You are on page 1of 54

Cognition and Addiction A Researcher

s Guide from Mechanisms Towards


Interventions 1st Edition Antonio
Verdejo García (Editor)
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://textbookfull.com/product/cognition-and-addiction-a-researcher-s-guide-from-m
echanisms-towards-interventions-1st-edition-antonio-verdejo-garcia-editor/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Neuroscience for addiction medicine : from prevention


to rehabilitation : methods and interventions First
Edition Ekhtiari

https://textbookfull.com/product/neuroscience-for-addiction-
medicine-from-prevention-to-rehabilitation-methods-and-
interventions-first-edition-ekhtiari/

Neurocognitive Mechanisms: Explaining Biological


Cognition Gualtiero Piccinini

https://textbookfull.com/product/neurocognitive-mechanisms-
explaining-biological-cognition-gualtiero-piccinini/

Reliable Software Technologies Ada Europe 2015 20th Ada


Europe International Conference on Reliable Software
Technologies Madrid Spain June 22 26 2015 Proceedings
1st Edition Juan Antonio De La Puente
https://textbookfull.com/product/reliable-software-technologies-
ada-europe-2015-20th-ada-europe-international-conference-on-
reliable-software-technologies-madrid-spain-
june-22-26-2015-proceedings-1st-edition-juan-antonio-de-la-
puente/
How to Be a Researcher A Strategic Guide for Academic
Success Jonathan St B T Evans

https://textbookfull.com/product/how-to-be-a-researcher-a-
strategic-guide-for-academic-success-jonathan-st-b-t-evans/
Becoming A Writing Researcher Ann M. Blakeslee

https://textbookfull.com/product/becoming-a-writing-researcher-
ann-m-blakeslee/

The Re Use Atlas A Designer s Guide Towards the


Circular Economy 1st Edition Duncan Baker-Brown

https://textbookfull.com/product/the-re-use-atlas-a-designer-s-
guide-towards-the-circular-economy-1st-edition-duncan-baker-
brown/

The ADA practical guide to soft tissue oral disease


Second Edition Hall

https://textbookfull.com/product/the-ada-practical-guide-to-soft-
tissue-oral-disease-second-edition-hall/

Psychodynamic Therapy Techniques: A Guide to Expressive


and Supportive Interventions Brian A Sharpless

https://textbookfull.com/product/psychodynamic-therapy-
techniques-a-guide-to-expressive-and-supportive-interventions-
brian-a-sharpless/

Becoming A Behavioral Science Researcher, 2nd ed: A


Guide To Producing Research That Matter 2nd Edition Rex
B. Kline

https://textbookfull.com/product/becoming-a-behavioral-science-
researcher-2nd-ed-a-guide-to-producing-research-that-matter-2nd-
edition-rex-b-kline/
Cognition and Addiction
A Researcher’s Guide from Mechanisms
Towards Interventions

Edited by
Antonio Verdejo-Garcia
Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier
125 London Wall, London EC2Y 5AS, United Kingdom
525 B Street, Suite 1650, San Diego, CA 92101, United States
50 Hampshire Street, 5th Floor, Cambridge, MA 02139, United States
The Boulevard, Langford Lane, Kidlington, Oxford OX5 1GB, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.


No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including
photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Details on how to seek permission, further information about the Publisher’s permissions policies and our arrangements with
organizations such as the Copyright Clearance Center and the Copyright Licensing Agency, can be found at our website: www.
elsevier.com/permissions.
This book and the individual contributions contained in it are protected under copyright by the Publisher (other than as may
be noted herein).
Notices
Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experience broaden our understanding,
changes in research methods, professional practices, or medical treatment may become necessary.
Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge in evaluating and using any
information, methods, compounds, or experiments described herein. In using such information or methods they should be
mindful of their own safety and the safety of others, including parties for whom they have a professional responsibility.
To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or editors, assume any liability for any
injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or
operation of any methods, products, instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
ISBN: 978-0-12-815298-0

For information on all Academic Press publications visit our


website at https://www.elsevier.com/books-and-journals

Publisher: Nikki Levy


Acquisition Editor: Joslyn Chaiprasert-Paguio
Editorial Project Manager: Tracy Tufaga
Production Project Manager: Mohana Natarajan
Cover Designer: Greg Harris
Typeset by TNQ Technologies
I have been dedicated to edit this book during the last couple of years, but the
knowledge and collaborations that have made it possible span over 15 years.
Thus, I would like to dedicate this book to my mentors: Professors Miguel
Perez-Garcı́a, Antoine Bechara, and Karen Bolla and to the many colleagues and
friends (and colleagues who became friends) with whom I have shared amazing
research and life adventures in Granada, Barcelona, Iowa, Baltimore, Cambridge,
and Melbourne. To my parents, for fighting to give me a university education and
teaching me to always decide by myself. Huge thanks to my wife, Natalia, for
being an inspiration and spring of energy for so many personal and career
choicesdI look up to you every single day.
Antonio Verdejo-Garcı́a,
Melbourne, May 2019
Contributors

Merideth A. Addicott, Department of Psychiatry, Uni- Nicolas Cabé, Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, PSL Uni-
versity of Arkansas for Medical Science, Little Rock, versité de Paris, EPHE, INSERM, U1077, CHU de
AR, United States Caen, GIP Cyceron, Neuropsychologie et Imagerie de la
Robin L. Aupperle, Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Mémoire Humaine, Caen, France
Tulsa, OK, United States Zhipeng Cao, School of Psychology, University College
Alex Baldacchino, Division of Populations and Health Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Science, St Andrews Medical School, University of Adrian Carter, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental
St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia;
Joël Billieux, Addictive and Compulsive Behaviours UQ Centre for Clinical Research, University of
Laboratory, Institute for Health and Behaviours, Uni- Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
versity of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg Natalie Castellanos-Ryan, Universite de Montreal, CHU
Marilisa Boffo, Addiction Development and Psychopathol- Ste Justine, Montreal, QC, Canada
ogy (ADAPT) Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Luke Clark, Centre for Gambling Research at UBC,
University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Department of Psychology, University of British
Department of Psychology, Education and Child Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Patricia Conrod, Universite de Montreal, CHU Ste Jus-
The Netherlands tine, Montreal, QC, Canada
Matthias Brand, General Psychology: Cognition and Fleur Davey, Research and Development Department,
Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), NHS Fife, Dunfermline, Fife, United Kingdom
University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany;
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Andrew Dawson, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental
Essen, Germany Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Damien Brevers, Laboratory of Psychological Medicine Logan T. Dowdle, Department of Psychiatry and Behav-
and Addictology, Faculty of Medicine, Brugmann- ioral Sciences, College of Medicine, Medical University
Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, of South Carolina. Charleston, SC, United States
Belgium; Research in Psychology Applied to Motor Timothy C. Durazzo, Stanford University and Palo Alto
Learning, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, VA Medical Center, Stanford, CA, United States
Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium Hamed Ekhtiari, Laureate Institute for Brain Research,
Gabriel Brooks, Centre for Gambling Research at UBC, Tulsa, OK, United States
Department of Psychology, University of British Mario Ferrari, Centre for Gambling Research at UBC,
Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Department of Psychology, University of British
S.J. Brooks, School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Matt Field, Department of Psychology, University of
Kingdom; Department of Neuroscience, Uppsala Uni- Sheffield, Sheffield, South Yorkshire, United Kingdom
versity, Uppsala, Sweden
S. Funk, Department of Psychology, University of Cape
M. Aryana Bryan, Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Town, Cape Town, South Africa
Health Intervention Development, University of Utah,
Salt Lake City, UT, United States

xv
xvi Contributors

Gloria Garcia-Fernandez, School of Psychological Daniel H. Lench, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Sciences, College of Medicine, Medical University of
Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Aus- South Carolina. Charleston, SC, United States
tralia; Faculty of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Angéline Maillard, Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, PSL
Oviedo, Spain Université de Paris, EPHE, INSERM, U1077, CHU de
Eric L. Garland, Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Caen, GIP Cyceron, Neuropsychologie et Imagerie de la
Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, Mémoire Humaine, Caen, France
Salt Lake City, UT, United States Pierre Maurage, Laboratory for Experimental Psychopa-
Rita Z. Goldstein, Departments of Psychiatry and Neuro- thology, Psychological Science Research Institute,
science, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve,
York, NY, United States Belgium
Raul Gonzalez, Center for Children and Families, Dieter J. Meyerhoff, University of California San Fran-
Department of Psychology, Florida International Uni- cisco and San Francisco VA Medical Center, San
versity, Miami, FL, United States Francisco, CA, United States
Renee D. Goodwin, Department of Epidemiology and Scott J. Moeller, Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook
Biostatistics, School of Public Health, The City Uni- University School of Medicine, Stony Brook, NY,
versity of New York, New York, NY, United States; United States
Department of Epidemiology, Mailman School of Catharine Montgomery, School of Natural Sciences and
Public Health, Columbia University, New York, NY, Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University,
United States Liverpool, United Kingdom
Anna E. Goudriaan, Amsterdam UMC, Department of Laura O’Halloran, School of Psychology, Trinity College
Psychiatry, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research; Arkin
Mental Health; Department of Quality of Care and Ileana Pacheco-Colón, Center for Children and Families,
Research and Jellinek, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Psychology, Florida International Uni-
versity, Miami, FL, United States
Wayne Hall, UQ Centre for Clinical Research, University
of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia; Centre for Martin P. Paulus, Laureate Institute for Brain Research,
Youth Substance Abuse Research, University of Tulsa, OK, United States
Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia; National Tomás Paus, Bloorview Research Institute, Holland
Addiction Centre, Kings College London, London, Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON,
WC2R 2LS, United Kingdom Canada; Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry,
Adam W. Hanley, Center on Mindfulness and Integrative University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, MacKenzie R. Peltier, Yale School of Medicine, Depart-
Salt Lake City, UT, United States ment of Psychiatry, New Haven, CT, United States; VA
Colleen A. Hanlon, Department of Psychiatry and Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, United
Behavioral Sciences, College of Medicine, Medical States
University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United Brian Pennie, School of Psychology, Trinity College
States Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Matthew O. Howard, University of North Carolina at Anne Lise Pitel, Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, PSL Uni-
Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, United States versité de Paris, EPHE, INSERM, U1077, CHU de
Andrew Jones, Department of Psychological Sciences, Caen, GIP Cyceron, Neuropsychologie et Imagerie de la
University of Liverpool, Liverpool, Merseyside, United Mémoire Humaine, Caen, France
Kingdom Marc N. Potenza, Departments of Psychiatry and Neuro-
Daniel L. King, School of Psychology, The University of science and Child Study Center, Yale School of Med-
Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia; College of Educa- icine, New Haven, CT, United States; The Connecticut
tion, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Council on Problem Gambling, Wethersfield, CT,
Adelaide, SA, Australia United States; The Connecticut Mental Health Center,
New Haven, CT, United States
Jacob W. Koudys, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON,
Canada
Contributors xvii

Boris B. Quednow, Experimental and Clinical Pharma- Douglas Steele, Institute of Neuroscience, Ninewells
copsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy Hospital Medical School, University of Dundee,
and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Dundee, Tayside, United Kingdom
Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland; Neuroscience Centre Ryan M. Sullivan, Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook
Zurich, University of Zurich and Swiss Federal Institute University School of Medicine, Stony Brook, NY,
of Technology (ETH) Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland United States; Department of Psychology, University of
C. Rabier, Department of Psychology, University of Cape Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, United States
Town, Cape Town, South Africa Serenella Tolomeo, Division of Populations and Health
Kavya Raj, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Science, St Andrews Medical School, University of St
Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom
Tonisha Kearney Ramos, Department of Psychiatry and Tess den Uyl, Addiction Development and Psychopathol-
Behavioral Sciences, College of Medicine, Medical ogy (ADAPT) Laboratory, Department of Psychology,
University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
States Alireza Valyan, Allameh Tabataba’i University, Tehran,
Tara Rezapour, Institute for Cognitive Science Studies, Iran
Tehran, Iran; Iranian National Center for Addiction Antonio Verdejo-Garcia, School of Psychological Sci-
Studies, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, ences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health,
Tehran, Iran Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Carl A. Roberts, Institute of Psychology, Health and Fausto Viader, Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, PSL Uni-
Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United versité de Paris, EPHE, INSERM, U1077, CHU de
Kingdom Caen, GIP Cyceron, Neuropsychologie et Imagerie de la
Adam J. Rubenis, Turning Point Alcohol and Drug Mémoire Humaine, Caen, France
Centre, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Robert Whelan, School of Psychology, Trinity College
Anthony C. Ruocco, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Canada Reinout W. Wiers, Addiction Development and Psychopa-
H.B. Schiöth, Department of Neuroscience, Uppsala thology (ADAPT) Laboratory, Department of Psychology,
University, Uppsala, Sweden University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Ryan Smith, Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Oulmann Zerhouni, UFR Sciences Psychologiques et
OK, United States Sciences de l’Éducation (SPSE), Université Paris Nan-
Mehmet Sofuoglu, Yale School of Medicine, Department terre, Nanterre, France
of Psychiatry, New Haven, CT, United States; VA Anna Zilverstand, Department of Psychiatry, University
Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
United States
Biographies

Antonio Verdejo-García has a PhD in Psychology research center, and the University of Granada, and he
(Addiction Neuropsychology, University of Granada, is the Chair of the Neuroscience Interest Group of the
2006) and a Masters in Psychological and Biomedical International Society of Addiction Medicine.
Aspects of Health and Illness (University of Granada, Professor Verdejo-García has led numerous studies on
2002). After his PhD, he continued specialized training the cognitive and neural substrates of substance and
in addiction neuroscience in highly prestigious research behavioral addictions, and new cognitive training and
centers: Johns Hopkins Medical Institute (Neurology), remediation interventions for treating substance use dis-
IMIM-Hospital del Mar (Pharmacology) and the University orders. He is internationally recognized as an expert in this
of Cambridge (Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience field, as evinced by several international Editorial Board
Institute). positions including top-ranked addiction journals. He has
Currently, Antonio Verdejo-García is an Australian published more than 200 peer-reviewed articles, and his
Medical Research Future Fund Fellow and holds a Full work has attracted over 10,000 citations and has been
ProfessoreResearch appointment at the Turner Institute for translated into clinical trials of neurocognitive interventions
Brain and Mental Health (Monash University), where he is and policy recommendations regarding application of
the Deputy Lead of the Addiction and Mental Health neuroscience principles for the prevention and treatment of
Program. He also holds honorary appointments at Turning addictions.
Point, Australia’s leading national addiction treatment and

xix
Foreword

Since the seminal paper of Dr. Leshnerdthen the director variations of this model with new perspectives for treatment.
of the National Institute on Drug Abusedin Science In the Chapters 14e16, similar reviews are presented for
in 1997, addiction is generally regarded to be a bio- gambling and gaming disorders.
psychosocial disorder with strong genetic and neuro- In addition to their role as risk factors in the develop-
biological underpinnings and a chronic-intermittent course ment of addiction, cognitive impairments are often a
with periods of recovery followed by relapse and often with consequence of chronic, excessive drug use with negative
serious psychosocial deterioration. Based on animal studies effects on the course of the disorder and on treatment
and human genetic and neuroimaging studies, Dr. Leshner effectiveness. In the Chapters 8e13, reviews on the
concluded that addiction is a brain disease and that addicted consequences of drug use are presented for the different
people are patients who deserve (reimbursed) treatment. substances of abuse, including alcohol, tobacco, cannabis,
According to the underlying biopsychosocial model, cocaine, MDMA, and opioids. These chapters invariably
addiction is the outcome of a preexisting hyperactive brain show not only that drug use may lead to cognitive
reward system in combination with a deficient cognitive impairments but also that (sustained) abstinence will lead to
control system in combination with neuroplastic changes partial or complete recovery of cognitive functions and
caused by continued drug use. Originally, the model was probably to better long-term outcomes of treatment.
predominantly presented in terms of dysfunctional brain Treatment is the focus of the next part of the book
structures and neurotransmitter abnormalities with rela- (Chapters 17e23). In a series of highly informative reviews,
tively little attention to the cognitive representations of the authors show that there are currently many more treat-
these abnormalities. As a consequence, the link between the ment options than motivational interviewing and cognitive
neurobiological abnormalities and the behavioral mani- behavioral treatment, including cognitive bias modification,
festations of addictions was incomplete, and the search for working memory training, inhibition control training,
new treatments was mainly directed to the discovery of goal management training, mindfulness-based relapse
new medications against relapse. Recent developments in prevention (MBRP), transcranial magnetic stimulation, and
our knowledge about the neurocognitive aspects of the the use of cognition-enhancing medications. With the
development of addictive behaviors, the neurocognitive exception of approach bias modification and MBRP, these
consequences of chronic drug use, and the potential new interventions need to be tested in large-phase III trials,
treatments directed at improvement of preexisting and but most of them show great promise and will redirect
drug-induced neurocognitive deficits have created new treatment from talking to training and from face-to-face
hope for patients with an addiction. interventions to online treatments. In the last two chapters of
This is the first book that provides a comprehensive the book (Chapters 29 and 30), the authors provide an in-
review of what we now know about cognition and addic- tegrated review of both existing and new treatment options
tion. An impressive lineup of experts presents a broad and and a theory-based proposal for optimal combinations of
in-depth overview of what is known about the cognitive cognitive interventions based on a thorough understanding
underpinnings of addiction, neurocognitive approaches to of the underlying cognitive models.
treatment and future research perspectives. Chapters 24e28 are more contemplative in nature and
The book starts with a well-balanced presentation of make the reader think about population neuroscience, the
what we know about preexisting (genetic and learned) use of cognitive information in combination with genetics,
cognitive processes that are responsible for the change from the cognitive effects of reduced consumption versus
recreational drug use to goal-driven, drug seeking and finally complete abstinence, and finally the (limited) impact of
ending in chronic compulsive and habitual addictive scientific knowledge about cognition and addiction on
behaviors leading to physical and/or psychosocial decline policy development.
(Chapters 1e7). In addition to the well-known dual-process This book is a remarkable set of reviews and position
model, the authors present extensions and/or integrated papers presented as chapters edited by one of the best

xxi
xxii Foreword

scientists in the field of cognition and addiction. Thanks to interested in the topic. I therefore highly recommend this
his broad and in-depth knowledge of the field and his book to everybody in the field.
ability to recruit the best experts on such diverse topics; Wim van den Brink, MD PhD
this book is currently by far the best introduction to Em. Professor of Psychiatry and Addiction,
cognition and addiction for neuroscience and psychology Amsterdam University Medical Centers,
students and researchers and for clinical psychologists, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
psychiatrists, neurologists, and for all other people
Acknowledgments

The Editor (Antonio Verdejo-García) is funded by an acknowledge Professor Wim van den Brink, a trailblazer
Australian Medical Research Future Fund, Next Generation addiction researcher and inspirational figure for many (back
Clinical Researchers CDF2 Fellowship (MRFF 1141214) then) young researchers in cognition and addiction and a
and wish to acknowledge this support, trusting that the tireless supporter of early career scientists, for writing the
book will contribute to the fellowship legacy by training a foreword of the book. Last not least, sincere acknowledg-
new generation of addiction researchers. The Editor would ments to the Elsevier editorial team including Joslyn
also like to acknowledge all the contributors for generously Chaiprasert-Paguio, who planted the first seed of this book,
sharing their unique knowledge and limited timedthey and Tracy Tufaga, who has invaluably helped to make it
make up an outstanding cast and are the ones who give true grow.
value to this book. The Editor would also like to specially

xxiii
Introduction

Cognition refers to mental processes and encompasses gambling addictions, and the cognitive sequela asso-
“all forms of knowing and awareness, such as ciated with the chronic use of different substances, such
conceiving, remembering, judging, imagining, and as alcohol, cannabis, stimulants and opioids, and
problem solving” (APA Dictionary of Psychology). gambling modalities. The cognitive profiles associated
Addiction is a mental health condition and, not surp- with different drugs and addictive behaviors are dis-
risingly, is associated with cognitive biases and deficits. cussed in the context of current topics and contro-
The last 20 years have witnessed an unprecedented versies, such as the therapeutic effects of cannabinoids,
expansion in the understanding of the cognitive the aftermath of the synthetic opioid crisis, or the
underpinnings of addiction vulnerability and chronicity. advent of online gambling and gaming activities
This growth has been fueled by theoretical and techno- (Curran et al., 2016; Karilla et al., 2018; Kardefelt-
logical advancement. Neurobehavioral models of addi- Winther et al., 2017). Section (3) introduces novel
ction have shed light on the cognitive mechanisms of neuroscience-informed treatment approaches to
aberrant reward valuation, cognitive control, and rescue cognitive deficits and improve clinical out-
decision-making (Bickel et al., 2018; Everitt and Robbins, comes for people with addictions. These approaches
2016; Goldstein and Volkow, 2002; Verdejo-Garcia and include computerized cognitive training programs to
Bechara, 2009), overcoming old views about addictive retrain salience-related biases and build response
behavior been “self-destructive” or “morally weak” inhibition and working memory capacity, cognitive
(discussed in Hyman, 2007, Am J Bioeth). The advent of remediation therapies focused on executive functions,
automated computerized testing, neuroimaging, mindfulness interventions targeting cognitive control
and other biomedical techniques such as gene and monitoring, and pharmacological enhancement
sequencing and manipulation, and novel intervention and brain-stimulation techniques. Finally, Section (4)
approaches such as cognitive training and remediation, provides unique new vistas on research approaches
neuroscience-informed psychotherapies and neuro- that are pushing the boundaries of the cognition and
modulation have fueled the knowledge gain and addiction field. These exciting new avenues include
revamped the landscape of cognition and addiction population neuroscience, namely, the application of
research (Ekhtiari et al., 2019; Kwako et al., 2016; cutting-edge neuroscience tools to population-based
Mackey et al., 2016). This book attempts to provide a cohorts, neuroepidemiology, i.e., the leverage of
comprehensive view of this renewed landscape. epidemiology data to address neurocognitive ques-
The book is structured in four main sections: (1) tions, neuroethics, and longitudinal brain mapping. In
Cognitive Principles, (2) Cognitive Risk Factors and addition, it provides a pedagogic approach to the use
Consequences, (3) Cognitive Interventions, and (4) of new techniques for “big data” collection and
New Vistas. Section (1) covers updated neurocognitive analysis, and the application of genetic and neuro-
theories of addiction and its evidence base. These imaging techniques to understand the lifespan of
include views on addiction as a disorder that involves addiction pathophysiology, from preterm vulnerability
an aberrant transition from impulsivity to compulsivity, to adult abstinence-based neuroplasticity and recovery.
impaired response inhibition and salience attribution,
The book has been conceived with an inclusive and
decision-making dysfunctions, social cognition and
international perspective and includes contributors
interaction deficits, and personality comorbidities
from Europe, Africa, America, Australia, and the
underpinned by common alterations in executive
Middle East, which provide a truly global viewpoint.
functions. Section (2) reviews the cognitive alterations
The contributors are outstanding researchers and
that underlie vulnerability to substance use and

xxv
xxvi Introduction

clinicians, and the contents are geared towards a broad Biobehav. Rev. 104, 118e140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.
audience including research students, researchers 2019.06.007. Epub 2019 Jul 2. Review. PubMed PMID: 31271802.
and academics, and frontline clinicians interested Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2016. Drug addiction: updating actions to
habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu Rev Psychol 67, 23e50.
in learning and applying cognitive principles and
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.
techniques in addiction research, prevention, assess-
Goldstein, R.Z., Volkow, N.D., 2002. Drug addiction and its underlying
ment, treatment, and recovery. We hope to succeed in neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of
our main goal, that readers share our enthusiasm about the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 159 (10), 1642e1652.
this fascinating field. Hyman, S.E., 2007. The neurobiology of addiction: implications for
voluntary control of behavior. Am. J. Bioeth. 7 (1), 8e11.
Kardefelt-Winther, D., Heeren, A., Schimmenti, A., van Rooij, A.,
References Maurage, P., Carras, M., Edman, J., Blaszczynski, A., Khazaal, Y.,
Billieux, J., 2017. How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction
Bickel, W.K., Mellis, A.M., Snider, S.E., Athamneh, L.N., Stein, J.S.,
without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction 112 (10),
Pope, D.A., 2018. 21st century neurobehavioral theories of decision
1709e1715. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13763.
making in addiction: Review and evaluation. Pharmacol. Biochem.
Karila, L., Marillier, M., Chaumette, B., Billieux, J., Franchitto, N.,
Behav. 164, 4e21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2017.09.009.
Benyamina, A., 2018. New synthetic opioids: Part of a new addiction
Curran, H.V., Freeman, T.P., Mokrysz, C., Lewis, D.A., Morgan, C.J.,
landscape. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neu-
Parsons, L.H., 2016. Keep off the grass? Cannabis, cognition and addiction.
biorev.2018.06.010 pii: S0149-7634(18)30114-3.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17 (5), 293e306. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.28.
Kwako, L.E., Momenan, R., Litten, R.Z., Koob, G.F., Goldman, D., 2016.
Ekhtiari, H., Tavakoli, H., Addolorato, G., Baeken, C., Bonci, A.,
Addictions neuroclinical assessment: a neuroscience-based framework
Campanella, S., Castelo-Branco, L., Challet-Bouju, G., Clark, V.P.,
for addictive disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 80 (3), 179e189. https://
Claus, E., Dannon, P.N., Del Felice, A., den Uyl, T., Diana, M., di
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.024.
Giannantonio, M., Fedota, J.R., Fitzgerald, P., Gallimberti, L., Grall-
Mackey, S., Kan, K.J., Chaarani, B., Alia-Klein, N., Batalla, A.,
Bronnec, M., Herremans, S.C., Herrmann, M.J., Jamil, A, Khedr, E.,
Brooks, S., Cousijn, J., Dagher, A., de Ruiter, M., Desrivieres, S.,
Kouimtsidis, C., Kozak, K., Krupitsky, E., Lamm, C., Lechner, W.V.,
Feldstein Ewing, S.W., Goldstein, R.Z., Goudriaan, A.E.,
Madeo, G., Malmir, N., Martinotti, G., McDonald, W.M.,
Heitzeg, M.M., Hutchison, K., Li, C.S., London, E.D., Lorenzetti, V.,
Montemitro, C., Nakamura-Palacios, E.M., Nasehi, M., Noël, X.,
Luijten, M., Martin-Santos, R., Morales, A.M., Paulus, M.P., Paus, T.,
Nosratabadi, M., Paulus, M., Pettorruso, M., Pradhan, B.,
Pearlson, G., Schluter, R., Momenan, R., Schmaal, L., Schumann, G.,
Praharaj, S.K., Rafferty, H., Sahlem, G., Salmeron, B.J., Sauvaget, A.,
Sinha, R., Sjoerds, Z., Stein, D.J., Stein, E.A., Solowij, N., Tapert, S.,
Schluter, R.S., Sergiou, C., Shahbabaie, A., Sheffer, C.,
Uhlmann, A., Veltman, D., van Holst, R., Walter, H., Wright, M.J.,
Spagnolo, P.A., Steele, V.R., Yuan, T.F., van Dongen, J.D.M., Van
Yucel, M., Yurgelun-Todd, D., Hibar, D.P., Jahanshad, N.,
Waes, V., Venkatasubramanian, G., Verdejo-García, A., Verveer, I.,
Thompson, P.M., Glahn, D.C., Garavan, H., Conrod, P., 2016. Genetic
Welsh, J.W., Wesley, M.J., Witkiewitz, K., Yavari, F.,
imaging consortium for addiction medicine: from neuroimaging to
Zarrindast, M.R., Zawertailo, L., Zhang, X., Cha, Y.H., George, T.P.,
genes. Prog. Brain Res. 224, 203e223. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Frohlich, F., Goudriaan, A.E., Fecteau, S., Daughters, S.B.,
bs.pbr.2015.07.026.
Stein, E.A., Fregni, F., Nitsche, M.A., Zangen, A., Bikson, M.,
Verdejo-García, A., Bechara, A., 2009. A somatic marker theory of
Hanlon, C.A., 2019 Sep. Transcranial electrical and magnetic stimu-
addiction. Neuropharmacology 56 (Suppl. 1), 48e62. https://doi.org/
lation (tES and TMS) for addiction medicine: A consensus paper on
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.035.
the present state of the science and the road ahead. Neurosci.
Chapter 1

Cognition: the interface between nature


and nurture in addiction
Antonio Verdejo-Garcia
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Introduction externalization among people with addiction (e.g., “I have a


disease, I can’t do anything about it”) (Hall et al., 2015b).
A key question in the field of addiction is whether some In this chapter, I will argue that a focus on cognitiond
people are hardwired to develop addictive disorders or if, encompassing thinking, emotion, and related behaviors,
conversely, drugs and “addictive products” or related as well as their neural underpinningsdcan provide a more
contexts generate addictions. Historically, the answers to comprehensive and integrative understanding of the nature
this question have fluctuated as a function of prevailing and the course of addiction. Cognition sits at the interface
theories. Old moral views and personality models saw of biological, psychological, and social drivers of addictive
inherent weaknesses in the individual (Eysenck, 1997; disorders, hinging on the interplay between nature and
Peele, 1987). Learning theories have focused on the ability nurture. Genetic and early environmental influences shape
of drugs and “addictive products” (e.g., electronic gaming the cognitive traits that make us vulnerable or resilient to
machines) to generate aberrant learning, which is then drug use/gambling and related social contexts (e.g., product
resistant to extinction relatively uniformly across availability, peer pressure) (Belcher et al., 2014). At the
individuals (Robinson and Berridge, 2003, 2008). Social same time, drugs and gambling modify learning and
theories have treated addiction as a manifestation of a cognitive control processes and change the way we interact
certain context and environment (e.g., the classic studies on with others and the environment (Everitt and Robbins,
Vietnam War veterans) (Moore, 1993; Zinberg, 1984). 2016; Goldstein and Volkow, 2011; Moeller and Goldstein,
Nowadays, there is agreement that none of these models, in 2014). By focusing on cognition, we can overcome
isolation, can satisfactorily explain the nature and the the reductionism of the “disease model,” i.e., it’s not in
course of addiction, but at the same time, there is a lack of the brain, it’s at the interface between the brain and the
comprehensive frameworks. Contemporary models have environment, and foster self-agency about recovery,
embraced a biopsychosocial approach, but there is a bias i.e., it’s not indelible, the same influences that originally
toward the bio-, at least in discovery science and thera- shaped cognition in a certain way can help restore or
peutic development, especially after the advent of neuro- compensate cognitive mechanisms to facilitate recovery
imaging tools and genetic manipulation techniques within (Garavan and Weierstall, 2012). To articulate this vision,
animal studies (Hall et al., 2015a). The current prevailing I will first discuss the role of cognition in contemporary
view is that addiction is a “brain disorder,” characterized by addiction theories and outline a cognition-centered
drug- or gambling-related neuroadaptations that ultimately integrative approach. Next, I will summarize cognitive
have an impact on psychological functioning (changes in neuroscience evidence showing that individual variations in
thinking, emotions, and behaviors) and social interaction core cognitive processes, particularly reward-related
(Volkow et al., 2011; Volkow et al., 2016). Critics of this processes and higher-order cognitive skills, plus
view argue that it mistakenly reproduces a physical disease disorder-specific impacts on such processes, can explain
model (inadequate for mental disorders or social both addiction vulnerability and disordered states and
constructs), lacks integration of social and environmental chronicity.
drivers, and fosters feelings of hopelessness and

Cognition and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-815298-0.00001-0 1


Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2 Cognition and Addiction

Cognition to bridge the gap between Translating the key notions of these models into a
cognitive framework can help to broaden their scope and
neurobiological models and social
impact. Essentially, contemporary neurobiological theories
accounts of addiction characterize the “backend” of addiction-related alterations.
Contemporary theories of addiction generally posit neuro- The “frontline” is the complex harmful behavior and the
biological alterations in three systems: the incentive negative social consequences that policy makers and
salience (or reward) system, the stress system, and the preventionists try to counteract and clinicians have to face
executive control system, which map into the striatum, (i.e., uncontrolled drug/gambling use, distress, deterioration
extended amygdala, and prefrontal cortex circuits, respec- of health and quality of life, lack of social support, personal
tively (Koob and Volkow, 2010). Incentive salience and social burden). In between these two, there is a range of
alterations are responsible for reward sensitization cognitive alterations involving reward valuation and
(increased motivation toward drugs/gambling resulting learning, emotion processing and affect regulation, and
from repeated administrationdi.e., instead of the expected executive control and decision-making affecting personal
habituation) and reward prediction errors (i.e., expecting and social domains (e.g., valuation of individual rewards
more reward than what is actually received). Heightened such as salary and career and social rewards such as
motivation toward drugs/gambling also occurs at the cost of friendships and relationships). And these alterations trace
reduced motivation toward natural reinforcers (Goldstein back to specific traits that interact with environmental and
and Volkow, 2002, 2011). Alterations in the stress system social factors before and during the emergence of addiction
account for persistently elevated negative affect, which can and can potentially cooperate with contextual factors in the
manifest as chronic stress and depression, as well as path to addiction recovery (Celma-Merola et al., 2018).
predominance of negative reinforcement mechanisms in the As addictions take time to develop, we can chronologically
control of behavior. That is, negative affective states map the unfolding of cognitive drivers and their interaction
become the norm, behaviors are mostly energized to try to with environmental and social factors. Cognitive-affective
get rid of unpleasant feelings, and this behavior results in traits, such as reward sensitivity, negative affectivity, and
short-term relief but long-term augmentation of the stress impulsivity/self-control, influence child and adolescent
response. Finally, executive alterations are responsible for learning and academic and social development. The inter-
the tendency to focus on immediate responses and short- action between these traits and environmental/social
term outcomes, neglecting goals and long-term conse- influences (socioeconomic disadvantage, trauma, poor
quences. Different theories emphasize two or more of these parenting, academic failure, peer pressure or social isola-
alterations and related brain systems. For example, “dual tion) predicts the onset of addictive behaviors. Once
models” focus on the imbalance between incentive salience initiated, drug use and gambling contribute to exacerbate
(ventral striatum) and executive control (dorsolateral preexisting traits, for example, they sensitize reward
prefrontal cortex [DLPFC] and anterior cingulate cortex) learning and stress responses and deteriorate cognitive
(McClure and Bickel, 2014). Stress models emphasize the control, fostering impulsive decisions and compulsive
link between negative affect (hypothalamicepituitarye behaviors. These changes contribute to worsen the
adrenal axis, amygdala, hippocampus) and poor control contextual milieu (i.e., loss of productivity, income, social
over stress-related responses (dorsal striatum, DLPFC), capital, and support) and foster a spiral of distress and
which has been ascribed to impulsivity (e.g., negative impoverishment of quality of life. Although this scenario
urgencydthe tendency to act impulsively under negative describes the “typical” developmental course of addictive
affect) or compulsivity (e.g., repetitive behaviors that “fly behaviors across adolescence and young adulthood, it can
under the radar” of top-down executive supervision) also apply to late-onset addiction, in which interpersonal
(Figee et al., 2016; Verdejo-García et al., 2007). Executive and social factors (e.g., trauma, relationship problems,
control and decision-making models highlight the unemployment) interact with cognitive characteristics
misalignment between goal-related systems (ventromedial (e.g., emotion regulation and resilience against negative
prefrontal cortex) and motivational, emotional, and affect, self-control, cognitive flexibility) as well as drug-
contextual drivers (striatum, insula, amygdala/hippocam- and gambling-related effects to generate protection against,
pus) (Bechara, 2005; Redish et al., 2008; Verdejo-García or escalation toward, addiction problems. Therefore,
and Bechara, 2009). Although the significance of these cognitive traits predict the onset of addictive behaviors via
models dwells in the way they characterize the drivers of direct and indirect pathways (e.g., interaction with
addictive behaviors, while acknowledging that these drivers academic and social factors and stressful life events). At the
are shaped by and operate in social contexts, they often same time, drug use and gambling deteriorate or exacerbate
come across as reductionist biological accounts of cognitive traits and their underlying neural processes,
behavior. giving rise to abnormal cognitive states or cognitive
Cognition: the interface between nature and nurture in addiction Chapter | 1 3

deficits, as well as contextual and social factors adolescence) and overlapping brain underpinnings
(i.e., disordered states), leading to the vicious cycle of involving lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate
addiction. regions (Vijayakumar et al., 2014a,b). Moreover, consci-
entiousness and cognitive control are strongly correlated
with intelligence quotient (IQ) and particularly fluid intel-
Evidence for the double role of ligence, probably reflecting meaningful interactions
cognition in addiction vulnerability and between trait characteristics and cognitive skills, as well as
consequences between these two and some of the social-contextual
determinants of IQ (e.g., social disadvantage correlates
The view that cognition may be the key to unlock the with low conscientiousness, poorer cognitive control and
nature and the course of addiction stands on evidence from low IQ) (Yücel et al., 2012). It could be argued that more
longitudinal studies, endophenotype-based approaches, and “affective” traits such as reward sensitivity or negative
“neurotoxicity-controlled designs” comparing people with emotionality do not fit in this pattern, but there are good
addiction versus nonaddicted recreational users and people reasons to think they do. For example, the trait of reward
with substance versus behavioral addictions. Longitudinal sensitivity overlaps with the ability to learn from reward
designs enable researchers to identify cognitive traits as feedback, which is critical for cognitive and social devel-
well as other factors that predate the onset of addictive opment as well as academic achievement (Telzer, 2016).
behaviors and to track the changes that result from drug/ In this context, longitudinal evidence has established
gambling use once initiated. It is worth noting that animal that lower general cognitive skills, lower conscientiousness
studies can also successfully address this transition (from (or higher disinhibition/impulsivity), and higher negative
trait characteristics to disorder-related states) but this affectivity predate and predict the onset of substance use/
approach will be covered in Chapter 2, and thus here I will gambling and the development of addictive disorders.
only focus on human research. Endophenotype studies rely In population-based cohorts within the general population,
on the assumption that cognition is an intermediate feature lower cognitive ability (IQ) measured in late adolescence
between the biological drivers and the complex behavioral predicts the risk of subsequent substance addiction during
manifestations of addictive disorders (Verdejo-García et al., adult life (Latvala et al., 2016). Although the association
2008). As such, the cognitive traits that predispose certain seems primarily due to genetic influences, these influences
people to addiction can be identified in their first-degree are indirectly inferred from behavioral genetic analyses,
relatives, and the cognitive differences between people which can slightly overestimate genetic versus environ-
with addiction and their unaffected relatives reflect drug mental effects (Joseph, 2002; Kendler et al., 2016). Inter-
use/gambling-related changes. Similarly, studies comparing estingly, the most predictive aspects of IQ in relation to
people with addiction and recreational users, or people with addiction are the inductive and verbal domains (strongly
substance versus behavioral addictions, can contribute to associated with cognitive control) versus the visuospatial
disentangling addiction-related traits and addiction- and technical domains. Within high-risk cohorts, which
resulting deficits. In the following sections, I summarize target individuals at greater risk of developing addiction
relevant evidence from each of these approaches. problems by virtue of family history or personality, studies
have consistently found that high levels of impulsivity/
disinhibition (or low levels of conscientiousness and
Longitudinal studies
cognitive control) measured in childhood predict the onset
A key assumption of the cognitive framework articulated in of addictive behaviors in adolescence and the development
this chapter is that certain cognitive traits and skills predate of addictive disorders (substance use disorders and
and influence onset of addictive behaviors. Before discus- gambling disorder) during young adulthood (Acheson
sing the evidence, it is important to note that although et al., 2011; Lovallo et al., 2013; Slutske et al., 2012; Tarter
“traits” and “skills” have been traditionally approached et al., 2004; Tarter et al., 2003). This association is direct
from different disciplines, i.e., personality versus cognitive and significant after controlling for other environmental and
sciences, respectively, now we know that they are mean- social factors (e.g., background, socioeconomic status,
ingfully intertwined and underpinned by common neural parenting), although it is possible that some of these factors
circuits and processes. As an example, the construct of conflate into impulsivity measures within the high-risk
“conscientiousness,” which has a long tradition in person- samples. Recently, longitudinal studies have focused on
ality science, is very similar to the concept of “cognitive adolescence as a natural model of risk within the general
control” or “disinhibition” within modern cognitive population and have incorporated multisite designs and
neuroscience (Nigg, 2017). In support of this view, these imaging measures as part of multimodal assessment
two constructs have similar developmental trajectories protocols including history (i.e., background characteris-
(peaking between 12 and 16 years during childhood and tics), personality, and cognitive/brain measures. One of
4 Cognition and Addiction

these studies, the IMAGEN consortium, found that a siblings. In the executive tests of working memory and
combination of background characteristics, the personality planning, stimulant users performed poorer than siblings
facets of conscientiousness and anxiety sensitivity, and and controls, and siblings poorer than controls. Conversely,
structural (GM:WM ratio; parenchymal volume) and inhibitory control measured with the Stop-Signal Task
cognitive-evoked functional brain features (right precentral (a motor impulsivity task) differed between the sibling pairs
gyrus activation during reward outcome and inhibition and controls but not between stimulant users and their
failure and bilateral superior frontal gyrus during reward siblings, suggesting that inhibitory control deficits are more
outcome) predict future binge drinking (a proxy of prob- of a vulnerability for and less of a consequence of addic-
lematic alcohol use and addictive behaviors) (Whelan et al., tion. Interestingly as well, there were no significant
2014). Conversely, ventromedial prefrontal cortex activa- differences between the groups on tests of memory or
tion during emotional reactivity and face recognition was a attention, suggesting that only certain cognitive functions,
better predictor of current binge drinking, probably i.e., those related to cognitive control/executive functions,
reflecting alcohol-related neuroadaptations. play a crucial role in the nature and course of addiction.
Altogether, longitudinal results support the dynamic Finally, structural neuroimaging measures revealed a set of
model sketched in the previous sections, whereby cognitive regions displaying overlapping abnormalities in both
traits and difficulties associated with cognitive control/ stimulant users and their siblings, compared with healthy
disinhibition and negative affectivity contribute to addic- controls: the putamen and the amygdala had both signifi-
tion vulnerability and are subsequently exacerbated by cantly larger volumes, while the posterior insula, the left
addictive behaviors. postcentral gyrus, and the superior temporal gyrus had
lower volumes (Ersche et al., 2012a).
Endophenotype studies Altogether, endophenotype studies support the notion
that specific trait characteristics, including heightened
In a series of studies including sibling pairs with the same impulsivity and negative affectivity and related neural
biological parents, one affected and one unaffected with systems, confer vulnerability to addiction and are subse-
stimulant use disorders, Ersche and colleagues identified quently exacerbated by drug use, leading to greater deficits
cognitive and brain features associated with addiction in executive functions and further behavioral dysregulation.
vulnerability (i.e., shared by affected and unaffected
siblings) and changes associated with stimulant use. With
Neurotoxicity-controlled studies
regard to trait impulsivity, indicated by self-reports
reflecting different aspects of impulsiveness and novelty This section summarizes the findings from two approaches:
seeking, stimulant users showed greater impulsivity than (i) comparing dependent versus recreational cocaine users
both sibling and controls, but there were also sizable and (ii) comparing substance users and gamblers. The first
differences between siblings and controls (Ersche et al., approach offers insights into which aspects of cognition
2010). Therefore, this approach shows that impulsivity is a deteriorate as a function of addiction progression (i.e., those
vulnerability factor for addiction, but crucially it is also observable in dependent vs. recreational users and
exacerbated by substance use. Interestingly, the main controls). The second approach offers insights into which
differences between siblings and controls were in the aspects of cognition are more sensitive to substance-
nonplanning aspect of impulsivity (theoretically associated induced neuroadaptations (i.e., those observable in
with the construct of conscientiousness) (Whiteside and substance users vs. gamblers) and which aspects are related
Lynam, 2001), whereas the main difference between to common vulnerability factors (overlapping between
stimulant users and siblings was in the disinhibition aspect substance users and gamblers and different from healthy
of novelty seeking, which reflects behavioral dysregulation, controls).
as manifested, for example, in uncontrolled drug use and/or
gambling. In a subsequent study that included trait Dependent versus recreational users
measures of impulsivity and compulsivity, emotional
sensitivity (negative affectivity and stress) and self- In a series of studies by Quednow and colleagues, people
evaluation (self-efficacy, locus of control, and social with cocaine addiction (meeting Diagnostic and Statistical
comparison), and cognitive measures of executive control Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM] criteria for depen-
and disinhibition, results showed that stimulant users, dence) and those with recreational cocaine use (but not
siblings, and controls differed in most of these measures dependence) underwent trait and neuropsychological
(Ersche et al., 2012b). Both sibling pairs differed from assessments. Both groups used cocaine as their primary
controls on impulsivityecompulsivity, negative affectivity, drug (>40 g per month) and were currently abstinent, but
and self-evaluation, suggesting that stimulant use exacer- critically cocaine exposure was eight times higher in the
bates the traits that are already elevated in nonaffected dependent versus the recreational group, as indicated by
Cognition: the interface between nature and nurture in addiction Chapter | 1 5

hair toxicology analyses. With regard to trait and cognitive measures of cognitive flexibility (reversal learning perfor-
aspects of impulsivity, findings showed significant differ- mance and related brain activation) (Verdejo-Garcia et al.,
ences between the two cocaine groups and controls in 2015). The perseveration error rate (an index of cognitive
general impulsiveness and novelty seeking. However, only inflexibility) was higher in cocaine users versus gamblers
attentional-impulsive traits differed between dependent and and controls. In addition, cocaine users showed less
recreational usersdthey were higher in the dependent DLPFC activation during reversal shifting (i.e., the “flexi-
group. In addition, there were no differences between the bility” trials) versus gamblers and controls, whereas both
groups on cognitive measures of disinhibition, including cocaine users and gamblers showed reduced ventrolateral
attentional and motor impulsivity tasks (Rapid Visual prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) activation compared with
Processing or Stop-Signal Task) (Vonmoos et al., 2013b). controls and no differences between each other. The
When examining executive functions, findings showed DLPFC is strongly and generally implicated in executive
significant differences between dependent users, recrea- control and seems to be specifically affected by cocaine
tional users, and controls in tests of working memory and use, whereas the VLPFC is more specifically implicated in
recall consistency (an index of strategic retrieval). goal-related cognitive control and seems to be similarly
In addition, sustained attention was affected in recreational associated with cocaine and gambling use.
users and correlated with cumulative cocaine exposure Altogether, these studies suggest that positive urgency
(Vonmoos et al., 2013a). A subsequent study in this cohort and reduced response inhibition represent vulnerability
incorporated measures of decision-making, involving both factors for substance use and behavioral addictive disor-
individual rewards and social rewards. Both dependent and ders, whereas working memory and cognitive flexibility
recreational cocaine users exhibited social decision-making deficits are specific consequences of substance use.
deficits (more self-serving behavior in social economic
exchange games). However, only dependent users showed
deficits in tasks involving individual-based rewards, Cognition at the interface between
including the Iowa Gambling Task and Delay Discounting, nature and nurture
which also correlated with cocaine dose and duration
(Hulka et al., 2014). Here, I started by proposing that cognition sits at the
Altogether, these studies suggest that impulsivity and interface between the vulnerability for and the conse-
related cognitive constructs (sustained attention) represent quences of addiction and that it is pivotal to understand the
vulnerability factors or early signs of exposure to drug use, interplay between individual-based factors (genetics,
whereas working memory and executive-memory retrieval neuroadaptations) and environmental and social drivers.
deficits are specific consequences of substance use. The evidence reviewed suggests that an array of
cognitive traits and skills, which encompasses “personal-
ity” concepts of conscientiousness and negative affectivity
Stimulant users versus gamblers
(as manifested in emotional sensitivity or negative urgency)
These studies compared the trait characteristics and the and “cognitive” constructs associated with fluid intelli-
cognitive performance of people with cocaine addiction gence, attention, (dis)inhibition, and social decision-
versus those with gambling disorder and minimal exposure making, are part of the vulnerability to substance use and
to substance use, which was restricted to alcohol abuse behavioral addictive disorders, whereas traits associated
(as per DSM-IV-TR) and smoking. In an initial study with behavioral dysregulation and cognitive deficits in
examining trait impulsivity and executive functions, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and reward-based
findings showed that the negative urgency trait (acting decision-making are associated with the consequences of
impulsively under negative affect) was higher in cocaine substance and behavioral addictions. Of note, there is an
users versus gamblers and controls and higher in gamblers important overlap and continuity between premorbid traits
than controls. Conversely, positive urgency (acting impul- and “disordered states,” given conceptual similarities and
sively under negative affect) was elevated in cocaine users shared variance between, e.g., fluid intelligence, conscien-
and gamblers versus controls (showing no differences tiousness, and executive functions.
between each other) (Albein-Urios et al., 2012). The anal- The relevance of social cognition processes in the
ysis of executive function tests showed that cocaine users vulnerability to addiction (revealed by endophenotype and
had poorer working memory than both gamblers and neurotoxicity-controlled studies) also illustrates the inter-
controls, whereas response inhibition performance active nature of cognitionedrugs/addictive productse
(measured with an attentional inhibition task, the Stroop environment relationships, as faulty social decision-making
Test) was similar among cocaine users and gamblers and in processes can facilitate deviant social behavior (reduced
both cases poorer than in controls. A subsequent study social integration or heightened sensitivity to peer pressure)
examined the same cohort using the brain and behavioral and drug/gambling use, which can ultimately exacerbate
6 Cognition and Addiction

and broaden social-cognitive deficits, fostering a vicious Garavan, H., Weierstall, K., 2012. The neurobiology of reward and
cycle of social impoverishment. cognitive control systems and their role in incentivizing health
Cognition is central to addiction development and behavior. Prev. Med. 55, S17eS23.
Goldstein, R.Z., Volkow, N.D., 2002. Drug addiction and its underlying
maintenance. Higher-order cognitive traits and processes
neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of
such as conscientiousness and cognitive control over salient
the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 159 (10), 1642e1652.
stimuli (attention) and emotions (negative affectivity) as Goldstein, R.Z., Volkow, N.D., 2011. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex
well as social-cognitive characteristics determine vulnera- in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat.
bility for addictive disorders. At the same time, substance Rev. Neurosci. 12 (11), 652.
use and behavioral addictions are associated with cognitive Hall, W., Carter, A., Forlini, C., 2015a. The brain disease model of
deficits in working memory, mental flexibility, and reward- addiction: is it supported by the evidence and has it delivered on its
based decision-making, which are essential for goal promises? Lancet Psychiatry 2 (1), 105e110.
achievement and successful social interactions. These Hall, W., Carter, A., Forlini, C., 2015b. Brain disease model of addiction:
cognitive alterations can then contribute to explaining the misplaced priorities? Lancet Psychiatry 2 (10), 867.
core manifestations of addiction (e.g., the persistence of Hulka, L., Eisenegger, C., Preller, K., Vonmoos, M., Jenni, D.,
Bendrick, K., et al., 2014. Altered social and non-social decision-
behavior despite negative consequences, recurrent cravings,
making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychol. Med.
relapses) and compromise treatment and recovery goals.
44 (5), 1015e1028.
Joseph, J., 2002. Twin studies in psychiatry and psychology: science or
References pseudoscience? Psychiatr. Q. 73 (1), 71e82.
Kendler, K., Ohlsson, H., Edwards, A., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, K.,
Acheson, A., Vincent, A.S., Sorocco, K.H., Lovallo, W.R., 2011. Greater Sundquist, J., 2016. A novel sibling-based design to quantify genetic
discounting of delayed rewards in young adults with family histories and shared environmental effects: application to drug abuse, alcohol
of alcohol and drug use disorders: studies from the Oklahoma family use disorder and criminal behavior. Psychol. Med. 46 (8), 1639e1650.
health patterns project. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 35 (9), 1607e1613. Koob, G.F., Volkow, N.D., 2010. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuro-
Albein-Urios, N., Martinez-González, J.M., Lozano, Ó., Clark, L., Verdejo- psychopharmacology 35 (1), 217.
García, A., 2012. Comparison of impulsivity and working memory Latvala, A., Kuja-Halkola, R., D’onofrio, B.M., Larsson, H.,
in cocaine addiction and pathological gambling: implications for Lichtenstein, P., 2016. Cognitive ability and risk for substance misuse
cocaine-induced neurotoxicity. Drug Alcohol Depend. 126 (1), 1e6. in men: genetic and environmental correlations in a longitudinal
Bechara, A., 2005. Decision making, impulse control and loss of will- nation-wide family study. Addiction 111 (10), 1814e1822.
power to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8 Lovallo, W.R., Farag, N.H., Sorocco, K.H., Acheson, A., Cohoon, A.J.,
(11), 1458e1463. Vincent, A.S., 2013. Early life adversity contributes to impaired
Belcher, A.M., Volkow, N.D., Moeller, F.G., Ferré, S., 2014. Personality cognition and impulsive behavior: studies from the Oklahoma Family
traits and vulnerability or resilience to substance use disorders. Trends Health Patterns Project. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 37 (4), 616e623.
Cognit. Sci. 18 (4), 211e217. McClure, S.M., Bickel, W.K., 2014. A dual-systems perspective on
Celma-Merola, J., Abella-Pons, F., Mata, F., Pedra-Pagés, G., Verdejo- addiction: contributions from neuroimaging and cognitive training.
Garcia, A., 2018. Self-changing behaviour in smoking cessation Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1327 (1), 62e78.
linked to trait and cognitive impulsivity. Addiction 113 (1), 107e112. Moeller, S.J., Goldstein, R.Z., 2014. Impaired self-awareness in human
https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13942. addiction: deficient attribution of personal relevance. Trends Cognit.
Ersche, K.D., Jones, P.S., Williams, G.B., Turton, A.J., Robbins, T.W., Sci. 18 (12), 635e641.
Bullmore, E.T., 2012a. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stim- Moore, D., 1993. Beyond Zinberg’s “social setting”: a processural view of
ulant drug addiction. Science 335 (6068), 601e604. illicit drug use. Drug Alcohol Rev. 12 (4), 413e421.
Ersche, K.D., Turton, A.J., Chamberlain, S.R., Müller, U., Bullmore, E.T., Nigg, J.T., 2017. Annual research review: on the relations among self-
Robbins, T.W., 2012b. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control,
personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. J. cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for devel-
Psychiatry 169 (9), 926e936. opmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58 (4),
Ersche, K.D., Turton, A.J., Pradhan, S., Bullmore, E.T., Robbins, T.W., 361e383.
2010. Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation- Peele, S., 1987. A moral vision of addiction: how people’s values deter-
seeking personality traits. Biol. Psychiatry 68 (8), 770e773. mine whether they become and remain addicts. J. Drug Issues 17 (2),
Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2016. Drug addiction: updating actions to 187e215.
habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 23e50. Redish, A.D., Jensen, S., Johnson, A., 2008. Addiction as vulnerabilities in
Eysenck, H., 1997. Addiction, personality and motivation. Hum. Psy- the decision process. Behav. Brain Sci. 31 (4), 461e487.
chopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 12 (2), S79. Robinson, T.E., Berridge, K.C., 2003. Addiction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54,
Figee, M., Pattij, T., Willuhn, I., Luigjes, J., van den Brink, W., 25e53. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237.
Goudriaan, A., et al., 2016. Compulsivity in obsessiveecompulsive Robinson, T.E., Berridge, K.C., 2008. The incentive sensitization theory of
disorder and addictions. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26 (5), addiction: some current issues. Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci. 363 (1507),
856e868. 3137e3146.
Cognition: the interface between nature and nurture in addiction Chapter | 1 7

Slutske, W.S., Moffitt, T.E., Poulton, R., Caspi, A., 2012. Undercontrolled Vijayakumar, N., Whittle, S., Yücel, M., Dennison, M., Simmons, J.,
temperament at age 3 predicts disordered gambling at age 32: a longi- Allen, N.B., 2014b. Prefrontal structural correlates of cognitive con-
tudinal study of a complete birth cohort. Psychol. Sci. 23 (5), 510e516. trol during adolescent development: a 4-year longitudinal study.
Tarter, R.E., Kirisci, L., Habeych, M., Reynolds, M., Vanyukov, M., 2004. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26 (5), 1118e1130.
Neurobehavior disinhibition in childhood predisposes boys to sub- Volkow, N.D., Baler, R.D., Goldstein, R.Z., 2011. Addiction: pulling at
stance use disorder by young adulthood: direct and mediated etiologic the neural threads of social behaviors. Neuron 69 (4), 599e602.
pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend. 73 (2), 121e132. Volkow, N.D., Koob, G.F., McLellan, A.T., 2016. Neurobiologic ad-
Tarter, R.E., Kirisci, L., Mezzich, A., Cornelius, J.R., Pajer, K., vances from the brain disease model of addiction. N. Engl. J. Med.
Vanyukov, M., et al., 2003. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in child- 374 (4), 363e371.
hood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. Am. J. Vonmoos, M., Hulka, L.M., Preller, K.H., Jenni, D., Baumgartner, M.R.,
Psychiatry 160 (6), 1078e1085. Stohler, R., et al., 2013a. Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and
Telzer, E.H., 2016. Dopaminergic reward sensitivity can promote dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disor-
adolescent health: a new perspective on the mechanism of ventral der, craving and early age at onset. Br. J. Psychiatry bjp. bp.
striatum activation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 57e67. 112.118091.
Verdejo-García, A., Bechara, A., 2009. A somatic marker theory of Vonmoos, M., Hulka, L.M., Preller, K.H., Jenni, D., Schulz, C.,
addiction. Neuropharmacology 56, 48e62. Baumgartner, M.R., Quednow, B.B., 2013b. Differences in self-
Verdejo-García, A., Bechara, A., Recknor, E.C., Pérez-García, M., 2007. reported and behavioral measures of impulsivity in recreational and
Negative emotion-driven impulsivity predicts substance dependence dependent cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133 (1), 61e70.
problems. Drug Alcohol Depend. 91 (2), 213e219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.05.032.
Verdejo-Garcia, A., Clark, L., Verdejo-Román, J., Albein-Urios, N., Whelan, R., Watts, R., Orr, C.A., Althoff, R.R., Artiges, E.,
Martinez-Gonzalez, J.M., Gutierrez, B., Soriano-Mas, C., 2015. Banaschewski, T., et al., 2014. Neuropsychosocial profiles of current
Neural substrates of cognitive flexibility in cocaine and gambling and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature 512 (7513), 185.
addictions. Br. J. Psychiatry 207 (2), 158e164. Whiteside, S.P., Lynam, D.R., 2001. The five factor model and impul-
Verdejo-García, A., Lawrence, A.J., Clark, L., 2008. Impulsivity as a sivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impul-
vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings sivity. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 30 (4), 669e689.
from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association Yücel, M., Fornito, A., Youssef, G., Dwyer, D., Whittle, S., Wood, S.J.,
studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32 (4), 777e810. et al., 2012. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex,
Vijayakumar, N., Whittle, S., Dennison, M., Yücel, M., Simmons, J., intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology 26 (3), 347.
Allen, N.B., 2014a. Development of temperamental effortful control Zinberg, N.E., 1984. Drug, Set, and Setting: The Basis for Controlled
mediates the relationship between maturation of the prefrontal cortex Intoxicant Use. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.
and psychopathology during adolescence: a 4-year longitudinal study.
Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 30e43.
Chapter 2

From impulses to compulsions


Kavya Raj1 and Antonio Verdejo-Garcia2
1
Brain, Mind and Society Research Hub, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia; 2School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for
Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Introduction outcomes of such behaviors are negative (Zapata et al.,


2010). Environmental stimuli associated with the effects of
Addiction can be conceptualized as a loss of control over drug self-administration gain incentive salience via
drug-seeking and -taking behavior, whereby drug use that Pavlovian conditioning, and drug-seeking responses are
was initially voluntary and recreational progresses to a automatically elicited by conditioned S-R loops (Everitt
habit, and ultimately a compulsion, continuing to persist and Robbins, 2005). Drug-seeking and -taking behavior
despite harmful consequences (Belin et al., 2013; Everitt controlled by this S-R process is severed from the value of
and Robbins, 2005, 2013, 2016). It is hypothesized that this the drug and can operate without full engagement of
behavioral transition from impulsive choices to compulsive cognitive control processes (Belin et al., 2013). Whether
drug seeking and taking is underpinned by a shift in the drug use is controlled by A-O or S-R mechanisms, at a
neural loci of control from the ventral to the dorsal striatum behavioral level, can be tested through outcome devalua-
(Belin and Everitt, 2008; Everitt and Robbins, 2013, 2016), tion procedures to observe whether instrumental respond-
as well as a progression from prefrontal cortical to ing for drug rewards changes as the outcome value changes
subcortical striatal control (Chen et al., 2013; Everitt and (Dickinson et al., 2002).
Robbins, 2016; Murray et al., 2012; Renteria et al., 2018). The shift from A-O to S-R mechanisms mediating drug
This chapter summarizes the animal and human evidence use is documented by a consistent body of animal research.
that sustains this notion, starting with preclinical studies, In a sophisticated series of experiments, Olmstead et al.
continuing with human neuroimaging and cognitive (2001) utilized a heterogenous chained schedule of intra-
studies, and concluding with ideas for future directions. venous cocaine self-administration in which rats performed
an initial drug-seeking response, which in turn provided
Animal models of drug-seeking habits access to a secondary drug-taking response that resulted in
delivery of cocaine. After briefly establishing cocaine self-
and compulsions administration, drug-seeking responses were shown to be
A key approach to understanding the transition from sensitive to devaluation when the taking link of the chained
voluntary to compulsive drug use is through the lens of schedule was extinguished, indicating that at an initial stage
Pavlovian and instrumental learning (Robbins and Everitt; of cocaine seeking, behavior is goal-directed and influenced
Everitt and Robbins, 2013). According to this perspective, by the value of the response outcome. To replicate and
initial drug use is goal-directed and controlled by action- extend this finding, Zapata et al. (2001) utilized a modified
outcome (A-O) mechanisms. In this context, drug-related chained schedule of intravenous cocaine self-administration
actions are sensitive to changes in outcome value, and and demonstrated that, following a moderate number of
thus drug seeking can stop if drug value decreases or is training sessions, initial cocaine seeking is goal directed
outweighed by alternative reinforcers (Everitt and Robbins, and sensitive to devaluation. However, as sessions
2005). However, as drug use escalates, a parallel instru- increased and the cocaine-seeking experience was
mental learning mechanism comes to dominate responding, extended, 43% of the sample continued to respond after the
and behavior shifts to a stimuluseresponse (S-R) habit outcome had been devalued, indicating that drug-seeking
process that is insensitive to outcome devaluation, i.e., behavior had become habitual for this subgroup and was
precludes stopping or changing behavior, even when the operating through an S-R mechanism. Similarly, while

Cognition and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-815298-0.00002-2 9


Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
10 Cognition and Addiction

instrumental responding for alcohol at early stages of (Letchworth et al., 2001; Porrino et al., 2004). In rats with
training is goal-directed, after 8 week of training, behav- well-established habitual cocaine-seeking behavior,
ioral control over alcohol seeking shifts to S-R habit presentation of drug-related cues during cocaine seeking
mechanisms and is no longer sensitive to devaluation results in marked increases of extracellular dopamine in the
(Corbit et al., 2012). These findings illustrate how escala- dorsal striatum but not in the nucleus accumbens core or
tion of drug use results in resistance of drug seeking to shell (Ito et al., 2002). Moreover, dopamine receptor
outcome devaluation, and thus habitual S-R behaviors. antagonist infusion into this region of the dorsal striatum in
While habitual drug-seeking and -taking behavior alone which elevated extracellular dopamine is detected signifi-
cannot provide a comprehensive explanation for addiction, cantly reduces S-R driven, cocaine-seeking behavior
it provides the foundation for compulsive drug use (Everitt (Belin and Everitt, 2008; Vanderschuren et al., 2005). The
and Robbins, 2016). Perseveration of habitual drug-seeking same infusion in the nucleus accumbens, however,
behavior despite drastically negative outcomes, such as produces no effect on habitual cocaine seeking (Vander-
deterioration of health and family bonds or loss of social schuren et al., 2005), indicating that once behavior has
support and employment, is a core aspect of addiction become S-R dominant, ventral striatal regions that are
(Ersche et al., 2011). Aversive conditioning studies in rats entwined with A-O mechanisms lose influence over actions
show that, after sufficient training and exposure, respond- that have become compulsive.
ing for oral cocaine (Miles et al., 2003) and alcohol In addition to the ventral striatum progressively losing
(Dickinson et al., 2002) perseveres even after devaluation control over drug-seeking behavior, the transition from
through nausea-inducing lithium chloride injections voluntary to compulsive drug use is further underpinned by
(Nachman, 1963). Recent evidence suggests that as few as a transition within competing regions of the dorsal striatum:
16 training sessions are sufficient to render alcohol-seeking from the posterior dorsomedial striatum (pDMS, early
behavior unresponsive to devaluation through aversive acquisition) to the anterior dorsolateral striatum (aDLS,
conditioning (López et al., 2016). Drug reinforcers are habitual drug seeking) (Balleine et al., 2009; Murray et al.,
significantly more resistant to aversive conditioning than 2012; Yin et al., 2004). Electrophysiological evidence from
natural foodebased reinforcers, for which responding is rodents performing motivational tasks shows that early
more readily reverted to an A-O process, perhaps due to acquisition correlates with DMS activity, but as training
evolutionary reasons (Dickinson et al., 2002; Miles et al., extends and behavior becomes S-R dominant, DLS activity
2003). This indicates that drug reinforcers induce an S-R increases (Thorn et al., 2010). At early stages of cocaine
habit process far more rapidly than natural rewards. self-administration, pharmacologically disabling the pDMS
Moreover, drug-induced difficulty to shift between S-R and by infusing that region with a D2 agonist infusion reduces
A-O processes suggests that a dominant habit system may initial cocaine seeking (Murray et al., 2012). At the same
become “compulsive” and trigger automatic drug-seeking time point, deactivating the aDLS with the same D2 agonist
behaviors under aversive conditions. infusion has no effect on cocaine seeking, indicating that
early A-Oedriven, drug seeking is dependent on the DMS
Neural circuits: transitioning from the and not the DLS. However, once self-administration is
well-established and S-R driven, cocaine-seeking behavior
ventral to dorsal striatum can only be reduced by disabling the aDLS, whereas
Converging evidence from animal research shows that the disabling the pDMS had no effect on behavior (Murray
transition from voluntary to habitual and eventually et al., 2012). A similar study utilizing microinjections of
compulsive use is neurally underpinned by a progression lidocaine to deactivate striatal regions has shown that
from the ventral to the dorsal striatum, via dopaminergic disabling the DLS in cocaine-seeking rats can successfully
circuits (Belin and Everitt, 2008; Everitt and Robbins, renew A-O control over compulsive cocaine-seeking
2005; Vanderschuren et al., 2005; Vanderschuren and behavior (Zapata et al., 2010), signifying that, within the
Everitt, 2005). This transition has been neatly revealed in dorsal striatum, the DMS relinquishes control to the DLS as
animal models of stimulant addiction. Early acquisition of drug use becomes S-R driven. Similar findings are reported
cocaine seeking depends on the nucleus accumbens core, a for alcohol seeking (Corbit et al., 2012). Deactivating the
region of the ventral striatum; lesions to this area disrupt DMS at early stages of alcohol exposure shifts control to
establishment of drug-seeking behavior (Ito et al., 2004). the habit system, preventing behavior from being sensitive
Yet, as cocaine exposure escalates and becomes chronic, to devaluation. However, after prolonged alcohol exposure,
drug-related neuroadaptationsdsuch as changes in meta- deactivation of the DLS (yet not DMS) is needed to reen-
bolic activity and density of dopamine transporter binding gage the goal-directed system. Moreover, this rich body of
sitesdincreasingly extend from the ventral striatum to research suggests that control over actions occurs through
encompass dorsal striatal regions, the caudate and putamen competing goal-directed (DMS) and habit (DLS) systems,
From impulses to compulsions Chapter | 2 11

and that while typically the DMS and DLS are simulta- cocaine despite electric foot shocks, extended cocaine
neously able to control the same behavior (Renteria et al., self-administration induced substantial hypoactivity in the
2018; Yin et al., 2006), this competition may be repeatedly prelimbic cortex. Furthermore, in vivo optogenetic stimu-
dominated by the DLS in substance use disorders, lation of the prelimbic cortex significantly diminished
ultimately resulting in compulsive drug use. cocaine-seeking responses. In contrast, in vivo optogenetic
It is important to note that while control of behavior inhibition of the prelimbic cortex during cocaine
may devolve to the DLS, ventral regions of the striatum self-administration significantly increased cocaine-seeking
may continue to influence and interact with the dorsal responses. More recent work by Limpens et al. (2014)
striatum. Belin and Everitt (2008) showed that combining a provides comparable findings and directly implicates the
unilateral lesion of the nucleus accumbens core with prelimbic cortex in loss of executive control. Inactivation of
contralateral dorsolateral striatum dopamine receptor the prelimbic cortex significantly reduces a previously
blockade reduces habitual cocaine intake, yet leaving newly conditioned suppression of cocaine seeking in rats, thereby
learned instrumental-seeking responses unaffected (Belin enabling the expression of compulsive drug-seeking
and Everitt, 2008; Everitt and Robbins, 2016). Therefore, behavior. Hypofunction of the prelimbic cortex has also
notwithstanding the progressive transition from ventral to been reported in opiate reward learning, wherein down-
dorsal striatal regions, ongoing interactions between the regulation of prefrontal-excitatory N-methyl D-aspartate
nucleus accumbens core and DLS will have a critical role in receptors substantially increases sensitivity to the
the formation and persistence of compulsive drug-seeking rewarding effects of opiate administration (Bishop et al.,
behavior. 2010). Together, these results not only indicate that
extended drug use induces marked neuroplastic changes in
Devolving from prefrontal to striatal prefrontal excitability but also that these changes are
critically involved in the expression of compulsive drug-
control seeking behavior. Moreover, when considered alongside
Along with the progressive strengthening of DLS-driven previous animal research (Belin and Everitt, 2008; Zapata
S-R habit processes, compulsive drug use is concurrently et al., 2010), these findings support the hypothesis that
mediated by weakened prefrontal control over striatal compulsive drug-seeking behavior may be driven by
regions (Koob and Volkow, 2010; Renteria et al., 2018). a combination of DLS hyperactivation and prelimbic/
Habitual behavior that occurs in the face of aversive con- frontal hypoactivation, whereby weakened prefrontal
ditions would typically promote the transition back to A-O activity enables the striatum and habit system to maintain
processes and encourage ventral striatal regions to regain control.
control of behavior (Everitt and Robbins, 2016; Murray In addition to the prelimbic cortex, hypoactivation of
et al., 2012); however, in substance use disorders, drug- the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (key region for decision-
seeking behavior perseveres despite harmful outcomes making in humans) has also been implicated in compul-
(Ersche et al., 2011). Dorsomedial and ventral striatal sive drug-seeking behavior (Renteria et al., 2018). Habitual
regions that are integral to the functioning of the ethanol use in mice induces marked reductions in OFC
goal-directed system receive prefrontal cortical projections, excitability (Renteria et al., 2018). The OFC directly
and prefrontal cortex (PFC) hypofunction has been reported projects onto the DMS and is critically involved in excit-
across substance use disorders (Goldstein and Volkow, atory circuits of the goal-directed system (Renteria et al.,
2011). Thus, a contributing factor to the development and 2018). Ethanol-induced suppression of OFC excitability
maintenance of compulsive drug seeking may be hypo- reduces glutamatergic transmission from the OFC to the
function of prefrontal regions and weakened cortical DMS via the direct output pathway and thus disrupts
projections to the striatum, which enables the DLS habit goal-directed control over ethanol-seeking behavior
system to remain dominant in controlling behavior (Renteria et al., 2018). Importantly, stimulating the OFC to
(Limpens et al., 2014). increase excitatory activity in the OFC-striatal circuit
Animal studies indicate that hypofunction of the notably reinstates goal-directed control over previous S-R
prelimbic cortexdwhich is homologous to the dorsolateral ethanol-seeking behavior (Renteria et al., 2018). Yet
prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in humans (Bizon et al., 2012; again, these findings indicate that habit-driven drug seeking
Koob and Volkow, 2016)dmay be tightly linked to may stem from drug-induced OFC hypoactivation and
compulsive drug seeking (Chen et al., 2013; Bishop et al., subsequent disruption of communication to striatal regions
2010; Limpens et al., 2014). In fact, lesions to the prelimbic critical for goal-directed control. Collectively, animal
area enable the formation of inflexible S-R habits (Killcross research across models of various substance use disorders
and Coutureau, 2003). A seminal study by Chen et al. indicates that compulsive drug-seeking behavior may arise
(2013) demonstrated that, in rats that compulsively seek from a complex interaction between an intrastriatal shift
12 Cognition and Addiction

toward dorsolateral control and PFC hypoactivation, which which may suggest that greater dorsal striatal volume
together enable rigid S-R habit processes to continue con- increases risk for drug-related S-R conditioning and
trolling and perpetuating drug-seeking behavior under substance use disorders (Everitt and Robbins, 2016).
harmful or aversive conditions. Together, these findings suggest that dorsal striatal regions
(the putamen and caudate) are crucial to S-R driven
habitual craving responses in cocaine users, and that
Translating animal models to dopamine dysfunction within these regions may play a
understand compulsivity in people with fundamental role in the severity of drug use.
Similar findings have been reported in alcohol-
substance use disorders dependent individuals (Sjoerds et al., 2013, 2014;
Advancing from animal research, human studies also Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010). Alcohol-related cues provoke
indicate that compulsive drug use may be underpinned by greater cue-induced activation in the left dorsal striatum in
an over reliance on S-R mechanisms as well as prefrontal- heavy drinkers (Schulte et al., 2012; Vollstädt-Klein et al.,
striatal adaptations (Ersche et al., 2011; Sjoerds et al., 2013; 2010). In addition, Vollstädt-Klein et al. (2010) found that
Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010). In this section, we focus on while heavy drinkers show more cue-evoked activation in
cue-reactivity studies, in which drug-related cues are used the dorsal striatum, light drinkers show a stronger response
to provoke a conditioned craving response analogous to in the ventral striatum. Moreover, they showed a significant
habitual S-R behaviors (Koob and Volkow, 2010; Pickens positive relationship between activation of the dorsal
et al., 2011; Sinha and Li, 2007; Tricomi et al., 2009; Yoder striatum and scores on an obsessive-compulsive scale
et al., 2009). Craving (a compelling desire to use the drug) (a proxy of compulsivity), whereas ventral striatal activa-
is a paramount clinical phenomenon and a proxy of tion had a negative relationship with the same scale. This
compulsive drug use in clinical studies (Skinner and Aubin, study provides persuasive support for the notion of a
2010; Tiffany et al., 2012). The cue-reactivity procedure ventral to dorsal striatal control shift as alcohol use
enables researchers to observe behavioral and neural becomes more severe and compulsive (Vollstädt-Klein
responses to drug-related cues, providing an indirect et al., 2010). More recent work by Sjoerds et al. (2013,
measure of the severity of compulsive and automatic 2014) has shown that, during an instrumental learning task
responding in drug users (Carter and Tiffany, 1999; designed to explore potential imbalances between goal-
Tricomi et al., 2009). directed and habit learning processes, alcohol-dependent
A consistent finding that emerges in cue-reactivity individuals demonstrate a significant overreliance on S-R
studies across various substance use populations is the habit learning, resulting in poor task performance
importance of the dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen in outcomes. Additionally, poor task performance coincides
humans) in the transition from recreational to compulsive with decreased activation of brain regions implicated in
use (Ersche et al., 2011, 2012a,b; Sjoerds et al., 2013; goal-directed processes and increased activation in habit
Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006; Zhou et al., 2018). learning brain regions, namely the posterior putamen
Early functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and (analogous to the DLS in rodents). These results provide
positron emission tomography studies in cocaine users considerable evidence to indicate an overactive and
established that cue-induced craving is associated with inflexible habit system in alcohol-dependent individuals,
increased metabolic activity, dopamine release, and dopa- which appears to heavily depend on dorsal striatal regions
mine receptor occupancy in the dorsal striatum (Garavan (Sjoerds et al., 2013). Furthermore, duration of alcohol
et al., 2000; Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006). dependence was strongly associated with greater
Interestingly, the caudate (which is the human homologue cue-induced activation of the posterior putamen (Sjoerds
of the DMS in rodents; Balleine and O’Doherty, 2009) et al., 2014), further supporting the hypothesized ventral to
response to sexually explicit stimuli is blunted in cocaine dorsal striatal shift as behavior advances toward addiction.
users, suggesting that S-R tendencies are sensitized to drug As the putamen is analogous to the DLS in rodents and the
cues and relatively insensitive to natural reinforcers caudate analogous to the DMS, this is a crucial detail for
(Garavan et al., 2000). Furthermore, increases in dopamine the S-R habit hypothesis first proposed by Everitt and
receptor occupancy (Wong et al., 2006) and dopamine Robbins (2005), which would predict greater putamen
release (Volkow et al., 2006) in the dorsal striatum are adaptations as seen in rodent studies (Everitt and Robbins,
proportionate to increases in cue-induced cocaine cravings 2016).
and also corresponded with addiction severity (Volkow Changes in neural activation and adaptations in the
et al., 2006). In addition to functional differences, structural dorsal striatum are also observed in nicotine- (McClernon
MRI studies indicate that, when compared with their et al., 2009) and cannabis-use disorder groups (Zhou et al.,
noncocaine using siblings, cocaine-dependent individuals 2018, 2019). Following a 24-h period of abstinence in adult
have enlarged putamen volumes (Ersche et al., 2011, 2013), tobacco smokers, smoking cues elicited greater activation
From impulses to compulsions Chapter | 2 13

in the dorsal striatum, specifically the putamen, relative to hypoactivation may strongly contribute to the perpetuation
neutral cues (McClernon et al., 2009). Participants with of compulsive drug-seeking behavior, wherein multiple
cannabis dependence, compared with drug-naïve controls, prefrontal regions are critically involved in enabling dor-
show an aberrant pattern of functional connectivity sostriatal S-R control of behavior.
involving less efficient communication between both
ventral and dorsal striatum and the PFC (Zhou et al., 2018). Recommendations for future research
More specifically, when comparing dependent versus
nondependent cannabis users exposed to a cue-reactivity Animal experiments and cross-sectional human neuro-
paradigm, dependent users alone demonstrated heightened imaging studies strongly support the role of dorsal striatal
dorsal striatal activation during cue exposure (Zhou et al., control and weakened prefrontal regulation in compulsive
2019). drug use. However, human studies have not yet fully
Cumulatively, the results of human imaging cue- demonstrated the proposed dynamic shift between im-
reactivity studies support the consensus within animal pulses and compulsions or ventral to dorsal striatal control
literature and suggest that dorsostriatal adaptations are that putatively occurs in each individual over time. We
involved in compulsive drug use. In addition to evidence of propose two ways to trace this dynamic shift in the future.
dorsostriatal changes, human studies also implicate hypo- One is through longitudinal studies. Assessing PFC-
activation in multiple prefrontal regions across substance striatal function and related phenotypes (cue-conditioned
use disorders (Goldstein and Volkow, 2011; Zilverstand craving, impulsivity, and compulsivity) among children
et al., 2018; see Chapter 3 in this book), much the same as and adolescents before exposure to drugs and then
animal research. Individuals with alcohol use disorder throughout adolescence and young/mid adulthood would
exhibit decreased engagement of the ventromedial PFC compellingly tell us if neural and cognitive transitions
(encompassing the OFC), which directly projects to the similar to those observed in animals occur over the course
ventral striatum, indicating OFC-striatal dysfunction of human addiction. The advent of new ambitious and
(Sjoerds et al., 2013). Similarly, individuals with alcohol exciting longitudinal research initiatives, such as the
use disorder who relapse into compulsive use show hypo- Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study
activation of the medial PFC during a goal-directed (https://abcdstudy.org/), has created a unique opportunity
decision-making task (Sebold et al., 2017), again suggest- to pursue this otherwise incredibly complex challenge.
ing disrupted prefrontal control of behavior. Structural The ABCD study will track the neurobiological and
imaging research by Ersche et al. (2013) demonstrates behavioral development of >11,000 children and includes
significant reductions in OFC gray matter volume in detailed measures of cognition, brain function, and drug
compulsive cocaine users, while recreational cocaine users use, providing a unique opportunity to address longitudi-
exhibit increased OFC volume. The increase in gray matter nal research questions. The study also has a remarkable
volume for recreational users may reflect a protective factor open data sharing policy, opening endless possibilities for
or potential resilience against progressing to compulsive early career researchers. Another potential approach is
drug use (Ersche et al., 2013). Lastly, more recent using rapidly evolving epigenetic tools. Discovering
preliminary research aiming to directly translate animal epigenetic markers of ventral and dorsal striatal nuclei
studies on hypoactivation of the prelimbic cortex (which is gene expression and conducting intraindividual analyses
the homolog of the human DLPFC) and optogenetic of changes in these markers over the course of substance
stimulation has utilized repetitive transcranial magnetic use and addiction, paralleled by detailed phenotyping,
stimulant (rTMS) to electrically stimulate the DLPFC in would be another persuasive (although currently futuris-
human cocaine users (Terraneo et al., 2016). rTMS of the tic) approach.
DLPFC significantly improved symptomology in
individuals with cocaine use disorder, as well as substan- References
tially reducing craving for cocaine, signifying that as with Balleine, B.W., O’Doherty, J.P., 2009. Human and rodent homologies in
in vivo optogenetic stimulation of the prelimbic cortex in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and
animals reducing cocaine-seeking responses, electrical habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35 (1), 48e69.
stimulation of the human homolog has similar potential to Balleine, B.W., Liljeholm, M., Ostlund, S.B., 2009. The integrative
reduce drug-seeking behaviors (Terraneo et al., 2016). function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behav.
For a more detailed discussion on brain stimulation Brain Res. 199 (1), 43e52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034.
findings, see Chapter 22 of this book. Belin, D., Everitt, B.J., 2008. Cocaine seeking habits depend upon
Altogether, findings from structural MRI, functional dopamine-dependent serial connectivity linking the ventral with the
dorsal striatum. Neuron 57 (3), 432e441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
imaging using tasks of cue-reactivity (dorsal striatal
neuron.2007.12.019.
hyperactivity) and decision-making (PFC hyporeactivity),
and emerging brain stimulation studies indicate that PFC
14 Cognition and Addiction

Belin, D., Belin-Rauscent, A., Murray, J.E., Everitt, B.J., 2013. Addiction: for drug users and drug stimuli. Am. J. Psychiatry 157 (11),
failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits. Curr. Opin. Neu- 1789e1798. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1789.
robiol. 23 (4), 564e574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.025. Goldstein, R.Z., Volkow, N.D., 2011. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex
Bishop, S.F., Lauzon, N.M., Bechard, M., Gholizadeh, S., Laviolette, S.R., in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat.
2010. NMDA receptor hypofunction in the prelimbic cortex increases Rev. Neurosci. 12, 652. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3119.
sensitivity to the rewarding properties of opiates via dopaminergic and Ito, R., Dalley, J.W., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 2002. Dopamine release
amygdalar substrates. Cerebr. Cortex 21 (1), 68e80. https://doi.org/ in the dorsal striatum during cocaine-seeking behavior under the
10.1093/cercor/bhq060. control of a drug-associated cue. J. Neurosci. 22 (80), 1e7. https://doi.
Bizon, J., C Foster, T., E Alexander, G., Glisky, E., 2012. Characterizing org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-06247.2002.
cognitive aging of working memory and executive function in animal Ito, R., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 2004. Differential control over
models. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi. cocaine-seeking behavior by nucleus accumbens core and shell. Nat.
2012.00019. Neurosci. 7, 389e397. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1217.
Carter, B.L., Tiffany, S.T., 1999. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in Killcross, S., Coutureau, E., 2003. Coordination of actions and habits in
addiction research. Addiction 94 (3), 327e340. https://doi.org/10. the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cerebr. Cortex 13 (4), 400e408.
1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.4.400.
Chen, B.T., Yau, H.-J., Hatch, C., Kusumoto-Yoshida, I., Cho, S.L., Koob, G.F., Volkow, N.D., 2010. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuro-
Hopf, F.W., Bonci, A., 2013. Rescuing cocaine-induced prefrontal psychopharmacology 35, 217. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.
cortex hypoactivity prevents compulsive cocaine seeking. Nature 496, 110.
359. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12024. Koob, G.F., Volkow, N.D., 2016. Neurobiology of addiction: A neuro-
Corbit, L.H., Nie, H., Janak, P.H., 2012. Habitual alcohol seeking: time circuitry analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry 3 (8), 760e773. https://doi.
course and the contribution of subregions of the dorsal striatum. Biol. org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8.
Psychiatry 72 (5), 389e395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012. Letchworth, S.R., Porrino, L.J., Smith, H.R., Friedman, D.P., Nader, M.A.,
02.024. 2001. Progression of changes in dopamine transporter binding site
Dickinson, A., Wood, N., Smith, J.W., 2002. Alcohol seeking by rats: density as a result of cocaine self-administration in Rhesus monkeys.
action or habit? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (3), 965e985. https://doi.org/10. J. Neurosci. 21 (8), 2799e2807. https://doi.org/10.1523/
1080/027249902440001. JNEUROSCI.21-08-02799.2001.
Ersche, K.D., Roiser, J.P., Abbott, S., Craig, K.J., Mller, U., Suckling, J., Limpens, H.W., Damsteegt, J., Broekhoven, R.H., Voorn, M.,
et al., 2011. Response perseveration in stimulant dependence is Vanderschuren, P.L., 2014. Pharmacological inactivation of the pre-
associated with striatal dysfunction and can be ameliorated by a D2/ limbic cortex emulates compulsive reward seeking in rats. Brain
3receptor agonist. Biol. Psychiatry 70 (8), 754e762. https://doi.org/ Research 1628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.045.
10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.033. López, M., Soto, A., Bura, S., 2016. Alcohol seeking by rats becomes
Ersche, K.D., Jones, P.S., Williams, G.B., Turton, A.J., Robbins, T.W., habitual after prolonged training. Psicothema 28 (4), 421e427. https://
Bullmore, E.T., 2012a. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stim- doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.114.
ulant drug addiction. Science 335 (6068), 601e604. https://doi.org/10. McClernon, F.J., Kozink, R.V., Lutz, A.M., Rose, J.E., 2009. 24-h
1126/science.1229223. smoking abstinence potentiates fMRI-BOLD activation to smoking
Ersche, K.D., Turton, A.J., Chamberlain, S.R., Müller, U., Bullmore, E.T., cues in cerebral cortex and dorsal striatum. Psychopharmacology 204
Robbins, T.W., 2012b. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive (1), 25e35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1436-9.
personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. j. Miles, F.J., Everitt, B.J., Dickinson, A., 2003. Oral cocaine seeking by
psychiatry 169 (9), 926e936. https://doi.org/10.1176/ rats: action or habit? Behav. Neurosci. 117 (5), 927e938. https://doi.
appi.ajp.2012.11091421. org/10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.927.
Ersche, K.D., Jones, P.S., Williams, G.B., Smith, D.G., Bullmore, E.T., Murray, J.E., Belin, D., Everitt, B.J., 2012. Double dissociation of the
Robbins, T.W., 2013. Distinctive personality traits and neural corre- dorsomedial and dorsolateral striatal control over the acquisition and
lates associated with stimulant drug use versus familial risk of stim- performance of cocaine seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology 37 (11),
ulant dependence. Biol. Psychiatry 74 (2), 137e144. https://doi.org/ 2456e2466. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.104.
10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.016. Nachman, M., 1963. Learned aversion to the taste of lithium chloride and
Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2005. Neural systems of reinforcement for generalization to other salts. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 56 (2),
drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8 343e349.
(11), 1481e1489. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1579. Olmstead, M.C., Lafond, M.V., Everitt, B.J., Dickinson, A., 2001. Cocaine
Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2013. From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: seeking by rats is a goal-directed action. Behav. Neurosci. 115 (2),
devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. 394e402.
Rev. 37 (9), 1946e1954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02. Pickens, C.L., Airavaara, M., Theberge, F., Fanous, S., Hope, B.T.,
010. Shaham, Y., 2011. Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving.
Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2016. Drug addiction: updating actions to Trends Neurosci. 34 (8), 411e420. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.
habits to compulsions ten years on, vol. 67. Social Science Research 1016/j.tins.2011.06.001.
Network, pp. 23e50. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414- Porrino, L.J., Daunais, J.B., Smith, H.R., Nader, M.A., 2004. The
033457. expanding effects of cocaine: studies in a nonhuman primate model of
Garavan, H., Pankiewicz, J., Bloom, A., Cho, J.K., Sperry, L., Ross, T.J., cocaine self-administration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27 (8),
et al., 2000. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity 813e820. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.013.
From impulses to compulsions Chapter | 2 15

Renteria, R., Baltz, E.T., Gremel, C.M., 2018. Chronic alcohol exposure Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.-J., Telang, F., Fowler, J.S., Logan, J.,
disrupts top-down control over basal ganglia action selection to pro- Childress, A.-R., et al., 2006. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal
duce habits. Nat. Commun. 9 (1), 1e11. https://doi.org/10.1038/ striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J. Neurosci. 26
s41467-017-02615-9. (24), 6583e6588. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.
Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 1999. Drug addiction: bad habits add up. 2006.
Nature 398 (6728), 567e570. https://doi.org/10.1038/19208. Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O.,
Schulte, T., Oberlin, B.G., Kareken, D.A., Marinkovic, K., Müller- Ende, G., et al., 2010. Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is
Oehring, E.M., Meyerhoff, D.J., Tapert, S., 2012. How acute and characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal
chronic alcohol consumption affects brain networks: insights from striatum. Addiction 105 (10), 1741e1749. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
multimodal neuroimaging. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 36 (12), 1360-0443.2010.03022.x.
2017e2027. Wong, D.F., Kuwabara, H., Schretlen, D.J., Bonson, K.R., Zhou, Y.,
Sebold, M., Nebe, S., Garbusow, M., Guggenmos, M., Schad, D.J., Nandi, A., et al., 2006. Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in
Beck, A., et al., 2017. When habits are dangerous: alcohol expec- human striatum during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Neuro-
tancies and habitual decision making predict relapse in alcohol psychopharmacology 31 (12), 2716e2727. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.
dependence. Biol. Psychiatry 82 (11), 847e856. https://doi.org/https:// npp.1301194.
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.019. Yin, H.H., Knowlton, B.J., Balleine, B.W., 2004. Lesions of dorsolateral
Sinha, R., Li, C.S., 2007. Imaging stress- and cue-induced drug and striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in
alcohol craving: association with relapse and clinical implications. instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 181e189. https://doi.org/
Drug Alcohol Rev. 26 (1), 25e31. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x.
09595230601036960. Yin, H.H., Knowlton, B.J., Balleine, B.W., 2006. Inactivation of dorso-
Sjoerds, Z., de Wit, S., van den Brink, W., Robbins, T.W., lateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the actioneoutcome
Beekman, A.T.F., Penninx, B.W.J.H., Veltman, D.J., 2013. Behav- contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 166 (2),
ioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in 189e196. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.012.
alcohol-dependent patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e337. https://doi.org/ Yoder, K.K., Morris, E.D., Constantinescu, C.C., Cheng, T.-E.,
10.1038/tp.2013.107. Normandin, M.D., O’Connor, S.J., Kareken, D.A., 2009. When what
Sjoerds, Z., van den Brink, W., Beekman, A.T.F., Penninx, B.W.J.H., you see isn’t what you get: alcohol cues, alcohol administration,
Veltman, D.J., 2014. Cue reactivity is associated with duration and prediction error, and human striatal dopamine. Alcohol Clin. Exp.
severity of alcohol dependence: an fMRI study. PLoS One 9 (1), 1e9. Res. 33 (1), 139e149. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084560. 00821.x.
Skinner, M.D., Aubin, H.J., 2010. Craving’s place in addiction theory: Zapata, A., Minney, V.L., Shippenberg, T.S., 2001. Shift from goal-
contributions of the major models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34 (4), directed to habitual cocaine seeking after prolonged experience in
606e623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.024. rats. J. Neurosci. 30 (46), 15457e15463. https://doi.org/10.1523/
Terraneo, A., Leggio, L., Saladini, M., Ermani, M., Bonci, A., JNEUROSCI.4072-10.2010.
Gallimberti, L., 2016. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of dorsolat- Zapata, A., Minney, V.L., Shippenberg, T.S., 2010. Shift from goal-
eral prefrontal cortex reduces cocaine use: a pilot study. Eur. Neuro- directed to habitual cocaine seeking after prolonged experience in
psychopharmacol. 26 (1), 37e44. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10. rats. J. Neurosci. 30 (46), 15457e15463. https://doi.org/10.1523/
1016/j.euroneuro.2015.11.011. JNEUROSCI.4072-10.2010.Shift.
Thorn, C.A., Atallah, H., Howe, M., Graybiel, A.M., 2010. Differential Zhou, F., Zimmermann, K., Xin, F., Scheele, D., Dau, W., Banger, M.,
dynamics of activity changes in dorsolateral and dorsomedial striatal et al., 2018. Shifted balance of dorsal versus ventral striatal commu-
loops during learning. Neuron 66, 781e795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. nication with frontal reward and regulatory regions in cannabis-
neuron.2010.04.036. dependent males. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39 (12), 5062e5073. https://
Tiffany, S.T., Friedman, L., Greenfield, S.F., Hasin, D.S., Jackson, R., doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24345.
2012. Beyond drug use: a systematic consideration of other outcomes Zhou, X., Zimmermann, K., Xin, F., Derck, R., Sassmannshausen, A.,
in evaluations of treatments for substance use disorders. Addiction 107 Scheele, D., et al., 2019. Cue-reactivity in the ventral striatum char-
(4), 709e718. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03581.x. acterizes heavy cannabis use, whereas reactivity in the dorsal striatum
Tricomi, E., Balleine, B.W., O’Doherty, J.P., 2009. A specific role for pos- mediates dependent use. BioRxiv (Preprint). https://doi.org/10.1101/
terior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29 516385.
(11), 2225e2232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06796.x. Zilverstand, A., Huang, A.S., Alia-Klein, N., Goldstein, R.Z., 2018.
Vanderschuren, L.J.M.J., Everitt, B.J., 2005. Behavioral and neural Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in
mechanisms of compulsive drug seeking. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526 human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron 98 (5), 886e903.
(1e3), 77e88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048.
Vanderschuren, L.J.M.J., Di Ciano, P., Everitt, B.J., 2005. Involvement of
the dorsal striatum in cue-controlled cocaine seeking. J. Neurosci. 25
(38), 8665e8670. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0925-05.2005.
Chapter 3

Dual models of drug addiction: the


impaired response inhibition and
salience attribution model
Anna Zilverstand1 and Rita Z. Goldstein2
1
Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States; 2Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Icahn School of
Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, United States

Dual models of addiction that the impairments of two neuropsychological


functionsdimpaired response inhibition and salience
Addiction is a chronically relapsing disorder, characterized attributiondand their underlying neural substrates
by continued drug seeking despite reduced pleasure from contribute to the clinical symptomatology of addiction
drug-taking and substantial negative consequences to the encompassing craving, intoxication, bingeing, and with-
individual and their kin. Neurobiological models of drug drawal across a broad range of substance addictions,
addiction seek to explain this perplexing phenomenon by including nicotine, alcohol, and illicit drug addictions
understanding the changes in the brain driving this (Goldstein and Volkow, 2002, 2011; Zilverstand et al.,
behavior. Early neurobiological models focused on the role 2018). This model proposed for the first time that broad
of the reward system (also named “reinforcement,” higher-order cognitive functions involved in the “ability to
“approach,” “drive,” “motivational,” or “dopamine” sys- track, update, and modulate the salience of a reinforcer as a
tem), proposing that it was the repeated activation of the function of context and expectation” and the “ability to
positive reinforcement system that fueled repeated drug control and inhibit prepotent responses,” and their under-
taking (Wise and Bozarth, 1987). This theory was later lying neural networks, were impaired in human drug
refined as the “incentive-sensitization theory,” which pro- addiction (Goldstein and Volkow, 2002). Indeed, recent
posed that the attribution of incentive salience is the un- evidence suggests that impairments in iRISA in human
derlying function of the dopaminergic system, and that the drug addiction are linked to the altered function of six
increased attribution of salience to drugs or drug cues is large-scale brain networks: the limbic-orbitofrontal reward
associated with a “sensitization” or upregulation of this network, the fronto-insular-parietal salience network, the
system in drug addiction (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). prefrontal executive network, the fronto-parietal self-
However, while these early models explained how drug directed network, the subcortical habit, and memory net-
taking would propel the urge to seek drugs, they could not works (Fig. 3.1). The conclusions made and the evidence
explain the inability of addicted individuals to inhibit this reviewed in this chapter are, unless stated otherwise, a
urge (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999). Therefore, later dual summary of the evidence discussed in a systematic review
models of addiction proposed an interaction between a of 105 task-related neuroimaging studies published from
“drive” system (e.g., the reward system) and a “control” 2010 until 2018, which compared individuals with alcohol,
system located in the prefrontal cortex, which would need cannabis, heroin, stimulant, and other addictions to healthy
to be deployed to inhibit a sensitized reward system, but controls (Zilverstand et al., 2018). With very rare excep-
shows impairments with chronic drug use (Jentsch and tions, findings were consistent across different drug-
Taylor, 1999). A contemporary updated dual model, pri- addicted populations and are hence discussed under the
marily based on evidence from human neuroimaging assumption that the iRISA model provides a common
studies, is the impaired Response Inhibition and Salience model of addiction, independent of the primary drug of
Attribution (iRISA) model (Goldstein and Volkow, 2002, choice.
2011; Zilverstand et al., 2018). The iRISA model proposes

Cognition and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-815298-0.00003-4 17


Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
18 Cognition and Addiction

FIGURE 3.1 Evidence from more than 100 neuroimaging studies in drug-addicted individuals supports dual models of addiction, such as the impaired
response inhibition and salience attribution (iRISA) model. Findings demonstrate that abnormal levels in the reward and salience networks can be linked
to a shift in incentive salience, with decreased incentive salience of nondrug-related stimuli and increased salience of drug-related cues, whereas changed
function of the salience and executive networks underlies impaired response inhibition. Additionally, neuroimaging data suggest that altered function of
the habit, memory, and self-directed networks underlie altered learning processes linked to both impaired response inhibition and salience attribution.
Adapted from Zilverstand, A., Huang, A.S., Alia-Klein, N., Goldstein, R.Z., 2018. Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in
human drug addiction. A systematic review. Neuron 98, 886e903.

Neuroimaging evidence for dual models model. Importantly, this shift in incentive value was
stronger in drug-addicted individuals who had used drugs
The reward network consists of the ventral striatum, sub- more frequently and for longer durations, in general man-
genual/rostral cingulate, and orbitofrontal and anterior ifesting more severe addiction, suggesting that these
prefrontal cortex, which together support the appraisal of changes may be a direct consequence of drug use (Zilver-
incentive value by estimating subjective value based on stand et al., 2018). Stronger abnormalities in the activation
expected reward outcomes. This network has been termed levels of the reward network were also linked to a greater
the “reward network,” as these brain regions show a likelihood of relapse in drug users seeking to remain
consistently strong response during rewarding events (in abstinent (Li et al., 2015; Seo et al., 2013), indicating that
contrast to the “salience network,” which reacts robustly to the incentive value shift toward drug-related stimuli con-
both pleasant and unpleasant events). In task neuroimaging tributes to the maintenance of drug seeking and taking in
studies, the reward network showed enhanced activation human drug addiction. Finally, treatment of individuals
levels when, compared to controls, addicted individuals with drug addiction led to a reduction of abnormalities in
were exposed to drug cues (Filbey et al., 2016; Hong et al., the reward network (Zilverstand et al., 2016), suggesting
2017; Kim et al., 2014; Kober et al., 2016; Li et al., 2012, such behavioral and neural normalizations may be a treat-
2015; Ray et al., 2015; Tabatabaei-Jafari et al., 2014), ment target, potentially reducing drug use and enhancing
providing evidence for an increased incentive value of drug abstinence.
cues to drug users. In contrast, studies that used gambling A second network shown to have abnormal brain
paradigms in drug users as compared with controls found function in drug addiction is the salience network, which
reduced activation levels of the reward network during gain encompasses the insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex,
anticipation (Luijten et al., 2017), as well as during loss and inferior parietal cortex. This network integrates inter-
anticipation and realization of monetary loss (Gowin et al., oceptive information with external inputs to detect salient
2017; Gradin et al., 2014; Worhunsky et al., 2017), sug- events, controlling the allocation of attentional control to-
gesting reduced incentive value of monetary rewards and ward them. Similar to the reward network, the salience
losses in drug addiction. Studies employing social- network was found to be hyperengaged when drug users
emotional stimuli to provoke an affective reaction simi- were confronted with drug-related stimuli (Kühn and Gal-
larly demonstrated a blunted reward network response in linat, 2011; Zilverstand et al., 2018) and hypoengaged
addicted individuals (Asensio et al., 2010; Caldwell et al., when drug users were anticipating monetary gains (Luijten
2015; Canterberry et al., 2016; Costumero et al., 2017; et al., 2017) or anticipating or realizing monetary loss in
Hong et al., 2017; Seo et al., 2013; Wesley et al., 2016), gambling tasks (Gowin et al., 2017; Gradin et al., 2014;
further indicating that the incentive value of nondrug- Stewart et al., 2014; Wesley et al., 2011) or when they were
related stimuli may be decreased in drug addiction. Taken confronted with social-emotional stimuli in emotion prov-
together, these findings suggest a shift of incentive value ocation task designs (Asensio et al., 2010; Costumero et al.,
away from nondrug rewards and toward drug-related cues 2017; Gilman et al., 2010; Hong et al., 2017; Landi et al.,
across addicted populations, as proposed by the iRISA 2011; Maurage et al., 2012; Seo et al., 2013). Beyond the
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
Haste thee, nymph, and bring with thee
Jest and youthful Jollity,
Quips and cranks and wanton wiles,
Nods and becks and wreathèd smiles
Such as hang on Hebe’s cheek,
And love to live in dimples sleek,—
Sport that wrinkled Care derides,
And Laughter holding both his sides.
Come, and trip it, as ye go,
On the light fantastic toe;
And in thy right hand lead with thee
The mountain nymph, sweet Liberty:
And, if I give thee honor due,
Mirth, admit me of thy crew,
To live with her, and live with thee,
In unreproved pleasures free.

FROM IL PENSEROSO
By John Milton

Oft, on a plat of rising ground,


I hear the far-off curfew sound
Over some wide watered shore,
Swinging slow with sullen roar;
Or, if the air will not permit,
Some still removed place will fit,
Where glowing embers through the room
Teach light to counterfeit a gloom.

THE LOTOS-EATERS
By Alfred Tennyson

The Lotos blooms below the barren peak:


The Lotos blows by every winding creek:
All day the wind breathes low with mellower tone:
Through every hollow cave and alley lone
Round and round the spicy downs the yellow Lotus-dust is blown.
We have had enough of action, and of motion we,
Rolled to starboard, rolled to larboard, when the surge was seething
free,
Where the wallowing monster spouted his foam-fountains in the sea.
Let us swear an oath, and keep it with an equal mind,
In the hollow Lotus-land to live and lie reclined
On the hills like gods together, careless of mankind.
For they lie beside their nectar, and the bolts are hurled
Far below them in the valleys, and the clouds are lightly curled
Round their golden houses, girdled with the gleaming world;
Where they smile in secret, looking over wasted lands,
Blight and famine, plague and earthquake, roaring deeps, and fiery
sands,
Clanging fights, and flaming towns, and sinking ships, and praying
hands.
But they smile, they find a music centered in a doleful song
Steaming up, a lamentation and an ancient tale of wrong,
Like a tale of little meaning, though the words are strong;
Chanted from an ill-used race of men that cleave the soil,
Sow the seed, and reap the harvest with enduring toil,
Storing yearly little dues of wheat, and wine, and oil;
Till they perish and they suffer—some, ’tis whispered—down in hell
Suffer endless anguish, others in Elysian valleys dwell,
Resting weary limbs at last on beds of asphodel.
Surely, surely, slumber is more sweet than toil, the shore
Than labor in the deep mid-ocean, wind and wave and oar;
O rest ye, brother mariners, we will not wander more.

HOME, WOUNDED
By Sydney Dobell

Blare the trumpet, and boom the gun,


But, O, to sit here thus in the sun,
To sit here, feeling my work is done,
While the sands of life so golden run,
And I watch the children’s posies,
And my idle heart is whispering,
“Bring whatever the years may bring,
The flowers will blossom, the birds will sing,
And there’ll always be primroses.”

THE MINARET BELLS


By William M. Thackeray

Tink a tink, tink a tink,


By the light of the star,
On the blue river’s brink,
I heard a guitar.

I heard a guitar
On the blue waters clear,
And knew by its music
That Selim was near!

Tink a tink, tink a tink,


How the soft music swells,
And I hear the soft clink
Of the minaret bells!

SPRINGTIME
By Leonard G. Nattkemper

May-time’s Spring-time,
O let us steal away.
Spring-time’s love-time,
So let us go to-day.

Oh! the dawn, while dew is on,


Awakes a fragrant breeze;
It fills my room with rich perfume
From snow-white locust trees.

Across the grain there floats a strain


Of ancient witchery;
A robin’s throat hath freed a note
Of rarest ecstasy.

And while he sings, within me springs


An echo to his lay—
But how can words e’er match this bird’s
Sweet song of Spring, I pray!

Such noon-day dreams by babbling streams,


There’s nothing to compare;
Soft zephyrs blow where waters flow,
Entangling my hair.

In shady nooks, fond lover looks


In eyes as blue as skies;
And her reply, though quaint and shy,
Is what true love implies.

So May-time’s Spring-time,
Now let us steal away;
Spring-time’s love-time,
And let us go to-day.

A SINGING LESSON
By Jean Ingelow

A nightingale made a mistake—


She sang a few notes out of tune—
Her heart was ready to break,
And she hid away from the moon.
She wrung her claws, poor thing,
But was far too proud to weep;
She tucked her head under her wing,
And pretended to be asleep.

A lark, arm-in-arm with a thrush,


Came sauntering up to the place;
The nightingale felt herself blush,
Though feathers hid her face.
She knew they had heard her song,
She felt them snicker and sneer;
She thought this life was too long,
And wished she could skip a year.

“Oh, nightingale,” cooed a dove,


“Oh, nightingale, what’s the use?
You bird of beauty and love,
Why behave like a goose?
Don’t skulk away from our sight
Like a common contemptible fowl;
You bird of joy and delight,
Why behave like an owl?

“Only think of all you have done—


Only think of all you can do;
A false note is really fun
From such a bird as you!
Lift up your proud little crest;
Open your musical beak;
Other birds have to do their best,
But you need only speak.”

The nightingale shyly took


Her head from under her wing,
And giving the dove a look
Straightway began to sing.
There was never a bird could pass—
The night was divinely calm—
And the people stood on the grass
To hear that wonderful psalm.
The nightingale did not care—
She only sang to the skies;
Her song ascended there,
And there she fixed her eyes.
The people who listened below
She knew but little about—
And this tale has a moral, I know,
If you’ll try to find it out.

MORAL
Never give up, always look up.
Cheer the discouraged.
Strive for heavenly applause.
Care not for the praise of men, but for the praise of God.

THE WOLVES OF THE SEA


By Herbert Bashford

From dusk until dawn they are hurrying on,


Unfettered and fearless they flee;
From morn until eve they plunder and thieve—
The hungry, white wolves of the Sea!

With never a rest, they race to the west,


To the Orient’s rim do they run;
By the berg and the floe of the northland they go
And away to the isles of the sun.

They wail at the moon from the desolate dune


Till the air has grown dank with their breath;
They snarl at the stars from the treacherous bars
Of the coasts that are haunted by Death.

They grapple and bite in a keen, mad delight


As they feed on the bosom of Grief;
And one steals away to a cave with his prey,
And one to the rocks of the reef.

With the froth on their lips they follow the ships,


Each striving to lead in the chase;
Since loosed by the hand of the King of their band
They have known but the rush of the race.

They are shaggy and old, yet as mighty and bold


As when God’s freshest gale set them free;
Not a sail is unfurled in a part of the world
But is prey for the wolves of the Sea!

—Copyright by Harr Wagner Co., San Francisco, and used by kind


permission of author and publisher.

OLD IRONSIDES
By Oliver Wendell Holmes

Ay, tear her tattered ensign down!


Long has it waved on high,
And many an eye has danced to see
That banner in the sky;
Beneath it rung the battle shout,
And burst the cannon’s roar;—
The meteor of the ocean air
Shall sweep the clouds no more.

Her deck, once red with hero’s blood,


Where knelt the vanquished foe,
When winds were hurrying o’er the flood,
And waves were white below,
No more shall feel the victor’s tread,
Or know the conquered knee;
The harpies of the shore shall pluck
The eagle of the sea!
Oh, better that her shattered hulk
Should sink beneath the wave;
Her thunders shook the mighty deep,
And there should be her grave;
Nail to the mast her holy flag,
Set every threadbare sail,
And give her to the god of storms,
The lightning and the gale!

COLUMBUS
By Joaquin Miller

Behind him lay the gray Azores,


Behind the Gates of Hercules;
Before him not the ghost of shores;
Before him only shoreless seas.
The good mate said: “Now must we pray,
For lo! the very stars are gone.
Brave Adm’r’l, speak; what shall I say?”
“Why, say: ‘Sail on! sail on! and on!’”

“My men grow mutinous day by day;


My men grow ghastly wan and weak.”
The stout mate thought of home; a spray
Of salt wave washed his swarthy cheek.
“What shall I say, brave Adm’r’l, say,
If we sight naught but seas at dawn?”
“Why, you shall say at break of day:
‘Sail on! Sail on! Sail on! and on!’”

They sailed and sailed, as winds might blow,


Until at last the blanched mate said:
“Why, now, not even God would know
Should I and all my men fall dead.
These very winds forget their way;
For God from these dread seas is gone.
Now speak, brave Adm’r’l; speak and say—”
He said: “Sail on! sail on! and on!”

They sailed: they sailed. Then spake the mate:


“This mad sea shows his teeth to-night,
He curls his lip, he lies in wait,
With lifted teeth, as if to bite!
Brave Adm’r’l, say but one good word:
What shall we do when hope is gone?”
The words leapt like a leaping sword:
“Sail on! Sail on! Sail on! and on!”

Then pale and worn, he kept his deck,


And peered through darkness. Ah, that night
Of all dark nights! And then a speck—
A light! A light? A light! A light!
It grew; a starlit flag unfurled!
It grew to be Time’s burst of dawn.
He gained a world; he gave that world
Its grandest lesson: “On! sail on!”

—Copyright by Harr Wagner Co., San Francisco, and used by kind


permission of author and publisher.

DAYBREAK
By Robert Browning

Day!
Faster and more fast,
O’er night’s brim, day boils at last:
Boils pure gold, o’er the cloud-cup’s brim
Where spurting and suppressed it lay,
For not a froth-flake touched the rim
Of yonder gap in the solid gray
Of the eastern cloud, an hour away;
But forth one wavelet, then another, curled,
Till the whole sunrise, not to be suppressed,
Rose, reddened, and its seething breast
Flickered in bounds, grew gold, then overflowed the world.

Oh, Day, if I squander a wavelet of thee,


A mite of my twelve hours’ treasure,
The least of thy gazes or glances,
(Be they grants thou art bound to or gifts above measure)
One of thy choices or one of thy chances,
(Be they tasks God imposed thee or freaks at thy pleasure)
—My Day, if I squander such labor or leisure,
Then shame fall on Asolo, mischief on me!

Thy long, blue, solemn hours serenely flowing,


Whence earth, we feel, gets steady help and good—
Thy fitful sunshine-minutes, coming, going,
As if earth turned from work in gamesome mood—
All shall be mine! But thou must treat me not
As prosperous ones are treated, those who live
At hand here, and enjoy the higher lot,
In readiness to take what thou wilt give,
And free to let alone what thou refusest;
For, Day, my holiday, if thou ill-usest
Me, who am only Pippa,—old-year’s sorrow,
Cast off last night, will come again to-morrow:
Whereas, if thou prove gentle, I shall borrow.
Sufficient strength of thee for new-year’s sorrow.
All other men and women that this earth
Belongs to, who all days alike possess,
Make general plenty cure particular dearth,
Get more joy one way, if another, less:
Thou art my single day, God lends to leaven
What were all earth else, with a feel of heaven.

—From “Pippa Passes.”

MY SWORD SONG
By Richard Realf

Day in, day out, through the long campaign,


I march in my place in the ranks;
And whether it shine or whether it rain,
My good sword cheerily clanks;
It clanks and clanks in a knightly way
Like the ring of an armored heel;
And this is the song which day by day,
It sings with its lips of steel:

“O friend, from whom a hundred times,


I have felt the strenuous grip
Of the all-renouncing love that climbs
To the heights of fellowship;
Are you tired of all the weary miles?
Are you faint with your swooning limbs?
Do you hunger back for the olden smiles,
And the lilt of olden hymns?

“Has your heart grown weak since that rapt hour


When you leapt, with a single bound,
From dreaming ease to sovereign power
Of a living soul world-crowned?
Behold! the aloes of sacrifice
Are better than radiant wine,
And the bloody sweat of a cause like this
Is an agony divine.

“Under the wail of the shuddering world


Amoan for its fallen sons;
Over the volleying thunders hurled
From the throats of the wrathful guns;
Above the roar of the plunging line
That rocks with the fury of hell,
Runs the absolute voice: O Earth of mine,
Be patient, for all is well!”
Thus sings my sword to my soul, and I,
Albeit the way is long,
As soiled clouds darken athwart the sky—
Still keep my spirit strong:
Whether I live, or whether I lie
On the stained ground, ghastly and stark,
Beyond the carnage I shall descry
God’s love shines across the dark.

—Copyright by Funk & Wagnalls Co., New York, and used by their
kind permission.

LABOR
Anonymous

Toil swings the ax, and forests bow;


The seeds break out in radiant bloom,
Rich harvests smile behind the plow,
And cities cluster round the loom;
Where towering domes and tapering spires
Adorn the vale and crown the hill,
Stout labor lights its beacon-fires,
And plumes with smoke the forge and mill.

The monarch oak, the woodland’s pride,


Whose trunk is seamed with lightning scars,
Toil launches on the restless tide,
And there unrolls the flag of stars;
The engine with its lungs of flame,
And ribs of brass and joints of steel,
From Labor’s plastic fingers came,
With sobbing valve and whirling wheel.

’Tis Labor works the magic press,


And turns the crank in hives of toil,
And beckons angels down to bless
Industrious hands on sea and soil.
Here sun-brown Toil, with shining spade,
Links lake to lake with silver ties
Strung thick with palaces of trade,
And temples towering to the skies.

THE ARROW AND THE SONG[17]


By Henry W. Longfellow

I shot an arrow into the air,


It fell to earth, I knew not where;
For, so swiftly it flew, the sight
Could not follow it in its flight.

I breathed a song into the air,


It fell to earth, I knew not where;
For who has sight so keen and strong,
That it can follow the flight of song?

Long, long afterward, in an oak


I found the arrow, still unbroke;
And the song, from beginning to end,
I found again in the heart of a friend.

THE BELLS OF SAN GABRIEL


By Charles Warren Stoddard
[Particularly note the possibility of onomatopoesy in the following
refrain. What answer do the bells give to the questions of the poet?
There is no other answer than their steady, monotonous toll. The
answer must be found in your own heart, viz., that no good work,
done with high zeal and enthusiasm, with self-sacrifice, ever can be
in vain. Then read the refrain as a bell would sound, if it were struck
at the end of each line, prolonging the sound to correspond with the
continued resonance of the bell.]
Thine was the corn and the wine,
The blood of the grape that nourished;
The blossom and fruit of the vine
That was heralded far away.
These were thy gifts; and thine,
When the vine and the fig-tree flourished,
The promise of peace and of glad increase
Forever and ever and aye.
What then wert thou, and what art now?
Answer me, O, I pray!

And every note of every bell


Sang Gabriel! rang Gabriel!
In the tower that is left the tale to tell
Of Gabriel, the Archangel.

Oil of the olive was thine;


Flood of the wine-press flowing;
Blood o’ the Christ was the wine—
Blood o’ the Lamb that was slain.
Thy gifts were fat o’ the kine
Forever coming and going
Over the hills, the thousand hills,
Their lowing a soft refrain.
What then wert thou, and what art now?
Answer me, once again!

And every note of every bell


Sang Gabriel! rang Gabriel!
In the tower that is left the tale to tell
Of Gabriel, the Archangel.

Seed o’ the corn was thine—


Body of Him thus broken
And mingled with blood o’ the vine—
The bread and the wine of life;
Out of the good sunshine
They were given to thee as a token—
The body of Him, and the blood of Him,
When the gifts of God were rife.
What then wert thou, and what art now,
After the weary strife?

And every note of every bell


Sang Gabriel! rang Gabriel!
In the tower that is left the tale to tell
Of Gabriel, the Archangel.

Where are they now, O bells?


Where are the fruits o’ the Mission?
Garnered, where no one dwells.
Shepherd and flock are fled.
O’er the Lord’s vineyard swells
The tide that with fell perdition
Sounded their doom and fashioned their tomb
And buried them with the dead.
What then wert thou, and what art now?—
The answer is still unsaid.

And every note of every bell


Sang Gabriel! rang Gabriel!
In the tower that is left the tale to tell
Of Gabriel, the Archangel.

Where are they now, O tower!


The locusts and wild honey?
Where is the sacred dower
That the bride of Christ was given?
Gone to the builders of power,
The misers and minters of money;
Gone for the greed that is their creed—
And these in the land have thriven.
What then wert thou, and what art now,
And wherefore hast thou striven?

And every note of every bell


Sang Gabriel! rang Gabriel!
In the tower that is left the tale to tell
Of Gabriel, the Archangel.

—Copyright by John Lane, New York, and used by kind


permission.

A WELCOME TO ALEXANDRA
March 7, 1863
By Alfred, Lord Tennyson

Sea-kings’ daughter from over the sea, Alexandra!


Saxon and Norman and Dane are we,
But all of us Danes in our welcome of thee, Alexandra!
Welcome her, thunders of fort and of fleet!
Welcome her, thundering cheer of the street!
Welcome her, all things youthful and sweet,
Scatter the blossoms under her feet!
Break, happy land, into earlier flowers!
Make music, O bird, in the new-budded bowers!
Blazon your mottoes of blessing and prayer!
Welcome her, welcome her, all that is ours!
Warble, O bugle, and trumpet, blare!
Flags, flutter out upon turrets and towers!
Flames, on the windy headland flare!
Utter your jubilee, steeple and spire!
Clash, ye bells, in the merry March air!
Flash, ye cities, in rivers of fire!
Rush to the roof, sudden rocket, and higher
Melt into stars for the land’s desire!
Roll and rejoice, jubilant voice,
Roll as a ground-swell dash’d on the strand,
Roar as the sea when he welcomes the land,
And welcome her, welcome the land’s desire.
The sea-kings’ daughter is happy as fair,
Blissful bride of a blissful heir,
Bride of the sire of the kings of the sea—
O joy to the people and joy to the throne,
Come to us, love us and make us your own;
For Saxon or Dane or Norman we,
Teuton or Celt, or whatever we be,
We are each all Dane in our welcome of thee, Alexandra!

CHRISTMAS IN INDIA
By Rudyard Kipling

Dim dusk behind the tamarisks—the sky is saffron yellow—


As the women in the village grind the corn,
And the parrots seek the river-side, each calling to his fellow
That the Day, the staring Eastern Day is born.
Oh, the white on the highway! Oh, the stenches in the byway!
Oh, the clammy fog that hovers over earth!
And at home they’re making merry ’neath the white and scarlet berry

What part have India’s exiles in their mirth?

Full day behind the tamarisks—the sky is blue and staring—


As the cattle crawl afield beneath the yoke,
And they bear one o’er the field path, who is past all hope or caring,
To the ghat below the curling wreaths of smoke.
Call on Rama, going slowly, as ye bear a brother slowly—
Call on Rama—he may hear, perhaps, your voice!
With our hymn-books and our psalters we appeal to other altars,
And to-day we bid good Christian men rejoice!

High noon behind the tamarisks—the sun is hot above us—


As at home the Christmas Day is breaking wan,
They will drink our healths at dinner—those who tell us how they
love us,
And forget us till another year be gone!
Oh, the toil that knows no breaking! Oh, the Heimweh, ceaseless,
aching!
Oh, the black dividing sea and alien plain.
Youth was cheap, wherefore we sold it.
Gold was good—we hoped to hold it,
And to-day we know the fullness of our gain.

Gray dusk behind the tamarisks—the parrots fly together—


As the sun is sinking slowly over Home;
And the last ray seems to mock us shackled in a lifelong tether
That drags us back howe’er so far we roam.
Hard her service, poor her payment—she in ancient, tattered
raiment—
India, she the grim stepmother of our kind.
If a year of life be lent her, if her temple’s shrine we enter,
The door is shut—we may not look behind.

Black night behind the tamarisks—the owls begin their chorus—


As the conches from the temple scream and bray.
With fruitless years behind us, and the hopeless years before us,
Let us honor, O my brothers, Christmas Day!
Call a truce, then, to our labors—let us feast with friends and
neighbors,
And be merry as the custom of our caste;
For if “faint and forced the laughter,” and if sadness follow after,
We are richer by one mocking Christmas past.

THE BELLS OF SHANDON


By Francis Mahony

With deep affection


And recollection
I often think of
Those Shandon bells,
Whose sound so wild would,
In the days of childhood,
Fling round my cradle
Their magic spells.
On this I ponder
Where’er I wander,
And thus grow fonder,
Sweet Cork, of thee—
With thy bells of Shandon
That sound so grand on
The pleasant waters
Of the River Lee.

I’ve heard bells chiming


Full many a clime in,
Tolling sublime in
Cathedral shrine;
While at a glib rate
Brass tongues would vibrate;
But all their music
Spoke naught like thine.

For memory, dwelling


On each proud swelling
Of thy belfry, knelling
Its bold notes free,
Made the bells of Shandon
Sound far more grand, on
The pleasant waters
Of the River Lee.

I’ve heard bells tolling


Old Adrian’s Mole in
Their thunder rolling
From the Vatican;
And cymbals glorious
Swinging uproarious
In the gorgeous turrets
Of Notre Dame;

But thy sounds were sweeter


Than the dome of Peter
Flings o’er the Tiber,
Pealing solemnly.
O, the bells of Shandon
Sound far more grand, on
The pleasant waters
Of the River Lee.

There’s a bell in Moscow


Where on tower and kiosko
In Saint Sophia
The Turkman gets,
And loud in air
Calls men to prayer,
From the tapering summits
Of tall minarets.

Such empty phantom


I freely grant them;
But there’s an anthem
More dear to me;
’Tis the bells of Shandon
That sounds so grand, on
The pleasant waters
Of the River Lee.

THE DAY AND THE WORK


By Edwin Markham

To each man is given a day and his work for the day;
And once, and no more, he is given to travel this way.
And woe if he flies from the task, whatever the odds;
For the task is appointed to him on the scroll of the gods.

There is waiting a work where only your hands can avail;


And so if you falter, a chord in the music will fail.
We may laugh to the sky, we may lie for an hour in the sun;

You might also like