You are on page 1of 7

Authoritarian parenting outcomes: What happens to the kids?

2010 – 2022 Gwen Dewar, Ph.D., all rights reserved


https://parentingscience.com/authoritarian-parenting/

The authoritarian parenting style is about being strict and stern. It insists on


unquestioning obedience, and enforces good behavior through psychological control —
threats, shaming, and other punishments. As defined by psychologists, it’s also a style
associated with relatively little parental warmth and responsiveness (Baumrind 1991).
That doesn’t bode well for children’s health outcomes, especially in stressful
environments. As I note elsewhere, warmth and responsiveness can protect kids from
the effects of toxic stress.

But what about other outcomes? Are the children of authoritarian parents well-adjusted?
Do they get along with others? Do they excel in school? If authoritarian parents are
demanding, shouldn’t their children be better-behaved and more successful in the
classroom?

It sounds plausible, but of course we need some context. Better-behaved and more
successful compared with what? Typically, researchers compare authoritarian parenting
with two other styles:

 permissive parenting, in which parents are emotionally warm, but


reluctant to enforce rules or standards of conduct; and
 authoritative parenting, in which parents set limits and enforce
standards, but also show high levels of warmth and responsiveness.

Note that “authoritative” parenting goes by a label that sounds deceptively similar to
“authoritarian”. But the contrast is stark, and it isn’t only because authoritative
parents show more warmth. Unlike authoritarian parents, authoritative parents
encourage their children to ask questions, and they explain the rationale behind the
rules. In addition, authoritative parents avoid controlling their kids through the
induction of shame, guilt, or the withdrawal of love.

So — with these alternatives in mind — how does authoritarianism measure up?


Evidence that authoritarian parenting makes behavior problems worse

When kids have really difficult behavior problems, it might seem that the only
remedy is severe disciple — to control children through threats, harsh punishments,
or shaming. But research suggests these tactics don’t result in long-term behavioral
improvements. On the contrary, they seem to make things worse.

For instance, let’s consider what psychologists call “externalizing problems” —


disruptive, aggressive, defiant, or anti-social behavior. If authoritarian disciplinary
tactics were effective, we would expect them to lead to fewer such behavior
problems as children get older.

But that isn’t what we observe when we track children’s development. In a meta-
analysis of more than 1400 published studies, Martin Pinquart found that harsh
discipline and psychological control were actually the biggest predictors of a child’s
behavior worsening over time (Pinquart 2017). Kids subjected to authoritarian tactics
at one time point tended to develop more externalizing behavior problems at later
time points.

Why? We can’t assume it’s entirely due to authoritarian parenting. Maybe genetic
factors are partly to blame. After all, we know that genetic factors can raise a child’s risk
of developing certain kinds of behavior problems, and such kids may tend to provoke
authoritarian responses from their caregivers. Parents see their children misbehaving,
and they struggle to find a solution. They feel stressed and frustrated. They crack down
with harsh discipline — threats and punishments — and show less warmth toward their
misbehaving children.

It’s a scenario where behavior problems and authoritarian parenting are linked, but not
necessarily because authoritarian parenting causes behavior problems. Instead, the
kids themselves are part of the story. Their misbehavior triggers authoritarian
responses.

So how do we find out what’s really going on? When researchers have tried to tease
apart causation, they’ve confirmed that children’s pre-existing behavior problems can
indeed provoke authoritarian reactions from parents. But there is also evidence that
authoritarianism is harmful. It appears to make children’s behavior problems
worse.
For example, in a behavioral genetics study of twins, Rebecca Waller and her
colleagues focused on kids with “callous unemotional traits” — traits that like low
empathy and poor moral self-regulation (Waller et al 2018).

These traits are linked with serious behavior problems, and Waller’s team confirmed
that genes matter. Some kids were at higher genetic risk for developing “callous
unemotional traits.” But parenting style also had an effect. When parents showed
children lots of warmth and affection, kids were less likely to develop callous
unemotional traits. Even kids who were at high genetic risk showed fewer symptoms.

What about the development of social skills? Does authoritarianism help or hurt?

It’s hard to know if authoritarian parenting causes children to develop differently. But


when researchers compare different parenting styles, they’ve found that authoritarian
parenting is linked with lower social competence (Steinberg et al 1994; Chen et al 1997;
Zhou et al 2004; Martinez et al 2007; Garcia and Gracia 2009; Garcia et al 2020).

For example, in a study of more than 2500 adults and adolescents living in Spain, folks
who identified their parents as authoritarian tended to score lower on empathy
(Martinez-Escudero et al 2020).

In Turkey, high school students from authoritarian families were rated as less
resourceful than kids from authoritative or permissive parents (Turkel and Tzer 2008).

And in the Netherlands, kids with authoritarian parents were rated as less helpful and
less popular by their teachers and classmates. They were also rated as less mature in
their reasoning about moral issues (Dekovic and Jannsens 1992; Jannsens and
Dekovic 1997). In addition, there is evidence that American teenagers feel lower levels
of social acceptance from their peers when they are raised by authoritarian parents
(Lamborn et al 1991; Steinberg et al 1992; Steinberg et al 1994).

Is there any connection between authoritarian parenting and bullying?

Yes. Numerous studies — conducted in a variety of cultural settings — suggest that


kids are at higher risk for involvement in bullying if their parents use authoritarian tactics
or attempt to control them through the use of punitive discipline (Georgiou et al 2013;
Luk et al 2016; Krisana et al 2019; Martínez-Ferrer  et al 2019; Zurcher et al 2018;
Gómez-Ortiz et al 2016).
Links between authoritarian parenting and emotional problems

Does authoritarianism put kids at greater risk of emotional problems? It might.

In places as diverse as Germany, the Caribbean, China, Taiwan, and the United States,
researchers have noticed links between authoritarian parenting and emotional troubles
— including childhood anxiety, symptoms of depression, and poor self-regulation
(Azman et al 2012; Wolfradt et al 2003; King et al 2016; Lipps et al 2012; Chang 2003;
Wang et al 2006; Gan et al 2021; Huang et al 2019; Gan et al 2021).
Moreover, researchers in Latin cultures have recorded lower levels of self-esteem
among adolescents from authoritarian homes (Martinez et al 2020; Queiroz et al 2020),
and the effects may linger into adulthhood: Spanish adults report less happiness and life
satisfaction if they were raised by strict, authoritarian parents (Garcia et al 2020).

Is this simply a case of shared genetics? Maybe authoritarian parents tend to be at


higher genetic risk for developing emotional problems, and their kids inherit those same
genes. But there’s evidence against this as the primary explanation.

For example, in a behavioral genetics study of Chinese twins, researchers found that
kids with authoritarian fathers were more likely to suffer from a psychiatric disorder —
even after accounting for the influence of genes (Yin et al 2016). Similarly, a behavioral
genetics study conducted in the United States suggests that authoritarian parenting puts
kids at higher risk for experiencing major depression during adulthood (Long et al 2015).

And in a meta-analysis of 31 studies on the development of foster children, researchers


found that kids were more likely to end up with emotional problems if their foster parents
took an authoritarian approach to discipline (Chodura et al 2021).

Yet it’s worth noting: The authoritarian parenting style isn’t always linked with emotional
problems.

Some studies of American adolescents have failed to find emotional differences


between kids from authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive homes (Lamborn et al
1991; Steinberg et al 2006). And research on adolescents in the Middle East has also
failed to find a link between authoritarian care-giving and psychological problems like
depression (Dwairy 2004; Dwairy and Menshar 2006).

Why the inconsistencies? I suspect the effects of authoritarianism depend on how


harsh, cold, or punitive the parent is. For instance, as I noted in my article about
spanking, research suggests that corporal punishment is linked with higher rates of
depression and anxiety among children. Maybe some forms of authoritarian parenting
are more intense — more negatively impactful — than others.

It also seems likely that culture plays a role. If kids perceive authoritarianism as normal
and mainstream, they may be less distressed by it (Dwairy 2004). Consistent with this
idea, in societies where it’s normative for parents to exert lots of psychological control,
links between parental control and poor emotional outcomes are weaker (Lansford et al
2018).

How do children from authoritarian homes perform academically?

Several lines of evidence suggest that authoritarianism puts children at an academic


disadvantage. For example, experimental research suggests that authoritarian
approaches interfere with learning.

In a fascinating study of kindergartners, Melissa Kamins and Carol Dweck have shown
that a common tactic of authoritarian care-giving — shaming a child for poor
performance — can make kids perform more poorly on problem-solving tasks (Kamins
and Dweck 1999). Moreover, experiments suggest that people learn better from positive
feedback than from negative feedback, and this may be especially true for kids
(Schmittmann et al 2006; van Duijvenvoorde et al 2008).

There is also evidence that authoritarian parents are less helpful in teaching their
children to think critically.

In a recent study of nearly 150 American parents, Candace Mills and colleagues found
that parents who endorsed authoritarian values responded differently to a child’s difficult
science questions (Mills et al 2022). These parents were less likely to confess their own
uncertainty, even though they were more likely to provide answers that were incorrect.
They were also less likely to model an approach for coping with uncertainty (like: “Hey,
let’s see if we can find the answer online…”).

Finally, researchers have detected correlations between authoritarianism and lower


school achievement.

For instance, several studies conducted in the United States have found that the
authoritarian parenting style was linked with lower school grades for all ethnic groups
(Dornbusch et al 1987; Steinberg et al 1989; Steinberg et al 1992), though it appears
that the effects may be less negative for Hispanic children than for white, non-Hispanic
children (Pinkart and Kauser 2018).

And in both Germany and China, children tend to experience worse academic outcomes
when their parents impose high levels of psychological control (Teuber et al 2021; Li
and Hein 2019; Wang et al 2012; Xu et al 2019).

Counter evidence: Do children ever benefit from authoritarian parenting?

When researchers have reviewed more than 425 studies conducted worldwide, they
found that “authoritarian parenting was associated with at least 1 negative outcome in
all regions of the globe” (Pinquart and Kauser 2018). But we should keep in mind: The
effects vary in their intensity, and, as noted above, not all research has been negative.

For example, we’ve mentioned cultural settings where authoritarian parenting doesn’t
seem to raise a child’s risk for emotional problems. In addition, some studies have failed
to demonstrate a negative effect on academic achievement for families of lower
economic status (Lamborn et al 1996; Steinberg et al 2009), and, in at least one case,
investigators have argued that these kids may actually do better in school when they
come from authoritarian homes (Leung et al 1988).

Moreover, there has been some controversy about the effects of authoritarian parenting
in traditional Chinese families. On the one hand, authoritarianism has been linked
with poorer school performance in Beijing (e.g., Chen et al 1997) and Taiwan (e.g.,
Pong et al 2010). On the other hand, some studies of Hong Kong Chinese (Leung et al
1998) and of Chinese immigrants to North America (Chao 2001) have linked
authoritarian parenting with higher school achievement. Why the discrepancies?
Researchers have suggested several possibilities.

 Perhaps kids living in dangerous, disadvantaged neighborhoods are less


likely to run afoul of authority figures—in and out of school—when they are
taught unquestioning obedience.
 Maybe peer pressure swamps the effects of parenting. Some peer groups
support school achievement. Others discourage it. In one study of U.S.
students, researchers found that the Asian Americans in the sample
tended to have peer groups that encouraged scholarship, and they
performed well at school even when their parents were authoritarian. By
contrast, the African Americans in the sample tended to have peer groups
that rejected high achievers. These kids did more poorly in school even
when their parents were authoritative and highly-educated (Steinberg et al
1992).
 Authoritarian parenting may have different meanings in different cultures.
Ruth Chao has argued that the Chinese version of authoritarian parenting
is fundamentally different. Unlike Western authoritarian parents, Chinese
authoritarian parents have closer relationships to their kids, and closeness
is a predictor of higher school achievement (Chao 2001).

But — personally — I’m skeptical about the idea that authoritarian parenting could make
some kids into better scholars. The experimental research is compelling. Moreover,
achievement in math, science, and many other academic fields depends on critical
thinking—something that authoritarian parenting seems to discourage.

Indeed, there is evidence that schools run along authoritarian principles produce inferior
students. In a study comparing American high schools, Lisa Pellerin found that
authoritative schools got the best results. Authoritarian schools had the worst rates of
dropouts (Pellerin 2004).

How does authoritarian parenting influence the development of a child’s moral


sense?

Authoritarian parents might see themselves as champions of morality. But, as noted


above, studies suggest that kids with authoritarian parents are actually less advanced
when it comes to self-regulation and moral reasoning (Dekovic and Jannsens 1992;
Jannsens and Dekovic 1997; Karreman et al 2006; Piotrowski et al 2013).

Moreover, kids from authoritarian families may be more likely to “tune out” their parents
as they get older. For instance, when researchers tracked American middle and high
school studies over 18 months, they found that kids who identified their parents as more
authoritarian were more likely to reject their parents as legitimate authority figures. They
were also more likely to engage in delinquency over time (Trinker et al 2012).

And in a study of American undergraduates, researchers asked students who they


consulted when they had to make moral decisions. Undergraduates with authoritative
parents were the most likely to say they would talk with their parents. Students
with authoritarian parents — like students from permissive families — were more likely
to reference their peers (Bednar et al 2003).

You might also like