You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/330101563

Social-Emotional Skill Assessment in Children and Adolescents: Advances


and Challenges in Personality, Clinical, and Educational Contexts

Article in Psychological Assessment · March 2019


DOI: 10.1037/pas0000591

CITATIONS READS

133 12,112

7 authors, including:

Loes Abrahams Ricardo Primi


Tilburg University Universidade São Francisco
18 PUBLICATIONS 214 CITATIONS 195 PUBLICATIONS 3,039 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Daniel Araujo Santos Patrick C. Kyllonen


Universidade Nove de Julho Educational Testing Service
79 PUBLICATIONS 1,927 CITATIONS 118 PUBLICATIONS 7,641 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Loes Abrahams on 28 March 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Assessment
Social-Emotional Skill Assessment in Children and
Adolescents: Advances and Challenges in Personality,
Clinical, and Educational Contexts
Loes Abrahams, Gina Pancorbo, Ricardo Primi, Daniel Santos, Patrick Kyllonen, Oliver P. John, and
Filip De Fruyt
Online First Publication, March 14, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000591

CITATION
Abrahams, L., Pancorbo, G., Primi, R., Santos, D., Kyllonen, P., John, O. P., & De Fruyt, F. (2019,
March 14). Social-Emotional Skill Assessment in Children and Adolescents: Advances and
Challenges in Personality, Clinical, and Educational Contexts. Psychological Assessment. Advance
online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000591
Psychological Assessment
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000
1040-3590/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000591

Social-Emotional Skill Assessment in Children and Adolescents: Advances


and Challenges in Personality, Clinical, and Educational Contexts

Loes Abrahams and Gina Pancorbo Ricardo Primi


Ghent University and Institute Ayrton Senna, São Paulo, Brazil University of San Francisco and Institute Ayrton Senna, São
Paulo, Brazil

Daniel Santos Patrick Kyllonen


University of São Paulo and Institute Ayrton Senna, São Paulo, Educational Testing Service, Princeton, New Jersey
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Brazil
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Oliver P. John Filip De Fruyt


University of California, Berkeley, and Institute Ayrton Senna, Ghent University and Institute Ayrton Senna, São Paulo, Brazil
São Paulo, Brazil

The development and promotion of social-emotional skills in childhood and adolescence contributes to
subsequent well-being and positive life outcomes. However, the assessment of these skills is associated
with conceptual and methodological challenges. This review discusses how social-emotional skill
measurement in youth could be improved in terms of skills’ conceptualization and classification, and in
terms of assessment techniques and methodologies. The first part of the review discusses various
conceptualizations of social-emotional skills, demonstrates their overlap with related constructs such as
emotional intelligence and the Big Five personality dimensions, and proposes an integrative set of
social-emotional skill domains that has been developed recently. Next, methodological approaches that
are innovative and may improve social-emotional assessments are presented, illustrated by concrete
examples. We discuss how these innovations could advance social-emotional assessments, and demon-
strate links to similar issues in related fields. We conclude the review by providing several concrete
assessment recommendations that follow from this discussion.

Public Significance Statement


This review discusses how the conceptualization and assessment of social-emotional skills could be
advanced to improve child and youth well-being. After presenting various social-emotional frame-
works and reviewing methodological approaches that may improve or innovate social-emotional
assessments, five concrete recommendations that may guide further research and practice are
provided.

Keywords: social-emotional skills, 21st century skills, educational assessment, subclinical assessment,
formative and summative assessment

Over the past decades, social-emotional skills have gained in- achievement, citizenship behavior, and employability, to physical
creasing attention given their relevance for predicting a broad and mental health and well-being (see Organisation for Economic
series of consequential outcomes, ranging from educational Cooperation and Development [OECD], 2015 for a review). The

nia, Berkeley, and eduLab21, Institute Ayrton Senna; Filip De Fruyt,


Loes Abrahams and Gina Pancorbo, Department of Developmental, Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Psychology, Ghent
Personality and Social Psychology, Ghent University, and eduLab21, In- University, and eduLab21, Institute Ayrton Senna.
stitute Ayrton Senna, São Paulo, Brazil; Ricardo Primi, Graduate Program Loes Abrahams and Gina Pancorbo equally contributed to this article
in Psychological Assessment, University of San Francisco, and eduLab21, and share first authorship.
Institute Ayrton Senna, São Paulo, Brazil; Daniel Santos, Economics Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Loes Abra-
Department, University of São Paulo, and eduLab21, Institute Ayrton hams or Gina Pancorbo, Department of Developmental, Personality and Social
Senna; Patrick Kyllonen, Educational Testing Service, Princeton, New Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Gent, Belgium.
Jersey; Oliver P. John, Department of Psychology, University of Califor- E-mail: loes.abrahams@ugent.be or gina.pancorbo@ugent.be

1
2 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

promotion of these skills in educational contexts is driven by the tal factors, manifested as consistent patterns of thoughts, feelings,
importance of enhancing adaptive functioning in children and and behaviors, developed through formal and informal learning
youth through the development of personal strengths that can serve experiences, and that influence different outcomes throughout the
as tools to foster positive outcomes and prevent negative ones individual’s life.
(Kern, Benson, Steinberg, & Steinberg, 2016; Taylor, Oberle, Many alternative terms have been proposed in addition to
Durlak, & Weissberg, 2017). Initial work made extensive use of “social-emotional skills,” such as “noncognitive skills,” “21st cen-
clinically derived measures such as the Child Behavior Check List tury skills,” “character skills,” “transferable skills,” or “soft skills”
(CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) or the Strengths and Difficulties Ques- (see also Duckworth & Yeager, 2015 for a review; Kyllonen,
tionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1994) to assess social-emotional func- 2012). Not surprisingly, this variety of terminologies has been
tioning and evaluate the effects of social-emotional skill interven- described as a problem in the field (Kyllonen, Lipnevich, Burrus,
tions in education (Halle & Darling-Churchill, 2016). More & Roberts, 2014), although as stated by Duckworth and Yeager
recently, the field has undergone a paradigm shift, away from the (2015), all terms refer to the same “conceptual space” (p. 239) and
initial emphasis on screening for vulnerabilities and a curative share similar attributes with respect to their independence from
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

approach, and toward more general preventive and promoting cognitive abilities, derived benefits for students, and dependency
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

actions instead. This shift has challenged the field of social- on situational factors.
emotional skill assessment that was chiefly directed to the descrip- In addition to the numerous overarching terms to label skills,
tion of deficits. Frequently used social-emotional skill assessment several authors have independently concluded that there is huge
tools such as the SDQ (Goodman, 1994), for example, include variability in the number and nature of the skills included in
more items to describe difficulties than to assess adaptive func- different models and frameworks (Kyllonen et al., 2014; National
tioning. Research Council (NRC), 2011, 2012; Primi, Santos, John, & De
An additional paradigm shift occurred in the field of psycho- Fruyt, 2016; Voogt & Roblin, 2012) and that “there is no consen-
diagnostics, namely a change in the conceptualization of problem sus on the number and nature of the constructs needed to represent
behavior where adaptive and maladaptive patterns of cognitive, social-emotional characteristics” (Primi, Santos, et al., 2016, p. 6).
behavioral, or emotional functioning were increasingly considered The OECD (2015), for example, outlined four groups of skills:
from a dimensional, rather than a categorical, perspective. Today, cognitive, intrapersonal, interpersonal, and technical, while the
differences between adaptive and maladaptive functioning are National Research Council (NRC, 2011) proposed only the three
considered to be more quantitative than qualitative and there is domains of cognitive, intrapersonal, and interpersonal skills. From
growing evidence that this perspective is also useful to look at the positive psychology approach, the Positive Youth Develop-
many types of problem behavior in childhood and adolescence (De ment (PYD; Bowers et al., 2010) perspective proposes the “5Cs”
Fruyt & De Clercq, 2014; De Fruyt, De Clercq, De Caluwé, & model defined by competence, confidence, character, caring, and
Verbeke, 2017). connection, whereas Seligman’s PERMA model (Seligman, 2011)
These two paradigm shifts brought the fields of normal and outlines five pillars: positive emotion, engagement, relationships,
abnormal assessment closer to each other, fostering more intense meaning, and accomplishment. Popular measures describing psy-
collaboration between clinical, assessment, and educational psy- chological adjustment in children and youth that are used as
chologists. As a response to these changed perspectives, multiple indicators of social-emotional skills like the SDQ (Goodman,
social-emotional skill taxonomies and measurement tools have 1994) include four subscales that focus on four types of difficulties
been proposed by different groups of researchers to further inves- (conduct problems, hyperactivity, emotional symptoms, and peer
tigate social-emotional skills and their development, introducing problems) and one subscale on strengths (prosocial behavior).
various conceptual and methodological challenges. The first goal
of the present article is to review taxonomies of social-emotional Overlap and Related Constructs
skills and propose an integrative and overarching conceptual
framework to describe them. The second and major goal is to Social-emotional skill frameworks not only differ in terms of
discuss measurement challenges and innovations to improve the labels and scope, but many of their skills also show overlap and
assessment of social-emotional skills. The review will close with share similarities in their underlying constructs (Geisinger, 2016;
suggestions to advance synergies in the emerging field of social- John & Mauskopf, 2015; Kyllonen, 2016; Primi, Santos, et al.,
emotional assessment and facilitate collaboration between clini- 2016; Voogt & Roblin, 2012). The National Research Council
cally and educationally oriented assessment psychologists.1 (NRC, 2012), for example, documented communalities between
social-emotional skills, abilities (see e.g., Occupational Network
or OⴱNET) and personality dimensions, and provided evidence for
Structuring the Social-Emotional Skills Domain the occurrence of the “jangle fallacy,” referring to the use of
different terminology to describe the same construct.
Defining the Social-Emotional Skill Space The lack of shared definitions and overlap among skills en-
closed in different frameworks have implications for their mea-
Social-emotional skills have been broadly and pragmatically
surement. As stated by the NRC (2011), a clear definition of the
defined as “the skills that are essential for navigating education
constructs behind the skills is a necessary condition for assess-
and the workplace in the current century” (Binkley et al., 2012,
ment. Ziegler and Brunner (2016) emphasized this idea by claim-
p. v). A more conceptual and operational definition of social-
emotional skills was recently provided by De Fruyt et al. (2015)
suggesting that social-emotional skills are individual characteris- 1
Our study did not go through any ethics committee review because no
tics that originate from biological predispositions and environmen- additional data was collected.
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 3

ing that a precise definition of the construct is crucial for formu- Finally, other researchers have used a combination of clinical
lating items that can accurately measure the construct. If not, two inventories to examine children’s social-emotional functioning
measures that intend to capture the same construct may have items (e.g., Briggs-Gowan & Carter, 2008; Carter, Garrity-Rokous,
with divergent content, which leads to different testing results for Chazan-Cohen, Little, & Briggs-Gowan, 2001; Leerkes, Blankson,
the same target. For that reason, the authors highlighted the im- & O’Brien, 2009; Semple, Lee, Rosa, & Miller, 2010; Squires,
portance of defining the behavioral manifestations of the construct, Bricker, Heo, & Twombly, 2001). For example, the SDQ (Good-
as well as specifying the way the construct relates to others man, 1994) has been used for social-emotional screening in clin-
(Ziegler, Booth, & Bensch, 2013), hence building the nomological ical and nonclinical samples (see Woerner et al., 2004 for a
net of a construct (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). review). In some cases, the SDQ has been linked to measures of
In response to this conceptual problem, some authors have put emotional intelligence (Poulou, 2014) and the Big Five (Muris,
efforts on examining the convergences and divergences between Meesters, & Diederen, 2005; Slobodskaya, 2007), showing a clear
social-emotional skills measures, and started clustering their dif- overlap between several subscales of these measures. It is note-
ferent terms into a smaller number of overarching constructs (e.g., worthy to mention that in many studies with nonclinical popula-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

NRC, 2012; Primi, Santos, et al., 2016). For example, convincing tions the difficulty subscales of the SDQ have been used separately
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

evidence suggests that the skills included in social-emotional skill to assess, for example, the effects of school connectedness on
frameworks can be organized in the broad domains of the Big Five school climate (Loukas, Suzuki, & Horton, 2006), the predictors of
personality traits of Conscientiousness, Negative Emotionality, engagement and achievement in education (Sanders, Munford, &
Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Openness to Experience (see Thimasarn-Anwar, 2016), and the effects of a physical activity
John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008 for a review of the Big Five). In line intervention in schools (Bunketorp Käll, Malmgren, Olsson, Lin-
with this perspective, Kyllonen et al. (2014) and Primi, Santos, et dén, & Nilsson, 2015). Clearly, the scales of the SDQ are relevant
al. (2016) independently proposed frameworks of social-emotional for assessing the psychological adjustment of children and youth
skills that have a strong parallel with the Big Five after theoretical (e.g., externalizing and internalizing behavior problems); nonethe-
and empirical analyses. Primi, Santos, and colleagues (2016) em- less, it might be more useful to approach the assessment of
pirically examined the commonality across more than 200 items children and youth’s adaptations and positive functioning in envi-
selected from eight instruments that are widely used to measure ronmental settings like the school context by focusing on strengths
social-emotional skills, and found that the main social-emotional skill rather than on difficulties, and by using specifically designed
dimensions show substantial overlap with the dimensions specified by measures that assess a broader range of social-emotional con-
the Big Five. Other researchers, using a more top-down approach, structs.
explicitly developed social-emotional skill sets aligned with the Big
Five (e.g., Guerra, Modecki, & Cunningham’s PRACTICE model;
Toward a Common Structure
Guerra, Modecki, & Cunningham, 2014; Lipnevich, Preckel, & Rob-
erts, 2016). Guerra and colleagues (2014) emphasize that while the There have been many initiatives to identify the commonalities
Big Five predicts a variety of (work-related) outcomes, a more fine- across different frameworks to integrate skills into more compre-
grained skill-based approach seems crucial to improve performance hensive and distinct categories. As suggested by Kyllonen and
and well-being. Social-emotional learning programs may, for exam- colleagues (2014), the advantages of coming up with a common
ple, benefit from “the identification of modifiable skills that are social-emotional skills framework “means that the maximum num-
aligned with more enduring traits” (Guerra et al., 2014, p. 5). In ber of researchers can contribute to advances in our knowledge of
addition, specific social-emotional skill interventions may be better how to assess, how to intervene, and how to develop students,
targeted to develop narrow skills, or subskills, instead of trying to using standard terminology” (p. 1), and that the evidence collected
affect the broader skill domain defined at the level of the Big Five. could be well-accepted by scientific, educational, and other com-
Other social-emotional skill frameworks have been developed munities of practice.
from more specific literatures in the domain of individual differ- John and De Fruyt (2015) recently reviewed the various social-
ences. The Collaborative for Academic, Social and Emotional emotional skill frameworks that were advocated during the past
Learning (CASEL) defines social-emotional learning as: two decades, grouping them under the broad umbrella of the Big
Five, in line with the empirical evidence provided by Kyllonen et
. . . the processes through which children and adults acquire and al. (2014) and Primi, Santos, et al. (2016). Building on this
effectively apply the knowledge, attitudes, and skills necessary to
evidence, as well on recent reviews and analyses of more-specific
understand and manage emotions, set and achieve positive goals, feel
and show empathy for others, establish and maintain positive rela-
aspects or facets (DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson, 2007; Soto &
tionships, and make responsible decisions. John, 2017), Primi, John, Santos, and De Fruyt (2017) proposed an
integrative set of five domains of social-emotional skill character-
(CASEL, 2013, p. 4) and is strongly rooted in the emotional istics, along with a set of more specific skills that cover the broad
intelligence literature. Van der Zee, Thijs, and Schakel (2002) domain of social-emotional functioning in youth. The ambition of
examined the underlying structure of emotional intelligence do- the model was not completeness of coverage, as completeness
mains, and found three main dimensions: Empathy, Autonomy, would have resulted in an overly large, unmanageable and, thus,
and Emotional Control. Clearly, these dimensions could also be impractical set of constructs. Rather, the goal was more pragmatic:
placed under the umbrella of social-emotional skills, and are to capture a comprehensive yet manageable set of skill constructs
included in numerous social-emotional taxonomies (see e.g., CA- that have predictive value for a diverse set of consequential out-
SEL, 2013; Guerra et al., 2014; Zins, Weissberg, Wang, & Wal- comes, and that could serve either as stand-alone skills or as
berg, 2004). building blocks to form more complex, “hybrid” skills (see below
4 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

for further detail). Such a pragmatic focus is the only realistic consensus on grouping and labeling skills will facilitate the inclu-
option given the time-constraints in large-scale assessments, espe- sion of social-emotional skills as end-points in the educational
cially in school settings, where the proposed skill set needs to be curricula, help to determine the target group and the ways to
rich enough to include those social-emotional skills that connect to achieve social-emotional learning, identify the contexts and situ-
a broad range of desirable goals that policy and decision makers ations where the behaviors associated with the skills are to be
want to promote, or to negative outcomes they want to prevent, in manifested, and select the most appropriate method for assessing
educational settings. This integrative skill model can, of course, be them (NRC, 2011). The Government of Ireland (Irish National
extended by adding specific skills, depending on the particular Teachers’ Organisation, 2012), for example, fosters self-confidence,
objectives that researchers or practitioners may have. self-worth, and self-awareness attitudes in students in primary educa-
Primi et al.’s model (Primi et al., 2017) distinguishes among five tion to increase students’ sense of self-efficacy, to gain control over
broad social-emotional skill domains: Self-management (related to their own life, and to understand their environment. They chose to
Conscientiousness from the personality literature), the interper- implement these skills through active learning inside and outside the
sonal dimensions Engaging with Others (conceptually related to classroom, and to use formative methods such as self- and peer
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Extraversion) and Amity (related to Agreeableness), Negative- assessment to achieve these learning goals.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Emotion Regulation (associated with Negative Affectivity or Neu-


On the other hand, a well-defined framework of social-emotional
roticism) and Open-mindedness (associated with Openness to ex-
skills and validated instruments to measure them could also serve to
perience). The more specific skills grouped under these broad
promote adaptive functioning and prevent deficits in children and
headers are described in Table 1. These social-emotional domains
adolescents. The relatively large proportion of (young) children with
and facets should serve as an integrative framework that can be
social-emotional and behavioral problems emphasizes the crucial
used to build evidence, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, about
need for adequate social-emotional skill development and training in
the consequential outcomes of social-emotional skills throughout
the development of children and youth. education (Carter, Briggs-Gowan, & Davis, 2004). Prevalence esti-
mates vary, but the majority of studies report that between 10 and
15% of 2- and 3-year olds suffer from social-emotional problems.
Benefits of a Comprehensive Social-Emotional
Many of these problems, however, initially remain under the radar
Skill Framework
and are not addressed in a timely way by social service systems. As
The effort of agreeing on common conceptual and operational social-emotional problems are likely to persist over time and form risk
definitions of social-emotional skills is beneficial for multiple factors for the emergence of behavior problems and psychopathology
reasons in different areas of expertise. In the educational area, a (Carter et al., 2004; De Bolle, Beyers, De Clercq, & De Fruyt, 2012),

Table 1
Social-Emotional Skill Domains and Facets (Primi et al., 2017)

Domain Facet Definition

Self-management Determination Setting goals and high standards, motivating oneself, working very hard, and applying
oneself fully to the task, work, or project at hand.
Organization Possessing organizational skills and meticulous attention to detail that are useful for
planning and executing plans to reach longer-term goals.
Focus Focusing attention and concentrating on the current task, and avoiding distractions.
Persistence Overcoming obstacles to reach important goals.
Responsibility Possessing time management skills, being punctual, and honoring commitments.
Engaging with others Social initiative Approaching and connecting with others, both friends and strangers, initiating,
maintaining, and enjoying social contact and connections.
Assertiveness Speaking up, voicing opinions, needs, and feelings, and exerting social influence.
Enthusiasm Showing passion and zest for life; approaching daily tasks with energy, excitement,
and a positive attitude.
Amity Compassion Using empathy and perspective taking skills to understand the needs and feelings of
others, acting on that understanding with kindness and consideration of others.
Respect Treating others with respect and politeness.
Trust Assuming that others generally have good intentions and forgiving those that have
done wrong.
Negative-emotion regulation Stress modulation Modulating anxiety and response to stress.
Self-confidence Feeling satisfied with self and current life, having positive thoughts about self, and
maintaining optimistic expectations.
Frustration tolerance Regulating temper, anger, and irritation; maintaining tranquility and equanimity in the
face of frustrations.
Open-mindedness Intellectual curiosity Mustering interest in ideas and a passion for learning, understanding, and intellectual
exploration.
Creative Generating novel ways to think about or do things through experimenting, tinkering,
imagination learning from failure, insight, and vision.
Artistic interest Valuing, appreciating, and enjoying design, art, and beauty, which may be
experienced or expressed in writing, visual and performing arts, music, and other
forms of self-actualization.
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 5

early follow-up and monitoring of social-emotional development in disagree (nay-saying) with questionnaire items, regardless of their
education is vital. content, and its effects have long been recognized in adults (e.g.,
Cronbach, 1946; Jackson & Messick, 1958). When left uncon-
Assessment Challenges and Innovations trolled, individual differences in acquiescence tend to positively
bias interitem correlations, increasing the (positive) correlations
The growing attention to social-emotional skill development as
between two items keyed in the same direction but decreasing the
an explicit outcome in education is a new focus and in addition to
(negative) correlations between items keyed in the opposite direc-
mastery of math, languages, arts, and sciences. This new focus
tion (one true and one false keyed).
necessitates dedicated and high-quality monitoring tools to assess
Whereas the existence of acquiescence effects has long been
the social-emotional skill development and progress of children
recognized in adults, Soto, John, Gosling, and Potter (2008) stud-
and adolescents. The development and improvement of social-
ied acquiescence effects in children, expecting acquiescence to be
emotional assessment tools and methods is, therefore, a priority.
one of the culprits responsible for the much less clear factor
As we set out below, social-emotional skill assessment is consid-
structures typically found in younger respondents. Indeed, individ-
ered challenging because of multiple measurement issues such as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ual differences in acquiescence were much more pronounced in


response styles (e.g., acquiescence), bandwidth—fidelity prob-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

children, decreasing linearly and substantially (by about half) from


lems, and lack of convergence between measures (Ziegler &
age 10 to the typical adult level at age 19. Even more important,
Brunner, 2016) that are also prominent in the broader field of
acquiescence variance seriously distorted the factor structures of
individual differences assessment.
personality self-ratings in the youngest children; at age 10, for
Brackett et al. (2006), for example, found no relationship be-
example, the standard Big Five structure could not be recovered in
tween a self-report measure of emotional intelligence (EI) and
the raw data but did emerge when individual differences in acqui-
observable behaviors in a social encounter, and even the Mayer-
escence were controlled. These findings for children have been
Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT; Mayer,
extended to adults with lower educational attainment; for example,
Salovey, & Caruso, 2002), a performance measure of EI, was only
Rammstedt and Farmer (2013), found that the expected Big Five
able to predict social competence for men, but not for women
structure could be obtained for low educational-attainment groups
(Brackett et al., 2006). In addition, few studies have obtained
only after correcting for individual differences in acquiescence.
convergent validity evidence for the scale scores of the SDQ with
These findings have led to a renewed interest in understanding the
similar instruments in nonclinical populations of children and
causes and consequences of acquiescence, and researchers have
adolescents (e.g., Koskelainen, Sourander, & Kaljonen, 2000;
suggested different approaches to correcting individuals’ re-
Muris, Meesters, Eijkelenboom, & Vincken, 2004), and others
sponses to rating scales, such as within-person centering around an
have found low internal consistency in the test scores of some
acquiescence index (Rammstedt, Danner, & Bosnjak, 2017; Soto
subscales mainly because of problems with the reversed coded
et al., 2008), using anchoring vignettes (Mõttus et al., 2012), and
items (i.e., adaptive or positive items; Muris et al., 2004; Ortuño-
applying statistical corrections (Buckley, 2009).
Sierra et al., 2015; Ruchkin, Jones, Vermeiren, & Schwab-Stone,
Anchoring vignettes. Respondents from different cultural
2008).
backgrounds also seem to differ in the way they use Likert rating
These and other problems have led researchers to explore var-
scales. Using simple Likert scales, studies using the PISA 2003
ious ways either to optimize current assessment methods or to
data found a counterintuitive pattern of findings in which correla-
introduce alternative assessment methods to describe social-
tions between noncognitive factors (such as motivation) and actual
emotional skills. Duckworth and Yeager (2015) described a series
cognitive achievement measures were positive at the within-
of potential problems associated with the use of self-descriptions
country level (as expected), but unexpectedly negative at the
using Likert rating scales, but also discussed alternative types of
between-country level (Bertling, Borgonovi, & Almonte, 2016;
measures that may alleviate the shortcomings of rating scales, or
Kyllonen & Bertling, 2013). Assessing the same constructs with
suggested to complement rating scales with other methods. In the
different response formats in which response style effects were
following section, we will discuss assessment challenges and re-
avoided removed this anomaly, suggesting that Likert scales may
cent innovations in social-emotional skill research.
not be ideal for cross-country comparisons because of their sus-
ceptibility to response style effects. Kyllonen and Bertling (2013)
Optimizing Current Assessment Methods
and He and Van de Vijver (2016) suggested that the cross-country
Large-scale international assessments, such as the Programme comparability of students’ self-descriptions on Likert scales could
for International Student Assessment (PISA), have started to pay be improved by rescaling students’ ratings based on their re-
attention to students’ social-emotional skills (OECD, 2013), illus- sponses to so-called anchoring vignettes.
trating that education officials are shifting from their primary (or Anchoring vignettes serve the goal of taking individuals’ dif-
even exclusive) focus on academic abilities to a growing concern ferential interpretation and use of rating scales into account, and
about students’ social-emotional skills. The between-country com- may help correct various response styles such as acquiescence,
parisons of students in PISA pose additional challenges for assess- extreme responding, or differential use of the scale (Primi, Zanon,
ments, given likely cultural differences in the way students ap- Santos, De Fruyt, & John, 2016). Using the anchoring vignette
proach and respond to Likert-type rating scales. Different ways to technique, individuals rate the behavior or traits of a person in a
correct raw scores on rating scales have been suggested to deal hypothetical scenario using the same response format and on the
with these problems. same rating scale as they later rate their own behavior or traits (for
Correction for acquiescence. Acquiescent responding is an an example see Primi, Zanon, et al., 2016). Because all individuals
individual’s general tendency to consistently agree (yea-saying) or rate the same vignette, variability among individuals in their re-
6 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

sponses can be attributed to interindividual variability in scale use listed as a curriculum objective. Vergauwe, Wille, Hofmans, and
(Vonkova, Bendl, & Papajoanu, 2017). Individuals’ self-ratings De Fruyt (2017), for example, recently suggested a compound
can then be rescaled based on their ratings on the anchoring scale to assess individuals’ standing on the hybrid construct of
vignettes; in the PISA 2012 assessments, this rescaling led to the “charismatic leadership,” relying on an individual’s score on a set
elimination of the motivation-achievement paradox discussed earlier of distinct personality traits. Vergauwe and colleagues asked ex-
(He & Van de Vijver, 2016; Kyllonen & Bertling, 2013). A recent perts to rate a prototypical charismatic individual on all 30 facets
study (Vonkova et al., 2017) found that 7th-grade students used of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1995), subsequently retaining
vignette rating scales different across different contexts, and recom- those facets that were rated as prototypically high or prototypically
mended using vignettes closely related to the construct of interest. low, and computing an aggregate score relying on positively keyed
However, other research suggests that vignettes tapping into unrelated and reversed scored negatively keyed facets. Similarly, complex
behavior serve equally well for rescaling all of individuals’ responses, and more clinical constructs, such as personality disorders, have
suggesting that response tendencies are general rather than construct been approached in this way in clinical assessment (Bastiaansen,
specific (Kyllonen & Bertling, 2013). If this finding is generalizable, Rossi, & De Fruyt, 2013; Miller, Bagby, Pilkonis, Reynolds, &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

social-emotional skills assessments addressing a variety of skills are Lynam, 2005; Widiger, Costa, & McCrae, 2002). The same ap-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

therefore not bound to develop multiple skill-specific vignettes, which proach could be used relying on Primi et al.’s (2017) previously
is an advantage when assessment time is limited described social-emotional skill set to describe hybrid skill con-
Multiple methods and informants. Adopting multiple as- structs in education, such as “entrepreneurial skill” or “appreciat-
sessment methods—although time-consuming—may also help al- ing diversity.”
leviate the problems associated with using Likert scales and bring
additional information to the table for the assessment of social- Innovative Approaches to the Assessment of Social-
emotional skills (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; Duckworth & Yeager,
Emotional Skills
2015). Using multiple methods may minimize the weaknesses of
each of the individual methods (Scollon, Kim-Prieto, & Diener, In addition to correcting, optimizing, and/or regrouping estab-
2003) and increases reliability and (construct) validity of the test lished skill scales, several innovative assessment approaches have
scores (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; Eid & Diener, 2006). In addition, been introduced in the field of social-emotional skills assessment
adopting multiple methods allows for an estimation of the variance during the recent past as alternatives to Likert-based measurement.
that is systematically associated with each method (John & Soto, Forced-choice response format. A major weakness of the
2007). Thus, although using multiple methods requires additional unidimensional Likert scales providing test-takers with one item at
resources, it enriches the data substantially, and it may provide a time is their susceptibility to faking (i.e., intentionally responding
answers to a larger variety of research questions (McDonald, in an untruthful way) because it is often obvious to participants
2008). The multidimensional nature of social-emotional skills that either a high or a low level on a certain item is more favorable.
would be discredited by assessing it through only one method or In forced-choice assessments, individuals are asked to consider
by relying on a single informant. multiple statements at a time and to rate the extent to which items
However, when including multiple informants to assess social- apply to them relative to the other items (Brown & Maydeu-
emotional skills, researchers should also take into account the type Olivares, 2014). That is, students are asked to rank-order a pre-
of informant, as there may be substantial variability among infor- defined set of items or are instructed to indicate which item applies
mants’ perceptions. A review by Major, Seabra-Santos, and Martin most and which applies least to them. As several items may tap
(2015), for example, found larger agreement between teacher and into equally favorable traits and participants cannot simultaneously
parent ratings when assessing externalizing than internalizing endorse all items to the same extent, test-takers cannot use faking
problem behaviors, and overall the correlations indicating agree- to score equally high on all scales.
ment in their ratings of children’s social-emotional skills were There is meta-analytic evidence suggesting that forced-choice
modest. The authors suggested that this could be because of the measures are less susceptible to faking and this increases their
different settings where teachers and parents perceived children’s predictive validity for academic and job related outcomes when
behaviors. This is not surprising given that social-emotional skills compared with single statement Likert-scale measures (Salgado &
are multidimensional constructs that are manifested somewhat Táuriz, 2014). In addition, response style effects such as acquies-
differently in different contexts (Ramsey, Spira, Parisi, & Rebok, cence and extreme responding may be avoided altogether in
2016). Therefore, the various actors (e.g., teachers, parents, and forced-choice measures because they do not use an explicit rating
students) involved may each value and focus on particular aspects scale on which such biases could develop (Kyllonen, 2014, 2016);
of the construct and, thus, informants’ unique frames of reference this is of particular interest because, as noted earlier, these re-
should not be discarded. sponse styles are more pronounced at younger ages (Soto et al.,
Compound scales. Educational curricula define learning goals 2008; see also Bachman, O’Malley, & Freedman-Doan, 2010;
usually referring to broad descriptions that are often highly value- Batchelor, Miao, & McDaniel, 2013). Although these advantages
laden, or refer to multidimensional constructs tapping into multiple make forced-choice measures highly promising for the assessment
skills or traits, such as “global citizenship,” “entrepreneurship,” or of social-emotional skills, analyzing forced-choice data poses sev-
“leadership.” The key challenge for the evaluation of such hybrid eral psychometric challenges because of the ipsative nature of the
and multidimensional skill terms is to first decompose them into responses. Although these issues can be overcome using item
their constituent skills defined in an existing skill taxonomy, and response theory modeling (Brown & Maydeu-Olivares, 2011), this
subsequently think about ways to psychometrically recombine the is clearly a less straightforward approach than the scoring of
constituting skill scores to serve as an indicator of the hybrid skill simple Likert scales. An additional main drawback of the use of
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 7

forced-choice formats for the assessment of children’s social- children and adolescents can understand and distinguish from each
emotional skills is that the forced-choice format might be too other. For formative assessment objectives, these narratives will
complex for young children (Meisenberg & Williams, 2008; Pet- also serve didactic purposes, providing feedback to the individual
way, Brenneman, & Kyllonen, 2016). Therefore, future assess- student (and eventually to the teacher/coach/counseling psycholo-
ments of social-emotional skills could aim to develop forced- gist) at what level s/he currently is, and what the next steps to
choice methods that are tailored to students’ age and cognitive attain are. To be in a better position to support youth in these
development, such as games or interactive tasks involving puppets developmental processes, we need meta-analytic research, exam-
for assessments with very young children. One example is research ining effects of social-emotional skill interventions to increase
using the Berkeley Puppet Interview to obtain self-reports of the mastery levels of specific skills. Such meta-analytic findings are
Big Five domains from children as young as age 5 (Measelle, John, preferably structured in terms of the social-emotional skill taxon-
Ablow, Cowan, & Cowan, 2005), which could be adapted to a omy advocated in this article. Assessment tools with rubrics will
forced-choice format. have to be constructed in such a way to be able to pick up “change”
Rubrics as Likert-scale anchors. Most often, Likert-scales in students’ standing on social-emotional skills.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

are presented with numbers or generic labels as scale anchors (e.g., Within-individual differences and dynamic performance.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

“fully disagree,” “disagree,” “agree,” or “fully agree”). An alter- Ambulatory assessment is another type of assessment that provides
native is to use behaviorally anchored-scales, using skill levels numerous advantages over traditional one-time self- and other-
defined in rubrics as anchors. Rubrics are a set of quality criteria reports. Although various types and names have been coined (e.g.,
for scoring a certain type of performance (Allen & Tanner, 2006) daily diary methods, Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003; ecological
and have been frequently used in formative assessment to provide momentary assessment, Stone & Shiffman, 1994; everyday expe-
feedback to students and in summative assessment to grade stu- rience methods, Reis & Gable, 2000; experience sampling, Larson
dents’ work both with self- and peer ratings (Panadero & Jonsson, & Csikszentmihalyi, 1983), the general idea of ambulatory assess-
2013; Reddy & Andrade, 2010). Rubrics have the potential to ment is that several times a day individuals report on their current
promote learning and to support instruction because the defined feelings, thoughts, or behaviors as they are experienced in their
skill levels create clear expectations of performance, making scor- everyday environment (Beal, 2015; Hofmans, De Clercq, Kup-
ing and feedback more transparent (Jonsson & Svingby, 2007). pens, Verbeke, & Widiger, 2018; Zirkel, Garcia, & Murphy,
The explicitly and clearly formulated assessment criteria expressed 2015). Most assessment methods that are currently used in social-
in rubrics provide advantages over other assessment methods both emotional skill measurement are designed to measure differences
in terms of children’s capability of self-assessment and in terms of between students. Ambulatory assessments on the contrary, are
their capacity to regulate their performance (Panadero & Jonsson, primarily suited for the assessment of intraindividual variability, in
2013). In this respect, rubrics might be more effective than simple other words how the student’s standing on social-emotional skills
Likert-scale based evaluations of competencies (Bartlett et al., fluctuates across situations. The multiple data points within the
2015). The Partnership for 21st Century Learning and the Assess- individual provide a rich variety of information on how experi-
ment and Teaching of 21st Century Skills project, for example, ences may vary over short time frames and contexts (Scollon et al.,
have developed rubrics to measure the progression of social- 2003; Zirkel et al., 2015), providing insight into the dynamics of
emotional skills in students as part of formative assessment initia- functioning of the student. In addition, several recent studies have
tives. It has been suggested, however, that merely providing rubric started to integrate situational measures in ambulatory assessments
descriptions to students is not sufficient for performance improve- to examine how state variability is shaped by situational variability
ment; constructive feedback is a necessary prerequisite for rubrics (e.g., Jones, Brown, Serfass, & Sherman, 2017; Sherman, Rauth-
to be an effective tool for learning (Panadero & Jonsson, 2013; mann, Brown, Serfass, & Jones, 2015). This might provide unique
Wollenschläger, Hattie, Machts, Möller, & Harms, 2016). opportunities for studying dynamic aspects of social-emotional
A review by Jonsson and Svingby (2007) concluded that the use skill development and functioning by enabling examinations of
of rubrics can enhance the internal consistency of test scores, possible situational sources of variability in students’ functioning.
especially when rubrics are topic-specific or assess a specific One of the major advantages of ambulatory assessment is that
dimension of performance, and their scoring system is well- because participants report on their state or behavior at the time it
defined. They concluded, however, that there is insufficient evi- is experienced, their responses are not affected by retrospective
dence to affirm that the use of rubrics can lead to valid inferences biases—such as recall inaccuracies and memory limitations—that
about performance because of the small number of convergent, might be present in common questionnaire assessments (Becker,
divergent, and construct validity studies on the use of rubrics. Goetz, Morger, & Ranellucci, 2014; Trull & Ebner-Priemer,
There is hence a large need to examine the validity and the effects 2013). Although ambulatory assessments are designed to be brief,
of using rubrics to assess cognitive and social-emotional skills’ a drawback of these multiple assessments is that participation
outcomes before these can be recommended to replace more requires considerable effort. This need to persist in the task of
concrete and tangible academic outputs such as writing essays, oral dutifully responding to recurring assessments could be especially
presentations, or solving math problems. challenging for children. That is, although participating in an
The construction of social-emotional skill rubrics will have to be ambulatory assessment study may be exciting for them at the
based on developmental theories of specific skills describing in- beginning, initial enthusiasm might fade away, potentially intro-
creasing mastery levels of that skill (e.g., different levels of em- ducing response biases. Another drawback may be that the recur-
pathy [Gladstein, 1983] or emotion regulation [Gross & Thomp- ring questions in ambulatory assessment might make participants
son, 2007]). The key features of these levels subsequently have to more perceptive of their own behavior, inducing extra self-
be operationally translated into short narrative descriptions that awareness expressed in reported behavioral change (Zirkel et al.,
8 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

2015), although such negative effects are not necessarily reported dividual’s reactions to different types of situations. Recently,
(Larson, 1989). Lievens and his colleagues (in press) demonstrated that university
Biodata. While ambulatory assessment targets individuals’ students’ intraindividual variability on certain traits predicted peer-
experiences in the present, biodata studies use standardized ques- rated academic task performance. An additional key finding was
tions to ask individuals about biographical details, such as the that within-individual variability as reflected in the responses to
frequency of the occurrence of certain events, behaviors, or expe- the hypothetical scenarios was related to actual everyday life
riences in the past (Kyllonen et al., 2014; Lipnevich, MacCann, & variability as reported in a subsequent experience sampling study.
Roberts, 2013; Oswald, Schmitt, Kim, Ramsay, & Gillespie, SJTs might, therefore, provide a more practicable alternative to the
2004). Biodata information has mainly been used for college relatively time-consuming experience sampling studies for the
admissions and has shown incremental validity for the prediction assessment of inter- and intraindividual variability observable in
of college students’ performance beyond SAT and Big Five per- social-emotional skills.
sonality (Oswald et al., 2004). Other research has demonstrated Performance measures. Although the aforementioned as-
biodata studies’ potential of assessing specific personality traits. sessment methods illustrate the positive progression toward the use
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Jackson and colleagues (2010), for example, developed a set of of tools that are less susceptible to biases and faking, one could
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

behavioral indicators of Conscientiousness and found that the argue that for high-stakes purposes, such as some forms of sum-
lower-order structure of these behavioral indicators was almost mative assessment in education, the validity of test scores is still
identical to the lower-order structure from extensive self-report questionable (Kyllonen, 2016). Instead, the “holy grail” (Kyllonen,
measures of Conscientiousness. In addition, individuals’ responses 2016, p. 205) for such assessments would be objective perfor-
on the behavioral indicators of Conscientiousness were strongly mance measures that are able to measure social-emotional skills.
related to their actual conscientious behaviors as reflected in an Numerous skill-specific performance measures have been de-
experience sampling study. Given that biodata measures are less veloped over the past decades, mainly to assess critical thinking
prone to faking than traditional self-report inventories and less and creativity (e.g., California Critical Thinking Skills Test, Fa-
time-consuming than ambulatory assessment, they might be a cione, 1991; Torrance Tests of Creativity, Torrance, 1966; among
practical method for the assessment of specific skill-related behav- others), which could imply that these two skills seem to lend
iors. themselves better for performance tests than others (e.g., emotional
Situational Judgment Tests. Another assessment method find- control, self-confidence). However, the challenging goal is to
ing a quick entry into the assessment of social-emotional skills are integrate multiple skills into one test. For example, the College and
Situational Judgment Tests (SJTs). In SJTs, respondents are pro- Work Readiness Assessment (CWRA) involves not only critical
vided with a set of hypothetical scenarios accompanied by several thinking but also analytical reasoning, problem solving, and writ-
plausible courses of action. Depending on the exact design of the ten communication skills (Partnership for 21st Century Skills,
SJT, test-takers are asked to rank or rate the varying responses 2009).
according to their appropriateness in that situation. Alternatively, Modern days’ technological advances have contributed to the
participants can be offered multiple response options and could be development of more sophisticated computer-based performance
requested to select the most appropriate response to the situation assessments. One example is the Objective Achievement Motiva-
(Motowidlo, Dunnette, & Carter, 1990). The use of SJTs has tion Test in which—as the name implies—individuals’ achieve-
shown a tremendous increase over the past 25 years (Campion, ment motivation is examined by playing of a sort of computer
Ployhart, & MacKenzie, 2014), and they “have emerged recently game (for more information see Schmidt-Atzert, 2004). In addition
in education as opportunities to enliven research, practice, and to being less susceptible to faking (Arendasy, Sommer, Herle,
policy” (Anderson, Thier, & Pitts, 2017, p. 48). For example, Schützhofer, & Inwanschitz, 2011; Elliot, Lawty-Jones, & Jack-
Klassen and his colleagues (2016) recently developed the first son, 1996; Ziegler, Schmidt-Atzert, Bühner, & Krumm, 2007),
SJT-based selection test for candidates of primary teacher educa- computer tasks can have an additional advantage in social-emotional
tion programs. Their empirical search led to scenarios in three skill assessment with children because of their user-friendliness and
broad domains that showed a large degree of overlap with the three playful nature. Despite these advantages, the often-found lack of
main domains of the theoretically based Classroom Assessment convergence between objective and self-report measures (Ortner &
Scoring System (CLASS; Pianta, La Paro, & Hamre, 2008), Proyer, 2015) suggests that there is room for improvement and that
namely emotional support, classroom organization, and instruc- these methods have not yet been exploited maximally to live up to
tional support. While researchers do not unanimously agree on their full potential.
whether the development of SJT scenarios should be inductively In addition, as Falk and Heckman (2009) argued, while many
driven— based on critical incidents as provided by subject matter social scientists are reluctant to employ laboratory studies because
experts—, or deductively driven— based on theoretical frame- they “lack realism and generalizability” (p. 535), experiments can
works—(Campion et al., 2014), the study by Klassen and his be designed to contain naturalistic validity and to provide con-
colleagues suggests that an integration of both should be the trolled variation. Assessing students’ social-emotional behavior in
ultimate goal. well-controlled tasks can then provide insight in what students
SJTs seem to be favored over self-reports in terms of the validity actually do, instead of what they or others say they do. For
of test scores’ interpretation and susceptibility to bias (Anderson et example, an intervention targeted at increasing students’ levels of
al., 2017), and numerous studies have shown the validity of SJTs’ grit (i.e., perseverance to reach long-term goals) led to a higher
test scores as predictors of job and academic performance (Sackett likelihood of choosing a challenging but rewarding experimental
& Lievens, 2008). In addition, like ESMs, they provide a feasible task as compared with an easy but less rewarding alternative (Alan,
avenue to capture within-individual variability, analyzing the in- Boneva, & Ertac, 2016).
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 9

Conclusions on Assessment Challenges as “social initiative,” “reciprocity,” “trust,” and “respect,” but also
and Innovations requires processing of information and cognitive resources (Bar-
tram, 2005; John & De Fruyt, 2015). The noun “skills” is used as
The above-mentioned methods show different ways to correct a more neutral term that is acceptable by a broad audience,
the response tendencies associated with Likert scales, traditionally referring to skills’ malleability, building on contemporary knowl-
used for assessing social-emotional skills. Lessons learnt from the edge of the development of personality characteristics and abilities
correction of individual differences in acquiescent responding in (Specht et al., 2014).
personality reports (e.g., Soto et al., 2008) and the promising Third, social-emotional skills are frequently assessed through
evidence on the use of anchoring vignettes to rescale and correct self-reports using Likert scales and these will remain an important
self-report measures of social-emotional skills (Primi, Zanon, et source of information (e.g., for formative assessment purposes,
al., 2016), constitute a great impulse to enhance the assessment of among others). Different methods to increase the validity of Likert
social-emotional constructs, especially in children and youth. scales were suggested, including acquiescence correction to con-
Moreover, new methods of assessment have been proposed, some trol for response styles, and the use of anchoring vignettes as a way
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of them with more accumulated evidence than others in the area of to learn how respondents use Likert scales or correct for potential
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

social-emotional assessment (e.g., forced choice response format group-reference bias. There remains work to be done, however, on
vs. biodata). However, all of them provide opportunities for mea- the use of anchoring vignettes, to further examine under what
suring social-emotional skills as most of them have been used circumstances anchoring vignettes correct for group reference bias,
successfully in related areas like personality assessment, aim to and how this correction relates to other individual differences such
approach the multidimensional characteristic of social-emotional as, for example, verbal ability.
constructs, consider within- and between-individual differences, Fourth, several alternative methods to assess social-emotional
and take into account the influence of the environment when skills were suggested, complementing Likert-based tools. Forced-
measuring the behavioral manifestation of the skills. choice assessments are a viable alternative, but can be challenging
for younger children when they are required to rank or choose
among multiple response alternatives. Forms of ambulatory assess-
Next Steps and Assessment Recommendations
ment and also SJT-based assessments have been suggested as
This review discussed the conceptual and methodological chal- methods that may expand the research agenda of social-emotional
lenges for the assessment of social-emotional skills, further identify- skill development considerably. Indeed, these methods open a new
ing opportunities to improve their measurement. Better assessment window on the dynamics of social-emotional skills, calling atten-
tools will improve monitoring of social-emotional development in tion to both between- and within-individual differences in social-
education as part of the teaching curriculum, help identify early emotional skill manifestation. In addition to differences in skill
deficits or risk factors of problem behavior, and ultimately contribute development between persons, there is now an opportunity to learn
to individuals’ well-being, resiliency, and achievement of adaptive about the variability in social-emotional skill use within the stu-
outcomes. In conclusion, five concrete recommendations directly dent. In addition, ambulatory and SJT assessments have the po-
follow from this discussion. tential to provide information about the situation wherein social-
First, this review documented the heterogeneity of existing emotional skills are manifested. Situation descriptions are an
social-emotional skill frameworks, often with overlapping though inherent part of the SJT approach, whereas ambulatory methods
differentially labeled skills. An overarching taxonomy of social- can capture in vivo situational excerpts (e.g., through registering
emotional skills was proposed, of which the domains are closely an audio or video sample). Currently, ambulatory assessments and
related to the Big Five personality framework, in line with empir- SJT methods are already frequently used in clinical assessment and
ical research (Kyllonen et al., 2014; Primi, Santos, et al., 2016). industrial/organizational contexts, though they have large potential
This five-dimensional framework accommodates more specific for the assessment of social-emotional skills in development and
skills that have been identified across various social-emotional education as well.
frameworks and focus more on positive strengths than on difficul- The use of rubrics has been recommended in this overview as an
ties. The proposed framework is meant to serve as a starting point elegant way to describe increasing levels of skill mastery that may
and an integrative effort to build agreement on those social- reflect developmental stages of a particular skill. Ambulatory and
emotional skills that are important for fostering child and youth SJT assessments include the potential to add a more dynamic
well-being. Such consensus will guide the further development of component to such rubrics, providing the opportunity to evaluate
this area, and will help this field connect with different disciplines whether students’ skill manifestations are flexible and in line with
in psychology where social-emotional dimensions have been tra- particular situational demands and constraints. The evaluation of
ditionally measured using multiple approaches, such as in clinical, such fine-tuning and dynamic use of social-emotional skills was
educational, and developmental psychology. not possible before and would add a significant layer of potential
Second, this outline also made clear that social-emotional skills to current assessment approaches. Ultimately, such dynamic func-
are a construct category that shows strong conceptual and empir- tioning is what humans makes unique and different from machines,
ical associations with other cognitive and noncognitive individual and assessment should be conducted exactly at this level.
differences’ constructs, such as personality traits, emotional intel- Fifth, rating-based assessments will have to be complemented
ligence, and cognitive abilities. The prefix “social-emotional” is by more objective and task-based assessments. Such objective
preferred over “noncognitive” because the skills referred to are tools and methods will be especially required for high stakes
often blends of cognitive and noncognitive factors. A skill like summative assessment purposes, for example, to evaluate whether
“collaboration,” for example, involves noncognitive elements such students have reached predefined levels of social-emotional skills
10 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

stipulated in the school curriculum. Objective methods will further conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
enable researchers to investigate the normative course of social- Houston, TX.
emotional skills, as well as to evaluate interventions that can help Beal, D. J. (2015). ESM 2.0: State of the art and future potential of
individuals optimally realize their skill potential and well-being. experience sampling methods in organizational research. Annual Review
The field has started to develop such more objective and task- of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2, 383–
407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111335
based assessments of social-emotional skills and some of these
Becker, E. S., Goetz, T., Morger, V., & Ranellucci, J. (2014). The impor-
were reviewed. The challenge in this subfield will be to make a tance of teachers’ emotions and instructional behavior for their students’
clear distinction between whether a student “can” or whether he or emotions—An experience sampling analysis. Teaching and Teacher
she “will” spontaneously manifest a particular skill when neces- Education, 43, 15–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2014.05.002
sary. Task-based measures often focus on the first, whereas Likert- Bertling, J. P., Borgonovi, F., & Almonte, D. E. (2016). Psychosocial skills
based ratings frequently ask whether one typically does, so it is in large-scale assessments: Trends, challenges, and policy implications.
probably recommended in many cases to examine their joint In A. A. Lipnevich, F. Preckel, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Psychosocial
effects on the outcomes of interest. skills and school systems in the 21st century: Theory, research, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In summary, the current review has tried to make a case for a practice (pp. 347–372). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

better conceptualization of social-emotional skills to promote .1007/978-3-319-28606-8_14


adaptive functioning in children and adolescents and to facilitate Binkley, M., Erstad, O., Herman, J., Raizen, S., Ripley, M., Miller-Ricci,
M., & Rumble, M. (2012). Defining twenty-first century skills. In P.
the inclusion of social-emotional skills as end-points in educa-
Griffin, B. McGaw, & E. Care (Eds.), Assessment and teaching of 21st
tional curricula, among others. We have highlighted possible av-
century skills (pp. 17– 66). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. http://dx
enues for the challenging task to improve social-emotional assess- .doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2324-5_2
ments, and reviewed innovative assessment approaches that have Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life
been developed in the recent past. There still remains much work as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579 – 616. http://dx.doi
to do, and we welcome future efforts to advance the field of .org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030
social-emotional skill assessment further. Bowers, E. P., Li, Y., Kiely, M. K., Brittian, A., Lerner, J. V., & Lerner,
R. M. (2010). The Five Cs model of positive youth development: A
longitudinal analysis of confirmatory factor structure and measurement
References invariance. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 720 –735. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010-9530-9
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4 –18
Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P.
and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of
(2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison
Psychiatry.
of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. Jour-
Alan, S., Boneva, T., & Ertac, S. (2016, April). Ever failed, try again,
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 780 –795. http://dx.doi
succeed better: Results from a randomized educational intervention on
.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.780
grit. HCEO Working Paper. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2761390
Allen, D., & Tanner, K. (2006). Rubrics: Tools for making learning goals Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2008). Social-emotional screening
and evaluation criteria explicit for both teachers and learners. CBE Life status in early childhood predicts elementary school outcomes. Pediat-
Sciences Education, 5, 197–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1187/cbe.06-06- rics, 121, 957–962. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-1948
0168 Brown, A., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2011). Item response modeling of
Anderson, R., Thier, M., & Pitts, C. (2017). Interpersonal and intrapersonal forced-choice questionnaires. Educational and Psychological Measure-
skill assessment alternatives: Self-reports, situational-judgment tests, ment, 71, 460 –502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164410375112
and discrete-choice experiments. Learning and Individual Differences, Brown, A., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2014). Modeling forced-choice re-
53, 47– 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.017 sponse formats. In P. Irwing, T. Booth, & D. Hughes (Eds.), The Wiley
Arendasy, M., Sommer, M., Herle, M., Schützhofer, B., & Inwanschitz, D. handbook of psychometric testing. London: Wiley.
(2011). Modeling effects of faking on an objective personality test. Buckley, J. (2009). Cross-national response styles in international educa-
Journal of Individual Differences, 32, 210 –218. http://dx.doi.org/10 tional assessments: Evidence from PISA 2006. Retrieved from https://
.1027/1614-0001/a000053 edsurveys.rti.org/PISA/documents/Buckley_PISAresponsestyle.pdf
Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., & Freedman-Doan, P. (2010). Response Bunketorp Käll, L., Malmgren, H., Olsson, E., Lindén, T., & Nilsson, M.
styles revisited: Racial/ethnic and gender differences in extreme re- (2015). Effects of a curricular physical activity intervention on chil-
sponding (Monitoring the Future Occasional Paper No. 72). Ann Arbor, dren’s school performance, wellness, and brain development. Journal of
MI: Institute of Social Research. Retrieved from http://www School Psychology, 85, 704 –713. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josh.12303
.monitoringthefuture.org/ Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant
Bartlett, K. W., Whicker, S. A., Bookman, J., Narayan, A. P., Staples, validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin,
B. B., Hering, H., & McGann, K. A. (2015). Milestone-based assess- 56, 81–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046016
ments are superior to Likert-type assessments in illustrating trainee Campion, M. C., Ployhart, R. E., & MacKenzie, W. I., Jr. (2014). The state
progression. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 7, 75– 80. http:// of research on Situational Judgment Tests: A content analysis and
dx.doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-14-00389.1 directions for future research. Human Performance, 27, 283–310. http://
Bartram, D. (2005). The Great Eight competencies: A criterion-centric dx.doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2014.929693
approach to validation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1185–1203. Carter, A. S., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Davis, N. O. (2004). Assessment of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1185 young children’s social-emotional development and psychopathology: Re-
Bastiaansen, L., Rossi, G., & De Fruyt, F. (2013). Comparing a set of five cent advances and recommendations for practice. Journal of Child Psychol-
FFM Personality Disorder Counts. European Journal of Personality, 27, ogy and Psychiatry, 45, 109 –134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630
377–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.1859 .2003.00316.x
Batchelor, J. H., Miao, C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013, April). Extreme Carter, A. S., Garrity-Rokous, F. E., Chazan-Cohen, R., Little, C., &
response style: A meta-analysis. Paper presented at the 28th Annual Briggs-Gowan, M. J. (2001). Maternal depression and comorbidity:
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 11

Predicting early parenting, attachment security, and toddler social- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1483–1494. http://dx.doi.org/10
emotional problems and competencies. Journal of the American Acad- .1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01289.x
emy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 18 –26. http://dx.doi.org/10 Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual
.1097/00004583-200101000-00012 foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.
CASEL. (2013). Effective social and emotional learning programs. Pre- 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
school and elementary school edition. Retrieved from http://casel.org/ Guerra, N., Modecki, K., & Cunningham, W. (2014). Developing social-
wp-content/uploads/2016/01/2013-casel-guide-1.pdf emotional skills for the labor market: The PRACTICE model. Retrieved
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical from The World Bank website http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/
personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory. en/970131468326213915/pdf/WPS7123.pdf; http://dx.doi.org/10.1596/
Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ 1813-9450-7123
s15327752jpa6401_2 Halle, T. G., & Darling-Churchill, K. E. (2016). Review of measures of
Cronbach, L. J. (1946). Response sets and test validity. Educational and social and emotional development. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychological Measurement, 6, 475– 494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Psychology, 45, 8 –18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.003
001316444600600405 He, J., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2016). The motivation-achievement
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological paradox in international educational achievement tests: Toward a better
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ understanding. In R. B. King & A. B. I. Bernardo (Eds.), The psychology
h0040957 of Asian learners [E-reader version] (pp. 253–268). Singapore: Springer.
De Bolle, M., Beyers, W., De Clercq, B., & De Fruyt, F. (2012). General http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287-576-1_16
personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children Hofmans, J., De Clercq, B., Kuppens, P., Verbeke, L., & Widiger, T. A.
and adolescents: An investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and com- (2018). Testing the structure and process of personality using ambula-
plication models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 958 –970. tory assessment data: An overview of within-person and person-specific
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027742 techniques. Psychological Assessment, ●●, ●●●–●●●.
De Fruyt, F., & De Clercq, B. (2014). Antecedents of personality disorder Irish National Teachers’ Organisation. (2012). Wellbeing in the classroom:
in childhood and adolescence: Toward an integrative developmental Discussion document and proceedings of the consultative conference on
model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 449 – 476. http://dx education 2012. Dublin, Ireland: Author.
.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153634 Jackson, D. N., & Messick, S. (1958). Content and style in personality
De Fruyt, F., De Clercq, B., De Caluwé, E., & Verbeke, L. (2017). assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 243–252. http://dx.doi.org/10
Personality and psychopathology. In J. Specht (Ed.), Personality devel- .1037/h0045996
opment across the lifespan (pp. 385– 400). London: Elsevier: Academic Jackson, J. J., Wood, D., Bogg, T., Walton, K. E., Harms, P. D., & Roberts,
Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804674-6.00024-7 B. W. (2010). What do conscientious people do? Development and
De Fruyt, F., Wille, B., & John, O. P. (2015). Employability in the 21st validation of the Behavioral Indicators of Conscientiousness (BIC).
century: Complex (interactive) problem solving and other essential Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 501–511. http://dx.doi.org/10
skills. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Sci- .1016/j.jrp.2010.06.005
ence and Practice, 8, 276 –281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/iop.2015.33 John, O. P., & De Fruyt, F. (2015). Education and social progress:
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets Framework for the longitudinal study of social and emotional skills in
and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and cities. Paris: OECD Publishing.
Social Psychology, 93, 880 – 896. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 John, O. P., & Mauskopf, S. (2015, June). Self-reported socio-emotional
.93.5.880 qualities: Five factors for 21st century skills? Poster presented at the
Duckworth, A. L., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Measurement matters: Assess- Bi-annual Meetings of the Association for Personality Research, Saint
ing personal qualities other than cognitive ability for educational pur- Louis, MO.
poses. Educational Researcher, 44, 237–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/ John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the
0013189X15584327 integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and concep-
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2006). Handbook of multimethod measurement in tual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
psychology (Vol. 553). Washington, DC: American Psychological As- Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 114 –158). New
sociation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/11383-000 York, NY: Guilford Press.
Elliot, S., Lawty-Jones, M., & Jackson, C. (1996). Effects of dissimulation John, O. P., & Soto, C. J. (2007). The importance of being valid: Reliability
of self-report and objective measures of personality. Personality and and the process of construct validation. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley,
Individual Differences, 21, 335–343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191- & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality
8869(96)00080-3 psychology (pp. 461– 494). London: Guilford Press.
Facione, P. A. (1991). Using the California Critical Thinking Skills Test in Jones, A. B., Brown, N. A., Serfass, D. G., & Sherman, R. A. (2017).
research, evaluation, and assessment. Millbrae, CA: California Aca- Personality and density distributions of behavior, emotions, and situa-
demic Press. tions. Journal of Research in Personality, 69, 225–236. http://dx.doi.org/
Falk, A., & Heckman, J. J. (2009). Lab experiments are a major source of 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.10.006
knowledge in the social sciences. Science, 326, 535–538. http://dx.doi Jonsson, A., & Svingby, G. (2007). The use of scoring rubrics: Reliability,
.org/10.1126/science.1168244 validity and educational consequences. Educational Research Review, 2,
Geisinger, K. F. (2016). 21st century skills: What are they and how do we 130 –144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2007.05.002
assess them? Applied Measurement in Education, 29, 245–249. http:// Kern, M. L., Benson, L., Steinberg, E. A., & Steinberg, L. (2016). The
dx.doi.org/10.1080/08957347.2016.1209207 EPOCH measure of adolescent well-being. Psychological Assessment,
Gladstein, G. A. (1983). Understanding empathy: Integrating counseling, 28, 586 –597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000201
developmental, and social psychology perspectives. Journal of Coun- Klassen, R. M., Durksen, T. L., Kim, L. E., Patterson, F., Rowett, E.,
seling Psychology, 30, 467– 482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167 Warwick, J., . . . Wolpert, M.-A. (2016). Developing a proof-of-concept
.30.4.467 selection test for entry into primary teacher education programs. Inter-
Goodman, R. (1994). A modified version of the Rutter parent questionnaire national Journal of Assessment Tools in Education, 4, 96 –114. http://
including extra items on children’s strengths: A research note. Journal of dx.doi.org/10.21449/ijate.275772
12 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

Koskelainen, M., Sourander, A., & Kaljonen, A. (2000). The Strengths and Measelle, J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P.
Difficulties Questionnaire among Finnish school-aged children and ad- (2005). Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self-reports on
olescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 277–284. http:// the big five dimensions? A longitudinal study from ages 5 to 7. Journal
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s007870070031 of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 90 –106. http://dx.doi.org/10
Kyllonen, P. C. (2012, May). Measurement of 21st century skills within the .1037/0022-3514.89.1.90
common core state standards. Paper presented at the Invitational Re- Meisenberg, G., & Williams, A. (2008). Are acquiescent and extreme
search Symposium on Technology Enhanced Assessments, Princeton, response styles related to low intelligence and education? Personality
NJ. and Individual Differences, 44, 1539 –1550. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Kyllonen, P. C. (2014). Noncognitive skills assessment can be improved .paid.2008.01.010
with innovative new measures [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from http:// Miller, J. D., Bagby, R. M., Pilkonis, P. A., Reynolds, S. K., & Lynam,
educacaosec21.org.br/wp-content/uploads/2013/08/Patrick-Kyllonen- D. R. (2005). A simplified technique for scoring DSM–IV personality
Brazil-Researchers-Forum-OECD-March-2014-FINAL-update-4-08- disorders with the Five-Factor Model. Assessment, 12, 404 – 415. http://
2014.pdf dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191105280987
Kyllonen, P. C. (2016). Designing tests to measure personal attributes and Motowidlo, S. J., Dunnette, M. D., & Carter, G. W. (1990). An alternative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

noncognitive skills. In S. Lane, M. R. Raymond, & T. M. Haladyna selection procedure: The low-fidelity simulation. Journal of Applied
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Eds.), Handbook of test development (pp. 190 –211). New York, NY: Psychology, 75, 640 – 647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.75.6
Routledge. .640
Kyllonen, P. C., & Bertling, J. P. (2013). Innovative questionnaire assess- Mõttus, R., Allik, J., Realo, A., Rossier, J., Zecca, G., Ah-Kion, J., . . .
ment methods to increase cross-country comparability. In L. Rutkowski, Johnson, W. (2012). The effect of response style on self-reported Con-
M. von Davier, & D. Rutkowski (Eds.), Handbook of international scientiousness across 20 countries. Personality and Social Psychology
large-scale assessment: Background, technical issues, and methods of Bulletin, 38, 1423–1436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167212451275
data analysis (pp. 277–285). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Diederen, R. (2005). Psychometric properties of
Kyllonen, P. C., Lipnevich, A. A., Burrus, J., & Roberts, R. D. (2014). the Big Five Questionnaire for Children (BFQ-C) in a Dutch sample of
Personality, motivation, and college readiness: A prospectus for assess- young adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1757–
ment and development (ETS Research Report No. RR-14 – 06). Prince- 1769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.11.018
ton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ets2 Muris, P., Meesters, C., Eijkelenboom, A., & Vincken, M. (2004). The
.12004 self-report version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: Its
Larson, R. (1989). Beeping children and adolescents: A method for study- psychometric properties in 8- to 13-year-old non-clinical children. Brit-
ing time use and daily experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, ish Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 437– 448. http://dx.doi.org/10
18, 511–530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02139071 .1348/0144665042388982
Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling National Research Council (NRC). (2011). Assessing 21st century skills:
method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Sci- Summary of a workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies
ence, 15, 41–56. Press.
Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects National Research Council (NRC). (2012). Education for life and work:
of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social- Developing transferable knowledge and skills in the 21st century. Wash-
emotional functioning. Child Development, 80, 762–775. http://dx.doi ington, DC: The National Academies Press.
.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x OECD. (2013). PISA 2012 results: Ready to learn: Students’ engagement,
Lievens, F., Lang, J., De Fruyt, F., Corstjens, J., Van de Vijver, M., & drive and self-beliefs (Vol. III). Paris, France: OECD Publishing.
Bledow, R. (in press). A closer look at the assessment and predictive Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD).
power of people’s intra-individual variability across situations. Journal (2015). Skills for social progress: The power of social and emotional
of Applied Psychology. skills. Paris, France: OECD Publishing.
Lipnevich, A. A., MacCann, C., & Roberts, R. D. (2013). Assessing Ortner, T. M., & Proyer, R. (2015). Objective personality tests. In T. M.
non-cognitive constructs in education: A review of traditional and Ortner & F. J. R. Van De Vijver (Eds.), Behavior-based assessment in
innovative approaches. In D. H. Saklofske, C. R. Reynolds, & V. psychology: Going beyond self-report in the personality, affective, mo-
Schwean (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of child psychological assess- tivation, and social domains (pp. 133–152). Göttingen, Germany:
ment [E-reader version] (pp. 750 –772). New York, NY: Oxford Uni- Hogrefe.
versity Press. Ortuño-Sierra, J., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Aritio-Solana, R., Velasco, A. M.,
Lipnevich, A. A., Preckel, F., & Roberts, R. D. (2016). Psychosocial skills de Luis, E. C., Schumann, G., . . . the IMAGEN consortium. (2015).
and school systems in the 21st century: Theory, research, and practice. New evidence of factor structure and measurement invariance of the
Springer, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. SDQ across five European nations. European Child & Adolescent Psy-
Loukas, A., Suzuki, R., & Horton, K. D. (2006). Examining school con- chiatry, 24, 1523–1534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-015-0729-x
nectedness as a mediator of school climate effects. Journal of Research Oswald, F. L., Schmitt, N., Kim, B. H., Ramsay, L. J., & Gillespie, M. A.
on Adolescence, 16, 491–502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795 (2004). Developing a biodata measure and situational judgment inven-
.2006.00504.x tory as predictors of college student performance. Journal of Applied
Major, S. O., Seabra-Santos, M. J., & Martin, R. P. (2015). Are we talking Psychology, 89, 187–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.187
about the same child? Parent-teacher ratings of preschoolers’ social- Panadero, E., & Jonsson, A. (2013). The use of scoring rubrics for forma-
emotional behaviors. Psychology in the Schools, 52, 789 –799. http://dx tive assessment purposes revisited: A review. Educational Research
.doi.org/10.1002/pits.21855 Review, 9, 129 –144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2013.01.002
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2002). The Mayer-Salovey- Partnership for 21st Century Skills. (2009). Assessment: A 21st Century
Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), Version 2.0. Toronto, Skills Implementation Guide. Retrieved from http://www.p21.org/
Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. storage/documents/p21-stateimp_assessment.pdf
McDonald, J. (2008). Measuring personality constructs: The advantages Petway, K. T., II, Brenneman, M. W., & Kyllonen, P. C. (2016). Connect-
and disadvantages of self-reports, informant reports and behavioural ing noncognitive development to the educational pipeline. In M. S.
assessments. Enquire, 1, 75–94. Khine & S. Areepattamannil (Eds.), Non-cognitive skills and factors in
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL SKILL ASSESSMENT 13

educational attainment (pp. 13–29). Rotterdam: SensePublishers. http:// mindful attention to enhance social-emotional resiliency in children.
dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6300-591-3_2 Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 218 –229. http://dx.doi.org/10
Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., & Hamre, B. K. (2008). Classroom .1007/s10826-009-9301-y
Assessment Scoring System (CLASS). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publish- Sherman, R. A., Rauthmann, J. F., Brown, N. A., Serfass, D. G., &
ing. Jones, A. B. (2015). The independent effects of personality and
Poulou, M. S. (2014). How are trait emotional intelligence and social situations on real-time expressions of behavior and emotion. Journal
skills related to emotional and behavioural difficulties in adoles- of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 872– 888. http://dx.doi
cents? Educational Psychology, 34, 354 –366. http://dx.doi.org/10 .org/10.1037/pspp0000036
.1080/01443410.2013.785062 Slobodskaya, H. R. (2007). The associations among the Big Five, Behav-
Primi, R., John, O. P., Santos, D., & De Fruyt, F. (2017). SENNA inventory. ioural Inhibition and Behavioural Approach systems and child and
São Paulo, Brazil: Institute Ayrton Senna⬎. adolescent adjustment in Russia. Personality and Individual Differences,
Primi, R., Santos, D., John, O. P., & De Fruyt, F. (2016). Development of 43, 913–924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.012
an inventory assessing social and emotional skills in Brazilian youth. Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2):
European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 32, 5–16. http://dx.doi Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000343 bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Primi, R., Zanon, C., Santos, D., De Fruyt, F., & John, O. P. (2016). Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp00
Anchoring vignettes: Can they make adolescent self-reports of social- 00096
emotional skills more reliable, discriminant, and criterion-valid? Euro- Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The develop-
pean Journal of Psychological Assessment, 32, 39 –51. http://dx.doi.org/ mental psychometrics of big five self-reports: Acquiescence, factor
10.1027/1015-5759/a000336 structure, coherence, and differentiation from ages 10 to 20. Journal of
Rammstedt, B., Danner, D., & Bosnjak, M. (2017). Acquiescence response Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 718 –737. http://dx.doi.org/10
styles: A multilevel model explaining individual-level and country-level .1037/0022-3514.94.4.718
differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 107, 190 –194. Specht, J., Bleidorn, W., Denissen, J. J. A., Hennecke, M., Hutteman, R.,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.038 Kandler, C., . . . Zimmermann, J. (2014). What drives adult personality
Rammstedt, B., & Farmer, R. F. (2013). The impact of acquiescence on the
development? A comparison of theoretical perspectives and empirical
evaluation of personality structure. Psychological Assessment, 25, 1137–
evidence. European Journal of Personality, 28, 216 –230. http://dx.doi
1145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033323
.org/10.1002/per.1966
Ramsey, C. M., Spira, A. P., Parisi, J. M., & Rebok, G. W. (2016). School
Squires, J., Bricker, D., Heo, K., & Twombly, E. (2001). Identification of
climate: Perceptual differences between students, parents, and school
social-emotional problems in young children using a parent-completed
staff. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 27, 629 – 641.
screening measure. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 16, 405– 419.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09243453.2016.1199436
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(01)00115-6
Reddy, Y. M., & Andrade, H. (2010). A review of rubric use in higher
Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment
education. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 35, 435– 448.
(EMA) in behavioral medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02602930902862859
199 –202.
Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event-sampling and other methods for
Taylor, R. D., Oberle, E., Durlak, J. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2017).
studying everyday experience. In T. H. Reis & M. C. Judd (Eds.),
Promoting positive youth development through school-based social and
Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.
emotional learning interventions: A meta-analysis of follow-up effects.
190 –222). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Child Development, 88, 1156 –1171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev
Ruchkin, V., Jones, S., Vermeiren, R., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2008). The
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: The self-report version in .12864
American urban and suburban youth. Psychological Assessment, 20, Torrance, E. P. (1966). The torrance tests of creative thinking—Norms,
175–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.20.2.175 technical manual research edition—Verbal tests, forms A and B—Fig-
Sackett, P. R., & Lievens, F. (2008). Personnel selection. Annual Review of ural tests, forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.
Psychology, 59, 419 – 450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59 Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. (2013). Ambulatory assessment. Annual
.103006.093716 Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 151–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/
Salgado, J. F., & Táuriz, G. (2014). The Five-Factor Model, forced-choice annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510
personality inventories and performance: A comprehensive meta- Van der Zee, K., Thijs, M., & Schakel, L. (2002). The relationship of
analysis of academic and occupational validity studies. European Jour- emotional intelligence with academic intelligence and the Big Five.
nal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 23, 3–30. http://dx.doi.org/ European Journal of Personality, 16, 103–125. http://dx.doi.org/10
10.1080/1359432X.2012.716198 .1002/per.434
Sanders, J., Munford, R., & Thimasarn-Anwar, T. (2016). Staying on-track Vergauwe, J., Wille, B., Hofmans, J., & De Fruyt, F. (2017). Development
despite the odds: Factors that assist young people facing adversity to of a five-factor model charisma compound and its relations to career
continue with their education. British Educational Research Journal, 42, outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 99, 24 –39. http://dx.doi.org/
56 –73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/berj.3202 10.1016/j.jvb.2016.12.005
Schmidt-Atzert, L. (2004). OLMT. Objektiver Leistungsmotivationstest Vonkova, H., Bendl, S., & Papajoanu, O. (2017). How students report
[Objective Achievement Motivation Test]. Moedling, Austria: Schuh- dishonest behavior in school: Self-assessment and anchoring vignettes.
fried. Journal of Experimental Education, 85, 36 –53. http://dx.doi.org/10
Scollon, C. N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling: .1080/00220973.2015.1094438
Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Voogt, J., & Roblin, N. P. (2012). A comparative analysis of international
Studies, 4, 5–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023605205115 frameworks for 21st century competences: Implications for national
Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of curriculum policies. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 44, 299 –321. http://
happiness and well-being. New York, NY: Free Press. dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220272.2012.668938
Semple, R. J., Lee, J., Rosa, D., & Miller, L. F. (2010). A randomized Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A proposal for Axis
trial of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for children: Promoting II: Diagnosing personality disorders using the Five-Factor Model. In
14 ABRAHAMS ET AL.

P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five modeling. In A. A. Lipnevich, F. Preckel, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.),
factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 431– 456). Washington, DC: Psychosocial skills and school systems in the 21st century: Theory,
American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10423- research, and practice (pp. 29 –55). Cham, Switzerland: Springer Inter-
025 national Publishing.
Woerner, W., Fleitlich-Bilyk, B., Martinussen, R., Fletcher, J., Cucchiaro, Ziegler, M., Schmidt-Atzert, L., Bühner, M., & Krumm, S. (2007). Faking
G., Dalgalarrondo, P., . . . Tannock, R. (2004). The Strengths and susceptibility of different measurement methods: Questionnaire, semi-
Difficulties Questionnaire overseas: Evaluations and applications of the projective, and objective. Psychological Science, 49, 291–307.
SDQ beyond Europe. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Zins, J., Weissberg, R., Wang, M., & Walberg, H. (Eds.). (2004). Building
13(Suppl. 2), II47–II54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-004-2008-0 academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the
Wollenschläger, M., Hattie, J., Machts, N., Möller, J., & Harms, U. (2016). research say? New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
What makes rubrics effective in teacher-feedback? Transparency of Zirkel, S., Garcia, J. A., & Murphy, M. C. (2015). Experience-sampling
learning goals is not enough. Contemporary Educational Psychology, research methods and their potential for education research. Educational
44 – 45, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.11.003 Researcher, 44, 7–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0013189X14566879
Ziegler, M., Booth, T., & Bensch, D. (2013). Getting entangled in the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nomological net. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 29, Received June 8, 2017
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

157–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000173 Revision received February 16, 2018


Ziegler, M., & Brunner, M. (2016). Test standards and psychometric Accepted February 21, 2018 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like