You are on page 1of 5

Regulatory Toxicology and Pharmacology 32, 73–77 (2000)

doi:10.1006/rtph.2000.1403, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

Regulatory Action Criteria for Filth and Other Extraneous Materials


IV. Visual Detection of Hair in Food

Patricia Valdes Biles 1 and George C. Ziobro


Microanalytical Branch, U.S. Food and Drug Administration, HFS-315, 200 C Street S.W., Washington, DC 20204

Received February 19, 2000

the product (Eisenberg, 1981). Federal regulations, pol-


Under Section 402(a)(3) of the Food, Drug and Cos- icy, and good manufacturing practices (GMP) require the
metic Act the distribution of foods which may contain prevention of gross contamination of food with large,
repulsive or offensive matter, considered as filth, is visibly objectionable adulterants (FDA, 1998).
prohibited. Filth includes contaminants such as rat, Hair and hair fragments in a food product are con-
mouse, and other animal hairs and excreta; whole in- sidered an aesthetic adulterant. The presence of aes-
sects, insect parts, and excreta; and other extraneous thetic adulterants may reduce the quality level of a
materials which, because of their repulsiveness, food product. Aesthetic types of filth and extraneous
would not knowingly be eaten or used. The presence of materials are categorized as adulterants within the
such filth renders food adulterated. Some foods, even meaning of section 402(a)(3) of the FD&C Act. Regula-
when produced under good manufacturing practice,
tory action can involve detention or seizure of the adul-
contain low levels of natural or unavoidable defects
terated goods (FDA, 1996). Possible sources of hair in
such as dirt, insect parts, and hair that are not haz-
food are from animal grooming, clothing, or domestic
ardous to health. Although the health hazard is elim-
inated, most consumers find the presence of “any” vis-
animals (Vasquez, 1981). Many mammals groom their
ible filth contaminant such as hair in a food product coats by licking and often swallowing much of the
objectionable. The objective of this study was to deter- dislodged hair. These hairs, though modified by the
mine when a defect (hair) in a food (mushrooms) is digestive system and excreted, are often readily recog-
visible to a consumer by testing the visual acuity of a nizable and are sometimes the only evidence that the
consumer panel under controlled laboratory condi- food has been contaminated with fecal matter. Cloth-
tions. Of the panelists participating in the study, when ing made of wool or rabbit fur sheds hair and hair
presented with a single hair on a flat surface with a fragments. Rabbit fur is used extensively for fur-lined
solid-colored background, 27% were able to detect a gloves and hats for workers in the frozen food industry.
1-mm hair, 58% were able to detect a 3-mm hair, and In some parts of the world it is still routine to use dogs
75% were able to detect a 10-mm hair. Twenty-five and cats in food processing facilities to control rodent
percent of the panelists surveyed were able to detect a populations. The natural shedding by these animals
5- or 10-mm hair on sliced, canned mushrooms. The can lead to hair contamination in a product.
experimental design and results of this study are dis- The presence of any visible filth contaminant such as
cussed. hair in a food product is objectionable to most consum-
ers. In this investigation the visual acuity of the con-
sumer to hair on a food product (sliced, canned mush-
INTRODUCTION rooms) is analyzed. The experimental design and
results of this study are presented. The results of this
The Food, Drug, and Cosmetic Act (FD&C Act, 1985) investigation provide reference information for the
states, “A food shall be deemed to be adulterated . . . ) (3) presence of hair in food. This is the fourth report in a
if it consists in whole or in part of any filthy, putrid or series in the development of a transparent science base
decomposed substance . . . ” FD&C Act as amended Sec for a revised FDA regulatory policy in the area of filth
420(a)(3). Filth is defined as any objectionable foreign and extraneous materials in food.
matter of animal origin found in a food, drug, or medical
device. This matter may be excreta; maggots, worms, METHODOLOGY
insects, or insect parts; or any other material that defiles
A semitrained laboratory panel was used to evaluate
1
To whom reprint requests should be addressed. the visual acuity of a consumer to the filth analyte hair.
73

0273-2300/00
74 BILES AND ZIOBRO

FIG. 1. Mushroom panel survey questionnaire.

Laboratory panels are used to study the human per- Administration’s Center for Food Safety and Applied
ception of food attributes such as visual appearance. Nutrition (CFSAN). Each panelist filled out a confiden-
The visual appearance of an object is related to the tial demographic survey on known visual problems and
illumination by which it is viewed, conditions under frequency of eating mushrooms (Fig. 1). A special test-
which it is viewed, and the visual response character- ing area with a separate waiting room was used to
istics of the observer (Hoffman, 1958). minimize distractions and to control test conditions. No
The test design was similar to a dilution or a thresh- telephones were present in the waiting room or test
old test used in sensory evaluation. The dilution tech- area. The panelists were allowed to choose an appoint-
nique establishes the smallest amount of unknown ment for the tests that would best fit their schedules.
that can be detected when mixed with a standard ma- Panelists reported to a separate waiting room where
terial (Amerine et al., 1965). In this study the analyte they were given white laboratory coats to wear and
was hair. The standard materials were test panel cards were escorted to the viewing room. (Canada Depart-
divided into nine colored squares used in training and ment of Agriculture, 1977; Jellinek, 1985).
Phase I and sliced, canned mushrooms used in Phase
II. Viewing Room
The test area consisted of a 33 ⫻ 70-in. stainless-
Panelists
steel examining table covered with white 70-lb, 42-in.-
The volunteer panelists were recruited from scien- wide 100% cotton rag paper onto which was mounted
tific, clerical, and support staff at the Food and Drug two 31-in.-wide ⫻ 36-in.-deep ⫻ 24-in.-high white
VISUAL DETECTION OF HAIR IN FOOD 75

TABLE 1
Panel Square Color Data

Values a

Mushroom Red Green Blue Color

Button mushroom, Agaricus bisporis (Lange) Imbach 255 247 239 Seashell
Wrinkled thimble-cap or early morel, Verpa bohemica Krombh. 81 43 20 Dark chocolate brown
Meadow mushroom, Agaricus campestris L. ex Fr. 224 176 144 Beige
Cinnabar-red chanterelle, Cantharellus cinnabarinus Schw. 236 117 85 Red-orange
Chanterelle, Cantharellus cibarius Fr. 255 255 132 Light yellow
Rare red reishi, Ganoderma lucidum (Leysser) Karsten 255 145 35 Pumpkin
a
Values range is 0 –255 with 0 being the darkest shade of the color and 255 the lightest shade of the color. The values for pure black are
0, 0, 0, and pure white 255,255,255.

foamboard observation booths. A door cutout in the panel cards were used as a training exercise prior to
back of each booth was used to present the samples. Phase I of the survey to help reinforce this visual
Lighting consisted of an Examoloite Model TC-440 process. Test subjects were told a brief review of the
overhead luminaire equipped with Deluxe Examolite study and given up to 60 s to observe a panel card for
4-ft two-prong F40T12/EXX 40 W fluorescent tubes the presence of hair and record their results on a stan-
and Examolite 33WA19/EX 4.33 W long-life bulbs. The dardized form. Participants were then asked for any
lighting was measured with a Sper Scientific Ltd. light comments or suggestions that might help with the
meter Model FC 840021 and determined to be 150-ft testing. This training helped the panel leader in direct-
candles. The viewing distance ranged from 524 to 554 ing the participants and helped the participants to
mm. understand the evaluation process. Based on training
observations, the study parameters of hair length, hair
Test Panel Card Preparation colors, and test panel card square colors were refined
A series of cards with nine 1.5 ⫻ 1.5-in. colored and the observation time was reduced to 30 s per card
squares arranged in a 3 ⫻ 3 grid were prepared by in Phase I of the study.
using Corel WordPerfect 7 Presentations and printed
Mushroom Test
on a HP Color LaserJet 5/5M printer onto HP Color
LaserJet 5/5M premium glossy paper and mounted A single layer of sliced commercially canned Agari-
onto gray poster board. Nine colors were used in the cus spp. mushrooms was arranged in a 100 ⫻ 15-mm
training panel of which six were used in Phase I. The plastic petri dish which had been divided into four
background colors chosen for the training and Phase I quadrants. A total of 16 different petri dishes were
portion of the survey were picked by comparing color prepared. In each quadrant, a single hair was ran-
charts to commercially prepared, canned mushrooms. domly placed on a mushroom slice. Unbeknownst to
The names of the commercial mushrooms; the red, the panelists, the same hair type and length were
green, and blue color values; and common name of the placed in all four quadrants on the same petri dish.
colors used in Phase I are listed in Table 1. Onto each Each petri dish was given a random three-digit num-
square was mounted a single hair of a known length (0, ber. Four petri dishes were arranged on a white Sty-
1, 3, 5, 7, 10 mm) and type [human black, human rofoam tray. The trays were placed carefully on the
brown, human white, cat/dog (cat or dog), rat/mouse table prior to examination (two trays per booth). Pan-
(rat or mouse)] using clear fingernail polish. Hair elists were not allowed to handle the petri dishes as
placement upper, lower, right and left, and center was this could compromise the position of the mushrooms
randomly chosen by rolling a 20-sided percentile die. and/or hair. Care was taken to prevent the desiccation
Multiples of numbers 1–5 were used for a defined po- of the mushrooms during the test by misting the mush-
sition on the square. The test panel cards were labeled rooms with distilled water as necessary and keeping
with a unique, random three-digit number. The cat/dog the petri dishes covered with lids. The petri dish lids
hair was obtained from personal pets; human hair were removed just prior to testing. Panelists were
white/brown/black, from staff; and rat/mouse hair, given 1 min to observe each tray and record their
from authentic animal pelts. results on a standardized form.

Training Statistical Design


Visual perception and recognition may be considered Experiments were designed to generate a balanced
as behavioral processes (Ryback et al., 1998). Ten test data set where there were an equal number of obser-
76 BILES AND ZIOBRO

TABLE 2
Percentage of Panelist Observations of Hair on Colored Squares

Human
All hairs black/brown b Human white Cat/dog Rat/mouse
a
Sig % Sig % Sig % Sig % Sig %

10 mm A 75.2 A 38.9 A 90.1 A 90.1 A 81.5


5 mm B 56.6 B 21.0 B 75.9 B 81.5 B 48.4
3 mm B 58.5 B 20.4 AB 83.3 AB 83.3 B 46.9
1 mm C 27.0 C 7.4 C 53.7 C 30.9 C 16.0
None D 9.0 B 17.9 D 5.6 D 6.2 D 6.2
a
% observed with the same letter are not significantly different at P ⬎ 0.05.
b
(/) ⫽ OR.

vations for every combination of hair lengths, types, old. This closely resembles the age distribution of the
and background colors. ANOVA T test was calculated CFSAN. A minority of the participants (14.8%) did not
by using PC SAS 6.12 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) with eat mushrooms. Of those eating mushrooms 25.9% ate
significance set at P ⬎ 0.05. them one to five times a week; 55.6%, one to five times
a month; and 3.7%, one to five times a year. Of those
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION that ate mushrooms, the most commonly consumed
type was fresh (85.2%), canned (59.3%), and dried
The goal of this study was to test the ability of an (37.0%). The percentage total for the types of mush-
average consumer to detect the presence of hair in food rooms consumed exceeds 100% because the partici-
using sliced, canned mushrooms as the test media. The pants eat more than one type of mushroom (Fig. 1).
test design was similar to a dilution test used in sen-
The training panel showed that there was no statis-
sory evaluation. On the basis of the recommendations
tical difference in response to one pair and one triplet
of the Committee on Sensory Evaluation of the Insti-
of the nine background colors. The Phase I survey
tute of Food Technologists (Amerine et al., 1965), 48
showed that there was no statistical difference in re-
people were recruited for a semitrained sensory panel
of which 27 people completed both phases of the study. sponse to human black and human brown hair and
The surveyed population was drawn from the scien- that there was no difference in the ability to observe
tific, clerical, and support staff at the Food and Drug the hair lengths 7 and 10 mm.
Administration’s CFSAN. Of the participants that Tables 2 and 3 summarize the data from the survey
completed all phases of the study, the population was listing the percentage of panelists that observed the
74.1% male and 25.9% female, 81.5% of whom wore presence of hair by length, type, and overall appear-
corrective lenses and 29.6% of the population had ance. Those percentages with the same letter in the
astigmatism. No one was color blind. The majority same column are not significantly different from each
(70.4%) of people surveyed were between the ages of 40 other at P ⬎ 0.05. There was no significant difference
and 59 years old followed by 22.2% between the ages of between the different types of hair. As expected it was
25 and 39 years old and 7.4% were more than 60 years easier to see a longer hair on a solid-colored back-
old. There were no participants younger than 25 years ground than a shorter hair, with no statistical differ-

TABLE 3
Percentage of Panelist Observations of Hair on Mushroom Slices

Human
All hairs black/brown b Human white Cat/dog Rat/mouse

Sig a % Sig % Sig % Sig % Sig %

10 mm A 24.3 A 28.7 B 3.7 A 27.8 A 37.0


5 mm A 25.5 A 35.2 A 20.4 A 38.7 B 17.6
1 mm B 15.7 A 35.2 B 9.3 B 14.8 C 3.7
None C 8.8 B 10.2 B 7.4 B 13.0 C 4.6
a
As in Table 2.
b
As in Table 2.
VISUAL DETECTION OF HAIR IN FOOD 77

ence in observation between a 3- and 5-mm hair. To limit found in this study. Observation of hair on a food
simplify further testing the 3-mm hair was eliminated matrix is more difficult than observation on a flat col-
from the sliced, canned mushroom panel. The percent- ored surface as was demonstrated by the panelists who
age of hair observations dropped when sliced mush- detected only 25% of the 5- and 10-mm hairs on the
rooms were used instead of flat, solid-colored squares. canned, sliced mushrooms. We conclude 5 and 10 mm
For example, 75.2% of the people observed a 10-mm to be a visible length of hair in food. The results of this
hair on a flat, solid colored background. This dropped study are consistent with existing United States De-
to an observation rate of 24.3% when sliced, canned partment of Agriculture guidelines that define a 7-mm
mushrooms were used. At first glance a mushroom strand of hair as readily discernible and/or highly ob-
slice appears to be uniform in color, but color and jectionable (USDA, 1996).
specular reflectance differences exist between the
stem, cap, and gills of the mushroom. Furthermore, ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
within a container of mushrooms, there is a range of
colors depending on the age of the mushroom, when it The authors thank the employees of the U.S. Food and Drug
was picked, and how long it was held before processing. Administration Center for Food Safety and Applied Nutrition who
participated in this study.
The panel had a more difficult time observing human
white hair and cat/dog hair than human black/brown
or rat/mouse hair. The cat/dog hairs were light in color REFERENCES
comparable to human white hair while; the rat/mouse
Amerine, M., Pangborn, R. M., and Roessler, E. B. (1965). Principles
hair had similar coloration to human black/brown hair. of Sensory Evaluation of Food, pp. 220 –225, 275–313, 417– 423,
For observation of hair on sliced mushrooms, the ori- and 428. London, England.
entation of the hair was a factor in the visibility to the Canada Department of Agriculture (1977). Laboratory Methods for
panelist. For instance, a hair placed perpendicular to a Sensory Evaluation, Publication 1637, pp. 6 –7. Canada Dept. of
gill or striation is more easily seen than a hair placed Agriculture, Ottawa, Canada.
parallel to the same. This factor, as well as differences Eisenberg, W. V. (1981). Sources of Food Contaminants. In FDA
Technical Bulletin No. 1: Principles of Food Analysis for Filth,
in specular reflectance, luminance, chromaticity, and Decomposition, and Foreign Matter (J.R. Gorham, Ed.), pp. 11–25.
transparency may help to explain why the 5-mm hair U.S. Gov. Printing Office, Washington, DC.
was observed more than the 10-mm hair on the mush- Food, Drug, and Cosmetic Act, as Amended, and Related Laws.
rooms (Table 3). There was a readily apparent differ- (1985). U.S. Gov. Printing Office, Washington, DC.
ence in the specular reflectance and transparency be- Food and Drug Administration (FDA) (1980). Laboratory Procedures
tween human white hairs and the darker hairs used in Manual, Chapter 18, part 1. U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
this study. On a sliced mushroom the human white man Services, Washington, DC.
hair appeared to reflect the color and luster of the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) (1996). FDA Compliance Pol-
icy Guides, Chapter 5. U.S. Department of Health and Human
mushroom. This characteristic made them much more Services, Rockville, MD.
difficult to observe. This was not so when human white Food and Drug Administration (FDA) (1998). Natural or Unavoid-
hair was viewed on a flat, solid-colored square. The able Defects in Food for Human Use That Present no Health Haz-
panelists were able to detect the dark hair on a mush- ard, Title 21 Code of Federal Regulations, Section 110.110. U.S.
room slice more often than on the colored squares. The Department of Health and Human Services, Washington, DC.
color contrast between the mushroom slices and dark Hoffman, J. A. (1958). Principles of Psychological Test Methods
hair was greater than the color contrast between the (Judgemental Methods—Appearance), Basic Principles of Sensory
Analysis, ASTM STP 433, pp. 98 –105. Am. Soc. Testing Materials,
dark hair and the colored squares. Observation of hairs Philadelphia, PA.
either on flat colored squares or on mushroom slices Jellinek, G. (1985). Sensory Evaluation of Food, p. 322. Chichester,
gave similar rates of false positives (9.0 vs 8.8%). England.
Ryback, I. A., et al. (1998). BMV Behavioral Model of Visual Percep-
CONCLUSIONS tion and Recognition, scientific pages of Ilya Rybak http://
www.voicenet.com⬃rybak/vnc.html. p. 1.
In this laboratory panel survey study more than 50% United States Department of Agriculture (1996) Inspection Proce-
of the participants were able to detect as small as a dures for Foreign Material, File Code 172-A-1. pp. 1–18. U.S. Gov.
Printing Office, Washington, DC.
single 3-mm hair placed on a flat, solid-colored surface.
Vazquez, A. W. (1981). Hairs. In FDA Technical Bulletin No. 1:
The positive observation rate of approximately 20% Principles of Food Analysis for Filth, Decomposition, and Foreign
(e.g., the false-positive rate multiplied by the normal Matter (J.R. Gorham, Ed.), pp. 125–170. U.S. Gov. Printing Office,
distribution constant, 1.96) is near the visual detection Washington, DC.

You might also like