You are on page 1of 5

Dysphagia 9:83-87 (1994)

Dysphagia
9 Springer-u NewYorkInc. 1994

Temperature Acceleration in Cold Oral Stimulation


Marilyn Selinger, PhD,~ Thomas E. Prescott, PhD,t and Irwin Hoffman, PhD 2
Denver VAMC/126, Denver, Colorado, and Greenwood Village, Colorado, USA

Abstract. This study measured the temperature acceler- variation in different individuals. Using 126 subjects, no
ation of a cold probe as it contacts human tissue. Both the single area consistently elicited a swallow. The anterior
effects of touching a cold probe to the oral cavity and faucial pillars, however, were reported to be the most
maintaining contact of the cold probe with the oral cavity sensitive to light tactile stimulation with a blunt glass rod;
were investigated. The results indicated a rapid warming this sensitivity was present 77% of the time. Pomme-
pattern. This warming is effected first by temperature renke concluded that the anterior pillars "bear a more
changes resulting from the probe being moved from ice important relationship to the swallowing mechanism."
into room temperature and second by the contact to oral Somewhat more recently, Doty and Bosma [2]
mucosa. In fact, in some cases, the probe had reached examined electromyographic patterns of deglutition.
minimal cold sensation levels by the time it reached the They concluded that there were three anatomical regions
oral cavity. Results also indicated that 6 sec after the comprising a "leading complex" of muscles which fire
probe is lifted from the ice, the temperature closely concurrently to initiate the act of swallowing. This com-
aproximates temperatures perceived as warm or at least plex contains suprahyoid suspensory muscles, muscles
neutral, but not cold. involved in the closing of the palatopharyngeal isthmus,
Key words: Thermal stimulation - - Oral mucosa - - and the muscles surrounding the faucial pillars.
Deglutition - - Deglutition disorders. The relationship between temperature stimulation
and deglutition has also been discussed in the literature
[3-9]. Early studies reported the importance of punctate
stimulation and temperature sensitivity in the oral mu-
Thermal stimulation is a technique used with many dys- cosa [10,11]. These findings suggested that there were
phagic patients for swallowing treatment. The sensitivity warm and cold spots responsible for thermal sensitivity in
and perception of the oral cavity is not completely under- the mouth. The results also indicated that the warm re-
stood. Both the responsivity of the oral mucosa and the ceptors were less dense than those of the cold receptors.
study of cold thermal application need more quantifica- Subsequently, cold was concluded to be a stronger stim-
tion. One of the first aspects of this research is under- ulus for the swallowing reflex. Grossman and Hattis [3]
standing the nature of the stimuli used in cold application reported that the medial portion of the anterior faucial
in the oral cavity. In order to begin quantifying the re- pillars exhibited high sensitivity to cold, that sensitivity
sponse to cold application we looked at acceleration of was reported to decrease as the stimulation moved later-
temperature in a cold probe (not a mirror) as it contacts ally on the pillars.
the human oral mucosa. The origins of cold thermal stimulation treatment
Stimulation of the oral cavity, in order to examine for swallowing disorders probably come from these early
the origins of the deglutition reflex, was reported by investigations concerning tactile stimulation and cold re-
Pommerenke [1] in 1928. His findings indicated a large ceptors. More recent studies, however, have stressed the
importance of spatial summation over larger areas of
tissue as the origins of temperature responsivity in the
oral cavity. Green [7] reported that even though there
This investigation was supported in part by Veterans Administration
Merit Review (Research Advisory Group) grant #331. appears to be a greater amount of innervation for the cold
Address offprint requests to: Marilyn Selinger, Ph.D., Denver VAMC/ sense, the oral cavity was actually more sensitive to
126, 1055 Clermont St., Denver, CO 80220, USA warm temperatures. Utilizing a series of experiments
84 M. Selingeret al.: TemperatureAccelerationin Cold Oral Stimulation

concerned with stimulation through larger spatial sum- swallow." It was felt that the actual thermal stimulation
mation with warm and cool liquids, it was found that procedure as it is described is somewhat imprecise. It was
thermal sensitivity was greater to higher temperatures possible to have missed an important step that would
than cool ones. Therefore, it was concluded that small have made the difference between efficacious and nonef-
areas of stimulation are poor predictors of oral respon- ficacious treatment. Naturally, this concern is generaliz-
siveness to stimulation of larger areas regardless of the able to all patients and all clinicians.
amount of innervation for temperature sensitivity. The As a first step in examining issues related to ther-
more recent data on oral sensitivity and perception raises mal stimulation, this study was designed to measure tem-
interesting issues and opens new avenues for research in perature acceleration in a cold probe as it contacts human
the area of temperature responsivity in the human oral tissue. The following questions were asked:
cavity.
The technique referred to as "thermal stimula- 1. What is the temperature acceleration of a cold probe
tion" in reference to swallowing treatment is often rec- upon contact with the faucial pillars?
ommended to heighten sensation and therefore improve 2. What is the temperature of the cold probe over time
the speed at which the pharyngeal swallow is triggered in with repeated contact to the pillars?
dysphagic patients. Logemann (1983, 1986) recom-
mends the training of patients with thermal stimulation.
Thermal stimulation is believed to enhance the sensitivity Methods
of the faucial arches, thereby triggering the pharyngeal
The measurementof temperature accelerationwas accomplishedutiliz-
swallow more rapidly. Logemann [12,13] recommends ing the IBM Personal Science Lab (PSL) temperature probe. This
using cold stimulation with a laryngeal mirror for height- program allows quantificationof temperaturechange using a computer
ened sensitivity. probe peripheral. The PSL probe is an active metal device from the
Lazarra et al. [14] have reported one experiment AD950 familysimilarto a transistorand is encasedin an insulatormade
using cold thermal stimulation as a means of improving of ceramic. The probe is sensitivefrom -40~ to 105~ The thermal
time constant of the probe is 0.14 sec. The changes at the probe are
swallowing in 25 neurologically impaired patients. The measured by samplingtemperature 10 timesper sec. This unit is manu-
cold stimulation technique described by the authors in- factured by IBM EducationalSystems (Atlanta, GA). The heating and
volves "lightly touching the area with the back of an iced cooling curves follow the laws of exponential models and conse-
size 00 laryngeal mirror." Changes in transit time were quently, ambientchangesin temperatureare quite rapid.
The measurement sequence was designed to approximate the
measured using videofluoroscopic recording of the swal-
techniquedescribedby Logemann[12] so that it wouldbe similarto the
lows. These authors report that the thermal sensitization technique used in this treatmentfor dysphagia. Temperaturesampling
improved immediate transit times in 23 of 25 patients. was carried on for 25 sec for each of 10 trials. This resulted in 250
Measured improvement in transit times were concluded temperature measurementsbased on the samplingrate of 10 times per
to be large enough to reduce the risk of aspiration. sec. The probewas placedagainstan ice cube in a glass of ice; the probe
was left in the ice for the first five sec. AlthoughLogemann[12] has
Rosenbek et al. [15] followed the recommended
recommended "approximatelyten seconds" our probe temperaturere-
treatment for cold thermal stimulation by icing a "00" mained consistently stable, that is, with no significant temperature
laryngeal mirror and subsequently stroking each faucial decrement over 5 sec at a temperature just below freezing (31.4-
pillar three times. These authors report results of limited 31.9~ Based on this measurement,it was felt that 5 sec was adequate
change from thermal stimulation treatment in patients for reaching and maintaininga level at freezing. At 5 sec the probe was
lifted from the ice and moved into the experimeutor's mouth; it was
with multiple strokes.
subsequentlyplacedon the right faucialpillar. The mostobviousdiffer-
Selinger, et al. [16] examined the effects of cold ence betweenthe techniquereported here and that of Logemannis that
thermal stimulation on a patient exhibiting dysfunctional only one pillar was stimulatedand the proberemainedstationaryon that
swallow with laryngeal penetration. It was concluded pillar rather than being used to stroke the pillar. Specific temperatures
that cold thermal stimulation with a 00 laryngeal mirror from the 250 temperaturemeasureswere sampledat 0 sec, 5 sec, 8 sec,
15 sec, and 25 sec for use in the analysis of variance (ANOVA)
was not efficacious for the patient. Several issues were
procedure. Measurementsat 0 and 5 sec were of the probe in the ice, at
raised from this attempt to follow the actual technique 8 sec, the probe had been out of the ice for 3 sec and had been placed
reported by Logemann [12,13]. In 1983, Logemann againstthe faucialpillar, and the next two temperaturesamples(15 and
states that "To stimulate the reflex, the mirror is held in 25 sec) representthe temperatureof the probe in the mouth.
ice water for approximately 10 seconds and lightly
touched to the base of the anterior faucial arch . . . . Light
contact is repeated 5 to 10 times." Logemann [13] stated Results
that "The mirror is placed in ice in order to make it cold,
and it is then put in contact in a stroking motion with the In order to determine if there were any systematic tem-
anterior faucial arch. After five or six contacts on each perature differences across the sampling period, a one-
side of the oral cavity at the faucial arches . . . asked to way ANOVA for repeated measures [17] was performed.
M. Selinger et al.: TemperatureAccelerationin Cold Oral Stimulation 85

Table 1. A N O V A for repeated measures

Source SS DF MS F P

Blocks/Subjects 135.345 9
Temperature 32197.638 4 8049.409 2165.498 < 0.001
Error 133.816 36 3.717
Total 32466.800 49

SS = sum of squares; DF = degrees of freedom; MS = mean square.

40.00

31.96

I 23.92

15,88
Fig. 1. The graph represents the "line of best fit"
7.8<- for Y given X at 5 sec and at 15 sec. Utilizing the
line, it can be seen that an increase in the time that
the probe was withdrawn from the ice resulted in a
-0.200 linear increase (slope = 2.59) in the temperature of
5.05 8.02 15.0 24,94 the probe. Correlation coefficient of r = 0.82 was
SECONDS obtained.

The analysis yielded a significant [F(4,36)= 3.717, sion line to be 2.59108. Utilizing that line it can be seen
p < 0.001) effect. Table 1 summarizes the ANOVA for that an increase in the time that the probe was withdrawn
the sampled temperatures. from the ice resulted in a linear increase in the tempera-
To more precisely determine where the signifi- ture of the probe. In other words, if one wishes to predict
cant effects occurred, a post hoc Scheffe test [18] was the temperature of the probe it can be done by knowing
performed on the temperature means. This analysis indi- the length of time of removal of the probe from the ice
cated that the significant effect occurred between the (r = 0.82). The data indicated that removal of the cold
probe in the ice (X = 0 ~ F) and when it was placed in the probe resulted in a neutral temperature sensation of the
mouth (X = 32.15 ~ F), and between the initial mouth probe in 6-7 sec. Thermal neutrality is defined as temper-
placement and the subsequent measure at 15 sec atures at which subjects report only mild or no sensation
(X = 85.55 ~ F). This measure represents 10 sec out of of hot or cold in the oral cavity [6] (Green, 1985).
the ice. The temperature of a closed mouth approximates
98-99~
In order to further define the relationship between Discussion
temperature acceleration and time, a first-degree (linear)
equation was used [19]. Results from this analysis indi- These results indicate that the temperature changes did
cated a significant (F(1,36) = 76.37, p < 0.01) linear not happen by chance. Careful examination of the tem-
trend. Subsequent analysis for a second degree (qua- perature responses to 2 of the 10 temperature acceleration
dratic) equation indicated no significant (0.05) trend. The measures indicates that between 1.15 and 2.8 sec after
relationship between temperature acceleration and time the probe is lifted from the ice its temperature has accel-
was then defined as linear. This finding is consistent with erated to one of only moderate sensation of cold (20~
the results of the Scheffe test which indicated that the [8]. In addition, between 6 and 7 sec after the probe is
significant effects occurred between the probe in the ice lifted from the ice the temperature has accelerated to one
and the probe into the mouth. In other words, the two of warmth sensation (30~ [8].
temperature measurements at each end of the data were The initial question addressed in this investiga-
not different from the measurement that preceded it. tion was, What is the temperature acceleration of a cold
Subsequently, the "line of best fit" [20] was com- probe upon contact with the faucial pillars? The results
puted (Fig. 1). The data indicated the slope of the regres- indicated a rapid warming pattern. This warming is ef-
86 M. Selinger et al.: Temperature Acceleration in Cold Oral Stimulation

fected first by temperature changes resulting from the and temperature measurements from this study with a
probe being moved from ice into room temperature and thermal stimulation procedure, it can be seen that by the
second by the contact to oral mucosa. In fact, in some time the probe comes in contact with the first faucial
cases, the probe had reached minimal cold sensation lev- pillar it has reached a temperature of minimally cold or
els by the time it reached the oral cavity. neutral in relation to the temperature of the mouth. Re-
The second question addressed in this investiga- peated contact or stroking of the faucial pillars beyond a
tion was, What is the temperature of the cold probe over maximum of 4 sec amounted to probable light tactile
time with repeated contact to the pillars? The results stimulation and possibly minimal warm stimulation but
indicated that 6 sec after the probe is lifted from the ice, did not necessarily result in cold thermal stimulation.
the temperature closely aproximates temperatures per- Results from studies reporting on the efficacy of
ceived as mildly warm or at least neutral, but not cold. thermal stimulation [14-16] have provided conflicting
At this time, actual quantification of the differ- conclusions. It is possible that the reported differences in
ences between the PSL probe and a 00 laryngeal mirror the success of thermal stimulation result from the impre-
have not been made. It is possible, however, to make cise methodology of the thermal stimulation procedure.
some comparative statements regarding the nature of the Multiple stroking of each faucial pillar, when done care-
two instruments and thus hypothesize on the ability of fully, probably results in increased amounts of time in the
each to retain cold temperatures. mouth, with actual cold stimulation being present in only
First, the probe is lower in thermal mass than the the first few sec. Changes in the temperature of the probe
mirror. Lower thermal mass will result in a more rapid over time and in relation to the oral cavity temperature
temperature acceleration because it does not store as probably cause the thermal effects to average out to a
much energy as the larger mirror. The probe is insulated neutral point. Thus, the original idea of cold stimulation
by ceramic material which acts like metal but does not for heightening oral sensation may not be an actuality in
store as much energy. However, the probe handle has technique. The only potentially consistent stimulation to
additional insulation to prevent additional temperature the oral cavity is tactile and this too is not controlled for
acceleration as a result of body heat. in terms of time or amount.
Alternatively, the mirror may be more affected by
body heat when it is handled and by room temperature
because it is not insulated. In addition, the mirror has two
temperature time constants involved in temperature References
changes: metal and glass. The temperature response of
1. Pommerenke WT: A study of sensory areas eliciting the swal-
both the metal and glass may need to be taken into ac- lowing reflex. Am J Physio184(1): 36-41, 1928
count separately when quantifying the thermal properties 2. Doty RW, Bosma JF: An electromyographic analysis of reflex
of the mirror. deglutition. J Neurophysiology 19:44-60, 1956
Neither the mirror nor the probe can be frozen and 3. Grossman RC, Hattis BF: Oral mucosal sensory innervation and
sensory experience: a review. In: Bosma JF (ed.): Oral Sensa-
kept frozen while they are in ambient temperature atmo-
tion and Perception. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1967, pp 5-62
spheres. Taking all of these differences into account the 4. Miller AJ: Deglutition. PhysiolRev 62:129-184, 1982
PSL probe may be more sensitive to temperature acceler- 5. Green RG: Thermal perception on lingual and labial skin. Per-
ation, implying that the subsequent measured tempera- cept Psychophys 36 (3):209-220, 1984
ture changes may be more rapid. 6. Green BG: Heat pain thresholds in the oral-facial region. Per-
cept Psychophys 38 (2): 110-114, 1985
The differences in the tools used to apply cold
7. Green BG: Oral perception of the temperature of liquids. Per-
stimulation is important in that it points out the lack of cept Psychophys 39 (1):19-24, 1986
control over the general parameters involved in thermal 8. Green BG, Gelhard B: Perception of temperature on oral and
stimulation. However, the application of the PSL probe facial skin. Somatosens Res 4(3): 191-200, 1987
is a first step in controlling and quantifying the treatment 9. Kennedy JG, Kent RD: Physiological substrates of normal
deglutition. Dysphagia 3:24-37, 1988
techniques that are used with dysphagic patients. Various
10. Zotterman Y: Specific action potentials in the lingual nerve of
tools are used for this treatment (laryngeal mirrors, cat. ScandArch Physiol 75:105-119, 1936
Q-Tips) resulting in probable variance in temperature 11. Yamada M, Maruhashi J, Miyake N: The distribution of sensory
acceleration curves and varying results. All of the vary- spots on the oral mucous membrane. Jpn J Physiol 2: 328-332,
ing techniques and tools need to be known if one wishes 1952
12. Logemann J: Evaluation and Treatment of Swallowing Disor-
to attribute changes, or lack thereof, to any treatment.
ders. San Diego: College Hill Press, 1983
In general, these findings indicated that thermal 13. Logemann JA: Treatment for aspiration related to dysphagia: an
stimulation of the faucial pillars with a cold probe may overview. Dysphagia 1:34-38, 1986
not be what it is believed to be. By interfacing the time 14. de Lama Lazzara G, Lazarus C, Logemann JA: Impact of ther-
M. Selinger et al.: Temperature Acceleration in Cold Oral Stimulation 87

mal stimulation on the triggering of the swallowing reflex. Dys- 17. Madigan S, Lawrence V: Anova H. Northridge: Human Systems
phagia 1:73-77, 1986 Dynamics, 1982
15. Rosenbek JC, Robbins J, Fishback B, Levine RL: Effects of 18. Roscoe JT: Fundamental Research Statistics for the Behavioral
thermal application on dysphagia after stroke. J Speech Hearing Sciences. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1975
Res 34:1257-1268, 1991 19. Winer BJ: Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. New
16. Selinger M, Prescott TE, McKinley R: The efficacy of thermal York: McGraw-Hill, 1971
stimulation: a case study. Rocky Mountain J Commun Dis 6:21- 20. Edwards AL: Statistical Methods of the Behavioral Sciences.
23, 1990 New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1963

You might also like