You are on page 1of 10

Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Cyber victimization in middle school and relations to social emotional


outcomes
Christina F. Brown, Michelle Kilpatrick Demaray ⇑, Stephanie M. Secord
Psychology Department, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Cyber or electronic bullying is a growing problem among school-aged children and research on cyberbul-
Available online 16 March 2014 lying is still relatively young. The primary purposes of the current study were: (a) to investigate gender
and grade level differences among cyber victims (b) to investigate the overlap between traditional vic-
Keywords: timization and cyber victimization and (c) to investigate the associations among cyber victimization
Online victimization and social emotional outcomes. Data were collected through self-report questionnaires on cyber victim-
Cyber victimization ization, traditional victimization, and social-emotional outcomes in a school-based sample of 106 middle
Cyberbully
school students. Results demonstrated that levels of cyber victimization did not differ by grade or by gen-
Gender differences
Adolescents
der, cyber victimization and traditional victimization are distinct but related constructs, and relations
between cyber victimization and social emotional outcomes varied by gender, with girls suffering more
than boys. This study also confirmed that traditional bullying continues to be significantly related to a
number of negative outcomes for all students. These findings, as well as implications and direction of
future research, are discussed.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction recruited, how or if cyber victimization is defined, and how these


behaviors are assessed. The development of psychometrically
Cyber or electronic bullying is a growing problem among sound measures of cyber victimization has been limited. Many
school-aged children. As technology becomes even more accessible researchers have developed their own measure or questionnaire,
and relied upon, victims of cyberbullying will likely become even but provide limited information on the reliability and validity of
more common. Current estimates are that as many as 20–35% of the instrument (Beran & Li, 2005; Dehue, Bolman, & Trijntje,
children and adolescents report experiencing cyberbullying 2008; Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Kowalski & Limber, 2007). Several
(Diamanduros, Downs, & Jenkins, 2008; Kowalski & Limber, early studies have relied on outdated national surveys; therefore, it
2007). Because research on cyber victimization is still relatively is difficult to generalize findings from studies. Samples also vary
young, there is not a clear understanding of basic information such across studies, as some have utilized online convenience samples
as gender and developmental differences in experiences of cyber of Internet users, cross sectional databases, and others have admin-
victimization. From a socio-ecological framework, it is important istered measures in school settings. The methodological limitations
to consider the role of both traditional (face-to-face) victimization of cyber victimization research may, in part, explain the inconsis-
and cyber victimization in understanding the relation to negative tent findings across studies. Thus, the current study also developed
outcomes. The primary purposes of the current study were: (a) a measure of cyber victimization and documented evidence of reli-
to investigate gender and grade level differences among cyber vic- ability and validity for use of this measure to answer the study’s re-
tims (b) to investigate the overlap between traditional victimization search questions. Furthermore, because amount of time online has
and cyber victimization and (c) to investigate the associations been found to be related to cyber victimization (Hinduja & Patchin,
among cyber victimization and social emotional outcomes. 2008), the current study controlled for time spent online.
To date, studies investigating cyber victimization have had sig-
nificant methodological limitations including how participants are
1.1. Cyber victimization
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Psychology Department, Psychology-Computer
Science Building, Rm. 400. Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115, USA. Tel.: Currently there is not a standard definition of cyber victimiza-
+1 (815) 753 7077. tion, but all the definitions generally contain elements of inten-
E-mail address: mkdemaray@niu.edu (M.K. Demaray). tional and repeated harm inflicted through the use of technology.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.014
0747-5632/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21 13

For example, Hinduja and Patchin (2008) define cyberbullying as the individual from participating in bullying behaviors. Unlike tra-
‘‘willful and repeated harm inflicted through the medium of elec- ditional bullying, which is limited to locations with a bully and a
tronic text’’ (p. 131). Mason (2008) defined cyberbullying as ‘‘an victim present, cyberbullying and cyber victimization may occur
individual or a group willfully using information and communica- in any environment in which an individual has access to technol-
tion involving electronic technologies to facilitate deliberate and ogy. Norms, rules and expectations differ between the two realms.
repeated harassment or threat to another individual or group by Online, social cues are less immediate, visual responses are not
sending or posting cruel text and/or graphics using technological part of the interaction, and children likely have easier access to
means’’ (p. 323). Individuals who are on the receiving end of cyber- cyber victims. Anonymity and impersonation may or may not be
bullying behaviors are considered cyber victims. at play, and kids may feel more powerful and more protected inter-
acting with others from the safety of their bedroom or behind an
1.1.1. Gender differences electronic device. Because kids communicate through technology
To date, gender differences in cyber victimization are not con- and then see each other in school, on the playground, or in the
sistent. Kowalski and Limber (2007) found that middle school girls community, there are likely overlaps between the two systems.
were more often cyber victims (15% of girls versus 7% of boys) and In addition, outcomes of online behaviors may be realized in the
cyber bully/victims (10% of girls versus 4% of boys) than middle physical world and dealt with there. On the other hand, since the
school boys. Dempsey, Sulkowski, Nichols, and Storch (2009) found rules and norms in the two systems are different some children
similar gender differences for cyber victimization in a large middle may be involved in cyberbullying who would not typically be
school sample. On the other hand, Li (2006) found that 25% of involved in traditional bullying (Espelage & Swearer, 2004).
males and 25.6% of females reported that they were cyberbullied Little is known about how many school-aged children experi-
in a junior high school sample. Several other studies have not ence bullying both online and at school. For some youth, online
found significant gender differences for cyberbullying or cyber vic- victimization may be part of a larger spectrum of victimization
timization (Beran & Li, 2005; Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Williams & experiences (Mitchell, Ybarra, & Finkelhor, 2007). Mitchell et al.
Guerra, 2007). (2007) found that almost three quarters of children ages 10–17
Inconsistent gender related findings may be related to a number whom reported being victimized online at least once in the past
of factors, including researchers examining different age groups, year also reported being victimized at least once in the past year
students in different countries, and utilizing different reference in the traditional sense. Erdur-Baker’s (2010) study of adolescents
time periods in their assessments. Some researchers asked partic- ages 14–18 in Turkey found that 32% of the students were victims
ipants to comment on online experiences in the past year, while of both cyber and traditional bullying and 26% of the students bul-
others asked about the past 30 days, the past few months, or even lied others both electronically and traditionally. Experience with
lifetime experiences. It is difficult to compare results from studies traditional bullying has been found to be a strong predictor for
which examine differing reference periods. Furthermore, since cyberbullying and cyber victimization (Li, 2007). Hinduja and
technology changes so quickly, it is important to utilize data that Patchin (2008) found that those who admitted to bullying others
is no more than 10 years old. Finally, samples vary across studies. and those who were bullied by others were more than 2.5 times
Hindjua and Patchin (2008) relied on an online convenience sam- more likely to cyberbully others or be victimized online.
ple of Internet users, while other researchers have used cross sec-
tional databases and still others have administered measures in 1.1.4. Social emotional outcomes associated with cyber victimization
school settings. It is clear that additional research is needed to Social emotional outcomes related to cyber victimization have
understand if gender differences are a consistent and important as- been found to be comparable to those related to victimization from
pect of online bullying. traditional bullying (Smith et al., 2008). Initial research has shown
that being a victim of cyberbullying can negatively impact physi-
1.1.2. Developmental differences cal, social, and cognitive functioning, development, and well-being.
Results from research on developmental differences have also It can also cause psychological, emotional and academic problems
been inconsistent. Some studies have found no association be- (Beran & Li, 2005; Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Mitchell et al., 2007;
tween age and cyber victimization (Beran & Li, 2005; Varjas, Ybarra, Espelage, & Mitchell, 2007). A number of studies have
Henrich, & Meyers, 2009), while other studies suggest that cyber found an association between cyber victims, cyber bullies and
victimization may be more prevalent among middle school stu- depression and suicidal ideation (Hinduja & Patchin, 2010) and
dents. Slonje and Smith (2008) surveyed 360 adolescents ages externalizing behaviors (Ybarra, Diener-West, & Leaf, 2007; Ybarra,
12–20 in Sweden and found that rates of cyber victimization were Espelage, et al., 2007). Ybarra and Mitchell (2004) found that males
much higher for students ages 12–15. Williams and Guerra (2007) who reported depressive-like symptoms were more than eight
looked at Internet bullying in elementary, middle, and high school times as likely to have experienced cyber victimization. Further-
and found that Internet bullying and victimization was most fre- more, victims report higher levels of anger and frustration, espe-
quent in middle school and declined in high school. Kowalski cially towards the aggressor (Patchin & Hinduja, 2006). Research
and Limber (2007) found that among middle school students, 6th also shows that both cyberbullies and victims are at higher risk
graders were the least likely to be involved in cyberbullying and for school problems (e.g., suspension, truancy, cheating) and other
victimization. Thus far, it appears that cyber victimization may deviant behaviors (e.g., drug and alcohol abuse) (Patchin &
be more common in middle school. Hinduja, 2012). However, many of these studies did not use
validated measures of psychosocial functioning in their analysis.
1.1.3. Overlap between traditional and cyber victimization Some research has examined the simultaneous effects of tradi-
Researchers have applied a social-ecological framework tional and cyberbullying and victimization to determine if unique
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979) to traditional bullying, a social phenomena associations exist with various outcomes. Dempsey et al. (2009)
that can be understood within the larger social context in which it surveyed 1684 middle school students to assess cyber victimiza-
occurs (Espelage & Swearer, 2004). Bullying does not occur in iso- tion in the past 30 days, anxiety related to social situations in
lation and bullying behaviors are supported or not supported based the last 30 days, and depressive symptoms in the past seven days
on the environments in which they occur. According to the social- using validated measures of depression and social anxiety. Cyber
ecological perspective, a complex interplay among variables at all victimization was found to be weakly related to social anxiety
levels of the model combine and interact to influence or inhibit but not to depression after controlling for traditional victimization.
14 C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21

Fredstrom, Adams, and Gilman (2011) found cyber victimization to cyber victimization? It was hypothesized that cyber victimization
be related to lower levels of self-esteem and higher levels of social would be significantly and positively related to the following com-
stress, anxiety, depressive symptoms, and locus of control after posite scales of the BASC-2: Emotional Symptoms Index, Internal-
controlling for traditional victimization. Recently, Bonanno and izing Problems, and School Problems and significantly and
Hymel (2013) found cyber victimization to be related to depression negatively related to Personal Adjustment. It was also predicted
and suicidal ideation above and beyond the contribution of tradi- that cyber victimization would be significantly and positively re-
tional victimization in a sample of 399 students in 8th to 10th grade. lated to the following BASC-2 subscales: Anxiety, Social Stress,
Cyberbullying may be particularly harmful to victims for sev- and Depression, and significantly and negatively related to Inter-
eral reasons. First, it is very difficult to escape from cyberbullying, personal Relations and Self-Esteem (Hinduja & Patchin, 2008;
as it can occur across settings. Second, the aggressors may be more Mitchell et al., 2007; Shariff, 2005; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004).
volatile due to their ability to remain anonymous. Without having
direct contact with the victim, aggressors may be less likely to feel
empathy or remorse. Third, there is a greater breadth of audience. 2. Method
The use of technology and the Internet allows the aggressor to
reach large audiences in a short time frame. Finally, there is less 2.1. Participants
adult supervision on the Internet, reducing the chances of inter-
vention (Dempsey et al., 2009; Smith & Slonje, 2010). Participants for this study (N = 106) were recruited from a large
middle school in northern Illinois. There were 1099 students en-
1.2. Current investigation rolled in the school at the time so approximately 10% of the stu-
dent population participated in the study. Specifically, there were
Research on cyberbullying is still relatively young; therefore, 42 (40%) 6th graders, 49 (46%) 7th graders, and 15 (14%) 8th graders
there is not yet a clear understanding of basic information such in this study. Fifty-nine of the participants were females (56%) and
as gender and developmental differences in experiences of cyber 47 (44%) were males. The sample was predominantly White (73%),
victimization. To date, studies investigating cyberbullying have followed by Latino (9%), Asian (5%), and African American (2%), and
yielded inconsistent results regarding gender and developmental Other/Missing (13%). The socioeconomic status (SES) make-up of
differences regarding the social emotional outcomes of victims of the school was 7.2% low income.
cyberbullying. Furthermore, studies have had significant methodo-
logical limitations including how participants are recruited, how or
2.2. Materials
if cyberbullying and victimization is defined, and how these behav-
iors are assessed. Thus, the current study also developed a measure
All students were asked to complete a measure of cyber victim-
of cyber victimization and documented evidence of reliability and
ization developed for the current study, the Cyber Victimization
validity for use of this measure to answer the study’s research
Survey (CVS). Participants also completed the Revised Olweus
questions.
Bully/Victim Questionnaire (OBVQ; Olweus, 1996) and the Behav-
Although research on cyber victimization is rapidly growing,
ior Assessment System for Children (BASC-2 SRP; Reynolds &
there remains significant gaps and limitations in the current liter-
Kamphaus, 2004). Participants were also asked to respond to two
ature. One major limitation that will be addressed in the current
items about the time they spend online on computers and the time
study includes methodological issues associated with the measure-
they spend on their cell phone. The response options were: 1 = less
ment of cyber victimization. With a psychometrically sound mea-
than an hour, 2 = 1–2 h, 3 = 2–4 h, 4 = 4–6 h, 5 = more than 6 h. These
sure of cyber victimization, more accurate data in the current study
items were summed to get a Total Time Online score. This variable
will add to the literature by investigating (a) gender and develop-
allowed the researcher to control for time spent online.
mental differences in social emotional outcomes of victims of
cyberbullying and (b) the overlap between cyber and traditional
victimization. 2.2.1. Cyber Victimization Survey
The Cyber Victimization Survey (CVS) is a 15-item measure that
1.2.1. Research questions and predictions was developed for use in the current study to assess victimization
The following research questions guided the current investiga- from online bullying. Participants were asked how often certain
tion: (1) Are there gender or grade level differences in prevalence online behaviors/incidents happened to them in the last 2–3
rates of cyber victimization? While the literature to date is not nec- months. All items were answered on a 5-point scale, based on
essarily conclusive, it does appear that more females, especially in the response options on the Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Ques-
middle school, are cyber victimized; therefore, it was predicted tionnaire, (1 = it hasn’t happened at all in the past couple of months,
that females would have higher cyber victimization scores than 2 = only 1 or 2 times in the past couple of months, 3 = 2 or 3 times a
males (Dehue et al., 2008; Dempsey et al., 2009; Kowalski & month, 4 = about once a week, and 5 = several times a week).
Limber, 2007). Furthermore, based on results found in the litera- Initial items for the Cyber Victimization Survey were developed
ture, it was predicted that 6th graders would report the lowest in a number of ways. A review of current measures was conducted
overall levels of cyber victimization as compared to 7th and 8th and several items from Kowalski and Limber’s (2007) and Hinduja
graders (Kowalski & Limber, 2007; Slonje & Smith, 2008; Smith and Patchin’s (2009) cyberbullying measures were used. New
et al., 2008; Williams & Guerra, 2007). (2) What is the overlap be- items were also developed based on a review of the literature. Fol-
tween victims of traditional bullying and cyber victims? It was lowing item development, several school psychology graduate stu-
hypothesized that the majority of students classified as cyber vic- dents and psychology professors provided feedback, which was
tims would also be involved in some aspect of traditional victim- used to improve, revise, and discard specific items. The measure
ization. Traditional bullying has been found to be a strong was pilot tested in a sample (N = 137) of middle and high school
predictor for cyberbullying and cyber victimization (Hinduja & students. Based on pilot study results, several items were dropped,
Patchin, 2008; Li, 2007; Mitchell et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2008; the wording of several items was improved, and four items were
Ybarra, Diener-West, et al., 2007; Ybarra, Espelage, et al., 2007). added to create the version utilized in the current study. Thus,
(3) What social emotional outcomes, above and beyond traditional the Cyber Victimization Survey utilized in the current study ini-
victimization, while controlling for time online, are associated with tially had 20 items.
C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21 15

The psychometric properties of the Cyber Victimization Survey (Solberg & Olweus, 2003). Only the total victimization score, the
were confirmed in the current study sample. Inter-item correla- sum of items 6 through 13, was used in the current study.
tions were analyzed. Correlations ranged from .080 to .862. Items
that correlated poorly with other items were deleted from the 2.2.3. Behavior assessment system for children, second edition, self-
measure. An item was considered to correlate poorly with another report of personality
item if the correlation was less than .30 (Tabachnick & Fidell, The BASC-2 SRP is a personality inventory and provides 5 com-
1996). Any items that correlated poorly with 11 or more other posite scores: School Problems, Internalizing Problems, Inatten-
items were dropped from the measure. Based on this analysis, 4 tion/Hyperactivity, Personal Adjustment and the Emotional
items were deleted from the scale. All remaining items were Systems Index, an overall composite score. Two forms, the child
summed and item-total correlations were conducted. Correlations form (ages 8 through 11) and the adolescent form (ages 12 through
ranged from .519 to .799. All but one item total correlation was sig- 21) were used in the current study. Respondents rate questions in
nificant at the p < .01 level. The remaining 16 items were factor one of two ways. Some items require a True or False response while
analyzed with Principal Axis Factoring. An oblique (Promax) rota- others are answered based on a 4-point Likert type scale, ranging
tion was used because it was hypothesized that any factors would from Never to Almost Always. The child form consists of 139 items
be correlated with one another. Tabachnick and Fidell (1996) char- and the adolescent form contains 176 items. The following com-
acterize a sample size of 250 as ‘‘fair’’. Because this data set was posite scores: Emotional Symptoms Index, Personal Adjustment,
relatively small, Bartlett’s test of sphericity and Kaiser–Meyer-Olk- School Problems, and Internalizing Problems, and subscale scores:
in (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy were examined. As a rule, Anxiety, Depression, Interpersonal Relations, Social Stress, and
Tabachnick and Fidell indicate that a KMO value above .6 is re- Self-Esteem were used in the current investigation. There is strong
quired for factorability of the items. The use of Bartlett’s test is rec- evidence for psychometric properties of the BASC-2 SRP.
ommended when there are fewer than five cases per variable. For
this data set, the (KMO) measure of .863 indicated a high sampling 2.3. Procedure
adequacy for the factor analysis. Bartlett’s test of sphericity, which
tests whether the correlation matrix is an identity matrix, is signif- Students from a large Midwest middle school were recruited for
icant (p < .001), indicating that the factor model is appropriate. participation in the study. Informed consent letters for voluntary
Initially, the factor analysis was run without forcing any factors. participation, including a description of the study were sent home
Results indicated a four factor solution which explained 75% of the with all students. Only students who obtained parental consent
variance. An examination of the scree plot indicated there was were allowed to participate in the study. Data were collected by
likely one strong factor. An examination of the pattern matrix also the primary researcher, research assistants, and teachers during
pointed to a single factor solution. The four factor solution did not the students’ study hall. Participating students were given a brief
make theoretical sense so a second factor analysis was run forcing description of the study which included assurances of anonymity
a single factor. Results indicated that a single factor explained 50% and confidentiality unless students indicated they were going to
of the variance. Factor loadings ranged from .473 to .804. Item 1 harm themselves or others, and that participation was voluntary.
had the lowest factor loading (.473) and was a global question, ask- After the study description was given, students were asked to sign
ing participants if they had ever been cyberbullied. All remaining an assent form, which indicated their willingness to participate in
items were more specific, therefore, item 1 was dropped and the the study.
factor analysis was rerun. This model accounted for 52% of the var-
iance. Loadings on the single factor ranged from .622 to .806. The 2.4. Statistical analyses
final scale consisted of 15 items with an internal consistency alpha
equal to .924. In order to answer the first research question (Are there gender
To provide evidence of validity, correlations were run between or grade level differences in prevalence rates of cyber victimization?), a
the Cyber Victimization Survey and Hinduja and Patchin’s (2007) Gender (male, female) by Grade Level (6th, 7th, 8th) ANOVA was
Cyberbullying and Online Aggression Survey Instrument and conducted to determine if there were gender and grade level differ-
Kowalski and Limber’s (2007) Cyberbullying Measure. The correla- ences in cyber victimization. In this analysis the independent vari-
tion between the Cyber Victimization Survey and Hinduja and Pat- ables were Gender and Grade Level and the dependent variable
chin’s Cyberbullying and Online Aggression Survey Total Score was was cyber victimization.
.589 and the correlation between the CVS and Kowalski and Lim- To answer the second research question (What is the overlap be-
ber’s Cyberbullying Total Score was .523. Both were significant at tween victims of traditional bullying and cyber victims?), a correla-
the p < .01 level. tion between the CVS score (i.e., cyber victimization) and the
OBVQ victimization score (i.e., traditional victimization) was uti-
lized. Additionally, descriptive analyses comparing the top 20% of
2.2.2. The Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire cyber victims, as determined by the Cyber Victimization Survey,
The Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire (OBVQ) is a 40- and the top 20% of traditional victims, as determined by the Re-
item self-report questionnaire that measures bullying and victim- vised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire, were used.
ization. The OBVQ is one of the most commonly used measures of In order to answer the third research question (What social emo-
bullying. Internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and construct tional outcomes, above and beyond traditional victimization, while
validity have been found to be satisfactory (Kyriakides, Kaloyirou, controlling for time online, are associated with cyber victimization?),
& Lindsay, 2006; Olweus, 1997). The measure begins with a de- a number of regression analyses were conducted separately for
tailed definition of bullying and students are asked to respond to boys and girls using cyber victimization (i.e., CVS score) as the
items based on a 2–3 month reference period. Response options independent variable. In addition, the OBVQ Victimization (i.e., tra-
are on a 5-point Likert type scale. The measure is comprised of ditional victimization) score and Time Online score were entered
two global questions. The first asks ‘‘How often have you been bul- as independent variables to hold traditional victimization and
lied at school in the past couple of months?’’ and the second asks amount of time spent online constant in all analyses. The following
‘‘How often have you taken part in bullying another student(s) at BASC-2 composite scores, Emotional Symptoms Index, Personal
school in the past couple of months?’’ These items are each fol- Adjustment, School Problems, and Internalizing Problems, were
lowed by more specific items related to victimization and bullying entered as the dependent variables.
16 C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21

3. Results 3.2. Main analyses

3.1. Preliminary analyses 3.2.1. Research question one


In order to answer the first research question (Are there gender
Prior to conducting the main analyses that answer the research or grade level differences in prevalence rates of cyber victimization?), a
questions of interest, several preliminary analyses were conducted Gender (male, female) by Grade Level (6th, 7th, 8th) ANOVA was
to examine gender and grade level differences on each of the vari- conducted to determine if there were gender and grade level differ-
ables. These analyses were conducted to better understand any ences in cyber victimization. Results should be interpreted with
gender and grade level differences in all of the study variables. A caution due to a small n in some cells. The dependent variable
series of 2 (Gender) by 3 (Grade Level) ANOVAs were conducted was the Cyber Victimization Survey (CVS) total score. Neither the
on OBVQ Victimization score, Time Online, and the BASC-2 Com- gender main effect, F(1, 105) = .195, p = .660, the grade main effect,
posites scores (i.e., Emotional Symptoms Index, Personal Adjust- F(2, 105) = 1.728, p = .183, nor the interaction, F(2, 105) = 1.082,
ment, School Problems, and Internalizing Problems). Results p = .343, were significant. Means and standard deviations on the
revealed significant differences in traditional victimization by CVS score by gender and grade level are located in Table 2.
Grade Level, F(2, 105) = 3.888, p < .05, with 8th graders (M = 15.49)
scoring significantly higher than 6th graders (M = 13.02) on tradi- 3.2.2. Research question two
tional victimization. A significant difference in School Problems A correlation between the CVS score (i.e., cyber victimization)
by Gender was also found, F(1, 105) = 8.070, p < .01, with boys and the OBVQ victimization score (i.e., traditional victimization)
(M = 46.40) reporting greater levels of school problems than girls was used to answer the second research question (What is the over-
(M = 40.68). No other significant differences were found. See lap between victims of traditional bullying and cyber victims?). The
Table 1 for the means and standard deviations of main study correlation between the two scales (.174) was not significant with
variables by total sample and gender. p = .074. Additionally, the top 20% of cyber victims, as determined
by the Cyber Victimization Survey (a score of 22 or above), and the
top 20% of traditional victims, as determined by the Revised Olw-
eus Bully/Victim Questionnaire (a score of 13 or above), were com-
pared. Twelve students met the criteria on the CVS (i.e., cyber
victims) and 14 met criteria on the OBVQ (i.e., traditional victims).
Table 1
Four students scored in the top 20% on both measures. See Table 3
Means and standard deviations of primary variables by total sample and by gender.
for demographic information for each group of students, cyber vic-
Variable N Mean (SD) tim only, traditional victim only, and cyber victim and traditional
Total sample Time online 106 3.189 (1.91) victim.
CVS total score 106 16.30 (6.44)
OBVQ total score 106 9.65 (2.23)
BASC-2 composite scores 3.2.3. Research question three
Emotional symptoms index 106 42.18 (8.71) In order to answer the third research question (What social emo-
Personal adjustment 106 56.85 (8.85) tional outcomes, above and beyond traditional victimization, while
School problems 106 43.22 (7.96) controlling for time online, are associated with cyber victimization?),
Internalizing problems 106 43.13 (8.78)
BASC-2 subscale scores
a number of regression analyses were conducted separately for
Anxiety 106 44.44 (9.62) boys and girls using cyber victimization (i.e., CVS score) as the
Depression 106 43.91 (7.24) independent variable. In addition, the OBVQ Victimization score
Interpersonal relations 106 55.06 (8.19) and Time Online score were entered as independent variables to
Social stress 106 43.33 (9.61)
hold traditional victimization and amount of time spent online
Self-esteem 106 55.68 (6.75)
constant in all analyses. The following BASC-2 composite scores,
Female Time online 59 3.42 (1.75)
Emotional Symptoms Index, Personal Adjustment, School Prob-
CVS total score 59 15.53 (3.11)
OBVQ total score 59 9.51 (2.11) lems, and Internalizing Problems, were entered as the dependent
BASC-2 composite scores variables.
Emotional symptoms index 59 41.25 (8.55) Results of the BASC-2 composite scores for girls revealed that all
Personal adjustment 59 58.19 (7.79) regressions, with the exception of School Problems, were signifi-
School problems 59 40.68 (6.25)
Internalizing problems 59 41.93 (9.28)
cant. Specifically, the overall regression using the Emotional Symp-
BASC-2 subscales toms Index Score was significant, F(3, 58) = 8.185, p < .001. Time
Anxiety 59 44.27 (10.68)
Depression 59 43.14 (6.74)
Interpersonal relations 59 57.02 (6.17) Table 2
Social stress 59 41.25 (9.01) Means and Standard deviations by gender, grade level, and total sample on the CVS.
Self-esteem 59 55.40 (7.47)
Gender Grade N CVS Score Mean (SD)
Male Time online 47 2.89 (2.08)
CVS total score 47 17.26 (8.99) Female 6th grade 28 15.96 (2.32)
OBVQ total score 47 9.83 (2.37) 7th grade 22 16.92 (3.72)
BASC-2 composite scores 8th grade 9 17.56 (3.81)
Emotional symptoms index 47 43.34 (8.85) Total 59 16.56 (3.14)
Personal adjustment 47 55.17 (9.85) Male 6th grade 14 15.93 (1.98)
School problems 47 46.40 (8.75) 7th grade 27 20.37 (12.32)
Internalizing problems 47 44.64 (7.94) 8th grade 6 16.17 (1.60)
BASC-2 subscales Total 47 18.51 (9.59)
Anxiety 47 44.66 (8.20)
Total 6th grade 42 15.95 (2.19)
Depression 47 44.87 (7.78)
7th grade 49 18.82 (9.56)
Interpersonal relations 45 52.53 (9.73)
8th grade 15 17.00 (3.12)
Social stress 47 45.94 (9.79)
Self-esteem 45 56.04 (5.74) CVS total score 106 17.43 (6.83)
C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21 17

Table 3 accounted for a significant amount of variance in the Personal


Demographics on students who scored high on cyber and traditional victimization. Adjustment Problems for girls, R2 = .187, p < .01; however, no un-
Demographic Top CVS Top OBVQ Top scorers on CVS and ique predictors emerged. Results of analyses using the Internaliz-
variables scorers scorers OBVQ ing Problems Composite score were similarly significant,
Gender F(3, 58) = 10.117, p < .001. Time Online, CVS, and OBVQ scores ac-
Male 7 8 2 counted for a significant amount of variance in the Internalizing
Female 5 6 2 Problems Composite for girls R2 = .356, p < .001. Only the OBVQ
Grade score was a unique, significant predictor (b = .434, p < .01). See
6th 2 3 1 Table 4 for specific regression analyses for girls.
7th 9 7 2
8th 1 4 1
For boys, results of the regression analysis for the BASC-2 com-
posite scores revealed that all regressions were significant. The
Ethnicity
African American 1 1 1
overall regression using the Emotional Symptoms Index Score
Asian American 1 1 0 was significant, F(3, 46) = 6.665, p < . 01. Time Online, the CVS (cy-
White 8 9 3 ber victimization), and OBVQ (traditional victimization) scores ac-
Hispanic 0 1 0 counted for a significant amount of variance in the Emotional
American
Symptoms Index score, R2 = .317, p < .01. The OBVQ score was the
Other 1 0 0
Missing 1 2 0 only significant unique predictor (b = .542, p < .001). Similarly,
the overall regression using the Personal Adjustment Problems
Total 12 14 4
score was significant, F(3, 46) = 5.410, p < . 01. Time Online, the
CVS, and OBVQ scores accounted for a significant amount of vari-
ance, R2 = .274, p < .01. The OBVQ score was the only significant un-
Online, the CVS (cyber victimization), and OBVQ (traditional vic- ique predictor (b = .489, p < .01). Regressions using School
timization) scores accounted for a significant amount of variance Problems, F(3, 46) = 3.778, p < . 05, and Internalizing Problems,
in the Emotional Symptoms Index Score, R2 = .309, p < .001. Further F(3, 46) = 8.496, p < .001, were also significant overall. Time Online,
analysis determined that both the CVS score (b = .282, p < .05) and the CVS, and OBVQ scores accounted for a significant amount of the
the OBVQ scores (b = .352, p < .05) were significant individual pre- variance in the School Problems composite, R2 = .209, p < .05, and
dictors. Results of the regression analyses utilizing the Personal the Internalizing Problems Composite, R2 = .372, p < .001. Time On-
Adjustment Problems Composite score were also significant, line was a significant unique predictor for these scores, b = .439,
F(3, 58) = 4.208, p < .01. Time Online, the CVS and OBVQ scores p < .01, b = .549, p < .001, respectively. The OBVQ score was also a

Table 4
Regression analysis for the CVS total score, the OBVQ total score, and BASC-2 composite and subscale scores for girls.

Dependent variable Independent variable B SE B B R2


BASC-2 composite
Emotional symptoms index Time online .046 .551 .009 .309***
CVS* .768 .361 .282
OBVQ* 1.426 .539 .352
Personal adjustment Time online .437 .544 .098 .187**
CVS .540 .356 .218
OBVQ .917 .532 .249
School problems Time online .818 .455 .229 .119
CVS .326 .298 .164
OBVQ .291 .445 .098
Internalizing problems Time online .473 .577 .089 .356***
CVS .720 .378 .244
OBVQ** 1.907 .564 .434
BASC-2 subscales
Anxiety Time online 1.057 .659 .174 .366***
CVS .853 .431 .251
OBVQ** 2.166 .644 .429
Depression Time online .341 .440 .089 .289***
CVS* .638 .288 .297
OBVQ* 1.024 .430 .321
Interpersonal relations Time online .074 .422 .021 .223**
CVS .275 .276 .140
OBVQ** 1.111 .413 .380
Social stress Time online .251 .574 .049 .323***
CVS* .988 .376 .345
OBVQ* 1.318 .562 .309
Self-esteem Time online .203 .534 .046 .209**
CVS .602 .349 .244
OBVQ .986 .522 .269

OBVQ = Revised Olweus Bully/Victim questionnaire traditional victimization total score.


CVS = cyber victimization total score.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
18 C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21

significant unique predictor for Internalizing Problems (b = .549, F(3, 46) = 4.674, p < .01, Depression, F(3, 46) = 5.096, p < .01, Inter-
p < .001). See Table 5 for specific regression analyses for boys. personal Relations, F(3, 46) = 8.372, p < .001, Self-Esteem,
Results of the regression analyses using the BASC-2 subscales F(3, 46) = 3.374, p < .05, and Social Stress, F(3, 46) = 7.474, p < .001.
for girls revealed that all regressions were significant and Time On- Together, Time Online, the CVS, and the OBVQ scores accounted
line, the CVS (cyber victimization), and OBVQ (traditional victim- for a significant amount of variance in Anxiety, R2 = .246, p < .01,
ization) scores accounted for a significant amount of variance in Depression, R2 = .262, p < .01, Interpersonal Relations, R2 = .369,
the BASC-2 scores in all assessed areas. Specifically, results re- p < .001, Self-Esteem, R 2 = .191, p < .05, and Social Stress,
vealed that overall regression analyses were significant for Anxiety, R2 = .343, p < .001. Furthermore, results revealed the OBVQ (tradi-
F(3, 58) = 10.566, p < .001, Depression, F(3, 58) = 7.464, p < .001, tional victimization) score to be a significant individual predictor
Interpersonal Relations, F(3, 58) = 5.274, p<.01, Self-Esteem, in all regression analyses, Anxiety, b = .496, p < .01, Depression,
F(3, 58) = 4.854, p < .01, and Social Stress, F(3, 58) = 8.764, p < .001. b = .505, p < .001, Interpersonal Relations, b = .605, p < .001, Self-
Together, Time Online, the CVS, and the OBVQ scores accounted Esteem, b = .414, p < .01, and Social Stress, b = .562, p < .001. Nei-
for a significant amount of variance in Anxiety, R2 = .366, p < .001, ther Time Online nor the CVS (cyber victimization) total score was
Depression, R2 = .289, p < .001, Interpersonal Relations, R2 = .223, found to be a unique predictor in any subscale analyses for boys.
p < .01, Self-Esteem, R2 = .209, p < .01, and Social Stress, R2 = .323, See Table 5 for specific regression analyses for boys.
p < .001. Furthermore, results revealed the OBVQ score to be a sig-
nificant individual predictor of Anxiety, b = .429, p < .01, Depres-
sion, b = .321, p < .05, Interpersonal Relations, b = .380, p < .01, 4. Discussion
and Social Stress, b = .309, p < .05. The CVS score was found to be
a significant individual predictor of Depression, b = .297, p < .05, 4.1. Gender and grade level differences among cyber victims
and Social Stress, b = .345, p < .05. There were no unique predictors
for Self-Esteem. See Table 4 for specific regression analyses for It was predicted that females would report higher levels of cy-
girls. ber victimization compared to males and that cyber victimization
Results of the regression analyses using the BASC-2 subscales would be more common in 7th and 8th grades as opposed to 6th
for boys revealed that all regressions were significant and Time On- grade. Neither of these predictions was supported. No significant
line, the CVS (cyber victimization), and OBVQ (traditional victim- gender or grade level differences were found in the levels of cyber
ization) scores accounted for a significant amount of variance in victimization, indicating that levels of cyber victimization do not
the BASC-2 scores in all assessed areas. Specifically, results re- differ by grade or gender. Caution, however, should be used inter-
vealed that overall regression analyses were significant for Anxiety, preting the grade level results due to the small n in some cells.

Table 5
Regression analysis for the CVS total score, the OBVQ total score, and BASC-2 composite and subscale scores for boys.

Dependent variable Independent variable B SE B B R2


BASC-2 composites
Emotional symptoms index Time online .818 .568 .192 .309**
CVS .131 .123 .142
OBVQ*** 2.020 .471 .542
Personal adjustment Time online .509 .652 .107 .274**
CVS .253 .141 .237
OBVQ** 2.031 .542 .489
School problems Time online** 1.851 .604 .439 .209*
CVS .112 .131 .123
OBVQ .777 .502 .211
Internalizing problems Time online* 1.147 .489 .300 .372***
CVS .030 .106 .037
OBVQ*** 1.840 .406 .549
BASC-2 subscales
Anxiety Time online .213 .553 .054 .246**
CVS .019 .120 .022
OBVQ** 1.717 .459 .496
Depression Time online .468 .519 .125 .262**
CVS .080 .113 .098
OBVQ*** 1.655 .431 .505
Interpersonal relations Time online .168 .595 .036 .369***
CVS .046 .129 .046
OBVQ*** 2.460 .494 .605
Social stress Time online .864 .617 .183 .343***
CVS .004 .134 .004
OBVQ*** 2.320 .512 .562
Self-esteem Time online .244 .404 .088 .191**
CVS 1.08 .088 .179
OBVQ** 1.008 .335 .414

OBVQ = Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire traditional victimization total score.


CVS = cyber victimization total score.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21 19

Research on gender differences has been mixed. Li (2006) and involved in traditional bullying are involved in cyber bullying.
Slonje and Smith’s (2008) work found that differences between Ybarra, Espelage, et al. (2007) and Ybarra, Diener-West, et al.
males and females who reported being cyber victims were not sta- (2007) found that the majority of cyber victims were not tradi-
tistically significant. Raskauskas (2010) examined bullying via text tional victims; however, the cyber victims who were victimized
only and found no significant victimization differences between most often were also traditional victims. A number of studies (Li,
girls and boys. Several other studies have also failed to find signif- 2007; Mitchell et al., 2007; Raskauskas & Stoltz, 2007; Smith
icant gender differences for cyber victimization (Hinduja & Patchin, et al., 2008) found that the majority of cyber victims were involved
2008; Williams & Guerra, 2007). On the other hand, Kowalski and in traditional bullying behavior as bullies, victims, or bully-victims.
Limber (2007) found that middle school girls were more often cy- Erdur-Baker (2010) found that almost one-third of respondents
ber victims and cyber bully/victims than middle school boys. were both traditional and cyber victims. Future research that looks
Dempsey et al. (2009) found that among a large middle school at traditional and online bullying as well as victimization and fur-
sample, significantly more females reported being a cyber victim ther explores the overlap between these distinct, yet related con-
in the past 30 days than males, but this relationship was weak. structs is needed. Although this study did not find as great a
Contrastingly, Erdur-Baker’s (2010) study found that boys were cy- crossover between traditional and cyber victims, it is clear that stu-
ber victims more often than girls. It remains unclear if gender dif- dents who are victims of both traditional and online bullying are at
ferences are an important aspect of online bullying. Further greater risk than students who are victimized by only one type of
research is needed to better understand how gender may or may bullying. Further research should be conducted to better under-
not influence online behavior. stand this population.
Regarding grade level differences, similar to this study, a Cana-
dian study of 7th, 8th, and 9th graders did not find prevalence nor 4.3. Social emotional outcomes associated with cyber victimization
frequency differences by grade for cyber victimization (Beran &
Li, 2005). Kowalski and Limber (2007) found that among middle Time online, traditional victimization, and online victimization
school students, 6th graders were the least likely to be involved were significantly related to all outcomes for girls, with the excep-
in cyberbullying. Williams and Guerra (2007) looked at Internet tion of School Problems, and all outcomes for boys. For girls, tradi-
bullying in elementary, middle and high school and found that tional victimization was a significant unique predictor for all
Internet bullying was most frequent in middle school and declined significant analyses with the exception of Self Esteem; cyber vic-
in high school. It appears that cyber victimization may peak in timization was a significant predictor of the Emotional Symptoms
middle school, but further research comparing a wider range of Index, Depression, and Social Stress. Time Online was not a signif-
grades (e.g., middle versus high school) is needed. As technology icant predictor for any outcomes.
becomes more accessible and younger children have access to cell Similar to the girls, traditional victimization emerged as a un-
phones, tablets, and computers it will be interesting to see if and ique predictor for most outcomes for boys. Specifically, traditional
how late-aged elementary school children are impacted by online victimization predicted the Emotional Symptoms Index, Personal
aggression. Adjustment, Internalizing Problems, Anxiety, Depression, Interper-
The lack of consensus on a definition of cyberbullying likely sonal Relations, Social Stress, and Self Esteem. Unlike results for
influences results as well. Many researchers failed to look at com- girls, cyber victimization was not a unique predictor for any of
prehensive methods or modes of cyberbullying and some limited the boys’ outcomes while Time Online was a unique, significant
responses to specific technological methods (e.g., texting only) or predictor for the Internalizing Problems Composite Score and the
modes (e.g., cell phones only). Different time reference points, var- School Problems Composite Score for boys. The Internalizing Prob-
ied sample characteristics, international data, and inconsistent sur- lems Composite score consists of items that measure atypicality,
vey methodology make generalizing prevalence rates difficult. locus of control, social stress, depression, and sense of inadequacy
Some studies asked respondents about online experiences in the while the School Problems Composite consists of items that mea-
last 7 or 30 days (Dempsey et al., 2009; Hinduja & Patchin, sure attention and learning problems. A score indicates possible
2007), or the past 5–6 months (Raskauskas, 2010; Sourander academic difficulties, problems with motivation, attention, and
et al., 2010), while others asked about experiences over their life- learning (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). Male students who spend
time (Li, 2010). a lot of time online may do so because they have poor social rela-
tionships with peers. These students may have poor social skills
4.2. The overlap between traditional and cyber victims and therefore may choose to spend time alone with technology
or they may opt to seek out virtual relationships.
Regarding the overlap between traditional and cyber victimiza- This is one of the first studies to examine social emotional out-
tion, a moderate to strong positive correlation was predicted. This comes by gender and an interesting pattern emerged. When the
was not supported, indicating that there was not much overlap be- sample was analyzed by gender, cyber victimization did not pre-
tween these two types of victimization and they are distinct con- dict any social emotional outcomes for males; however, cyber vic-
structs. Additional support for this comes from the comparisons timization was a significant, unique predictor of Depression, Social
among students who scored highest on the OBVQ (i.e., traditional Stress, and the Emotional Symptoms Index score for girls. These re-
victimization), students who scored highest on the CVS (i.e., cyber sults suggest that girls may be at greater risk of poor emotional and
victimization), and students who scored high on both measures. social outcomes due to what happens online. Some research has
When we looked at students who scored in the top 20% on cyber found that girls may experience higher rates of internalizing
and traditional victimization, only four students, not quite 4%, distress as a result of bullying (Klomek, Marrocco, Kleinman,
met this criteria. This study looked only at victimization and did Schonfeld, & Gould, 2007; Turner, Exum, Brame, & Holt, 2013).
not assess online bullying. If both traditional and online bullying Girls may be more susceptible to cyber victimization because
had been considered as well, greater overlap may have been cyberbullying tends to be more relationally aggressive in nature
discovered. (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Fredstrom et al. (2011) found that, after
Traditional bullying has been found to be a strong predictor for controlling for school-based victimization, cyber victimization re-
cyberbullying and cyber victimization (Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Li, mained a significant predictor of lower rates of self-esteem, higher
2007). There appears to be a certain amount of overlap between rates of social stress, anxiousness, depressive symptoms, and locus
traditional bullying and cyber victimization, but not all students of control. Results of the current study found that girls experienced
20 C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21

similar outcomes. It is possible that girls are more sensitive in their 4.5. Implication of findings
interpretation of and response to online bullying behaviors.
Agatston, Kowalski, and Limber (2007) conducted focus groups Although a number of predictions were only partially supported
with middle and high school students. In this study, a majority of in this study, a number of significant and important results
female students reported that cyberbullying was a problem at their emerged. It was found that the relations between cyber victimiza-
school. Male students were less likely to report that it was a prob- tion and social emotional outcomes differed by gender such that
lem, suggesting that males and females may interpret and respond for girls, cyber victimization was related to internalizing problems,
to online behaviors differently. A better understanding of the con- depression, and social stress while relations were not found for
text and how girls as opposed to boys understand and define boys. Students who are victimized both traditionally and electron-
cyberbullying behaviors may allow researchers and practitioners ically are a small, but at risk population. School professionals and
to better understand these behaviors. practitioners should be aware that the risk for these students is
Emotional distress, depression, school problems, substance greater than the risk to children suffering only one form of victim-
abuse, delinquency, and significant psychosocial challenges have ization. Due to the increasing use of technology among adoles-
been associated with cyberbullying and victimization (Beran & Li, cents, it may be beneficial for school-based bullying prevention
2005; Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Mitchell et al., 2007; Shariff, programs to focus on both cyberbullying and traditional bullying
2005; Ybarra, Diener-West, et al., 2007; Ybarra, Espelage, et al., behaviors.
2007; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004). However, only a few studies Future research should delve into the conflicting gender results
(Dempsey et al., 2009; Fredstrom et al., 2011) have relied on reli- in terms of prevalence, levels, and social emotional outcomes asso-
able and valid psychometric instruments to measure outcomes. A ciated with online victimization. Better understanding of why
study published by Fredstrom et al. (2011), used the BASC-2 to some students experience more negative outcomes due to the
measure outcomes. Fredstrom’s work was based on a much larger additive effects of traditional and online victimization would help
sample (n = 802) of students who were a bit older (mean age 15.84 schools and practitioners better identify at-risk students, intervene
years) than the current study. The researchers found cyber victim- early, and implement prevention programs. Better understanding
ization to be a significant predictor of low self-esteem, higher the overlap between traditional and online victims will help pro-
social stress, anxiousness, depressive symptoms, and locus of con- fessionals identify and assist this small, but at risk population.
trol above and beyond the contributions of traditional There is much more to learn about how technology, aggression,
victimization. It will be important for future research to continue and communication intersect to make students vulnerable to
to look at the similar, yet distinct social emotional outcomes of tra- negative outcomes from both a victimization and a bullying
ditional and cyber victimization using psychometrically sound perspective.
instruments.
4.6. Conclusions
4.4. Limitations of current study
The results of this study indicate that levels of cyber victimiza-
Several limitations of the current study should be noted. The tion do not differ by grade or by gender, cyber victimization and
most significant limitation is sample size. Grade level analyses, traditional victimization are distinct but related constructs, and
for example, should be interpreted with caution given that there relations between cyber victimization and social emotional out-
were only 15 students in the 8th grade. In addition, the sample comes vary by gender, with girls suffering more than boys. This
was predominantly White non-Hispanic and from a rural area in study also confirmed that traditional bullying continues to be sig-
the Midwest. Additionally, all data collected were self-report. Some nificantly related to a number of negative social emotional out-
may consider self-report data to be a limitation. However, cyber- comes for all students. This is one of few studies to examine
bullying research to date has relied on self-report data and the ano- cyber victimization outcomes by gender. While there were no dif-
nymity and sometimes covert nature inherent in some cyber ferences in the levels of cyber victimization for boys and girls, cy-
behaviors may mean that self-report data is necessary to examine ber victimization alone significantly predicted a number of
and understand this construct. Self-selection bias may also have negative outcomes for girls and not for boys. This may mean the
impacted the results. Results should be interpreted with these lim- additive effect of traditional and online bullying is more problem-
itations in mind. atic for girls. It is also possible that traditional bullying remains the
The manner of categorizing students as cyber or traditional vic- primary means of significant, negative peer interactions among
tims might be considered a limitation, as well. In this study, stu- middle school males such that any additive effect of online bully-
dents were considered victims if they scored in the top 20% of ing does not more negatively affect most boys. Further research
the sample. The prevalence rate of cyber victimization in the sam- should continue to explore developmental and gender differences
ple is fairly low so it is possible that a few respondents who were of online bullying to better identify at risk students and to develop
classified as victims may have been victimized minimally. Other effective prevention and intervention programs.
researchers have considered a respondent a victim based on spe-
cific responses to certain items (Fredstrom et al., 2011; Wang, Ian- References
notti, & Nansel, 2009) or scoring above a certain score (Raskauskas,
2010). Agatston, P. W., Kowalski, R., & Limber, S. (2007). Students’ perspectives on cyber
Despite these limitations, a strength of the study was the use of bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(6), S59–S60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.jadohealth.2007.08.017.
instruments with strong psychometric properties to measure on- Beran, T., & Li, Q. (2005). Cyber-harassment: A study of a new method for an old
line and traditional victimization, and social emotional outcomes. behavior. Educational Computing Research, 32(3), 265–277. http://dx.doi.org/
Since the development of psychometrically sound measures of 10.2190/8YQM-B04H-PG4D-BLLH.
Bonanno, R. A., & Hymel, S. (2013). Cyber bullying and internalizing difficulties:
cyberbullying has been limited, the Cyber Victimization Survey
Above and beyond the impact of traditional forms of bullying. Journal of Youth
(CVS) was developed to investigate cyber victimization and answer and Adolescence, 42, 685–697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9937-1.
the study’s research questions of interest. The CVS shows promis- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA:
ing evidence of reliability and validity and future research Harvard University Press.
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-
should continue to investigate the psychometric properties of this psychological Adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722. http://dx.doi.org/
measure. 10.2307/1131945.
C.F. Brown et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 35 (2014) 12–21 21

Dehue, F., Bolman, C., & Trijntje, V. (2008). Cyberbullying: Youngsters’ experiences Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2006). Bullies move beyond the schoolyard: A
and parental perception. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11, 217–223. http:// preliminary look at cyberbullying. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 4(2),
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.0008. 148–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1541204006286288.
Dempsey, A. G., Sulkowski, M. L., Nichols, R., & Storch, E. A. (2009). Differences Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2012). Cyberbullying: An update and synthesis of
between peer victimization in cyber and physical settings and associated research. In J. W. Patchin & S. Hinduja (Eds.), Cyberbullying prevention and
psychosocial adjustment in early adolescence. Psychology in the Schools, 46, response (pp. 13–35). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
962–972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits. Raskauskas, J. (2010). Text-bullying: Associations with traditional bullying and
Diamanduros, T., Downs, T., & Jenkins, S. J. (2008). The role of school psychologists depression among New Zealand adolescents. Journal of School Violence, 9(1),
in the assessment, prevention, and intervention of cyberbullying. Psychology in 74–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15388220903185605.
the Schools, 45, 693–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.20335. Raskauskas, J., & Stoltz, A. D. (2007). Involvement in traditional and electronic
Erdur-Baker, O. (2010). Cyberbullying and its correlation to traditional bullying, bullying among adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 43, 564–575. http://
gender and frequent and risky usage of internet mediated communication dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.564.
tools. New Media & Society, 12, 109–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior assessment system for children
1461444809341260. (2nd ed). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (Eds.). (2004). Bullying in North American schools. Shariff, S. (2005). Cyber-dilemmas in the new millennium: School obligations to
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. provide student safety in a virtual school environment. Journal of Education, 40,
Fredstrom, B. K., Adams, R. E., & Gilman, R. (2011). Electronic and school-based. 467–487. <http://www.proquest.com>.
victimization: Unique contexts for adjustment difficulties during adolescence. Slonje, R., & Smith, P. (2008). Cyberbullying: Another main type of bullying?
Journal Youth Adolescence, 40, 405–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010- Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 147–154. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
9569-7. j.14679450.2007.00611.x.
Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2007). Offline consequences of online victimization: Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., & Tippett, N. (2008).
School violence and delinquency. Journal of School Violence, 6(3), 89–112. http:// Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary schools pupils. Journal of
dx.doi.org/10.1300/J202v06n03_06. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 376–385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2008). Cyberbullying: An exploratory analysis of factors 7610.2007.01846.
related to offending and victimization. Deviant Behavior, 29, 129–156. http:// Smith, P. K., & Slonje, R. (2010). Cyberbullying: The nature and extent of a new kind
dx.doi.org/10.1080/01639620701457816. of bullying, in and out of school. In S. R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage
Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2009). Bullying beyond the schoolyard preventing and (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective
responding to cyberbullying. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. (pp. 249–262). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2010). Bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide. Archives Solberg, M., & Olweus, D. (2003). Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the
of Suicide Research, 14, 206–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13811118. Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 239–268. http://
2010.494133. dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.10047.
Klomek, A. B., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., Schonfeld, I. S., & Gould, M. S. (2007). Sourander, A., Klomek, A. B., Ikonen, M., Londroos, J., Luntamo, T., Koskelainen, M.,
Bullying, depression, and suicidality in adolescents. Journal of the American et al. (2010). Psychosocial risk factors associated with Cyberbullying among
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 40–49. http://dx.doi.org/ adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 720–728. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1097/01.chi.0000242237.84925.18. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.79.
Kowalski, R. M., & Limber, S. P. (2007). Electronic bullying among middle school Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.).
students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, S22–S30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
j.jadohealth.2007.08.017. Turner, M. G., Exum, M. L., Brame, R., & Holt, T. J. (2013). Bullying victimization and
Kyriakides, L., Kaloyirou, C., & Lindsay, J. (2006). An analysis of the revised Olweus adolescent mental health: General and typological effects across sex. Journal of
bully/victim questionnaire using the Rasch measurement model. British Criminal Justice, 41(1), 53–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2012.12.005.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 781–801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ Varjas, K., Henrich, C. C., & Meyers, J. (2009). Urban middle school students’
000709905X53499. perceptions of bullying, cyberbullying, and school safety. Journal of School
Li, Q. (2006). Cyberbullying in schools: A research of gender differences. Violence, 8, 159–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15388220802074165.
School Psychology International, 27(2), 157–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Wang, J., Iannotti, R. J., & Nansel, T. R. (2009). School bullying among adolescents in
0143034306064547. the United States: Physical, verbal, relational, and cyber. Journal of Adolescent
Li, Q. (2007). New bottle but old wine: A research of cyberbullying in schools. Health, 45, 368–375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.012.
Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1777–1791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Williams, K. R., & Guerra, N. G. (2007). Prevalence and predictors of internet
j.chb.2005.10.005. bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, S14–S21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Li, Q. (2010). Cyberbullying in high schools: A study of students’ behaviors and j.jadohealth.2007.08.018.
beliefs about this new phenomenon. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Ybarra, M. L., Diener-West, M., & Leaf, P. J. (2007). Examining the overlap in internet
Trauma, 19, 372–392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10926771003788979. harassment and school bullying: Implications for school intervention. Journal
Mason, K. L. (2008). Cyberbullying: A preliminary assessment for school personnel. of Adolescent Health, 41, S42–S50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 323–348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.20301. 2007.09.004.
Mitchell, K. J., Ybarra, M., & Finkelhor, D. (2007). The relative importance of online Ybarra, M. L., Espelage, D. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2007). The co-occurrence of Internet
victimization in understanding depression, delinquency, and substance use. harassment and unwanted sexual solicitation victimization and perpetration:
Child Maltreatment, 5, 312–324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077559507305996. Associations with psychosocial indicators. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(6),
Olweus, D. (1996). The revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire, Mimeo. Bergen, S31–S41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.010.
Norway: Research Center for Health Promotion (HIMIL), University of Bergen. Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2004). Youth engaging in online harassment:
Olweus, D. (1997). Bully/victim problems in school: Knowledge base and an Associations with caregiver-child relationships, internet use, and personal
effective intervention program. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 18, 170–190. characteristics. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 319–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.285.16.2094. j.adolescence.2004.03.007.

You might also like