You are on page 1of 5

John B.

Watson's Advice on Child Real'ing

Some Historical Context

Kathryn M. Bigelow and Edward K. Morris


University ofKansas
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

John D. Wal.son (1878·/9$8) wrole /or lite popular press on a


society (Watson. t 927d). the institution of marriage (Watson,
number of lapics during the J910s, often in Ihe oreo of dUd
1929b), and of course child rearing .
rearing. His facu about child dewlopment were not disputed,
Watson's first article on child rearing appeared in 1922
but his advice WO,J often criticized. 1'hi.J paper examines the
(Watson, 1922), IUs las' one in 1929 (Watson, 1929a). In
validity of the crtlleum by reviewing what Watson advised in
between. he published a series of six articles in McCall's
Ihe context of his day. We found Ihol. ai/hough most of his
(Watson. 19270, 1927b, 1927c, 1928a, 1928b, 1928c) tha,­
advice was consultn, wi/It child rearing practices then and
with the assistance of his wife, Rosalie Rayner Watson - was
now (e.g about child misbehavior. positive supports. p�n­
.•

lion), some of it was nol (e.g about love and 01l«I;on). al­
expanded and published as their book, Psychological Care �/
Ihe In/ani and Child (Watson &. Watson, 1928; on Watson s
.•

though this loiter advice was not unique to his day. We then
career, see Buckley, 1989; Cohen, 1979; Morris &. Todd,
comment on sewral sources a/ variance In this advice.
1999; Todd 8< Morris, 1994).
tn the 19205. Americans were looking to the new scie�ce
Durin8 the 19201, John B. Watson (1818-1958), the founder of
of psychology and 10 studies in child development for adVice
behaviorism. wrote (or both the academic and the popular
about child rearing (Child Study Association, 1926; see
pren, and ,ave public lectures on a variety of topi�s. AJn?n,
Frank. 1962; Lomax, Kagan, 8< Rosenkrantz. 1978; Senn.
the topics be addressed in the popular press was child re�"
1975). Parenthood was increasingly viewed as a vocation that
where he provided advice about raising happy, well-adjusted
sbould be based on ftnn knowledge. Rather than relying on
children. equipped to function in modem society. Although his
popular writings and lectures were generalIy acclaimed or � �
practices that came to them "��l1y," par�n began. pl �cing
. . more emphasis on the apphcatlon of sClenllfic pnoclples.
!
making the new science of behavior acce,:,�b e to the publ .I�,
They looked to science for innovative child-rearing methods,
his child-rearing advice has often been cnuclUd. 1ne cnU­
different from the previous generation's old-fashioned tech­
dsm, though. is problematic on two accounts. Fint. it is often
niques (Stendler, 1950). Wltson provided some of lhis advice
based on selected aspects of his advice. Second, it is usually
for producing "children who would be able to cope with the
predicated on the nont'd of • more modem era, not those of
realities of modem life" (Watson & Wltson, 1928, p. 10).
Watson', day. In order to uscn the validity of this criticism,
His primary message was that. within normal biological lim­
we undertook a review of what WalSon actually advised, the
its, children were made. not bom, and that the responsibility
criticisms of his advice at the time, and bow his advice com­
for raising happy children fell squarely on their parents'
pared and contrasted with other advice of the day. We specu­
sboulden. He was concerned with teaching parents how to
late, IS well, on some sources of the discrepancies in the ad­
produce a child '''who lul.lUy enten . manhood so bul,:"arked
vice found in that era.
with stable work and emotional habits that no advenlty can
quite overwhelm him" (Watson & Watson, 1928, p. 10).
Historical Context
Watson's Child ReQring Advice
Watson left academic psychology in 1920 and began a second
career as an advertising executive. in which he was highly suc­
In Psychological CQr� 0/ th� In/ant and Child, Watso � and
cessful. However, he continued to make important contribu­
Watson (1928) described child rearing as a science, a sCience
tions to psychology u a science and a discipline, and he began
based in part on Watson's research on infant development .t
writing for the popular press. Indeed, as a contributor to
Johns Hopkins University (e.g., Watson, 1925a. 1925b,
Harper's. McCall's, and Cosmopolilan magazines, he became
1925c: Watson & Rayner, 1920); albeit not a science yet fully
America's first "pop" psychologist. Some of these articles ad­
developed. Nonetheless. on the basis of this science, Watson
dressed psychology proper, while others addr �ssed social is­
offered child rearing advice on several topics, among them
sues of the day. for instance, the role of women m then-modem
negative emotional reactions, misbehavior, love and affec­
tion, and daily routines.

Vo1.1, Fall 2001. Bahavloral Cavalopmant Bullatln


26
Negative emotional reactions. Watson's advice about der desirable habits, especially at bedtime, mealtime, and
negative emotional reactions was that they be kept to a mini­ playtime (e.g., a half hour of quiet bedlime play). The ir.lpor­
mwn by maintaining a positive, non-threatening home environ­ lance of daily exercise and social contacts was also ad­
ment. As for children's fears, parents should, for example, dressed. In general, Watson and Watson (1921) noced that
protect their children from sudden, loud sounds and objects that daily routines provided children with the "nice habits, con­
could hun them, but at the same time, they should not shelter ventions, and customs" demanded by "polite society' (p,
children from nonnal noises. More proactively, he recom­ 113).
mended establishing some common sense negative reactions to
undesirable activity, such as reaching toward forbidden objects, Criticisms of Wauon's Advice
but the nwnber of these reactions was to be kept to a minimwn.
As for temper tantrums, Watson also emphasized prevention, Although Watson's child.rearing advice was, in general, well
advising parents to promote their children's independence at received, his specific advice about love and affection often
completing tasks that might otherwise cause tantrums, for in­ met with strong criticism, both then and now (Lomax tt al..
stance, tasks related to bathing and dressing. Even then, Watson 1978, p. 131). In his own time, for instance, the House� ins'
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

warned, children would still occasionally have tantrums, for no League was outraged. Its president, Mrs. Julian Heath, said
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

parent can control all the factors that produce them. that "Watson must be a very unhappy man to offer such
ideas" (Cohen, 1979, p. 212). A woman attending one of
Misbehavior. On the topic of misbehavior, Watson again Watson's public lectures said she was happy she had not
emphasized prevention. In panicular, he advised parents to keep heard of Watson's advice before raising her own children
their children busy throughout the day, engaged in appropriate because she was then able to enjoy them (Cohen, 19'19, p.
activities. Only when prevention was ineffective should pun­ 210), Pediatricians blamed Watson for an apparent innease
ishment be used, But even then, punishment should be no more in infant sleeping problems (Scnn, 1975). Newspaper etlitori·
aversive than smanly rapping the child's fingers or hands with a als referred to him as "subnormal" and "subhwnan" (C!Jhen,
pencil. Punishment was not generally advised. 1979, p. 209), One colwnnist wrote that "the theorist sltOuld
be 'backed against the wall and let him have it full force from
Love and affection, In discussing love and affection, the shoulder in plain speech that he is plainly Wflrng'"
Watson made two points. First, love was not a broadly general­ (quOied in Cohen, 1979, p. 212). When asked 10 assess Wat­
ized instinctive pattern of behavior, In infants, it was a reaction son's innuence on the study of children, the noted develop­
that could only be brought about by one stimulus - strOking the mental psychologist, William Kessen, stated that "his impact
skin; later in development, associated stimuli would come to on the field has been almost completely deleterious. His
elicit this reaction. Second, love was a reaction that, if evoked attitude toward children, his attitude toward parents in the
too much or too often, would lead to overcoddling, which psychological care of the child is, it seems to me, pathologi­
would later be manifest in adults who needed coddling. In addi­ cal" (Senn, 1975, p. 29).
tion, the excessive emotional attachment that overcoddling In his critique of the mechanization of Western culture,
might engender could interfere with later marital adjustment To Dell (1930) argued that Watson's child-rearing adv:ce was
guard against these outcomes, Watson and Watson (1928) ad­ factually in error, citing studies demonstrating that withhold·
vised the following: ing parental affection can result in children's failure to grow
and thrive. Moreover, according to Dell, Watson's advice
Let your behavior always be objective and kindly firm. Never was simply beside the point. Pychologically healthy and
hug and kiss lhem. never let them sit in your lap. If you mw\, mature mothers did not engage in the neurotic, silly "love-­
kiss them once on lhe forehead when they say good night. Shake tricks" Watson described. Mothers who were emotionally and
hands with them in the moming. Give them a pat on the head if sexually fulfilled did not misplace their affections on their
they have made an extraordinarily good job of a difficult cask. children, They could love their children as children, not "as a
(pp.81-82) substitute lover," without at all hanning them. Conversely,
Dell said, neurotic mothers who engaged in '"pseud�
Also, to insure that children would be independent of any maternal" behavior could follow Watson's advice, yet still
one adult's love and affection, parents should bring different victimiu their children. According to Dell, Watson failed to
nurses into their homes on a weekly basis, Comparable positive see the possibility of these differential outcomes.
effects on children'S independence might also be achieved by Dell's (1930) criticisms notwithstanding, he generally
puning them in a fenced yard for large parts of the day. If par. praised Watson and Watson's (1928) book, noting that ..there
ents wished to watch over their children on these laner occa­ are so many fine things in [itJ that one regrets the necessity of
sions, they should do so inconspicuously. Children should be exposing its follies" (p. 137). More than six decades later,
left to cope with and solve their own problems. Salzinger (1994) offered a similar assessment. He noted that
although the Watsons' book contained some "poor advice
Daily routines. Watson especially emphasized the im­ and advice beyond data" (p. 154), it also made recommenda­
ponance of establishing a daily routine. He acknowledged that tions that form the foundation of many present child·rearing
no hard and fast rules were applicable to every family, but he practices (e,g., the role of daily routines).
did advise that a formal routine of daily activities could engen-

Vol. 1, Fa1i2001. Behavioral Development Bulielin


27
Compilrlsons lInd ContrllJtJ as one of the one hundred books published before 1900 that
most "influenced the life and culture of the American people"
Although these criticisms of Watson's advice concerning love (Park '" Mason, 1957; ciled in Lomax el aI., 1978, p. 148).
and affection are apt by North American standards, the question Other authorities commented on both the value and the
remains about whether his advice was or was not consistent with danger of parental love and affection. For instance, according
the standards of his day. Our review of the child-rearing litera­ �o Blatz and Bott (1929), afef ctionate behavior can promote
ture of the 1920s suggests that it was the nonn of the day, ex­ I�rtant emotional bonds, but at the same time make chil­
cept on one point, but even here opinion was divided. dren overly dependent on their parents and engender the
premature development of sexual impUlses. Thorn (1927)
Comparisons. As for Watson', advice about establishing likewise noted that, although parents should create an "at.
routines for sleeping, eating, playing, sociaHzing, and exercis­ mosphere of affection, kindly consideration. and fair play" (p.
ing, this was also broadly recommended by most experts at the 38), oversolicitous parents may arouse unreasonable fears
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

time (e.g., Jessup, 1923; Kerley, 1923). Watson's advice about and anxieties. Indeed, ov. :solicitousness "often produces the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

punishment was also generally agreed upon - punishment was selfish. self-centered, clinging vine type of child" (p. 34).
to be discouraged (i.e., Weill, 1930, cited in Lomax et aJ., 1978,
p. 134). For example, in a widely-read monthly colunm for Contrasts. In contrast to this advice regarding the delete­
Good Housekeeping. Kenyon (1926) described strategies for rious effects of love and affection, our review of the child­
raising well-disciplined children without using corporal pun­ rearing literature of the 19205 suggests thaI it was not the
ishment. Other experts concurred: Although a "sharp smack on norm. At the very least. the advice was more balanced.
the hands" might serve a useful purpose during the preschool Goodspeed and Johnson (1929), for instance, advised that it
was unwise to:
yells, "corporal punishment could be entirely eliminated as a
diSCiplinary measure without great loss" (Thorn, 1927, p. 127).
In 1 cnera), parents were advised to minimize punishment in ...allow a child to display excessive afe
f ction for anyone mem­

ordcr to prevent children from becoming "permanently sulky or ber of the family, nor should he be allowed to look toward any
hostile," as well as from becoming less likely to tell the truth f ction. A whole­
one of the family for an excessive show of afe

(Grove, '" Grove" 1924, p. 95). some display of affection based upon a true spiril of kindliness
With respec:t to Watson's advice about love and affection. and courtesy will establish the proper ',ive and take' in family
some child-rearina guides of his day supported his recommen­ relations. (p. 206)

dations, but not always for the same reuons. Infant Can was
one such public.,jon (We,� 1914). Prlnled in 1914 by Ihe U.S. In general, though, most of the advice slmply noted that
Department of labor Children's Bureau, about 3 million copies affection between parents and children was highly appropri­
had been disttibuted nationally by 1925. This child-rearing ate. Fenton's (1925) Practical Psychology of Babyhood, for
guide elJ1)hasized the ;q,ortance of initiating training, disci­ example, recommended that parents should freely accept the
pline, and routines as early as possible. Parents were also spontaneous affections of their infants. Also, given that ba­
W&nled against Jdssing babies on the mouth. but here for medi­ bies learn to be affectionate through imitation, she encour­
cal, not for psychological, reuons, because in the early pan of aged parents to behave affectionately toward them Likewise.
the 20· centwy, an infant's health was often at risk (Lomax et Groves and Groves (1924) advised parents that '1"he little
ai., 1978, p. 131). In order Co prevent the spread of infections, baby ClMOt be loved too much. He needs love and thrives on
parenti were advised not only to avoid Jdssing their infants, but it as a plant thrives on sunlight" (p. 12). Older children, age
also 001 to bold or pet them beyond what was needed to assure six to 10, also need affection: "He craves affection and stabil­
their physical care and nutrition. Similar advice was offered in ity. No amount of material luxury can make up to him for
Holl', (1894/1932) no CDr< and Fuding a/ Children (IS ciled lack of affection. Affection must not be spasmodic. A moder­
In Lomax et ai., 1978), which offered I co�ndium of infor­ ate love that flows gently on, uninterrupted by outbWlts of
mation on infant and child physical growth. nuttition. and emotion. is surely best for the child." (p. 142).
hellth. and made reconvnendations concerning their physical
Clle.
Discrepancies In the Child Rearing Advice
The medjcal advice against Jdssing and petting infants nol­
withstandina, these and other source. also advised against some In sununaty, Watson', advice about love and affection was
demonstrations of love and affection for reasons closer to Wat­ consistent with that offered by some "experts" of his day, but
was contradicted by most others. The basis of this discrep­
son's. Infanl Care (1914). for instance, warned parents not to
play with infants, so as to avoid upsetting their regular habits or ancy is difficult to determine. however, in order to encourage
inducing a "nervous disturbance" (ciled in Lomax et aI., 1978, further research. we speculate briefly about some possible
p. 130). Holl (1894/1932) likewise recommended chal parenlS sources.
I. 10e child-rearing advice differed depending on its rea­
not play with infants under six months of age, and play with
older infants but minimally, for in addilion 10 making them sons. Some "experts" recommended that parents offer
love and afe f ction for psychological reasons, both mental
sleep poorly. suffer from indigestion. and cease glining weight,
and emotional, while others recommended against such
it could make them nervous and irritable. Holt's text was widely
innuential. In 1946, the Grolier Club - a book club - selected it

Vol. I, Fall 2001. aehavloral Devel opmont Bullolln


8
displays - especially, kissing - but only for medical reasons In the end, Watson (1936) regretted some of the advice he
(Lomax et aI., 1978). offered in Psychological Care of Infant and Child. It was
2. Child-rearing ad...ice that downplayed coddling and affec­ hasty and insufficiently infonned. Had he been able to con-­
tion may ha...e reflected a generational change - a shift duct further basic and applied research, his advice might have
from a romantic sentimentality toward children to the new been different. Perhaps that is the lesson to be drawn from
generation's tum toward science and technology (Stendler, this episode in the history of developmental psycholog) We
1950). should never cease analyzing behavior, Where...er beha1 ioral
3. Sociological change was also occurring, in particular, in development is historically dependent, not an ineVitable
women's desire for more independence (Frank, 1962; Har­ outcome, child rearing advice should be continuously and
ris, 1984). In a longitudinal analysis of women's maga­ empirically infonned.
zines, for instance, Stendler (1950) found an increase, in
1920, in the proportion of the articles on child rearing that REFERENCES
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ad...ocated a tinner attitude toward infant discipline (e.g.,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tight scheduling, letting infants "cry it out"). This was, per­ Blatz, W. E" & Botl, H. (1929), Pa""u aNi 1M preschool child. New
haps, a means for bringing more predictability and control York: Morrow
into the Ii...es of women. Buckley, K, W, (1989). Mechanical man: Joh" Broodw Watson and lite
4. Child rearing ad...ice may also ...ary with gender of those I¥g",II;IIRI 0/ I¥ hav.or;IM, New York: Guilford,
who pro...ide it. Watson's ad...ice regarding love and affec­ Child Siudy Association of America. In<:. (1926). Concerning pa"nu: A
tion was written by a "man's man," while the countervail­ sympoSIUM on preH"t day porertlhood New York: New Republic.
ing ad...ice was often written or co-authored by women Cohen, D. (1979). j, B. Wal.Jo,,: The /olllllkr 0/ behaviorism. London:
(e.g., Fenton, 1925; Goodspeed & Johnson, 1929; Groves Routlcdac & Kcpn Paul.
& Groves, 1924). Dell, F, (19)0). Low I" tM machine age. New Yon: FIltTII & Rinchan.
5. Although Watson's ad...ice was based, in part, on his re­ Fenlon, J. C. (1925). PrQClical psychology o/ babyltood. Cambridae, MA:
search in child de...elopmenl, personal experience may also Riverside Press.
ha...e played a role. His emolional attachment to his father Frank, L K. (1962), The bcaiMinp of child development and flmily life
was de...astatingly broken when his father deserted Ihe fam­ educalion in the twentieth century. Me"iII·Palnwr Qwukrly, 8. 207·
ily without warning (Morris & Todd, 1999). Watson's con­ 221.
ditioned negative reactions to this event may have influ­ Goodspeed, 1-1, C., .t Johnson, E. (1929). Care andIrai"illg of cll,ldmt.
enced his advice regarding love and affection. Philadelphia, PA: Uppincott.
Groves, E. R" & Groves, O. H. (192-4). WhoIeUH1N childhood. Boscon:
CONCLUSION �Iou&.hlon Mimin,
Harris, 8, (1984), "Give me a dozen healthy Infants... ": John B. Wauon',
In summary, the child-rearing advice of the 1920s cannot be popular advice on childrearina. women, and the family, In M, Lewin
characterized as agreeing on any one set of methods for raising (Ed.),III tlte shadow o/tIN /XU': P,f)'ChoIolfY portrayllM lUll (PP,
children. The advice often varied '\cross and within those who 126-154), New York: Colombi. University Press.
pro...ided it, and across particular domains of development. In Holt, L E. (1929), 1M caIY aNi/ceding 0/ chi/dun: A calechis m / ar 1M
this context, Walson's advice on misbehavior and daily routines ... 0/ molIN" and nllr_l, New Yorlt: Appleton.
was consislent with that of his day, while his advice about Jove Jessup, M, D. (192), May), H.ppy babies and h.ppy machen. Good Hou.u·
and affection was at variance, albeit not unique. Four conclu­ iuepi,," 76, 85, 222.22), 22-4·225,
sions follow from this and the preceding analyses. Kenyon, lit (1926, Nov,). Discipline, Good Howeke,pin.. 8J, 98,
First, Watson's child-rearing advice varied from domain to Kerley, C. G, (1923, April), Habits 10 make and break. McCall's, jO, -41.
domain and, by today's standards, was infonned in some do­ Lomax. E, M. R., K.gan, J., & Rosenkrantz, B,G, (1971), Science and
mains, but not in others. Although he had adopted the assump­ paller,., a/ ch"dcare. San Francisco: Freeman,
tion Ihat, within nonnal biological limits, children were made, Morris, E. K., & Todd, J.T, (1999). W'lSOnian beh ....iorism, In W. T.
not born, he had no o...erarching theory about the outcome of O'Oonohue .t R. Kilchener (Ed5.), Handbook 0/ behavuN'bm (pp,
development per se. Second, again by today's standards, Wat­ 15�). New York: Academic Press,
son was not alone in being uninfonned about love and affection. Plitt, E, A., & Mason, II. H. (1957), Luther Emmetl 1-1011. In B. S, Veeder
Advice such as his was prescribed by behaviorists and nonbe­ (Ed.), Pedial,ic pro/ii,s (pp, 3)-60). 51, Louis, MO: Mosby,
haviorists alike, and Ihus was nol unique to his behaviorism. Salzinacr, K. (1994), On Watson. In J. T. Todd & E. K, Morris (Eds,).
Third, although Watson's advice is today seen as insensitive to Moder" perspectlws on JoII" B. WaISO" and claulcal behaviorism
Ihe putative inborn social-emotional needs of infants and chil­ (pp, 151·158). Westport, cr: Greenwood,
dren, this was not intenlional on his part. He simply did not Senn, M, J. E. (1975), Insi&.hu on the child dcvc:lopment movement in lhe
presume any such needs in Ihe tirst place. Fourth, much of United States. Monographs O/IM $Deletylor R,search j" Clllld Dc·
Watson's advice was prescient of current best practices in child wlopmell', 40 (1-4, Serial No, 161),
rearing, especially those concerning punishment, positive envi­ Stendler, C, (1950), Sixty years of child lrainina practice, JOIU'NJI 0/ Pedi·
ronmental supports, and prevention Ihrough Ihe teaching of alrlcl, J6, 122·114.
alternati ...e repertoires, Thom. D.. A. (1927). £wryday prablewu O/IM twryday child. New YOf1t:
ApP'eton.

Vol. 1, Fall 2001. Behavioral Development Bulletin


29
Todd. 1. T., II: Morris, E. K. CEdi.). (I�). Mode", J¥rJINctiw$ 011 Jolut B. WIlSOn, J. B. (1921c, Milch). NiJ,ht and dlytime c:are o( the child.
WIIboft _ ckwkGI klttwIorlMff. Wa.pon. CT: Om:nwood McCQ/l'$. JJ. ll. 66.
W..,.., J. B. (1922). Can scicntc determine )'OUt beby'$ c.-eer befOre it CMI WIlSOn, J. B. (1929.). Should a child have 1I'IOft;!han one mothcr'1 UHrf)',
wk? New Yon Sunday MII6aziM,January 3 , p.-4. June 29, 31·U
WIUOn, 1. B.(1925a). Experimmtal studies on lbe po�h o(lbe emo4tons.. W8lSOn, J. B. (I929b). Will men many fifty)'Cll1i from I'IOYII COJMOpOIj­
P�dtI606ical s,m'nIIf]i. 11, 321-).41. III" 11(Junc,29),I0-4,I06.
W.uon. J. B. (l925b). Recalt experiments on how we k>se and chan. OI,U Watson. J. B. (1936). John Broadus Watson. In C. Murchison (Ell), A
emotional equipment Pedogo61C111 ScmlNU)'. 11. ).49-371. hb/Dry oj/U)ocho/ogy IfIlINlobiOJraphy(Voi. 3. pp. 271 -211). WOf­
W.uon. J. B. (192Sc). What the nurwry tw to Rly about instinc.u. PNo60,j. chester. MA: 0"'" UnivCl$ity Press.
col s,mitflllY. 11. 293·327. Watson, J. B &. Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned cmocional reactions.
••

WIllSOn, J. B. (1927, Oct). Arc you aivinl your child a chMoc -lbe betwv­ JOtmtlJIoj&pmfMlIlIIl Psych%U. J, 1-1-4.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

iorisc spcU.s. McCall'$. J4. 6-4, 7-4. Wat.5On. J. B &. Wauon, R.R. (1921).P$)"Ch%giCIII CIIn oj IN In/tIrII
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.•

WIlSOn, J. B. (1927b, Sept.). Can PSYCholocY hdp me rear my child? IIndC/llfd. New York: Nonon.
McCall'$. U, 4-4, 72. Weill. B. C. (1930). Arc)OO tBininl your baby to be happy? lIuon INJU'
W�'.son, J. B. (l927c,Nov.). Oildml's (cars and how they g,row. McCIIU'$, nol,ll chIld INJno,ffMltl. Washinl'on,OC: U.S. Govcrmcnt Print­
H. 6',7•. in,Office.
W.uon, J. B. (l927d). The weakncu o(womm. 1M Nillion. 11J (JUoIy 6). 9- West. Mrs. M. (191-4). In/liN CIIn. Clln ojchlldnlt. Scnes No.2 . BUlQU
10. Publi�km No. I. Wl$hinl'on. OC: U.S. Govemment Printin, 0(·
WIlSOn, J. B.(192'" Feb.). R.&inl youth. McCaU'$, $$, $$. (ju.
WIItSof\, J. B. (l921b, Jan.). A IJ)Od child just a linie spoiled. McClIIl'$. JJ,
50.66.

Authors' Note
We thank Hayne W, Reese for his excellent and detailed
comments on an earlier version of this manuscript Cor·
respondence should be sent to the firsl author, Dcpan·
menl of Human Development and Family Life, Dole
Human Developmenl Cenler. Univmil)l of Kansas.
1000 Sunnyside Avenue, Lawrence, KS 6604S·21J).
E·mail: kmblgtlow@juno.com.

Vol. 1, Fall 2001. Behavioral Development BulieUn


30

You might also like