret the infant’s behavior so that the parent’s own behavior can be
regulated to respond appropriately. Decades of research has documented that maternal and paternal
behavior that is contingent on, sensitive to, and responsive to a child’s signals and behavior is critical
for adequate development (Tamis-LeMonda and Baumwell, 2001; Tamis-LeMonda, Baumwell, and
Cabrera, 2013). Beginning in infancy, fathers’ sensitivity to a variety of cues (e.g., auditory distress
signals during feeding [sneeze, spit up, cough], vocalizations, mouth movements) is just as marked as
mothers’ responsivity to these cues (Parke and Sawin, 1976, 1980) in spite of the fact that fathers may
spend less time in caregiving activities. Moreover, the amount of milk consumed by infants when
either their mothers and fathers were the feeding agent is similar; fathers and mothers are equally
successful in feeding their infant. Other evidence is similar: Mothers and fathers show similar levels
of sensitivity to their 24- and 30-month-old children’s behavior during play (Tamis-LeMonda et al.,
2004) and are responsive to their 12-month-olds when engaged in an interactive task (Notaro and
Volling, 1999). Invoking a competence/performance distinction, fathers may not necessarily act
as a caregiver or interactive partner as often as mothers, but when called on, they are sensitive and
competent interactive agents. Fathers’ ability to perform caregiving tasks matches mothers’ skill in
middle childhood as well. As Russell and Russell (1987) found, both parents reported that they were
involved on a regular basis in a variety of caregiving activities (e.g., having a cuddle, sit and have a
talk) even though mothers were higher in their frequencies. Finally, fathers can function effectively as
managers and supervisors of their children’s activities, but do so less than mothers on a routine basis
(Ladd, 2005; Parke et al., 2003). Again, it appears that fathers are capable of this type of caregiving
function but execute this function less regularly than mothers. On balance, the evidence suggests that
fathers are competent caregiving agents.

Qualitative Effects: Stylistic Similarities and Differences


in Mother and Father Interaction
Although the levels of involvement may differ for mothers and fathers, their childrearing styles do
not differ greatly. Both mothers and fathers express warmth and responsiveness in their interactions
with their children, and in general, the notion of the aloof and unresponsive father is an outdated
view. For example, among residential fathers with preteens, 73% reported that they showed affec-
tion (87% for mothers),62% expressed love (85% for mothers), and 37% offered appreciation to
their children (55% for mothers; Child Trends, 2006). Similarly, evidence from other ethnic groups
reveals a similar picture. African American fathers showed moderate levels of responsiveness and low
levels of negative behavior directed toward infants (Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, and Cabrera, 2006)
and toddlers (Mitchell and Cabrera, 2009). In the case of Asian fathers, Chao and Tseng (2002)
observed, “Parenting differences purported between Asian fathers and mothers have been based on
the traditional adage ‘strict father, kind mother’—wherein fathers exert high degrees of authoritar-
ian control and mothers manifest high degrees of warmth” (p. 73). Although there is some support
for this view among Chinese families, other studies of Asian American families fail to support this
profile. Consistent with a view endorsing a more involved mother, in a study of Chinese adolescents’
perceptions, maternal parenting was viewed as being more positive than paternal parenting (Shek,

73
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

2000). Adolescents stated that fathers showed less concern, less responsiveness, and higher levels of
harshness than mothers. Also, father-adolescent communication was perceived less positively and as
occurring less frequently than mother-adolescent communication (Shek, 2000). Others (Xu and
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Zhang, 2008) report similar findings of low levels of communication and emotional support for
Chinese fathers. In contrast to this traditional picture of Asian families, studies of Asian American
immigrant families have found few differences between mothers and fathers in levels of authorita-
tive parenting control and warmth (Chao and Kim, 2000). Again, acculturation clearly plays a role in
helping to understand differences and similarities in mother-father childrearing styles. These patterns
present a clear challenge to our stereotype of the maternal and paternal childrearing styles among
Asian American families. In contrast to traditional portraits of Latino fathers as patriarchal, authori-
tarian, and aloof (Mirande, 2008), Latin American fathers are characterized as warm and nurturing
(Gamble, Ramakumar, and Diaz, 2007; Leidy et al., 2011; Toth and Xu, 1999) especially as a result
of acculturation (Taylor and Behnke, 2005). Similarly, African American fathers showed moderate
levels of responsiveness and low levels of negative behavior directed toward infants (Shannon et al.,
2006) and toddlers (Mitchell and Cabrera, 2009). However, mothers and fathers may exhibit differ-
ences in level of control. For example, African American fathers set limits less often than mothers
but have more sole responsibility for disciplining their children than European American fathers
(Child Trends, 2006). Among Latin American families, fathers often exercise more control over
children than mothers, especially in the case of adolescent daughters. Finally, among Asian American
immigrants, few mother-father differences in strictness have been found (Chao and Kim, 2000) in
contrast to higher levels of paternal compared with maternal strictness among mainland Chinese
families (Berndt et al.,1993). Clearly, acculturation and exposure to new cultural values play a role
in parenting styles.
In spite of similarities in interactive stylistic features, the contexts that mothers and fathers choose
for interacting with their children tend to differ. Fathers participate less than mothers in caregiving
and in providing meals, school lunches, and clothing but spend a greater percentage of the time avail-
able for interaction in play activities. Among North American families, fathers regardless of ethnicity
(European American, Asian American, African American, and Latin American) spent a greater per-
centage of time with their infants in play than mothers (Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013). However, in
absolute terms, mothers spend more time than fathers in play with their children (Yeung et al., 2001).
The quality of play across mothers and fathers differs too. Among young infants, older infants, and
toddlers, fathers’ hallmark style of interaction is physical play with characteristic degrees of arousal,
excitement, and unpredictability in terms of the pace of the interaction. In contrast, mothers’ playful
interactive style is characterized by a more modulated and less arousing tempo. Moreover, mothers
play more conventional motor games or toy-mediated activities and are more verbal and didactic
(Hossain and Roopnarine, 1994; Parke, 1996). They engage in more pretend play and role play than
fathers as well as more teaching activities than fathers (by labeling colors and shapes) as they engage
their infant in play (Power and Parke, 1982). Fathers engage in more physical play with sons than
daughters (Power and Parke, 1982), whereas mothers facilitate pretend play of their daughters more
than of their sons (Bornstein, 2007). In turn, girls engage in more frequent and more sophisticated
pretend play than boys (Bornstein, 2007). Nor are these effects evident only in infancy. MacDonald
and Parke (1984) found that fathers engaged in more physical play with their 3- and 4-year-old chil-
dren than mothers, whereas mothers engaged in more object-mediated play than fathers. The fathers’
distinctive role as a physical play partner changes with age, however. Physical play is highest between
fathers and 2-year-olds, and between 2 and 10 years of age, there is a decreased likelihood that fathers
engage their children physically (MacDonald and Parke, 1986). In spite of the decline in physical play
across age, fathers are still more often physical play partners than mothers. In an Australian study of
parents and their 6- to 7-year-old children (Russell and Russell, 1987), fathers were more involved
in physical/outdoor play interactions and fixing things around the house and garden than mothers.

74
Fathers and Families

In contrast, mothers were more involved in caregiving, household tasks, reading, toy play, and arts
and crafts. In sum, fathers are tactile and physical, and mothers tend to be verbal, didactic, and toy-
mediated in their play. Clearly, infants and young children experience qualitatively different stimula-
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

tory patterns from mothers and fathers.


In adolescence, the quality of maternal and paternal involvement continues to differ. Just as in
earlier developmental periods, mothers and fathers may complement each other and provide models
that reflect the tasks of adolescence—connectedness and separateness. Across development, the focus
on physical play on the part of fathers declines and is replaced in adolescence by verbal playfulness in
the form of sarcasm, humor, and word play, even though this often increases emotional distance but
perhaps encourages independence and more equal and egalitarian exchanges as well (Shulman and
Klein, 1993): “Fathers, more than mothers conveyed the feeling that they can rely on their adoles-
cents, thus fathers might provide a ‘facilitating environment’ for adolescent attainment of differentia-
tion from the family and consolidation of independence” (Shulman and Klein, 1993, p. 53). Mothers
are more emotionally available to their adolescents, and mother-adolescent dyads spend more time
together than father-adolescent dyads (Larson and Richards, 1994; Larson and Sheeber, 2007). Moth-
ers continue to be more involved in arts, crafts, and reading, and maintain more open communica-
tion and emotional closeness with their offspring during adolescence. Although the style of fathers’
involvement as a play or recreational partner appears to have reasonable continuity from infancy
through adolescence, the meaning and function of this interaction style shift across development.
Why do mothers and fathers play differently? Both biological and environment factors probably
play a role. Experience with infants, the amount of time spent with infants, and the usual kinds of
responsibilities that a parent assumes are all factors that influence parents’ style of play. For example,
as a result of spending less time with infants and children than mothers, fathers may use their distinc-
tive arousing style as a way to increase their salience to compensate for their more limited interaction
time. Mothers who overall spend more time with their infants and young children than fathers focus
on less arousing types of activities such as symbolic pretend play (Bornstein, 2007). Biological factors
cannot be ignored in light of the fact that male monkeys show the same rough-and-tumble physi-
cal style of play as U.S. human fathers, and infant male monkeys tend to respond more positively
to bids for rough-and-tumble play than females (Parke and Suomi, 1981): “Perhaps [both monkey
and human] males may be more susceptible to being aroused into states of positive excitement and
unpredictability than females” (Maccoby, 1988, p. 761)—speculation that is consistent with gender
differences in risk-taking and sensation seeking. In addition, human males, whether boys or men,
tend to behave more boisterously and show more positive emotional expression and reactions than
females (Maccoby, 1998). The fact that girls and their mothers engage in more symbolic pretend
play than fathers and boys suggests that mothers and fathers may enact complementary roles in their
children’s lives. Together, these threads of the puzzle suggest that predisposing biological differences
between males and females may influence the play patterns of mothers and fathers. At the same time,
the cross-cultural data underscore the ways in which cultural and environmental contexts shape play
patterns of parents and remind us of the high degree of plasticity of human social behaviors.
How mutable are these stylistic differences? The common wisdom concerning these parent gen-
der differences has been questioned. Instead, a profile has emerged of overlapping play styles between
and within mothers and fathers as well as adoption of a range of play styles by both mothers and
fathers has emerged. This revision does not diminish the contribution of either parent to children’s
development but opens up new questions about the processes through which parents contribute.
Moreover, we may need to reevaluate the distinctiveness of maternal and paternal contributions
to developmental outcomes but still recognize that both parents can play important roles. Despite
evidence about the unique styles of mothers and fathers, questions remain. The gender-of-parent
differences, on average, are relatively small, and there is a good deal of overlap between mothers
and fathers in both the style of play as well as in the absolute amount of time devoted to playful

75
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

interactions (Pleck, 2010; Yeung et al., 2001). And fathers do not own the physical play franchise;
mothers have a mixed play repertoire, too, and can and do bouncing and tickling as well as reading
and conversing with their children. In the same vein, fathers, like mothers, play with toys, read books,
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and engage in pretend play in addition to their supposedly signature style of arousing and stimulating
physical play (Lamb and Lewis, 2010). Parents may do a variety of types of play not only in a single
play session but also across different days, weeks, or even times of day depending on their mood,
their energy level, the child’s momentary interest, and their child’s daily schedule. Both mothers and
fathers contribute to their children’s development in a myriad of playful ways (Roggman, Boyce,
Cook, Christiansen, and Jones, 2004; Tamis-LeMonda, 2004):

The stylistic differences in play between fathers and mothers became enshrined in our
views of mothers and fathers based on work conducted 20–30 years ago when traditional
conceptions of fathers’ role predominated, maternal employment was still relatively uncom-
mon and was viewed negatively, and fathers were much less involved in the day-to-day care
of their infants.
(Lamb and Lewis, 2010, p. 116)

As men in contemporary society have expanded their range of involvement to include more car-
egiving and managerial parenting activities, the predominance of play as the distinctive feature of
the father role has diminished in importance. Play has become merely one of a variety of ways that
fathers (and mothers) are involved with their children. Some leading father scholars have revised
their earlier views of the uniqueness of father play: “There is less and less justification for viewing the
identification of fatherhood with play and companionship as something with unique psychological
significance as was once thought” (Lamb and Lewis, 2010, p. 117).

Other Cultures, Other Interactive Styles, and Other Play Partners


Another challenge to the uniqueness of these play distinctions comes from scrutiny of other cultures,
which clearly demonstrates that both the quantity and quality of parental involvement vary across
cultures. This evidence has challenged the assumption of the universality of paternal physical play as
well as the widely shared view that physical play is the hallmark of fathers’ interactive style. In some
cultures that are similar to mainstream U.S. culture, such as England and Australia, there are compara-
ble differences between mothers and fathers in play. However, findings from several other cultures do
not find that physical play is a central feature of the father-infant relationship (Roopnarine and Hos-
sain, 2013; Shwalb et al., 2013). Neither in Sweden nor among Israeli Kibbutz families were fathers
more likely than mothers to play with their children or to engage in different types of physical play
(Hwang, 1987). Similarly, among Chinese Malaysian, Indian and Aka pygmy (Central Africa) par-
ents, parents, they rarely engage in physical play with their children (Hewlett and MacFarlan, 2010;
Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013). Instead, both parents display affection and engage in plenty of close
physical contact. Perhaps societies that value sharing and cooperation are less likely to encourage a
physical playful interactive style, whereas industrialized societies which are characterized by a high
degree of competition and value independence and assertiveness commonly support this interactive
style (Paquette, 2004). In fact, children in Western technologically advanced and highly individually
oriented societies have the highest levels of competition between partners in their play (Hughes,
1999). However, there is less competitive play among North American children reared in coopera-
tion-oriented communes (Plattner and Minturn, 1975) and perhaps less prevalence of physical play
between fathers and children, although the possible decrease in physical play has not been established.
Another challenge to the traditional view of mothers and fathers as play partners comes from
cross-cultural observations that parents are not always active play partners. Instead, in some cultures,

76
Fathers and Families

the play duties are assumed by other members of the community. In Italy, neither mothers nor fathers
but other women in the extended family or within the community are more likely to play physically
with infants (New and Benigini, 1987), and in Mexico, this physical play role often falls to siblings
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

(Zukow-Goldring, 2002). These findings suggest that the physical play role of the father is not
universal and that the play role may be assumed by other social agents in some cultures. Moreover,
these cross-national differences suggest that cultural context is one of the factors that may reduce
the ­differences—in this case, in terms of play style—between mother and father interaction patterns.
These observations argue for a reevaluation of the pathways through which fathers influence their
children and suggest that we reconsider the father’s physical play role as a major contributor to chil-
dren’s emotional regulation—at least in some cultures.
The findings from other cultures suggest that our focus on the gender of the parent may be too
narrow a conceptualization of the issue of necessary adult input for adequate child development.
Instead, it may be helpful to recast the issue by asking whether exposure to male and female parents
is the key, or whether it is exposure to the interactive style typically associated with either mothers
or fathers that matters. In an experimental examination of this issue, Ross and Taylor (1989) found
that boys prefer the physical play style, whether it is mothers or fathers who engage in it. Their
work suggests that boys may not necessarily prefer their fathers but rather their physical style of
play. Together, these studies suggest that gender of the agent of delivery of playful input may be less
important than the type of stimulation itself. Further evidence is consistent with this interchangeabil-
ity argument, which suggests that mothers and fathers can substitute for each other as we saw in the
case of caregiving. To illustrate gender of parent substitutability, consider a classic study of the effects
of having a secure or insecure attachment relationship with mother or father on an infant’s sociabil-
ity with a stranger, a friendly clown (Main and Weston, 1981). Infants who had secure attachment
ties to both mother and father were most responsive to the friendly stranger, whereas those with
insecure attachment relationships with both parents were the most wary and least responsive. Infants
who had a secure attachment with either their mother or their father and an insecure attachment to
the other parent exhibited a mid-level of social responsiveness. However, the gender of the parent
with whom the infant developed a secure attachment did not matter; the parents were substitutable
for one another. Studies of the effects of having supportive or unsupportive parents on toddlers’ and
5-year-olds’ cognitive development came to a similar conclusion (Martin, Ryan, and Brooks-Gunn,
2007; Ryan, Martin, and Brooks-Gunn, 2006). When both mothers and fathers were supportive, the
children’s cognitive development scores were highest, and when both parents were unsupportive, the
children scored lowest. The scores were between the two high and low extremes when one parent
was supportive and the other parent was unsupportive. However, the scores were similar regardless
of which parent was supportive or unsupportive. Clearly, it is better to have two supportive parents,
but if the child has only one supportive parent, sex of parent is irrelevant and mother and father are
equivalent.
Together, this evidence indicates that the style of parenting and the gender of the parent who
delivers it can be viewed as at least partially independent. These data help us address the uniqueness
of fathers’ and mothers’ roles in the family. Moreover, they help provide clarity on the important issue
of how essential fathers (Silverstein and Auerbach, 1999) and mothers (Parke, 2002, 2013) are for the
successful socialization of their children. Work on same-gender parents that shows that parenting
processes are more important than the family type (same gender versus heterosexual) for children’s
development is consistent with this view (Golombok, 2019; Miller, Kors, and Macfie, 2017; Patter-
son, 2016). Finally, the focus on parents themselves as the sole agents in the socialization matrix is
too narrow a framing as well, as we saw in the cross-national examples in Mexico and Italy. These
findings suggest that a variety of socialization agents beyond mothers and fathers, such as siblings,
extended family members, and nonkin figures in the community, play important roles in children’s
lives.

77
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Beyond the “New Involved Father” in Intact Married Families


It is increasingly clear that the previous descriptions of the albeit slow emergence of fathers who are
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

more actively involved in the care of their children, and shared the responsibilities of both caregiver
and breadwinner with their partners, do not apply to all fathers (Doucet, 2013; Edin and Nelson, 2013).
There is increasing recognition that there is a two-tier family system based to a large extent on social
class (Furstenberg, 2014). The cultural trends involving new and involved fathers apply most clearly to
economically and educationally advantaged families, but they apply less readily to less economically
well off and less educated fathers and families. Especially as economic inequality has increased, it is
important to recognize that economic disparities have yielded more variability across social class in the
patterns of fathering than is usually recognized. Many fathers are either in cohabitation with a partner
rather than married; others may be divorced or never in residence with their partner but still involved
in the lives of their children. Reviews of fathering after divorce and stepfathering are beyond the scope
of this chapter (see Ganong, Coleman, and Sanner, 2019; Weinraub and Bowler, 2019). Instead, several
recent trends in fathering will be examined in this section including the father’s role in multiple partner
fertility families. In a subsequent section, work on patterns of “fathering at a distance” in which fathers
do not reside with their children on a regular basis for a variety of reasons, such as military deployment,
incarceration, or work in another country, will be examined.

Disadvantaged Families and Patterns of Father-Child Contact


Much of the recent work on fathers and families who have experienced disadvantage is based on
large national samples of fathers and children, such as the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
(NLSY), the National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH), the National Survey of Families and
Households (NSFH), and the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (Carlson and McLana-
han, 2010). These studies and recent qualitative studies (Edin and Nelson, 2013) provide important
insights into fathers’ roles in the lives of children as well as the effects of these fathers on children’s
development. According to the Fragile Families study, unmarried fathers are generally younger than
married fathers at the time of the birth of their first child (27 versus 32 years), of minority back-
ground (African American or Latin American), and less educated and less likely to be employed
(Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). A variety of other features characterizes these families, such as early
onset of parenthood and a decision not to marry. However, many parents are either cohabitating or
in a partner relationship at the time that the baby is born (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). In spite
of stereotypes, these men often had a close relationship with their partners, with 82% being roman-
tically involved at the time of the birth, 50% were cohabiting, and another 32% were romantically
involved but not in residence. Clearly, the belief that most out-of-wedlock births are a product of
casual relationships is misleading (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). Across time, however, just as is the
case of early-timed marriages, these relationships are unstable. Only about two fifths of unmarried
couples are still romantically involved 5 years after the birth of the child, compared with 77% of
married couples (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010).
Moreover, disadvantaged men may be involved with multiple partners across time and, in turn,
have children with subsequent partners. Almost 20% of fathers between the ages of 15 and 44 years
have children with more than one partner (Guzzo, 2014; Guzzo and Furstenberg, 2007). Age at first
sexual experience, age at birth of the first child, and relationship status of partners are correlates of
multi-partner fertility (Guzzo, 2014). Men whose first children are born outside of marriage are
three times as likely to experience multi-partner fertility than are men who are married to the
mother of their first child at the time of birth (Carlson and Furstenberg, 2006).
What about involvement with their children? Several studies converge in finding that con-
tact between unmarried fathers and their children decreases over time and, in contrast to the

78
Fathers and Families

new contemporary fathers, have less contact with their offspring (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010).
However, contact is not absent, and there is a notable amount of variation in the amount of time
that unmarried fathers spend with their children as well as variations in their forms of support for
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

their child. Studies of unmarried fathers indicate a surprising amount of paternal involvement for
extended periods following the birth. Results from Fragile Families indicate that 87% of fathers
saw their child in the year after the birth and 63% saw the child more than once a month (Carlson
and McLanahan, 2010). There is considerable instability in levels of father involvement with their
children across time. In a sample of low-income African American, unwed fathers, nearly 40% either
increased or decreased their level of involvement between the child’s birth and 3 years (Coley and
Chase-Lansdale, 1999). Similarly in the Fragile Families study (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010), by
3 years, only 71% of fathers had seen their child in the prior 2-year period and less than half (47%)
had seen their child more than once in the past month. By 5 years, 63% of fathers had seen their child
since age 3, and 43% had seen their child more than once in the past month. These declines in father
participation continue across childhood and adolescence (Furstenberg and Harris, 1993).
A word of caution in needed because the pattern of instability over time in contact patterns of
nonresident fathers and their children may apply more to poor fathers than all nonresidential fathers.
To illustrate, using a nationally representative sample, Cheadle, Amato, and King (2010) found that
most nonresidential fathers have fairly stable patterns of contact over a 12-year period. A large group
(38%) maintained a high level of contact across the study, and another group (32%) were initially
uninvolved and continued to remain at a distance from their children across time. A third group (23%)
of men were initially involved with their offspring but became relatively uninvolved across time, as
a small group (7%) were low at the beginning but gradually increased their level of involvement.
There is great variety in forms of father involvement beyond financial or even emotional and
practical support for the mother. Fathers provide nurturance, play and leisure activities, safety, moral
guidance, and discipline as well as connections with the extended family and community (Carlson
and McLanahan, 2010; Palkovitz, 2002). In the Fragile Families study, formal child support was rare
but informal support in the form of money, toys or diapers for the baby, or transportation were
common around the birth of the baby (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010; Edin and Nelson, 2013).
A broadened definition of support has clearly corrected the myth of the “deadbeat dad” among poor
and unmarried fathers (Tamis-LeMonda and McFadden, 2010). And the stereotype that African
American fathers are uninvolved is not true, either. In the Fragile Families study, African American
men were more likely to maintain contact with their children than either European American or
Latin American men (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). Similarly, others report that fewer African
American fathers (12%) have no contact with their offspring than European American (30%) and
Latin American (37%) fathers.
Finally, to fully appreciate the variability in contact between poor, often nonresidential, fathers
and their offspring, another feature of fathering among poor unwed men needs attention, namely,
that fathers do not maintain similar levels of contact across all biological children. Middle-class men
are more likely to marry, divorce, and remarry, a pattern that has been termed the “Marriage go
round” (Cherlin, 2009), but men in poverty are less likely to marry but more likely than middle-class
men to have multiple children with different partners. According to Edin and Nelson (2013), these
men are on “the ‘Family-go-round’ where good fatherhood is accomplished by moving from one
child to another” (2013, p. 189) as romantic relationships end and they move on to a new relationship
and often a new bout of fathering. Fathering efforts are not evenly distributed across all offspring
because few men are able to support or even maintain ties with several children. Instead, a pattern
of selective fathering is more normative among poor unmarried men whereby a father selectively
and serially invests his fathering capital in one or a few offspring. Instead of supporting the child’s
mother, many women are left to their own resources as many poor men are unable to play the tra-
ditional breadwinner role due to high unemployment or sporadic and low paying employment or

79
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

intermittent incarceration. Instead of a contemporary package deal in which a middle-class father is


expected to be both a breadwinner and a nurturer, guide, and support for their child, poor men may
elect to play only the nurturing role. This selective involvement choice represents a reversal of roles
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

between the sexes because it is the mother who is the financial provider and the father the playmate
to their child. Although selective involvement allows men to perhaps father at least one child well,
it leaves not only the women who have borne these other children to bear the major responsibil-
ity for the financial burden of rearing a child but also these “nonselected” children without support
from their biological father. As we elaborate ahead, this arrangement may have detrimental effects on
children. Clearly, the patterns of contact between fathers and their offspring, the modes of support
provided to either children or the biological mother, and the couple relationships among these poor
families in which fathers are often nonresidential are sufficiently unique from more typical married
families to raise caution about any single profile of fathers in the current era. Instead, there continue
to be multiple faces of fatherhood.

Paternal Incarceration and Father Contact


Another group of fathers who have begun to receive attention and who are disproportionately
drawn from poor, disadvantaged, and minority groups are incarcerated fathers. A recent estimate
suggests that more than 5,000,000 children, or 7% of all U.S. children, have experienced a coresi-
dent parent leaving to spend time in jail or prison (Murphey and Cooper, 2015; see Dalliare, 2019).
A recent report revealed that 1 in 28 children in the United States had an incarcerated parent in 2010,
up from 1 in 125 children in 1985 (The Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010). Estimates suggest that cumu-
latively, 1 in 25 European American children and 1 in 4 African American children born in 1990
had experienced parental imprisonment by their 14th birthday (Wildeman, 2009). There has been
increasing interest in both the modes and the frequency of contact between incarcerated parents and
their children (Poehlmann-Tynan, 2014). A variety of factors alters the contact patterns between
incarcerated parents and their children. First, proximity is a major factor, and the greater the distance
between the incarceration site and the family, the less the likelihood of visitation (Myers, Smarsh,
Amlund-Hagen, and Kennon, 1999). Therefore, those in more distant state or federal prisons, rather
than in local jails, are less likely to experience regular visitation (Dallaire, 2019).
Second, the appropriateness and friendliness of visiting arrangements affect both the likelihood
of visitation and the positive or negative effects of the visit on both parent and child (Loper, Carlson,
Levitt, and Scheffel, 2009). Noisy, inhospitable contact settings, such as plexiglass barriers that limit
direct contact between parent and child, may reduce the benefits of visitation. In light of these diffi-
culties surrounding live visitation, remote forms of contact such as phone calls or written correspond-
ence offer a viable alternative for reliable contact between incarcerated parents and their children
(Poehlmann et al., 2010). Letter writing is the most frequent vehicle for parent-child contact, with
an estimated 52% of parents in state prisons having at least monthly mail contact with at least one
of their children (Glaze and Maruschak, 2008). Moreover, Tuerk and Loper (2006) found that fre-
quency of letter writing, rather than the frequency of personal visits or phone calls, accounted for the
association between more child contact and less parenting stress. As Poehlmann et al. (2010) noted,
“Parents have control over the content of letters and can plan and anticipate what their children
may need to hear in ways that are not available to them in a noisy or unpredictable visitation envi-
ronment” (p. 586). Other forms of remote communication are increasingly feasible such as video
calls, instant messaging, and e-mail, but these forms of communication are not uniformly available
to incarcerated individuals or are limited to a few contacts and at a cost to the imprisoned person
(Poehlmann-Tynan, 2014). Boudin (1997) described a program by which incarcerated parents read
and recorded stories for their children. The effects of these forms of contact remain to be rigorously
evaluated.

80
Fathers and Families

Third, according to a national survey of state and federal prisoners, imprisoned mothers more
frequently reported at least monthly phone calls (47% versus 38%) and mail correspondence with
children (65% versus 51%) than did imprisoned fathers (Glaze and Maruschak, 2008). Fathers are
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

less likely to receive visitation contact with their children than incarcerated mothers, and contact
is likely to vary with the earlier quality of the parent-child relationship (Poehlmann, Shlafer, Maes,
and Hanneman, 2008). Others found that imprisoned fathers (and mothers) perceiving stronger
coparenting alliances were more likely to experience child contact (letters in particular) than those
who disagreed about coparenting issues (Loper et al., 2009). Most children with incarcerated fathers
live with their mothers during the incarceration period, whereas children with incarcerated mothers
are more likely to live with their grandparents, other family members, or in foster care (Glaze and
Maruschak, 2008). Particularly in the case of young children, the non-incarcerated caregiver often
restricts contact with the incarcerated parent due to concerns about negative reactions to the visit
(Arditti, 2003). Additionally, because many young incarcerated fathers have limited direct experience
caring for children, interventions to pair parent education with one-on-one time with the child
appear to be having an impact on father-child time and relationships (Barr et al., 2011). Finally, prior
incarceration is related to less child contact in the post-incarceration period due to “the social stigma,
lower earning capability, and complicated relationships with mothers typically experienced after they
are released” (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010, p. 253). As we note ahead, incarceration has a clear
negative effect on children and families and although some forms of contact may be beneficial, the
effects of contact between incarcerated fathers and their children remain poorly understood.

Transnational Fathers and Patterns of Contact


Another understudied aspect of fathering at a distance involves transnational fathers who often leave
their country of origin and migrate to another country to improve their economic prospects but
who often leave either their partner or children or sometimes both behind in the process. Some esti-
mates suggest that 25% of children in U.S. immigrant families have at least one parent abroad (Maz-
zucato and Schans, 2011), and most often it is the father who is the emigrating parent. The decision
to divide childcare and provisioning responsibilities across international borders is best viewed as a
family strategy aimed at helping the family survive economically by finding better-paying jobs in
another country. At the same time, the consequences of these economic strategies for father-mother
relationships and father-child relationships are poorly understood. Little is known about the varia-
tions in the amount of time that fathers (or mothers) are separated from their children (or partners).
Thus, we need better information on the patterns of contact between transnational parents and their
children and or partners who remain in the country of origin.
Clearly, most parents make an effort to stay in contact with their children. Although visiting their
children is obviously important, and according to an internet survey (Cookston, Boyer, Vega, and
Parke, 2017), 64% of parents who were residing in the United States cited “visiting children living
there” as their primary reason for visiting their families in Mexico, visiting their home country is
often difficult in terms of logistics and cost. In spite of their motivation to see their children, 75%
of parents had not been back to visit their children in over a year. Alternatively and as a result of the
limited opportunities to have face-to face visits, most transnational parents use some form of “com-
munication at a distance” strategy to keep in contact with their children. In one study (Dreby, 2010),
most U.S.-based parents—mothers and fathers—reported calling home to Mexico once a week.
Similarly, 61% of children in this study reported talking to their parents once a week or more. The
frequency of contact needs to be better understood, and the modes of communication need to be
better specified. In the internet survey, Cookston et al. (2017) found that 83% of parents communi-
cated with their children via telephone, 85% via the internet, and 67% by video conference. Only
24% communicated by traditional postal mail.

81
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Many questions remain to be addressed. What is the quality of internet contact? What topics are
most frequently discussed? And how are variations in contact patterns related to fathers’ (and moth-
ers’) perceptions of his role as father? What factors determine variations in separation periods? How
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

does the age of the child alter the patterns of separation? Do fathers return home more often when
children are younger (or older)? Perhaps, fathers return more frequently to visit infants to establish
attachment bonds. Are they less (or more) likely to leave at all when children are young? Or do
fathers visit more when children are adolescents? Fathers may view themselves as more critical in
curbing deviant behavior during the adolescent period.

Father Duty: Military Fathers During Deployment and Back Home


Another form of fathering at a distance involves military fathers who are deployed to foreign lands as
part of their military duties. Patterns of contact between these physically absent fathers and their chil-
dren is beginning to receive attention (MacDermid Wadsworth, 2013). The evidence suggests that
deployed fathers use a variety of communication strategies to maintain contact with their partners
and children, such as phone contact, video conference, and other forms of electronic communication.
Ahead, we examine the impact of this form of father-child separation on children’s development.
This form of fathering at a distance is an especially useful comparison because many of the con-
founding factors, such as stigma and poverty, associated with other forms of distant fathering, such as
incarceration, are avoided.
Fathers who are in the military and are deployed due to military service experience a unique set
of challenges. On the one hand, their physical separation makes direct contact a challenge; however,
access to modern telecommunications means fathers can interact with their children regularly while
deployed and their relationships with their children’s other parent means they are able to remain
involved in the lives of their children. However, as we would expect with long periods of absence,
when military deployed fathers are separated from their children for extended periods, children
appear to increase in their adjustment problems at both school (Chandra, Martin, Hawkins, and
Richardson, 2010) and home (Lester and Flake, 2013). For the most part, children who experience
deployment fare well, but a subset of children experience adversity as a result of the deployment
separation (Mustillo, Wadsworth, and Lester, 2015). Importantly, a DVD intervention that employed
Sesame Street characters that was designed to help families prepare for and cope with deployment
was linked to better functioning among caregivers and children (Flittner O’Grady et al., 2016).
For many military fathers who have seen active duty, a return to family life can prove challeng-
ing, and it is common for these fathers to experience difficulties in their marriages and parenting
(Mustillo, Wadsworth, and Lester, 2015). In one sample of men who had served in the National
Guard (Mustillo et al., 2015), combat exposure was linked to major depression and the experience
of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Further, the quality of the marital relationship was associ-
ated with father internalizing symptoms, and parenting difficulties were greatest when depressive
symptoms were greatest.
Cozza, Holmes, and van Ost (2013) drew attention to risks associated with childhood in a military
family and suggested that a deficit perspective has emerged for research on the risks of a life in the
military. In fact, when both risk and protective factors associated were measured, evidence emerged
that the protective factors within families increased when risk was present (MacDermid Wadsworth
et al., 2016). For these reasons, the experiences of military families offer an important perspective for
consideration of the role of fathers in the lives of their children. In a qualitative study of deployed
fathers, parent identity emerged as a salient determinant whether and how fathers remain involved in
the lives of their children (Willerton et al., 2011). Given that the military is a workplace with expec-
tations and rules that bind members, opportunities exist to promote education and opportunities
for men to fully engage in the lives of their children and partners. An evaluation of a mobile phone

82
Fathers and Families

application designed to help military fathers learn about child development, track milestones, and
communicate with their child’s other parent demonstrated that the content resonated with military
fathers and could be a useful tool to engage military fathers in the lives of their children despite
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

possibly being separated by a great distance (Lee and Walsh, 2015). Clearly as the previous sections
illustrate, both differences in fathering across cultures and the considerable variability in patterns of
father involvement across social and ethnic groups in our own culture need to be recognized.

Determinants of Father Involvement


The importance of examining the determinants of father involvement stems from the view that
the paternal role is less culturally scripted and determined than the maternal role and few clear role
models for defining fatherhood exist. It is assumed that a multifactor approach to father involvement
is necessary because a variety of factors determines the degree of father involvement with children.
It is useful to distinguish individual, familial, and societal levels of analysis in assessing the determi-
nants of father involvement with children. As outlined in Figure 3.1, it is useful to distinguish both
biological (hormonal and neurological) and social (individual, familial, and societal levels) determi-
nants of father involvement with children (Doherty, Kouneski, and Erikson, 1998; Li and Fleming,
2019; Parke and Buriel, 2006). It is important to recognize that these factors operate together in
determining fathering behavior even though most prior investigators have examined single or a few
predictors within an individual study. The advantages of an additive and interactive approach over a
model focusing on single components to understanding father-child interaction are illustrated in a
study by Holmes and Huston (2010), who found that a variety of factors including father parenting
beliefs, child language and social skills, maternal employment and mother-child interaction quality
together predicted father-child interaction patterns (see Parke and Buriel, 2006; Pleck, 2010; Pleck
and Masciadrelli, 2004 for reviews).

Biological Factors in Paternal Behavior

Hormonal Influences
It has long been recognized that females undergo a variety of hormonal changes during pregnancy
and childbirth that may facilitate maternal behavior. It was long assumed that hormones play an
unimportant role in paternal behavior because exposure to rat pups increases paternal activity with-
out any changes in hormone levels (Li and Fleming, 2019). More recent evidence has challenged the
assumption that hormonal levels are unimportant determinants of paternal behavior by examining
this issue in species other the rat, which is not a natural paternal species. In naturally paternal spe-
cies, such as canid species who constitute less than 10% of mammalian species (Storey and Walsh,
2013), researchers have found that in a variety of animal species, males experience hormonal changes
including increases in prolactin and decreases in testosterone prior to the onset of parental behavior
and during infant contact (Li and Fleming, 2019).
Human fathers, too, undergo hormonal changes during pregnancy and childbirth (Feldman, 2019;
Storey and Walsh, 2013). Males experience hormonal changes prior to the onset of parental behav-
ior and during infant contact. Storey, Walsh, Quinton, and Wynne-Edwards (2000) found that men
experienced significant pre-, peri-, and postnatal changes in each of these hormones—prolactin,
cortisol, and testosterone—a pattern of results that was similar to the women in their study. Specifi-
cally, prolactin levels were higher for both men and women in the late prenatal period than in the
early prenatal period, and cortisol levels increased just before birth and decreased in the postnatal
period for both men and women. Testosterone levels were lower in the early postnatal period, which
corresponds to the first opportunity for interaction with their infants. Hormonal levels and changes

83
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

were linked with a variety of social stimuli as well. Men with lower testosterone held test baby dolls
longer and were more responsive to infant cues (crying) than men with higher testosterone. Men
who reported a greater drop in testosterone also reported more pregnancy or couvade symptoms.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Together, these findings suggest that lower testosterone in the postnatal period may increase pater-
nal responsiveness, in part, by reducing competitive non-nurturing behavior (Storey et al., 2000).
Similarly, prolactin levels were higher in men showing greater responsiveness to infant cries and in
men reporting more couvade symptoms during pregnancy. Storey et al. (2000, p. 91) argued that the
“cortisol increases in late pregnancy and during labor may help new fathers focus on and become
attached to their newborns.” Men’s changes in hormonal levels are linked not only with baby cries
and the time in the pregnancy birth cycle but also to the hormonal levels of their partners. Women’s
hormonal levels were closely linked with the time remaining before delivery, and men’s levels were
linked with their partner’s hormone levels, not with time to birth. That father hormone levels are
linked with those of his partner suggests that contact with the pregnant partner may play a role in
paternal responsiveness, just as the quality of the marital relationship is linked with paternal involve-
ment in later infancy. Clearly, social variables need to be considered in understanding the operation
of biological effects. Perhaps intimate ties between partners during pregnancy stimulates hormonal
changes, which, in turn, are associated with more nurturance toward babies. This perspective rec-
ognizes the dynamic or transactional nature of the links between hormones and behavior in which
behavior changes can lead to hormonal shifts and vice versa.
Other evidence is consistent with a psychobiological view of paternal behavior. Fleming and
colleagues (2002) found that fathers with lower baseline levels of testosterone are more sympathetic
and show a greater need to respond when hearing infant cries than men with higher baseline tes-
tosterone levels. Moreover, fathers with higher baseline prolactin levels are more positive and alert
in response to infant cries. Just as in the case of mothers, contact with the baby is linked to the
level of the hormone oxytocin for new fathers too; paternal oxytocin correlated with the degree
of stimulatory parenting behaviors, including proprioceptive contact, tactile stimulation, and object
presentation (Feldman, Gordon, Schneiderman, Weisman, and Zagoory-Sharon, 2010). This stimula-
tory play style is the typical and unique way that many Western fathers interact with their infants.
But all of these studies were cross-sectional snapshots, and alternative interpretations are possible.
Perhaps men who show lower testosterone are more likely to partner and reproduce, whereas higher
testosterone men stay single and do not become parents. Longitudinal work suggests that becoming
a father does, indeed, lead to a drop in testosterone which, in turn, may better prepare them for the
nurturing aspect of parenting. Gettler and his colleagues (2011) followed a group of over 600 men
in the Philippines over a 5-year period from the time that they were single to the time some became
fathers. Men with high levels of testosterone were more likely to become partnered fathers over the
course of the study than men with lower levels of the hormone, possibly because men with higher
testosterone were more assertive in competing for women or appeared healthier and more attrac-
tive. However, the men who became partnered fathers showed a larger drop in testosterone than did
single non-fathers, whereas the drop for the men who became fathers was nearly twice as large as the
decline in testosterone shown by the single men. These findings show that relations between testos-
terone and men’s reproductive strategies are bidirectional. High testosterone is helpful in the mating
process but declines rapidly once men become fathers and begin the process of parenting where
lower levels of testosterone are better for maintaining a family. As anthropologist Peter Gray noted,
“A dad with lower testosterone is maybe a little more sensitive to cues from his child, and maybe he’s
a little less sensitive to cues from a woman he meets at a restaurant” (Simon, 2011).
Moreover, childcare experience also plays a role. Fathers who have more experience with babies
have lower testosterone and higher prolactin levels than first-time fathers (Fleming, Corter, Stall-
ings, and Steiner, 2002), even after controlling for paternal age. In the Gettler, McDade, Feranil,
and Kuzawa (2011) study, fathers with extensive involvement in childcare (3 hours a day or more

84
Fathers and Families

playing, feeding, bathing, toileting, reading, or dressing them) showed larger decreases in testosterone
than fathers who were less involved with the routine care of their children. Similar links between
testosterone and involvement in childcare have been found in Tanzania (Muller, Marlowe, Bugumba,
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and Ellison, 2009). In two neighboring cultural groups, fathers in the group in which paternal care
is the cultural norm had lower testosterone than among fathers in the group in which paternal care
is absent. In a study of a polygamous Senegalese society, fathers who were highly invested in their
children, as reported by the children’s mothers, had lower testosterone compared with fathers who
were less invested (Alvergne, Faurie, and Raymond, 2009).
This perspective recognizes the dynamic or transactional nature of the links between hormones
and behavior in which behavior changes can lead to hormonal shifts and vice versa. In contrast to
the myth of the biologically unfit father, this work suggests that men may be more prepared—even
biologically—for parenting than previously thought. Men, just like women, are biologically evolved
to be parents, and cooperation between parents is an adaptive strategy.
More work is needed to explore the implications of these hormonal changes for the long-term
relationship between fathers and their offspring. For example, are the ties between children and
fathers who do not experience hormone-related changes at birth weaker, or can experience com-
pensate for this lack of hormonal shift? Do childless men or women show hormonal changes as a
result of opportunities to engage infants and children as well? However, it is clear that hormonal, in
combination with social, factors are an important class of factors to recognize because the shifts in
paternal hormones may decrease differences in maternal versus paternal parenting behavior. It is not
just hormones that reflect fathers’ biological preparedness for parenting.

Insights From the Brain


Perhaps some of the most striking evidence that humans—mothers and fathers—are biologically
prepared for caregiving comes from studies of how our brains react when we are exposed to babies.
From the earliest days of life, fathers as well as mothers are neurologically primed to respond to
infants. Using brain imaging techniques such as fMRI, they show more neural activation when
shown pictures of babies than pictures of animate objects (Swain, Lorberbaum, Kose, and Strathearn,
2007). Brain imaging reveals that mothers and fathers compared with nonparents exhibited more
pronounced neural responses in the right amygdala (an area involved in emotional processing) in
response to infant crying than to infant laughter (Seifritz et al., 2003). Evidence for differences in
neural responses to the emotional responses of children suggests parents may experience a cry as an
emotionally important signal which requires their attention and demonstrates that the emotion areas
of the brain may be involved in fathers and mothers listening/responding to babies distress signals
(Seifritz et al., 2003). Fathers and mothers show higher levels of activity in emotional processing
areas of the brain when exposed to infant cries than nonparents (Swain, 2008; Swain et al., 2011).
Other brain imaging studies found that men respond neurologically more to the cries of their own
infants than to the distress signals of unrelated infants (Swain et al., 2007). Structural changes in the
brain of fathers over the first 4 months postpartum have been documented (Kim et al., 2014). Fathers
exhibited increase in gray matter (GM) volume in several neural regions involved in parental motiva-
tion, including the hypothalamus, amygdala, striatum, and lateral prefrontal cortex. Fathers exhibited
decreases in GM volume in the orbitofrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and insula. These
changes may support the development of close attachment relationships with their offspring during
early infancy. The findings provide evidence for neural plasticity in fathers’ brains that accompanies
interaction experience between fathers and their infants. How much and what type of social inter-
actional experience between fathers and infants is related to these brain changes remains unexplored.
Moreover, hormonal changes in vasopressin may be related to these brain changes (Kim et al., 2014), a
reminder that neurological and hormonal influences may act together in shaping paternal responding.

85
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

In sum, our brains as well as our hormones prepare not just mothers but also fathers for the
challenges of caregiving. In contrast to the myth of the biologically unfit father, this work suggests
that men may be more prepared even biologically for parenting than previously thought. Finally, it
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

is critical to underscore that these hormonal possibly neurological changes are not necessary for the
elicitation of fathering behaviors in either animals or humans (Li and Fleming, 2019). In humans, for
example, studies of father-infant relationships in the cases of adoption clearly suggest that hormonal
shifts are unnecessary for the development of positive father-infant relationships (Grotevant and
McDermott, 2014; Pinderhughes and Brodinsky, 2019). Next, we turn to a discussion of the social
determinants of father involvement.

Individual Factors
Men’s own psychological and family background, attitudes toward the fathering role, motivation to
become involved, and childcare and childrearing knowledge and skills all play a role in determining
men’s level of involvement with their children.

Men’s Relationships With Their Family of Origin


The quality of relationship that fathers develop with their own mothers and fathers has been viewed
as a possible determinant of fathers’ involvement with their own children. However, evidence in sup-
port of this proposition is not clear-cut. Two views have guided this inquiry. First, from social learn-
ing theory (Bandura, 1989) comes a modeling hypothesis that suggests that men model themselves
after their fathers, and this modeling process will be enhanced if their fathers were nurturant and
accessible. Second, a compensatory or reworking hypothesis argues that fathers tend to compensate
or make up for deficiencies in their childhood relationships with their own fathers by becoming
better and more involved when they themselves assume this role (Roy and Smith, 2013). There is
support for both views. In support of the modeling hypothesis, a number of studies suggest that posi-
tive relationships with fathers in childhood are related to higher levels of later father involvement
(Cowan and Cowan, 1992; Sagi, 1982). Hofferth (1999) found that men whose fathers were active
participants in rearing them are more involved with their own offspring, take more responsibility, are
warmer, and monitor them more closely than men reared by less involved fathers. Subsequent results
from the 1979 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth showed that men were more involved with
their children when their own fathers were more involved when they were children (Hofferth, Pleck,
and Vesely, 2012). Importantly, the link between more involved fathering as a child and involved
fathering as a parent was not better explained by mothering or adolescent adjustment. Moreover,
fathers parent more like their own fathers than their mothers (Losh-Hesselbart, 1997). There is also
intergenerational transmission of harshness. Scaramella and Conger (2003) found that children who
experienced hostile parenting were more likely to use similar tactics when they became parents.
Support for the second hypothesis is also evident. Men may reject their own experiences with
their fathers and opt for alternative compensatory parenting practices. Baruch and Barnett (1986)
found that men who viewed their own relationships with their fathers as negative tended to be
more involved with their 5- and 9-year-old children. Similarly, studies of men reared in father
absent homes suggest that many men became more involved when they themselves became fathers
(Roy, 2006). As Sagi (1982, p. 214) argued, these two hypotheses “are not mutually exclusive since
either process is possible depending on the circumstances.” Prior history often combines with cur-
rent circumstances as well as child temperament in determining cross-generational father involve-
ment. Low-income men who had poor childhood relationships with their fathers and poor current
circumstances (poor coparent relationship, limited education) were low on involvement with their
children, whereas men with negative histories with their fathers but a positive current situation were

86
Fathers and Families

highly involved with their children (McFadden, Tamis-Lemonda, Howard, Shannon, and Cabrera,
2009). Moreover, cross-generational continuity of parental hostility is more likely if children were
temperamentally negative (high in negative reactivity), but less continuity was found when children
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

were temperamentally more positive (Scaramella and Conger, 2003). Another factor that can disrupt
the intergenerational transmission of hostility is the presence of a warm and supportive coparent
(Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, and Cabrera, 2006). It is oversimplified to assume that family of origin
is the only major influence on fathering, and a broader set of cultural factors needs to be recognized.
Roy, Buckmiller, and McDowell (2008) found that only about half of the men reported that their
own fathers taught them how to be a father. In a qualitative study, Daly (1993) interviewed fathers
of young children about the sources of their role models for their own fatherhood identity. Some
fathers emulate their own fathers, others compensate, and still others report little influence of their
own fathers as mentors or models. However, most fathers interviewed by Daly either did not view
their fathers as a model or wanted to do better as fathers than their own fathers. Many fathers in
Daly’s study opted for a piecemeal approach to defining fathering. Instead of emulating one person,
many men tried to piece together an image of fathering from many different sources including
extended family members, cultural figures, and friends. This view is echoed by Roy and Smith
(2013), who suggested that “young men’s lessons about how to care emerge through the enactment
of relationships as sons to their parents, as siblings or as junior family members with aunts and uncles,
cousins or grandparents” (p. 324).
Men thus draw on models from their own generation of contemporary fathers as well as fathers
from earlier eras and generations. As men become fathers, they struggle to reconcile past and pre-
sent images and models of fathering behavior with the changed historical circumstances that face
modern fathers. Even if they chose to emulate their own fathers, the rapid changes in our society
make it difficult for current fathers to apply these lessons from the past in any simple way. It is clear
that the process of intergenerational transmission of parenting is an active one in which the father
himself plays a central role in sorting, retaining, and discarding images and guidelines from a variety
of sources. There is no simple or single route to developing a father identity; there are many different
paths just as there are many different kinds of fathers.

Socialization of Boys Into the Fathering Role


It is not only the specific nature of the relationship a child develops with his parents but the ways in
which girls and boys are differentially prepared for parenthood during childhood that determine the
ways in which girls and boys enact their parenting roles in adulthood. It is well known that girls and
boys are treated differently by parents during socialization but most of this work focuses on occu-
pational aspirations, activity, and object preferences and modes of relating to others. However, the
differential treatment of girls and boys may help shape a boy’s expectation about his future fathering
role. A variety of strands of evidence supports this view. First, children’s household chores are based
on gender, with girls engaging in more household duties such as cleaning, ironing, cooking, and
childcare, whereas boys take out the garbage and mow the lawn (Goodnow and Lawrence, 2001;
McHale, Crouter, and Whiteman, 2003). Cross-national studies of over 30 countries confirm these
findings, with girls engaging in more household work and boys engaging in non-household labor
such as farm- or business-related activities (Putnick and Bornstein, 2016). Of particular importance
are the differential opportunities afforded to girls and boys to learn childcare skills. Cross-cultural
evidence (Whiting, Edwards, Ember, and Erchak, 1988), as well as evidence from our own culture,
suggests that boys are given dolls less often than girls and are discouraged from playing with dolls
(Rheingold and Cook, 1975; Pomerleau, Bolduc, Malcuit, and Cossotte, 1990). In middle child-
hood, boys are less likely to be babysitters (Goodnow, 1999) and are less likely to be caregivers for
their siblings (East, 2010). This lack of opportunities for socialization into parenthood makes boys

87
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

less prepared for fatherhood than girls are for motherhood and may lead to lower levels of parental
self-efficacy for men than women (Rose and Halverson, 1996). However, some qualitative evidence
suggests that in single-mother families or in economically disadvantaged households, boys as well as
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

girls are more likely to be recruited into household work, including sibling caregiving duties, in part
due to the more limited parental resources in these households (Roy and Smith, 2013). When boys
are obligated to care for younger siblings, some suggest that “these first steps toward learning to par-
ent may lead to a new sense of self confidence, empathy or leadership” (Roy and Smith, 2013, p. 325).
Although these qualitative studies suggest that these childhood experiences may enhance later father
involvement, further quantitative work is needed to firmly establish these links.

Men’s Attitudes, Motivation, and Skills


Paternal attitudes, motivation, and skills are important determinants of father involvement (Lamb
and Lewis, 2010). In early work on attitudes and father involvement, the focus was on the relation
between sex role attitudes and paternal involvement. However, weak or no links between sex role
attitudes and father involvement have led to a more specific focus on men’s attitudes concerning the
paternal role (Parke, 2002). When the focus is more specifically oriented toward beliefs about parental
roles, clearer links between beliefs and behavior are evident (Bornstein, Putnick, and Suwalsky, 2017).
This work has been guided, in part, by identity theory which recognizes that men play a variety of
roles as spouse, parent, and worker and that the relative importance men assign to these roles in their
formation of a self-identity is a useful predictor of father involvement (Rane and McBride, 2000).
Paternal attitudes about parenting relate to measures of father involvement with their 3-month-
old infants (Beitel and Parke, 1998). Specifically, fathers’ belief in the biological basis of sex differ-
ences, their perception of their caregiving skills, and the extent to which they valued the father’s role
were predictors of father’s involvement. Finally, a variety of types of involvement relates to paternal
attitudes including play, caregiving, and indirect care (e.g., packs diaper bag, changes crib linen).
Other studies have confirmed that paternal attitudes are important beyond infancy. Russell (1983)
found that Australian men who do not accept the notion of maternal instinct participate in childcare
more with their 3- to 6-year-old children. Consistent with this work, McBride and Rane (1997)
found that fathers’ attitudes about the importance of father for preschool age children’s development
were linked to several indices of father involvement (responsibility, interaction, and accessibility),
and their perceived investment in the worker role was inversely related to their workday accessibil-
ity and responsibility. Rane and McBride (2000) found that fathers who considered the nurturing
role highly central to their sense of self engaged in more interaction and assumed more responsibil-
ity behaviors with their children than fathers low on nurturing role centrality. Bonney, Kelley, and
Levant (1999) found that fathers with more liberal gender ideology and who view fathers as criti-
cal for child development and as capable of performing childcare as mothers have higher levels of
involvement in childcare than more traditional fathers. Similarly, fathers who have more traditional
childrearing attitudes (high in control and emotionally distant) have lower quality interactions with
their children than less traditional fathers (Holmes and Huston, 2010). Finally, Hofferth, Pleck, Gold-
scheider, Curtin, and Hrapczynski (2013) suggested that paternal attitudes (e.g., “fathers should be as
involved as mothers,” “fathers and mothers are equally good at meeting their children’s needs”) are
undergoing change but also predict father involvement across time. Compared with 1997, in 2003,
fathers’ attitudes concerning their paternal role became more positive. And positive attitudes were
linked with several indices of father involvement (i.e., engagement, warmth, control, discussion of
rules) across this period.
In spite of the fact that men are competent caregivers, there are wide individual differences among
men in either their perceived or actual level of skill in caregiving. In turn, these variations in skill may
be related to level of father involvement.

88
Fathers and Families

Paternal Mental Health


A variety of studies supports the prediction that personal resources—including mental health—alter
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

parenting behaviors (Cummings, Merrilees, and Ward George, 2010). Although many studies have
found that maternal psychopathology, such as depression, will alter parenting behavior, evidence sug-
gests that paternal depression is an important determinant of fathering as well (Cummings, Keller,
and Davies, 2005). Fathers who report depression at 8 weeks postpartum have children with more
conduct problems 3 years later (Ramchandani, Stein, Evans, and O’Connor, 2005). Others find that
paternal depression is linked with poorer peer relationships and more behavior problems (Dave et al.,
2005). Labeled the “paternal mental health hypothesis” (Cummings et al., 2010), this perspective
focuses on the role of parental psychological functioning in accounting for the effects of marital
conflict either as a risk or protective factor. While recognizing that the mental health of both parents
is important, a strong version of this position suggests that father mental health may be particularly
influential in altering children’s adjustment. Papp, Cummings, and Schemerhorn (2004) found that
paternal (as well as maternal) depression was linked with poor child adjustment indirectly through a
poor marital relationship. Mothers’ depression was directly related to child problems, which suggests
that paternal and maternal depression may be linked to child outcomes through different pathways.
Other work suggests that the effects of paternal and maternal depression may affect different child
outcomes and vary by gender of child (Cummings et al., 2005; Shelton and Harold, 2008).
As we explore ahead, fathers’ parenting may be more vulnerable to problematic marital relation-
ships than mothers’ parenting. Some evidence suggests that paternal depression may have a greater
impact on their parenting and, in turn, their children’s adjustment than maternal depression (Low
and Stocker, 2005), a finding that would bolster the paternal mental health hypothesis. Cummings,
El-Sheikh, Kouros, and Keller (2007) found that children’s sympathetic nervous system reactivity in
response to marital conflict was linked to child adjustment more strongly for fathers’ than moth-
ers’ dysphoria, a result that supports the paternal mental health argument. Further support for this
hypothesis comes from evidence that suggests that problem drinking may negatively impact not only
the marital relationship but also parenting and, in turn, children’s adjustment; again, paternal drinking
is more consequential for child outcomes than maternal alcohol consumption (Keller, Cummings,
Davies, and Petersen, 2009).

Characteristics of the Child


In recognition that fathers and children mutually influence each other, it is important to note that
a variety of child characteristics merits consideration as well. Several individual child features have
been identified, including child gender, temperament, and children’s social skill and language com-
petence. Support for gender of child has been found in infancy, with fathers devoting more time
to boys than girls (Lamb and Lewis, 2010) but less support for a clear gender effect beyond infancy
(Pleck and Masciadrelli, 2004). Child sociability positively predicted a father’s time with his child,
his accessibility to his child, and his responsibility for the child’s care (McBride, Schoppe, and Rane,
2002) and to the quality of father-child interaction (Holmes and Huston, 2010). Moreover, children
with high receptive and expressive language skills engage in more positive father-child interactions
than children with low skills (Holmes and Huston, 2010). Children’s health appears to be important
as well. Greater neurobiological risk at birth and a longer hospitalization at birth are associated with
more distress at 6 months of age for fathers but not mothers (Gueron-Sela, Atzaba-Poria, Meiri, and
Marks, 2015). Further, although diagnoses of a developmental delay in children are resolved similarly
by mothers and fathers, mothers tend to employ methods of emotion coping, and fathers favor more
cognitive coping strategies (Barak-Levy and Atzaba-Poria, 2013). When children experience sleep
difficulties, fathers (more so than mothers) report their children as more difficult, but when fathers

89
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

of children with sleep difficulties become more engaged, it serves to protect mothers from stress
(Millikovsky-Ayalon, Atzaba-Poria, and Meiri, 2015). Finally, child temperament has been linked to
father-child interaction quality. When children have difficult temperaments, fathers as well as moth-
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

ers are more likely to use coercive parenting strategies (Rothbart, 2011) or be less involved with
their children (Grych and Clark, 1999). And temperament may affect the quality of coparenting as
well (Wong, Mangelsdorf, Brown, Neff, and Schoppe-Sullivan, 2009). In sum, both father and child
characteristics play roles in determining the father-child relationship.

Family Factors
Individual factors are not the only determinants of father involvement. Family-level variables includ-
ing maternal attitudes concerning father involvement and the marital relationship are both family
level factors that require examination.

Maternal Attitudes: Mother as Gatekeeper


Consistent with a family systems view, maternal attitudes need to be considered as a determinant of
paternal participation in childcare (Schoppe-Sullivan and Altenburger, 2019). In spite of advances
in women’s participation in the workplace, many women still feel ambivalent about father involve-
ment in domestic issues (Doucet, 2006). Due, in part, to the “cult of maternalism” (Duffy, 1988),
which stresses the notion that mothers are indispensable, natural, and necessary, many women are
reluctant to actively and wholeheartedly involve fathers in the daily routines of caregiving. As Allen
and Hawkins (1999) suggest, their ambivalence “may be because increased paternal involvement
intrudes on a previously held monopoly over the attentive and intuitive responsibilities of family
work which if altered may compromise female power and privilege in the home” (p. 202). However,
contradictory attitudes about father involvement may reflect a more deeply held belief that predates
recent increases in father involvement. As a father of five in 1927 lamented, “There often seems to
be a conspiracy on the part of the female of the species to exclude father from any active part in
the care of the baby” (What Should a Child Demand of His Father, Parents’ Magazine, 1927; cited
by Milkie and Denny, 2014). These maternal attitudes may lead to behaviors which, in turn, limit
father involvement and constitute a form of gatekeeping. A major advance in the conceptualization
of gatekeeping was made by Allen and Hawkins (1999, p. 200), who defined the term as follows:

Maternal gatekeeping is a collection of beliefs and behaviors that ultimately inhibit a col-
laboration effort between men and women in families by limiting men’s opportunities for
learning and growing through caring for home and children.

These investigators identified three conceptual dimensions: Mothers’ reluctance to relinquish respon-
sibility over family matters by setting rigid standards, external validation of a mothering identity, and
differentiated conceptions of family roles. In a study of dual-earner families, Allen and Hawkins
(1999) found that 21% of the mothers were classified as gatekeepers who, in turn, did more hours
of family work per week and had less equal divisions of labor than women classified as collabora-
tors. Unfortunately, the scales used to measure gatekeeping included both housework and child-
care, although the authors noted that “separating housework and child care into unique measures
produced similar results” (1999, p. 210). Nor were the number and ages of the children specified in
analyses. Other studies of gatekeeping have focused specifically on father involvement in childcare.
Beitel and Parke (1998) examined the relation between maternal attitudes and father involve-
ment with 3- to 5-month-old infants. A variety of maternal attitudes concerning father involvement
including judgments about their husband’s motivation and interest in childcare activities, maternal

90
Fathers and Families

perception of his childcare skills, and the value that she places on his involvement linked with father
involvement. Mothers’ belief in innate sex differences in female and male ability to nurture infants
and the extent to which mothers viewed themselves as critical of the quality of their husbands’
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

caregiving were negatively related to father involvement. As these results suggest, maternal attitudes
play a significant role in understanding father involvement, but the type of involvement needs to be
considered, because maternal attitudes may vary across different types of involvement (e.g., play, role
responsibility, indirect care).
But gatekeeping is not restricted to infancy. Fagan and Barnett (2003) found a direct link between
maternal gatekeeping and father involvement in a sample of fathers of children between 3 and
17 years of age Similarly, McBride and Rane (1997) found that maternal perceptions of their partners
investment in the parental as well as the spousal role were related to father involvement, whereas
their perceptions of their husbands’ investment in the worker role was negatively related to fathers’
involvement. In fact, mothers’ perceptions were the best predictors of total father involvement.
Other evidence suggests that fathers’ participation may be more self-determined than these earlier
studies suggest. Bonney, Kelley, and Levant (1999) found that mothers’ attitudes about the degree to
which fathers should be involved in childcare were unrelated to fathers’ participation in childcare.
Instead, they found that fathers’ participation appears to influence mother’s beliefs about the father’s
role. Or perhaps, as Bonney et al. (1999) argue, a transactional perspective best characterizes this
relation between maternal attitudes and father involvement in which fathers who are more involved
have female partners who develop more positive attitudes about their involvement which, in turn,
increases fathers’ level of participation. Longitudinal studies are needed to more definitively deter-
mine the direction of causality in this domain.
Finally, gatekeeping effects are not confined to intact families as studies of postdivorce efforts on
the part of the custodial parent demonstrate. Custodial parents, usually the mother, will often attempt
to restrict access to the noncustodial father after divorce, especially if there is a high level of couple
conflict and distrust (Fabricius et al., 2010; see Ganong et al., 2019). Moreover, in nonresidential
father families, mothers or, in some cases, maternal grandparents control paternal access to the child
and may facilitate or restrict his access (Edin and Nelson, 2013; Fagan and Barnett, 2003). Similarly,
visitation opportunities between incarcerated fathers and their children are regulated by their non-
incarcerated partner (Poehlmann et al., 2010), just as in the case on transnational fathers where
custodial mothers or other relatives may control the duration and frequency of phone contact with
their children (Dreby, 2010).
Two qualifications to our discussion of gatekeeping are needed. First, the term is gender neu-
tral and fathers as well as mothers engage in gatekeeping activities in other domains of family life
(Allen and Hawkins, 1999; Pruett, Williams, Insabella, and Little, 2003; Schoppe-Sullivan, Brown,
and Cannon, 2008; Trinder, 2008), and theoretical work suggests that there are likely to be bidi-
rectional influences across partners with respect to gatekeeping attitudes and behaviors (Adamsons,
2010). Second, gates can open as well as close, and the term needs to be broadened to recognize
that parents—­mother and fathers—can facilitate as well as inhibit the type and level of domestic
involvement of each other. Work on “parental gatekeeping” needs to include gateopening as well as
gateclosing to underscore the dual nature of the inhibitory and faciliatory processes that are part of
the coparenting enterprise.

Couple Relationships and Father-Child Relationships


Models that limit examination of the effects of interaction patterns to only father-child and mother-
child dyads and the direct effects of one individual on another are inadequate for understanding the
impact of social interaction patterns in families (Belsky, 1984; Cummings et al., 2010; Parke, 2013).
From a family systems viewpoint, the couple relationship needs to be considered as well. The bulk of

91
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

attention has been given to intact families, as noted earlier, but the quality of the couple relationship
in non-intact families suggests that the quality of the couple relationship is a major determinant of
the degree of father involvement.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Studies of nonresident unmarried fathers suggest that when the relationship between the paren-
tal partners is harmonious (romantic or not) and their quality of coparenting is satisfactory, father
involvement is higher (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010; Lerman and Sorensen, 2000), but if either
parent has moved on to a new partner, father participation may decrease or at least be more selective
in terms of which child is the recipient of father involvement (Edin and Nelson, 2013; Edin, Tach,
and Mincy, 2009).
Among intact families, the primary focus has been on the effects of the quality of the couple
relationship (conflictful versus cooperative/constructive) on their children. Two perspectives con-
cerning the link between couple conflict and children’s adaptation can be distinguished. Accord-
ing to a direct effects model, direct exposure to couple conflict influences children’s behavior; the
indirect model suggests that the impact of couple conflict on children is indirectly mediated by
changes in the parent-child relationship (Crockenberg and Langrock, 2001; Cummings and Davies,
2010; Grych and Fincham, 1993). Several decades of investigation suggest that dimensions of marital
functioning are related to aspects of children’s long-term overall adjustment and immediate coping
responses in the face of interparental conflict (Cummings and Davies, 2010). Couple discord and
conflict are linked to a variety of child outcomes, including antisocial behavior, internalizing and
externalizing behavior problems, and changes in cognition, emotions, and physiology in response
to exposure to marital conflict (Cummings et al., 2010). Although less empirical work has been
directed specifically toward examination of the “carryover” of exposure to couple conflict to the
quality of children’s relationships with significant others, such as peers and siblings, exposure to
couple discord is associated with poor social competence and problematic peer relationships (Katz
and Gottman, 1991; Kerig, 1996). Importantly, among divorcing families when conflict tends to be
high, fathering can compensate for the negative risk associated with exposure to conflict (Sandler,
Miles, Cookston, and Braver, 2008), and intervention with fathers after divorce appears to reduce
interparental conflict while increasing perceived coparenting by mothers (Cookston, Braver, Griffin,
deLusé, and Miles, 2007).
Couple discord can have an indirect influence on children’s adjustment through changes in the
quality of parenting (Cummings and Davies, 2010; Fauber and Long, 1991). Factors that include
affective changes in the quality of the parent-child relationship, lack of emotional availability, and
adoption of less optimal parenting styles each has been implicated as a potential mechanism through
which marital discord disrupts parenting processes. Marital conflict is linked with poor parenting,
which, in turn, is related to poor social adjustment on the children. Other work has focused on the
specific processes by which the marital relationship itself directly influences children’s immediate
functioning and long-term adjustment. Several aspects of parental conflict appear to be relatively
consistently associated with poor outcomes for children. More frequent interparental conflict and
more intense or violent forms of conflict have been found to be particularly disturbing to children
and likely to be associated with externalizing and internalizing difficulties (Cummings and Davies,
2010) and poor peer relationships (Parke et al., 2001). Conflict that was child-related in content was
more likely than conflict involving other content to be associated with behavior problems in chil-
dren (Cummings and Davies, 2010; Grych and Fincham, 1993). Finally, when parents resolve their
conflict, the negative effects on children are reduced.
Resolution of conflict reduces children’s negative reactions to exposure to inter-adult anger and
conflict. Exposure to unresolved conflict is associated with negative affect and poor coping responses
in children (Cummings and Davies, 2010). In addition, the manner in which conflict is resolved may
also influence children’s adjustment. Katz and Gottman (1993) found that couples who exhibited a
hostile style of resolving conflict had children who tended to be described by teachers as exhibiting

92
Fathers and Families

antisocial characteristics. When husbands were angry and emotionally distant while resolving marital
conflict, children were described by teachers as anxious and socially withdrawn.
Conflict is inevitable in most parental relationships and is not detrimental to family relationships
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and children’s functioning under all circumstances. In particular, disagreements that are extremely
intense and involve threat to the child are likely to be more disturbing to the child. In contrast, when
conflict is expressed constructively, is moderate in degree, is expressed in the context of a warm and
supportive family environment, and shows evidence of resolution, children may learn valuable lessons
regarding how to negotiate conflict and resolve disagreements (Cummings and Davies, 2010). How-
ever, although the evidence suggests that couple conflict may alter the parenting of both mothers
and fathers, evidence suggests that fathers are more likely to be affected by marital conflict than their
female partners. Several studies in the United States (Cummings and Davies, 2010), Great Britain
(Pike, Caldwell, and Dunn, 2005), and other cultures (e.g., Japan; Durrett, Otaki, and Richards, 1984)
support the conclusion that the quality of the mother-father relationship is related to the quality of
the father- and mother-child relationships, which, in turn, may alter the child’s adjustment. Several
major theoretical and empirical advances in this domain over the past several decades are noteworthy.
First, the vulnerability of the father to disruptions in quality of the couple relationship has continued
to receive attention. Often termed the father “vulnerability hypothesis” (Cummings et al., 2010), this
notion suggests that the father-child relationship is altered more than the mother-child relationship
by the quality of the couple relationship. This position continues to receive support but several mod-
erating process that govern its operation have been identified. Second, possible mediators between
the impact of couple quality on fathering and child outcomes have been discovered. Third, longi-
tudinal evidence in support of this hypothesis has begun to accumulate. Fourth, the positive effects
of a high-quality couple relationship on fathering (and mothering) have received more scrutiny as a
corrective to the usual focus of the effects of marital conflict.

The Father Vulnerability Hypothesis


This notion has a long history of support over the last 30 years. A meta-analysis of the relations
between interparental conflict and parenting provided strong evidence of this position including
stronger links between a variety of aspects of fathering behaviors such as parental control, accept-
ance, harsh discipline, and overall parenting quality than for mothering behaviors (Krishnakumar
and Buehler, 2000). However, this hypothesis needs to be qualified in several ways (Cummings et al.,
2010). First, contextual factors need consideration because fathers’ parenting is linked with poor
marital quality in both dyadic and triadic settings, but mothers exhibit the negative effects of marital
conflict on parenting only in triadic contexts (Clements, Lim, and Chaplin, 2002). Perhaps the pres-
ence of her conflictful partner in the triadic interactional context highlight their mutual estrange-
ment leading to a deterioration of mothering, whereas fathers may have more deeply internalized
the negativity associated with the marital conflict. Second, the child’s gender matters as well. Some
studies have found that fathering boys (Kitzmann, 2000) is more vulnerable to marital quality and
conflict than mothering of boys, but others found that fathering girls is more negatively affected than
fathering boys (Cowan and Cowan, 1992). Third, some child outcomes may be more strongly altered
by marital conflict than others. For example, fathers’ parenting declines in response to marital quality
were linked with increases in boys’ internalizing, whereas maternal parenting in high conflict marital
contexts is associated with higher externalizing problems among boys (Kaczynski et al., 2006). There
is overall support for the father vulnerability hypothesis, but a variety of factors may qualify its opera-
tion and probably more will be discovered.
Even stronger support for this hypothesis comes from longitudinal studies. For example, across
time, interparental hostility and withdrawal are both linked to parental emotional unavailability and,
in turn, to children’s adjustment (Sturge-Apple, Davies, and Cummings, 2006). Fathers’ emotional

93
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

unavailability is more consistently related to a range of child outcomes (internalizing, externalizing,


and school-related problems), whereas maternal unavailability is related only to scholastic problems.
Other longitudinal evidence supports this hypothesis and suggests greater complexity across time.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

A reverse pathway from parent-child to marital relationships was found for fathers, but not mothers,
which suggests that the father-child relationship may be more closely tied to broader family relation-
ships than the mother-child relationship (Schermerhorn, Cummings, and Davies, 2008). This bidi-
rectionality of influence between parenting and other aspects of family dynamics is not surprising
and is consistent with a general family systems view of fathering.
A number of factors aid in explaining the father vulnerability hypothesis. First, fathers’ level
of participation is, in part, determined by the extent to which the mother permits participation
(Allen and Hawkins, 1999; Beitel and Parke, 1998). Second, because the paternal role is less well
defined than the maternal role, spousal support may help crystallize the boundaries of appropriate
role behavior (Lamb and Lewis, 2010; Parke, 2013). Third, men have fewer opportunities to acquire
and practice caregiving skills during socialization and, therefore, may benefit more than mothers
from informational support (Parke and Brott, 1999).
Although the preponderance of evidence has focused on the negative effects of couple distress
on parent-child relationships and child adjustment, it is important to recognize that positive couple
relationships can have a protective effect on parenting and, in turn, child outcomes. In both cross-
sectional and longitudinal studies, support for this link has been found. Bonney et al. (1999), in a
cross-sectional study of couples with children ages 1–4, found that for fathers, higher marital satisfac-
tion was associated with more participation in common childcare activities. Lee and Doherty (2007)
followed couples from the second trimester of pregnancy to 6 and 12 months postpartum. Fathers’
marital satisfaction and involvement were positively related, and mothers’ employment status and
fathers’ attitudes toward father involvement were important moderators. Nor are the links evident in
only European American families. A study focused on the potential benefits of positive marital qual-
ity for children in working-class, first-generation Mexican American families (Leidy, Parke, Clavis,
Coltrane, and Duffy, 2009) examined the links between positive marital quality and child internaliz-
ing and externalizing behaviors 1 year later when the child was in sixth grade. Positive marital quality
was negatively correlated with child internalizing behaviors. Not all studies found couple satisfaction
negatively related to paternal involvement (Nangle, Kelley, Fals-Stewart, and Levant, 2003), so the
issue remains open. However, this body of work serves as an important corrective to the sole focus
on the negative aspects of couple relationships on fathering.

Changing Societal Conditions as Determinants


of Father-Child Relationships
A number of society-wide changes in the United States have produced a variety of shifts in the nature
of early family relationships. Fertility rates and family size have decreased, the percentage of women
in the workforce has increased, the timing of onset of parenthood has shifted, divorce rates have risen,
and the number of single-parent families has increased (for reviews, see Goldberg, 2014; Golombok,
2015; Parke, 2013). In this section, the effects of two of these changes—timing of parenthood and
recent shifts in family employment patterns—are explored to illustrate the impact of social change
on father-child and family relationships. Exploration of these shifts underscores an additional theme,
namely, the importance of considering the historical period or era in which social change occurs.

Timing of Parenthood and the Father’s Role


A number of factors need to be considered to understand the impact on parenting of childbearing
at different ages. First, the life course context, which is broadly defined as the point at which the

94
Fathers and Families

individual has arrived in his or her social, educational, and occupational timetable, is an important
determinant. Second, the historical context, namely, the societal and economic conditions that pre-
vail at the time of the onset of parenting, interacts with the first factor in determining the effects
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

of variations in timing. Consider early and delayed childbirth in light of these issues. Patterns of
the timing of the onset of parenting are changing for both mothers and fathers. In comparison to
1986–1987, when women were on average 22.7 years old and fathers were 25.3 years when they
became parents, in 2013, both women and men were about 2 years older (24.6 and 27.8, respectively)
when they made this transition (Eickmeyer, 2016). However, there is considerable variability, and
the age of entry into this role varies with marital status, ethnicity, and education (Eickmeyer, 2016).
Married fathers were older (29.4 years) than cohabitating fathers (26.7 years), who, in turn, were
older than single men (21.5 years). European American men became fathers at later ages (28.3 years)
than either African American men (26.5 years) or either native born (27.5) or foreign born (26.2)
Latin American men. As educational attainment increases, men become fathers at later ages. Men
who completed at least an undergraduate college degree were the oldest (30.9), and those without a
high school diploma were the youngest (23.9) to become fathers. Those men who completed high
school or attained a GED and those with some college entered fatherhood around 27 years of age.
At the same time, it is important to recognize that, as the overall shift toward later childbearing has
occurred, there has been a decline in births to teenage mothers over the last several decades. There
were approximately 60 births per 1,000 to teen women in 1991, but by 2014, this number dropped
to 24.3 per 1,000, which is a decline of about one third. In spite of the fact that teenage parenthood
is still higher in the United States than in many Western countries, this is a decline in part due to
delays in initiation of sexual activity and increased use of birth control (Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention, 2016). Moreover, most fathers or young mothers are, in fact, older, usually in their
20s or later rather than teenagers themselves. The focus of attention has shifted from teen or adoles-
cent parenthood to early parenthood more broadly conceived as a challenge of young or emerging
adulthood (Arnett, 2011). Movement through the educational system and into a stable position in
the workforce is a longer, slower, and more challenging process than in prior eras. “Transitions of
today’s young adults are both delayed and elongated: delayed because young adults take more time
to complete their education and enter the job market and elongated because each subsequent transi-
tion (marriage and family formation) takes longer to complete. Typical transitions to adulthood can
stretch from the late teens to early thirties” (Clarke, 2009, p. 14, cited by Leadbeater, 2014). Therefore
the issue of early versus late-timed onset of fathering needs to be reframed as an issue of financial and
educational readiness for the fathering role and more generally how poverty alters father roles. Edu-
cational level is positively linked with later onset of fathering (and mothering), which, in turn, alters
job and financial prospects. In this section, we examine recent work on men who become fathers
at a time before they are fully prepared for this role, and in a later section, we explore the effects of
delayed entry into fatherhood.

The Challenges of Entry Into Fatherhood Before Preparation


Men who become fathers in young adulthood are often unprepared financially and emotionally to
undertake the responsibilities of parenthood (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). As Lerman (1993,
p. 47) noted, “Young unwed fathers are generally less well educated, had lower academic abilities,
started sex at earlier ages and engaged in more crime than did other young men.” Low family income
and having lived in a welfare household increase the likelihood of entry into young unwed father-
hood. This profile is especially evident for European American unwed fathers. In spite of the fact
that African American males are four times as likely to be an unwed father as European American
males, African American unwed fatherhood is less likely to be linked with adverse circumstances
but is a more mainstream issue. Several factors reduce the likelihood of becoming a father earlier

95
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

than optimal including church attendance, military service, and higher reading scores. In view of the
low rates of marriage and high rates of separation and divorce for adolescents, early and less pre-
pared fathers, in contrast to “on-schedule” fathers, have less contact with their offspring. However,
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

contact is not absent; in fact, studies of unmarried adolescent fathers indicate a surprising amount
of paternal involvement for extended periods following the birth. Data (Lerman, 1993) based on
a national representative sample of over 600 young unwed fathers indicated that three-fourths of
young fathers who lived away from their children at birth never lived in the same household with
them. However, many unwed fathers remain in close contact with their children. According to Ler-
man’s (1993) analysis of the national survey data, nearly half visited their youngest child at least once
a week and nearly a quarter almost daily. Only 13% never visited, and 7% visited only yearly. There is
considerable instability in levels of father involvement with their children across time. In a sample of
low-income African American, unwed fathers, nearly 40% either increased or decreased their level
of involvement between the child’s birth and 3 years (Coley and Chase-Lansdale, 1999).
These declines in father participation continue across childhood and adolescence. Furstenberg
and Harris (1993) reported the pattern of contact between adolescent fathers and their offspring
from birth through late adolescence. Under half of the children lived at least some time with their
biological father at some time during their first 18 years, but only 9% lived with their father during
the entire period. During the preschool period, nearly half of the children were either living with
their father or saw him on a weekly basis. By late adolescence, 14% were living with him, and only
15% were seeing him as often as once a week; 46% had no contact, but 25% saw him occasionally
in the preceding year.
In contrast to early-timed childbearing, when childbearing is delayed, considerable progress in
occupational and educational spheres has potentially already taken place. Education is generally
completed and career development is well underway for both males and females. Delayed fathers
have described themselves to be in more stable work situations than early-timing fathers, to be more
experienced workers, and have their jobs and careers more firmly established than early-timing peers
(Daniels and Weingarten, 1982). The financial strains associated with early career status may be more
likely to create conflict between the work and family demands of early/normal-timing fathers than
delayed-timing fathers. Neville and Parke (1997) found some support for this proposition, but quali-
fied by the sex of the child. Specifically, younger fathers of girls and older fathers of boys reported
more interference by work in family life than older fathers of girls and younger fathers of boys.
What are the effects of late-timed parenthood for the father-child relationship? Retrospective
accounts by adults who were the firstborn children of older parents report having felt especially
appreciated by their parents (Yarrow, 1991). Daniels and Weingarten (1982) found late-timed fathers
are three times more likely to be involved in the daily care of a preschool child. Cooney, Pedersen,
Indelicato, and Palkovitz (1993) found that late-timed fathers were more likely to be classified as
being highly involved and experiencing positive affect associated with the paternal role than “on-
time” fathers. Finally, men who delayed parenthood until their late 20s contributed more to indirect
aspects of childcare, such as cooking, feeding, cleaning, and doing laundry than men who assumed
parenthood earlier (Coltrane and Ishii-Kuntz, 1991).
There are qualitative differences in styles of interaction for on-time versus late-timed fathers.
In a self-report study, MacDonald and Parke (1986) found that age of parent is negatively related
to the frequency of physical play, especially physical activities (i.e., bounce, tickle, chase, and pig-
gyback), that require more physical energy on the part of the play partner. Moreover, Neville and
Parke (1997) found older parents likely to engage in more cognitively advanced activities with
children and to report holding their children more than younger fathers. These and other studies
(Zaslow, Pedersen, Suwalsky, Rabinovich, and Cain, 1985) suggest that older fathers may be less tied
to stereotypic paternal behavior, adopting styles more similar to those that have been considered
traditionally maternal.

96
Fathers and Families

Observational studies of father-child interaction confirm these early self-report investigations.


Volling and Belsky (1991), who studied fathers and their 3- and 9-month-olds, found that older
fathers were more responsive, stimulating, and affectionate. Neville and Parke (1997) found that early
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

fathers relied on physical arousal to engage their preschoolers, whereas delayed fathers relied on more
cognitive mechanisms to remain engaged. Waiting too long to become a parent has a downside not
only for women but also for men. As men age, they produce lower quality sperm that increases the
risk of birth defects (Wyrobek et al., 2006). Men as well as women have ticking biological clocks.
Timing effects are important not just for fathers, but also for grandfathers as well. In their study
of grandfathers interacting with their 7-month-old grandchildren, Tinsley and Parke (1987) divided
grandfathers into three categories: Younger (36–49 years), middle (50–56), and older (57–68).
Grandfathers in the middle age group were rated significantly higher on competence (e.g., confident,
accepting), affect (e.g., warm, attentive), and play style (e.g., playful, responsive, stimulatory). From
a life-span developmental perspective, the middle group of grandfathers could be viewed as being
optimally ready for grandparenthood, both physically and psychologically (Tinsley and Parke, 1984)
because they were less likely to be chronically tired or to have been ill or still be heavily involved in
their work activities. Moreover, the age of the middle group of grandfathers fits the normative or
age-appropriate time at which grandparenthood is most often achieved.

Women’s and Men’s Employment Patterns and Their Parental Roles


in the Family
Relations between employment patterns of both women and men and their family roles are increas-
ingly being recognized (Coltrane and Adams, 2008). In this section, a variety of issues concerning the
links between the worlds of work and family is considered to illustrate the impact of shifts in work
patterns on both men’s and women’s family roles. The impact of changes in the rate of maternal
employment on both quantitative and qualitative aspects of father participation is examined as well
as the influence of variations in family work schedules.
Since the mid-1960s, there has been a dramatic shift in the participation rate of women in the
labor force. Between 1960 and 2016, the employment rate for mothers of children under age 6
increased dramatically from about 20% to 65% and among mothers of children between 6 and 17, it
is even higher (75%) (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2017). In exploring these work trends, two issues are
relevant. First, does father involvement shift with increases in maternal employment? Second, how
does the quality of work alter maternal and paternal parenting behavior?
Maternal employment is a robust predictor of paternal involvement which, as noted earlier, has
increased considerably over the last half century (Pleck, 2010). However, we are clearly not at 50/50
yet. Working women decrease their time devoted to housework, but they still spend time on child-
care, especially in the case of children under 6 (Bianchi, 2009). Women who do not work outside
the home continue to do a significantly larger share of childcare than their husbands (Coltrane and
Adams, 2008).
In addition to documenting the quantitative shifts in father behavior as a consequence of maternal
employment, it is also necessary to examine the impact of this shift on the quality of the father-child
relationship. Pedersen, Anderson, and Cain (1980) assessed the impact of dual wage-earner families
on mother and father interaction patterns with their 5-month-old infants. Fathers in single wage-
earner families tended to play with their infants more than mothers did, but in the dual wage-earner
families, the mothers’ rate of social play was higher than the fathers’ rate of play. Perhaps mothers
used increased play as a way of reestablishing contact with the infant after being away from home
for the day: “It is possible that the working mother’s special need to interact with the infant inhib-
ited or crowded out the father in his specialty” (Pedersen et al., 1980, p. 10). However, as studies of
reversed role families suggest, the physical style of fathers play remains evident (Field, 1978). These

97
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

data suggest that both mothers and fathers may exhibit distinctive play styles, even when family role
arrangements modify the quantity of their interaction. However, the father-infant relationship is
altered as a result of maternal employment. Specifically, insecure infant-father attachment is higher
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

in dual-career families, although only for sons and not daughters (Belsky, Rovine, and Fish, 1989).
Other evidence suggests that fathers in dual-earner families are less sensitive with their male infants,
and this pattern may explain the more insecure infant-father attachments (Braungart-Rieker, Court-
ney, and Garwood, 1999). Grych and Clark (1999) reported similar findings, namely, that in families
where mothers work full time (defined as 25 hours or more), fathers were more negative in interact-
ing with their infants at 4 months, although the relation was not evident at 12 months.

Father’s Work Quantity and Quality and Father Involvement


Although there has been an increase in the number of parents who are employed, many workers
experienced increases in work hours, a decrease in job stability, a rise in temporary jobs, and especially
among low-wage workers, a decrease in income (Williams, 2010). As a result of these changes, the
theoretical questions have shifted. Instead of examining whether one or both parents are employed,
researchers have begun to address the issue of the impact of the quality and nature of work on the
parenting of both mothers and fathers (see Cho and Ciancetta, 2016, for a review). Both how much
and when parents work matter for children. As studies of poor nonresidential fathers indicate, lack of
employment opportunities and, therefore, financial ability to support children is a major determinant
of lack of father involvement (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010). Men who provide either informal
or formal financial support are more likely to maintain contact with their children, whereas fathers
who rarely or never visit are less likely to pay child support (Lerman, 1993; Lerman and Sorensen,
2000) which, in turn, adds to mothers’ financial burden and may indirectly have negative effects on
the children. The direction of effects is still being debated; perhaps fathers who can provide financial
support are more likely to stay involved as a parent, or alternatively, those who stay involved are more
likely to seek employment to fulfill their role as provider (Carlson and McLanahan, 2010; Coley and
Chase-Lansdale, 1999).
Parental work schedules can be stressful and are associated with negative outcomes for children
and adults (Li et al., 2013; Williams, 2010). In the 24-hour economy, 40% of employed individuals
in the United States work part of their time outside normal working hours or what Europeans call
“unsocial hours.” In 51% of two-job families with children, at least one parent works nonstand-
ard hours, with the evening shift being the most common (Deutsch, 1999; Gornick and Meyers,
2003). As Williams (2010) observed, this trend toward nonstandard schedules takes its toll on family
relationships. People working “unsocial hours,” not surprisingly, tend to have strained relationships.
These schedules are associated with higher family-work conflict, lower marital quality, and reduced
time with children. They are also associated with a lower likelihood of eating meals together, provid-
ing homework supervision, and sharing leisure (Han and Fox, 2011; Williams, 2010). In a Canadian
study, children had more behavioral problems when parents worked nonstandard hours (Strazdins,
Korda, Lim, Broom, and D’Souza, 2004). Nor are marriages immune from the effects of work sched-
ules; nonoverlapping work hours for husbands and wives had negative effects on marital relationships
(Perry-Jenkins, Repetti, and Crouter, 2000) and may increase the chances of divorce (Presser, 2004).
And mothers’ household workload goes up more than fathers’ with nonstandard job schedules (Craig
and Powell, 2011). Although such work schedules are often undertaken out of economic necessity,
they still are a hardship for parents and children. Finally, job loss and underemployment have serious
effects on family life, including marital relationships, parent-child relationships, and child adjustment
(Conger et al., 2002; Parke et al., 2004; White and Rogers, 2000).
In terms of the quality issue, there are two types of linkage between father work and fathering.
One type of research focuses on work as an “emotional climate” which, in turn, may have short-term

98
Fathers and Families

spillover effects to the enactment of roles in home settings. A second type of linkage focuses on the
type of skills, attitudes, and perspectives that adults acquire in their work-based socialization as adults
and how these variations in job experience alter their behavior in family contexts on a longer term
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

basis.
Work in the first tradition (Repetti, 1994, 2005) has shown that fathers in high-stress jobs (e.g.,
air traffic controller) were more withdrawn in marital interactions after high-stress shifts and behav-
iorally and emotionally withdrawn during father-child interactions. Distressing social experiences at
work were associated with higher expressions of anger and greater use of discipline during interac-
tion with the child later in the day. Similarly Crouter, Bumpus, Maguire, and McHale (1999) found
that mothers and fathers who felt more pressure on the job reported greater role overload, which,
in turn, was linked to heightened parent-adolescent conflict (see Repetti, 2005, for a review). Posi-
tive work experiences can enhance the quality of fathering. Grossman, Pollack, and Golding (1988)
found that high job satisfaction was associated with higher levels of support for 5-year-old children’s
autonomy and affiliation in spite of the fact that positive feelings about work were negatively related
to the quantity of time spent interacting with the child. This finding underscores the importance of
distinguishing quantity and quality of involvement. Finally, when parents have better work-family
balance, children’s mental health is better (Repetti, 2005). This line of research underscores the
importance of distinguishing between different types of work-related stress on subsequent father-
child interactions and of considering direct short-term carryover effects versus long-term effects of
work on fathering.
Research in the second tradition of family-work linkage, namely, the effects of the nature of
men’s occupational roles on their fathering behavior, dates back to Miller and Swanson (1958) and
Kohn and Schooler (1983). Men who experience a high degree of occupational autonomy value
independence in their children, consider children’s intentions in discipline, and use reasoning and
withdrawal of rewards instead of physical punishment. In contrast, men who are in highly supervised
jobs with little autonomy value conformity and obedience, focus on consequences rather than inten-
tions, and use more physical forms of discipline. In short, they repeat their job-based experiences in
their parenting roles.
In extensions of Kohn’s original focus on the implications of job characteristics for parenting,
scholars have explored the effects of these variations in parenting for children’s development. Fathers
with more complex jobs (i.e., characterized by mentoring others versus taking instruction or serving
others) spend more time alone with sons, more time developing their sons’ skills (e.g., academic, ath-
letic, mechanical, interpersonal), behave more warmly and responsively, and use less harsh and less lax
control, but this is not the case for daughters (Greenberger, O’Neil, and Nagel, 1994). In fact, fathers
spend more time in work and work-related activities if they have daughters, but report more firm
but flexible control with daughters. Fathers who have jobs characterized by a high level of challenge
(e.g., expected to solve problems, high level of decision-making) devote more time to developing
sons’ skills, give higher quality explanations to their sons, and use less harsh and more firm but flexible
control in their interactions with their boys. Finally, fathers with time-urgent jobs (e.g., fast pace, few
breaks) spend more time on work activities, less time interacting, and use less lax control if they have
daughters. When fathers have complex, stimulating, and challenging jobs, boys seem to benefit much
more than girls. Grimm-Thomas and Perry-Jenkins (1994) found that fathers with greater complex-
ity and autonomy at work reported higher self-esteem and less authoritarian parenting. In a sample of
European American, rural, dual-earner couples, Whitbeck et al. (1997) found that fathers with more
job autonomy had more flexible parenting styles, which were linked to a sense of mastery and control
in their adolescents. In another rural sample, poor job characteristics (e.g., low autonomy and flex-
ibility, high pressure and supervisor criticism) were linked with lower levels of father engagement and
less sensitive parenting (Goodman, Crouter, Lanza, and Cox, 2008) and lower quality of parent-infant
interaction (Goodman et al., 2011). Some evidence suggests that these job characteristics effects may

99
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

be more evident for fathers than mothers (Parcel and Menaghan, 1994; Whitbeck et al., 1997). Per-
haps mothers show fewer links due to the more heavily culturally- or evolutionarily-scripted nature
of maternal roles with the focus on caregiving. However, as women’s involvement in the workplace
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

continues, the effects of workplace characteristics on women may increase.


Although these myriad factors have been discussed individually, as our earlier theoretical model
suggests and as empirical evidence supports, the challenge for the future is to test the additive and
interactive effects of these individual factors in comprehensive models so that the complexity of
predictive patterns of determinants of father involvement can be better understood.

Consequences of Father-Child Relationships


Variations in father involvement have implications for men themselves, their families, and their
children.

Consequences of Fatherhood for Men Themselves


Becoming a father impacts on a man’s own psychological development and well-being. As Parke
(1981) noted, “The father-child relationship is a two-way process and children influence their fathers
just as fathers alter their children’s development”(p. 9). Several aspects of this issue have been exam-
ined, namely (1) mother-father relationships, (2) work and occupational issues, (3) self-identity, (4)
psychological adjustment, (5) physical health, and (6) social connectedness beyond the family.

Impact on Mother-Father Relationships


Perhaps most attention has been devoted to the impact of the transition to parenthood on marriage.
The general finding from a large number of studies is that there is a decline in marital satisfaction,
especially on the part of men, as a consequence of the birth of a child (see Mitnick, Heyman, and
Smith Slep, 2009, for a meta-analytic review). The psychological adjustments associated with the
transition to fatherhood are clearly evident in the Cowan and Cowan (1992) longitudinal study.
Their project followed families from pregnancy until the children were 5 years of age. Fathers’ mari-
tal satisfaction showed a modest decrease from pregnancy to 6 months, but a sharp decline between
6 and 18 months. In contrast, mothers show a linear decline beginning in the postpartum period
and continuing across the first 2 years. In this same period of 18 months, 12.5% of the couples sepa-
rated or divorced; by 5 years of age, this figure was up to 20%. Another study found lower marital
satisfaction at 1 year postpartum than at 3 months (Shapiro, Gottman, and Carrere, 2000), a finding
confirmed by a study of first-time fathers that found that marital satisfaction was lowest at 1 year
post-birth (Condon, Boyce, and Corkindale, 2004). A more recent longitudinal study (Doss et al.,
2009) compared couples who did and did not have children across 8 years of marriage. Both groups
showed a decline, but in the case of the couples with children, there was a sharp drop-off in marital
satisfaction after the onset of parenthood, whereas the decline was more gradual in childless couples.
Perhaps most persuasive is the recent meta-analysis of 37 studies that followed couples from before
to after birth (Mitnick et al., 2009). There were small-to-moderate effect sizes indicating a decline in
marital satisfaction from pregnancy through 11–14 months after birth for men and women.
In spite of the dip in marital satisfaction, two caveats should be noted. First, even though marital
satisfaction decreases for men (and women) after the onset of parenthood, marital stability (i.e., the
likelihood of staying in the marriage) increases relative to childless couples (Cowan and Cowan,
1992). Second, not all of the couples showed a decline in marital satisfaction; 18% of couples showed
increased satisfaction with their marital relationship. This figure rose to 38% for couples who partici-
pated in a supportive intervention program during the transition to parenthood (Cowan and Cowan,

100
Fathers and Families

1992). During the transition to parenthood, marital quality declined in about 30% of the families,
improved in another 30%, and in nearly 40% of the families showed no change (Belsky, Rovine, and
Fish, 1989). Similar diversity in the pattern of change in marital satisfaction is evident in other studies
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

(McDermid, Huston, and McHale, 1990; Shapiro et al., 2000)


A variety of reasons has been suggested for this decline in men’s marital satisfaction, including
(1) physical strain of childcare, (2) increased financial responsibilities, (3) emotional demands of
new familial responsibilities, (4) the restrictions of parenthood, and (5) the redefinition of roles
and role arrangements (Goldberg, 2014; Mitnick et al., 2009). Several lines of evidence suggest that
discrepancies in expectations on the part of mothers and fathers concerning the relative roles that
each will play may be an important determinant of postpartum marital satisfaction. When there
was a larger discrepancy between the wives’ expectations of their husbands’ involvement in infant
care and husbands’ level of actual participation, there was a greater decline in marital satisfaction
(Cowan and Cowan, 1992). Similarly, McDermid et al. (1990) found greater negative impact of
the onset of parenthood when there was a discrepancy between spouses’ sex role attitudes and the
division of household and childcare labor, and McBride (1989) found that traditional fathers who
held conservative sex-role attitudes, but were nonetheless involved in childcare, reported lower
levels of dissatisfaction. On the positive side, when couple’s expectations and behaviors match,
some evidence suggests that marital satisfaction is correspondingly high (Cowan and Cowan,
1992). In summary, discrepancies in parental expectations about roles, rather than the level of
change per se, may be a key correlate of men’s marital satisfaction after the onset of fatherhood. It
is important to underscore that marital relationships are both determinants as well as consequences
of paternal involvement (Pleck, 2010).
Much of the literature is focused on infancy. Less is known about the impact of being a father on
marital satisfaction after infancy. An exception is the longitudinal study by Heath (1976; Heath and
Heath, 1991) that followed a cohort of college men into their 30s and mid-40s. Competent fathers were
in satisfying marriages. However, these two indices also related to psychological maturity, leaving open
the possibility that fathering activities lead to marital satisfaction and maturity or that maturity is the
common correlate of being both a competent father and husband. Snarey (1993) found support for the
relation between paternal involvement in childhood or adolescence on marital satisfaction. In a follow-up
longitudinal study, Snarey assessed the marital success of men at mid-life (age 47):

Fathers who provided high levels of social-emotional support for their offspring during
the childhood decade (0–10 years) and high levels of intellectual, academic and social
emotional support during the adolescent decade (11–21 years) were themselves as men at
mid-life, more likely to be happily married.
(Snarey, 1993, p. 111)

In summary, it is clear that there are hints of long-term positive effects of father involvement on
marriage, but these data must be interpreted cautiously for several reasons. First, there are negative
effects of increased father involvement as noted earlier in our discussions of maternal gatekeeping.
Some women may view increased father involvement as intrusive and unwelcome. Second, some of
the literature is based on cohorts studied several decades ago. In light of the changing work and fam-
ily lives of both men and women in the new millennium, conclusions based on earlier periods may
not be readily applicable (Pleck, 2010).

Fatherhood and Men’s Work Patterns


There are two perspectives on this issue. First, a short-term perspective suggests that as fathers increase
their involvement, they perceive higher levels of work-family conflict (Grzywacz and Carlson, 2007).

101
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

This work-family stress is more likely to be reported by fathers in dual- rather than single-earner
families (Volling and Belsky, 1991). Many fathers wish they had more time for family and more
flexible job arrangements (Parke and Brott, 1999), and, although there are clear trends toward more
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

family-friendly policies, workplace barriers remain formidable A long term perspective suggests that
father involvement is positively linked with occupational mobility, wage levels, and asset accumula-
tion. Recent evidence suggests that

the links between fatherhood and men’s work behaviors are a complex function of many
factors including: human capital, marital status (non) resident status with children, age at
the transition to fatherhood, race-ethnicity, desires and abilities to embrace “new” father
ideals and selection effects.
(Eggebeen, Knoester, and McDaniel, 2013, p. 349)

Men who become fathers work longer hours, are more likely to be employed, receive higher wages
and have more assets than men who are childless (Eggebeen, Dew, and Knoester, 2010; Hodges and
Budig, 2010). However, married professional fathers, especially European American men, benefit
more than others in terms of earnings and asset accumulation in part due to institutionalized dis-
criminatory practices that penalize other groups of men such as African American fathers (Eggebeen
et al., 2010; Glauber, 2008). Moreover, married professional European American men may be mar-
ried to partners who assume a greater domestic burden, which allows men to specialize in outside
employment. Young fathers, although their wages and assets are lower, often increase their commit-
ment to work by increasing their employment hours in part due to the recognition that the onset
of fatherhood requires greater responsibility including a heightened commitment to work (Astone,
Dariotis, Sonensteon, Pleck, and Hynes, 2010; Settersten and Cancel-Tirado, 2010).
Finally, occupational mobility is also affected by father involvement. In his longitudinal study,
Snarey (1993) found that fathers’ childrearing involvement across the first two decades of the child’s
life moderately predicted fathers’ occupational mobility (at age 47) above and beyond other back-
ground variables (e.g., parents’ occupation, his IQ, current maternal employment). In general, father-
hood and work are linked, but the patterns clearly vary across age, level of professionalism, and
ethnicity. It is also evident that fathering practices and work involvement may mutually influence
one another.

Fatherhood and Men’s Self-Identity


Men’s sense of themselves shifts as a function of the transition to fatherhood. A variety of dimen-
sions has been explored in prior research, including their role definitions, their self-esteem, and their
sense of generativity. Roles change for both men and women after the onset of parenthood. Cowan
and Cowan (1992, 2010) assessed role shifts during the transition to fatherhood and found that men
who become fathers decreased the “partner/lover” aspect of their self and increased the “parent” per-
centage of their self-definition. In contrast, men who remained childless significantly increased the
“partner/lover” aspect of their relationship over a nearly 2-year assessment period. Grossman (1987)
found that first-time fathers who were both more affiliative (i.e., connected to others, empathetic
relationships) and more autonomous had significantly higher life adaptation scores. Fathers of first-
borns who were more affiliative at 1 year also reported being higher in emotional well-being than
less affiliative fathers. Thus, it appears that “separateness and individuation are not sufficient for men’s
well-being; they need connections as well” (Grossman, 1987, p. 107). Does fatherhood have a longer
term impact on men’s self-identity? In a longitudinal study of college men, Heath (1977) found that
fatherhood related to men’s ability to understand themselves, to understand others sympathetically,
and to integrate their own feelings.

102
Fathers and Families

Fatherhood and Psychological Adjustment of Men


Becoming a father is linked with both positive and negative psychological consequences for men
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

themselves (Eggebeen et al., 2013). As Palkovitz (2002) reported in his qualitative examination
of fathers, men report joy, anticipation, and excitement about being a father while also reporting
increased stress, anxiety, frustration, depression and, as we noted earlier, marital conflict. Married
or cohabitating men are less likely than single men to experience depression on becoming a father
(Woo and Raley, 2005). Stepfathers may report more stress and lower levels of life satisfaction (Kohler,
Behrman, and Skytthe, 2005). With longer duration of the stepfamily relationship, the negative effects
increased stress, and altered perceptions of life satisfaction decrease (Umberson and Montez, 2010).
The early-timed entry into fatherhood has clear negative effects on men’s psychological adjust-
ment. A variety of disruptions in their lives associated with early fatherhood, such as interruptions
in education, increased financial and social responsibilities, and unstable partner relationships, all
contribute to lower levels of life satisfaction for young fathers (Hofferth and Goldscheider, 2010). In
contrast, on-time and late-timed fathers are generally better adjusted psychologically, although later
fatherhood may be complicated by divorce and remarriage (Setttersten and Cancel-Tirado, 2010).
Some ethnicity-related effects have been found as well, with African American fathers reporting
higher rates of depression than European American fathers (Gross et al., 2008). And fathers (and
mothers) are often negatively affected (i.e., increases in depression) when adult children encounter
stressful events such as job loss, financial problems, illness, or divorce (Pudrovska, 2009).

Fatherhood and Physical Health


As in the case of psychological adjustment, the onset of fatherhood can have both positive and nega-
tive effects on men’s physical well-being but depends on the life stage, residential and marital status,
and income level. Becoming a father may improve health by reducing risky behavior in part to pro-
vide a better model for children and to maintain their health to fulfill their parental role (Palkovitz,
2002). The majority of fathers attempt to curtail or quit smoking (Everett et al., 2005), and some
have found that alcohol consumption decreases (Garfield, Isacco, and Bartlo, 2010). Even in the case
of divorced fathers, who are likely to be stressed and potentially use excessive alcohol, those who
are involved extensively in their children’s lives decrease alcohol and marijuana use (DeGarmo,
2010). Rearing a difficult child or encountering other life stressors, such as financial challenges, may
increase alcohol consumption (Pelham and Lang, 1999). Or transitioning into fatherhood at a young
age may lead to a continuation of risky behavior or patterns of drug abuse (Little, Handley, Leuthe,
and Chassin, 2009), limited job opportunities, and more poverty, all of which may contribute to
poorer long-term health outcomes (Williams, 2003). Even lower mortality is linked with fatherhood,
especially in the context of marriage (Friedman and Martin, 2011) but decreases after the number of
children increases (Lawlor et al., 2003).

Fatherhood and Social Connections Beyond the Family


Becoming a father has consequences for the roles that men play beyond the family in the wider
community. This effect of fatherhood is reflected in increased generativity, a concept derived from
Erikson’s (1975) theoretical writings. Snarey (1993, pp. 18–19) provided a succinct summary:

The psychosocial task of middle adulthood, Stage 7 [in Erikson’s Stage theory] is the
attainment of a favorable balance of generativity over stagnation and self-absorption. Most
broadly, Erikson (1975) considers generativity to mean any caring activity that contributes
to the spirit of future generations, such as the generation of new or more mature persons,

103
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

products, ideas, or works of art. Generativity’s psychosocial challenge to adults is to create,


care for, and promote the development of others from nurturing the growth of another
person to shepherding the development of a broader community.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Snarey (1993) described three types that apply to fathers, namely (1) biological generativity (indi-
cated by the birth of a child), (2) parental generativity (indicated by childrearing activities), and
(3) societal generativity (indicated by caring for other younger adults, such as serving as a mentor,
providing leadership, and contributing to generational continuity). The concept of generativity is a
useful marker for assessing the long-term relation between fathering behavior and other aspects of
mature men’s lives. It also serves as a theoretical corrective to earlier views of fathers as inadequate
and deficient (Hawkins and Dollahite, 1997; Palkovitz and Palm, 2009). The onset of fatherhood is
linked with a strengthening of intergenerational ties (Eggebeen et al., 2010) as illustrated by more
contact (letters, phone calls) with parents and relatives than nonfathers.
There are more instrumental exchanges, such as giving and receiving monetary, social and car-
egiving assistance, by fathers as well. A series of studies has examined relations between fatherhood
and community-based social generativity. Fathers compared with non-fathers are more involved in
community service and volunteer activities not only with child-related activities, such as team sports
or girl or boy scout groups, but also in a wide variety of community organizations (labor unions, pro-
fessional societies, or local/national political groups; Eggebeen et al., 2013; Knoester and Eggebeen,
2006). Snarey found that men who nurtured their children’s socioemotional development during
childhood (0–10 years) and who also contributed to both socioemotional and intellectual-academic
development during the second decade (11–21 years) were at mid-life more likely to become gen-
erative in areas outside their family. In summary, although the processes are not yet well understood,
it is clear that involved fathering relates in positive ways to other aspects of men’s lives. As Snarey
(1993, p. 119) noted, “Men who are parentally generative during early adulthood usually turn out
to be good spouses, workers and citizens at mid life.” Although further empirical work is necessary
to adequately evaluate the utility of this generativity perspective, it represents a promising direction
for fatherhood research.
Palkovitz (2002; Palkovitz and Palm, 1998) argued that engagement in fatherhood roles may
present a sensitive period for men in the development of religious faith and in religious practice.
In support of this expectation, men become more religious and attend religious institutions more
often after the onset of fatherhood (Knoester, Petts, and Eggebeen, 2007). This embrace of religion
is important because religiousness (faith and practice) is linked not only with paternal and maternal
warmth and parenting efficacy but also with increases in parental control. In turn, warmth and par-
enting efficacy are linked with higher social competence and school performance in children while
parental control is related to increases in internalizing and externalizing (Bornstein et al., 2017).
However, the conditions under which religiousness will yield positive or negative child outcomes
remains unclear. The effects are evident across nine countries and religions (Protestant, Catholic,
Buddhist, Muslim) and underscore the importance of religion as a determinant of fathering.

Implications of Father Involvement for Children’s Development


Three types of approaches to the issue of the impact of father involvement on children’s social,
emotional, and cognitive development can be distinguished. First, in a modern variant of the earlier
father-absence theme, sociologists, in particular, have examined the impact of nonresident fathers’
frequency and quality of contact on children’s development. In contrast to this paternal deprivation
approach, a second strategy examines the impact of paternal enhancement. This approach asks about
the lessons learned from focusing on unusually highly involved fathers, such as occurs in role sharing

104
Fathers and Families

and reversed role families. The third or normative approach focuses on the consequences of the qual-
ity and quantity of father-child interaction on children’s development in intact families.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Beyond Intact Father Families: Effects on Children’s Development

Disadvantaged Fathers and Child Development


What are the effects of contact between disadvantaged fathers and children on children’s develop-
ment? It is noteworthy that unmarried fathers continue to see their children even though the ties
with their partner have deteriorated. Some have suggested that the package deal whereby close ties
between partners and involvement in child contact is a guiding principle may not apply to unmar-
ried fathers who see contact with children and a relationship with the child’s mother as separate
components (Edin and Nelson, 2013; Edin, Tach, and Mincy, 2009). However, the selective invest-
ment of many poor fathers in a subset of their biological children may leave the nonselected biologi-
cal children at a developmental disadvantage. Not only do these nonselected children receive less
contact with their biological fathers, but also they experience the added stress of multiple transitions
in their lives, as their mothers may form new partnerships and, in turn, have more children. As Edin
and Nelson (2013) noted,

Children may be exposed to the confusion of new parental figures who come and go, and
they will likely accumulate half siblings along the way. Kids are resilient but the rate of fam-
ily change among children of unwed fathers has become so rapid and now leads to such
complicated family structures that kids might have a hard time adjusting.
(p. 226)

Although child outcomes were not the focus of the Edin-Nelson project, other studies have found
that the higher the rate of family transitions in and out of coresidential unions or significant roman-
tic relationships, the more behavior problems exhibited by the children (Fomby and Cherlin, 2007;
Osborne and McLanahan, 2007) and more problems with peers (Cavanaugh and Huston, 2006).
However, as Furstenberg (2014) cautioned, “The research challenge is sorting out the selection of
couples with very different demographic and psychological attributes from the social process gen-
erated when couples encounter the challenges of supporting and raising children across different
households” (p. 22).
Moreover, quality, not presence/absence alone, is important in assessing the impact of nonresi-
dent fathers. In a follow-up study of 18- to 21-year-old children of African American adolescent
mothers, Furstenberg and Harris (1993) found little impact of contact alone on young adults’ out-
comes but clear beneficial effects if the quality of the relationship were taken into account. Those
who reported a strong bond or attachment with their father during adolescence had higher edu-
cational attainment, were less likely to be imprisoned, and were less depressed. These effects were
especially evident in the case of children living with the father and were only marginally evident for
nonresident biological fathers. The data suggest that both presence and quality matters, but quality
is especially important because fathers’ presence is unrelated to outcomes when quality (degree of
attachment to father) is controlled. Others report that paternal nurturing behaviors are related with
better cognitive and behavioral outcomes (Martin et al., 2007; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004), and
involvement by nonresidential fathers is related to lower rates of delinquency (Coley and Medeiros,
2007). Additionally, more father engagement in shared activities during adolescence is linked to more
healthy cortisol responses to stressful events (Ibrahim, Somers, Luecken, Fabricius, and Cookston,
2017). The Amato and Gilbreth (1999) meta-analysis is consistent with findings; a measure of the

105
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

affective relationship between the father and child (feeling close) was positively associated with child
academic success, and negatively with child internalizing and externalizing problems. The effect
sizes were modest in magnitude. Second, fathers’ authoritative parenting was associated with higher
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

academic success and fewer internalizing and externalizing problems. Authoritative parenting was
a better predictor than either frequency of contact or the “feeling close” measure. In addition to
quality of involvement, one other variable, namely, the amount of father’s payment of child sup-
port, was a significant predictor of children’s outcomes, including academic success and children’s
externalizing behavior although not internalizing behavior. This finding is not surprising, because
“fathers’ financial contributions provide wholesome food, adequate shelter in safe neighborhoods,
commodities (such as books, computers, and private lessons) that facilitate children’s academic suc-
cess and support for college attendance” (Amato and Gilbreth, 1999, p. 559). These findings were
evident for both boys and girls and African American and European American families. Finally, in
the Fragile Families project, few links have been found between father contact and child outcomes,
but if the parents have an effective coparenting relationship, children have fewer behavior problems
(Carlson and McLanahan, 2010) This work reflects earlier and recurring themes in the parent-child
literature, namely, that quality of the father-child and couple relationship are the critical factors in
determining children’s development.

Incarcerated Fathers and Children’s Development


What are the effects of paternal incarceration on children? Murray, Farrington, and Sekol (2012)
examined evidence on the associations between parental incarceration and children’s later antisocial
behavior, mental health problems, drug use, and educational performance. In their meta-analysis,
the most rigorous studies that controlled for sociodemographic risk factors (e.g., ethnicity, educa-
tion, poverty), children’s antisocial behavior before parental incarceration, or parental criminality
(e.g., prior criminal convictions), they found that parental incarceration was associated with chil-
dren’s increased risk for antisocial behavior, but not for mental health problems, drug use, or poor
educational performance. Although previous studies have found multiple types of adverse effects of
parental incarceration on children’s outcomes, results suggest that when important co-occurring risk
factors are taken into account, only children’s antisocial behavior is uniquely affected by parental
incarceration: “Although incarceration is likely not the cause of these compromised outcomes, it
instead serves as one indicator of other co-occurring risks and vulnerabilities that make these fami-
lies particularly fragile” (Shlafer, Gerrity, Ruhland, Wheeler, and Michaels, 2013, p. 9). For exam-
ple, incarceration increases financial insecurity and housing instability, heightens stigmatization, and
increases the probability that a non-incarcerated mother will repartner (Sykes and Pettit, 2014).
Together, these shifts will have negative effects on children, including an increase in homelessness
(Wildeman, 2014) and elevated food insecurity (Cox and Wallace, 2013).
Most evidence suggests that the parent-child attachment relationship is more likely to be nega-
tively affected when the mother is incarcerated than when fathers are incarcerated because mothers
are more likely to be the primary caregiver than fathers prior to incarceration. Only a quarter of
fathers compared with three quarters of mothers reported providing primary care for their children
prior to arrest (Maruschak, Glaze, and Mumola, 2010). However, the conditions surrounding the
father’s incarceration are linked to the non-incarcerated caregiver-child relationship. Although no
studies have directly examined the effects of paternal incarceration on children’s attachment to their
fathers, paternal imprisonment is linked with poorer caregiver (mother)-child attachment, especially
if the child is exposed to either their father’s crime or the arrest of their father. However, if the
maternal caregiver was sensitive and provided a stimulating home environment, the levels of insecure
attachment between child and the non-incarcerated caregiver were reduced (Poehlmann-Tynan,
Burnson, Runyon, and Weymouth, 2017).

106
Fathers and Families

An examination of how the contact patterns are managed during incarceration suggests a com-
plex picture. On the one hand, Shlafer and Poehlmann (2010) found that no parent-child contact
was associated with children’s feelings of alienation; on the other hand, visitation may not always be
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

associated with positive outcomes. For example, there are associations between visits with parents
(mothers) in corrections facilities and representations of insecure attachment relationships in chil-
dren (Poehlmann, 2005). Children who experienced more frequent no contact barrier visits with
their mothers in the jail setting exhibited more depression and anxiety symptoms, whereas the use
of alternative forms of contact (mail, phone) was associated with fewer symptoms of internalizing
behavior (Dallaire, Zeman, and Thrash, 2015). The child-unfriendly nature of visitation environ-
ments probably contributed to this link. As observations of children during visits with their jailed
fathers (and mothers) indicate, children exhibit more emotional dysregulation in jail settings than
at home (Poehlmann-Tynan et al., 2014). However, when the visitation setting is more child- and
parent-friendly (direct physical contact between parent and child), visitation is associated with more
positive outcomes for children (Snyder, Carlo, and Coats Mullins, 2001). In one small-scale interven-
tion (weekly parent-child visit in a child-friendly setting) for incarcerated fathers and their young
children, Landreth and Lobaugh (1998) found that children’s self-esteem increased across a 10-week
intervention. Moreover, a longer term view suggests that the post-incarceration period needs to
be examined. Some report more visitation and mail contact during incarceration was linked with
more father-child contact and better father-child relationships and child outcomes in the post-­
incarceration period (LaVigne, Naser, Brooks, and Castro, 2005) as well as lower recidivism rates for
inmates (Bales and Mears, 2008). In summary, the context and type of contact are important modera-
tors of these links between contact and outcomes.

Transnational Fathers and Children’s Development


Some evidence suggests that the frequency of contact is an important factor in child outcomes for
transnational fathers. For example, Nobles (2011) found a positive association between the amount
of father-child contact and school performance for both sons and daughters. In an internet survey,
Cookston, Boyer, Vega, and Parke (2017) found a positive relation between frequency of telephone
contact between transnational Mexican parents and their children and the quality of parent-child
relationships. How is the quality of the father-child relationship modified by the frequency and by
the quality of these contacts? How are contact patterns related to children’s perceptions of their
father? Clearly, this topic needs more attention by father researchers, especially in view of the con-
tinuing increase in transnational parenting.

Deployed Fathers and Children’s Development


Assessments of the impact of being separated from their families and children due to military deploy-
ment is a further avenue for exploring the effects of fathering on children’s development. A variety
of negative outcomes on children of deployed parents (usually fathers) has been reported. In one
study (Chandra et al., 2010) after controlling for family and service-member characteristics, children
(11–17 years old) had more emotional difficulties compared with national samples. Older youth
and girls of all ages reported significantly more school-, family-, and peer-related difficulties with
parental deployment. Length of parental deployment and poorer non-deployed caregiver mental
health were associated with a greater number of challenges for children both during deployment
and deployed-parent reintegration. A study using deployment records and parent reports by primary
caregivers revealed that greater exposure to deployment was linked with impaired family function-
ing and marital instability (Lester et al., 2016). Parental depressive and post-traumatic stress symp-
toms were associated with impairments in socioemotional adjustment in young children, increased

107
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

anxiety in early childhood, and adjustment problems in school-age children. Conversely, parental
sensitivity was associated with improved social and emotional outcomes across childhood. In another
study (Mansfield et al., 2011) of over 300,000 children between 5 and 17 years of age, those with
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

parental deployment had more mental health diagnoses during a 4-year period compared with chil-
dren whose parents did not deploy. After the children’s age, gender, and mental health history were
adjusted for, excess mental health diagnoses associated with parental deployment were greatest for
acute stress reaction/adjustment, depression, and pediatric behavioral disorders and increased with
total months of parental deployment. Boys and girls showed similar patterns within these same cat-
egories, with more diagnoses observed in older children within gender groups and in boys relative to
girls within age groups. However, caution is needed because a meta-analysis found a small association
between deployment and poorer adjustment. This association varied across several features of the
studies. Age moderation was such that associations are strongest in middle childhood and weakest
during adolescence (Card et al., 2011). However, more recent studies have found stronger effects
(McDermid-Wadsworth, 2013).
Together, these studies suggest that various forms of father-child separation and loss of contact
are linked with several forms of negative developmental outcomes for children. However, this long-
standing issue remains difficult to interpret because concurrent circumstances as a result of the
separation (e.g., quality of caregiving, economic hardships associated with separation) may, in part,
account for these negative outcomes. In spite of these problems, it is vital to better understand these
various forms of fathering at a distance because significant numbers of children are developing under
these family forms.

Impact of Normal Variations in Intact Families


on Children’s Development
A voluminous literature has emerged over the last three decades that clearly demonstrates relations
between quality of paternal involvement and children’s social, emotional, and cognitive development
(Parke, 1996; Pleck, 2010). At the same time, considerable evidence shows a good deal of redundancy
between fathers’ and mothers’ impact on children. However, accumulated evidence suggests that
fathers make a unique contribution to children’s development independent of maternal influences.
In a review of the effects of fathers on children, Marsiglio, Amato, Day, and Lamb (2000) found
moderate negative associations between authoritative fathering and child internalizing and external-
izing problems. The relation held for children and adolescents regardless of age. Aldous and Mulligan
(2002), using a national data set, found that positive paternal engagement is linked to lower frequency
of later behavior problems for boys and for “difficult to rear” children. Similarly, in a large sample
of British teenagers, positive paternal engagement predicted positive school attitudes (Flouri, 2004)
and lower rates of police contact (Flouri, 2004). Still other investigators have found that children of
involved fathers have higher academic achievement (McBride, Schoppe-Sullivan, and Ho, 2005) and
better language and cognitive competence (Cabrera, Shannon, and Tamis-LeMonda, 2007). A meta-
analytic review of 22 studies found positive effects of paternal involvement on children’s cogni-
tive and personality development, whereas negative effects of involvement were rare (McWayne,
Downer, Campos, and Harris, 2013). Moreover, Amato and Rivera (1999) found that the positive
influence on children’s behavior was similar for European American, African American, and Latin
American fathers. Similarly, others have found similar links between positive fathering and fewer
behavior problems in adolescents regardless of ethnicity (Latin American and European American)
(Leidy et al., 2011).
In spite of this consistent body of evidence that positive father involvement is advantageous to
child and adolescent development, Marsiglio et al. (2000) offered three important caveats to this con-
clusion. First, most studies rely on a single data source, which raises the problem of shared method

108
Fathers and Families

variance. Second, many researchers do not control for the quality of the mother-child relationship
when examining father effects. Because the behavior and attitudes of parents are often highly related,
this step is critical. However, Pleck and Masciadrelli (2004) concluded that in over 70% of studies
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

that were methodologically sound (control for maternal effects and independent data sources), there
is evidence of a positive correlation of paternal involvement with child outcomes. Although there
is overlap between the effects of mothers and fathers on their children’s academic, emotional, and
social development, evidence is emerging that fathers make a unique contribution to their children’s
development.
A third caveat concerns problems of inferring direction of causality because studies are correla-
tional and involve concurrent rather than longitudinal assessments. However, two strands of evidence
suggest that the direction of effects plausibly flow from paternal behavior to child outcomes. First,
longitudinal studies support the view that fathers influence their children (see Amato and Rivera,
1999; Cookston and Finlay, 2006; Parke and Buriel, 2006; Pleck, 2010; Pleck and Masciadrelli, 2004,
for reviews). Nor are the effects of fathering on developmental outcomes restricted to childhood.
Boys who were separated from their fathers before age 10 due to paternal incarceration had more
internalizing problems in adulthood (Murray and Farrington, 2008). In addition, father-adolescent
closeness at age 16 predicted lower levels of depression and higher marital satisfaction at age 33
(Flouri, 2004). In a follow-up of Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957), Koestner, Franz, and Weinberger
(1990) found that the most powerful predictor of empathy in adulthood (age 31) for both men and
women was paternal childrearing involvement at age 5 and it was a better predictor than several
maternal variables. Franz, McClelland, and Weinberger (1991), reported that men and women with
better social relationships (marriage quality; extrafamilial ties) at age 41 had experienced more pater-
nal warmth as children. Although these studies support a father effects perspective, it is likely that
reciprocal relationships will become evident, in which children and fathers mutually influence each
other across the life course (Parke, 2013).

Reverse Role Families


The other strand of evidence that supports the plausibility of a father-to-child direction of effects
model comes from reports of reverse role families and intervention studies. A small minority of
families has explored alternative family arrangements such as role sharing and reversing family roles.
In spite of their rarity, these alternative family arrangements can inform us about the possible ways
in which families can reorganize themselves to provide flexibility for mothers, fathers, and children
(see Parke, 2013, for a recent review) and to assess the impact of increased father involvement on
children’s development. Although this arrangement is a relatively recent phenomenon, it is on the
rise. In 2007, stay-at-home fathers in the United States made up approximately 2.7% of the nation’s
stay-at-home parents, triple the percentage from 1997 and has been consistently higher each year
since 2005 (Shaver, 2007; U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). According to a 2010 census report, there are
154,000 stay-at-home dads in the United States, up from 140,000 in 2008. These fathers cared for
287,000 children (U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). The news from Canada is even more dramatic. The
number of men self-identifying as stay-at-home dads has increased threefold over the past 30 years
(Statistics Canada, 2010), with the number of stay-at-home dads gradually rising, reaching 60,000 in
2011, up from 20,000 in 1976. Canadian men now account for 12% of stay-at-home parents, com-
pared with only 4% in 1986. To put these figures into perspective: One in every 100 stay-at-home
parents were fathers in the 1970s and in 2011, it is 1 in every 8 fathers.
There is a variety of reasons for the increase in this family arrangement but also challenges that
may account for the small number of families who choose this alternative and persist in it. For many
families, it is economic; perhaps the father is laid off, or the mother earns more money so it makes
more financial sense for the father to stay home and the mother to assume the financial provider role.

109
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

In fact, between 2009 and 2010, during the recession the United States, the earnings of men with
college degrees dropped, but women’s earnings increased. In some cases, the wife’s job provides a
better set of health benefits than her spouse’s job. In fact, as unemployment rises, the number of men
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

who are thrust into primary care roles rises, in part, because the unemployment rates for men tend to
be higher than those for women (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2010). In some cases, women enjoy
their job and prefer to work outside the home, and men may dread their work and prefer to become
the primary caregiver. In other families, parents report that they simply believe that they should share
the care of their children.
There are distinct consequences for mothers, fathers, and children from parents’ role sharing.
According to Russell’s (1983, 1986) study of Australian families in which fathers took major or equal
responsibility for childcare, mothers reported increased stimulation as a result of outside employment,
greater independence, and increased self-esteem. Nearly half of the fathers reported difficulties asso-
ciated with the demands—the constancy and boredom—associated with their full-time caregiving
role. On the positive side, 70% of fathers reported that their relationship with their children and their
understanding of children improved, and they noted a greater awareness of mother-housewife roles,
and enjoyed the freedom from career pressures. In a 2-year follow-up (Russell, 1986), nearly two
thirds of both parents continued to view improved father-child relationships as the major advantage
of this sharing arrangement, but there was an increase in tension and conflict in the father-child
as well as the marital relationship. According to a study of 3,600 couples who participated in the
National Survey of Families and Households, Sayer et al. (2011) found that rates of divorce were
higher in role-reversal families.
Others argue that we still view breadwinning and masculinity as linked, and some men feel that
their sense of manhood is undermined by not being employed in an outside job and instead taking
on a traditional female role of caregiver (Doucet, 2006, 2013). To combat this feeling of threatened
loss of masculinity after giving up participation in the full-time labor force for a caregiving role,

many fathers replace employment with “self-provisioning” work that allows them to con-
tribute economically to the household economy as well as to display masculine practices,
both to themselves and their wider community. For example, fathers play a role in children’s
extracurricular activities such as sporting as well as in community work which emphasizes
leadership, sports, construction, and building while also easing community scrutiny of their
decision to give up work.
(Doucet, 2004, pp. 278–279)

In short, men who assume these primary caregiver roles adopt strategies to overcome some of the
still remaining social isolation and prejudice associated with this new family form and the prevailing
narrow definition of masculinity. To appreciate the potential impact of these changes in men’s roles
on children, we need to examine the ways in which fathers’ styles of interaction shift when mothers
and fathers reverse their customary roles as caregiver and outside-the-home worker. Evidence from
the United States, Australia, and Israel suggests that when fathers stay home, their style of interaction
becomes more like that of primary caregiving mothers with more imitative and vocal exchanges
(Field, 1978); more indoor activities such as talking, singing and drawing, and less exclusive focus on
roughhousing and outdoor games such as football (Russell, 1983); and more nurturance (Sagi, 1982).
However, these stay-at-home fathers still played as physically as traditional fathers, which suggests
that some aspects of paternal style may be resistant to change.
How do the children reared in reverse role families fare? There are no apparent major negative
effects, but there are some positive effects for children. Children were higher in problem-solving
skills and personal and social skills (Pruett, 1987) and higher verbal ability than children reared in tra-
ditional families (Radin, 1993) and higher in internal locus of control and in empathy than children

110
Fathers and Families

of fathers with low involvement (Sagi, Koren, and Weinberg, 1987). Similarly, Pruett (2000) found
that adolescents of primary caregiver fathers showed greater emotional balance, stronger curiosity,
and a stronger sense of self-assurance than adolescents reared in homes with less involved fathers.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Although this role-reversal arrangement clearly violates the notion of an “ideal” family form, it is
clear that men as primary caregivers not only do an adequate job of rearing children but also their
increased involvement may benefit their children’s development as well. At the same time, our cul-
tural acceptance of men as primary caregivers is still slowly evolving. Finally, evidence suggesting that
children from these families fare better must be treated cautiously because stay at home fathers may
be different in other ways from parents who maintain more traditional roles. Perhaps they might have
treated their children differently from traditional parents, no matter which parent stayed home with
the children. At the same time, it is likely that parents who reverse roles are significantly affected by
their choice, and that, therefore, the nontraditional environment in which children develop is at least
partially responsible for the differences between children reared in role-reversal and traditional families.

Intervention Evidence of the Effects of Father Involvement


Intervention studies in which parenting skills are taught show that changes in paternal parenting
behavior are related to enhanced social and cognitive development in children and, in turn, suggest
that the direction of effects flow, in part, from father to child (see Holmes, Cowan, Cowan, and Hawk-
ins, 2013; Knox, Cowan, Cowan, and Bildner, 2011, for reviews). For example, intervention studies
of fathers and infants have found that fathers who receive parenting training not only increase their
involvement (Parke, Hymel, Power and Tinsley,1980), but also their infants show increased social
responsiveness and more play initiations (Dickie and Gerber, 1980). Studies of fathers with older
children, too, show positive changes in father involvement, which, in turn, improves the father-child
relationship (Levant, 1988). Unfortunately, relatively few of these studies systematically assess children’s
developmental outcomes. An exception is the work of the Cowans and their colleagues (Cowan,
Cowan, and Heming, 2005), who used a randomized design to assess the effects of parental interven-
tions on children’s outcomes. Prior to their children’s entry into elementary school parents of 5-year-
old first children were assigned randomly to one of three intervention conditions: (1) a couples group
in which facilitators focused more on parent-child issues (e.g., discipline), (2) a couples group in which
facilitators focused more on issues between the parents as a couple, and (3) a brief consultation condi-
tion, which served as a control group. In comparison with parents in the control condition, parents in
the group interventions displayed increases in positive parenting (e.g., greater warmth, engagement,
structuring behaviors). Parents in the marital focus groups showed more positive parenting, reduced
conflict in problem-solving as a couple, and positive changes in the overall quality of their relationship
as a couple. Furthermore, children of parents in the marital-focus group displayed a greater reduction
in externalizing behaviors between kindergarten and first grade and greater increases in achievement
(Cowan, Cowan, Ablow, Johnson, and Measelle, 2005). Revisiting the families 10 years later when the
children had made the transition to high school, they found that positive outcomes associated with
parents’ relationship satisfaction, couple communication, and the teens’ externalizing behaviors had
persisted for families in which parents had attended the groups with an emphasis on couple relation-
ship issues (Cowan, Cowan, and Barry, 2011). The results of this study suggest that there is added and
lasting value of including couple relationship content in parenting interventions.
Later work focusing on low-income fathers confirmed and extended these findings. The Sup-
porting Father Involvement program (SFI) was developed with the aim of enhancing fathers’ involve-
ment with their children in low-income families (Cowan, Cowan, Pruett, Pruett, and Wong, 2009)
as well as strengthening the relationship between the parents. A sample of low-income participants
(two thirds were Mexican American) with a child from birth to age 7 were assigned randomly either
to (1) a one-time, 3-hour informational meeting for a group of couples, (2) 16-week groups attended

111
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

by fathers only (32 hours), or (3) 16-week groups attended by both mothers and fathers (32 hours).
Parents in the informational meeting showed significant declines in couple relationship satisfaction
and increases in their children’s problem behaviors. Participants in the fathers-only groups showed
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

increased involvement in the care of their children but declining relationship satisfaction, whereas
parents who attended the groups as couples reported increases in fathers’ involvement, declines in
parenting stress, and stable levels of both relationship satisfaction and child behaviors over 18 months.
In a replication with a more diverse high-risk sample, Cowan, Cowan, Pruett, Pruett, and Gillette
(2014) found that participation in a couples group intervention produced significant positive effects
on parents’ reports of their own well-being, their relationships with each other and their children,
and their children’s externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. The couple’s groups were
equally effective for participants of different ethnic groups, different economic levels, and different
levels of anxiety or depressive symptoms. From the perspective of policy, the SFI program demon-
strates the importance of (1) early intervention, (2) explicitly inviting fathers to participate (versus
recruiting fathers through mothers), and (3) focus on the coparenting and couple relationship as a
means to engage men (Pruett, Pruett, Cowan, and Cowan, 2017). Although these studies involved
both mother and fathers as the unit of intervention, these data suggest that experimental modifi-
cation of fathering behaviors can be an effective way of more clearly establishing the direction of
causality in fathering research.

Beyond Description: Processes Which Link Fathering and Child Outcomes


Work that has focused on fathers’ special style of interacting, namely play, reveals processes through
which fathers influence children’s development (see Leidy, Schofield, and Parke, 2013; Paquette,
2004, for reviews). As noted in Figure 3.1, three sets of mediating processes link father-child involve-
ment and child outcomes, namely, emotional, attentional regulatory, and cognitive representational
processes. Parke and his colleagues, for example, examined the relation between father-toddler play
and children’s adaptation to peers. In one study (MacDonald and Parke, 1984), fathers and their
3- and 4-year-old girls and boys were observed in 20 minutes of structured play in their homes.
Teachers ranked these sample children in terms of their popularity among their preschool classmates.
For both girls and boys, fathers who were rated as exhibiting high levels of physical play with their
children, and who elicited high levels of positive affect in their children during the play sessions, had
children who received the highest peer popularity ratings. For boys, however, this pattern was quali-
fied by the fathers’ level of directiveness. Boys whose fathers were both highly physical and low in
directiveness received the highest popularity ratings, and the boys whose fathers were highly directive
received lower popularity scores. Mize and Pettit (1997) found that preschool children whose play
with fathers is characterized by mutuality or balance in making play suggestions and following part-
ners’ suggestions were less aggressive, more competent, and better liked by peers. Similarly, Hart et al.
(1998, 2000) found that greater playfulness, patience, and understanding with children, especially on
the part of the father, are associated with less aggressive behavior with peers among both Western
and Russian children. Flanders and colleagues (2009, 2010) found that rough-and-tumble play was
associated with less aggression in preschoolers but only when the fathers control the flow of play and
keeps the play from exceeding the fathers’ acceptable level of excitability. It is not any kind or level
of physical play but modulated and regulated physical play that is positively linked to social outcomes
such as less aggression and better emotional regulation.

Emotional Processes
Possibly, children who interact with a physically playful father learn how to recognize and send emo-
tional signals during these play interactions. Several studies reveal a link between children’s emotional

112
Fathers and Families

encoding and decoding abilities that are presumably acquired, in part, in these playful interchanges
and children’s social adaptation to peers (Parke et al., 1988, 1992). In addition, fathers’ affect displays,
especially anger, seem to be a potent correlate of children’s social acceptance. In studies in both the
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

laboratory (Carson and Parke, 1996) and the home (Boyum and Parke, 1995), fathers’ negative affect
is inversely related to preschool and kindergarten children’s sociometric status. By contrast, when
fathers help their children cope with negative emotions, such as sadness and anger, at age 5, their
children were more socially competent with their peers at age 8 (Gottman et al., 1997). Later work
isolated other emotional processes such as emotional regulation (McDowell, Kim, O’Neil, and Parke,
2002; Parke, McDowell, Kim, and Leidy, 2006) and knowledge and use of display rules (McDowell
and Parke, 2000) that, in turn, are influenced by paternal interaction patterns and are predictive of
children’s social acceptance. Other aspects of children’s development that may be influenced by
fathers’ arousing and unpredictable play style include risk-taking (Kromelow, Harding, and Touris,
1990; Le Camus, 1995), the capacity to manage unfamiliar situations (Grossmann et al., 2002), and
the skill to manage competition (Bourcois, 1997). However, these topics have received less attention
than the issue of social competence with peers.

Attention Regulation
Closely related to emotional regulatory processes are a distinct but important additional mediator
between fathering and child outcomes, namely, attention regulatory abilities. These processes include
the ability to attend to relevant cues, sustain attention to refocus attention through such processes as
cognitive distraction and cognitive restructuring and other efforts to purposely reduce the level of
emotional arousal in stressful situations. Attentional processes organize experience and play a central
role in cognitive and social development beginning in early infancy (Rothbart, 2011). It has been
proposed that attention regulatory processes serve as a “shuttle” linking emotional regulation and
social-cognitive processes because attentional processes organize both cognitions and emotions, and
thus influence relationship competence (Katz,Wilson and Gottman, 1999). Using a national longi-
tudinal study (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2008), it was found that both mother
and father relationship quality at 54 months predicted children’s ability to sustain attention (using an
independent laboratory-based measure) as well as ratings of attentional problems in first grade and,
in turn, mediated the links between parenting and higher social skills ratings in first and third grade.
Maternal and paternal interactions accounted for unique variance in these outcomes. In summary,
the ability to regulate attention is a further important mediating factor through which paternal (and
maternal) behavior may influence children’s social competence.

Cognitive Processes
In addition to learning to manage emotions in social encounters, children also develop cognitive
representations or cognitive scripts that serve as guides to social exchanges with peers. Attachment
theorists offer cognitive working models, whereas social and cognitive psychologists have suggested
scripts or cognitive maps, as guides for social action. Research within the attachment tradition has
found support for Bowlby’s argument that representations vary as a function of child-parent attach-
ment history (working models Bowlby, 1969; Bretherton and Munholland, 2008; Sroufe et al., 2005).
For example, children who had been securely attached infants were more likely to represent their
family in their drawings in a coherent manner, with a balance between individuality and connection,
than children who had been insecurely attached. In turn, securely attached children have better peer
relationships (Sroufe et al.,2005)
Research in the social interactional tradition reveals links between parent and child cognitive
representations of social relationships and, in turn, their peer relationships (Burks and Parke, 1996;

113
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

McDowell, Parke, and Spitzer, 2002). For example, children of fathers with confrontational strate-
gies and goals for dealing with interpersonal conflict dilemmas were lower in peer competence. In
contrast, children of fathers with relational goals were rated higher in social competence by peers
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and teachers. Similarly, Rah and Parke (2008) found that children who had positive interactions
with their fathers in fourth grade had fewer negative goals and strategies for solving interpersonal
problems with either their fathers or their peers in fifth grade. In turn, they were more accepted by
their peers, which suggests that positive father-child interaction can lead to more constructive social
scripts for dealing with peers. Other work suggests that father-child interaction is related to children’s
“theory of mind” competence, a clear asset for achieving social skills (LaBounty et al., 2008).
More recently, another cognitive mediator between father behavior and children’s outcomes has
received attention, namely, father mattering. This aspect of the parent-child relationship is chil-
dren’s appraisal of how much they “matter” to their parents (Rosenberg and McCullough, 1979).
Beliefs that one is important to one’s parent may give children and adolescents a sense of security
in their relatedness and connectedness to others which, in turn, may have important implications
for their mental health (Elliot, 2009). In support of this expectation, parental mattering is related to
fewer internalizing and externalizing problems, more self-esteem, healthier self-concepts and sense
of efficacy, and reduced intrafamilial violence and suicidal ideation in adolescents (e.g., Elliott, Cun-
ningham, Colangelo, and Gelles, 2011; Marshall, 2001, 2004; Wu and Kim, 2009). Suh et al. (2016)
explored relations between parental mattering and adolescent mental health in the context of step-
father families (i.e., adolescents living with biological mothers and stepfathers but who also had a
nonresidential biological father). Mattering to residential stepfathers or to nonresidential biological
fathers was associated with adolescent mental health problems, over and above mattering to moth-
ers. Results showed that mattering to nonresidential biological fathers significantly predicted fewer
internalizing problems; mattering to residential stepfathers significantly predicted fewer internalizing
and externalizing problems. For teacher-reported externalizing problems specifically, mattering to
the two father types interacted such that mattering to either father predicted lower externalizing
problems; perceived mattering to the second father did not predict a further reduction in problems.
These data suggest that mattering to residential stepfathers and to nonresidential biological fathers
is important for adolescent mental health and—for teacher-observed externalizing problems—that
a sense of mattering to one father can buffer an adolescent against the potential negative effects of
feeling unimportant to the second father (see also Stevenson et al., 2014). Further, it appears that
children and adolescents make sense of their relationships with their fathers by forming attributions
to explain his behaviors (Finlay et al., 2014) and by engaging in guided cognitive reframing with
mothers, fathers, and others (Cookston et al., 2012; Cookston et al., 2015).
Although father involvement in infancy and childhood is quantitatively less than mother involve-
ment, the data suggest that both fathers and mothers have important and possibly unique impacts on
their offspring’s social-emotional development. Just as earlier research indicated that quality rather
than quantity of mother-child interaction was the important predictor of cognitive and social devel-
opment, a similar assumption appears to hold for fathers. At the same time, there is a long history
of documentation that maternal involvement is related to child outcomes independent of paternal
effects (Parke and Buriel, 2006). More interesting is evidence suggesting that mothers’ verbal style
of interaction may enhance children’s intellectual development including memory, problem-solving,
and language advancement (Bornstein, 2002; Cabrera, Shannon, West, and Brooks-Gunn, 2006)
as well as emotional understanding. For example, mothers’ emotional expressiveness is related to
children’s understanding of emotions (LaBounty et al., 2008). Perhaps children’s knowledge of inter-
nal emotional states—a consequence of maternal labeling of emotions and feeling states during
social and caregiving interactions—is learned through mother-child exchanges (Denham, Basset,
and Wyatt, 2014).

114
Fathers and Families

Remaining Issues and Future Trends for Fathering Scholarship


A number of issues remain to be examined in future research if we are to describe fully the complex-
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

ities, specify the determinants and processes, and outline the consequences of father-child relation-
ships. These include the choice of the levels of analysis, unit of analysis, the effects of family variation,
types of developmental change, the role of secular trends, the need for interdisciplinary perspectives,
increased attention to context, and methodological issues.

Levels of Analysis: Beyond Individual Levels Toward Multilevel Models


In spite of calls for recognizing the multiple-levels of influence on fathering, most studies focus on
single or, at most, several levels of analysis. However, as this review underscores a wide range of factors
from biological to individual, couple, family, community, and culture operate together in determin-
ing father behavior. We need more studies that sample across levels of analysis and, in turn, analytic
models that begin to reveal the complex mediating and moderating role of factors at these many
levels of influence (see Feldman, 2016; Holmes and Huston, 2010).

Unit of Analysis
Current work clearly recognizes the importance of considering fathers from a family systems per-
spective. However, our conceptual analysis of dyadic and triadic units of analysis is still limited
(Barrett and Hinde, 1988; McHale and Lindahl, 2011; Parke, 1988). Considerable progress has been
made in describing the behavior of individual interactants (e.g., mother, father, child) within dyadic
and to a lesser extent triadic settings, but less progress has been achieved in developing a language
for describing interaction in dyadic and triadic terms. However, such terms as “reciprocity,” “syn-
chronicity,” and “co-regulation” are receiving empirical operationalization and evaluation in recent
work (e.g., Feldman, 2007). In addition, greater attention needs to be paid to the family as a unit of
analysis.

Fathers and Family Variation


An advance of the last decade is recognition of the importance of individual differences in children;
one of the next advances will be the recognition of individual differences among families and fathers.
Recognition of individual variability across families implies the necessity of expanding sampling pro-
cedures. In spite of our greater awareness of family diversity, the range of family types that are studied
is still relatively narrow. Although progress has been made in describing fathers and children in differ-
ent cultures (Shwalb et al., 2013; Roopnarine and Hossain, 2013) and in different ethnic groups and
social classes in the United States (Cabrera et al., 2013; Furstenberg, 2014) more detailed description
as well as process-oriented work is necessary if we are to achieve an understanding of how cultural
beliefs and practices influence fathering behaviors. Finally, recent work on gay and lesbian families
has raised provocative issues for fatherhood research. The evidence suggests that children in families
of same-gendered parents develop adequately in terms of socioemotional adjustment (Golombok,
2015; Patterson, 2016). As Parke (2013, p. 105) has argued:

Our focus on the gender of the parent may too narrow; instead, it could be helpful to
recast the issue and ask whether it is the extent to which exposure to males and/or females
is critical or whether it is exposure to the interactive style typically associated with either
mother or father that matters.

115
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Perhaps the style of parenting and gender of the parent who enacts the style can be viewed as par-
tially independent. More attention to the kinds of parenting styles evident in same-gendered parental
households will help us address the uniqueness of father and mother roles in the family and help
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

provide needed clarity on the important issue of how essential opposite-sex parental dyads are for
children’s development (Parke, 2013; Silverstein and Auerbach, 1999).

Types of Developmental Change


Moreover, we need to move beyond childhood and examine more closely father relationships with
their adult children—if we are to achieve a life-span view of fathering. Although development
traditionally has marked change in the individual child, this perspective is too limited, and fathers,
as well as children, continue to develop across time (Elder et al., 2015; Parke, 1988). Fathers’ man-
agement of a variety of life-course tasks, such as marriage, work, and personal identity, will clearly
determine how they will execute parental tasks; in turn, these differences may find expression in
measures of father-child interaction. Because developmental shifts in children’s perceptual, cogni-
tive, and social development, in turn, may alter parental attitudes and behaviors and/or the nature of
the adults’ own developmentally relevant choices, such as work and family commitment, we need
to recognize two developmental trajectories—a child developmental course and an adult devel-
opmental sequence. The description of the interplay between these two types of developmental
curves is necessary to capture adequately the nature of developmental changes in a father’s role in
the family.

Monitoring Secular Trends


There is a continuing need to monitor secular trends and to describe their impact on father-child
interaction patterns (Furstenberg, 2014). “Family is not a static institution but one that is constantly
being re-worked, reshaped, reimagined and reenacted in complex and dynamic ways” (Goldberg,
2010, p. 186). Clearly, a historical perspective on fathers and families is necessary, because changing
economic, social, political, and technological changes have real and measurable effects on family
organization, form, and functioning. As historical events such as the Great Depression, the Midwest
farm crisis of the 1980s, the tragedy of 9/11, and the recent Great Recession illustrate, families are
altered by these secular events, and we need to continually monitor their effects. While these events
are potential disruptive forces on families, these changes can be viewed as natural experiments that
can be useful in evaluating theoretical models of family functioning, coping, and resilience. Secu-
lar change is complex and clearly does not affect all individuals equally or all behavior patterns to
the same extent. In fact, it is a serious oversimplification to assume that general societal trends can
isomorphically be applied across all individual fathers and families. Moreover, better guidelines are
necessary to illuminate which aspects of processes within families are most likely to be altered by
historical events and which processes are less amenable to change (Parke and Elder, in press).
A variety of technological advances from the new reproductive technologies to new forms of
electronic communication continues to play a major role in shaping fathers’ roles. Just as the intro-
duction of television and phones altered family interaction patterns, new advances in communica-
tion technologies (i.e., internet, cellphones, instant messaging, video conferencing, social media) are
reshaping the ways that families interact. Our understanding of the impact of these new advances
on father relationships and processes is limited and lags behind the adoption of these new strategies
by families themselves. As families become increasingly geographically dispersed, communication
technologies offer new forms of “at-a-distance” contact that can benefit families. Advances such
as video conferencing allow “face-to-face” contact with extended family members, such as distant
grandparents or other relatives and for fathers who are separated from their children due to mobility,

116
Fathers and Families

divorce, military deployment, incarceration, and fragmented immigration patterns. More systematic
examination of these issues is needed.
Even more profound is the potential impact of the new reproductive technologies on parenting
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

in general and fathering in particular (Parke, 2013). New reproductive technologies are expanding
the ways in which individuals become parents and opening up new possibilities for infertile as well
as same-sex couples (Golombok, 2015; Paulson and Sachs, 1999). The implication of these recent
advances including in vitro fertilization, sperm and egg donors, and surrogate mothers for our defini-
tions of parenthood, including fatherhood, are only beginning to be explored. Although knowledge
about the impact of these alternative pathways to fatherhood on children’s development is limited, to
date, the effects on children’s development are generally benign (Golombok, 2015). Increasingly, our
definition of fatherhood is becoming divorced from biology and instead is recognized as a socially
constructed category.

Methodological Issues
No single methodological strategy will suffice to understand the development of the father’s role in
the family. Instead, a wide range of designs and data collection and data analysis strategies is necessary.
To date, there is still a paucity of information concerning interrelations across molar and molecular
levels of analysis. However, it is becoming increasingly clear that a microanalytic strategy is not always
more profitable in terms of describing relationships among interactive partners; in fact, in some cases,
ratings may be a more useful approach. A set of guidelines concerning the appropriate level of analy-
sis for different questions would be helpful.
More attention to tasks used for the assessment of the effects of fathers on their children is needed
in light of the work of Grossman et al. (2002) and Paquette and colleagues (Flanders et al., 2009,
2010; Paquette, 2004), which suggests that challenging situations may be well suited to uncovering
the unique effects of father interaction on children. The continued utilization of similar tasks for
mothers and fathers may, in fact, undermine our efforts to better understand the distinctive ways that
fathers and mothers influence their children’s development.
To address the important issue of direction of effects in work on the impact of fathers on children
and families, experimental strategies are necessary. By experimentally modifying either the type of
paternal behavior or level of father involvement, firmer conclusions concerning the causative role
that fathers play in modifying their children’s and their wives’ development will be possible. Interven-
tion studies (e.g., Doherty, Erickson, and LaRossa, 2006; Holmes et al., 2013) aimed at modifying
fathering behavior provide models for this type of work, and studies (Pruett et al., 2017) that include
measures of child, mother, and father development are providing evidence of the impact of changes
in fathering behavior on developmental outcomes. Moreover, these experimentally based interven-
tions have clear policy implications by exploring the degree of plasticity of fathering behavior and
can serve as a vehicle for evaluation of alternative theoretical views of fatherhood.

An Interdisciplinary Perspective on Fatherhood


In many ways, a psychological approach to fathering has a unique identity, with its focus on intrafa-
milial processes, such as actor attitudes, cognitions, and beliefs and the dynamic interchanges between
and among family members. However, it is unlikely that we can fully understand fathers without rec-
ognizing the contributions of other disciplines. Sociologists inform us about issues of ethnicity, class,
inequality, and demographic shifts (Edin and Nelson, 2013), anthropologists alert us to cross-cultural
variations (Hewlett and Macfarlan, 2010), and economists document shifts in economic opportuni-
ties and struggles (Bishai, 2013). Medical professionals provide insights about family illness, disease,
and wellness-promoting strategies (Yogman and Garfield, 2016), and legal scholars offer glimpses

117
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

into the ways in which families can be helped or hindered by laws and social policies that directly
affect families (Sugarman, 2008). Historians remind us that cross-time shifts in family forms, beliefs,
and practices are constantly under revision (Pleck, 2004). Beyond these traditional contributors to
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

the study of fathers, some disciplines have not received sufficient attention, namely, architecture and
urban design (Parke, 2013). The effects of living in multifamily households or in intergenerational
housing on father roles is poorly understood. Our challenge is to examine how innovations in hous-
ing arrangements alter various aspects of family life. Father scholars need to better understand how
these cross-disciplinary insights modify process-oriented explanations of father functioning. Clearly,
solutions to the problems facing contemporary families need scholars and practitioners from many
disciplines.

Contextual Issues
Greater attention needs to be paid to the role of context in determining father-child relationships.
How do father-child interaction patterns shift between home and laboratory settings and across dif-
ferent types of interaction contexts such as play, teaching, and caregiving? Moreover, it is important
to consider the social as well as the physical context. Recognition of the embeddedness of fathers in
family contexts is critical, and, in turn, conceptualizing families as embedded in a variety of extra-
familial social settings is important for understanding variation in father functioning (Parke, 2013).
The legal system is one of many institutions that affect father functioning as illustrated by the legal
issues surrounding custody, adoption, and the regulation of the new reproductive technologies. The
family links with educational institutions including schools as centers of academic learning and
schools as social service systems (i.e., childcare centers and after-school programs) need more atten-
tion. Medical settings and social service providers all play central roles in family life and need more
systematic scrutiny, especially in light of the need to make fathers more welcome in these settings.
Work contexts have historically received attention beginning with maternal employment issues, but
as work patterns have changed, new issues have emerged such as time management, work schedules
and stress, and family leave policies for fathers as well as mothers. As intergenerational perspectives
become more prevalent, family leave for both parents needs to accommodate not only infants and
ill children but also elderly relatives as well because it is likely as family roles shift fathers as well as
mothers will be called on to provide elder care. Another topic, fathers and religion, has not yet been
fully embraced by our field in spite of some exceptions (e.g., Bollinger and Palkovitz, 2003; Born-
stein et al., 2017; Vermeer, 2014). We still do not fully appreciate the myriad ways religious beliefs
and practices and religious institutions and leaders play in family life as both moral guides and social
supports. Nor are the attitudes, practices, and organization of family life across different religious
groups such as Buddhists, Hindus, and Muslims, who are becoming increasingly part of contempo-
rary Western culture, fully understood. Clearly, the links among fathers, families, and community
institutions continue to merit our attention.

Conclusion
In spite of a relatively brief recent history of serious research devoted to fatherhood, considerable
progress has been achieved in our understanding of the paternal role and the impact of fathers on
themselves and others. Several conclusions are warranted. First, some modest increases over the
past several decades have occurred in the level of father involvement with children. However, not
all types of involvement shown have been equally affected, and managerial aspects of family life
remain largely a maternal responsibility. Moreover, not all fathers have increased their involve-
ment and the variability in involvement across social class, education and ethnicity needs to con-
tinue to be explored. Second, fathers are clearly competent caregivers and playmates in spite of

118
Fathers and Families

their limited overall involvement in childcare. Third, stylistic differences in interaction with chil-
dren especially in play between mothers and fathers continue to be evident in spite of recent shifts
toward greater father involvement in children’s daily lives, although some cross-cultural evidence
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

suggests that the paternal physical play style may not be as universal as previously assumed. Fourth,
the father role appears less scripted than the mother role, which may account for the variability
that characterizes enactments of fathering. Fifth, evidence continues to suggest that fathers impact
children’s social, emotional, and cognitive development. However, quality of fathering remains an
important determinant of paternal influence on children’s development, and the independent con-
tribution of fathers relative to mothers remains only weakly documented. Sixth, recent evidence
suggests that fathering activities alter men’s marital relationships as well as men’s own sense of self
and their societal generativity.
The study of father-child relationships has matured in the last several decades and is now a
more fully contextualized issue. Fathers in the context of their social relationships both within and
beyond the family are increasingly the appropriate point of entry for understanding the issue of
both paternal roles and their impact on themselves and others. Because our conceptual paradigms
continue to outstrip our empirical understanding, reducing this gap is the challenge of the next
decades of research. Children, fathers, and families will benefit from this increased understanding.

References
Adamsons, K. (2010). Using identity theory to develop a midrange level model of parental gatekeeping and
parenting behavior. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 2, 137–148.
Aldous, J., and Mulligan, G. M. (2002). Fathers’ child care and children’s behavior problems. Journal of Family
Issues, 23, 624–647.
Allen, J. M., and Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mothers’ beliefs and behaviors that inhibit greater
father involvement in family work. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 199–212.
Alvergne, A., Faurie, C., and Raymond, M. (2009). Father–offspring resemblance predicts paternal investment in
humans. Animal Behavior, 78: 61–69.
Amato, P. A., and Gilbreth, J. G. (1999). Nonresident fathers and children’s well-being: Media analysis, Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 61, 15–73.
Amato, R. R., and Rivera, F. (1999). Paternal involvement and children’s behavior. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 61, 375–384.
Arditti, J. A. (2003). Locked doors and glass walls: Family visiting at a local jail. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 8,
115–138.
Arnett, J. (2011). Emerging adulthood(s): The cultural psychology of a new life stage. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.),
Bridging cultural and developmental psychology: New syntheses in theory, research, and policy. Oxford: Research and
Policy, Oxford University Press.
Astone, N. M., Dariotis, J. K., Sonenstein, F. L., Pleck, J. H., and Hynes, K. (2010). Men’s work efforts and the
transition to fatherhood. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 31, 3–13.
Bandura, A. (1989). Cognitive social learning theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development. Green-
wich, CT: JAI Press.
Bales, W. D., and Mears, D. P. (2008). Inmate social ties and the transition to society: Does visitation reduce
recidivism? Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 45, 287–321.
Barak-Levy, Y., and Atzaba-Poria, N. A. (2013). Paternal versus maternal coping styles with child diagnosis of
developmental delay. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2040–2046.
Barr, R., Brito, N., Zocca, J., Reina, S., Rodriguez, J. and Shauffer, C. (2011). The baby elmo program: Improv-
ing teen parent-child interactions within juvenile justice facilities. Children and Youth Services Review, 33,
1555–1562.
Barrett, J., and Hinde, R. A. (1988). Triadic interactions: Mother-first born-second born. In R. A. Hinde and
J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Towards understanding families. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baruch, G. K., and Barnett, R. C. (1986). Fathers’ participation in family work and children’s sex-role atti-
tudes. Child Development, 57, 1210–1223.
Beitel, A., and Parke, R. D. (1998). Maternal and paternal attitudes as determinants of father involvement. Journal
of Family Psychology, 12, 268–288.
Belsky, J. (1984). Determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83–96.

119
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Belsky, J., Rovine, M., and Fish, M. (1989). The developing family system. In M. Gunnar and E. Thelen (Eds.),
Systems and development: Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 119–166). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Berndt, T. J., Cheung, P. C., Lau, S., Hau, K. T., and Lew, W. (1993). Perceptions of parenting in mainland China,
Taiwan, and Hong Kong: Sex differences and societal differences. Developmental Psychology, 29, 156–164.
Bhavnagri, N., and Parke, R. D. (1991). Parents as direct facilitators of children’s peer relationships: Effects of age
of child and sex of parent age of child and sex of parent. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 423–440.
Bianchi, S. M. (2009). “What gives” when mothers are employed? Parental time allocation in dual- and single-
earner two-parent families. In C. D. Russell and E. J. Hill (Eds.), Handbook of families and work (pp. 305–330).
Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
Bianchi, S. M., Robinson, J. P., and Milkie, M. (2006). Changing rhythms of American family life. New York: Rus-
sell Sage Foundation.
Bishai, D. (2013). The economics of fatherhood. In N. Cabrera and C. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father
involvement (pp. 361–378). New York, NY: Routledge.
Bollinger, B. and Palkovitz, R., (2003). The relationship between the expression of spiritual faith and parental
involvement in three groups of fathers. Journal of Men’s Studies, 11, 117–130.
Bonney, J. F., Kelley, M. L., and Levant, R. F. (1999). A model of fathers’ involvement in child care in dual-earner
families. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 401–415.
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 5). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bornstein, M. H. (2007). On the significance of social relationships in the development of children’s earliest
symbolic play: An ecological perspective. In A. Gönçü and S. Gaskins (Eds.), Play and development: Evolutionary,
sociocultural, and functional perspectives (pp. 101–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., and Suwalsky, J. D. (2017). Parenting cognitions → parenting practices → child
adjustment? The standard model. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301–319.
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Lansford, J. E., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D., Bombi, A. S., Chang, L., Deater-
Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring,
E., Steinberg, L., Tapanya, S., Uribe Tirado, L. M., Zelli, A., and Alampay, L. P. (2017). “Mixed blessings”:
Parental religiousness, parenting, and child adjustment in global perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 58, 880–892.
Boudin, K. (1997). The children’s center programs of Bedford hills correctional facility. In C. L. Blinn (Ed.), Mater-
nal ties (pp. 55–86). Lanham, MD: American Correctional Association.
Bourçois, V. (1997). Modalités de présence du père et développement social de l’enfant d’âge préscolaire, Enfance, 3, 389–399.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Boyum, L., and Parke, R. D. (1995). Family emotional expressiveness and children’s social competence. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 57, 593–608.
Braungart-Rieker, J., Courtney, S., and Garwood, M. M. (1999). Mother- and father-infant attachment: Families
in context. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 535–553.
Bretherton, I., and Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating
a central construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy and P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 102–127). New York: Guilford.
Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2017, April). Employment characteristics of families—2016. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Labor.
Burks, V., and Parke, R. D. (1996). Parent and child representations of social relationships: Linkages between
families and peers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 358–378. Cabrera, N. J., Aldoney, D., and Tamis-Lemonda,
C. (2013). Latino fathers. In N. J. Cabrera, and C. S. Tamis-Lemonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement:
Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 244–260). New York: Taylor and Francis.
Cabrera, N. J., Hofferth, S. L., and Schae, S. (2011). Patterns and predictors of father–infant engagement across
race/ethnic groups. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 365–375.
Cabrera, N., Fitzgerald, H., Bradley, R., and Roggman, L. (2014). The ecology of father–child relationships: An
expanded model. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 6, 336–354.
Cabrera, N., Shannon, J., West, J., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Parental interactions with Latino infants: Varia-
tion by country of origin and English proficiency. Child Development, 74, 1190–1207.
Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2007). Fathers’ influence on their children’s cognitive
and emotional development: From toddlers to pre-K. Journal of Applied Developmental Science, 11, 208–213.
Cabrera, N., Shannon, J., Mitchell, S., and West, J. (2009). Mexican American mothers and fathers’ prenatal
attitudes and father prenatal involvement: Links to mother-infant interaction and father engagement. Sex
Roles, 60, 510–526.

120
Fathers and Families

Card, N. A., Bosch, L. A., Casper, D. M., Wiggs, C. B., Hawkins, S. A., and Borden, L. M. (2011). A meta-analytic
review of internalizing, externalizing, and academic adjustment among children of deployed military service
members. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 508–520.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Carlson, M. J., and Furstenberg, F. F. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of multipartnered fertility and urban
U.S. parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 68, 718–732.
Carlson, M., and McLanahan, S. (2010). Fathers in fragile families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in
child development (5th ed., pp. 241–269). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Carson, J., and Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children’s peer
competency. Child Development, 67, 2217–2226.
Cavanaugh, S. E., and Huston, A. (2006). Family instability and children’s early problem behavior. Social Forces,
85, 551–581.
Chandra, A., Lara-Cinisomo, S., Jaycox, L. H., Tanielian, T., Burns, R. M., Ruder, T., and Han, B. (2010). Chil-
dren on the homefront: The experience of children from military families. Pediatrics, 125, 16–25.
Centers for disease control and prevention. (2016). Teen pregnancy in the United States. Atlanta, GA: CDC.
Chao, R., and Kim, K. (2000). Parenting differences among immigrant Chinese fathers and mothers in the
United States. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 1, 71–79.
Chao, R., and Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 4: Social
conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59–93). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cheadle, J. E., Amato, P. R., and King, V. (2010). Patterns of nonresident father contact. Demography, 47,
205–225.
Cherlin, A. (2009). The marriage-go-round. New York: Random House.
Child Trends. (2006). Charting parenthood: A statistical report of fathers and mothers in America. Washington, DC:
Child Trends.
Chuang, S., and Su, Y. (2008). Transcending Confucian teachings on fathering: A sign of the times or accultura-
tion? In S. Chuang and R. P. Moreno (Eds.), Understanding immigrant fathers in North America (pp. 217–229).
New York: Lexington Books.
Chuang, S. S., and Zhu, M. (2018). Where are you Daddy?: An exploration of father involvement in Chinese
families in Canada and Mainland China. In S. S. Chuang and C. S. Costigan (Eds.), An international approach
to parenting and parent-child relationships in immigrant families. New York: Springer.
Cho, E., and Ciancetta, L. (2016). Child outcomes associated with parent work-family experiences. In T. D.
Allen and L. T. Eby (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of work and family. New York: Oxford University Press.
Clarke, W. (2009). Delayed transitions of young adults. Canadian Social Trends Catalogue number 11-008-
XWE, number 84.
Clements, M. L., Lim, K. G., and Chaplin, T. M. (2002, November). Marriage, parenting, and co-parenting:
Effects of context, parent gender, and child gender on interactions. In F. Hughes (Chair), Bridging the marital
dyad and the family triad. Symposium presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Advancement of
Behavior Therapy, Reno, NV.
Coley, R. L., and Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1999). Stability and change in paternal involvement among urban
African-American fathers. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 416–435.
Coley, R., and Medeiros, B. (2007). Reciprocal longitudinal relations between nonresident father involvement
and adolescent delinquency. Child Development, 78, 132–147.
Coltrane, S., and Adams, M. (2008). Gender and families (Gender Lens Series, 2nd ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman
and Littlefield.
Coltrane, S., and Ishii-Kuntz, M. (1992) Men’s housework: A life course perspective. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 54, 43–57.
Condon, J. T., Boyce, P., and Corkindale, C. J. (2004). The first-time fathers study: A prospective study of the
mental health and wellbeing of men during the transition to parenthood. Australian and New Zealand Journal
of Psychiatry, 38, 56–64.
Conger, R. D., Ebert-Wallace, L., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., and Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic
pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental
Psychology, 38, 179–193.
Cookston, J. T., Braver, S. L., Griffin, W., deLusé, S. R., and Miles, J. C. (2007). Effects of the dads for life
intervention on interparental conflict and co-parenting in the two years after divorce. Family Processes, 46,
123–137.
Cookston, J. T., and Finlay, A. K. (2006). Paternal involvement and adolescent adjustment: Longitudinal findings
from add health. Fathering, 4, 137–158.
Cookston, J. T., Olide, A., Adams, M., Fabricius, W. V., Braver, S. L., and Parke, R. D. (2012). In L. Juang and
A. Umana-Taylor (Issue Eds.), Family conflict among Chinese and Mexican-origin adolescents and their

121
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

parents. In L. Jensen and R. Larson (Eds.), New directions in child and adolescent development (pp. 83–103). San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Cookston, J. T., Olide, A., Parke, R. D., Fabricius, W. V., Saenz, D., and Braver, S. L. (2015). He said what?
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Guided cognitive reframing about the co-resident father/stepfather-adolescent relationship. Journal of Research
on Adolescence, 25, 263–278.
Cookston, J. T., Boyer, C. J., Vega, E., and Parke, R. D. (2017, April). Transnational fathers (and mothers): Modes of
contact and parent-child relationships. Paper presented at the 2017 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research
on Child Development, Austin, TX.
Cooney, T. M., Pedersen, F. A., Indelicato, S., and Palkovitz, R. (1993). Timing of fatherhood: Is “on-time”
optimal? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 205–215.
Costigan, C., and Su, T. F. (2008). Cultural predictors of the parenting cognitions of immigrant Chinese mothers
and fathers in Canada. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 432–442.
Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. (1992). When parents become partners. New York: Basic Books.
Cowan, P. A., and Cowan, C. P. (2010). How working with couples fosters children’s development: From pre-
vention science to public policy. In M. Schulz, M. Pruett, P. Kerig, and R. Parke (Eds.), Strengthening couple
relationships for optimal child development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., Ablow, J., Johnson, V., and Measelle, J. (Eds.). (2005). The family context of parenting
in children’s adaptation to school. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., and Barry, J. (2011). Couples’ groups for parents of preschoolers: Ten-year outcomes
of a randomized trial. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(2), 240–250.
Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., and Heming, G. (2005). Two variations of a preventive intervention for couples:
Effects on parents and children during the transition to elementary school. In The family context of parenting in
children’s adaptation to elementary school (pp. 227–312). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., Pruett, M. K., and Pruett, K. D., and Wong, J. (2009). Promoting fathers’ engage-
ment with children: Preventive interventions for low-income families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 71,
663–679.
Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., Pruett, M. K., Pruett, K., and Gillette, P. (2014). Evaluating a couples group to
enhance father involvement in low-income families using a benchmark comparison. Family Relations: An
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 63, 356–370.
Cox, M. J., and Paley, B. (2003). Understanding families as systems. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12,
193–196.
Cox, R., and Wallace, S. (2013). The impact of incarceration on food insecurity among households with children. Fragile
Families Working Paper 13-05-FF, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ.
Cozza, S. J., Holmes, A. K., and van Ost, S. L (2013). Family-centered care for military and veteran families
affected by combat injury. Clinical Child Fam Psychology Review, 16, 311–321.
Craig, L., and Powell, A. (2011). Non-standard work schedules, work-family balance and the gendered division
of childcare. Work, Employment, and Society, 25, 274–291.
Crockenberg, S., and Langrock, A. (2001). The role of specific emotions in children’s responses to interparental
conflict: A test of the model. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 163–182.
Crouter, A. C., Bumpus, M. R., Maguire, M. C., and McHale, S. M. (1999). Linking parents’ work pressure
and adolescents’ well-being: Insights into dynamics in dual earner families. Developmental Psychology, 35,
1453–1461.
Cummings, E. M., and Davies, P. T. (2010). Marital conflict and children: An emotional security perspective. New York
and London: Guilford.
Cummings, E. M., Keller, P. S., and Davies, P. T. (2005). Towards a family process model of maternal and pater-
nal depressive symptoms: Exploring multiple relations with child and family functioning. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 479–489.
Cummings, E. M., Merrilees, C. E., and Ward George, M. (2010). Fathers, marriages, and families. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 154–176) Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Cummings, E. M., El-Sheikh, M., Kouros, C. D., and Keller, P. S. (2007). Children’s skin conductance reactiv-
ity as a mechanism of risk in the context of parental depressive symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 48, 436–445.
Daly, K. (1993). Reshaping fatherhood: Finding the models. Journal of Family Issues, 14, 510–530.
Dave, S., Nazareth, I., Sherr, L., and Senior, R. (2005). The association of paternal mood and infant temperament:
A pilot study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 609–621.
Dalliare, D. H. (2019). Incarcerated parents. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting Vol. 4: Social conditions
and applied parenting (3rd ed., pp. 590–617). New York: Routledge.

122
Fathers and Families

Dallaire, D. H., Zeman, J. L., and Thrash, T. M. (2015). Children’s experiences of maternal incarceration-
specific risks: Predictions to psychological maladaptation. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
44, 109–122.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Daniels, P., and Weingarten, K. (1982). Sooner or later: The timing of parenthood in adult lives. New York: Norton.
Deater-Deckard, K., and Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlin-
ear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161–175.
DeGarmo, D. S. (2010). Coercive and prosocial fathering, antisocial personality, and growth in children’s postdi-
vorce noncompliance. Child Development, 81, 503–516.
Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., and Wyatt, T. (2014). The socialization of emotional competence. In J. Grusec and
P. Hastings (Eds.), The handbook of socialization (2nd ed., pp. 590–613). New York: Guilford Press.
Deutsch, C. (1999). Having it all. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Dickie, J., and Gerber, S. C. (1980). Training in social competence: The effect on mothers, fathers and infants.
Child Development, 51, 1248–1251.
Doherty, W. J., Erickson, M. F., and LaRossa, R. (2006). An intervention to increase father involvement and skills
with infants during the transition to parenthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 438-447.
Doherty, W. E. J., Kouneski, E. F., and Erikson, M. (1998). Responsible fathering: An overview and conceptual
framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 277–292.
Doss, B. D., Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2009). The effect of the transition to parenthood
on relationship quality: An 8-year prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 601–614.
Dreby, J. (2010). Divided by borders: Mexican migrants and their children. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Doucet, A. (2004). Fathers and the responsibility for children: A puzzle and a tension. Atlantis: Critical Studies in
Gender, Culture and Social Justice, 28, 103–114.
Doucet, A. (2006). Do men mother? Fathering, care and domestic responsibility. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Doucet, A. (2013). Gender roles and fathering. In C. Tamis-LeMonda and N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father
involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 297–319). New York: Routledge.
Duffy, A. (1988). Struggling with power: Feminist critiques of family inequity. In N. Mandell and A. Duffy (Eds.),
Reconstructing the Canadian family: Feminist perspectives (pp. 111–139). Toronto: Butterworths.
Durrett, M. E., Otaki, M., and Richards, P. (1984). Attachment and the mother’s perception of support from the
father. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 167–176.
East, P. L. (2010). Children’s provision of family caregiving: Benefit or burden? Child Development Perspectives,
4, 55–61.
Edin, K., and Nelson, T. (2013). Doing the best I can: Fathering in the inner city. Berkeley CA: University of Cali-
fornia Press.
Edin, K., Tach, L., and Mincy, R. (2009). Claiming fatherhood: Race and the dynamics of father involvement
among unmarried men. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 621, 149–177.
Eggebeen, D., Knoester, C., and McDaniel, B. (2013). The implications of fatherhood for men. In N. Cabrera
and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 338–
357). New York: Routledge.
Eggebeen, D. J., Dew, J., and Knoester, C. (2010). Fatherhood and men’s lives at middle age. Journal of Family
Issues, 31(1), 113–130.
Eickmeyer, K. J. (2016). Over 25 years of change in men’s entry into fatherhood 1987–2013. Family profiles. Bowling
Green, OH: National Center for Family and Marriage Research.
Elder, G H, Jr, Shanahan, M. l, and Jennings, A. (2015) Human development in time and in place. In T. Leventhal,
and M. Bornstein, (Eds.). Handbook of child psychology developmental science: Ecological settings developmental pro-
cesses in systems (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley Sons
Elliott, G. C. (2009). Family matters: The importance of mattering to family in adolescence. West Sussex, United King-
dom: John Wiley & Sons.
Elliott, G. C., Cunningham, S. M., Colangelo, M., and Gelles, R. J. (2011). Perceived mattering to the family and
physical violence within the family by adolescents. Journal of Family Issues, 32, 1007–1029.
Erikson, E. (1975). Life history and the historical moment. New York: W. W. Norton.
Everett, K. D., Gage, J., Bullock, L., Longo, D. R., Geden, E., and Madsen, R. W. (2005). A pilot study of smok-
ing and associated behaviors of low-income expectant fathers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 7, 269–276.
Fabricius, W. V., Braver, S. L., Diaz, P., and Velez, C. E. (2010). Custody and parenting time: Links to family
relationships and well-being after divorce. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th
ed., pp. 201–240). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Fagan, J., and Barnett, M. (2003). The relationship between maternal gatekeeping, paternal competence, moth-
ers’ attitudes about the father role, and father involvement. Journal of Family Issues, 24, 1020–1043.

123
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Fauber, R. L., and Long, N. (1991). Children in context: The role of the family in child psychotherapy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 813–820.
Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; Physiological precursors,
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 329–354.
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Schneiderman, I., Weisman, O., and Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2010). Natural variations
in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent–infant
contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 1133–1141.
Feldman, R. (2016). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 965–970.
Feldman, R. (2019). The social neuroendocrinology of human parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting. (3rd ed., pp. 220–249) New York: Routledge.
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Influs, M., Gutbir, T., and Ebstein, R. P. (2013). Parental oxytocin and early caregiving
jointly shape children’s oxytocin response and social reciprocity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38(7), 1154–1162.
Field, T. M. (1978). Interaction behaviors of primary versus secondary caretaker fathers. Developmental Psychology,
14, 183–185.
Finlay, A. K., Cookston, J. T., Saenz, D. S., Baham, M. E., Parke, R. D., Fabricius, W. V., and Braver, S. L. (2014).
Attributions of fathering behaviors among adolescents: The role of depressive symptoms, ethnicity, and fam-
ily structure. Journal of Family Issues, 35, 501–525.
Flanders, J. L., Leo, V., Paquette, D., Pihl, R. O., and Séguin, J. R. (2009). Rough-and-tumble play and the
regulation of aggression: An observational study of father-child play dyads. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 285–295.
Flanders, J. L., Simard, M., Paquette, D., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Pihl, R. O., and Séguin, J. R. (2010). Rough-and-
tumble play and the development of physical aggression and emotion regulation: A five-year follow-up
study. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 357–367.
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., and Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin are associated with
emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 399–413.
Flittner O’Grady, A. E., Burton, T., Chawla, N., Topp, D., and MacDermid Wadsworth, S. M. (2016). Evaluation
of a multimedia intervention for children and families facing multiple military deployments. The Journal of
Primary Prevention, 37, 53–70.
Flouri, E. (2004). Fathering and child outcomes. London, UK: Wiley
Flouri, E. and Buchanan, A. (2002). Childhood predictors of labor force participation in adult life. Journal of Fam-
ily and Economic Issues, 23, 101–120
Fomby, P., and Cherlin, A. J. (2007). Family instability and child well-being. American Sociological Review, 72, 181–204.
Franz, C. E., McClelland, D., and Weinberger, J. (1991). Childhood antecedents of conventional social accom-
plishment in midlife adults: A 26-year prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.
Friedman, H., and Martin, L. R. (2011). The longevity project: Surprising discoveries for health and long life from the
landmark eight decade study. New York: Hudson Street Press.
Furstenberg, F. F. (1988). Good dads–bad dads: Two faces of fatherhood. In A. J. Cherlin (Ed.), The changing
American family and public policy (pp. 193–218). Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press.
Furstenberg, F. F. (2014). Fifty years of change: From consensus to complexity. The Annals of the American Acad-
emy of Political and Social Sciences, 654, 12–30.
Furstenberg, F. F., Cook, T. D., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H. Jr., and Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make it: Urban
families and adolescent success. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Furstenberg, F. F., and Harris, K. M. (1993). When and why fathers matter: Impacts of father involvement on
children of adolescent mothers. In R. I. Lerman and T. J. Ooms (Eds.), Young unwed fathers (pp. 117–138).
Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Gadsden, V. L. (1999). Black families in intergenerational and cultural perspective. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Parenting
and child development in “nontraditional” families (pp. 221–246). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Gamble, W. C., Ramakumar, S., and Diaz, A. (2007). Maternal and paternal similarities and differences in par-
enting: An examination of Mexican-American parents of young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
22, 72–88.
Ganong, L., Coleman, M., and Sanner, C. (2019). Divorced and remarried parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 311–344). New York: Routledge.
Garfield, C. F., Isacco, A., and Bartlo, W. D. (2010). Men’s health and fatherhood in the urban midwestern
United States. International Journal of Men’s Health, 9, 161–174.
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., and Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood
decreases testosterone in human males. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 16194–16199.
Glass, J., and Owen, J. (2010). Latino fathers: The relationship among machismo, acculturation, ethnic identity,
and paternal involvement. Psychology of Men and & Masculinity, 11, 251–261.

124
Fathers and Families

Goldberg, A. E. (2010). Lesbian and gay parents and their children: Research on the family life cycle. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Goldberg, W. A., Tan, E. T., and Thorsen, K. (2009). Trends in academic attention to fathers, 1930–2006. Father-
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

ing: A Journal of Research, Theory, and Practice, 7, 159–179.


Goldberg, W. A. (2014). Father time: The social clock and the timing of fatherhood. London: Palgrave-MacMillan.
Goodman, W. B., Crouter, A. C., Lanza, S. T., and Cox, M. J. (2008). Paternal work characteristics and father-
infant interaction quality in low-income, rural families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 640–653.
Goodman, W. B., Crouter, A. C., Lanza, S. T., Cox, M. J., Vernon-Feagans, L., and the Family Life Project Key
Investigators. (2011). Paternal work stress and latent profiles of father-infant parenting quality. Journal of Mar-
riage and the Family, 73, 588–604.
Glauber, R. (2008). Race and gender in families and at work: The fatherhood wage premium. Gender and Society,
22, 8–30.
Glaze, L. E., and Maruschak, L. M. (2008). Parents in prison and their minor children. Bureau of Justice Statistics
Special Report. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice
Statistics.
Golombok, S. (2015). Modern families. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Golombok, S. (2019). Parenting and contemporary reproductive technologies. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 482–512). New York: Routledge.
Goodnow, J. J. (1999). From household practices to parents’ ideas about work and interpersonal relationships. In
S. Harkness, C. Super, and R. Niew (Eds.), Parenting ethnotheories. New York: Guilford.
Goodnow, J. J., and Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources and consequences of
parents ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Goodnow, J. J., and Lawrence, J. A. (2001). Work contributions to the family: Developing a conceptual and
research framework. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 94, 5–22.
Gornick, J. C., and Meyers, M. K. (2003). Families that work: Policies for reconciling parenthood and employment. New
York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., and Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Gray, P. B., and Anderson, K. G. (2010). Fatherhood: Evolution and human paternal behavior Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press.
Greenberger, E., O’Neil, R., and Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking workplace and home place: Relations between the
nature of adults work and their parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 30, 990–1002.
Grimm-Thomas, K., and Perry-Jenkins, M. (1994). All in a day’s work: Job experiences, self-esteem, and father-
ing in working-class families. Family Relations. 43, 174–181.
Grossman, F. K. (1987). Separate and together: Men’s autonomy and affiliation in the transition to parenthood.
In P. W. Berman and F. A. Pedersen (Eds.), Men’s transition to parenthood (pp. 89–112). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Grossmann, F. K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheurer-Englisch, H., and Zimmer-
mann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challeng-
ing play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year-long study. Social Development, 11, 307–331.
Grossman, F. K., Pollack, W. S., and Golding, E. (1988). Fathers and children: Predicting the quality and quantity
of fathers. Developmental Psychology, 24, 82–91.
Grotevant, H. D., and McDermott, J. M. (2014). Adoption: Biological and social processes linked to adaptation.
Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 235–266.
Grych, J. H., and Clark, R. (1999). Maternal employment and development of the father-infant relationship.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 893–903.
Grych, J. H., and Fincham, F. D. (1993). Children’s appraisals of marital conflict: Initial investigations of the
cognitive-contextual framework. Child Development, 64, 215–230.
Grzywacz, J. G., and Carlson, D. S. (2007). Conceptualizing work-family balance: Implications for practice and
research. Advances in Developing Human Resources, 9, 455–471.
Gueron-Sela, N., Atzaba-Poria, N., Meiri, G., and Marks, K. (2015). The caregiving environment and develop-
mental outcomes of preterm infants: Diathesis stress or differential susceptibility effects? Child Development,
86, 1014–1030.
Guzzo, K. B. (2014). Trends in cohabitation outcomes: Compositional changes and engagement among never-
married young adults. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 76, 826–842.
Guzzo, K. B., and Furstenberg, F. F. Jr. (2007). Multipartnered fertility among American men. Demography, 44,
583–601.

125
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Han, W-J., and Fox, L. E. (2011) Parental work schedules and children’s cognitive trajectories. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 73, 962–980.
Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., and McNeilly-Choque, M. K. (1998). Overt and rela-
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

tional aggression in Russian nursery-school-age children: Parenting style and marital linkages. Developmental
Psychology, 34, 687–697.
Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., McNeilly-Choque, M. K., Porter, C. L., and McKee,
T. R. (2000). Russian parenting styles and family processes: Linages with subtypes of victimization and
aggression. In K. A. Kerns, J. M. Contreras, and A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Family and peers: Linking two social
worlds. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
Hawkins, A. J., and Dollahite, D. C. (Eds.). (1997). Generative fathering: Beyond deficit perspectives. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Heath, D. H. (1976). Competent fathers: Their personalities and marriages. Human Development, 19, 26–39.
Heath, D. B. (1977). Some possible effects of occupation on the maturing professional man. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 11, 263–281.
Heath, D. H., and Heath, H. E. (1991). Fulfilling lives: Paths to maturity and success. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Hewlett, B., and Macfarlan, S. (2010). Fathers’ roles in Hunter-Gatherer and other small scale cultures. In
M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 413–434). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons, Inc.
Hodges, M., and Budig, M. J. (2010). Who gets the daddy bonus? Markers of hegemonic masculinity and the
impact of first-time fatherhood on men’s earnings. Gender and Society, 24, 717–745.
Hofferth, S. (1999, April). Race/ethnic differences in further involvement with young children: A conceptual framework and
empirical test in two- parent families. Paper presented at the Urban Seminar on Fatherhood, Harvard University,
Cambridge, MA.
Hofferth, S. (2003). Residential father family type and child well-being: Investment versus selection. Demography,
43, 53–77.
Hofferth, S., and Goldscheider, F. (2010). Does change in young men’s employment influence fathering? Family
Relations, 59, 479–493.
Hofferth, S. L., Pleck, J. H., and Vesely, C. K. (2012). The transmission of parenting from fathers to sons. Parenting,
Science and Practice, 12, 282–305.
Hofferth, S. L., Pleck, J. H., Goldscheider, F., Curtin, S., and Hrapczynski, K. (2013). Family structure and men’s
motivation for parenthood in the United States. In N. J. Cabrera and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook
of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 57–80). New York: Routledge.
Holmes, E. K., Cowan, P. C., Cowan, C. P., and Hawkins, A. (2013). Marriage, fatherhood, and parenting pro-
gramming. In N. J. Cabrera and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Ed.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary
perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 438–454). New York: Routledge.
Holmes, E. K., and Huston, A. C. (2010). Understanding positive father-child interaction: Children’s, fathers’,
and mothers’ contributions. Fathering, 8, 203–225.
Hossain, Z., and Roopnarine, J. L. (1994). African-American fathers’ involvement with infants: Relationship to
their functioning style, support, education, and income. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 175–184.
Hughes, F. (1999). Children, play, and development (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Hwang, P. (1987). The changing role of Swedish fathers. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father’s role: Cross cultural
perspectives, Hillsdale, NJ: Erblaum.
Ibrahim, M. H., Somers, J. A., Luecken, L. L., Fabricius, W. V., and Cookston, J. T. (2017). Father-adolescent
engagement in shared activities: Effects on cortisol stress response in young adulthood. Journal of Family
Psychology, 31, 485–494.
Ishii-Kuntz, M. (2009). Paternal roles and child care: Current conditions, determinants, and its consequences on
families. Japanese Journal of Research on Household Economics, 81, 16–23.
Jankowiak, W. (2010). Father and child relations in urban China. In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.), Parent-youth relations
(Vol. 1, pp. 345–363). New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine.
Jain, A., and Belsky, J. (1997). Fathering and acculturation: Immigrant Indian families with young children. Jour-
nal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 873–883.
Kaczynski, K. J., Lindahl, K. M., Malik, N. M., and Laurenceau, J. P. (2006). Marital conflict, maternal and
paternal parenting, and child adjustment: A test of mediation and moderation. Journal of Family Psychology,
20, 199–208.
Kan, M. Y., Sullivan, O., and Gershuny, J. (2011). Gender convergence in domestic work: Discerning the effects
of interactional and institutional barriers from large-scale data. Sociology, 45, 234–251.
Katz, L. F., and Gottman, J. M. (1991). Marital discord and child outcomes: A social psychophysiological approach. In
K. Dodge and J. Garber (Eds.), Emotion regulation in young children. New York: Cambridge University
Press.

126
Fathers and Families

Katz, L. F., and Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children’s internalizing and external-
izing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940–950.
Katz, L. F., Wilson, B., and Gottman, J. M., (1999). Meta-emotion philosophy and family adjustment: Making
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

an emotional connection. In M. J. Cox and J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Conflict and cohesion in families: Causes and
consequences. The advances in family research series, 131–165. Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.
Keller, P. S., Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., and Petersen, K. (2009). Marital conflict in the context of parental depres-
sive symptoms: Implications for the development of children’s adjustment problems. Social Development, 18, 536–555.
Kelly, R. F., Redenbach, L., and Rinaman, W. (2005). Determinants of sole and joint physical custody arrange-
ments in a national sample of divorces. American Journal of Family Law, 19, 25–43.
Kerig, P. K. (1996). Assessing the links between interparental conflict and child adjustment: The conflicts and
problem-solving scales. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 454–473.
Kim, S. W., and Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: A meta-analysis of parental involvement and
students’ academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 919–934.
Kim, P., Rigo, P., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Leckman, J. F., and Swain, J. E. (2014). Neural plasticity in fathers
of human infants. Social neuroscience, 9, 522–535.
Kitzmann, K. M. (2000). Effects of marital conflict on subsequent triadic family interactions and parenting.
Developmental Psychology, 36, 3-13.
Knoester, C., and Eggebeen, D. J. (2006). The effects of transition to parenthood and subsequent children on
men’s well-being and social participation. Journal of Family Issues, 27, 1532–1560.
Knoester, C., Petts, R., and Eggebeen, D. J. (2007). Commitments to fathering and the well being and social
participation of new fathers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, 991–1004.
Knox, V., Cowan, P. A., Pape Cowan, C., and Bildner, E. (2011). Policies that strengthen fatherhood and family
relationships: What do we know and what do we need to know? Annals of the American Academy of Political
and Social Science, 635, 216–239.
Kohler, H. P., Behrman, J. R., and Skytthe, A. (2005). Partner + Children = Happiness? The effects of partner-
ships and fertility on well-being. Population and Development Review, 31, 407–445.
Kohn, M. L., and Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry into the impact of social stratification. Nor-
wood, NJ: Ablex.
Koestner, R., Franz, C., and Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins of empathic concern: A 26-year longitu-
dinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.
Krishnakumar, A., and Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conflict and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic
review. Family Relations, 49, 25–44.
Kromelow, S., Harding, C., and Touris, M. (1990). The role of the father in the development of stranger sociabil-
ity during the second year. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 521–530.
LaBounty, J., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S., Lagattuta, K., and Liu, D. (2008). Mothers’ and fathers’ use of internal
state talk with their young children. Social Development, 17, 757–775.
Ladd, G. W., Profilet, S. M., and Hart, C. H. (1992). Parents management of children’s peer relations: Facilitating
and supervising children’s activities in the peer culture. In R. D. Parke and G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family-peer
relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 215–254). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Ladd, G. W. (2005). Children’s peer relations and social competence: A century of progress. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni-
versity Press.
Lamb, M. E., and Lewis, C. (2010). The development and significance of father—child relationships in two-
parent families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 94–153). New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., and Levine, J. A. (1985). The role of the father in child development: The effects of
increased paternal involvement. In B. Lahey and E. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol.
8). New York: Plenum.
Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J., Charnov, E. L., and Levine, J. A. (1987). A biosocial perspective on paternal behavior and
involvement. In J. B. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. Rossi, and L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the life span:
Biosocial perspectives. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Landreth, G. L., and Lobaugh, A. F. (1998). Filial therapy with incarcerated fathers: Effects on parental acceptance
of child, parental stress, and child adjustment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 76, 157–165.
Lapp, L.M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., and Cummings, E. M. (2004) Mothers’ and fathers’ psychological symptoms
and marital functioning: Examination of direct and interactive links with child adjustment. Journal of Child
and Family Studies, 13, 469–482.
LaRossa, R. (1997). The modernization of fatherhood. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
LaRossa, R. (2012). The historical study of fatherhood: Theoretical and methodological considerations. In M.
Oechsle, U. Müller, and S. Hess (Eds.), Fatherhood in late modernity: Cultural images, social practices, structural
frames. Germany: Barbara Budrich Publishers.

127
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Larson, R., and Richards, M. (1994). Divergent realities. New York: Basic Books.
Larson, R. W., and Sheeber, L. (2007). The daily emotional experience of adolescents. In N. Allen and L.
Sheeber (Eds.), Adolescent emotional development and the emergence of depressive disorders. New York: Cambridge
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

University Press.
Lau, E. Y. H. (2016). A mixed methods study of parental involvement in Hong Kong. British Educational Research
Journal, 42, 1023–1040.
LaVigne, N. G., Naser, R. L., Brooks, L. E., and Castro, J. L. (2005). Examining the effect of incarceration and in-
prison family contact on prisoners’ family relationships. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 21, 314–355.
Lawlor, D. A., Emberson, J. R., Ebrahim, S., Whincup, P. H., Wannamethee, S. G., Walker, M., and Smith, G. D.
(2003). Is the association between parity and coronary heart disease due to biological effects of pregnancy
or adverse lifestyle risk factors associated with child-rearing? Findings from the British Women’s Heart and
Health Study and the British Regional Heart Study. Circulation, 107, 1260–1264.
Leadbeater, B. J. (2014). Growing up fast: Re-Visioning adolescent mothers’ transitions to young adulthood. New York:
Psychology Press.
Leavell, A. S., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ruble, D. N., Zosuls, K. M., and Cabrera, N. J. (2012). African American,
White, and Latino fathers activities with their sons and daughters in early childhood. Sex Roles, 66, 53–65.
Lee, C. Y., and Doherty, W. J. (2007). Marital satisfaction and father involvement during the transition to parent-
hood. Fathering, 5, 75–96.
Lee, S. J., and Walsh, T. B. (2015). Using technology in social work practice: The mDad (Mobile Device Assisted
Dad) case study. Advances in Social Work, 16, 107–124.
Le Camus, J. (1995). Pères et bébés. Paris: L’Harmattan.
Leidy, M. S., Parke, R. D, Cladis, M., Coltrane, S., and Duffy, S. (2009). Positive marital quality, acculturative stress,
and child outcomes among Mexican Americans. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 71, 833–847.
Leidy, M. S., Schofield, T., Miller, M., Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., Cookston, J., Braver, S., Fabricius, W., Saenez,
D., and Adams, M. (2011). Fathering and adolescent adjustment: Variations by family structure and ethnic
background. Fathering, 9, 44–68.
Leidy, M., Schofield, T., and Parke, R. D. (2013). Fathers’ contribution to children’s social development (2nd
ed.) In C. Tamis-LeMonda and N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement (pp. 151–167). New York:
Taylor & Francis.
Lerman, R. I. (1993). A national profile of young unwed fathers. In R. I. Lerman and T. J. Ooms (Eds.), Young
unwed fathers (pp. 27–51). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Lerman, R., and Sorensen, E. (2000). Father involvement with their nonmarital children: Patterns, determinants,
and effects on their earnings. Marriage and Family Review, 29, 137–158.
Lester, P., Aralis, H., Sinclair, M., Kiff, C., Lee, K. H., Mustillo, S., and Wadsworth, S. M. (2016). The impact of
deployment on parental, family and child adjustment in military families. Child Psychiatry and Human Devel-
opment, 47, 938–949.
Lester, P., and Flake, L. C. E. (2013). How wartime military service affects children and families. The Future of
Children, 23, 121–141.
Levant, R. (1988). Education for fatherhood. In P. Bronstein and C. P. Cowan (Eds.), Fatherhood today (pp. 253–
275). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Leventhal, T., Dupéré, V., and Shuey, E. A. (2015). Children in neighborhoods In R. M. Lerner (Editor in chief ),
M. Bornstein, and T. Leventhal (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
Li, X., and Lamb, M. E. (2013). Chinese culture: From stern disciplinarians to involved parents. In D. W. Shwalb,
B. J. Shwalb, and M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Fathers in cultural context. New York: Routledge.
Li, M., and Fleming, A. S. (2019). Psychobiology of maternal behavior in non-human animals. In M. Bornstein
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (3rd ed., pp. 30–77). New York: Routledge.
Little, M., Handley, L., Leuthe, E., and Chassin, L. (2009). Substance use and early parenthood in a high risk
sample. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 661–682.
Loper, A. B., Carlson, L. W., Levitt, L., and Scheffel, K. (2009). Parenting stress, alliance, child contact, and adjust-
ment of imprisoned mothers and fathers. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 48, 483–503
Losh-Hesselbart, S. (1987). The development of gender roles. In M. B. Sussman and S. Steinmetz (Eds.), Hand-
book of marriage and family (pp. 535–563). New York: Plenum.
Low, S. M., and Stocker, C. (2005). Family functioning and children’s adjustment: Associations among parents’
depressed mood, marital hostility, parent-child hostility, and children’s adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology,
19, 394–403.
Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

128
Fathers and Families

Maccoby, E. E., and Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In P.
H. Mussen and E. M. Hetherington (Eds.). Handbook of child Psychology (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.
MacDonald, K., and Parke, R. D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent-child play interaction and peer interactive
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

competence. Child Development, 55, 1265–1277.


MacDonald, K., and Parke, R. D. (1986). Parent-child physical play: The effects of sex and age of children and
parents. Sex Roles, 7–8, 367–379.
Main, M., and Weston, D. (1981). The quality of the toddler’s relationship to mother and to father: Related to
conflict behavior and the readiness to establish new relationships. Child Development, 52, 932–940.
Mansfield, A. J., Kaufman, J. S., Engel, C. C., and Gaynes, B. N. (2011). Deployment and mental health diagnoses
among children of US Army personnel. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 165, 999–1005.
Maruschak, L. M., Glaze, L. E., and Mumola, C. J. (2010). Incarcerated parents and their children. In J. M. Eddy
and J. Poehlmann (Eds.), Children of incarcerated parents: A handbook for researchers and practitioners (pp. 33–51).
Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press.
Marshall, S. K. (2001). Do I matter? Construct validation of adolescents’ perceived mattering to parents and
friends. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 473–490.
Marshall, S. K. (2004). Relative contributions of perceived mattering to parents and friends in predicting adoles-
cents’ psychological well-being. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 591–601.
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. D., and Lamb, M. E. (2000). Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and
beyond. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1173–1191.
Martin, A., Ryan, R. M, and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). The joint influence of mother and father parenting on
child cognitive outcomes at age 5. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 423–439.
Mazzucato, V., and Schans, D. (2011). Transnational families and the well-being of children: Conceptual and
methodological challenges. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 704–712.
McBride, B. A. (1989). Stress and fathers’ parental competence: Implications for family life and parent educators.
Family Relations, 38, 385–389.
McBride, B. A., and Rane, T. R. (1997). Role identity, role investments, and paternal involvement: Implications
for parenting programs for men. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 173–197.
McBride, B. A., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., and Ho, M. (2005). The mediating role of fathers’ school involvement
on student achievement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 201–216.
McBride, B. A., Schoppe, S. J., and Rane, T. R. (2002). Child characteristics, parenting stress, and parental
involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 998–1011.
McDermid, S. M., Huston, T., and McHale, S. (1990). Changes in marriage associated with the transition to par-
enthood: Individual differences as a function of sex-role attitudes and changes in the division of household
labor. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 475–486.
McLanahan, S., and Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single parent: What hurts, what helps. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
MacDermid Wadsworth, S. (2013). Understanding and supporting the resilience of a new generation
of combat-exposed military families and their children. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review,
16, 415–420.
MacDermid Wadsworth, S. M., Cardin, J. F., Christ, S., Willerton, E., Flittner O’Grady, A., Topp, D., Coppola,
E., Lester, P., and Mustillo, S. (2016). Accumulation of risk and promotive factors among young children in
US military families. American Journal of Community Psychology, 57, 190–202
McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., O’Neil, R., and Parke, R. D. (2002). Children’s emotional regulation and social
competence in middle childhood: The role of maternal and paternal interactive style. Marriage and Family
Review, 34(3–4), 345–364.
McDowell, D. J., and Parke, R. D. (2000). Differential knowledge of display rules for positive and negative emo-
tions: Influences from parents, influences on peers. Social Development, 9, 415–432.
McDowell, D. J., and Parke, R. D. (2009). Family correlates of children’s peer relationships: An empirical test of
a tripartite model. Developmental Psychology, 45, 224–235.
McDowell, D. J., Parke, R. D., and Spitzer, S. (2002). Parent and child cognitive representations of social situa-
tions and children’s social competence. Social Development, 11(4), 469–486.
McFadden, K., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Howard, K., Shannon, J. D., and Cabrera, N. (2009, April). Old roads, new
paths: Men’s childhood relationships with their fathers and their current parenting. Presentation for the Society for
Research in Child Development conference.
McHale, J. P., and Lindahl, K. (2011). Coparenting: A conceptual and clinical examination of family systems. Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., and Whiteman, S. (2003). The family contexts of gender development in child-
hood and adolescence. Social Development, 12, 125–148.

129
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

McWayne, C., Downer, J. T., Campos, R., and Harris, R. D. (2013). Father involvement during early child-
hood and its association with children’s early learning: A meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 24,
898–922.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Milkie, M. A., and Denny, K. E. (2014). Changes in the cultural involvement of father involvement: Descriptions of
benefits to fathers, children and mothers in Parents’ Magazine, 1926–2006. Journal of Family Issues, 35, 223–253.
Miller, B. G., Kors, S., and Macfie, J. (2017). No differences? Meta-analytic comparisons of psychological adjust-
ment in children of gay fathers and heterosexual parents. Psychology of Sexual Orientation And Gender Diver-
sity, 4, 14–22.
Miller, D. R., and Swanson, G. E. (1958). The changing American parent. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Millikovsky-Ayalon, M., Atzaba-Poria, N. and Meiri, G. (2015). The role of the father in child sleep disturbance:
Child, parent, and parent-child relationship. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36, 114–127.
Mirande, A. (2008). Immigration and Latino fatherhood: A preliminary look at Latino immigrant and non-
immigrant fathers. In S. Chuang and R. P. Moreno (Eds.), Understanding immigrant fathers in North America
(pp. 217–229). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books & Rowman and Littlefield.
Mitchell, S., and Cabrera, N. (2009). An exploratory study of fathers’ parenting stress and toddlers’ social devel-
opment in low-income African American families. Fathering, 7, 201–225.
Mitnick, D. M., Heyman, R. E., and Smith Slep, A. M. (2009). Changes in relationship satisfaction across the
transition to parenthood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 848–852.
Mize, J., and Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers social coaching, mother-child relationship style and children’s peer
competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312–332.
Muller, M. N., Marlowe, F. W., Bugumba, R., and Ellison, P. T. (2009). Testosterone and paternal care in East
African foragers. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 276, 347–354.
Murphey, D., and Cooper, P. M. (2015, October). Parents behind bars: What happens to their children. Washington,
DC: Child Trends.
Murray, J., and Farrington, D. P. (2008). Parental imprisonment: Long-lasting effects on boys’ internalizing prob-
lems through the life-course. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 273–290.
Murray, J., Farrington, D. P., and Sekol, I. (2012). Children’s antisocial behavior, mental health, drug use, and
educational performance after parental incarceration: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 138, 175–210.
Mustillo, S., Wadsworth, S. M., and Lester, P. (2015). Parental deployment and well-being in children: Results
from a new study of military families. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 24, 82–91.
Myers, B. J., Smarsh, T. M., Amlund-Hagen, K., and Kennon, S. (1999). Children of incarcerated mothers. Journal
of Child and Family Studies, 8, 11–25.
Nangle, S. M., Kelley, M. L., Fals-Stewart, W., and Levant, R. F. (2003). Work and family variables as related to
paternal engagement, responsibility, and accessibility in dual-earner couples with young children. Fathering,
1, 71–90.
Nakazawa, J., and Shwalb, D. W. (2013). Fathering in Japan: Entering an era of involvement with children. In D.
W. Shwalb, B. J. Shwalb, and M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Fathers in cultural context (pp. 42–66). New York: Routledge.
Neville, B., and Parke, R. D. (1997). Waiting for paternity: Interpersonal and contextual implications of the tim-
ing of fatherhood. Sex Roles, 37, 45–59.
New, R., and Benigini, L. (1987). Italian fathers and infants: Cultural constraints on paternal behavior. In M. E.
Lamb (Ed.), The father role: Cross-cultural perspectives. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2008). Family-peer linkages: The mediational role of attentional
processes. Social Development, 18, 875–895.
Nobles, J. (2011). Parenting from abroad: Migration, nonresident father involvement, and children’s education in
Mexico. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 729–746.
Notaro, P. C., and Volling, B. L. (1999). Parental responsiveness and infant–parent attachment: A replication
study with fathers and mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 345–352.
O’Brien, M., and Moss, P. (2010). Fathers, work and family policies in Europe. In M. L. Lamb (Ed.), The role of
the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 551–577) Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Osborne, C., and McLanahan, S. (2007). Partnership instability and child well-being. Journal of Marriage and
Family, 69, 1065–1083.
Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father-child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human
Development, 47, 193–219.
Palkovitz, R. (1997). Reconstructing involvement: Expanding conceptualizations of men’s caring in contem-
porary families. In A. J. Hawkins and D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative fathering: Beyond deficit perspectives
(pp. 200–216). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Palkovitz, R. (2002). Involved fathering and men’s adult development: Provisional balances. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

130
Fathers and Families

Palkovitz, R., and Palm, G. (1998). Fatherhood and faith in formation: The developmental effects of fathering
on religiosity, morals and values. Journal of Men’s Studies, 7, 33–51.
Palkovitz, R., and Palm, G. (2009). Transitions within fatherhood. Fathering, 7, 3–22.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Papp, L. M., Cummings, E. M., and Schermerhorn, A. (2004). Pathways among marital distress, parental symp-
tomatology, and child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 368–384.
Parcel, T. L., and Menaghan, E. G. (1994). Parents’ jobs and children’s lives. New York: Aldine.
Parke, R. D. (1981). Fathers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Parke, R. D. (1988). Families in life-span perspective: A multilevel developmental approach. In E. M. Hethering-
ton, R. M. Lerner, and M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Child development in life-span perspective (pp. 159–190). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Parke, R. D. (2000). Father involvement: A developmental psychological perspective. Marriage and Family Review,
29, 43–58.
Parke, R. D. (2002). Parenting in the new millennium: Prospects, promises and pitfalls. In J. P. McHale and W. S.
Grolnick (Eds.), Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of families (pp. 65–93). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Parke, R. D. (2013). Future families: Diverse forms, rich possibilities. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Parke, R. D., and Brott, A. (1999). Throwaway dads. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.
Parke, R. D., and Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon
and R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) and N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.). New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Parke, R. D., Cassidy, J., Burks, V. M., Carson, J. L., and Boyum, L. (1992). Family contribution to peer compe-
tence among young children: The role of interactive and affective processes. In R. Parke and G. Ladd (Eds.),
Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., Power, J., Adams, M., Fabricius, W., Braver, S., and Saenz, D. (2004). Measurement of
father involvement in Mexican American families. In R. Day (Ed.), Measuring father involvement. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Parke, R. D., and Elder, G. H. Jr. (Eds.). (in press). Children in changing worlds: Socio-cultural and temporal perspectives.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Parke, R. D., Hymel, S., Power, T. G., and Tinsley, B. R. (1980). Fathers and risk: A hospital-based model of
intervention. In D. B. Sawin, R. D. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticuff (Eds.), Psychosocial risks in
infant-environment transactions (Vol. 4, pp. 174–189). New York: Bruner/Mazel.
Parke, R. D., Killian, C., Dennis, J., Flyr, M., McDowell, D. J., Simpkins, S., Kim, M., and Wild, M. (2003). Man-
aging the external environment: The parent and child as active agents in the system. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.),
Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Parke, R. D., Kim, M., Flyr, M., McDowell, D. J., Simpkins, S. D., Killian, C. M., and Wild, M. (2001). Managing
marital conflict: Links with children’s peer relationships. In J. Grych and F. Fincham (Eds.), Child development
and interparental conflict (pp. 291–314). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Parke, R. D., MacDonald, K., Beitel, A., and Bhavangri, N. (1988). The interrelationships among families, fathers
and peers. In R. Peters and R. J. McMahon (Eds.), Social learning and systems approaches to marriage and the
family. New York: Bruner/Mazel.
Parke, R. D., McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., Killian, C., Dennis, D., Flyr, M. L., and Wild, M. (2002). Fathers’ con-
tributions to children’s peer relationships. In C. S. Tamis-LeMonda and N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father
involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 141–167). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Parke, R. D., McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., and Leidy, M. S. (2006). Family-peer relationships: The role of emotional
regulatory processes. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, and J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotional regulation in families:
Pathways to dysfunction and health. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Parke, R. D., Power, T. G., and Gottman, J. M. (1979). Conceptualization and quantifying influence patterns in
the family triad. In M. E. Lamb, S. J. Suomi, and G. R. Stephenson (Eds.), Social interaction analysis: Methodologi-
cal issues. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Parke, R. D., and Sawin, D. B. (1976). The father’s role in infancy: A reevaluation. The Family Coordinator, 25,
365–371.
Parke, R. D., and Sawin, D. B. (1980). The family in early infancy: Social interactional and attitudinal analyses. In
F. A. Pedersen (Ed.), The father-infant relationship: Observational studies in the family setting. New York: Praeger.
Parke, R. D., and Stearns, P. N. (1993). Fathers and child rearing. In G. H. Elder, J. Modell, and R. D. Parke (Eds.),
Children in time and place. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Parke, R. D., and Suomi, S. J. (1981). Adult male-infant relationships: Human and nonhuman primate evidence.
In K. Immelmann, G. Barlow, M. Main, and L. Petrinovich (Eds.), Behavioral development: The Bielefeld Inter-
disciplinary Project (pp. 700–725). New York: Cambridge University Press.

131
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Patterson, C. J. (2016). Parents’ sexual orientation and children’s development. Child Development Perspectives, 11,
45–49.
Paulson, R. J., and Sachs, J. (1999). Rewinding your biological clock: Motherhood late in life Options, issues, and emotions.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

New York: W. H. Freeman and Co.


Pedersen, F. A., Anderson, B. J., Cain, R. L., Jr. (1980). Parent-infant and husband-wife interactions observed at
age five months. In F. A. Pedersen (Ed.), The father-infant relationship. New York: Praeger.
Pelham, W. E., and Lang, A. R. (1999). Can your children drive you to drink? Alcohol Research and Health, 23,
293–298.
Perry-Jenkins, M., Repetti, R. L., and Crouter, A. C. (2000). Work and family in the 1990’s. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 62, 981–998.
The Pew Charitable Trusts. (2010). Collateral costs: Incarceration’s effect on economic mobility. Washington, DC: The
Pew Charitable Trusts.
Pew Research Center. (2015, September 15). Modern immigration wave brings 59 million to U.S., driving
population growth and change through 2065. In views of immigration’s impact on U.S. society mixed. Washing-
ton, DC: Pew Charitable Trusts.
Pike, A., Coldwell, J., and Dunn, J. F. (2005). Sibling relationships in early/middle childhood: Links with indi-
vidual adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 523–32.
Pinderhughes, E., and Brodinsky, D. (2019). Parenting adopted infants. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parent-
ing Vol. 1: Children and parenting, (3rd ed., pp. 322–367). New York: Routledge.
Plattner, S., and Minturn, L. (1975). A comparative and longitudinal study of the behavior of communally raised
children. Ethos, 3, 469–480.
Pleck, E. (2004). Two dimensions of fatherhood: A history of the good dad–bad dad complex. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (4th ed., pp. 32–57). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Pleck, J. H. (2010). Paternal involvement. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed.,
pp. 58–93). Hoboken, MA: John Wiley & Sons.
Pleck, J. H., and Masciadrelli, B. P. (2004). Paternal involvement by US residential fathers: Levels, sources, and
consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (4th ed., pp. 222–271). Hoboken,
MA: John Wiley & Sons.
Poehlmann, J. (2005). Representations of attachment relationships in children of incarcerated mothers. Child
Development, 76, 679–696.
Poehlmann-Tynan, J. (2014). Children’s contact with their incarcerated parents: Summary and recommenda-
tions. In J. Poehlmann-Tynan (Ed.), Children’s contact with incarcerated parents: Implications for policy and interven-
tion. New York: Springer.
Poehlmann-Tynan, J., Burnson, C., Runion, H., and Weymouth, L. A. (2017). Attachment in young children
with incarcerated fathers. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 389–404.
Poehlmann, J., Dallaire, D., Loper, A. B., and Shear, L. D. (2010). Children’s contact with their incarcerated par-
ents: Research findings and recommendations. The American Psychologist, 65, 575–598.
Poehlmann-Tynan, J., Runion, H., Burnson, C., Maleck, S., Weymouth, L., Pettit, K., and Huser, M. (2014).
Young children’s behavioral and emotional reactions to plexiglass and video visits with jailed parents. In
J. Poehlmann-Tynan (Ed.), Children’s contact with incarcerated parents: Implications for policy and intervention:
Advances in child and family policy and practice. New York: Springer.
Poehlmann, J., Shlafer, R. J., Maes, E., and Hanneman, A. (2008). Factors associated with young children’s oppor-
tunities for maintaining family relationships during maternal incarceration. Family Relations, 57, 267–280.
Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., and Cossotte, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes
in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22, 359–367.
Power, T. G., and Parke, R. D. (1982). Play as a context for early learning: Lab and home analyses. In I. E. Sigel
and L. M. Laosa (Eds.), The family as a learning environment. New York: Plenum.
Presser, H. (2004). Employment in a 24/7 economy. In A. Crouter and A. Booth (Eds.), Work-family challenges for
low-income parents and their children (pp. 83–106). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Pruett, K. D. (1987). The nurturing father. New York: Warner Books.
Pruett, K. D. (2000). Fatherneed: Why father care is as essential as mother care to your child. New York: Free Press.
Pruett, M. K., Pruett, K., Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (2017). Enhancing father involvement in low-income
families: A couples group approach to preventive intervention. Child Development, 88, 398–407.
Pruett, M. K., Williams, T. Y., Insabella, G., and Little, T. D. (2003). Family and legal indicators of child adjust-
ment to divorce among families with young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 169–180.
Pudrovska, T. (2009). Parenthood, stress, and psychological well-being in late midlife. International Journal of Aging
and Human Development, 68, 127–147.
Putnick, D. L., and Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Girls’ and boys’ labor and household chores in low- and middle-
income countries. In M. H. Bornstein, D. L. Putnick, J. E. Lansford, K. Deater-Deckard, and R. H. Bradley

132
Fathers and Families

(Eds.), Gender in low- and middle-income countries. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 81,
104–122.
Ramchandani, P. G., Stein, A., Evans, J., and O’Connor, T. G. (2005). Paternal depression in the postnatal period
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

and child development: A prospective population study. Lancet, 365, 2201–2205.


Radin, N. (1993). Primary caregiving fathers in intact families. In A. Gottfried and A. Gottfried (Eds.), Redefining
families. New York: Plenum.
Rah, Y., and Parke, R. D. (2008). Pathways between parent–child interactions and peer acceptance: The role of
children’s social information processing. Social Development, 17, 341–357.
Rane, T. R., and McBride, B. A. (2000). Identity theory as a guide to understanding fathers’ involvement with
their children. Journal of Family Issues, 21, 347–366.
Repetti, R. L. (1994). Short-term and long-term processes linking job stressors to father-child interaction. Social
Development, 3, 1–15.
Repetti, R. L. (2005). A psychological perspective on the health and well-being consequences of parental
employment. In S. M. Bianchi, L. M. Casper, and R. B. King (Eds.), Work, family, health and well-being (pp. 245–
258). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Rheingold, H. L., and Cook, K. V. (1975). The content of boys and girls rooms as an index of parent behavior.
Child Development, 46, 459–463.
Roggman, A., Boyce, T. K., Cook, G. A., Christiansen, K., and Jones, D. (2004). Playing with daddy: Social toy
play, early head start, and developmental outcomes. Fathering, 2, 83–109.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New york: Oxford University Press.
Roopnarine, J. L., and Hossain, Z. (2013). African American and African Caribbean fathers. In N. J. Cabrera and
C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 223–
243). New York: Routledge.
Rose, H. A., and Halverson, C. F. (1996). A transactional model of differential self-socialization of parenting.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Georgia, Athens, GA.
Rosenberg, M., and McCullough, B. C. (1979). Mattering: Inferred significance and mental health among ado-
lescents. Research in Community and Mental Health, 2, 163–182.
Ross, H., and Taylor, H. (1989). Do boys prefer daddy or his physical style of play? Sex Roles, 20, 23–33.
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. New York: Guilford.
Rotundo, E. A. (1993). American manhood. New York: Basic Books.
Roy, K. (2006). Father stories: A life course examination of paternal identity among low-income African Ameri-
can men. Journal of Family Issues, 27, 31–54.
Roy, K. M., Buckmiller, N., and McDowell, A. (2008). Together but not “together”: Trajectories of relationship
suspension for low-income unmarried parents. Family Relations, 57, 198–210.
Roy, K., and Smith, J. (2013). Nonresident fathers, kin, and intergenerational parenting. In N. J. Cabrera and
C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 320–
337). New York: Routledge.
Russell, G. (1983). The changing role of fathers. St. Lucia, Australia: Queensland University Press.
Russell, G. (1986). Primary caretaking and role-sharing fathers. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father’s role: Applied
perspectives (pp. 29–57). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Russell, G., and Russell, A. (1987). Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle childhood. Child
Development, 58, 1573–1585.
Ryan, R. M., Martin, A., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Is one good parent good enough? Patterns of mother and
father parenting and child cognitive outcomes at 24 and 36 months. Parenting, 6, 211–228.
Sagi, A. (1982). Antecedents and consequences of various degrees of paternal involvement in childrearing: The
Israeli project. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Nontraditional families: Parenting and child development (pp. 205–232). Hills-
dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Sagi, A., Koren, N., and Weinberg, M. (1987). Fathers in Israel. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father’s role: Cross-cultural
perspectives (pp. 197–226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Sandler, I., Miles, J., Cookston, J. T., and Braver, S. L. (2008). Effects of father and mother parenting on children’s
mental health in high- and low-conflict divorces. Family Court Review, 46, 282–296.
Sayer, L. C., England, P., Allison, P., and Kangas, N. (2011). She left, he left: How employment and satisfaction
affect men’s and women’s decisions to leave marriages. American Journal of Sociology, 116, 1982–2018.
Scaramella, L. V., and Conger, R. D. (2003). Intergenerational continuity of hostile parenting and its con-
sequences: The moderating influence of children’s negative emotional reactivity. Social Development, 12,
420–439.
Schwalb, D. W., Nakazawa, J., Yamamoto, T., and Hyun, J-H (2010). Fathering in Japan, China and Korea:
Changing contexts, images and roles. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(5th ed., pp. 341–387). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

133
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Cabrera, N. (2006). Fathering in infancy: Mutuality and stability
between 6 and 14 months. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6, 167–188.
Shapiro, A., Gottman, J., and Carrere, S. (2000). The baby and the marriage: Identifying factors that buffer against
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

decline in marital satisfaction after the first baby arrives. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 59–70.
Schermerhorn, A. C., Cummings, E. M., and Davies, P. T. (2008). Children’s representations of multiple family
relationships: Organizational structure and development in early childhood. Journal of Family Psychology, 22,
89–101.
Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., and Altenburger, L. E. (2019). Parental gatekeeping. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 167–198). New York: Routledge.
Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., Brown, G. L., and Cannon, E. A. (2008). Maternal gatekeeping, coparenting quality, and
fathering behavior in families with infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 389–398.
Shwalb, D. W., Shwalb, B. J., and Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2013). Fathers in cultural context. New York: Routledge.
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., and Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of childrearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J. G., Luthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, G., and DiSalle, F. (2003). Differential
sex-independent amygdale response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological
Psychiatry, 54, 1367–1375.
Settersten, R. A., Jr., and Cancel-Tirado, D. (2010). Fatherhood as a hidden variable in men’s Development and
life courses. Research in Human Development, 7, 83–102.
Schwalb, D.W., Nakazawa, J.,Yamamoto,T., and Hyun, J-H (2010). Fathering in Japan, China and Korea :Chang-
ing contexts, images and roles. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.) The role of the father in child development 5th ed. (pp.341–
387). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Cabrera, N. J. (2006). Fathering in infancy: Mutuality and stability
between 8 and 16 months. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6, 167–188.
Shek, D. T. (2000). Parental marital quality and well-being, parent-child relational quality, and Chinese adoles-
cent adjustment. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 28, 147–162.
Shaver, K. ( June 17, 2007). Stay-at-home dads forge new identities, roles. Washington, DC: Washington Post.
Shlafer, R., Gerrity, E., Ruhland, E., Wheeler, M., and Michaels, C. (2013). Children with incarcerated parents:
Considering children’s outcomes in the context of family experiences. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.
Shlafer, R. J., and Poehlmann, J. (2010). Attachment and caregiving relationships in families affected by parental
incarceration. Attachment and Human Development, 12, 395–415.
Shelton, K. H., and Harold, G. T. (2008). Interparental conflict, negative parenting, and children’s adjustment:
Bridging links between parents’ depression and children’s psychological distress. Journal of Family Psychology,
22, 712–724.
Shields, S. A. (2008). Gender: An intersectionality perspective. Sex Roles, 59, 301–311.
Shulman, S., and Klein, M. M. (1993). Distinctive role of the father in adolescent separation-individuation. New
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 62, 41–57.
Silverstein, L. B., and Auerbach, C. F. (1999). Deconstructing the essential father. American Psychologist, 54,
397–407.
Simon, S. (2011, September). Fatherhood, not testosterone, makes the man (NPR report). Retrieved from http//
www.npr.org/2011/09/17
Snarey, J. (1993). How fathers care for the next generation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Snyder, Z. K., Carlo, T. A., and Coats Mullins, M. M. (2001). Parenting from prison: An examination of a chil-
dren’s visitation program at a women’s correctional facility. Marriage and Family Review, 32, 33–61.
Song, Y-J. (2004). Fatherhood and childcare: A comparative analysis of Chinese and Korean families (Doctoral
dissertation). Retrieved from Dissertations Abstracts.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., and Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The minnesota study
of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
Statistics Canada. (2010). On shifts in rates of stay at home fathers. Ottawa, Canada: Statistics Canada.
Stearns, P. (1991). Fatherhood in historical perspective: The role of social change. In F. W. Bozett and S. M. H.
Hanson (Eds.), Fatherhood and families in cultural context (pp. 28–52). New York: Springer.
Stevenson, M. M., Braver, S. L., Ellman, I. M., and Votruba, A. M. (2013). In N. Cabrera and C. Tamis La Monda
(Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 379–396). New York, NY: Routledge
Stevenson, M. M., Fabricius, W. V., Cookston, J. T., Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., Braver, S. L. and Saenz, D. (2014).
Marital problems, maternal gatekeeping attitudes, and father-child relationships in adolescence. Developmental
Psychology, 50, 1208–1218.
Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., and Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2000). Hormonal correlates of paternal
responsiveness in new and expectant fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 79–95.

134
Fathers and Families

Storey, A. E., and Walsh, C. J. (2013). Biological basis of mammalian parenting behavior. In M. J. Cabrera and
C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 3–22).
New York: Routledge/Taylor and Francis.
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

Strazdins, L., Korda, R. J., Lim, L. L., Broom, D. H., and D’Souza, R. M. (2004). Around-the-clock: Parent work
schedules and children’s well-being in a 24-h economy. Social Science and Medicine, 59, 1517–1527.
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., and Cummings, E. M. (2006). The impact of hostility and withdrawal in
interparental conflict on parental emotional unavailability and children’s adjustment difficulties. Child Devel-
opment, 77, 1623–1641.
Sugarman, S. (2008). What Is a “Family”? Conflicting messages from our public programs. American Bar Associa-
tion Family Law Quarterly, 42, 231–261.
Suh, G. W., Fabricius, W. V., Stevenson, M. W., Parke, R. D., Cookston, J. T., Braver, S. L., and Saenz, D. S.
(2016). Effects of the interparental relationship on adolescents’ emotional security and adjustment: The
important role of fathers. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1666–1678.
Sun, L. C., and Roopnarine, J. L. (1996). Mother–infant and father–infant interaction and involvement in child-
care and household labor in Taiwanese families. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 121–129.
Suppal, P., and Roopnarine, J. L. (1999). Paternal involvement in child care as a function of maternal employ-
ment in nuclear and extended families in India. Sex Roles, 40, 731–744.
Swain, J. E. (2008). Baby stimuli and the parent brain: Functional neuroimaging of the neural substrates of
parent-infant attachment. Psychiatry, 5, 28–36.
Swain, J. E., Kim, P., and Ho, S. S. (2011). Neuroendocrinology of parental response to baby-cry. Journal of Neu-
roendricronology, 23, 1036–1041.
Swain, J. E., Lorberbaum, J. P., Kose, S., and Strathearn, L. (2007). Brain basis of early parent-infant interactions:
Psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
48, 262–287.
Swain, J. E., Kim, P., Spicer, J., Ho, S. S., Dayton, C. J., Elmadih, A., and Abel, K. M. 2014). Brain basis of human
parental caregiving in mothers and fathers. Brain Research, 11, 78–101.
Sykes, B. L., and Pettit, B. (2014). Mass incarceration, family complexity and the reproduction of childhood
disadvantage. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 654, 127–149.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2004). Playmates and more: Fathers’ role in child development. Human Development, 47,
220–227.
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Baumwell, L., and Cabrera, N. J. (2013). Father’s role in children’s language develop-
ment behavior. In N. J. Cabrera and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement (2nd ed.,
pp. 135–150). New York: Routledge.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., and Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s
achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and McFadden, K. E. (2010). Low-income fathers: Myth and evidence. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 296–318). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N., and Bradley, B. (2004). Mothers and fathers at play with their
2- and 3-year olds. Child Development, 75, 1806–1820.
Taylor, B. A., and Behnke, A. (2005). Fathering across the border: Latino fathers in Mexico and the U.S. Father-
ing, 3, 99–120.
Trinder, L. (2008). Maternal gate closing and gate opening in postdivorce families. Journal of Family Issues, 29,
1298–1324.
Tinsley, B. R., and Parke, R. D. (1984). Grandparents as support and socialization agents. In M. Lewis (Ed.),
Beyond the dyad (pp. 161–194). New York: Springer.
Tinsley, B. J., and Parke, R. D. (1987). Grandparents as interactive and social support agents for families with
young infants. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 25, 259–277.
Toth, J. F., and Xu, X. (1999). Ethnic and cultural diversity in fathers’ involvement: A racial/ethnic comparison
of African American, Hispanic, and White fathers. Youth and Society, 31, 76–99.
Tuerk, E. H., and Loper, A. B. (2006). Contact between incarcerated mothers and their children: Assessing par-
enting stress. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 43, 23–43.
Umberson, D., and Montez, J. K. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 51(Suppl), S54–S66.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2008). 2005–2007 American community survey PUMS. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). Who’s minding the kids? Child care arrangements. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau.
Vermeer, P. (2014). Religion and family life: An overview of current research and suggestions for future
research. Religions, 5(2), 402–421.

135
Ross D. Parke and Jeffrey T. Cookston

Volling, B. L., and Belsky, J. (1991). Multiple determinants of father involvement during infancy in dual-earner
and single-earner families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 461–474.
Wadsworth, S. M. (2013). Understanding and supporting the resilience of a new generation of combat-exposed
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.104 At: 14:32 15 Mar 2022; For: 9780429433214, chapter3, 10.4324/9780429433214-3

military families and their children. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 16, 415–420.
Wang, R., and Bianchi, S. M. (2009). ATUS Fathers’ Involvement in Childcare. Social Indictors Research, 93,
141–145.
Weinraub, M., and Bowler, G. (2019). Single parenthood. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting Vol. 3:
Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 271–310). New York: Routledge.
Whiting, B. B., Edwards, C. P., Ember, C. R., and Erchak, G. M. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation
of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
White, B. L., Kaban, B., Shapiro, B., and Attonucci, J. (1976). Competence and experience. In I. C. Uzgiris and
F. Weizmann (Eds.), The structuring of experience (pp. 115–152). New York: Plenum.
Whitbeck, L. B., Simons, R. L., Conger, R. D., Wickrama, K. A. S., Ackley, K. A., and Elder, G. H., Jr. (1997). The
effects of parents’ working conditions and family economic hardship on parenting behaviors and children’s
self-efficacy. Social Psychology Quarterly, 60, 291–303.
Wildeman, C. (2009). Parental imprisonment, the prison boom, and the concentration of childhood disadvan-
tage. Demography, 46, 265–280.
Wildeman, C. (2014). Parental incarceration, child homelessness, and the invisible consequences of mass impris-
onment. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 651, 74–79.
Willerton, E., Schwarz, R. L., MacDermid Wadsworth, S. M., and Oglesby, M. S. (2011). Military fathers’ per-
spectives on involvement. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 521–530.
Williams, D. R. (2003). The health of men: Structured inequalities and opportunities. American Journal of Public
Health, 93, 724–731.
Williams, J. (2010). Reshaping the work-family debate. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, E. K., Dalberth, B. T., and Koo, H. P. (2010). “We’re the heroes!”: Fathers’ perspectives on their role
in protecting their preteenage children from sexual risk. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 42,
117–124.
Woo, H., and Kelly Raley, R. (2005). A small extension to “costs and rewards of children: The effects of becom-
ing a parent on adults’ lives”. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67, 216–221.
Wong, M. S., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Brown, G. L., Neff, C., and Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. (2009). Parental beliefs, infant
temperament, and marital Quality: Associations with infant-mother and infant-father attachment. Journal of
Family Psychology, 23, 828–838.
Wu, N. H., and Kim, S. Y. (2009). Chinese American adolescents’ perceptions of the language brokering experi-
ence as a sense of burden and sense of efficacy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 703–718.
Wyrobek, A. J., Eskenazi, B., Young, S., Arnheim, N., Tiemann-Boege, I., Jabs, E. W., Glaser, R. L., Pearson,
F. S., and Evenson, D. (2006). Advancing age has differential effects on DNA damage, chromatin integ-
rity, gene mutations, and aneuploidies in sperm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103,
9601–9606.
Xu, A., and Zhang, L. (2008). Rural fathers’ will, acts and experience of parenting: Experienced researcher of
Shanghai’s suburb, Hunan Normal University Bulletin of Social Science, 3, 72–76.
Yarrow, A. (1991). Latecomers: Children of Parents over 35. New York: Free Press.
Yeung, W. J., Sandberg, J., Davis-Kean, P., and Hofferth, S. (2001). Children’s time-use with fathers in intact
families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 136–154.
Yogman, M., and Garfield, C. F., and the Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health.
(2016). Fathers’ roles in the care and development of their children: The role of pediatricians. Pediatrics, 138,
e20161128.
Zaslow, M., Pedersen, F., Suwalsky, J., Rabinovich, B., and Cain, R. (April, 1985). Fathering during the infancy
period: The implications of the mother’s employment role. Paper presented at the meetings of the Society for
Research in Child Development, Toronto.
Zukow-Goldring, P. (2002). Sibling caretaking. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Being and becom-
ing a parent (Vol. 3, 2nd ed., pp. 253–286). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

136

You might also like