You are on page 1of 53

Handbook of Personality Disorders

Theory Research and Treatment W.


John Livesley
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-personality-disorders-theory-research-a
nd-treatment-w-john-livesley/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Integrated Modular Treatment for Borderline Personality


Disorder A Practical Guide to Combining Effective
Treatment Methods 1st Edition W. John Livesley

https://textbookfull.com/product/integrated-modular-treatment-
for-borderline-personality-disorder-a-practical-guide-to-
combining-effective-treatment-methods-1st-edition-w-john-
livesley/

Higher Education Handbook of Theory and Research Laura


W. Perna

https://textbookfull.com/product/higher-education-handbook-of-
theory-and-research-laura-w-perna/

The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of


Personality Disorders John M. Oldham

https://textbookfull.com/product/the-american-psychiatric-
publishing-textbook-of-personality-disorders-john-m-oldham/

Treatment of severe personality disorders Resolution of


aggression and recovery of eroticism 1st Edition Otto
F. Kernberg

https://textbookfull.com/product/treatment-of-severe-personality-
disorders-resolution-of-aggression-and-recovery-of-eroticism-1st-
edition-otto-f-kernberg/
Mentalization Based Treatment for Personality
Disorders: A Practical Guide 1st Edition Anthony
Bateman

https://textbookfull.com/product/mentalization-based-treatment-
for-personality-disorders-a-practical-guide-1st-edition-anthony-
bateman/

Higher Education: Handbook Of Theory And Research:


Volume 36 1st Edition Laura W. Perna

https://textbookfull.com/product/higher-education-handbook-of-
theory-and-research-volume-36-1st-edition-laura-w-perna/

The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes


First Edition John F Rauthmann

https://textbookfull.com/product/the-handbook-of-personality-
dynamics-and-processes-first-edition-john-f-rauthmann/

Antisocial Narcissistic and Borderline Personality


Disorders A New Conceptualization of Development
Reinforcement Expression and Treatment 1st Edition
Daniel J. Fox
https://textbookfull.com/product/antisocial-narcissistic-and-
borderline-personality-disorders-a-new-conceptualization-of-
development-reinforcement-expression-and-treatment-1st-edition-
daniel-j-fox/

Handbook of Preschool Mental Health Development


Disorders and Treatment 2nd Edition Joan L. Luby

https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-preschool-mental-
health-development-disorders-and-treatment-2nd-edition-joan-l-
luby/
ebook
THE GUILFORD PRESS
HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY DISORDERS
Also Available

Integrated Treatment for Personality Disorder:


A Modular Approach
Edited by W. John Livesley, Giancarlo Dimaggio,
and John F. Clarkin

Practical Management of Personality Disorder


W. John Livesley
Handbook of
Personality
Disorders
Theory, Research, and Treatment

SECOND EDITION

Edited by

W. John Livesley
Roseann Larstone

THE GUILFORD PRESS


New York  London
Copyright © 2018 The Guilford Press
A Division of Guilford Publications, Inc.
370 Seventh Avenue, Suite 1200, New York, NY 10001
www.guilford.com

All rights reserved

No part of this book may be reproduced, translated, stored in a retrieval system,


or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying,
microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the publisher.

Printed in the United States of America

This book is printed on acid-free paper.

Last digit is print number: 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1

The authors have checked with sources believed to be reliable in their efforts to provide
information that is complete and generally in accord with the standards of practice that are
accepted at the time of publication. However, in view of the possibility of human error or
changes in behavioral, mental health, or medical sciences, neither the authors, nor the editors
and publisher, nor any other party who has been involved in the preparation or publication
of this work warrants that the information contained herein is in every respect accurate or
complete, and they are not responsible for any errors or omissions or the results obtained
from the use of such information. Readers are encouraged to confirm the information
contained in this book with other sources.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


Names: Livesley, W. John, editor. | Larstone, Roseann, editor.
Title: Handbook of personality disorders : theory, research, and treatment /
edited by W. John Livesley, Roseann Larstone.
Description: Second edition. | New York : The Guilford Press, [2018] |
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2017023842 | ISBN 9781462533114 (hardback)
Subjects: LCSH: Personality disorders—Handbooks, manuals, etc. | BISAC:
PSYCHOLOGY / Personality. | MEDICAL / Psychiatry / General. | SOCIAL
SCIENCE / Social Work. | PSYCHOLOGY / Clinical Psychology.
Classification: LCC RC554 .H36 2018 | DDC 616.85/81—dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2017023842
About the Editors

W. John Livesley, MD, PhD, is Professor Emeritus in the Department of Psychiatry at the Uni-
versity of British Columbia, Canada. His research focuses on the structure, classification, and
origins of personality disorder, and on constructing an integrated framework for describing and
conceptualizing personality pathology. His clinical interests are directed toward developing a
unified approach to treatment. Dr. Livesley is a Fellow of the Royal Society of Canada. He is a
past editor of the Journal of Personality Disorders.

Roseann Larstone, PhD, is Research Associate in the Northern Medical Program at the Uni-
versity of Northern British Columbia, Canada. She holds an adjunct appointment in the Faculty
of Medicine at the University of British Columbia. Her research has focused on personality
and psychopathology, adolescent social–emotional development, and adolescent mental health.
Dr. Larstone is currently involved in community-based research and program evaluation in the
area of health promotion for mental health service recipients. She is a past assistant editor and
current editorial board member of the Journal of Personality Disorders.

v
Contributors

Timothy A. Allen, MA, Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota,


Minneapolis, Minnesota
Emily Ansell, PhD, Department of Psychology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York
Arnoud Arntz, PhD, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam,
Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Anthony W. Bateman, MD, Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families,
London, United Kingdom
Lorna Smith Benjamin, PhD, ABPP, Department of Psychology, University of Utah,
Salt Lake City, Utah
David P. Bernstein, PhD, Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastrict University,
Maastricht, The Netherlands
Donald W. Black, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver
College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa
Nancee Blum, MSW, Department of Psychiatry, Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver College of Medicine,
University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa
Sarah J. Brislin, MS, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida
Nicole Cain, PhD, Department of Psychology, Long Island University, Brooklyn, New York
Chloe Campbell, PhD, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology,
University College London, London, United Kingdom
Andrew M. Chanen, PhD, Orygen, The National Centre of Excellence in Youth Mental Health,
Melbourne, Australia; Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne,
Melbourne, Australia
Lee Anna Clark, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame,
Notre Dame, Indiana

vii
viii Contributors

John F. Clarkin, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medical College,


New York, New York
Maartje Clercx, MSc, Faculty of Psychology and Neurosciences, Maastricht University,
Maastricht, The Netherlands
Emil F. Coccaro, MD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science,
Pritzker School of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
Stephanie G. Craig, PhD, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
Kenneth L. Critchfield, PhD, Department of Psychology, James Madison University,
Harrisonburg, Virginia
Elizabeth Daly, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana
Kate M. Davidson, PhD, Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow,
Glasgow, United Kingdom
Roger D. Davis, PhD, Department of Psychology, Ateneo de Manila University,
Port Charlotte, Florida
Jennifer R. Fanning, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science,
Pritzker School of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
Peter Fonagy, PhD, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology,
University College London, London, United Kingdom
John G. Gunderson, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School,
Boston, Massachusetts
Michael N. Hallquist, PhD, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University,
University Park, Pennsylvania
Julie Harrison, PhD, Harrison Psychological Consultations, Indianapolis, Indiana
André M. Ivanoff, PhD, School of Social Work, Columbia University, New York, New York
Kerry L. Jang, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
British Columbia, Canada
Carsten René Jørgensen, PhD, Department of Psychology, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Christie Pugh Karpiak, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Scranton,
Scranton, Pennsylvania
Stephen Kellett, PhD, Centre for Psychological Services Research, University of Sheffield,
Sheffield, United Kingdom
Robert F. Krueger, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis, Minnesota
Roseann M. Larstone, PhD, Northern Medical Program, University of Northern
British Columbia, Prince George, British Columbia, Canada
Mark F. Lenzenweger, PhD, Department of Psychology, State University of New York
at Binghamton, Binghamton, New York; Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medical College,
New York, New York
Kenneth N. Levy, PhD, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University,
University Park, Pennsylvania
 Contributors ix

Marsha M. Linehan, PhD, ABPP, Department of Psychology, University of Washington,


Seattle, Washington
W. John Livesley, MD, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia,
Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Jill Lobbestael, PhD, Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University,
Maastricht, The Netherlands
Patrick Luyten, PhD, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven,
Leuven, Belgium; Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology,
University College London, London, United Kingdom
Paul Markovitz, MD, PhD, Interventional Psychiatric Associates, Santa Barbara, California
Birgit Bork Mathiesen, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen,
Copenhagen, Denmark
Kevin B. Meehan, PhD, Department of Psychology, Long Island University, Brooklyn, New York
Robert Mestel, PhD, Helios Clinics, Berlin, Germany
Theodore Millon, PhD (deceased), Institute for Advanced Studies in Personology
and Psychopathology, Port Jervis, New York
Marlene M. Moretti, PhD, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
Leslie Morey, PhD, Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas
Theresa A. Morgan, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior,
Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
Roger T. Mulder, MD, PhD, Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago,
Christchurch, New Zealand
Morgan R. Negrón, MA, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame,
Notre Dame, Indiana
Shani Ofrat, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota
Lacy A. Olson-Ayala, PhD, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, California
Joel Paris, MD, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University and Jewish General Hospital,
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Christopher J. Patrick, PhD, Department of Psychology, Florida State University,
Tallahassee, Florida
Anthony Pinto, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine
at Hofstra/Northwell and Zucker Hillside Hospital, Glen Oaks, New York
Maria Elena Ridolfi, MD, Fano Department of Mental Health, Fano, Italy
Clive J. Robins, PhD, ABPP, ACT, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
Duke University, Durham, North Carolina
Anthony C. Ruocco, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Anthony Ryle, DM, FRCPsych (deceased), St. Thomas’ Hospital, London, United Kingdom
x Contributors

Maria Cristina Samaco-Zamora, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of San Francisco,


San Francisco, California
Jaime L. Shapiro, MA, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame,
Notre Dame, Indiana
Rebecca L. Shiner, PhD, Department of Psychology, Colgate University, Hamilton, New York
Merav H. Silverman, MA, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis, Minnesota
Erik Simonsen, MD, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences,
University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Andrew E. Skodol, MD, Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, University of Arizona,
Tucson, Arizona
Tracey Leone Smith, PhD, Center for Innovations in Quality and Effectiveness and Safety,
Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, Texas
Paul H. Soloff, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine,
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
Don St. John, MA, PA-C, Department of Psychiatry, Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver
College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa
Jennifer L. Tackett, PhD, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
Katherine N. Thompson, PhD, Orygen, the National Centre of Excellence
in Youth Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia; Centre for Youth Mental Health,
University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
Marianne Skovgaard Thomsen, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen,
Copenhagen, Denmark
Emily N. Vanderbleek, MA, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame,
Notre Dame, Indiana
Philip A. Vernon, PhD, Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
Michael G. Wheaton, PhD, Department of Psychology, Barnard College, New York, New York
Thomas A. Widiger, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky,
Lexington, Kentucky
Stephen C. P. Wong, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Saskatchewan,
Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada
Aidan G. C. Wright, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh,
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
Noga Zerubavel, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University,
Durham, North Carolina
Mark Zimmerman, MD, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School,
Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
Preface

Since the first edition of the Handbook was pub- knowledge, presented something of a chal-
lished nearly 20 years ago, much has changed in lenge in planning and organizing the volume.
the study and treatment of personality disorder Although we wanted to produce a text that is
(PD). Most importantly, the emergence of PD comprehensive and represents the overall scope
from relative obscurity to become the important of the current study of PD, it was clear that we
area of clinical practice and research noted in could not include all developments. Even with
the first edition has been consolidated perhaps the first edition, we needed to be selective about
more than could have been envisioned at the what to include and how to approach the con-
time. Research has increased substantially in cept of PD. The progress of the last two decades
quantity and scope. New areas of inquiry have added to the challenge. One of the difficulties
opened up, and old ones have been extended in that we faced is that the growth in empirical
new ways. Topics that were previously largely research that has so enriched the PD database
domains of theoretical speculation are now ac- has also added to the fragmentation of the field,
tive areas of empirical inquiry. It is not just that and we wanted to find ways to foster the idea of
empirical research has been consolidated and integration or at least begin to connect different
extended; similar changes have occurred in areas of scholarship.
clinical practice. New therapies have been de- As with the first edition, our intent to em-
veloped, adding richness and depth to our thera- phasize empirical findings led to us continue to
peutic armamentarium and, more importantly, organize the volume around major themes such
a substantial increase in outcome studies is be- as conceptual and theoretical issues, psychopa-
ginning to form a solid foundation for evidence- thology, etiology and development, epidemiolo-
based treatment. These developments continue gy and course, assessment, and treatment rather
to challenge traditional ideas and are opening than specific diagnoses, because we continue to
up new perspectives on the essential nature of be concerned about the validity of categorical or
PD, its causes and development, and more ef- typal diagnoses whether described in terms of
fective treatments. criteria sets or trait constellations. However, we
Given progress on so many fronts, it seemed have softened our stance a little on this matter by
timely to consider a second edition of the Hand- including a few chapters on specific diagnoses.
book to document these changes, to comment The reason is not that we think evidence on the
on the current state of knowledge, and perhaps validity of categorical diagnoses has changed:
even to consider potential new directions. How- to the contrary, the evidence against categorical
ever, the progress being made and the increased diagnoses has strengthened substantially in the
data about PD, along with the current state of intervening years. Rather, clinical knowledge

xi
xii Preface

about PD is largely organized around specific connecting and even integrating different ap-
diagnoses, and recent interesting developments proaches. We have also tried to foster attention
have occurred in our understanding of the psy- to the need to think in a more integrative way
chopathology associated with some putative by providing brief introductions to each section
disorders that we thought should be discussed. that discuss the key themes illustrated by the
Consequently, we have included chapters on chapters in the section, and, in some instances,
diagnoses that show some resemblance to em- we propose tentative links between the ideas
pirically derived structures, namely, antisocial/ discussed in the different contributions.
psychopathic, obsessive–compulsive, and bor- We also wanted to promote greater interest
derline PDs. Our assumption is that these pat- in the subtleties, nuances, and complexities of
terns of psychopathology have some degree of clinical presentations. An unfortunate conse-
validity and will ultimately be represented in quence of the DSM preoccupation with reliabil-
some way in any future evidence-based taxono- ity and hence with diagnostic criteria sets is that
my. In contrast to our softened position on spe- clinical interest has increasingly focused on the
cific diagnoses, we have been more rigorous in simplest and most overt aspects of personality
the section on treatment in our emphasis on evi- pathology, leading to an almost total neglect
dence-based approaches. We only invited chap- of personality processes and functioning, and
ters on specific therapies that were supported the complex interaction between different do-
by at least one randomized controlled trial and mains of personality pathology. It has even led
on approaches that were evidence-based. to a tendency to neglect trying to understand
In editing this volume, we also wanted to the person manifesting the diagnostic criteria
promote a greater interest in two issues that under consideration. The result is an impover-
seem important for the continued development ished understanding of the descriptive richness
of the field and the construction of more ef- of these disorders. Unfortunately, we found it
fective treatment methods: greater attention to difficult to address this problem to the degree
theoretical and conceptual issues, and a broader we think necessary, although a few chapters do
interest in the psychopathology of PD. The most address some aspects of the problem.
pressing problems confronting the study of PD The idea for the first edition of the Handbook
seem to us to be conceptual rather than empiri- came from Seymour Weingarten, Editor-in-
cal. The value of collecting ever more data is Chief at The Guilford Press, and we are very
compromised by the lack of conceptual and grateful to him for his continued help and sup-
theoretical frameworks needed to organize and port. We also appreciate the support we have
systematize these data into a coherent account received from others at Guilford, including
of PD. However, conceptual progress since the Jim Nageotte and Jane Keislar. We also want
publication of the first edition has been lim- to acknowledge the help of our authors for both
ited. Also, our emphasis on empirical findings their support and advice and their tolerance and
means that we decided not to include chapters patience. Finally, we especially want to thank
describing general theories of either PD or spe- our respective spouses, Ann and Chris, for their
cific diagnoses unless they are based on em- continuous support and encouragement and also
pirical evidence. We consider this approach to for the remarkable tolerance they have shown in
be reasonable, because the field seems to be the final months of this project as we struggled
moving away from interest in grand theories to bring to a conclusion what at times seemed an
that seek to explain all aspects of a given dis- interminable project.
order or even PDs as a whole. Unfortunately, As with the first edition, our hope is that
these theories have not been replaced by more this volume will not only help to disseminate
specific conceptual developments focusing on current knowledge about PD but also encour-
specific issues such as the structure and nature age readers to become even more aware of the
of the disorder and etiology and development. complexities of PD and to question some of
We have tried to address this issue by includ- the fundamental assumptions that continue to
ing chapters that draw attention to the problem dominate and limit the field. We would also
by discussing the importance of theoretical and like to think that this handbook will contribute
conceptual development and the challenges of to a better and more enlightened understanding
developing more integrative frameworks in an of a disorder that is so painful and misunder-
attempt to promote discussion of how to begin stood.
Contents

I. CONCEPTUAL AND TAXONOMIC ISSUES 1


1. Conceptual Issues 3
W. John Livesley

2. Theoretical versus Inductive Approaches to Contemporary Personality Pathology 25


Roger D. Davis, Maria Cristina Samaco‑Zamora, and Theodore Millon

3. Official Classification Systems 47


Thomas A. Widiger

4. Dimensional Approaches to Personality Disorder Classification 72


Shani Ofrat, Robert F. Krueger, and Lee Anna Clark

5. Cultural Aspects of Personality Disorder 88


Roger T. Mulder

II. PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 101


6. Identity 107
Carsten René Jørgensen

7. Attachment, Mentalizing, and the Self 123


Peter Fonagy and Patrick Luyten

8. Cognitive Structures and Processes in Personality Disorders 141


Arnoud Arntz and Jill Lobbestael

9. Taking Stock of Relationships among Personality Disorders 155


and Other Forms of Psychopathology
Merav H. Silverman and Robert F. Krueger

xiii
xiv Contents

III. EPIDEMIOLOGY, COURSE, AND ONSET 169


10. Epidemiology of Personality Disorders 173
Theresa A. Morgan and Mark Zimmerman

11. Understanding Stability and Change in the Personality Disorders: 197


Methodological and Substantive Issues Underpinning Interpretive Challenges
and the Road Ahead
Mark F. Lenzenweger, Michael N. Hallquist, and Aidan G. C. Wright

12. Personality Pathology and Disorder in Children and Youth 215


Andrew M. Chanen, Jennifer L. Tackett, and Katherine N. Thompson

IV. ETIOLOGY AND DEVELOPMENT 229


13. Genetics 235
Kerry L. Jang and Philip A. Vernon

14. Neurotransmitter Function in Personality Disorder 251


Jennifer R. Fanning and Emil F. Coccaro

15. Emotional Regulation and Emotional Processing 271


Paul H. Soloff

16. Neuropsychological Perspectives 283


Marianne Skovgaard Thomsen, Anthony C. Ruocco, Birgit Bork Mathiesen,
and Erik Simonsen

17. Childhood Adversities and Personality Disorders 301


Joel Paris

18. Developmental Psychopathology 309


Rebecca L. Shiner and Timothy A. Allen

19. An Attachment Perspective on Callous and Unemotional Characteristics 324


across Development
Roseann M. Larstone, Stephanie G. Craig, and Marlene M. Moretti

V. DIAGNOSIS AND ASSESSMENT 337


20. Empirically Validated Diagnostic and Assessment Methods 341
Lee Anna Clark, Jaime L. Shapiro, Elizabeth Daly, Emily N. Vanderbleek,
Morgan R. Negrón, and Julie Harrison

21. Clinical Assessment 367


John F. Clarkin, W. John Livesley, and Kevin B. Meehan

22. Using Interpersonal Reconstructive Therapy to Select Effective Interventions 394


for Comorbid, Treatment‑Resistant, Personality‑Disordered Individuals
Lorna Smith Benjamin, Kenneth L. Critchfield, Christie Pugh Karpiak,
Tracey Leone Smith, and Robert Mestel
 Contents xv

VI. SPECIFIC PATTERNS 417


23. Clinical Features of Borderline Personality Disorder 419
Joel Paris

24. Theoretical Perspectives on Psychopathy and Antisocial Personality Disorder 426


Christopher J. Patrick and Sarah J. Brislin

25. Clinical Aspects of Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 444


Lacy A. Olson-Ayala and Christopher J. Patrick

26. Obsessive–Compulsive Personality Disorder and Component Personality Traits 459


Anthony Pinto, Emily Ansell, Michael G. Wheaton, Robert F. Krueger, Leslie Morey,
Andrew E. Skodol, and Lee Anna Clark

VII. EMPIRICALLY BASED TREATMENTS 481


27. Cognitive Analytic Therapy 489
Anthony Ryle and Stephen Kellett

28. Cognitive‑Behavioral Therapy 512


Kate M. Davidson

29. Dialectical Behavior Therapy 527


Clive J. Robins, Noga Zerubavel, André M. Ivanoff, and Marsha M. Linehan

30. Mentalization‑Based Treatment 541


Anthony W. Bateman, Peter Fonagy, and Chloe Campbell

31. Schema Therapy 555


David P. Bernstein and Maartje Clercx

32. Transference‑Focused Psychotherapy 571


John F. Clarkin, Nicole Cain, Mark F. Lenzenweger, and Kenneth N. Levy

33. Systems Training for Emotional Predictability and Problem Solving 586
Nancee Blum, Donald W. Black, and Don St. John

34. Psychoeducation for Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder 600


Maria Elena Ridolfi and John G. Gunderson

35. Pharmacotherapy 611


Paul Markovitz

36. A Treatment Framework for Violent Offenders with Psychopathic Traits 629
Stephen C. P. Wong

37. Integrated Modular Treatment 645


W. John Livesley

Author Index 677


Subject Index 694
PA RT I

CONCEPTUAL AND TAXONOMIC ISSUES

1
CHAPTER 1

Conceptual Issues

W. John Livesley

It is difficult to characterize the current state there is uncertainty about the value and signifi-
of the study of personality disorder (PD). The cance of these data. As a result, scholars prac-
field is obviously vigorous and productive. tice science, but the results of their efforts do not
Extensive empirical data are being collected constitute a science. Kuhn also noted that the
about an increasingly wide range of topics. In phase is marked by multiple schools of thought
important areas, conclusions based on empiri- and intense debates about legitimate methods,
cal findings are replacing traditional ideas that problems, and standards of evidence that serve
were more speculative in nature. However, the more to define the different schools than to pro-
field is hampered by the lack of a coherent con- duce agreement. In some ways, this seems an
ceptual framework to guide research and sys- apt commentary on contemporary study of PD.
tematize findings, resulting in a mass of infor- Extensive data are being collected. Multiple
mation that often seems to lack coherence. This schools and perspectives exist, such as cogni-
makes it difficult to evaluate the extent to which tive therapy, psychoanalysis, trait psychology,
progress is being made because science is orga- neurobiology, interpersonal theory, behavioral
nized knowledge (Medawar, 1984): It involves theory and therapy, traditional phenomenology,
facts and findings that have internal coherence and so on, each with its own focus of interest,
because they are held together by general prin- methodology, and mode of explanation. Since
ciples and laws. Current theories of PD do not communication between schools is limited,
offer a solution to this problem: Most are con- knowledge tends to get stovepiped. From time
ceptual positions rather than actual theories and to time, there is talk of integration, but it never
are insufficiently developed to bring coherence occurs.
to the field (Lenzenweger & Clarkin, 2005). However, it may also be argued that the study
This situation reflects the early state of the of PD does have a paradigm and has for much
field’s development. All sciences begin this of its recent history: the paradigm of the medi-
way, amassing vast amounts of relatively unre- cal model than underpins contemporary psy-
lated observations. This is how biology started chiatry. The model has structured the field and
as natural history. Viewing the situation from informs most aspects of practice and research.
the perspective of Kuhn’s (1962) description of However, recently, concerns have been raised
the nature of scientific change, the current situ- about the model and its relevance to mental
ation may be viewed as either characteristic of disorders, raising additional concerns about the
the preparadigmatic phase in the development conceptual foundations of the study of PDs.
of a science or as a period that Kuhn referred Although the medical model is usually as-
to as “extraordinary science.” In the prepara- sumed to be a unitary framework, there are
digmatic phase, data collection dominates, but several versions (Bolton, 2008). The version

3
4 C onceptual and T a x onomic I ssues

implicitly adopted by psychiatry is a somewhat and procedures, extreme and speculative con-
simplified form of the traditional disease-as- cepts emerge, and there is usually an increased
entity model of modern medicine (Sabbarton- interest in the philosophical assumptions of the
Leary, Bortolitti, & Broome, 2015). With this field. The latter point is interesting given the
model, symptoms are organized into discrete recent spate of texts and articles on the philoso-
syndromes that are explained by an underly- phy of psychiatry.
ing impairment that is generally assumed to Whether the current situation represents the
be biological. The model’s appeal to psychiatry preparadigmatic or extraordinary science peri-
is understandable given its success in general ods in the emergence of a science of PD is a
medicine, and its assumed relevance was un- matter for philosophers of psychiatry to explore.
doubtedly bolstered by its success at the be- However, both perspectives have similar conse-
ginning of the 20th century with the discovery quences: Either way, the field needs an agreed
that general paresis, a relatively common form paradigm and conceptual framework to guide
of psychosis at the time, was a form of tertiary the acquisition and interpretation of empirical
syphilis due to the spirochete Treponema pal- findings. However, such developments need not
lidum. This created the expectation that major involve a sudden change. The Kuhnian model
causes of other mental disorders would also be of scientific progress is one of revolutionary
identified (Pearce, 2012). Despite the fact that a change, with the creation of a new paradigm
century later this early success has not been re- that leads off a period that he called normal sci-
peated, the idea that “big causes” will be identi- ence, in which progress is incremental until an-
fied for mental disorders lingers on, with infec- other paradigm crisis. Other views of scientific
tious agents being replaced with causes such as progress consider change to occur for a variety
genes, with major effects and specific impair- of reasons and to involve a more gradual pro-
ments in neural mechanisms. cess. This seems more appropriate to PD. This
This version of the medical model was ad- chapter explores these issues. In the first sec-
opted by the neo-Kraepelinian movement (Kl- tion, I begin by briefly tracing the history of the
erman, 1978), which sought to reaffirm the field prior to the publication of DSM-III in 1980
medical foundations of psychiatry. Since the because current conceptions of PD have tangled
neo-Kraepelinian perspective formed the con- roots that continue to exert an influence. The
ceptual foundation for DSM-III and subsequent second section deals with what is referred to as
editions, this version of the model underpins the “DSM era,” dating from the publication of
much of the contemporary study of PD. Recent- DSM-III to the publication of DSM-5. DSM-
ly, however, several authors have noted that the III was a landmark event that helped establish
disease-as-entity version of the model is not ap- systematic empirical research on PD and the
plicable to many disorders in general medicine, assumptions underlying DSM-III continue to
let alone mental disorders (Bolton, 2008; Kend- shape and dominate the contemporary study of
ler, 2012b). The model does not work for dis- PD. Although authors of successive revisions
orders with a complex, multifaceted etiology. of DSM often emphasize the distinctiveness
Since most mental disorders, and certainly most of their revision, continuity across editions is
PDs, have this feature, the models’ relevance to extensive compared to the differences between
the study of PD requires reconsideration. them (Aragona, 2015). The section focuses
Kuhn referred to periods in the evolution of a particularly on the impact and relevance of the
science when an established paradigm is no lon- medical model and the problem of diagnostic
ger viable as periods of extraordinary science. validity. The third section examines principles
Current problems with the medical model and that may contribute to a new conceptual frame-
problems arising from the neo-Kraepelinian work for a science of PDs, including an alterna-
paradigm, most notably the failure to identify tive version of the medical model. In the final
discrete diagnostic categories and the extensive section I briefly consider how these principles
patterns of diagnostic co-occurrence among all might contribute to a more coherent nosology.
forms of mental disorder, may be considered
to create within psychiatry, and hence within
PD, a situation resembling Kuhn’s ideas of ex- Early Conceptions of PD
traordinary science (Aragona, 2009). In such
periods, progress is fragmented, there is wide- Although interest in personality patterns that
spread disagreement about appropriate methods are similar to modern PD diagnoses date to
 Conceptual Issues 5

antiquity, Berrios (1993) argued that the con- century. Maudsley (1874) extended Pritchard’s
temporary concept of PD only truly emerged concept with the observation that some individ-
with the work of Schneider (1923/1950). Nev- uals seemed to lack a moral sense, thereby dif-
ertheless, several developments during the 19th ferentiating what was to become the concept of
century helped to structure current ideas. The psychopathy in the more modern sense. Toward
term “character” was widely used during that the end of the 19th century, German psychiatrist
time to describe the stable and unchangeable Julius Koch proposed the term “psychopathic”
features of a person’s behavior. Writings on the as an alternative to moral insanity. At about the
topic also used the concept of “type,” and Ber- same time, the concept of degeneration, taken
rios noted that “character” became the preferred from French psychiatry, was introduced to ex-
term to refer to psychological types. Although plain this behavior.
the term “type” was used in the contemporary The significance of these developments was
sense to describe discrete patterns of behavior, that the idea of psychopathy as distinct from
the term “personality” was used largely to refer other mental disorders gained acceptance,
to the mode of appearance of the person (Berri- which set the stage for Schneider’s concept of
os, 1993), a usage derived from the Greek term psychopathic personalities as a distinct noso-
for “mask.” Gradually, the term took on a more logical group. Before this occurred, however,
psychological meaning when used to refer to Kraepelin (1907) introduced a different per-
the subjective aspects of the self. Hence, 19th- spective by suggesting that personality distur-
century writings about the disorders of person- bances were attenuated forms ( formes frustes)
ality referred to mechanisms of self-awareness of the major psychoses. Kraepelin’s seminal
and disorders of consciousness, and not to the contributions to nosology with the distinction
behavior patterns that we now recognize as PD. between dementia praecox and manic–depres-
It was only in the early 20th century that the sive illness are generally considered to firmly
term “personality” began to be used in its pres- establish the medical model as the basis for
ent sense. However, it is interesting to note the conceptualizing and classifying mental disor-
recent resurgence of interest in self-awareness ders. Subsequently, Kretschmer (1925) took the
as a core impairment of PD. idea of PDs as attenuated forms of mental state
The evolution of the concept of PD during disorders further by positing a continuum from
the 19th century was influenced by studies of schizothyme through schizoid to schizophre-
moral insanity by Pritchard (1835) and others. nia—an idea that anticipated current thinking
Although “moral insanity” is often considered about schizophrenia spectrum disorders. The
the predecessor of psychopathy, Pritchard’s de- notion that PDs such as borderline personality
scription shows little resemblance to Cleckley’s disorder (BPD) are on a continuum with some
(1941/1976) concept of psychopathy or DSM an- major mental state disorders rather than distinct
tisocial personality disorder (ASPD; Whitlock, nosological entities, and hence that PDs are not
1967, 1982). Rather, Pritchard used the term to a distinct nosological grouping, continues to be
describe forms of insanity that did not include raised intermittently despite extensive concep-
delusions. The predominant understanding tual and empirical evidence to the contrary.
of the time was that delusions were an inher- Nonetheless, the overriding assumption of
ent component of insanity, an idea developed psychiatric classification for much of the last
by John Locke. The term “moral insanity” de- century has been that mental state disorders
scribed diverse conditions, including mood dis- and PDs are distinct, although the nature of
orders that had in common the absence of delu- this distinction has differed across conceptual
sions. Berrios (1993) suggested that Pritchard frameworks. Jaspers (1923/1963) offered a co-
encouraged the development of a descriptive gent theoretical rationale for the distinction by
psychopathology of mood disorders that pro- differentiating personality developments from
moted the differentiation of these disorders disease processes. The idea had little impact
from related conditions and the differentiation on American psychiatry, although it is probably
of personality from other disorders by distin- worth revisiting. Personality developments are
guishing more transient symptomatic states assumed to result in changes that are under-
from more enduring characteristics. This im- standable in terms of the individual’s previous
portant development promoted the emergence personality, whereas the changes associated
of PDs as a separate diagnostic group. Interest with disease processes are not predictable from
in moral insanity continued throughout the 19th the individual’s premorbid status. Jaspers sug-
6 C onceptual and T a x onomic I ssues

gested that these different forms of psychopa- are defined in term of social deviance, where-
thology require different methods of classifi- upon the diagnosis is then used to explain devi-
cation, with conditions arising from disease ant behavior.
processes being conceptualized as either pres- Although psychopathic personalities were
ent or absent and hence classified as discrete portrayed as types, it is important to note that
categories, whereas PDs (and neuroses) should Jaspers’s (1963) and Schneider’s (1923/1950)
be classified as ideal types. This issue is still concept of ideal type was not that of a simple di-
unresolved and contributed to much of the con- agnostic category, as is the case with DSM-III to
fusion associated with the DSM-5 classification DSM-5. Ideal types are patterns of being rather
of PD. than diagnoses. According to Jaspers, an ideal
Schneider’s volume Psychopathic Person- typology consists of polar opposites such as
alities published in 1923 was a landmark event dependency and independence or introversion
that largely established the contemporary ap- and extraversion. Diagnosis does not involve
proach to PDs. Berrios (1993) suggested that ascribing a typal diagnosis. Instead, individuals
by adopting the term “personality,” Schneider are compared to contrasting poles of the type
made concepts such as temperament and char- to illuminate clinically important aspects of
acter redundant. There is much to be said for their behavior and personality. Thus, the typol-
this position, although, unfortunately, this clar- ogy is essentially a framework for conducting
ity has not been widely accepted (for further clinical assessment and formulating individual
discussion, see Chanen, Tackett, & Thompson, cases. Moreover, ideal types are not stable in the
Chapter 12, this volume). Schneider also made sense that DSM diagnoses were originally as-
the important conceptual distinction between sumed to be stable. Instead, some are episodic
abnormal and disordered personality, an issue and reactive. Thus, Schneider’s (1923/1950) sys-
of current significance given the demonstrated tem represents a more complex understanding
continuity between PDs and normal personal- of types and the relationship between normal
ity. Schneider defined abnormal personality as and disordered personality than that of DSM-III
“deviating from the average.” Thus, abnormal to DSM-5. Although he used the term “type,”
personality merely represents the extremes of his conceptualization implicitly acknowledges
normal personality variation. However, Schnei- continuity with normal personality. In addition,
der also recognized that this was not an ad- Schneider’s “types” are not discrete categories;
equate definition of pathology because extreme rather, they refer to individuals at the extremes
variation does not necessarily imply dysfunc- of a continuum, much as Eysenck used the term
tion or disability. He referred to the subgroup later to refer to those as the poles of the con-
of abnormal personalities that are dysfunctional tinuum introversion–extraversion. In this sense,
in a clinical sense as psychopathic personali- Schneider anticipated current ideas derived
ties, which were defined as “abnormal person- from trait models that PDs represent extremes
alities who either suffer personally because of of normal variation, although he added criteria
their abnormality or make a community suffer to differentiate pathological from nonpatho-
because of it” (p. 3). Schneider did not discuss logical variation. Schneider also disagreed with
abnormal personality in detail but concentrated Kraepelin’s idea that PDs are systematically
instead on describing 10 varieties of psycho- related to the major psychoses, although he as-
pathic personality: hyperthymic, depressive, sumed that personality affected the form that
insecure (sensitives and anankasts), fanatical, a psychosis takes. Schneider’s position is not
attention-seeking, labile, explosive, affection- without problems, particularly in regard to the
less, weak-willed, and asthenic. Here the term definition of suffering. Nevertheless, he intro-
“psychopathic personality” was used to cover duced into the classification of PD a conceptual
all forms of PD and neurosis. In the preface to clarity that has rarely been matched.
the ninth edition, written in 1950, Schneider Within British and American psychiatry,
noted that the term “psychopath” was not well the concepts of psychopathy and psychopathic
understood and that his work was not the study personality were defined more narrowly to de-
of asocial or delinquent personality. He added scribe what we now call ASPD, although the
that “some psychopathic personalities may act two are not synonymous. Descriptions of psy-
in an antisocial manner but . . . this is secondary chopathy and, later, descriptions of PDs, were
to the psychopathy” (p. x). Thus, he avoided the largely based on clinical observation. Theoreti-
tautology inherent in conceptions of ASPD that cal factors that influenced Jaspers (1963) and
 Conceptual Issues 7

Schneider (1923/1950) played little part in no- The 1960s and 1970s saw the first empirical
sological development, and various definitions investigations with pioneering work of Grinker,
emerged as individual clinicians emphasized Werble, and Drye (1968), followed quickly in
different facets of these disorders and different the United Kingdom with studies by Presly and
aspects of the overall class. Walton (1973) and Tyrer and Alexander (1979).
Parallel to these developments, psychoana- However, the pre-DSM-III era was dominated
lytic concepts also contributed to classification by clinical description by the classical Euro-
and enriched ideas about personality pathology, pean phenomenologists and clinical constructs
but in the process they increased diagnostic formulated by psychoanalytic thinkers.
and descriptive confusion. Although Freud was Thus, DSM-III was developed in the context
not primarily interested in PD, his theory of of a rich but confusing array of conceptions of
psychosexual development led to descriptions PD (see Rutter, 1987). These included PD as (1)
of character types associated with each stage a forme fruste of major mental state disorders as
(Abraham, 1921/1927) that became the basis proposed by Kraepelin (1907) and Kretschmer
for dependent, obsessive–compulsive, and hys- (1925); (2) the failure to develop important com-
terical (changed to histrionic in DSM-III) PDs. ponents of personality, as illustrated by Cleck-
This development shifted assumptions about ley’s (1941/1976) concept of psychopathy as the
etiology away from the biological mechanisms failure to learn from experience and to show
stressed by the medical model toward psycho- remorse; (3) a particular form of personality
social factors. Subsequently, the concept of structure or organization as illustrated by Kern-
character was formulated more clearly by Reich berg’s (1984) concept of borderline personality
(1933/1949), who proposed that psychosexual organization defined in terms of identity diffu-
conflicts lead to relatively fixed patterns that sion, primitive defenses, and reality testing; and
he referred to as “character armor.” Reich also (4) social deviance as illustrated by Robins’s
influenced diagnostic concepts of PD because (1966) concept of sociopathic personality as
his interest in treating characterological con- the failure of socialization. In the background
ditions with psychoanalysis led to the descrip- there also lurked the idea of abnormal person-
tion of individuals who were neither psychotic ality in the statistical sense, as represented by
nor neurotic, which ultimately led to concept conceptions of PD derived from normal per-
of BPD, also considered largely psychosocial sonality structure. These different conceptions
in nature. The phenomenological tradition was also placed different emphases on the medical
also interested in borderline conditions, al- model as the basis for conceptualizing PDs.
though these were understood differently. The
“border” in which these phenomenologists were
interested was between normality and psycho- The DSM Era
sis stemming from observations that patient’s
family members often showed unusual features, The DSM-III classification and the relatively
a conception that was more rooted in the medi- minor revisions in DSM-III-R, DSM-IV, and
cal model. Hence prior to DSM-III, the term DSM-5 (except for parts of the alternative
“borderline” referred to a variety of syndromes models listed in Section III) have dominated
derived from diverse positions (Stone, 1980) research and treatment. Despite frequent revi-
and hence conceptualized and described dif- sions, continuities across editions far outweigh
ferently: Those derived from phenomenological specific changes (Aragona, 2015), and these
psychiatry were largely descriptive concepts, continuities have profoundly influenced all
whereas those based on psychoanalysis were aspects of the field. The DSM-III decisions to
described in terms of inner mental structures place PDs on a separate axis, and to diagnose
and processes. Later, psychoanalytic concepts them using the diagnostic criteria approach
of PD were further extended with the formula- used with other disorders, stimulated clinical
tion of narcissistic conditions by Kohut (1971) interest and empirical research. It is perhaps
and others. This period from approximately the ironic that these innovations have had such a
1930s to the 1970s was associated with strong lasting impact because neither has stood the
reactions against the medical model by many test of time. Multiaxial classification was aban-
psychoanalysts and to a substantial decrease in doned for DSM-5, and the assumption of dis-
interest in classification, although much more crete categories is inconsistent with empirical
so in America than in Europe. findings. Nevertheless, the development of di-
8 C onceptual and T a x onomic I ssues

agnostic criteria for PDs was an important step: DSM classification in terms the medical model
It encouraged construction of semistructured and the problem of validity. The intent is not to
interviews during the 1980s that in turn facili- provide an in-depth review of DSM-III–DSM-5
tated empirical research. Although these mea- but rather to highlight issues that are critical to
sures are unlikely to make a strong contribution improving the conceptualization and diagnostic
to future research, they established the impor- classification of PD. A more detailed review of
tance of psychometrically sound measures. official classifications is provided by Thomas
To appreciate the impact of DSM-III, it is Widiger (Chapter 3, this volume).
useful to recall the context in which it was de-
veloped. In the decades preceding its publica-
tion, psychiatry was under attack from many The Medical Model
directions (Blashfield, 1984). First, psychiatry’s
credibility was challenged by concern about The medical model was the foundation for un-
diagnostic reliability and marked international derstanding mental disorders and hence for
differences in diagnostic practices. Second, classification for much of the early 20th cen-
concerns were voiced from multiple sources, tury. Subsequently, its role was diluted by the
including humanistic psychology, psychoanaly- impact of psychoanalysis, and its relevance was
sis, and the antipsychiatry movement, about the challenged by the various critiques of psychia-
emphasis placed on the medical model and its try discussed earlier. The neo-Kraepelinians
relevance to psychiatry. Third, criticism also sought to change this situation. As a result of
arose from sociology and labeling theory that their influence on DSM-III, their version of
the diagnostic labels psychiatrists used became the medical model exerted an enormous im-
self-fulfilling prophecies that strongly affected pact both directly through an emphasis on
the person being labeled. This criticism was discrete syndromes and the search for a major
reinforced by Rosenhan’s (1973) study show- causes and specific pathologies for given di-
ing that mental health professionals could not agnoses, and indirectly through the neglect of
differentiate severely mentally ill from healthy possible contributions of other perspectives,
individuals. The study involved eight healthy most notably normal personality research. The
individuals seeking admission to 12 different neo-Kraepelinan understanding of the medical
inpatient units. They reported accurate infor- model more than anything else accounts for the
mation about themselves except their names (to way the study of PD has evolved over the last
preserve their privacy) and having heard a voice 30 years and for the failure of the DSM to show
saying a single word such as “thud” or “hollow.” evidence of consistent improvement across re-
All were admitted for an average of about 22 visions. This section explores the relevance of
days, and in 11 instances, participants were di- this model to PD and its impact on the field.
agnosed as having schizophrenia; the other par-
ticipant was diagnosed as having mania. In all
Relevance to PD
cases, the discharge diagnosis was schizophre-
nia in remission. The medical model adopted by psychiatry
These criticisms led to the formation of the works best for disorders with a specific etiol-
neo-Kraepelinian movement (Blashfield, 1984) ogy and pathogenesis. It does not work well
that reaffirmed psychiatry as a branch of medi- when disorders have complex etiology involv-
cine and the medical model as the foundation for ing multiple interacting mechanisms (see Kend-
conceptualizing and treating mental disorders. ler, 2012a, 2012b). This circumstance clearly
The neo-Kraepelinian credo, as summarized by applies to PDs: A wide range of psychosocial
Klerman (1978), consisted of nine propositions and biological risk factors has been identified
that strongly influenced DSM-III. The propo- in the last two decades. Psychosocial factors
sitions with most impact on the classification are extremely variable, ranging from attach-
of PD included the following: psychiatry is a ment problems to cultural influences (see Paris,
branch of medicine; there is a boundary be- Chapter 17, this volume). Each factor seems to
tween the normal and the sick; there are discrete exert a small effect, and none is necessary or
mental illnesses; diagnostic criteria should be sufficient to cause disorder. Biological influ-
codified; and research should be directed at im- ences have a similar structure. Although PDs
proving the diagnostic reliability and validity. are heritable, multiple genes contribute to the
In the rest of this section I critically examine the predisposition toward PDs, each having a small
 Conceptual Issues 9

effect, so that the absence of a given gene prob- including actions, emotions, beliefs, meaning
ably has little effect. More importantly, PD does systems, interpretations, motivations, thoughts,
not appear to be explained by a specific genetic and cognitive processes. With PDs, the situa-
mechanism (Turkheimer, 2015). This situation tion is even more complex. Other mental dis-
also appears to apply to other biological risk orders bear some similarity to general medical
factors. Although there is in PDs an underlying disorders in that they may also be represented
biology in the general sense that any psycholog- by symptoms and signs, as are the disorders
ical process must be accompanied by some kind of general medicine, albeit with more complex
of neural event, major biological cause has not symptoms. However, PDs are also diagnosed on
been identified. Here, the term “major biologi- the basis of attitudes and traits (Foulds, 1965,
cal cause” is used in Meehl’s (1972) sense of a 1976), and current diagnostic conceptions also
biological factor that is found in all individuals include identity problems, self pathology, rela-
with the disorder but not in individuals without tionship issues, and narratives. This introduces
the disorder. The failure to find major biologi- a different order of complexity, one that is diffi-
cal cause is not specific to PDs but has proved cult to capture fully using the disease-as-entity
elusive for most mental disorders (Turkheimer, version of the medical model espoused by psy-
2015). This does not mean that the effort to chiatric nosology.
unravel the biological mechanisms associated A second problem is that features used to
with PDs is unimportant. To the contrary, such diagnose PDs are not necessarily indicative of
research can only add to our understanding of disorder, a circumstance that applies to other
these conditions and enhance treatment options. mental disorders. This contrasts with the symp-
It does, however, mean that these mechanisms toms of general medicine. Pain, for example,
need to be understood as part of a complex eti- always indicates a change for the normal state,
ology, and that they are unlikely to be very help- even if the pain is transient and without lasting
ful in resolving taxonomic problems. diagnostic significance. However, it is hard to
The etiology of PD also incorporates a com- find a feature of PD that invariably indicates
plexity not observed with most medical condi- disorder. In fact, it is hard to find any feature
tions: The diverse etiological factors contribut- that does not occur in healthy individuals. Thus,
ing to a given clinical picture often influence the significance of a diagnostic item cannot be
different components of psychopathology. For determined in isolation: It always needs to be
example, with the DSM diagnostic construct evaluated within the context of the person’s
of BPD, trauma and abuse may primarily affect total personality and life experience.
emotional reactivity and stress responsivity, The problems created for the medical model
whereas consistent invalidation may primarily approach to classification and diagnosis are
affect self pathology through the development compounded by the diverse psychopathology
of self-invalidating thinking. This is a very dif- of PD and by the way pathology extends to all
ferent circumstance from that occurring with parts of the personality system. As a result,
many medical conditions in which the primary many psychopathological features are com-
causal factor is implicated in most symptoms. mon to multiple putatively distinct diagnoses,
Recently, other concerns about the rele- and few features are specific to a given condi-
vance of the medical model to psychiatry have tion. Discrete and nonoverlapping clusters of
emerged that go beyond matters of etiology by symptoms so characteristic of general medical
raising questions about the very nature of men- disorders do not occur with PD. This fact that
tal disorders that have prompted the suggestion this has often been downplayed and even ig-
that psychiatry has a unique status among medi- nored by DSM in order to create distinct types
cal specialties (see Gadamar, 1996). One such has sometimes been distorted the way PD is
conceptual challenge relates to the fact that psy- represented. A good example is the decision to
chiatry addresses a far wider range of “symp- exclude quasi-psychotic features and transient
toms” than other medical disciplines (Varga, psychotic states from BPD criteria in DSM-III
2015). Whereas most general medical disorders in an attempt to ensure a clear distinction from
are diagnosed through relatively straightfor- schizotypal personality disorder, a decision
ward symptoms consisting primarily of sen- later reversed in DSM-IV.
sations, perceptions, and motility anomalies, The rich and diverse pathology observed in
mental disorders are diagnosed on the basis of all cases creates the additional problem of how
more complex, less readily observed features, to decide what features to focus on for diag-
10 C onceptual and T a x onomic I ssues

nostic purposes. With most disorders in gen- Consequences of the Medical Model
eral medicine, symptoms are obvious, few in
The version the medical model applied to psy-
number, easily identified, and closely related to
chiatry and PD has hindered progress by focus-
tissue pathology. PDs are palpably different in
ing attention on the identification of discrete
this respect in that they represent differences in
types, decreasing interest in alternative models,
kind. As a result, rules or guidelines are needed
to establish what is and what is not pertinent to and inadvertently leading to a neglect of psy-
diagnosis. Currently such guidelines are poorly chopathology.
developed. With DSM, diagnostic features were
selected through a committee process presum- Assumption of Discrete Categorical Diagnoses
ably guided by traditional clinical opinion. As
a result, most sets of criteria are a mixture of A brief examination of recent articles in key
items that include general behaviors, specific journals or conference presentations reveals
behaviors, traits, interpersonal matters, self- the extent to which research and treatment are
problems, and self-attitudes, and the constructs dominated by the assumption that disorders
used vary widely across diagnoses. The case distinct from each other and from normal per-
could be made that some medical conditions are sonality variation exist. We only need to look at
symptomatically more diverse than has been how DSM performs in practice to see that the
suggested. However, this merely strengthens system is fatally flawed. The rampant patterns
the case against applying the diseases-as-enti- of diagnostic co-occurrence refute the neo-
ty model to PDs. Such disorders tend to have Kraepelinian assumption of discrete disorders
a complex etiology, and these are the disorders on which DSM-III to DSM-5 rest, and the prob-
that have prompted the observation that the lem is compounded by the prevalence of per-
medical model is not even applicable to some sonality disorder not otherwise specified (Ver-
disorders of general medicine (Bolton, 2008; heul & Widiger, 2004). There is no need to look
Kendler, 2012b). beyond DSM to realize that it fails to meet its
The contemporary study of PDs has either design criteria. However, if we turn to research
largely neglected these problems or reframed designed to evaluate the system, the magnitude
them in terms of the medical model. Thus, di- of the problem is even more apparent. We have
agnostic criteria are commonly referred to as known for nearly a quarter of a century that the
“symptoms” of PD even though they are highly features of PD are continuously distributed (see
inferential in nature and radically different in early reviews by Livesley, Schroeder, Jackson,
content and form from the symptoms of gen- & Jang, 1994; Widiger, 1993), conclusions con-
eral medicine. The traditional medical practice firmed by the failure of more recent studies to
of defining symptoms as features of illness that identify replicable personality types (Eaton,
patients complain about is neglected in what Krueger, South, Simms, & Clark, 2011; Leis-
often seems to be an attempt to medicalize PDs. ing & Zimmermann, 2011; Widiger, Livesley,
Similarly, diagnostic overlap due to the absence & Clark, 2009). However, the dominance of the
of discrete boundaries between putatively dis- medical model is such that the field is impervi-
tinct disorders and the failure to conceptualize ous to empirical evidence on this point. Perhaps
distinct entities is referred to as “comorbidity,” the most blatant example of disregard for evi-
although the term was originally developed to dence is provided by DSM-5: Although the Per-
refer to the co-occurrence of distinct condi- sonality and Personality Disorders Work Group
tions. This casual use of “medical” creates that concluded that “personality features and psy-
impression of continuity between psychiatry chopathological tendencies do not tend to delin-
and general medicine when there are impor- eate categories of persons in nature” (Krueger
tant differences and imply the relevance of the et al., 2011, pp. 170–171), categorical diagnoses
medical model when this is not the case. The were retained and the work group even opted to
rigid application of such a narrow version of retain typal diagnoses in the alternative model
the medical model to PDs has led to the con- presented in Section III of DSM-5.
tinued use of a mode of diagnostic assessment The consequences of the persistence reliance
ill-suited to either understanding and treating on categorical diagnoses are not trivial. Con-
the heterogeneity and individuality of clinical siderable research effort is devoted to studying
presentations or providing the foundation for a problems such as diagnostic overlap, which are
science of PD. largely artifacts of the assumption of discrete
 Conceptual Issues 11

disorders, and to identifying the most effec- processes critical to understanding the psycho-
tive way to diagnose each type. However, the pathology involved.
effects of pursuing pseudoproblems are modest
compared to the extent to which the category
Neglect of Normal Personality Science
assumption distorts research by influencing the
problems studied, the research questions asked, Another indirect consequence of the medical
and the methods used. It also promotes the as- model is the failure to draw on normal per-
sumption that there is a limited array of PDs as sonality research in the search for better con-
opposed to multiple ways in which personality ceptual and taxonomic models. This neglect is
can be disordered, an alternative clinical con- curiously inconsistent with the medical model
ception that I explore later. that the field seeks to emulate. Disorder is a
normative concept that can only be understood
with reference to some kind of norm. Within
Inattention to Psychopathology
medicine, the norm is the normal structure and
An unintended consequence of the DSM’s ad- function of a given system (Bolton, 2008). This
herence to medical model and attendant empha- suggests that the norm for understanding PD is
sis on reliability is the comparative neglect of normal personality. However, conceptions of
the broader psychopathology of PDs. There is normal personality were largely neglected in
a tendency to assume that DSM is the ultimate formulating classifications including DSM-5.
authority on a disorder and its psychopathol- This neglect is somewhat understandable, since
ogy leading to a preoccupation with whether normal personality research is at an early stage
patients “meet criteria” for a given condition compared to the biological sciences underly-
(Andreasen, 2006). There is also a tendency to ing general medicine. Nevertheless, personality
equate diagnostic criteria with the diagnostic science is substantially more advanced than the
construct rather than to recognize criteria as a study of PD and it has the potential to enrich
few of many possible indicators of an underly- ideas about classification and treatment.
ing condition. The authority placed in sets of
DSM criteria also had an ossifying effect that
has discouraged exploration of alternative con- The Problem of Validity
ceptual frameworks. Some authors also see see
this stance as contributing to a growing discon- DSM-III was primarily concerned with improv-
nect between advances in the neurosciences and ing diagnostic reliability to address attacks on
psychiatry (Hyman, 2010). However, more con- psychiatry’s credibility, with the assumption
cerning in the case of PDs is how heightened that once this problem was solved, attention
concern with reliability has led to an impover- would subsequently focus on validity (Klerman,
ished understanding of psychopathology. Diag- 1986). This progression has not occurred. As a
nostic criteria are essentially lists of relatively result, it is difficult to find evidence that DSM-
superficial features selected from a wide range IV/5 is more valid than DSM-III, or indeed that
of possibilities rather than definitive defini- it is more valid than the taxonomy Schneider
tions as is so often assumed. Each criterion also proposed nearly a century ago. Nevertheless,
tends to be seen as a distinct “self-contained” proponents of DSM commonly proclaim the va-
entity that can be assessed independently of the lidity of both the system and specific diagnoses.
personality and the individual’s other qualities Such claims often reflect different understand-
and life experiences. The result, as Andreasen ings of the meaning of diagnostic validity. As
(2006) noted, is that DSM inadvertently led to Kendell and Jablensky (2003) noted, validity is
a neglect of descriptive psychopathology and to often confused with clinical utility—the issue
a dehumanizing effect on clinical practice. Al- of whether a diagnosis is clinically informative.
though Andreasen was referring to the general One could argue that DSM PDs have clinical
impact of DSM, her comments seem especially utility because clinicians find them useful, but
pertinent to PDs. The syndrome-based descrip- evidence of validity is lacking.
tive categories of DSM seem remarkably crude Confusion also occurs because validity is
when viewed against the rich psychopathology often approached from the different perspec-
of individual cases. They are simply lists of tives of clinical medicine and academic psy-
common features divorced from any coherent chology. Although these perspectives are some-
understanding of the disorder and the complex times intertwined, they tend to be pursued
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back
back

You might also like