You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Associations between the broad autism phenotype, adult attachment, and T


relationship satisfaction among emerging adults
Jenna H. Beffela, , Kyla M. Carya, Amy K. Nuttalla, William J. Chopikb, Megan K. Maasa

a
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Michigan State University, United States of America
b
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, United States of America

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Traits comprising the broad autism phenotype (aloof personality, pragmatic language deficits, and rigid per-
Broad autism phenotype sonality) have been linked to difficulty forming and maintaining close relationships, as well as attachment
Adult attachment insecurity. The present study examined how the three individual broad autism phenotype traits were associated
Relationship satisfaction with emerging adults' romantic relationship satisfaction via attachment anxiety and avoidance. Results of a
Emerging adulthood
parallel mediation model revealed that aloof personality was associated with lower relationship satisfaction via
anxiety and avoidance, pragmatic language was associated with lower relationship satisfaction via avoidance,
and rigidity was associated with lower relationship satisfaction via anxiety. In conclusion, individuals high in
BAP may be at risk for lower relationship satisfaction as a result of higher attachment insecurity.

1. Introduction relationship formation and maintenance is associated with a variety of


positive outcomes. Romantic attachments are each associated with
Subclinical traits of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) referred to as greater life satisfaction among emerging adults (Guarnieri et al., 2015).
the broader autism phenotype (BAP) are of growing interest to re- Romantic relationship experiences in emerging adulthood are pre-
searchers (Ruzich et al., 2016). BAP is comprised of three key char- dictive of perceived adulthood and identity development (Barry et al.,
acteristics: aloof personality, pragmatic language deficits, and rigid 2009). Similarly, romantic relationship quality predicts happiness and
personality (Hurley et al., 2007). BAP is common among family mem- well-being among emerging adults (Demir, 2008; Gómez-López et al.,
bers of individuals with ASD (Sasson et al., 2013) and in the general 2019). Although the association between ASD traits and romantic re-
population (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Constantino and Todd, 2003). In lationship satisfaction has been observed in adult, married couples
both populations, studies have consistently demonstrated that BAP is (Pollmann et al., 2010; Pruitt et al., 2018), research of this association
associated with risks for relational outcomes. However, further research within emerging adulthood is lacking. Given the regarding social
is needed to understand the influence of BAP on adult attachment and struggles experienced by individuals high in ASD traits (Jobe and
relationship satisfaction in order to inform practitioners and educators Williams White, 2007), potential pathways between ASD traits and
focused on healthy relationship development and maintenance. relationship satisfaction deserve further exploration.
ASD traits such as those that comprise BAP result in challenges in
1.1. BAP and relationship satisfaction establishing and maintaining social connections (American Psychiatric
Association, 2013). As such, one would expect that individuals high in
BAP is associated with fewer friendships (Jobe and Williams White, BAP would struggle to maintain romantic relationships. Although the
2007) and lower relationship satisfaction among individuals in ro- quality of non-romantic relationships has been found to be impacted by
mantic relationships (Pruitt et al., 2018), highlighting the risks BAP an individual's BAP level (Faso et al., 2016), the impact on the quality
poses to relational outcomes. The effects of BAP are particularly salient of romantic relationships remains under-studied. Among a sample of
among emerging adults, given the stage-salient developmental task of heterosexual married couples, individuals higher in subclinical traits of
forming and maintaining meaningful romantic relationships (Collins ASD similar reported lower martial satisfaction (Pollmann et al., 2010;
and van Dulmen, 2007). During emerging adulthood, intimate Pruitt et al., 2018). Yet, the mechanisms involved in the associations

Corresponding author at: 552 West Circle Drive, 13D Human Ecology, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States of America.

E-mail addresses: beffelje@msu.edu (J.H. Beffel), carykyla@msu.edu (K.M. Cary), nuttall@msu.edu (A.K. Nuttall), chopikwi@msu.edu (W.J. Chopik),
maasmeg1@msu.edu (M.K. Maas).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110409
Received 6 July 2020; Received in revised form 17 September 2020; Accepted 19 September 2020
Available online 24 September 2020
0191-8869/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J.H. Beffel, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

between BAP and romantic relationship satisfaction remain unknown. 1.3. The present study
When studying the risks BAP poses to relational outcomes, it is
important to investigate how the three key BAP characteristics may The present study examined the BAP traits of aloofness, pragmatic
differentially impact outcomes. Extant literature demonstrates that language, and rigidity as they relate to relationship satisfaction among
aloofness, pragmatic language deficits, and rigid personality may have emerging adults. We modeled the three BAP traits separately due to
distinct effects on relational outcomes. Although aloofness and rigid evidence supporting differential associations between each BAP trait
personality have been shown to be negatively associated with re- and relational outcomes (Pruitt et al., 2018). Additionally, we ex-
lationship satisfaction, pragmatic language difficulties have shown no amined the potential mediating effects of attachment anxiety and
association with relationship satisfaction (Pruitt et al., 2018). Thus, we avoidance on the association between BAP traits and relationship sa-
aim to re-examine associations between individual BAP traits and re- tisfaction. We hypothesized that 1) BAP traits would be associated with
lationship satisfaction. lower levels of relationship satisfaction; 2) BAP traits would be asso-
ciated with higher levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance; and 3)
higher levels of anxiety and avoidance would mediate the relationship
1.2. The mediating role of attachment among BAP traits and relationship satisfaction.

Adult romantic attachment is well-established as critical for main- 2. Material and methods
taining romantic relationship satisfaction and well-being (Butzer and
Campbell, 2008; Galinha et al., 2014; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2005). 2.1. Participants and procedures
Attachment styles—initially formed in childhood between caregivers
and infants—are carried over into and influence adult romantic re- Data was collected from an online survey study of romantic and
lationships (Hazan and Shaver, 1987), as evidenced by research de- family relationships among college students at a large, midwestern
monstrating that individual attachment styles are fairly stable over time university. Survey and procedures were approved by the university's
(Fraley, 2002; Fraley et al., 2011). Insecurely attached adults may be Institutional Review Board. Participants were recruited through a uni-
high in romantic attachment anxiety (i.e., preoccupation with social versity subject pool and compensated with course credit. The study
support and fear of abandonment/rejection) and/or high in romantic included 263 undergraduate students who were currently in a romantic
attachment avoidance (i.e., avoidance of intimacy and discomfort of relationship. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 25 years old
closeness) (Brennan et al., 1998). (M = 20.15, SD = 1.52; 79.5% women). In identifying race/ethnicity,
Both adult attachment and BAP have heritable components and 58.6% of participants identified as White/Caucasian, followed by
likely share common sources of variance (Barbaro et al., 2017; Chen 19.4% Asian or Pacific Islander, 16% Black or African American, 6.8%
and Johnson, 2012; Constantino and Todd, 2003).1 BAP is thought to Hispanic or Latinx, 1.9% American Indian or Alaskan Native, and 3.4%
precede the development of adult attachment, as BAP has clearer ge- other.
netic determinants, emerges early in life, and persists in a more stable Questionnaires were given in order of levels of proximity: First, we
way compared to contemporary frameworks of adult attachment (Parr asked about parent relationships, then sibling relationships and, lastly,
et al., 2015). Further, BAP may influence adult attachment primarily romantic relationships. If participants indicated that they were not
through its effects on empathic deficits that accumulate through currently in a romantic relationship, they ended the survey after the
childhood and adolescence (Lamport and Turner, 2014). Indeed, prior sibling questionnaires.
research has consistently found associations between ASD traits and
insecure adult attachment. For example, among a sample of emerging 2.2. Measures
adults, higher levels of ASD traits were associated with more avoidance,
suggesting that individuals higher in ASD traits are less likely to ex- 2.2.1. Broad autism phenotype questionnaire (BAPQ; Hurley et al., 2007)
perience emotional closeness and intimate communication with their The BAPQ is a 36-item, self-report questionnaire measuring the sub-
romantic partners (Gallitto and Leth-Steensen, 2015). Similarly, addi- clinical expressions of the defining features of ASD among individuals
tional research demonstrates that BAP is positively associated with both who are not diagnosed with ASD. Participant responses are recorded on
anxiety and avoidance (Lamport and Turner, 2014). Moreover, adult a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very rarely) to 6 (very often) and
attachment plays an important role in determining the success of ro- mean scores for each scale were calculated. The BAPQ consists of three
mantic relationships (Butzer and Campbell, 2008; Jones and subscales: aloof personality (12 items; α = 0.89), rigid personality (12
Cunningham, 1996). Thus, these associations between individuals items; α = 0.80), and pragmatic language deficits (12 items; α = 0.74).
higher in BAP may experience detriments in romantic relation- Sample items include, “I would rather talk to people to get information
ships—possibly through their associations with insecure attachment. than to socialize” for aloof personality, “people have to talk me into
trying something new” for rigid personality, and “I find it hard to get
my words out smoothly” for pragmatic language deficits.

2.2.2. Experiences in close relationships questionnaire-revised (ECR-R;


1
There is now little controversy about whether there is substantial herit- Fraley et al., 2000)
ability in psychological traits (like adult attachment and BAP; Barbaro et al., The ECR-R is a 38-item, self-report questionnaire that assesses an-
2017; Constantino and Todd, 2003). However, finding the exact genetic me- xious and avoidant attachment. Participants responded to the questions
chanisms (i.e., single nucleotide polymorphisms; SNPs) linking the two traits on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly
has proved elusive. Although there is evidence that the oxytocin receptor gene agree). Mean scores were calculated for each subscale. The ECR-R in-
might play a causal role in explaining variation in adult attachment and BAP cludes two subscales: attachment anxiety (18 items; α = 0.93) and
(Chen and Johnson, 2012), more representative work finds that the effect sizes
attachment avoidance (18 items; α = 0.95). Example items for the
of individual SNPs linking the two likely explain small amounts of variance and
attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance subscales include, “I
are not replicable, particularly when examining adult attachment (Fraley et al.,
2013). We acknowledge that both traits likely share some sources of genetic worry that my partner doesn't really love me” and, “I prefer not to show
variation but encourage future research to more formally examine the sources a partner how I feel deep down”, respectively.
of this variation in large, population-wide studies (Briley et al., 2019). Never-
theless, given the likely shared sources of variation in both, it is important to 2.2.3. Couple satisfaction index-16 (CSI-16; Funk and Rogge, 2007)
keep this in mind when examining associations between the two. The CSI-16 is a 16-item, self-report questionnaire measuring

2
J.H. Beffel, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

relationship satisfaction among individuals currently in a romantic re- estimated to lie between −3.26 and 0.04 with 95% confidence, which,
lationship. For 15 of the CSI-16 items, responses are recorded on a 6- contrary to our hypothesis, does not indicate a significant indirect ef-
point Likert scale which ranges from 0 to 5. For the remaining one fect. The direct effect of pragmatic language on relationship satisfaction
question, responses are recorded on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from was estimated to lie between 1.00 and 7.50 with 95% confidence, in-
0 to 6; all 16 items were summed for a total score (α = 0.96) Sample dicating a significant direct effect. Higher levels of pragmatic language
items include: “Please indicate the degree of happiness, all things difficulties were associated with more relationship satisfaction.
considered, of your relationship” with response options ranging from 0 The indirect effect of rigidity on relationship satisfaction through
(extremely unhappy) to 6 (perfect) and, “in general, how often do you the mediating variable of anxiety was estimated to lie between −0.01
think that things between you and your partner are going well?” with and − 0.18 with 95% confidence, indicating a significant indirect ef-
response options ranging from 0 (never) to 5 (all the time). fect. Higher levels of rigidity were associated with more anxiety, which
was associated with less relationship satisfaction. Surprisingly, the in-
2.3. Analytic strategy direct effect of rigidity on relationship satisfaction via avoidance was
estimated to lie between 0.16 and 2.45 with 95% confidence, demon-
Path analyses were used to evaluate (1) the association between strating a significant indirect effect indicating that higher levels of ri-
BAP traits (aloof personality, pragmatic language deficits, rigidity) with gidity were associated with more relationship satisfaction. The direct
relationship satisfaction, (2) BAP traits with anxiety and avoidance, and effect of rigidity on relationship satisfaction was estimated to lie be-
(3) indirect effects of BAP traits on relationship satisfaction via both tween −4.18 and 1.30 with 95% confidence, which does not indicate a
anxiety and avoidance in a parallel mediation model (Hayes, 2018). significant direct effect.
View the hypothesized mediation model2 in Fig. 1. Mediation was
tested using the percentile bootstrap method with 1000 bootstrapped 4. Discussion
samples to construct 95% confidence intervals (Falk, 2018; Hayes and
Scharkow, 2013). The model was tested in Mplus version 8.1 (Muthén The present study found that aloofness was associated with lower
and Muthén, 2018) using full information maximum likelihood to ac- relationship satisfaction via higher anxiety and avoidance, pragmatic
count for missing data. language was associated with lower relationship satisfaction via higher
avoidance, and rigidity was associated with lower relationship sa-
tisfaction via lower anxiety. Furthermore, pragmatic language was not
3. Results
associated with relationship satisfaction via anxiety, and rigidity was
associated with higher relationship satisfaction via avoidance. These
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are reported in
findings support previous literature reporting that BAP traits are asso-
Table 1. The model was saturated. Unstandardized path coefficients are
ciated with higher levels of insecure attachment (Lamport and Turner,
presented in Table 2, and standardized effects are reported here in text.
2014), and that insecure attachment predicts lower levels of relation-
Aloofness (b = 0.18, p = .03), pragmatic language (b = 0.27,
ship satisfaction (Jones and Cunningham, 1996).
p < .001), and rigidity (b = 0.17, p = .01) were positively associated
Findings regarding aloof personality are consistent with prior re-
with anxiety. Aloofness was positively associated with avoidance
search demonstrating that aloofness is associated with lower relation-
(b = 0.23, p = .01), but pragmatic language was not associated with
ship satisfaction (Pruitt et al., 2018). An antecedent of this effect may
avoidance (b = 0.16, p = .06). Interestingly, rigidity was negatively
be that aloofness is associated with a dislike of emotionally and phy-
associated with avoidance (b = −0.15, p = .02). The direct effects of
sically intimate situations, a or lack of awareness of when to provide
aloofness (b = −0.03, p = .68) and rigidity (b = −0.07, p = .32) on
support, and a dislike of socializing generally—all correlates of at-
relationship satisfaction were not significant. However, pragmatic lan-
tachment avoidance (e.g., Edelstein and Shaver, 2004; Mikulincer and
guage had a positive direct effect on relationship satisfaction (b = 0.18,
Shaver, 2005). Somewhat surprisingly, attachment anxiety also medi-
p = .01). Anxiety (b = −0.25, p < .01) and avoidance (b = −0.41,
ated the association between aloofness and relationship satisfaction.
p < .001) had negative direct associations with relationship satisfac-
Attachment anxiety may be more closely linked with aloofness through
tion.
the ways in which social anxiety and perseverative thoughts (Saffrey
The indirect effect of aloofness on relationship satisfaction through
and Ehrenberg, 2007) or extreme negative attributions (Collins, 1996)
anxiety was estimated to lie between −1.65 and − 0.004 with 95%
interfere with ability to identify social cues. Future research should
confidence, indicating a significant indirect effect. Higher levels of
distinguish the components of attachment anxiety and avoidance that
aloofness were associated with higher anxiety, which was associated
mediate aloofness-relational outcome associations.
with less relationship satisfaction. The indirect effect of aloofness on
Pragmatic language deficits were associated with lower relationship
relationship satisfaction through avoidance was estimated to lie be-
satisfaction via higher levels of avoidance. This is consistent with pre-
tween −3.17 and − 0.42 with 95% confidence, indicating a significant
vious research reporting a similar association (Faso et al., 2016). Due to
indirect effect. Higher levels of aloofness were associated with higher
challenges understanding others' communications, people with such
avoidance, which was associated with lower relationship satisfaction.
difficulties may struggle to create and maintain relationships (Wainer
The direct effect of aloofness on relationship satisfaction was estimated
et al., 2011). Therefore, these individuals may experience a sense of
to lie between −3.16 and 2.07 with 95% confidence, indicating a
inadequacy in relationships that leads them to avoid relationships
nonsignificant direct effect.
(Lamport and Turner, 2014). Additionally, attachment avoidance cre-
The indirect effect of pragmatic language on relationship satisfac-
ates a sense of feeling out-of-sync and disconnectedness with relational
tion through the mediating variable of avoidance was estimated to lie
partners (Weimer et al., 2004). Given our findings, pragmatic language
between −3.20 and − 0.41 with 95% confidence, indicating a sig-
flow appears to be an important antecedent to this process of feeling in-
nificant indirect effect. Higher levels of pragmatic language were as-
sync and connected. Along with difficulties in conversations, avoidant
sociated with higher avoidance, which, was associated with less re-
individuals may also have systematic biases that lead them to re-
lationship satisfaction. The indirect effect of pragmatic language on
member conversations as more adversarial and out-of-sync than they
relationship satisfaction through the mediating variable of anxiety was
typically were (Feeney and Cassidy, 2003; Simpson et al., 2010).
Furthermore, mediation through attachment anxiety was not sup-
2
A standard linear regression revealed that the interaction between anxiety ported. A search of the literature revealed that anxious individuals
and avoidance was non-significant, b = 0.64, s.e. = 0.59, p = .28, and thus was readily embrace the reciprocity and synchrony of close relationships
not included in the model. and conversational partners (Guerrero and Burgood, 1996), possibly

3
J.H. Beffel, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

Fig. 1. Hypothesized parallel mediation path model.

Table 1
Correlations, means, and standard deviations among survey measures.
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. BAPQ aloof personality –


2. BAPQ pragmatic language 0.58⁎⁎ –
3. BAPQ rigid personality 0.45⁎⁎ 0.39⁎⁎ –
4. ECR-R anxiety 0.41⁎⁎ 0.44⁎⁎ 0.35⁎⁎ –
5. ECR-R avoidance 0.26⁎⁎ 0.24⁎⁎ 0.02 0.50⁎⁎ –
6. CSI relationship satisfaction −0.17⁎⁎ −0.07 −0.11 −0.41⁎⁎ −0.50⁎⁎ –
M 2.94 2.80 3.25 3.13 2.53 65.92
SD 0.87 0.65 0.68 1.17 1.12 14.58

Note: ⁎⁎
p < .001; BAPQ = Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire.

Table 2 these increased levels of anxiety are associated with attachment anxiety
Unstandardized path coefficients for effects of BAP subscales on attachment and (Nielsen et al., 2017). Therefore, it is not surprising that rigidity was
relationship satisfaction. associated with lower relationship satisfaction via higher levels of at-
Unstandardized parameters Estimate SE p tachment anxiety.
Contrary to other dimensions of BAP, rigidity was not associated
Aloofness ➔ anxiety 0.24 0.11 0.03 with lower relationship satisfaction via higher avoidance. In fact, ri-
Pragmatic language ➔ anxiety 0.49 0.13 < 0.001
gidity was associated with more relationship satisfaction via lower
Rigidity ➔ anxiety 0.28 0.10 0.01
Aloofness ➔ avoidance 0.30 0.11 0.01 avoidance. This finding is surprising given that higher rigidity is asso-
Pragmatic language ➔ avoidance 0.28 0.15 0.06 ciated with lower relationship satisfaction (Pruitt et al., 2018). Given
Rigidity ➔ avoidance −0.24 0.10 0.02 the paucity of research examining the relationship between rigidity and
Anxiety ➔ relationship satisfaction −3.15 1.06 < 0.01 romantic relationship outcomes, further research is needed to better
Avoidance ➔ relationship satisfaction −5.32 1.11 < 0.001
Aloofness ➔ relationship satisfaction −0.54 1.32 0.68
understand and confirm this novel finding.
Pragmatic language ➔ relationship satisfaction 4.01 1.62 0.01 Importantly, future research should implement longitudinal
Rigidity ➔ relationship satisfaction −1.43 1.43 0.32 methods in order to examine the impact of BAP on the development of
Covariance parameters adult attachment over an extended time period. In addition, partici-
Anxiety with avoidance 0.50 0.07 < 0.001
pants did not report the length of their current romantic relationship,
Aloofness with pragmatic language 0.33 0.04 < 0.001
Aloofness with rigidity 0.27 0.04 < 0.001 thus we were unable to examine the possible impact of relationship
Pragmatic language with rigidity 0.17 0.03 < 0.001 length on outcome variables. Additionally, we included reports of re-
lationship satisfaction from only one individual within the couple pair
(and could not pair any potential partners in our anonymous survey).
because of their desire for relational affiliation (Brennan et al., 1998). Future research should examine BAP, attachment, and relationship sa-
Alternatively, avoidant individuals struggle establishing conversational tisfaction in couples to explore dyadic associations between these
rapport with relational partners (Schoenherr et al., 2019), withhold variables (e.g., Faso et al., 2016).
their thoughts and opinions because of this (Read et al., 2018) and
ultimately avoid face-to-face interactions altogether (Wardecker et al., 5. Conclusion
2016). This could be one explanation for why attachment avoidance
was found to explain the consequences of pragmatic language deficits, The present study underscores the implications of BAP on romantic
but attachment anxiety was not. relationship satisfaction through the mechanism of attachment in-
Our findings also revealed that rigidity was associated with lower security. This study adds to the literature on BAP by identifying asso-
relationship satisfaction via higher anxiety. This finding is consistent ciations between individual BAP traits (aloof personality, pragmatic
with research demonstrating that rigidity is associated with lower re- language deficits, and rigid personality), attachment anxiety and
lationship satisfaction (Pruitt et al., 2018). Moreover, rigid individuals avoidance, and relationship satisfaction. These associations are espe-
show increased levels of general anxiety (Ciarrochi et al., 2005), and cially important to understand emerging adults who may be seeking to

4
J.H. Beffel, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

build meaningful and lasting romantic relationships. Couple-based in- Fraley, R. C., Vicary, A. M., Brumbaugh, C. C., & Roisman, G. I. (2011). Patterns of sta-
terventions and healthy relationship education programs may be in- bility in adult attachment: An empirical test of two models of continuity and change.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(5), 974–992. https://doi.org/10.
formed by these findings which support the important role of individual 1037/a0024150.
differences in relationship satisfaction and well-being. Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Owen, M. T., & Holland, A. S. (2013).
Interpersonal and genetic origins of adult attachment styles: a longitudinal study
from infancy to early adulthood. Journal of personality and social psychology, 104,
CRediT authorship contribution statement 817–838. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031435.
Funk, J. L., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing
Jenna H. Beffel: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the couples satisfaction
index. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(4), 572–583. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-
Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing. Kyla M. Cary: 3200.21.4.572.
Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing - original Galinha, I. C., Oishi, S., Pereira, C. R., Wirtz, D., & Esteves, F. (2014). Adult attachement,
draft, Writing - review & editing. Amy K. Nuttall: Conceptualization, love styles, relationship experiences and subjective well-being: Cross-cultural and
gender comparisons between Americans, Porguguese, and Mozambicans. Social
Methodology, Writing - review & editing, Investigation, Resources,
Indicators Research, 119(2), 823–852.
Project administration, Supervision. William J. Chopik: Writing - re- Gallitto, E., & Leth-Steensen, C. (2015). Autistic traits and adult attachment styles.
view & editing, Investigation, Resources, Project administration, Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 63–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
Supervision. Megan K. Maas: Writing - review & editing, Supervision. 2015.01.032.
Gómez-López, M., Viejo, C., & Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2019, July 7). Well-being and romantic
relationships: A systematic review in adolescence and emerging adulthood.
References International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. https://doi.org/10.
3390/ijerph16132415 MDPI AG.
Guarnieri, S., Smorti, M., & Tani, F. (2015). Attachment relationships and life satisfaction
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis- during emerging adulthood. Social Indicators Research, 121(3), 833–847. https://doi.
orders (5th ed.). Arlintonhttps://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596. org/10.1007/s11205-014-0655-1.
Barbaro, N., Boutwell, B. B., Barnes, J. C., & Shackelford, T. K. (2017). Rethinking the Guerrero, L. K., & Burgood, J. K. (1996). Attachment styles and reactions to nonverbal
transmission gap: What behavioral genetics and evolutionary psychology mean for involvement change in romantic dyads: Patters on reciprocity and compensation.
attachment theory: A comment on Verhage et al. (2016). Psychological Bulletin, Human Communication Research, 22(3), 335–370.
143(1), 107–113. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000066. Hayes, A. F. (2018). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation:
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85(1),
autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning 4–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1352100.
autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Hayes, A. F., & Scharkow, M. (2013). The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of
Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471. the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: Does method really matter?
Barry, C. M. N., Madsen, S. D., Nelson, L. J., Carroll, J. S., & Badger, S. (2009). Friendship Psychological Science, 24(10), 1918–1927. https://doi.org/10.1177/
and romantic relationship qualities in emerging adulthood: Differential associations 0956797613480187.
with identity development and achieved adulthood criteria. Journal of Adult Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.
Development, 16(4), 209–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-009-9067-x. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511–524.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult Hurley, R. S. E., Losh, M., Parlier, M., Reznick, J. S., & Piven, J. (2007). The broad autism
attachment: An integrative review. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.). phenotype questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9),
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guliford Press. 1679–1690. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0299-3.
Briley, D. A., Livengood, J., Derringer, J., Tucker-Drob, E. M., Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. Jobe, L. E., & Williams White, S. (2007). Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader
W. (2019). Interpreting behavior genetic models: Seven developmental processes to autism phenotype in college students. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(8),
understand. Behavior Genetics, 49(2), 196–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh. 1479–1489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.021.
2017.03.040. Jones, J. T., & Cunningham, J. D. (1996). Attachment styles and other predictors of re-
Butzer, B., & Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and relationship lationship satisfaction in dating couples. Personal Relationships, 3(4), 387–399.
satisfaction: A study of married couples. Personal Relationships, 15(1), 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1996.tb00123.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00189.x. Lamport, D., & Turner, L. A. (2014). Romantic attachment, empathy, and the broader
Chen, F. S., & Johnson, S. C. (2012). An oxytocin receptor gene variant predicts attach- autism phenotype among college students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 175(3),
ment anxiety in females and autism-spectrum traits in males. Social Psychological and 202–213. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2013.856838.
Personality Science, 3(1), 93–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611410325. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Attachment theory and emotions in close re-
Ciarrochi, J., Said, T., & Deane, F. P. (2005). When simplifying life is not so bad: The link lationships: Exploring the attachment-related dynamics of emotional reactions to
between rigidity, stressful life events, and mental health in an undergraduate popu- relational events. Personal Relationships, 12(2), 149–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
lation. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 33(2), 185–197. https://doi.org/ 1350-4126.2005.00108.x.
10.1080/03069880500132540. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2018). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, Nielsen, S. K. K., Lønfeldt, N., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Hageman, I., Vangkilde, S., &
emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 810–832. Daniel, S. I. F. (2017). Adult attachment style and anxiety – The mediating role of
Collins, A., & van Dulmen, M. (2007). Friendships and romance in emerging adulthood: emotion regulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 21, 253–259. https://doi.org/10.
Assessing distinctiveness in close relationships. Emerging adults in America: Coming of 1016/j.jad.2017.04.047.
age in the 21st century (pp. 219–234). American Psychological Association. https:// Parr, J. R., De Jonge, M. V., Wallace, S., Pickles, A., Rutter, M. L., Le Couteur, A. S., &
doi.org/10.1037/11381-009. Bailey, A. J. (2015). New interview and observation measures of the broader autism
Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin phenotype: Description of strategy and reliability findings for the interview measures.
study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530. https://doi.org/10.1001/ Autism Research, 8(5), 522–533. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1466.
archpsyc.60.5.524. Pollmann, M. M. H., Finkenauer, C., & Begeer, S. (2010). Mediators of the link between
Demir, M. (2008). Sweetheart, you really make me happy: Romantic relationship quality autistic traits and relationship satisfaction in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Autism
and personality as predictors of happiness among emerging adults. Journal of and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 470–478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-
Happiness Studies, 9(2), 257–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-007-9051-8. 0888-z.
Edelstein, R. S., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Avoidant attachment: Exploration of an oxy- Pruitt, M. M., Rhoden, M., & Ekas, N. V. (2018). Relationship between the broad autism
moron. In D. Mashek, & A. Aron (Eds.). Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 397– phenotype, social relationships and mental health for mothers of children with
412). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 22(2), 171–180. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Falk, C. F. (2018). Are robust standard errors the best approach for interval estimation 1362361316669621.
with nonnormal data in structural equation modeling? Structural Equation Modeling, Read, D. L., Clark, G. I., Rock, A. J., & Coventry, W. L. (2018). Adult attachment and
25(2), 244–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1367254. social anxiety: The mediating role of emotion regulation strategies. PLoS One, 13(12),
Faso, D. J., Corretti, C. A., Ackerman, R. A., & Sasson, N. J. (2016). The broad autism 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207514.
phenotype predicts relationship outcomes in newly formed college roommates. Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., & Baron-Cohen, S.
Autism, 20(4), 412–424. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315585733. (2016). Subgrouping siblings of people with autism: Identifying the broader autism
Feeney, B. C., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Reconstructive memory related to adolescent-parent phenotype. Autism Research, 9(6), 658–665. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1544.
conflict interactions: The influence of attachment-related representations on im- Saffrey, C., & Ehrenberg, M. (2007). When thinking hurts: Attachment, rumination, and
mediate perceptions and changes in perceptions over time. Journal of Personality and post-relationship adjustment. Personal Relationships, 14, 351–368. https://doi.org/10.
Social Psychology, 85(5), 945–955. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.945. 1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00160.x.
Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and Sasson, N. J., Lam, K. S. L., Parlier, M., Daniels, J. L., & Piven, J. (2013). Autism and the
dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology broad autism phenotype: Familial patterns and intergenerational transmission.
Review, 6(2), 123–151. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of 1955-5-11.
self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Worrack, S., Strauss, B. M., Rubel, J. A., Schwartz, B., &
78(2), 350–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350. Altmann, U. (2019). Quantification of nonverbal synchrony using linear time series

5
J.H. Beffel, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110409

analysis methods: Lack of convergent validity and evidence for facets of synchrony. Behavioral Assessment, 33(4), 459–469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-011-9259-0.
Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 361–383. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018- Wardecker, B. M., Chopik, W. J., Boyer, M. P., & Edelstein, R. S. (2016). Individual dif-
1139-z. ferences in attachment are associated with usage and perceived intimacy of different
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Winterheld, H. A. (2010). Attachment working models communication media. Computers in Human Behavior, 59, 18–27. https://doi.org/10.
twist memories of relationship events. Psychological Science, 21(2), 252–259. https:// 1016/j.chb.2016.01.029.
doi.org/10.1177/0956797609357175. Weimer, B. L., Kerns, K. A., & Oldenburg, C. M. (2004). Adolescents’ interactions with a
Wainer, A. L., Ingersoll, B. R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2011). The structure and nature of the best friend: Associations with attachment style. Journal of Experimental Child
broader autism phenotype in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Psychology, 88(1), 102–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.003.

You might also like