You are on page 1of 48

Introduction to Health Psychology 4th

Edition Val Morrison


Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmass.com/product/introduction-to-health-psychology-4th-edition-val-morr
ison/
i

An Introduction to Health Psychology


iii

An Introduction to
Health Psychology

Fourth edition
Val Morrison and Paul Bennett
Pearson Education Limited
Edinburgh Gate
Harlow CM20 2JE
United Kingdom
Tel: +44 (0)1279 623623

Web: www.pearson.com/uk

First published 2006 (print)


Second edition published 2009 (print)
Third edition published 2012 (print)
Fourth edition published 2016 (print and electronic)

© Pearson Education Limited 2006, 2009, 2012 (print)


© Pearson Education Limited (print and electronic) 2016

The rights of Val Morrison and Paul Bennett to be identified as authors of this work have been
asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.

The print publication is protected by copyright. Prior to any prohibited reproduction, storage in a
retrieval system, distribution or transmission in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical,
recording or otherwise, permission should be obtained from the publisher or, where applicable, a
licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom should be obtained from the Copyright
Licensing Agency Ltd, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS.

The ePublication is protected by copyright and must not be copied, reproduced, transferred,
distributed, leased, licensed or publicly performed or used in any way except as specifically permitted
in writing by the publishers, as allowed under the terms and conditions under which it was purchased,
or as strictly permitted by applicable copyright law. Any unauthorised distribution or use of this text
may be a direct infringement of the authors’ and the publisher’s rights and those responsible may be
liable in law accordingly.

Pearson Education is not responsible for the content of third-party internet sites.

ISBN: 978–1-292–00313–9 (print)


978–1-292–00314–6 (PDF)
978–1-292–12944-0 (ePub)

British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data


A catalogue record for the print edition is available from the British Library

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


A catalog record for the print edition is available from the Library of Congress

10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
19 18 17 16 15

Cover image: © Getty Images

Print edition typeset in 9.75/13 pt Times LT Pro by Lumina Datamatics, Inc.


Printed in Slovakia by Neografia
NOTE THAT ANY PAGE CROSS REFERENCES REFER TO THE PRINT EDITION
Contents

Preface xi
Publisher’s acknowledgements xv

PART I  BEING AND STAYING HEALTHY 1


Chapter 1 What is health? 2
Learning outcomes 2
Chapter outline 4
What is health? Changing perspectives 4
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 12
What is health psychology? 24
Summary 29
Further reading 29

Chapter 2 Health inequalities 30


Learning outcomes 30
Chapter outline 32
Health differentials 32
Minority status and health 43
Gender and health 46
Summary 48
Further reading 49

Chapter 3 Health-risk behaviour 50


Learning outcomes 50
Chapter outline 52
What is health behaviour? 52
Smoking, drinking and illicit drug use 55
Unprotected sexual behaviour 72
Unhealthy diet 78
Obesity 81
Summary 85
Further reading 85

Chapter 4 Health-protective behaviour 86


Learning outcomes 86
Chapter outline 88
Adherence behaviour 88
vi Contents

Healthy diet 95
Exercise 99
Health-screening behaviour 108
Immunisation behaviour 116
Summary 120
Further reading 120

Chapter 5 Explaining health behaviour 122


Learning outcomes 122
Chapter outline 124
Distal influences on health behaviour 124
Models of health behaviour 131
Sociocognitive models of behaviour change 134
Stage models of behaviour change 147
Summary 156
Further reading 157

Chapter 6 Changing behaviour: mechanisms and approaches 158


Learning outcomes 158
Chapter outline 160
Developing public health interventions 160
Strategies for changing risk behaviour 162
Summary 175
Further reading 176

Chapter 7 Preventing health problems 178


Learning outcomes 178
Chapter outline 180
Working with individuals 180
Using the mass media 184
Environmental interventions 188
Public health programmes 191
Using new technology 200
Summary 203
Further reading 204

PART II  BECOMING ILL 205


Chapter 8 The body in health and illness 206
Learning outcomes 206
Chapter outline 208
The behavioural anatomy of the brain 208
The autonomic nervous system 211
The immune system 214
The digestive system 221
The cardiovascular system 225
The respiratory system 232
Summary 235
Further reading 236
Contents vii

Chapter 9 Symptom perception, interpretation and response 238


Learning outcomes 238
Chapter outline 240
How do we become aware of the sensations of illness? 240
Symptom perception 242
Symptom interpretation 248
Planning and taking action: responding to symptoms 263
Summary 272
Further reading 272

Chapter 10 The consultation and beyond 274


Learning outcomes 274
Chapter outline 276
The medical consultation 276
Factors that influence the process of consultation 279
Moving beyond the consultation 286
Summary 298
Further reading 299

Chapter 11 Stress, health and illness: theory 302


Learning outcomes 302
Chapter outline 304
Concepts of stress 304
Types of stress 316
Stress as a physiological response 322
The stress and illness link 331
Summary 336
Further reading 336

Chapter 12 Stress and illness moderators 338


Learning outcomes 338
Chapter outline 340
Coping defined 340
Stress, personality and illness 346
Stress and cognitions 358
Stress and emotions 362
Social support and stress 364
Summary 370
Further reading 371

Chapter 13 Managing stress 372


Learning outcomes 372
Chapter outline 374
Stress theory: a quick review 374
Stress management training 375
The third wave therapies 379
Preventing stress 382
Minimising stress in hospital settings 388
Summary 391
Further reading 392
viii Contents

PART III  BEING ILL 393


Chapter 14 The impact and outcomes of illness:
patient perspective 394
Learning outcomes 394
Chapter outline 396
The impact of illness 396
Coping with illness 408
Illness and quality of life 412
Measuring quality of life 424
Summary 431
Further reading 431

Chapter 15 The impact and outcomes of illness:


families and informal caregivers 434
Learning outcomes 434
Chapter outline 436
Illness: a family affair 436
Expectancies of care 440
Family systems and family members 443
Consequences of caring for the caregiver 448
Influences on caring outcomes 452
Summary 464
Further reading 464

Chapter 16 Pain 466


Learning outcomes 466
Chapter outline 468
The experience of pain 468
Biological models of pain 472
A psychobiological theory of pain 479
Future understandings of pain: the neuromatrix 482
Helping people to cope with pain 483
Summary 492
Further reading 493

Chapter 17 Improving health and quality of life 496


Learning outcomes 496
Chapter outline 498
Coping with chronic illness 498
Reducing distress 499
Managing illness 504
Preventing disease progression 512
Summary 517
Further reading 517
Contents ix

PART IV  FUTURES 519


Chapter 18 Futures 520
Learning outcomes 520
Chapter outline 522
The need for theory-driven practice 522
Getting evidence into practice 525
Summary 534
Further reading 534

Glossary 535
References 547
Index 642

Supporting resources
Companion website
For open-access student resources specifically written to complement this textbook
and support your learning, please visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/morrison

Lecturer resources
For password-protected online resources tailored to support the use of this textbook in
teaching, please visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/morrison
This page intentionally left blank
Preface xi

Preface

Background to this book integrated several case studies to bring the human and
clinical perspective even more to life.
Well, it’s that time again . . . time to bring out another
edition of the book. As always, this time provides an
opportunity to update, revise and generally ‘improve’ the
Aims of this textbook
text. And this edition certainly achieves these goals. It is The overall aim of this textbook is to provide a balanced,
not just an update on previous editions, but has a number informed and comprehensive UK/European textbook
of significant revisions, reflecting changes in emphasis in with sufficient breadth of material for introductory stu-
both the practice of health psychology and its developing dents, but which also provides sufficient research depth
research base. Despite these changes, we remain true to to benefit final year students or those conducting a
our original goal and beliefs. We believe health psychol- health psychology project including at Masters level. In
ogy is an exciting and vibrant discipline to study at both addition to covering mainstream health psychology top-
undergraduate and postgraduate level. It has developed ics such as health and illness beliefs, behaviour and
into an exciting professional discipline with a defined outcomes, we include topics such as socio-economic
training pathway and increasing numbers of relevant jobs influences on health, biological bases, individual and
both in health-care systems and other contexts. cultural differences, the impact on family and carers, and
We wrote the first edition of the book because we psychological interventions in health, illness and health
believed that a comprehensive European-focused text- care, as these are all essential to the study of health
book was required that didn’t predominantly focus on psychology.
health behaviours, but gave equal attention to issues in In this edition, we have stuck to a format in which
health, in illness, and in health-care practice and interven- chapters follow the general principle of issue first, theory
tion. In addition, we believed that health-care training second, research evidence third, and finally the
textbooks should be led by psychological theory and ­application of that theory and, where appropriate, the
constructs, as opposed to being led by behaviour or by effectiveness of any intervention. We first examine factors
disease. Diseases may vary clinically, but, psychologically that contribute to health, including societal, cognitive,
speaking, they share many things in common; the poten- emotional and behavioural factors, and how psycholo-
tial for life or behaviour change, distress and emotional gists and others can improve or maintain individuals’
growth, challenges to coping, potential for recovery, health. We then examine the process of becoming ill:
involvement in health care and involvement with health from the first perception of symptoms through their
professionals. We stick to this ideology; as clearly do ­interpretation and presentation to health care. We exam-
many other people, because we have been asked to ine the physiological systems that may fail in illness,
produce this fourth edition. We have maintained our com- psychosocial factors that may contribute to the develop-
prehensive coverage of health, illness and health care, ment and impact of illness, how we and our friends and
while updating and including reference to significant new families cope with illness, and how the medical system
studies, refining some sections, restructuring others, and copes with us when we become ill. Finally, we examine a
basically working towards making this new edition dis- number of psychological interventions that can improve
tinctive and (even) stronger than the last! Our readership the well-being and p ­ erhaps even health of those who
includes many medical students and therefore we have experience health problems.
xii Preface

This text is intended to provide comprehensive cover- can improve or maintain individuals’ health. Chapter 1
age of the core themes in current health psychology but considers what we actually mean when we talk about
it also addresses the fact that many individuals neither ‘health’ or ‘being healthy’ and presents a brief history to
stay healthy, nor live with illness, in isolation. The role of the mind–body debate which underpins much of our
family is crucial and therefore while acknowledging the research. We consider the important influence of current
role of significant others in many chapters, for example in health status, lifespan, ageing and culture on health, and
relation to influencing dietary or smoking behaviour, or in in doing so illustrate better the biopsychosocial model
providing support during times of stress, in this 4th edi- which underpins health psychology. Chapter 2 describes
tion we devote all of Chapter 15 to the impact of illness how factors such as social class, income and even post-
on the family and caregivers of people who are ill. code can affect one’s health, behaviour and access to
Another goal of ours in writing this textbook was to health care. Indeed, the health of the general population
acknowledge that Western theorists should not assume is influenced by the socio-economic environment in
cross-cultural similarity of health and illness perceptions which we live and which differs both within and across
or behaviours. Therefore from the first edition to this cur- countries and cultures. We have tried to reflect more of
rent edition we have integrated examples of theory and this diversity in the present volume.
research from non-Westernised countries wherever pos- Many of today’s ‘killer’ illnesses, such as some can-
sible. Throughout this text runs the theme of differentials, cers, heart disease and stroke, have a behavioural com-
whether culture, gender, age/developmental stage, or ponent. Chapters 3 and 4 describe how certain
socio-economic, and, as acknowledged by reviewers behaviours such as exercise have health-enhancing
and readers of the first three editions, our commitment to effects whereas others, such as non-adherence to
this is clearly seen in the inclusion of a whole chapter medicines, smoking or the use of illicit drugs, have
devoted to socio-economic differentials in health. health-damaging effects. More detail has been added
regarding two groups of behaviours – over-eating, and
illicit drug use. As well as updating the epidemiological
Structure of this textbook statistics regarding such health behaviours and outlining
current health policy and targets where they exist, we
Key changes from earlier editions of this book include continue to provide evidence of individual, lifespan, cul-
greater consideration of personality and cultural influences tural and gender differentials in health behaviours. These
on health behaviour in Chapters 3–5, and the addition of a behaviours have been examined by health and social
chapter focusing completely on theories and mechanisms psychologists over several decades, drawing on several
of behavioural change. These developments are reflected key theories such as social learning theory and socio-
in Chapter 6 of the new edition which is now devoted cognitive theory. In Chapter 5 we describe several mod-
entirely to exploring the nature of these behavioural els which have been rigorously tested in an effort to
change strategies and the theories from which they are identify which beliefs, expectancies, attitudes and nor-
derived. Chapter 7 then considers how these strategies mative factors contribute to health or risk behaviour.
can be used in preventing disease both through working More coverage of adherence behaviour has been added
with individuals and at a population level. In terms of the as has greater discussion of some of the broader deter-
chapters addressing the illness experience, in chapter 14 minants of behaviour. This chapter has also been
we now consider the impact of illness on a range of out- reworked to provide students with the opportunity for
comes directly impacting on the individual affected, while more critical reflection. This section of the book, there-
Chapter 15 is now devoted to the impact on the family and fore, presents evidence of the link between behaviour
caregivers of these individuals. Chapter 17 considers the and health and illness, and highlights an area where
implementation of strategies of change or emotional regu- health psychologists have much to offer in terms of
lation in people who have already developed disease. In understanding or advising on individual factors to target
addition to these structural changes, we have incorpo- in interventions. The section ends with two chapters on
rated more discussion of health psychology in the context intervention. Chapter 6 now focuses entirely on theories
of younger people and more qualitative research. of behavioural change, while Chapter 7 considers how
The textbook continues to be structured into three these may be applied, and with what success in inter-
broad sections. The first, Being and Staying Healthy, ventions designed to prevent people developing illness
contains seven chapters, which first examine factors that and poor health. It addresses interventions targeted at
contribute to health, including societal and behavioural both individuals and whole populations, in a strategy
factors, and then describe how psychologists and others known as public health intervention.
Preface xiii

The second section, Becoming Ill, contains six chap- s­ trategies. We also include a more positive view of stress
ters which take the reader through the process of and well-being, focusing on the concepts of ‘positive
becoming ill: the physiological systems that may fail in psychology’. In fact, positive beliefs become a recurring
illness, the psychosocial factors that may contribute to theme and also arise in Chapters 14 and 15. Chapter 13
symptom perception, and report and how we communi- turns to methods of alleviating stress, where it becomes
cate with the medical system. We start with a whole clear that there is not one therapeutic ‘hat’ to fit all, as we
chapter dedicated to describing biological and bodily describe a range of cognitive, behavioural and cognitive-
processes relevant to the physical experience of health behavioural approaches. The increasingly valued concept
and illness (Chapter 8). In this fourth edition, this chapter of mindfulness and mindfulness-based interventions is
covers a broader range of illnesses as well as some also introduced.
­individual case study examples and more signposts to In the third section, Being Ill, we turn our attention to
relevant psychological content to be found elsewhere in the impact of illness on the individual and their families
the book. Chapter 9 describes how we perceive, inter- across two chapters. Chapter 14 is devoted to the impact
pret and respond to symptoms, highlighting individual, of illness on the ill individual, focusing on both negative
sociocultural and contextual factors that influence the and positive outcomes. Perhaps unique to this textbook,
process of health-care-seeking behaviour, including the is a whole chapter, Chapter 15, devoted to the impact of
use of lay and online referral systems (how many of us providing care for a sick person within the family. This
have not ‘googled’ our symptoms at some point?), and fourth edition further highlights research that considers
has seen general updating in order that the increasing the dyad (patient–spouse most typically) demonstrating
number of studies addressing medically unexplained how such studies can add to our understanding of illness
symptoms (MUS) are addressed and that studies of the experiences and health outcomes. Chapter 16 addresses
dynamic and changing nature of illness perceptions and a phenomenon that accounts for the majority of visits to a
responses which more fully address the underlying theo- health professional – pain – which has been shown to be
retical assumptions are covered. In Chapter 10 present- much more than a physical experience. This chapter is
ing to, and communicating with, health professionals is the only disease-specific chapter in our text, but we
reviewed with illustrations of ‘good’ and ‘not so good’ chose to contain a chapter on pain and place it at this
practice. The role of patient involvement in decision- point towards the end of our book because, by illustrating
making is an important one in current health policy and the multidimensional nature of pain, we draw together
practice, and the evidence as to the benefits of patient much of what has preceded (in terms of predictors and
involvement is reviewed here. The chapter also considers correlates of illness, health-care processes, etc.). Pain
how health practitioners arrive at clinical decisions under illustrates extremely well the biopsychosocial approach
time pressure and information poverty: and why they health psychologists endeavour to uphold. In a similarly
sometimes get them wrong. holistic manner, Chapter 17 looks at ways of improving
Chapters 11 and 12 take us into the realm of stress, health-related quality of life by means of interventions
something that very few of us escape experiencing from such as stress management training, the use of social
time to time! We present an overview of stress theories, support, and illness management programmes.
where stress is defined either as an event, a response or Finally, we close the fourth edition of this text in the
series of responses to an event, or as a transaction same way we closed the first, with Chapter 18, which we
between the individual experiencing and appraising the have called Futures. This chapter has changed signifi-
event, and its actual characteristics. We also focus on cantly over time in that it now has three key foci: (i) how
aspects of stress beyond the individual, with considera- a number of psychological theories can be integrated to
tion of occupational stress, and how stress impacts on guide psychological interventions, (ii) how the profession
health through consideration of the growing field of psy- of health psychology is developing in a variety of coun-
choneuroimmunology. In both this and the subsequent tries and the differing ways it is achieving growth, and
chapter greater consideration of gender, personality and (iii) how psychologists can foster the use of psychological
lifespan issues are included. Chapter 12 presents the interventions or psychologically informed practice in
research evidence pertaining to factors shown to ‘moder- areas (both geographical and medical) where they are
ate’ the potentially negative effect of seemingly stressful unused. This ends our book therefore by highlighting
events, from distal antecedents such as socio-economic areas where health psychology research has or can per-
resources, social support and aspects of personality haps in the future, ‘make a difference’.
which we have increased coverage of (e.g. optimism, We hope you enjoy reading the book and learn from it
conscientiousness), to specific coping styles and as much as we learned while writing it. Enjoy!
xiv Preface

Acknowledgements have been! They also spotted errors and inconsistencies


that are inevitable with such a large project, and took
This project has been a major one which has required the their role seriously.
reading of literally thousands of empirical and review Many thanks also to the indomitable editorial team at
papers published by health, social and clinical psycholo- Pearson Education, with several development editors
gists around the globe, many books and book chapters, having taken their turn at the helm and guided us through
and many newspapers to help identify some hot health a few bad patches where academic demands and our
issues. The researchers behind all this work are thanked own research prevented us from spending time on ‘the
for their contribution to the field. book’. Particular thanks go to Lina Aboujieb who has
On a more personal level, several key researchers and been indomitable in her support and has pushed, pulled,
senior academics also acted as reviewers for our chap- advised and cajoled us up to the point where we hand
ters, some of whom have been with us from the first over to the production team.
edition and have shown great commitment and forbear-
ance! At each stage they have provided honest and Paul Bennett and Val Morrison
constructive feedback and their informed suggestions June 2015
have made this a better book than it might otherwise
Publisher’s
acknowledgements

We are grateful to the following for permission to repro- Heart Disease Statistics: A Compendium of Health Statis-
duce copyright material: tics, 2012 Edition, B ­ ritish Heart Foundation (2012) figure
4.9c, © British Heart Foundation 2012; Figure 3.5 from
Overweight and obesity as determinants of cardiovascu-
Figures lar risk, Archives of Internal Medicine, vol. 162, pp. 1867–
72 (Wilson, P.W.F., D’Agostino, R.B., Sullivan, L., Parise,
Figure 1.1 from Towards a Common Language for Func- H. and Kannel, W.B., 2002), American Medical Associa-
tioning, Disability and Health, World Health Organiza- tion; Figure 4.1 from Increasing parental provision and
tion (ICF, 2002) p. 9, World Health Organization; Figure children’s consumption of lunchbox fruit and vegetables
1.2 from EU Statistics on Income and Living Conditions in Ireland: the Food Dudes intervention, European Journal
survey, OECD Health Data (2012), Health at a Glance: of Clinical Nutrition, vol. 63, pp. 613–18 (Horne, P.J. et al.
Europe 2012, OECD Publishing, Paris, http://dx.doi. 2009), reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers
org/10.1787/9789264183896-en; Figure 2.1 from In- Ltd; Figure 4.2 from World Health Organization (2004),
equalities in health expectancies in England and Wales: www.euro.who.int; Figure 5.3 from http://www.hapa-
small area analysis from 2001 Census, Health Statis- model.de, Ralf Schwarzer; Figures 8.1, 8.2, 8.3 and 8.4
tics Quarterly, 34 (Rasulo, D., Bajekal, M. and Yar, M., from Physiology of Behaviour, 8th ed., Allyn and Bacon
2007), contains public sector information licensed under (Carlson, N., 2005) © 2005, reproduced by permission of
the Open Government Licence (OGL) v3.0, http://www. Pearson Education, Inc., Upper Saddle River, New Jer-
nationalarchives.gov.uk/doc/open-government-licence, sey; Figure 9.1 from Age- and gender-specific prevalence
also reproduced with the permission of the author; Figure of self-reported symptoms in adults, Central European
2.2 from Fair Shares for All, Edinburgh: HMSO (Scottish Journal of Public Health, 21, pp. 160–4 (Klemenc-Ketiš,
Executive, 1999) Report of the National Review of Re- Z., Krizmaric, M., and Kersnik, J., 2013), with permission
source Allocation for the NHS in Scotland, chaired by from the National Institute of Health and the authors; Fig-
Professor Sir John Arbuthnott, principal and vice-chan- ure 9.2 adapted from A symptom perception approach to
cellor of Strathclyde University, contains public sector common physical symptoms, Social Science and Medi-
information licensed under the Open Government Li- cine, 57, pp. 2343–54 (Kolk, A.M., Hanewald, G.J.F.P.,
cence (OGL) v3.0, http://www.nationalarchives.gov.uk/ Schagen, S. and Gijsbers van Wijk, C.M.T., 2003), with
doc/open-government-licence; Figure 3.1 from Alcohol permission from Elsevier; Figure 9.3 after Making Sense
in Europe: A Public Health Perspective, Institute of Al- of Illness: The Social Psychology of Health and Disease,
cohol Studies (Anderson, P. and Baumberg, B., 2006); Sage Publications (Radley, A., 1994) p. 69, with the kind
Figure 3.2 from General Lifestyle Survey 2010, Office for permission of Professor A. Radley, permission conveyed
National Statistics (2010) p. 6, contains public sector in- through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.; Figure 9.4
formation licensed under the Open Government Licence from Illness cognition: using common sense to under-
(OGL) v3.0, http://www.nationalarchives.gov.uk/doc/ stand treatment adherence and effect cognitive interac-
open-government-licence; Figure 3.3 from Opinions and tions, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16(2), p. 147
Lifestyle Survey, Drinking Habits Amongst Adults, 2012, (Leventhal, H., Diefenbach, M. and Leventhal, E., 1992),
Office for National Statistics (2012), contains public sec- with kind permission from Springer Science+Business
tor information licensed under the Open Government Li- Media and Howard Leventhal; Figure 9.5 adapted from
cence (OGL) v3.0, http://www.nationalarchives.gov.uk/ Determinants of three stages of delay in seeking care
doc/open-government-licence; Figure 3.4 from Coronary at a medical setting, Medical Care, 7, pp. 11–29 (Safer,
xvi Publisher’s acknowledgements

M.A., Tharps, Q.J., Jackson, T.C. et al., 1979), Springer/


Kluwer/Plenum; Figure 10.1 adapted from Breaking bad
Tables
news: a review of the literature, Journal of the American Table 1.1 adapted from World Health Statistics, WHO
Medical Association, 276, pp. 496–502 (Ptacek, J.T.P. (2013), World Health Organization; Table 1.2 adapt-
and Eberhardt, T.L., 1996), American Medical Associa- ed from Health at a Glance: Europe 2012, OECD
tion; Figure 11.1 adapted from Stress and Health: Biologi- (2012) p. 21, OECD Publishing, Paris, http://dx.doi.
cal and Psychological Interactions, Sage (Lovallo, W.R., org/10.1787/9789264183896-en; Table 2.1 adapted
1997) p. 77, Sage Publications, Inc.; Figure 11.2 from from Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for
Stress and Health, Brooks/Cole (Rice, P.L. 1992) p. 5, Health, WHO (2013), World Health O ­ rganization; Table 3.1
Cengage Learning, Inc.; Figure 11.3 adapted from A de- from Global Health Risks: M ­ ortality and Burden of Dis-
lay-differential equation model of the feedback-controlled ease Attributable to Selected Major Risks, WHO (2009)
hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis in humans, Math- p. 11, World Health Organization; T ­able 3.2 from Sen-
ematical Medicine and Biology, 22, pp. 15–33 (Lenbury, sible Drinking Guidelines, http://www.drinkingandyou.
Y and Pornsawad, P., 2005), by permission of Oxford
com/site/pdf/Sensibledrinking.pdf, Alcohol in Moderation;
University Press and The Institute of Mathematics and its
­Table 3.3 from The 1971 The Misuse of Drugs Act, HMSO
Applications; Figure 12.1 adapted from Stress and Emo-
(1971), contains public sector information licensed under
tion: A New Synthesis, Springer (Lazarus, R.S., 2006) p.
the Open Government Licence (OGL) v3.0, http://www.
198, republished with permission of Springer Publishing
nationalarchives.gov.uk/doc/open-government-licence;
Company, Inc., permission conveyed through Copyright
Clearance Center, Inc.; Figure 12.2 from Conscientious- Table 3.4 from HIV/AIDS Surveillance in Europe reports,
ness and health-related behaviours: a meta-analysis of http://ecdc.europa.eu/en/healthtopics/aids/surveillance-
the leading behavioral contributors to mortality, Psycho- reports/Pages/surveillance-reports.aspx, © European
logical Bulletin, 130, pp. 887 – 919 (Bogg, T. and Roberts, Centre for Disease Prevention and Control (ECDC); Table
B.X. 2004), p. 908, American Psychological Association, 4.1 from Global Recommendations on Physical ­Activity
reprinted with permission; Figure 12.3 adapted from Har- for Health, WHO (2010), World Health Organization; Ta-
diness and health: a prospective study, Journal of Per- ble 4.2 adapted from Health at a Glance: Europe 2012,
sonality and Social Psychology, 42, pp. 168–77 (Kobasa, OECD (2012) p. 107, OECD Publishing, Paris, http://
S.C., Maddi, S. and Kahn, S., 1982), American Psycho- dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264183896-en; Table on p. 174
logical Association, reprinted with permission; Figure 14.1 adapted from Does theory influence the effectiveness
from Positive effects of illness reported by myocardial in- of health behavior interventions? Meta-analysis, Health
farction and breast cancer patients, Journal of Psycho- Psychology, 33(5), pp. 465–74 (Prestwich, A., Sniehotta,
somatic Research, 47, pp. 537 – 43 (Petrie, K.J., Buick, F.F., Whittington, C., Dombrowski, S.U., Rogers, L. and
D.L., Weinman, J. and Booth, R.J., 1999), with permis- Michie, S., 2014), American Psychological Association,
sion from Elsevier; Figure 14.2 adapted from Turning the reprinted with permission; Table 7.2 from Effectiveness
tide: benefit finding after cancer surgery, Social Science analysis on the physical activity and the health benefit of
and Medicine, 59, pp. 653 – 62 (Schulz, U. and Mohamed, a community population based program, Biomedical and
N.E., 2004), with permission from Elsevier; Figure 14.3 Environmental Sciences, 26(6), pp. 468–73 (Jiang, Y.Y.,
from Quality of life: a process view, Psychology and
Yang, Z.X., Ni, R., Zhu, Y.Q., Li, Z.Y., Yang, L.C., Zhai,
Health, 12, pp. 753 – 67 (Leventhal, H. and Coleman,
Y. and Zhao, W.H., 2013), with permission from Elsevi-
S. 1997), with permission from Taylor & Francis; Figure
er; Table on p. 203 adapted from A social media-based
15.1 from Chronic illness and the life cycle: a conceptual
physical activity intervention: a randomized controlled trial,
framework, Family Process, 26(2), pp. 203–21 (Rolland,
­American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 43(5), pp. 527–
J.S., 1987), John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; Figure 15.2 from In-
terpersonal emotional processes in adjustment to chronic 32 (­Cavallo, D.N., Tate, D.F., Ries, A.V., Brown, J.D., DeV-
illness, in Social Psychological Foundations of Health and ellis, R.F. and Ammerman, A.S., 2012), Elsevier, Inc.; Table
Illness, p. 263 (De Vellis, R.F., Lewis, M.A. and Sterba, 11.1 from The social readjustment rating scale, Journal of
K.R. (J. Suls and K.A. Wallston (eds)) 2003), Blackwell, re- Psychosomatic Research, 11, pp. 213–18 (Holmes, T.H.
produced with permission of Blackwell Publishing; Figure and Rahe, R.H. 1967), Elsevier, Inc.; Table 11.2 adapted
on p. 504 from Effect of mindfulness training on asthma from Investigating the cognitive precursors of emotional
quality of life and lung function: a randomised controlled response to cancer stress: re-testing Lazarus’s transac-
trial, Thorax, 67, pp. 769–76 (Pbert, L., Madison, J.M., tional model, British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, pp.
Druker, S. et al., 2012), with permission from BMJ Pub- 97–121 (Hulbert-Williams, N.J., Morrison, V., Wilkinson,
lishing Group Ltd. C. and Neal, R.D., 2013), reproduced with permission of
Publisher’s acknowledgements xvii

Blackwell Publishing; Table 11.3 adapted from Stress and b – bottom; t – top) 2 Fotolia.com: godfer; 6 Fotolia.
Health, Brooks/Cole (Rice, P.L., 1992) pp. 188–92, Cen- com: flysnow; 17 Alamy Images: Corbis Premium RF;
gage Learning, Inc.; Tables on pp. 387 and 388 adapted 20 Fotolia.com: Robert Kneschke; 24 Alamy Images:
from Long-term effects of an intervention on psychosocial Radius Images; 30 Fotolia.com: Halfpoint; 39
work factors among healthcare professionals in a hospital Photofusion Picture Library: Robert Brook; 50 Alamy
setting, Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 68, Images: Agencja Fotograficzna Caro; 57 Photofusion
pp. 479–86 (Bourbonnais, R., Brisson, C. and Vézina, M., Picture Library: Libby Welch; 63 Alamy Images: Ace
2011), with permission from BMJ Publishing Group Ltd; Stock Limited; 74 Val Morrison; 86 Shutterstock.com:
Table 14.1 from EORTC QLQ-C30, http://groups.eortc. melis; 98 Bangor University: School of Psychology;
be/qol/eortc-qlq-c30, EORTC Quality of Life, for permis- 104 Change4Life; 112 Fotolia.com: Monkey Business;
118 Getty Images: Jacob Silberberg; 122 Tanya
sion to use contact: Quality of Life Department, EORTC
Louise Robinson; 129 Corbis: Ansgar Photography;
European Organisation for Research and Treatment of
149 Fotolia.com: Chad McDermott; 155 Pearson
Cancer, AISBL-IVZW, Avenue E. Mounier, 83/11, 1200
Education Ltd; 158 Alamy Images: Juice Images; 161
Brussels, Belgium, website: http://groups.eortc.be/qol;
Shutterstock.com: Rob Byron; 178 Fotolia.com: stan-
Table 15.1 from Understanding the health impact of care-
dardmixa; 187 Terence Higgins Trust; 192 Shutterstock.
giving: a qualitative study of immigrant parents and single com: Veniamin Kraskov; 197 Shutterstock.com:
parents of a child with cancer, Quality of Life Research, CandyBox Images; 206 Fotolia.com: adimas; 216
21, pp. 1595–605 (Klassen, A.F., Gulati, S., Granek, L., Science Photo Library Ltd: Eye of Science (b); Dr
Rosenberg-Yunger, Z.R., Watt, L., Sung, L., Klaassen, R., Andrejs Liepins (t); 238 Fotolia.com: detailblick-foto;
Dix, D. and Shaw, N.T., 2012), with kind permission from 242 Alamy Images: Bubbles Photo Library; 247 Val
Springer Science+Business Media and Anne K ­ lassen; Morrison; 269 Corbis: Hero Images; 274 Fotolia.com:
­Table on p. 478 from Pain experience of Iraq and Afghani- michaeljung; 280 Pearson Education Ltd; 289 Rex
stan Veterans with comorbid chronic pain and posttrau- Shutterstock: TM & 20thC.Fox/Everett; 295
matic stress, Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Devel- Shutterstock.com: Guzel Studio; 302 Fotolia.com:
opment, 51(4), pp. 559–70 (Outcalt, S.D., Ang, D.C., Wu, Focus Pocus LTD; 310 Pearson Education Ltd; 317
J., Sargent, C., Yu, Z. and Bair, M.J., 2014), Journal of Shutterstock.com: Charlie Edward; 328 Shutterstock.
Rehabilitation Research and Development. com: Moriz; 338 Alamy Images: Peter Bowater; 353
Tanya Louise Robinson; 368 Val Morrison; 372 Fotolia.
com: peshkova; 384 Shutterstock.com: Creativa; 389
Text Science Photo Library Ltd: John Cole; 394
Shutterstock.com: Wavebreak Premium; 410 Fotolia.
Displayed text on p. 282 after SPIKES – A six-step pro-
com: sutichak; 416 Alamy Images: OJO Images Ltd;
tocol for delivering bad news: application to the patient
423 Shutterstock.com: Featureflash; 434 Fotolia.com:
with cancer, The Oncologist, 5(4), pp. 302–11 (Baile,
chaiyon021; 439 Corbis: Reg Charity; 441 Fotolia.
W.F., Buckman, R., Lenzi. R., Glober, G., Beale, E.A. and
com: chuugo; 466 Science Photo Library Ltd: David
Kudelka, A.P., 2000), The Oncologist, AlphaMed Press;
Mack; 475 Getty Images: David Cannon; 487 Science
Newspaper headline on page 467 from ‘It’s good for
Photo Library Ltd: Will and Deni McIntyre; 496
women to suffer the pain of a natural birth’, says medical
Shutterstock.com: Goodluz; 510 Getty Images: Alvis
chief, The Guardian, 12/07/2009 (Campbell, D.), copy-
Upitis; 516 Paul Bennett; 520 Shutterstock.com:
right Guardian News & Media Ltd, 2015.
2jenn; 527 Alamy Images: Tetra Images; 532
Shutterstock.com: hxdbzxy.
Photos
The publisher would like to thank the following for their
kind permission to reproduce their photographs:(Key:
This page intentionally left blank
1

Part I
Being and staying
healthy
Chapter 1
What is health?

Learning outcomes
By the end of this chapter, you should have an understanding of:
• key perspectives on health, illness and disability, including the biomedical and
biopsychosocial models
• the influence of lifestage, culture and health status on health and illness concepts
• a range of influences on the domains of health considered important
• the role of psychology, and specifically the discipline of health psychology,
in understanding health, illness and disability
• how health is more than simply the absence of physical disease or disability
3

Health is global
In August 2014 Rome was converged upon, not by tourists (although they
were there too!), but by plane-loads of scientists from industry and academic
institutions, those working in health informatics, and possibly some health
psychologists, to attend the Third International Conference on Global Health
Challenges. Of relevance here is that the Rome conference addresses how
best to record and analyse global data relating to disease, death, lifestyle and
population change, the ‘big data’ that helps guide public health policies for the
future. In the conference, as in this chapter, it was essential to acknowledge
inequities in these data within and between countries. The conference also
addressed how health and mobile technologies can be best used to promote
individual and population health through changes in clinical practice, increased
health monitoring or behaviour change ‘nudging’, and how, globally, we can
prepare for pandemics and an ageing population.
In 2011 the UK report ‘Health is global: an outcomes framework for global
health 2011–15’ highlighted a need to promote health equity within and between
countries through our foreign and domestic policies, particularly through action on
the social determinants of health. There is a need to base our global health policies
and practice on sound evidence, whether it is drawn from public health evidence or
from psychological studies of human behaviour. We need to work with others to
develop evidence where it does not exist. The attention being paid to global issues
highlights that whilst we as individuals experience health and illness at a personal
level, the cultural and social economic setting, the dominant government and its
health policies, and even the time in which we live, all play a part. Health psychology
contributes an important part to the jigsaw that is health and well-being.
Around the world many of us attend conferences such as that happening
in Rome, in order to get a sense of what is ‘new’, what is cutting edge, what is
the exciting science that can perhaps have an impact on future health. This
textbook brings together evidence that can not only educate the aspiring
health psychologist but can also hopefully help inform such policy and prac-
tice. Whether we achieve it will depend on what we as health psychologists
‘do’ with our evidence as described in the final chapter. Hopefully over the
course of the next 18 chapters you will get a good sense of our successes,
and the challenges ahead nationally and internationally.
4 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

Chapter outline
What do we mean by health, and do we all mean the same thing when we use the
term? This chapter considers the different ways in which people have been found to
define and think about health, illness and disability: first, by providing an historical
overview of the health concept that introduces the debate over the influence of mind
on body; and, second, by illustrating how health and illness belief systems vary
according to factors such as age, culture and health status. We will introduce the issue
of developmental differences in health perceptions and examine whether children
define and think about health differently to a middle-aged or elderly person. Against
this backdrop of defining health and related belief systems, we introduce the reader to
key models on which our discipline is founded—the biomedical and the biopsychoso-
cial models of illness. To conclude the chapter we introduce the field of health psychol-
ogy and, by outlining the field’s key areas of interest, highlight the questions health
psychology research can address.

What is health? Changing to the process of ‘trephination’ (or trepanation), whereby


a hole is made in order to release evil spirits believed to

perspectives have entered the body from outside and caused disease.
Another early interpretation of disease seen in Ancient
Hebrew texts is that disease was a punishment from
Health is a word that most people will use without realis- the gods (1000–300 bc). As will be described in
ing that it may hold different meanings for different Chapter 9 ☛, similar beliefs remain today in some
people, at different times in history, in different cultures, cultures, and understanding such variations in belief
in different social classes, or even within the same fam- systems is extremely important to our understanding of
ily, depending, for example, on age or gender. Stone individuals’ response to illness ☛. Also important how-
(1979) pointed out that until we can agree on the mean- ever is the shaping, over time, of our understanding of the
ing of health and how it can be measured we are going association between mind and body.
to be unable to answer questions about how we can
protect, enhance and restore health. The root word of
health is ‘wholeness’, and indeed ‘holy’ and ‘healthy’
Mind–body relationships
share the same root word in Anglo-Saxon, which is per- It is in the writings from Ancient Greece (circa 500 bc)
haps why so many cultures associate one with the other: that we see differing explanations of health and disease
e.g. medicine men have both roles. Having its roots in to that seen in earlier times. Instead of attributing illness
‘wholeness’ also suggests the early existence of a broad to evil spirits or gods, the ancient Greek physician
view of health that included mental and physical aspects. Hippocrates (circa 460–377 bc) attributed it to the bal-
This view has not held dominance throughout history, as ance between four circulating bodily fluids (called
described below. humours): yellow bile, phlegm, blood and black bile. It
Early understanding of illness is reflected in archaeo- was thought that when a person was healthy the four
logical finds of human skulls from the Stone Age where humours were in balance, and when they were ill-­
small neat holes found in some skulls have been ­attributed balanced due to external ‘pathogens’, illness occurred.
What is health? Changing perspectives 5

The humours were attached to seasonal variations and to evil spirits entering one’s soul. Individuals were thought
conditions of hot, cold, wet and dry, where phlegm was to have little control over their health, whereas priests, in
attached to winter (cold–wet), blood to spring (wet–hot), their perceived ability to restore health by driving out
black bile to autumn (cold–dry), and yellow bile to sum- demons, did. The Church was at the forefront of society
mer (hot–dry). Hippocrates considered the mind and at this time and so science developed slowly. The mind
body as one unit, and thus it was thought that the level and body were generally viewed as working together, or
of specific bodily humours related to particular person- at least in parallel. However the prohibition of scientific
alities: excessive yellow bile was linked to a choleric or investigation such as dissection, limited medical pro-
angry temperament; black bile was attached to sadness; gress and advancements in understanding and therefore
excessive blood was associated with an optimistic or mental and mystical explanations of illness predomi-
sanguine personality; and excessive phlegm with a calm nated. Such causal explanations elicited treatment along
or phlegmatic temperament. Healing involved attempts the lines of self-punishment, abstinence from sin, prayer
to rebalance the humours, for example, through bleeding or hard work.
or starvation, or special diets and medicines. Even this These religious views persisted until the early four-
far back in time, eating healthily was considered helpful teenth and fifteenth centuries when a period of ‘rebirth’,
to the balance of the humours (Helman 1978). This a Renaissance, began. During the Renaissance, individ-
humoral theory of illness attributed disease states to ual thinking became increasingly dominant and the
bodily functions but also acknowledged that bodily ­religious perspective became only one among many. The
­factors impacted on the mind. scientific revolution of the early 1600s led to huge
This view continued with Galen (circa ad 129–199), growth in scholarly and scientific study and d­ evelopments
another influential Greek physician in Ancient Rome. in physical medicine. As a result, the understanding of
Galen considered there to be a physical or pathological the human body, and the explanations for illness, became
basis for all ill health (physical or mental) and believed increasingly organic and physiological, with little room
not only that the four bodily humours underpinned the for psychological explanations.
four dominant temperaments (the sanguine, the choleric, During the early seventeenth century, the French
the phlegmatic and the melancholic) but also that these philosopher René Descartes (1596–1650), like the
temperaments could contribute to the experience of spe- ancient Greeks, proposed that the mind and body were
cific illnesses. For example, he proposed that melan- separate entities. However, Descartes also proposed that
cholic women were more likely to get breast cancer, interaction between the two ‘domains’ was possible,
offering not a psychological explanation but a physical although initially the understanding of how mind–body
one because melancholia was itself thought to be under- interactions could happen was limited. For example,
pinned by high levels of black bile. This view was there- how could a mental thought, with no physical properties,
fore that the mind and body were interrelated, but only cause a bodily reaction (e.g. a neuron to fire) (Solmes
in terms of physical and mental disturbances both having and Turnbull 2002)? This is defined as dualism, where
an underlying physical cause. The mind itself was not the mind is considered to be ‘non-material’ (i.e. not
thought to play a role in illness aetiology. This view
dominated thinking for many centuries to come but lost
predominance in the eighteenth century when organic theory
medicine, and in particular cellular pathology, devel- a general belief or beliefs about some aspect of
oped and failed to support the humoral underpinnings. the world we live in or those in it, which may or
may not be supported by evidence. For example,
However, Galen’s descriptions of personality types were
women are worse drivers than men.
still in use in the latter half of the twentieth century
(Marks et al. 2000: 76–7). aetiology
During the early Middle Ages however (fifth–sixth (etiology): the cause of disease.
century), Galen’s theories had lost dominance when dualism
health became increasingly tied to faith and spirituality. the idea that the mind and body are separate
At this time illness was seen as God’s punishment for entities (cf. Descartes).
misdeeds or, similar to very early views, the result of
6 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

objective or visible, such as thoughts and feelings) and


the body is ‘material’ (i.e. made up of real mechanical mechanistic
‘stuff’, physical matter such as our brain, heart and a reductionist approach that reduces behaviour to
cells). Dualistic thinking considers the material and the the level of the organ or physical function.
non-material to be independent. Physicians acted as Associated with the biomedical model.
guardians of the body – viewed as a machine amenable biomedical model
to scientific investigation and explanation – whereas a view that diseases and symptoms have an
theologians acted as guardians of the mind – a place not underlying physiological explanation.
amenable to scientific investigation! The suggested
communication between mind and body was thought to
i­ndividuals perhaps more passively involved than previ-
be under the control of the pineal gland in the midbrain
ously (when at least they had been expected to pray or
(see Chapter 8 ☛), but the process of this interaction
exorcise their demons in order to return to health). This
was unclear. Because Descartes believed that the soul
approach underpins the biomedical model of illness.
left humans at the time of death, dissection and autopsy
study now became acceptable to the Church, and so the
eighteenth and nineteenth centuries witnessed a huge
growth in medical understanding. Anatomical research,
Biomedical model of illness
autopsy work and cellular pathology concluded that dis- In this model, health is defined as the absence of disease,
ease was located in human cells, not in ill-balanced and any symptom of illness is thought to have an under-
humours. lying pathology that will hopefully, but not inevitably,
Dualists developed the notion of the body as a machine be cured through medical intervention. Adhering rigidly
(a mechanistic viewpoint), understandable only in terms to the biomedical model would lead to proponents deal-
of its constituent parts (molecular, biological, biochemi- ing only with objective facts and assuming a direct
cal, genetic), with illness understood through the study of causal relationship between illness or disability, its
cellular and physiological processes. Treatment during symptoms or underlying pathology (disease), and adjust-
these centuries became more technical, diagnostic and ment outcomes (see ‘In the spotlight’ for a discussion of
focused on the physical evidence obtainable, with changing models of disability). The assumption is that

Photo 1.1 Having a disability does not equate with a lack of health and fitness
Source: Fotolia.com/flysnow.
What is health? Changing perspectives 7

removal of the pathology through medical intervention or other symptoms which are equivalent to reductions
will lead to restored health, i.e. illness or disability described by those receiving an active pharmaceutical
results from disease either originating outside the substance or treatment? (Chapter 16 ☛). Subjectivity
body (e.g. germs) or through involuntary internal in terms of beliefs, expectations and emotions interact
changes (e.g. cell mutations). This relatively mechanis- with bodily reactions to play an important role in the
tic view of how our bodies and its organs work, fail and illness or stress experience (see Chapter 9 in terms of
can be treated allows little room for subjectivity. The symptom perception, and Chapter 11 in terms of stress
biomedical view has been described as reductionist: i.e. reactivity ☛).
the basic idea that mind, matter (body) and human behav- Evidence of changed thinking was illustrated in an
iour can all be reduced to, and explained at, the level of editorial in the British Medical Journal (Bracken and
cells, neural activity or biochemical activity. How then Thomas 2002) suggesting a need to ‘move beyond the
would we deal with evidence of debilitating, but medi- mind–body split’. The authors note that simply because
cally unexplained symptoms? (see Chapter 9, ☛). neuroscience enables us to explore the ‘mind’ and its
Reductionism also tends to ignore evidence that dif- workings ‘objectively’ by the use of increasingly sophis-
ferent people respond in different ways to the same ticated scanning devices and measurements, this does
underlying disease pathology because they vary in, for not mean we are furthering our understanding of the
example, personality, cognition, social support resources subjective ‘mind’ – the thoughts, feelings and the like
or cultural beliefs (see later chapters). that make up our lives and give it meaning. They com-
Whilst the biomedical mode underpins many suc- ment that ‘conceptualising our mental life as some sort
cessful treatments including immunisation programmes of enclosed world living inside our skull does not do
which have contributed to the eradication of many life- justice to the reality of human experience’ (p. 1434). The
threatening infectious diseases, significant challenges to fact that this editorial succeeded in being published in a
a purely biomedical approach exist, as described below. medical journal with a traditionally biomedical stance is
evidence of a weakened Descartian ‘legacy’.
As our understanding of the bidirectional relationship
Challenging dualism: between mind and body has grown, dualistic thinking
psychosocial models of health has lessened, and psychology has played a significant
role in this altering perspective. A key role was played
and illness by Sigmund Freud in the 1920s and 1930s when he rede-
In terms of mind–body associations, what is perhaps fined the mind–body problem as one of ‘consciousness’
closer to the ‘truth’, as we understand it today, is that and postulated the existence of an ‘unconscious mind’
there is one type of ‘stuff’ (monist) but that it can be seen in a condition he named ‘conversion hysteria’.
perceived in two different ways: objectively and subjec- Following examination of patients with physical symp-
tively. For example, many illnesses have organic under- tomatology but no identifiable cause, and by using hyp-
lying causes, but also elicit uniquely individual nosis and free association techniques, he identified
responses due to the action of the mind, i.e. subjective unconscious conflicts which had been repressed. These
responses. So, while aspects of reductionism and dual- unconscious conflicts were considered to ‘cause’ the
istic thinking have been useful, for example, in further- physical disturbances including paralysis and loss of
ing our understanding of the aetiology and course of sensation in some patients where no underlying physical
many acute and infectious diseases (Larson 1999), the explanation was present (i.e. hysterical paralysis, e.g.
role of the ‘mind’ in the manifestation of, and response Freud and Breuer 1895).
to, illness is crucial to furthering our understanding of Freud stimulated much work into unconscious
the complex nature of health and illness. Consider, for ­conflict, personality and illness, which ultimately led to
example, the extensive evidence of ‘phantom limb pain’ the development of the field of psychosomatic medicine
experienced in amputees – how can pain exist in an (see later section). As a discipline, psychology has high-
absent limb? Consider the widespread acknowledge- lighted the need for medicine to consider the role played
ment of the placebo effect – how can an inactive in the aetiology, course and outcomes of illness, by
(dummy) substance lead to reported reductions in pain psychological and social factors.
8 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

IN THE SPOTLIGHT

Models of disability: from roles, and, as many studies have shown, increased
depression. For others, disability presents a challenge,
biomedical to biopsychosocial
a fact of life to be lived with, rather than something
Reflecting biomedical thinking, the World Health which prevents them living fully (see Chapter 14 ☛ ).
Organization’s 1980 International Classification of
Impairment, Disabilities and Handicaps (also the classi- Yet again, as seen in relation to developing concepts
fication of the consequences of disease), introduced a of illness, evidence of individual variation in the
hierarchical model which was utilised in a large body of response to impairment and disability challenges bio-
research exploring responses to disease. In the WHO IC medical thinking and opens the door for biopsychoso-
I-D-H model, impairments (abnormalities or losses at cial thought! People do not inevitably become equally
the level of a person’s organs, tissues, structures or or similarly ‘disabled’ or ‘handicapped’ even where
appearance), lead to disability (defined as a restriction or impairment is similar (e.g. Johnston and Pollard 2001).
inability to function as ‘normal for a human being’) which Reflecting this, the subsequent WHO model, the
in turn create individual handicap (whereby a person International Classification of Functioning, Disability
experiences disadvantage in fulfilling their normal social and Health (WHO 2001), which emerged out of a
roles). series of studies including 2,000 live ‘case’ evaluations
and 3,500 Case Summary evaluations conducted in
In this model, disability is placed within the individual
27 languages across 61 countries, takes a broader
and is considered an inevitable consequence of some
approach. The ICF presents a universal, dynamic and
form of impairment, and an inevitable precursor of
non-linear model whereby alterations in bodily struc-
handicap. How then, within this model, do we accom-
ture or function (replaces impairment); activities and
modate the Paralympian who in spite of sensory of
limitations therein (replaces disability), and participa-
physical impairments, functions at a level of physical
tion or restrictions therein (replaces handicap) can
performance many of us without such impairments
potentially all affect each other. Furthermore, the ICF
perform? How do we describe the person with juvenile
recognises that the relationship between structures,
diabetes who has ‘impairment’ in terms of pancreatic
activities and participation are influenced by both envi-
dysfunction (see Chapter 8), but as long as they
ronmental and personal factors. A person’s ability to
adhere to medication, function as any typical adoles-
perform at ‘capacity’ i.e. at the best possible, given
cent, without any evidence of disability? This same
their physical status, is not solely due to the level of
juvenile may, however, skip school as a result of per-
impairment. Disability no longer resides within the
ceived stigma and therefore miss out on the associ-
individual, but is a response to other factors including
ated social relationships and potential long-term
the physical, social and cultural environment the per-
employment benefits (i.e ‘handicap’ without disability).
son is trying to function within, and on their own per-
What then are the implications of such a medical and
sonal characteristics, behavioural and illness related
positivistic/functionalistic view for the treatment of
beliefs and feelings (Quinn et al. 2013, and see
impairments (especially if we believe in a need to nor-
malise)? For example, are cochlear implants for those
Chapter 9 ☛ ).

with hearing impairments a more appropriate response


than those around the individual with hearing difficul-
ties learning sign language? Whose ‘problem’ is hear- biopsychosocial
ing impairment? a view that diseases and symptoms can be
explained by a combination of physical, social,
For some people, acquiring a disability signifies the
cultural and psychological factors (cf. Engel 1977).
end of life, an exclusion from normal function and
What is health? Changing perspectives 9

Figure 1.1 The international classification of functioning, disability and health.


Source: WHO (2002b).

As a result of these shifts in thinking, this textbook In parallel with the above shifts in ways of thinking
reflects a biopsychosocial perspective on health, illness about health and illness is the increased recognition of
AND disability/activity limitation which offer up oppor- the role individual behaviour plays in that experience. It
tunity for a range of interventions, not solely targeting is to that we turn attention now, with key behaviours
the pathology or physical symptomatology. explored more fully in Chapters 3 and 4 ☛.

Biopsychosocial model of illness Behaviour, death and disease


The biopsychosocial model signals a broadening of a The dramatic increases in life expectancy witnessed in
disease or biomedical model of health to one encom- Western countries in the twentieth century, partially due to
passing and emphasising the interaction between body advances in medical technology and treatments, led to a
and mind, between biological processes and psychologi- general belief, in Western cultures at least, in the efficacy
cal and social influences (Engel 1977, 1980). In doing of traditional medicine and its power to eradicate disease.
so, it offers a complex and multivariate, but potentially This was most notable following the introduction of anti-
more comprehensive, model with which to examine the biotics in the 1940s (although Fleming discovered penicil-
human experience of illness. lin in 1928, it was some years before it and other antibiotics
As a result of the many challenges to the biomedical were generally available). Such drug treatments, alongside
approach described above, the biopsychosocial model is increased control of infectious disease through vaccination
employed in health psychology as well as in several allied and improved sanitation, are partial explanations of
health professions, such as occupational therapy. Although increases in life expectancy seen globally.
also increasingly assimilated within the medical profes- World Health Organization figures show that world-
sion (Turner et al. 2001; Wade and Halligan 2004), there wide the average life expectancy at birth is 71 years (68.5
exists some pessimism that it is feasible, no matter how for males, 73.5 for females), with significant and some-
valuable, given constraints facing our healthcare systems times shocking variation between countries. Table 1.1
(see editorial by Lane, 2014). Health is, however, more presents a selection from the top and bottom of the
than simply the absence of disease and therefore this text ‘league tables’. UK life expectancy at birth has increased
will illustrate that psychological, behavioural and social from 47 years in 1900 to 81 years in 2013, which is a
factors can add to the biological or biomedical explana- huge change in a relatively short period of time (WHO
tions and, rather than replacing these explanations of 2013). The most long-lived population is located in Japan
health and illness experiences, build on them. with a small gender difference only, whereas in Australia,
10 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

Table 1.1 Life expectancy in selected global countries

Overall (years) Male (years) Female (years) Rank

Japan 86.5 85.3 89.0 1


Australia 83.0 80.5 85.5 9
Sweden 83.0 81.4 84.6 10
Netherlands 81.5 79.5 83.5 20
UK 81.0 79.5 83.5 29
USA 79.8 77.4 82.2 35
Jamaica 74.8 71.5 78.2 105
Iraq 69.0 65.0 72.0 131
Afghanistan 60.0 59.0 61.0 164
Malawi 58.0 57.5 58.5 170
Somalia 50.0 48.0 52.0 190
Sierra Leone 47.5 47.0 48.0 193
Source: WHO (2013).

also a long-lived population, the gender differential is ations seen between Western, Eastern and central
5 years. At the other end of this ‘league table’ average regions, see Table 1.2 (which also includes 3 non- EU
life expectancy drops dramatically from the low–mid 70s members, Iceland, Norway, Sweden). The significant
through to a fairly horrendous overall life expectancy of decline in Ireland is attributed mainly to reductions in
47.5 years in Sierra Leone. Years of war account for deaths from cardiovascular and respiratory disease,
many of those figures at the low end of the table. which in turn may reflect improved living standards and
Such life expectancy at birth statistics tell us that in health-care investment. In countries where the decline is
some countries reaching a 60th birthday is not typical; fur- more modest, some, like Belgium, Greece and Sweden,
thermore country variations exist in the additional years of had lower rates to start with, whereas in Eastern Europe
life expected for those reaching the age of 60 between 2010 changes are slow and mortality rates remain higher.
and 2015 (UN study of Population Ageing and Development, The physical causes of death have changed dramati-
UN Dept of Economic & Social Affairs, 2013). cally also. If people living in 1900 had been asked what
These cultural variations can be explained to a large they thought being healthy meant, they may have replied,
extent by differences in lifestyle and diet, and the gender ‘avoiding infections, drinking clean water, living into my
differences seen perhaps also due to variations in health 50s/60s’. Death then frequently resulted from highly
behaviours (Chapter 3 ☛). There is some concern that, infectious disease such as pneumonia, influenza or tuber-
due to rising obesity in children and consequent health culosis becoming epidemic in communities unprotected
effects in adulthood, life expectancy may begin to show by immunisation or adequate sanitary conditions.
decreases in future generations, affecting disproportion- However, at least in developed countries over the last
ately developed countries such as the UK and the century, there has been a downturn in deaths resulting
USA which have high levels of obesity and inactivity from infectious disease, and the ‘league table’ make no
(Chapter 3 ☛). mention of tuberculosis (TB), typhoid, tetanus or measles.
Much of the fall in mortality seen in the developed In contrast, diseases such as heart, lung and respiratory
world preceded the major immunisation programmes and
likely reflects public health successes following wider
social and environmental changes over time. These mortality
include developments in education and agriculture, (death): generally presented as mortality statistics,
which led to changes in diet, or improvements in public i.e. the number of deaths in a given population
and/or in a given year ascribed to a given
hygiene and living standards (see also Chapter 2 ☛). condition (e.g. number of cancer deaths among
Mortality rates within the European Union have also women in 2000).
shown an overall 25 per cent reduction, with some vari-
What is health? Changing perspectives 11

Table 1.2 Decline in mortality rates from all causes, ● Diabetes mellitus (1.4 million)
1995–2010 (or nearest year) ● Road injury (1.3 million)
% decrease ● Prematurity (1.2 million)

Ireland 37 These figures show large geographic variation,


Estonia 35 explained in part by AIDS, which in many African and
Slovenia 33 Asian countries has a significant effect on life expec-
Portugal 31
tancy, possibly reducing it by as much as 15 years
Malta 30
Czech Republic 29
(WHO 2002a).
Luxembourg 28 With the exception of lung cancer, cancer does not
United Kingdom 27 appear in the top ten globally. Within more developed
Spain 27 countries, however, including Australia, USA, Europe,
Poland 27 cancer is consistently placed in the top five causes of
Germany 26
death. For example, recent England and Wales statistics
Italy 26
Denmark 26
(Office of National Statistics, 2014) show that 29 per
Hungary 26 cent of all deaths registered were as a result of cancer,
Finland 26 followed closely by circulatory diseases including heart
Netherlands 26 disease and stroke. For males, cancer was ‘top’ ranked
Austria 24 in 2013, whereas for females circulatory diseases were.
France 23 What is notable is that the leading causes of death
Latvia 23
have a behavioural component, linked, for example, to
Greece 20
Sweden 20 behaviour such as smoking, excessive alcohol con-
Belgium 19 sumption, sedentary lifestyles and poor diet/obesity
Lithuania 19 (Chapter 3 ☛). In fact, it has been estimated that up to
Bulgaria 17 three-quarters (Peto and Lopez 1990) of cancer deaths
Slovak Republic 16 are attributable, in part at least, to our behaviour. The
Iceland 29
upturn in cancer deaths seen over the last century is
Norway 24
Switzerland 23 also, however, due to people living longer with other
illnesses they previously would have died from, thus
Source: Adapted from Health at a Glance: Europe 2012, OECD.
they are reaching ages where cancer incidence is greater.
Projected worldwide mortality estimates placed heart
disease are the ‘biggest killers’ worldwide (along with disease, cerebrovascular disease including strokes,
‘accidents’). These causes have been relatively stable over chronic lung disease (COPD), lower respiratory infec-
the past few decades. The dementias are also attributed as tions, and throat and lung cancers as the top five killers
the cause of death in significant numbers, e.g. 1 in by 2020 (Murray and Lopez 1997), and certainly the
10 female deaths, 1 in 20 male deaths in 2011 in England WHO data above would suggest this projection is
and Wales (Office for National Statistics, 2012). proving reliable. These authors also predicted, using
­
Worldwide in 2011, the top ten leading causes of sophisticated statistical modelling, that, worldwide,
death (all ages) were recorded as: death from infectious diseases such as measles and
malaria, and from perinatal (birth) and nutritional
● Ischaemic heart disease (7 million)
● Stroke (6.2 million)
● Lower respiratory infection (3.2 million) incidence
● COPD (3 million) the number of new cases of disease occurring
during a defined time interval – not to be confused
● Diarrhoeal diseases (1.9 million) with prevalence, which refers to the number of
● HIV/AIDS(1.6 million) established cases of a disease in a population at
any one time.
● Lung diseases (including cancer) (1.5 million)
12 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

d­iseases, will significantly decline, whereas tobacco- Most countries are seeking, in what is known as the
related diseases will increase almost three-fold. ‘post 2015 development agenda’, to better measure their
Encouragingly, UK statistics point to a decline (42 per populations health and well-being, given the changing
cent for men, 40 per cent for women) in age-standard- nature of disease (from acute infectious disease to chronic
ised deaths from circulatory (heart) diseases over the disease) and the population (an ageing one). Within the
past 15–20 years and a lower but significant (13 per cent EU, targets have been recently set to gain an additional
for females, 15 per cent for males) fall for cancer and for two ‘healthy life years’ in everyone living in their mem-
respiratory disease, (26 per cent for males, 20 per cent ber states, with recognition that health is not simply about
for females) (Office for National Statistics 2010). the absence of disease. Healthy life expectancy and sub-
So, if as a reader you have been asking yourself, ‘why jective well-being are seen as key (WHO Report of a
do all these figures matter?’ the answer should now be Technical Meeting, Geneva 10–11 December, 2012).
clear! They matter because they demonstrate that our
own behaviour contributes significantly to our health
and mortality. As health psychologists, gaining an
understanding of why we behave as we do and how
behaviour can change or be changed, is a core part of our
Individual, cultural and
remit and therefore is something we discuss a lot in this
and in the subsequent six chapters! Such links between
lifespan perspectives
individual behaviour, health and illness provide a key
reason as to why health psychology has grown rapidly.
on health
It is not therefore surprising, given evidence of the
changes in what people are dying from, that views of
Lay theories of health
what health is has also changed over time. In the eight- If a fuller understanding of health and illness is to be
eenth century, health was considered an ‘egalitarian attained, it is necessary to find out what people think
ideal’, aspired to by all. Health was considered as poten- health and illness are. The simplest way of doing this is
tially being under an individual’s control; however, doc- to ask them. Here we explore lay perceptions of health.
tors were available to the wealthy as ‘aids’ to keeping In response to the question ‘What does being healthy
oneself well, but less so to the poor. By the mid-­ mean?’ a classic early study by Bauman (1961) found
twentieth century, accompanied or perhaps preceded by that people with diagnoses of quite serious illness made
new laws regarding sickness benefit, and medical and three main types of response whereby being healthy was
technological advances in diagnostic and treatment pro- considered:
cedures, health became increasingly and inextricably
1. a ‘general sense of well-being’;
linked to ‘fitness to work’. Doctors were required to
declare whether individuals were ‘fit to work’ or whether 2. identified with ‘the absence of symptoms of disease’;
they could adopt the ‘sick role’ (see also Chapter 10 ☛). 3. seen in ‘the things that a person who is physically fit
Many today continue to see illness in terms of its effects is able to do’.
on their working lives. However there is also evidence
She argued that these three types of response reveal health
of the opposite direction of effects, i.e. the influence
to be related to:
work role and conditions have on illness (see discussion
of occupational stress in Chapter 11 ☛). ● feeling
Also perhaps changing over time is the assumption that ● symptom orientation
traditional medicine can, and will, cure us of all ills. Over
● performance.
recent decades, many more people have acknowledged the
potential negative consequences of some treatments, Respondents in this study did not answer in discrete
­particularly pharmacological ones (consider for example categories however, with nearly half of the sample pro-
long-term use of anxiolytics such as Valium), and as a viding two of the above response types, and 12 per cent
result the ‘complementary’ and ‘alternative’ medicine using all three types. This highlights the fact that the way
industry has burgeoned. we think about health is often multifaceted.
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 13

Furthermore, Bauman’s sample consisted of those public, of whom 5,352 also completed a­ ssessment seven
with quite serious illness, yet we now know that cur- years later (Cox, Huppert and Whichellow, 1993). This
rent health status influences subjective views of survey asked respondents to:
health and reports of what ‘health is’. For example,
● Think of someone you know who is very healthy.
amongst almost 500 elderly people asked to rate fac-
tors in order of importance to their subjective health
● Define who you are thinking of (friend/relative etc. –
judgements, the most important factors emerging do not need specific name).
related to physical functioning and vitality (being able ● Note how old they are.
to do what you need/want to do). However, the cur- ● Consider what makes you call them healthy.
rent health status of the sample (poor/fair; good; very ● Consider what it is like when you are healthy.
good/excellent) influenced judgements; for example,
those in poor/fair health based their health assessment About 15 per cent could not think of anyone who was
on recent symptoms or indicators of poor health, ‘very healthy’, and about 10 per cent could not describe
whereas those in good health considered more ­positive what it was like for them to ‘feel healthy’. This inability
indicators (being able to exercise, being happy). to describe what it is like to feel healthy was particularly
Consistent with this, subjective health judgements evident in young males, who believed health to be a
were more tied to health behaviour in ‘healthier’ norm, a background condition so taken for granted that
individuals (Benyamini et al., 2003). they could not put it into words. By comparison, a
Although some people have been shown to find it smaller group of mostly older women could not answer
hard to distinguish health from an absence of illness, for exactly the opposite reason – they had been in poor
health is generally viewed as a state of equilibrium health for so long that either they could not remember
across various aspects of the person, encompassing what it was like to feel well or they were expressing a
physical, psychological, emotional and social well- pessimism about their condition to the interviewer
being (e.g. Herzlich, 1973). Bennett (2000: 67) consid- (Radley 1994: 39).
ers these representations of health to distinguish The categories of health identified from the survey
between health as ‘being’, i.e. if not ill, then healthy; findings were:
‘having’, i.e. health as a positive resource or reserve; ● Health as not ill: i.e. no symptoms, no visits to doctor,
and ‘doing’, i.e. health as represented by physical fit-
therefore I am healthy.
ness or function (as seen in Benyamini et al.’s study
above). Bauman’s respondents appear to have focused
● Health as reserve: i.e. come from strong family;
more on the ‘being’ healthy and ‘doing’ aspects, which recovered quickly from operation.
may be in part because ‘having’ health as a resource ● Health as behaviour: i.e. usually applied to others
was not prominent in the minds of her patient sample. rather than self; e.g. they are healthy because they
Similarly, Krause and Jay (1994) found that older look after themselves, exercise, etc.
respondents more often referred to health problems ● Health as physical fitness and vitality: used more often
when making their appraisals, whereas younger by younger respondents and often in reference to a
respondents referred to health behaviour. The frames of male – male health concept more commonly tied to
reference drawn on by people asked to evaluate their ‘feeling fit’, whereas females had a concept of ‘feeling
own health status therefore also differ. full of energy’ and rooted health more in the social
It does seem that health is considered differently world in terms of being lively and having good
when it is no longer present, i.e. it is considered to be ­relationships with others.
good when nothing is wrong (perhaps more commonly
thought in older people) and when a person is behaving
in a health-protective manner (perhaps more commonly health behaviour
thought in younger people). behaviour performed by an individual, regardless
A more representative picture of the health concept is of their health status, as a means of protecting,
perhaps obtained from a large, questionnaire-based Health promoting or maintaining health, e.g. diet.
and Lifestyles Survey of 9,003 members of the general
14 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

● Health as psychosocial well-being: health defined in will tend to perceive their peers as generally healthy, so
terms of a person’s mental state; e.g. being in ­harmony, if they feel that they are not, they will be less likely to
feeling proud, or, more specifically, enjoying others. draw this comparison. In contrast, older people when in
● Health as function: the idea of health as the ability to poorer health are more likely to compare themselves
perform one’s duties; i.e. being able to do what you with same-aged peers, who may generally be thought to
want when you want without being handicapped in have normatively poorer health (thus their own health
any way by ill health or physical limitation (relates to status seems less unusual). Asking a person to consider
the World Health Organization’s concept of handi- what it is that they would consider as ‘being healthy’
cap, now described as participation/participatory inevitably will lead people into making these types of
restriction, see ‘In the spotlight’: i.e. an inability to comparison. Health is a relative state of being.
fulfil one’s ‘normal’ social roles).

Such findings suggest that health concepts are perhaps


even more complex than initially thought, with evidence
World Health Organization
that the presence of health is considered as something definition of health
more than physical, i.e. as something encompassing psy- The dimensions of health described in the preceding
chosocial well-being. Categories found seem to fit with paragraphs are reflected in the WHO (1947) definition
Herzlich’s ‘being’ and ‘doing’ categorisations (see Bennett of health as a ‘state of complete physical, mental and
2000: 66) and Bauman’s findings of clusters of beliefs in social well-being and. . . not merely the absence of dis-
‘health as not ill’. Generally, we can conclude that these ease or infirmity’. This definition saw individuals as
dimensions of health are fairly robust (at least in Western ideally deserving of a positive state, an overall feeling
culture; see later section for culture differences). of well-being, fully functioning. This standpoint
It is worth noting that subjective well-being ratings informed and helped shape global health targets, includ-
have been found to correlate strongly with objective ing their own Global Strategy for Health for All by the
health indicators (e.g. blood pressure and heart rate; Year 2000 (WHO 1981) and in 1998 the ‘Health for all
Steptoe et al. (2012), English Longitudinal Study of in the 21st Century’ declarations. Each of these had the
Ageing) and also with wealth and educational levels aim of securing health security for all, global health
(White 2007). We discuss the ‘well-being’ concept more equity, increased life expectancy and access for all to
fully in relation to quality of life in Chapter 14 ☛ and essential health care. Many national policy documents
note that health is only one component of these typically followed, with the nature, specificity and time-frame of
self-rated concepts. What is relevant here, however, is targets varying from country to country. In general,
that subjective evaluations are typically reached through however, these set targets for reductions in deaths from
comparison with others, and in this way one’s concept of the leading causes of cancers, heart disease, lung dis-
what health is, or is not, can be shaped. For example, ease, strokes, or more explicitly targeted the associated
Kaplan and Baron-Epel (2003) found that young Israelis behaviours. For example, in England The Health of the
reporting suboptimal health did not compare themselves Nation White Paper,(Department of Health 1992) and
with people of the same age, whereas many older people the Saving Lives: Our Healthier Nation report,
in suboptimal health did. When in optimal health, more (Department of Health 1999) and in the Netherlands
young people than old compared themselves with people (Langer Gezond Leven – ‘Towards a Longer and
their age. This is interpreted as evidence that people try Healthier Life’, Ministry of Health 2003) the targets
to get the best out of their evaluations – a young person were disease incidence reductions, whereas in Belgium
the targets were more behavioural: reducing smoking
psychosocial behaviour, fat intake, fatal accidents, increasing uptake
of vaccination programmes and increasing health
an approach that seeks to merge a psychological
(more micro- and individually oriented) approach screening in the over-50s. Some progress has been
with a social approach (macro-, more community- made. For example, reductions have been seen in mor-
and interaction-oriented), for example, to health. tality in developed countries from lung, colon and pros-
tate cancers in men, and breast and colorectal cancers in
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 15

women, and uptake of smoking is showing reductions the family (Ahmad 2000; Mackenzie 2006; Moriarty
amongst young males. et al. 2011). Such beliefs can influence disclosure of
Some have questioned whether the WHO use of the symptoms and health-seeking behaviour (Vaughn et al.
term ‘complete’ in relation to physical well-being is 2009, and see Chapter 9, ☛).
unrealistic given the changing population age and preva- Westernised views of health differ in various ways
lence of chronic disease and the likelihood that most of from conceptualisations of health in non-Westernised
us will have some symptomatology as we age (Huber civilisations. Chalmers (1996) astutely notes that
et al. 2011). What is clear, however, is that health policy, Westerners divide the mind, body and soul in terms of
however health is defined, acknowledges the evidenced allocation of care between psychologists and psychia-
relationship between people’s behaviour, lifestyles and trists, medical professions and the clergy, whereas in
their health. What they less clearly acknowledge and some African cultures, these three ‘elements of human
therefore often fail to explicitly address are the socio- nature’ are integrated in terms of how a person views
economic and cultural influences on health, illness and them, and in how they are cared for. This holistic
health decisions. These important influences are view is similar to that found in Eastern and in
addressed in the next chapter (see Chapter Two, ☛). Aboriginal Australian cultures (e.g. Swami et al.
Other definitions of health do exist, for example 2009) where the social (e.g. social and community
Bircher (2005) describes a more context-aware view of norms and rituals) as well as the biological, the spir-
health, defining it as ‘a dynamic state of well-being char- itual and the interpersonal, are integral to explaining
acterized by a physical and mental potential, which sat- health and illness states.
isfies the demands of life commensurate with age, Spiritual well-being as an aspect of health has gained
culture, and personal responsibility’. Bircher’s view credence following inclusion in many quality of life
places the individual centrally in the experience of assessments (see Chapter 14 ☛), and, although faith or
health and illness whereas the WHO definition does not. God’s reward may sometimes be perceived as support-
Individual beliefs plays a major role in the experience of ing health, attributing one’s health to a satisfied ancestor
health and illness, and, as we discuss earlier, disability. may nonetheless raise a few eyebrows if stated aloud.
(see ‘In the spotlight’). The fascination for many health Negative supernatural forces such as ‘hexes’ or the ‘evil
psychologists, quite simply, is why do some people eye’ sometimes share the blame for illness and disabil-
respond in one way to health changes, illness or disabil- ity: for example, Jobanputra and Furnham (2005) found
ity, and others respond quite differently. that, when compared with British Caucasians, British
Gujarati Indian immigrants more often endorsed such
causes of illness. Among Hindus and Sikhs, in particu-
Cross-cultural perspectives lar, it has been reported that disability, and even demen-
tia may be considered a punishment for past sins within
on health the family (Katbamna et al. 2000; Mackenzie 2006).
What is considered to be ‘normal’ health varies across Such belief systems can have profound effects on living
cultures and as a result of the economic, political and with illness or, indeed, caring for someone with an ill-
cultural climate of the era in which a person lives. ness or disability (see Chapter 15 ☛).
Cultures vary in their health belief systems, health attri- In addition to beliefs of spiritual influences on
butions and health practices. Think of how pregnancy is health, studies of some African regions consider that the
treated in most Western civilisations (i.e. medicalised) ­community or family work together for the well-being
as opposed to many developing regions (naturalised).
The stigma of physical disability, mental illness, or of
dementia, among South Asian communities, and some holistic
Eastern European groups, may have consequences for root word ‘wholeness’; holistic approaches are
the family which would not be considered in Caucasian concerned with the whole being and its well-
families: for example, having a sibling with a disability, being, rather than addressing the purely physical
or a relative with dementia or depression, may affect or observable.
siblings’ marriage chances or even the social standing of
16 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

of all. This collectivist approach to staying healthy and misfortune is c­ommonly attributed to predestination;
avoiding illness is far different from our individualistic African Americans and Latinos are more likely than
approach to health (consider how long the passive White Americans to attribute illness causes externally
smoking evidence was ignored). Generally speaking, (e.g. to the will of God) (e.g. Vaughn et al. 2009).
Western European cultures are found to be more indi- Differences in collectivist values have also been
vidualistic, with Eastern and African cultures exhibiting shown in a study of 50 states within the USA (Vandello
more holistic and collectivist approaches to health. For and Cohen 1999). In Hawaii and the Deep South states
example, in a study of preventive behaviour to avoid (e.g. Louisiana, Mississippi, Georgia) and New York
endemic tropical disease in Malawians, the social State, there was greater collectivism than in the Midwest
actions to prevent infection (e.g. clearing reed beds) (e.g. Michigan, Ohio) or North East States (e.g. Maine,
were adhered to more consistently than the personal Massachusetts). Analyses found this to be associated
preventive actions (e.g. bathing in piped water or taking with a higher percentage of ethnic minorities, whereby
one’s dose of chloroquine) (Morrison et al. 1999). States with a higher population percentage of whites of
Collectivist cultures emphasise group needs and find a European ancestry were the most individualistic group,
meaning through links with others and one’s commu- and those with higher proportions of Asians and Blacks
nity to a greater degree than individualistic ones, which had the highest collectivism levels. Adding in figures
emphasise the uniqueness and autonomy of its members associated with former slavery made this correlation
i.e. promote and validate “independent selfs” (Morrison even stronger. Collectivism also correlated with
et al. 1999: 367). poverty.
Cultures that promote an interdependent self are Clearly, therefore, to maximise effectiveness of
more likely to view health in terms of social functioning health promotion efforts, it is important to acknowledge
rather than simply personal functioning, fitness, etc. For the existence and effects of such different underlying
example studies by George Bishop and colleagues (e.g. belief systems and resultant behaviours (see Chapters 6
Bishop and Teng 1992; Quah and Bishop 1996) have and 7 ☛). It is also worth noting that variations exist
noted that Chinese Singaporean adults view health as a within, not just between, cultures, especially where there
harmonious state where the internal and external sys- may have been exposure to multiple cultural influences
tems are in balance, and, on occasions where they (Tov and Diener 2007). This is also reported by Wong et
become imbalanced, health is compromised. Yin – the al. (2011) from studies in in Singapore where both Asian
positive energy – needs to be kept in balance with the and Western influences coexist but have differential
yang – the negative energy (also considered to be effects on subjective well-being ratings.
female!). Other Asian cultures, e.g. the Vietnamese, use In the Western world, the perceived value of alterna-
mystical beliefs relating to maintaining balance between tive remedies for health maintenance or treatment of
poles of ‘hot’ and ‘cold’. In Eastern cultures illness or symptoms is seen in the growth of alternative medicine
and complementary therapy industries; however,
Western medicine dominates. In contrast, in non-West-
collectivist ern countries a mixture of Western and non-medical/
a cultural philosophy that emphasises the traditional medicine can be found. For example in sub-
individual as part of a wider unit and places Saharan Malawi, a person may visit a faith healer or a
emphasis on duties above rights, with actions
herbalist as well as a local Western clinic for antibiotics
motivated by interconnectedness, reciprocity and
group membership, rather than individual needs (Ager et al. 1996), and in Malaysia, while Western-style
and wants. medicine is dominant, traditional medicine practice by
‘bomohs’ (faith healers) is still available (Swami et al.
individualistic
2009). Similarly, among some Aboriginal tribes spiritual
a cultural philosophy that places responsibility at
beliefs in illness causation coexist with the use of
the feet of the individual and emphasises rights
above duties; thus behaviour is often driven by Western medicines for symptom control (Devanesen
individual needs and wants rather than by 2000), with traditional medicine and healing processes
community needs or wants. consistent with cultural and spiritual beliefs used by
some still in the treatment of cancer (Shahid et al. 2010).
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 17

will. Perhaps reflecting this latter shift, the prevalence of


smoking behaviour is showing some decrease.
Furthermore, what is normal (or deviant) and what is
defined as sick (reflecting illness) in a given culture can
have consequences for how others respond: consider
how societal responses to illicit drug use have ranged
from prohibition through criminalisation to an illness
requiring treatment (see Chapter 3 ☛).

Lifespan, ageing and beliefs


about health and illness
Psychological well-being, social and emotional health
are affected by illness, disability and hospitalisation,
Photo 1.2 Visiting a herbalist to choose individually tailored
remedies. which can be experienced at any age. Growing older may
be associated with decreased functioning and increased
Source: Alamy Images/Corbis Premium RF.
disability or dependence; however, it is not of course
These examples illustrate that the biomedical view is only older people who live with chronic illness: think of
acknowledged and assimilated within different culture’s childhood asthma, epilepsy and diabetes, for example.
belief systems, and show that, whilst access to and There are developmental issues which health profes-
understanding of Western medicine and its methods and sionals should be aware of if they are to promote the
efficacy grows, better understanding of culturally rele- physical, psychological, social and emotional well-being
vant cognitions regarding illness and health behaviour is of their patient or client. While the subsequent section
needed (see Kitayama and Cohen 2007; Vaughn et al. introduces lifespan issues in relation to health percep-
2009). We need more research which considers the role tions, it is recommended that interested readers also
religion plays in health across and within cultures. consult a developmental health psychology text, e.g.
Swami et al. (2009) for example, in their study of 721 Resnick and Rozensky’s (1997) edited collection or the
Malaysian adults, found that Muslim participants had newer text by Turner-Cobb (2014).
higher beliefs in religious factors and fate as influences
on recovering from illness than did Buddhist or Catholic Developmental theories
participants and they were also more likely to believe
that their likelihood of becoming ill was uncontrollable. The developmental process is a function of the interac-
As we will discuss in a later chapter (Chapter 9 ☛), tion between three factors:
the use of health care, either traditional or Western, will in 1. Learning: a relatively permanent change in knowledge,
part be determined by the nature and strength of such skill or ability as a result of experience.
cultural values and religious beliefs. Illness discourse will
2. Experience: what we do, see, hear, feel, think.
reflect the dominant conceptualisations of individual cul-
tures and religions, and, in turn, how people think about 3. Maturation: thought, behaviour or physical growth,
health and illness will shape expectations, behaviour, and attributed to a genetically determined sequence of
use of health promotion and health-care resources. development and ageing rather than to experience.
The way in which certain behaviour is viewed also Erik Erikson (Erikson 1959; Erikson et al. 1986)
differs across time. For example, alcohol dependence described eight major life stages (five related to child-
has shifted from being viewed as a legal and moral prob- hood development, three related to adult development),
lem, with abusers seen as deviant, to a disease, with its which varied across different dimensions, including:
patients treated in clinics; and smoking has shifted from
being considered as glamorous, even desirable, to being ● cognitive and intellectual functioning;
socially undesirable and considered indicative of a weak ● language and communication skills;
18 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

● the understanding of illness; 4. Formal operational (age 12 to adulthood): abstract


● health care and maintenance behaviour. thought and imagination develop as does deductive
reasoning. Not everyone may attain this level.
Each of these dimensions is important when examin-
ing health and illness perceptions or behaviour. Deficits Piaget’s work has been influential in terms of provid-
or limitations in cognitive functioning (due to age, acci- ing an overarching structure within which to view cogni-
dent or illness) may, for example, influence the extent tive development. Of more relevance to a health
to which an individual can understand medical instruc- psychology text, however, is work that more specifically
tions, report their emotions or have their health-care addresses children’s developing beliefs and understand-
needs assessed. Communication deficits or limited lan- ing of health and illness constructs. We describe some of
guage skills can impair a person’s willingness to place this work now, using Piagetian stages as a broad
themselves in social situations, or impede their ability framework.
to express their pain or distress to health professionals
or family members. The understanding an individual Sensorimotor and preoperational
has of their symptoms or their illness is crucial to stage children
health-care-seeking behaviour and to adherence, and
individual health behaviour influences one’s perceived Little work with infants at the sensorimotor stage is pos-
and/or actual risk of illness and varies hugely across the sible in terms of identifying health and illness cogni-
lifespan. All these aspects are covered in this textbook tions, as language is very limited until the end of this
in the relevant chapters. We cannot, for example, stage. At the preoperational stage, children develop lin-
assume that explanations or models of adult behaviour guistically and cognitively, and symbolic thought means
or adult decision-making can be applied to children, that they develop awareness of how they can affect the
given normative cognitive development, or to adoles- external world through imitation and learning, although
cents, given variations in the salience of social influence they remain very egocentric. In preoperational children,
(Holmbeck 2002). health and illness are considered in black and white, i.e.
A maturational framework for understanding cogni- as two opposing states rather than as existing on a con-
tive development (Piaget, 1930, 1970) has provided a tinuum. Children are slow to see or adopt other people’s
good basis for understanding the developmental course viewpoints or perspectives, which is crucial if one is to
of concepts regarding health, illness and health proce- empathise with others. Thus a preoperational child is not
dures. Piaget proposed a staged structure to which, he very sympathetic to an ill family member, not under-
considered, all individuals follow in sequence as below: standing why this might mean they receive less
attention.
1. Sensorimotor (birth–2 years): an infant understands
the world through sensations and movement, but Illness concept
lacks symbolic thought and moves from reflexive to It is important that children learn over time some respon-
voluntary action. sibility for maintaining their own health; however, few
studies have examined children’s conception of health
2. Preoperational (2–7 years): symbolic thought devel-
which would be likely to influence health behaviour.
ops by around age 2, thereafter simple logical think-
Research has focused more often on generating illness
ing and language develop, generally egocentric.
concepts. For example, Bibace and Walsh (1980) asked
3. Concrete operational (7–11 years): abstract thought children aged 3–13 questions about health and illness, and
and logic develop hugely; can perform mental opera- suggested that an illness concept develops gradually. The
tions (e.g. mental arithmetic) and manipulate objects. questions were about knowledge – ‘What is a cold?’;
experience – ‘Were you ever sick?’; attributions – ‘How
egocentric does someone get a cold?’; and recovery – ‘How does
self-centred, such as in the preoperational stage someone get better?’ Responses revealed a progression
(age 2–7 years) of children, when they see things of understanding and attribution for causes of illness,
only from their own perspective (cf. Piaget). and six developmentally ordered descriptions of how
illness is defined, caused and treated emerged.
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 19

Under-7s generally explain illness on a ‘magical’ in development which can help the child to cope. They
level – explanations are based on association: also need to be encouraged to express their fears. Parents
need to strike a balance between monitoring a sick
● Incomprehension: child gives irrelevant answers or
child’s health and behaviour and being overprotective, as
evades question: e.g. sun causes heart attacks.
this can detrimentally affect a child’s social, cognitive
● Phenomenonism: illness is usually a sign or sound and personal development and may encourage feelings of
that the child has at some time associated with the dependency and disability (see Chapter 15 ☛ for further
illness, but with little grasp of cause and effect: e.g. a discussion of coping with illness in a family).
cold is when you sniff a lot.
● Contagion: illness is usually from a person or object
that is close by, but not necessarily touching the child;
Adolescence and formal
or it can be attributed to an activity that occurred operational thought
before the illness: e.g: ‘You get measles from people’. Adolescence is a socially and culturally created concept
If asked how? ‘Just by walking near them’. only a few generations old, and indeed many primitive
societies do not acknowledge adolescence, and instead
children move from childhood to adulthood with a ritual
Concrete operational stage children
performance rather than the years of transition Western
Children over 7 are described by Piaget as capable of societies consider a distinct period in life. Puberty is a
thinking logically about objects and events, although period of both physical and psychosocial change.
they are still unable to distinguish between mind and During early adolescence (11–13 years), as individuals
body until around age 11, when adolescence begins. prepare for increased autonomy, independence and
peers take on more credence than parents, much of life’s
Illness concept
health-damaging behaviour commences, e.g. smoking
Bibace and Walsh describe explanations of illness at
(see Chapter 3 ☛).
around 8 to 11 years as being more concrete and based
on a causal sequence: Illness concept
Bibace and Walsh describe illness concepts at this stage
● Contamination: i.e. children at this stage understand
as being at an abstract level, based on interactions
that illness can have multiple symptoms, and they
between the person and their environment:
recognise that germs, or even their own behaviour,
can cause illness: e.g. ‘You get a cold if you get ● Physiological: children now reach a stage of physio-
sneezed on, and it gets into your body’. logical understanding where most can define illness
● Internalisation: i.e. illness is within the body, and the in terms of specific bodily organs or functions (e.g.
process by which symptoms occur can be partially germs cause white blood cells to get active to try and
understood. The cause of a cold may come from out- fight them), and begin to appreciate multiple physical
side germs that are inhaled or swallowed and then causes, e.g. genes plus pollution plus behaviour.
enter the bloodstream. These children can differenti- ● Psychophysiological: in later adolescence (from
ate between body organs and function and can around 14 years) and in adulthood, many people
­understand specific, simple information about their grasp the idea that mind and body interact, and under-
illness. They can also see the role of treatment and/or stand or accept the role of stress, worry, etc. in the
personal action as returning them to health. exacerbation and even the cause of illness. However,
many people of all ages fail to achieve this level of
In this concrete operational stage, medical staff are
understanding about illness and continue to use more
still seen as having absolute authority, but their actions
cognitively simplistic explanations.
might be criticised/avoided: e.g. reluctance to give blood,
accusations of hurting unnecessarily, etc. may appear as It should be noted that Bibace and Walsh’s study
children can now begin to weigh up the pros and cons of focuses predominantly on the issue of illness causality,
actions. Children can be encouraged to take some per- and these findings have been supported by more recent
sonal control over their illness or treatment at this stage work of Koopman and colleagues (Koopman et al. 2004).
20 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

Extending illness cognitions further, other work has


shown that children and young people are able to think
about health and illness in terms of other dimensions,
such as controllability and severity (e.g. Forrest et al.
2006; Gray and Rutter 2007; see Chapter 9 for fuller
discussion of illness perceptions ☛).
Adolescents perceive more personal control over the
onset and course of illness and are more aware that their
actions can influence outcomes. Advice and interven-
tions are more fully understood as are complex remedial
and therapeutic procedures: e.g. they understand that
taking blood can help monitor the progress of a disease
or a treatment. They may, however, choose to be non-­
adherent if treatment is thought to disrupt one’s goals or Photo 1.3 Which of these are healthy? You can’t always tell by
lose peer approval, and efforts to minimise a child’s looking. Neither would you know by looking which of these rated
autonomy (from pre-adolescence) can be counterpro- themselves as ‘extremely healthy’ would you? Health is more
than objective symptoms.
ductive (Holmbeck et al. 2002).
Overall, childhood sees the development of health and Source: Fotolia.com/Robert Kneschke.

illness concepts and of attitudes and patterns of health


associated with sports participation and p­hysiological
behaviour which impact on the person’s future health sta-
functioning, whereas in younger children it was more
tus (see Chapter 3 ☛). How children communicate their
related to ‘going outside’. Older children also consid-
symptom experience to parents and health-care staff, their
ered ‘not being sick’ as more important. In terms of
ability to act on health advice, and the level of personal
lifestyle behaviour, older children more often referred
responsibility for disease management taken is, according
to good diet than did younger children; and in terms of
to such staged theories, determined by the level of cogni-
mental health, older children more often referred to
tive development attained. This approach has not met with
self-concept, whereas younger children referred more
universal support whereby illness concepts are thought to
to the quality of relationships with others. No effects
derive from a range of influences, such as past experience
were found in terms of gender or socio-economic back-
and knowledge, rather than from relatively fixed stages of
ground. In the context of this section on life stage and
cognitive development. Illustrating this point, a large
health concept, the important finding is that children as
questionnaire and interview-based study with 1,674
young as 5 elicited multidimensional concepts of
Canadian children aged 5–12 years old (Normandeau et al.
health that were more complex than suggested by a
1998) asked children to consider health in terms of their
shift from concrete to abstract thinking as described by
daily experiences (what signified good health in their
stage theorists. Very early on, children’s conceptions
friends; what behaviours are necessary to be healthy; what
included a mental health dimension, which is contrary
are the consequences of being healthy, and what things are
to that found in early research. Perhaps the methodol-
dangerous to health). The children generally identified
ogy of inviting children to talk about their concepts in
three main criteria for good health:
relation to their own lives and experience as opposed to
1. being functional (practising sports, absence of disease); more hypothetical questioning, accounts for this
difference.
2. mental health (well-being, looking healthy, feeling
good about oneself, good relationships with
others); Adulthood 17/18
3. lifestyle health behaviour (healthy diet, good hygiene,
Adulthood tends to be divided between early (17–40),
sleeping well).
middle age (40–60) and elderly (60/65). Early adulthood
Age influenced some components of these d­ imensions: blends out of adolescence as the person forges their
for example, functionality in older children was more identity and assumes the roles and responsibility of
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 21

adulthood – a time of consolidation. In contrast to the Ageing and health


years from 3–13 which Laslett (1996) describes as the
‘1st age’, where dependency, childhood and education In the UK, as elsewhere in the world, the ageing popula-
are key, adolescence and adulthood is considered as the tion (accepting the cut-off age for ‘older people’ to be 60
‘2nd age’, a period of developing independence, matu- or over) has burgeoned, but more particularly the per-
rity and responsibility. Early adulthood typically sees all centage of persons living into their late 70s or 80s has
sorts of transitions, such as graduating from school and increased and is projected to increase further. Worldwide
college, taking on new careers, pregnancy, marriage, 11.7 per cent of the population are aged over 60 years
childbirth; many will divorce, some will lose a parent. (compared to 8.6 per cent in 1980); 7 per cent over
Although Piaget did not describe further cognitive 65 years, and 1.7 per cent are over 80 years old – this
developments during adulthood, new perspectives latter percentage translates to 120,199,000 individuals
develop from experience across the lifespan, and what is (>120 million) (United Nations 2013). A worldwide
learned is ideally applied to achieving future life goals. increase is expected in the proportion of the populations
Adults are less likely than adolescents to adopt new aged over 65 from 5.2 % in 1950 to 10 per cent in 2025.
health-risk behaviour and are generally more likely to Within the UK, projections are that the number of peo-
engage in protective behaviour: e.g. screening, exercise, ple aged 60 or older will rise from 22 per cent at present
etc. for health reasons (see Chapter 4 ☛). Transitions (Age Concern 2010, based on National Population
in adulthood do not affect all sectors of the adult popula- Projections, 2008-based, Office for National Statistics
tion in the same way: for example, marriage was found 2009) to almost a third of the total population (31 per
to benefit health in men – i.e. they have lower illness cent) by 2058. Globally the United Nations predict a
scores than men living alone, whereas for women, being 7.6 per cent increase in those aged 60+ by 2050, with
married carries no such protection (Macintyre 1986; older people outnumbering children by 2047 and repre-
Blaxter 1987), suggesting differential social support senting half of those defined as ‘dependants’ by 2075.
perhaps (see Chapter 12 ☛ for a discussion of stress The shift in proportions of older persons living in our
moderators). However changes in the work force, with society is underpinned by many factors including
more married women now also working than was the reduced birth numbers/slower population growth, as
case in the 1980s when these studies were conducted, well as the reasons associated with longer life expec-
mean that such findings may not persist. tancy cited earlier. The implications for health and social
In contrast to generally positive views of early care resources are obvious, given the epidemiology of
adulthood, middle age has been identified as a period illness: i.e. the fact that the incidence of many diseases
of doubts and anxiety, reappraisal and change, some of increases with longevity.
it triggered by uncertainty of roles when children Laslett (1996) describes the ‘3rd age’ of those aged
become adults and leave home, i.e. ‘the empty nest’ 65+ as a period of fulfilment as not all of course become
syndrome, some of it triggered by awareness of physi- ill or infirm at this point! The English Longitudinal
cal changes – greying hair, weight gain, stiff joints, Study of Ageing (see also Chapter 14 ☛, Steptoe et al.
etc. Positive health behaviour changes may follow (see 2012/13), highlights that increased risk of dying prema-
Chapter 3 and 4). turely is associated with poorer enjoyment of life, thus
highlighting the role of subjective well-being, what-
ever the age. The ‘4th age’, however, is more strongly
What do you think?
Is middle age a state of mind? Are you ‘as young
as you feel’? epidemiology
the study of patterns of disease in various
Think of your parents, aunts and uncles or of family
populations and the association with other factors
friends in their 40s. Do they seem to share outlooks
such as lifestyle factors. Key concepts include
on life, expectancies and behaviours that are sig-
mortality, morbidity, prevalence, incidence,
nificantly different to those of you and your friends?
absolute risk and relative risk. Type of question:
How do you view growing older? Think about how
Who gets this disease? How common is it?
it makes you feel and question these feelings.
22 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

associated with disability and dependence, relating to all categorised or dichotimised: e.g. presence/absence of
the ‘oldest old’ those aged over 80 where health does diagnosis; sense of purpose/no sense of purpose, etc., in
decline more rapidly. order for each model to identify whether a person was
In an ageing society disability is common; 85 per cent ‘successfully aged’ or not:
may experience some chronic condition (Woods 2008),
Biomedical model: based on physical and psychiatric
with the main problems being associated with memory
functioning – diagnoses and functional ability.
loss, incontinence, depression, falls or immobility (UN
2013). Does the process of ageing influence how an Broader biomedical model: as above but includes so-
older person thinks about themselves and their health? cial engagement and activity.
Empirical research has shown that self-concept is Social functioning model: based on the nature and
relatively stable through ageing (e.g. Baltes and Baltes frequency of social functioning and networks, social
1990; Coleman 1999) and that changes in self-concept support accessed.
are not an inevitable part of the ageing process. Whilst Psychological resources model: based on personal
growing older may present an individual with new chal- characteristics of optimism and self-efficacy and on
lenges, this should not be seen as implying that ageing is sense of purpose, coping and problem solving, self-
itself a problem, in spite of the ageist attitudes that exist confidence and self-worth (see Chapter 12 ☛ for a
in many industrialised countries. discussion of many of these positive cognitions).
The elderly often report expecting to have poor
Lay model: based on the above variables plus socio-
health. Such expectations can result in poor health-care
economic variables of income and ‘perceived social
checks and maintenance as they regard health protective
capital’, which included access to resources and facil-
behaviour as pointless. They may think loss of mobility,
ities, environmental quality and problems (e.g. crime,
poor foot health and poor digestion is an inevitable and
traffic, pollution, places to walk, feelings of safety).
unavoidable part of ageing and so may not respond to
symptoms as they should (e.g. Sarkisian et al. 2001). The study assessed all the above variables in a sample
Exercise tends to decline in old age in the belief that it of 999 individuals aged over 65 years and assigned them
will overexert the joints, heart, etc. In fact, the elderly either as successfully aged or not based on achieving the
tend to underestimate their own physical capacities, yet ‘good’ score on each variable, e.g. no physical conditions
as we shall see in Chapter 4 ☛, exercise is both possi- versus one or more. The authors then tested which of
ble and beneficial. Even in the face of ‘objective’ signs these models ‘best’ distinguished those participants that
of illness, many older people retain a positive view of rated quality of life (QoL) as ‘Good’ (included ‘So good,
their health. If we can identify factors associated with could not be better’, or ‘Good’) instead of ‘Not good’
‘successful ageing’, then health promotion efforts can (included ‘Alright’ or ‘So bad, could not be worse’).
target the factors associated with this. What is ‘success- Although each model could independently predict
ful ageing’? QoL (Chapter 15 ☛), the strongest prediction was
achieved by the lay model. Those individuals who
scored as ‘successfully aged’ on the basis of lay model
Successful ageing variables were more than five times more likely to rate
Bowling and Iliffe (2006) describe five progressively their QoL as ‘Good’ rather than ‘Not good’. The odds of
more inclusive ‘models’ of successful ageing and the a ‘good’ QoL rating versus ‘not good’ was next best
variables considered within each model. Variables were among those classified on the broader biomedical model
(3.2 × more likely), than the biomedical model (2.6 ×
more likely), the psychological (2.4 × more likely) and
self-concept social models (1.99 × more likely).
Such findings highlight the importance of multidi-
that knowledge, conscious thoughts and beliefs
about yourself that allow you to feel you are mensional models of health in that medical or psycho-
distinct from others and that you exist as a logical or social variables are all important, but a more
separate person. holistic model is ‘better’. A broader model also opens up
a range of opportunities for intervention; the challenge
Individual, cultural and lifespan perspectives on health 23

IN THE SPOTLIGHT

Measuring self-rated or
subjective health status
Health is commonly viewed in terms of
how we feel and what we do. Our ‘health
status’ is not simply whether we are alive
or dead, nor is it defined simply on the
basis of the presence or absence of
symptoms – it is something we perceive
for ourselves, sometimes referred to as
‘subjective health status’. In fact, gener-
ally the relationship between subjective
health and markers of ‘objective’ health
is weak (e.g. Berg et al. 2006); however,
self-ratings of health (SRH), often
assessed as a simple single item (e.g.
‘How is your health in general?’), have
been found to predict major health out-
comes, including mortality (e.g. Bond
et al. 2006; Sargent-Cox et al. 2010). In
almost all European countries a majority
of the adult population will rate their
health as good or very good, although
this does not mean that the actual health
within the countries depicted is ‘objec-
tively’ better. These are self-reports, and
with this type of data come some chal-
lenges.

For example, data are potentially influ-


enced by the age composition of the
Figure 1.2 Adults’ self-reported health status, 2010.
sample, and furthermore the same asso-
Source: EU Statistics on Income and Living Conditions, OECD Health
ciations are not always found for both Data 2012.
genders. For example, Deeg and
Kriegsman (2003) find a relationship
between SRH and health outcomes only for men. Addressing measurement issues, Sargent-Cox and
Across all EU countries sampled for the data pre- colleagues conducted a study of over 2,000 Australian
sented above (OECD Health at a Glance 2012), men adults over the age of 65 assessed seven times
were more likely to rate their health as good or better, between 1992 and 2004. They used three different
and rating declined markedly after age 45 in many measures of SRH – comparing self with previous self
countries and then again after age 65. Socio-economic (a temporal comparison); comparing self with other
influences on reports are also reported (OECD 2012a) people of the same age (an age-group comparison)
(see Chapter 2 ☛ ). and a no-comparison global rating (simply rated
24 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

­ urrent health). They hypothesised that the age-group


c Findings such as this are important in highlighting that
social comparison would show an increase as the the measures we use can influence the results we find
sample increasingly engaged in downward social and thus the interpretations we make. For example, a
comparison (with those worse off) so as to enhance self-comparison measure is seen to plateau in the
their self rating; and that the temporal comparison context of an ageing population, perhaps out of a feel-
would show worsened SRH. In fact all three ratings ing that ‘my health cannot get any worse’ (and so, by
worsened over time but the extent and rate of worsen- scoring SRH as being ‘same as the year previously’,
ing varied: the global rating showed a steep decline this could be misinterpreted as implying that health is
over the 12 years; contrary to expectations, the age- better than expected given the passing of time – if we
group comparative ratings became more negative (in assume actual health deteriorates over time). Such
men but not the youngest i.e. those 65 on com- findings also have relevance for other age groups, and
mencement), and self-comparative ratings became for constructs other than self-rated health (for exam-
more negative although a ceiling effect is reported ple, Chapter 5 ☛ describes how the comparator,
whereby, over time, they are more likely to rate self as made in questions regarding drinking behaviour, or
having stayed the same as previously. disease risk, can also change responses).

now is to use such findings to develop and evaluate (see Chapters 6 and 7). Of note, however, is that the
health promotion interventions with older populations sample in this study was 98 per cent white and thus the
model of successful ageing best associated with QoL in
this sample may not hold for non-white samples.
This chapter has described what is often meant by
‘health’. In focusing on health, we have acknowledged
that health is a continuum, not simply a dichotomy of
sick versus healthy. Most of us will experience in our
lifetime varying degrees of health and well-being, with
periods of illness at one extreme and optimal wellness at
the other. Some may never experience optimal wellness.
‘Health refers to a state of being that is largely taken for
granted’ (Radley 1994: 5) and is often only appreciated
when lost through illness. In the final section of this
chapter we want to introduce what is broadly considered
as the discipline of health psychology. The final chapter
of this book addresses careers in health psychology
(Chapter 18 ☛).

What is health
psychology?
Before defining health psychology, let’s first look at
psychology as a discipline generally. Psychology can be
defined as the scientific study of mental and behavioural
Photo 1.4 Many activities can be enjoyed at any age. functioning. Studying mental processes through behav-
Source: Alamy Images/Radius Images. iour is limited, however, in that not all behaviour is
What is health psychology? 25

observable (for example, is thought not behaviour?) and questions such as why some people behave in a healthy
thus for many aspects of human behaviour we have to way and others do not. Is it all a matter of personality?
rely on self-report, the problems of which are described Does a person who behaves in a healthy manner in one
elsewhere. way, e.g. doesn’t smoke, also behaves healthily in other
Psychology aims to describe, explain, predict and, ways, e.g. attend dental screening? Are we rational and
where possible, intervene to control or modify behav- consistent beings? Does gender, age or socio-economic
ioural and mental processes, from language, memory, status affect health either directly or indirectly via their
attention and perception to emotions, social behaviour effects on other things? Why do some people appear to
and health behaviour, to name just a few. The key to get ill all the time while others stay healthy? What psy-
scientific methods employed by psychologists is the chosocial factors can help a person adjust to, or recover
basic principle that the world may be known through from, illness? Health psychology integrates many cogni-
observation = empiricism. Empirical methods go tive, developmental and social theories and explana-
beyond speculation, inference and reasoning to actual tions, and applies them solely to health, illness and
and systematic analysis of data. Scientific research starts health care. Health psychology was described at an early
with a theory, which can be defined as a general set of stage in the emergence of this new field of study by
assumptions about how things operate in the world. Matarazzo as ‘the aggregate of the specific educational,
Theories can be vague and poorly defined (e.g. I have a scientific and professional contribution of the discipline
theory about why sports science students generally sit of psychology to the promotion and maintenance of
together at the back of lectures) to very specific (e.g. health, the promotion and treatment of illness and
sports science students sit at the back of lectures because related dysfunction’ (1980: 815). This definition high-
they feel like ‘outsiders’ when placed with the large lights the main goals of health psychology, i.e. we seek
numbers of psychology majors). Psychologists scientifi- to develop our understanding of biopsychosocial factors
cally test the validity of their hypotheses and theories. involved in:
On an academic level this can increase understanding
● the promotion and maintenance of health;
about a particular phenomenon, and on an applied level
it can provide knowledge useful to the development of
● improving health-care systems and health policy;
interventions. ● the prevention and treatment of illness;
Psychologists use scientific methods to investigate all ● the causes of illness: e.g. vulnerability/risk factors.
kinds of behaviour and mental processes, from the
response activity of a single nerve cell to the role adjust- Unlike some other domains of psychology (such as
ments required in old age, and the research method cognitive science), health psychology can be considered
employed will depend on what specific questions are as an applied science, although not all health psychology
being asked. You obviously would not use the same research is predictive. For example, some research aims
methods to establish the extent of language in a two-year- only to quantify (e.g. what percentage of school pupils
old as you would to identify which areas of the brain drink under age?) or describe (e.g. what are the beliefs
were activated during speech. This text highlights those of under-age drinkers regarding the effects of alcohol?).
that are most commonly employed by health psycholo- Descriptive research ideally provides the foundation for
gists: for example, the use of questionnaires, interviews the generation of more causal questions: e.g. do beliefs
and psychometric assessments (such as of personality). about alcohol in primary schoolchildren predict age of
onset of under-age drinking? By simply measuring
health beliefs and attitudes, we can begin to grapple with
What connects psychology the issue of predictors (see Chapters 3–5 ☛) before
developing interventions.
to health?
As introduced in this chapter, people have beliefs about
empiricism
health, are often emotional about it and have a behav-
arising from a school of thought that all knowledge
ioural role to play in maintaining their health and coping
can be obtained through experience.
with illness. As such, we can address the underlying
26 Chapter 1 ● What is health?

Health psychology and other fields ‘hysterical paralysis’), and while evidence for direct
causality has proved limited, these developments in
Health psychology has grown out of many fields within thinking certainly did set the groundwork for fascinating
the social sciences. It has adopted and adapted models studies of physiological processes that may link person-
and theories originally found in social psychology, ality type to disease (see Chapter 11’s ☛ discussion of
behaviourism, clinical psychology, cognitive psychol- hostility and heart disease associations, for example).
ogy, etc. In fact you may also want to pick up an intro- Until the 1960s, psychosomatic research was predomi-
ductory psychology text and look at the learning, nantly psychoanalytical in nature, focusing on psycho-
motivation, social, developmental and cognitive sec- analytic interpretations of illness causation, such as
tions in more detail. Health psychology in Europe is, as asthma, ulcers or migraine being triggered by repressed
in the USA and Australasia, linked with other health emotions. However, one negative by-product of this
and social sciences (e.g. psychosomatic medicine, work is that among those with a biomedical viewpoint,
behavioural medicine, medical sociology and, increas- illnesses with no identifiable organic cause were often
ingly, health economics) and with medicine and allied dismissed as nervous disorders or psychosomatic condi-
therapeutic disciplines. Few academic or practitioner tions for which medical treatment was often not forth-
health psychologists work alone; most are involved in coming. Illnesses with no physical evidence are known
an array of inter- and multidisciplinary work (see as psychogenic (see Chapter 9 for discussion of medi-
Chapter 18 ☛). In each of the above there exists a cally unexplained symptoms ☛).
challenge to the dualist thinking regarding mind–body Psychosomatic medicine today is more concerned
separation, but as well as similarity there are differences with mixed psychological, social and biological/physio-
in theoretical underpinning (i.e. sociological, medical, logical explanations of illness. Illnesses are often viewed
psychological) and consequent differences in the meth- as ‘psychophysiological’, with increased acceptance that
ods of assessment, research and intervention suggested psychological factors can affect any physical condition.
or employed. This notion in the 1970s led to the emergence of an inte-
grated discipline known as behavioural medicine, and to
Psychosomatic medicine
health psychology itself.
Developed in the 1930s this initially was the domain of
now well-known psychoanalysts, e.g. Alexander, Freud,
Behavioural medicine
and offered an early challenge to biomedicine as dis-
This is essentially an interdisciplinary field drawing on
cussed earlier in the chapter. ‘Psycho-somatic’ refers to
a range of behavioural sciences, including psychology,
the fact that the mind and body are both involved in ill-
sociology and health education, in relation to medicine
ness, and where an organic cause is not easily identified
and medical conditions (Schwartz and Weiss 1977).
the mind may offer the trigger of a physical response that
Behavioural medicine developed in the 1970s and pro-
is detectable and measurable. In other words, mind and
vided further challenge to the biomedical model domi-
body act together, not just the mind. Early work asserted
nant at the time. As its name suggests, behavioural
that a certain personality would lead to a certain disease
principles (i.e. that behaviour results from learning
(e.g. Alexander’s ‘ulcer-prone personality’, or Freud’s
through classical or operant conditioning) were
applied to experimentally evaluate techniques of pre-
motivation vention and rehabilitation, and not solely treatment.
memories, thoughts, experiences, needs and Prevention does, however, receive less attention than
preferences that act together to influence (drive) rehabilitation and treatment of illness, which highlights
the type, strength and persistence of our actions. one of the key differences between behavioural medi-
operant conditioning cine and health psychology. In furthering the view that
attributed to Skinner, this theory is based on the the mind had a direct link to the body (e.g. anxiety can
assumption that behaviour is directly influenced by raise blood pressure, fear can elevate heart rate), some
its consequences (e.g. rewards, punishments, of the therapies proposed, such as biofeedback (see
avoidance of negative outcomes). Chapter 13 ☛) work on the principle of operant condi-
tioning and feedback.
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
prohibition against accepting unsolicited donations from donors in
such states who approach us with offers to donate.

International donations are gratefully accepted, but we cannot make


any statements concerning tax treatment of donations received from
outside the United States. U.S. laws alone swamp our small staff.

Please check the Project Gutenberg web pages for current donation
methods and addresses. Donations are accepted in a number of
other ways including checks, online payments and credit card
donations. To donate, please visit: www.gutenberg.org/donate.

Section 5. General Information About Project


Gutenberg™ electronic works
Professor Michael S. Hart was the originator of the Project
Gutenberg™ concept of a library of electronic works that could be
freely shared with anyone. For forty years, he produced and
distributed Project Gutenberg™ eBooks with only a loose network of
volunteer support.

Project Gutenberg™ eBooks are often created from several printed


editions, all of which are confirmed as not protected by copyright in
the U.S. unless a copyright notice is included. Thus, we do not
necessarily keep eBooks in compliance with any particular paper
edition.

Most people start at our website which has the main PG search
facility: www.gutenberg.org.

This website includes information about Project Gutenberg™,


including how to make donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation, how to help produce our new eBooks, and how
to subscribe to our email newsletter to hear about new eBooks.

You might also like