You are on page 1of 244

Autism and Child Psychopathology

Series

Series Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

For further volumes:


http://www.springer.com/series/8665
Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Functional Assessment
for Challenging
Behaviors
Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

ISSN 2192-922X e-ISSN 2192-9238


ISBN 978-1-4614-3036-0 e-ISBN 978-1-4614-3037-7
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7
Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London

Library of Congress Control Number: 2012937851

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012


All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the
written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring
Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or
scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval,
electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known
or hereafter developed is forbidden.
The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if
they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not
they are subject to proprietary rights.

Printed on acid-free paper

Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)


Contents

1 Introduction .................................................................................. 1
Johnny L. Matson
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied
Behavior Analysis ......................................................................... 3
Dennis R. Dixon, Talya Vogel, and Jonathan Tarbox
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging
Behavior ........................................................................................ 25
Robert Didden, Peter Sturmey, Jeff Sigafoos,
Russell Lang, Mark F. O’Reilly, and Giulio E. Lancioni
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors ............................................ 45
Giulio E. Lancioni, Nirbhay N. Singh, Mark F. O’Reilly,
Jeff Sigafoos, and Robert Didden
5 Populations and Problems Evaluated with Functional
Assessment .................................................................................... 65
John M. Huete, Patricia F. Kurtz, and R. Justin Boyd
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors......................... 91
Sarah Hurwitz and Noha F. Minshawi
7 Interview and Observation Methods in Functional
Assessment .................................................................................... 105
Alison M. Kozlowski and Johnny L. Matson
8 Experimental Functional Analysis.............................................. 125
Timothy R. Vollmer, Henry S. Roane,
and Amanda B. Rone
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data.......................... 143
Max Horovitz and Johnny L. Matson
10 Scaling Methods of Functional Assessment ............................... 159
Megan Sipes and Johnny L. Matson
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used in Conjunction
with Functional Assessment ........................................................ 179
Nicole M. Rodriguez, Wayne W. Fisher,
and Michael E. Kelley

v
vi Contents

12 The Role of Functional Assessment in Treatment


Planning ........................................................................................ 195
Deborah A. Napolitano, Vicki Madaus Knapp,
Elizabeth Speares, David B. McAdam, and Holly Brown
13 Ethical Issues and Considerations .............................................. 213
Alan Poling, Jennifer L. Austin, Stephanie M. Peterson,
Amanda Mahoney, and Marc Weeden

About the Editor .................................................................................. 235

Index ...................................................................................................... 237


Contributors

Jennifer L. Austin School of Psychology, University of Glamorgan,


Pontypridd, UK
R. Justin Boyd Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy Krieger
Institute and The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore,
MD, USA
Holly Brown Hillside Children’s Center, Rochester, NY, USA
University of Rochester School of Nursing, Rochester, NY, USA
Robert Didden Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University
Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Dennis R. Dixon Center for Autism and Related Disorders Inc., Tarzana, CA,
USA
Wayne W. Fisher Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, Munroe-Meyer
Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, 985450 Nebraska Medical
Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Max Horovitz Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
John M. Huete Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy Krieger
Institute and The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore,
MD, USA
Sarah Hurwitz Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of
Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Christian Sarkine Autism Treatment Center, James Whitcomb Riley Hospital
for Children, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Michael E. Kelley Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, Munroe-Meyer
Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, 985450 Nebraska Medical
Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Vicki Madaus Knapp Summit Educational Resources, Getzville, NY, USA
Alison M. Kozlowski Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Patricia F. Kurtz Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy Krieger
Institute and The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore,
MD, USA

vii
viii Contributors

Giulio E. Lancioni Department of Psychology, University of Bari, Bary,


Italy
Russell Lang College of Education, Texas State University, San Marcos,
TX, USA
Amanda Mahoney Department of Psychology, Western Michigan
University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Johnny L. Matson Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
David B. McAdam University of Rochester School of Medicine, Rochester,
NY, USA
Noha F. Minshawi Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of
Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Christian Sarkine Autism Treatment Center, James Whitcomb Riley Hospital
for Children, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Deborah A. Napolitano University of Rochester School of Medicine,
Rochester, NY, USA
Mark F. O’Reilly Department of Special Education, College of Education,
The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Stephanie M. Peterson Department of Psychology, Western Michigan
University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Alan Poling Department of Psychology, Western Michigan University,
Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Henry S. Roane State University of New York Upstate Medical University,
Syracuse, NY, USA
Nicole M. Rodriguez Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, Munroe-
Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, 985450 Nebraska
Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Amanda B. Rone Psychology Department, University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, USA
Jeff Sigafoos School of Educational Psychology and Pedagogy, Victoria
University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
Nirbhay N. Singh American Health and Wellness Institute, Raleigh, NC, USA
Megan Sipes Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Elizabeth Speares Hillside Children’s Center, Rochester, NY, USA
Peter Sturmey Department of Psychology, Queens College, City University
of New York, Flushing, NY, USA
Contributors ix

Jonathan Tarbox Center for Autism and Related Disorders Inc., Tarzana,
CA, USA
Talya Vogel Center for Autism and Related Disorders Inc., Tarzana, CA,
USA
Timothy R. Vollmer Psychology Department, University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, USA
Marc Weeden Department of Psychology, Western Michigan University,
Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Department of Special Education and Literacy Studies, Western Michigan
University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Introduction
1
Johnny L. Matson

Functional assessment has become a heavily of defining and observing behaviors are the first
studied and frequently used method in the field of building blocks in functional assessment and thus
applied behavior analysis. The literature as a constitute the first chapter in this section. This
result has grown rapidly, and thus, a volume on section is followed by four chapters on specific
the topic is timely. This book consists of chapters methods of assessment. These methods include
that cover the major issues in the field. The book interviews, observations, experimental functional
begins with a historical overview that highlights analysis, in vivo assessment, and different scal-
the roots of functional assessment in the begin- ing methods. The third and final section is com-
ning of applied behavior analysis and the focus prised of four chapters. These include practical
on antecedent events. Since functional assess- considerations such as treatment methods and
ment is used primarily for challenging behaviors planning, methods and procedures to clarify
such as self-injury and aggression, three chapters ambiguous functional assessment results, and
are devoted to the nature, prevalence, character- ethical considerations.
istics, and functions of these behaviors. Also, the The overall purpose of our book is to provide
populations studied and most likely to benefit an up-to-date resource for clinicians and research-
from these interventions are discussed at length. ers, covering all the major issues on functional
The second section of the book is a review of assessment. The book should serve as a reference
specific methods and procedures that are rou- and hopefully a guide for persons working in the
tinely used in a functional assessment. Methods field of applied behavior analysis.

J.L. Matson (*)


Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 1


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_1,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
A Brief History of Functional
Analysis and Applied Behavior 2
Analysis

Dennis R. Dixon, Talya Vogel, and Jonathan Tarbox

The history of functional analysis, as both a publication of this volume), but it was not always
concept and a procedure, can be traced back to so. Early applications of behavioral principles to
the earliest days of the discipline of applied problematic behaviors in humans often failed to
behavior analysis (ABA) and even to the earliest acknowledge the underlying function of behavior
days of basic research in behavior analysis that (Mace, 1994). In this model, the application of
formed the foundation for ABA. Indeed, it is not potent reinforcers and punishers were effective
unreasonable to state that the history of functional when they simply overcame the ongoing contin-
analysis is inextricably linked to the history of the gencies with which they were juxtaposed, not
discipline of ABA. The general discipline of ABA because they addressed the core underlying cause
and the concepts and methods of functional anal- or function of the behavior. The shift in focus to
ysis have been built upon the conceptual founda- understanding the operant function of behavior
tion of operant conditioning, and as advancements before treating it marked a major evolution in
have been made in the basic and conceptual are- ABA, but to say such a perspective was not pres-
nas of behavior analysis, new refinements have ent until standardized experimental assessment
been made in the area of application. methodology had been published is an overstate-
It is sometimes said that the development of ment. As discussed in this chapter, the importance
experimental functional analysis methodology by of understanding the environmental contingencies
Iwata, among others, led to a major shift in focus responsible for maintaining all behavior, aberrant
in the field of ABA toward an acknowledgment of or adaptive, was present since the beginnings of
the need for understanding the root causes of the field, in Skinner’s writings and elsewhere. But
behavior before simply applying potent conse- little in the way of practical procedures was avail-
quences. Some call this a transition from “behav- able for directly addressing the functions of
ior modification” to “applied behavior analysis” behavior in clinical settings. As we outline in
(Mace, 1994). Appreciating the function of behav- this chapter, the pioneering work of Bijou, Lovaas,
ior when planning treatment is now widely Iwata, and Carr, among others, spurred a revolu-
regarded as best practice (as evidenced by the tion in applied behavioral research by developing
a basic experimental format for functional analy-
sis that continues to thrive decades later.
D.R. Dixon (*) • T. Vogel • J. Tarbox In this chapter, we start by briefly outlining
Center for Autism and Related Disorders Inc., the history of behavior analysis, the development
19019 Ventura Blvd., Tarzana, CA 91356, USA
of ABA, and the origins of procedures designed
e-mail: d.dixon@centerforautism.com;
t.vogel@centerforautism.com; to ascertain the function of challenging behavior.
j.tarbox@centerforautism.com We then describe the landmark paper by Iwata,

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 3


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_2,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
4 D.R. Dixon et al.

Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982), the B. F. Skinner’s first major treatise, The Behavior
influence of which is evidenced in that, to date, it of Organisms (1938), spurred the development of
has been cited by 235 publications indexed in what would come to be known as the “experimen-
PsycINFO (plus an additional 533 publications tal analysis of behavior,” now known simply as
that cite its 1994 reprint). The remainder of the “behavior analysis.” Like Watson, Skinner was
chapter is dedicated to a brief overview of proce- interested in giving a scientific account for all
dural refinements which have been developed up behavior, but unlike other psychologists of his
to the current day, the majority of which are time, he found the S–R paradigm insufficient to
described in greater detail in other chapters within explain the majority of behavior, especially for
this volume. those behaviors for which there appeared to be no
obvious antecedent environmental causes (Cooper
et al., 2007). Somewhat serendipitously, while
Historical Roots of Behavior Analysis studying the ingestive behavior of rats, Skinner
observed that environmental events that immedi-
A bit of historical background will set the stage ately followed behaviors had as much or more
for a description of the beginnings of behavior influence on the future occurrence of those behav-
analysis, a thoroughly natural science of psychol- iors as did the antecedents. Skinner then promoted
ogy. Psychology in the early 1900s was domi- the idea that the traditional model of S–R cause-
nated by the study of mental processes and and-effect should be abandoned and replaced by a
introspection. Carefully observing one’s own more descriptive, functional analysis of the rela-
conscious mental, emotional, or feeling states tionship between independent and dependent vari-
was the primary method of investigation (Wolf, able (Sturmey & Bernstein, 2004), consisting of
1978). Until the introduction of behaviorism, the three-term contingency (antecedent–behavior–
introspective psychology dominated American consequence or stimulus–response–stimulus).
psychological thinking (Watson, 1913). Even It was soon found that Skinner’s S–R–S model
within this context, though, psychologists were was able to account for behaviors that the S–R
grappling with the causes of behavior. Early model did not sufficiently explain—behaviors that
functionalists, such as William James (1890), did not have apparent antecedent causes or for
posited that mental processes had evolved to which the consequences figured more promi-
serve useful functions for individuals struggling nently. He called these behaviors “operant,” that
to cope in complex environments. is, those behaviors are influenced by the conse-
Watson (1913) argued for an objective study quences of similar behaviors in an organism’s past
of behavior as a natural science, consisting of (Cooper et al., 2007).
direct observation of the relationships between The three-term contingency provided a model
environmental stimuli and behavioral responses. for studying behavior in a new way. Rather than
In doing so, he laid the groundwork for an analy- searching for inner psychic causes of behavior, as
sis of how the environment determined behavior. traditional psychologists had done, and rather
He was confident in stimulus–response (S–R) than searching for simplistic antecedent causes,
psychology’s ability to predict and control human as S–R psychologists had done, the task of the
behavior, so much so that many would argue he behaviorist was now to identify reliable three-
overstated the potential significantly (Skinner, term contingencies that describe behavior/
1974). Even so, Watson’s insistence upon study- environment relations. Scientific understanding
ing relations between behavior and environment, of behavior, then, was achieved by identifying
in their own right, was critical in establishing the and manipulating the environmental variables
belief that behavior could be studied as a natural that change the probability of its occurrence.
science, on par with biology, physics, and the Through repeated manipulation of environmental
other natural sciences (Cooper, Heron, & variables, a functional relationship between envi-
Heward, 2007). ronment (independent variable) and behavior
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 5

(dependent variable) can be discovered. For should be extended to explain and change the
Skinner, the term “functional relationship,” which behavior of people in everyday arenas such as
describes the relationship between behavior and education, work, clinical problems, and social
environment, essentially became synonymous behavior. While most experimental psychologists
with “cause-and-effect” relationship (Skinner, were inherently conservative in describing the
1953). In Skinner’s words, “the external variables generalizability of their work to practical situa-
of which behavior is a function provide for what tions, Skinner managed to address a wide array of
may be called a causal or functional analysis. We human situations from an entirely behavioral
undertake to predict and control the behavior of point of view. In the 1950s and 1960s, research on
the individual organism” (Skinner, 1953, p. 35). application of behavior analysis began in earnest.
The term “functional analysis,” in its original
use, simply meant an activity that shed light upon
the potential ways in which the environment may Applying Behavioral Principles
control behavior (Skinner, 1953). It did not origi- to Humans
nally connote an experimental analysis, as it often
does today, although Skinner always preferred Research during the 1950s and 1960s utilized the
experimental over descriptive analyses. In Verbal methods of the experimental analysis of behavior
Behavior (1957), Skinner described the book, an to examine whether the principles of behavior
almost entirely conceptual treatment of language, derived from experimentation with nonhumans
as a “functional analysis of language,” clearly not could be replicated with human subjects (Cooper
referring to an experimental procedure. In the et al., 2007). Early researchers established that
most general sense, the term retains the same the principles of behavior observed in animal
meaning today, although, practically speaking, studies were applicable to human behavior (Baer,
many people are referring to an experimental 1960; Bijou, 1955; Ferster & DeMyer, 1961,
functional analysis when they use the term “func- 1962; Lindsley, 1956, 1960) and provided the
tional analysis.” foundation for the development of ABA in general
Skinner’s early work described essential meth- and the functional analysis of clinically relevant
ods, concepts, and functional relations that would behavior in particular (Cooper et al., 2007).
serve as the foundation for the development of a One of the first studies to apply operant prin-
functional analysis of behavior. These included ciples to human behavior was conducted by
the focus on a single organism, the rate of Fuller (1949), who used positive reinforcement
response as the main dependent variable, the (sugar solution) to teach an 18-year-old man
operant–respondent distinction with related with profound intellectual disabilities to raise
difference between conditioned stimulus and dis- his hand. Prior to this, it was widely assumed
criminative stimulus, and the effects of various that individuals with severe intellectual disabili-
kinds of intermittent reinforcement (Michael, ties were not capable of learning. Indeed, Fuller
1980). In its most simplistic form, Skinner wrote that in regard to the specific participant,
recorded the rate at which a single animal emitted doctors “thought it was impossible for him to
a given behavior in a controlled operant chamber. learn anything,” but the results of this study
His investigative procedures evolved into an demonstrated that “if time permitted, other
experimental approach that “enabled clear and responses could be conditioned and discrimina-
powerful demonstrations of orderly and reliable tions learned” (Fuller, 1949, p. 590). A few years
functional relations between behavior and vari- later, Lindsley, Skinner, and Solomon (1953)
ous types of environmental events” (Cooper et al., applied the principles of operant conditioning to
2007, p. 11). However, behavior analysis was inpatients at a psychiatric state hospital, further
never intended as a science of animal behavior. establishing the basic concept that the behavior
In Science and Human Behavior (1953), Skinner of all individuals is subject to behavioral princi-
proposed that the techniques of behavior analysis ples of learning and motivation.
6 D.R. Dixon et al.

While a variety of methods and procedures individuals. In other words, Ferster conceptualized
were available for use within the animal studies, that a deficient behavioral repertoire is a social
little had been done to extend these procedures to problem. He argued that problem behaviors may
human subjects. Sidney Bijou’s early work (e.g., arise because of (a) an inadequate reinforcement
Bijou, 1955, 1957, 1958) was fundamental in history, (b) the schedule of reinforcement, or (c)
extending the methods and findings of animal punishment that may distort a performance which
research so that the behavior of humans could be otherwise would be reinforced (Ferster, 1958). In
experimentally analyzed. He proposed descrip- order to treat an individual’s inadequate behav-
tions of methodologies for “a systematic approach ioral repertoire, Ferster promoted the use of a
to an experimental analysis of child behavior” “functional program of therapy” (Ferster, 1958,
(Bijou, 1957, p. 250). He described specific p. 105) in which the therapist manipulates vari-
instrumentation, how data was to be recorded, ables in the patient’s environment to identify
and how to maximize control over independent potential reinforcers that maintain the problem
variables. He said that once the methodology was behavior. Thus, Ferster was one of the first to
properly refined, it would enable researchers to acknowledge the importance of functional rela-
study behavior “by relating the direct effect of tions between problematic behaviors and envi-
one variable upon another” (Bijou, 1957, p. 243). ronmental events.
These publications were seminal in establishing a The studies in the 1940s and 1950s did not
methodology through which the functional rela- attempt to make clinically meaningful behavior
tionships of human behavior could be analyzed in changes in the individuals they studied, but they
a well-controlled environment. served to establish further the notion that behav-
Ogden Lindsley was another pioneer in adapt- ior change in severely challenged populations
ing the methods of animal studies to the study of was possible and that human behavior, too, was
human behavior and was among the first to apply a function of the environment (Wilkins &
free-operant procedures to studying the behavior Matson, 2009). By demonstrating that operant
of chronic and acute psychotic children and principles could be applied to the behavior of
adults. Addressing the methodological limita- humans, the groundwork was laid for the devel-
tions of the emerging field, Lindsley (1956) opment of a discipline centered on using behav-
described how American psychologists had used ioral principals to bring about socially meaningful
a “confusing variety of apparatuses” (p. 120) to behavior change.
measure behavior, and psychology as an experi-
mental field had not yet agreed upon a universal
instrument to measure behavior; thus, researchers The Birth of Applied Behavior
were constantly designing new instruments or Analysis
modifying old ones. Lindsley cited Pavlov’s stud-
ies as exhibiting a higher degree of experimental The late 1950s through the 1960s saw the birth of
control than any other researcher in the early what came to be known as the field or discipline
1900s, and while more contemporary researchers of ABA. Ayllon & Michael’s, 1959 paper is often
described their various apparatuses, they failed to cited as the first ABA publication. In “The
design well-controlled experimental situations. Psychiatric Nurse as a Behavioral Engineer,” per-
He argued that “American psychologists in the sonnel in a state hospital were trained to use tech-
early part of this century imitated Pavlov’s verbal niques that were derived from behavioral
behavior, but they did not imitate his experimen- principles to improve the functioning of residents
tal behavior” (Lindsley, 1956, p. 124). with schizophrenia and other psychiatric diagnoses.
A significant step toward applying the meth- While no systematic approach to studying such
ods developed in animal studies to address abnor- problems was in existence, the authors stated that
mal behavior among humans was made by Ferster the aim of their research was “an attempt to
(1958), who was one of the first to recognize that discover and manipulate some of the environ-
aberrant behaviors were often maintained by other mental variables for the purpose of modifying
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 7

problem behavior” (Ayllon & Michael, 1959, (Wilkins & Matson, 2009). While much early
p. 323). Over the next decade, the researchers research demonstrated that mere management of
began to apply these same principles of behavior behavioral consequences could effectively
to a variety of socially problematic behavior and decrease challenging behaviors, behavior ana-
developmental disabilities (Jacobson & Holburn, lysts have long had concerns about unnecessary
2004). This new line of research represented a use of punishment-based procedures. Skinner
major advance in how severe behavior problems and many early behaviorists warned that punish-
were conceptualized. Studies were able to dem- ment may bring about undesirable side effects
onstrate experimentally that maladaptive behav- and that striving to promote control of behavior
iors, such as self-injury or aggression, could be through positive reinforcement as much as pos-
explained as functional responses to environmental sible was a valuable goal in and of itself. In addi-
stimuli (Durand, 1987). tion, the operant perspective assumes that
The application of operant principles and different behaviors have different functions for
experimental method to the analysis of human different people, and even multiple functions
behavior emerged as its own discipline in the within the same person, and so a prior under-
1960s with a mission to solve important social standing of the cause or function of the behavior
problems in a systematic and individualized man- will inevitably aid in designing an effective treat-
ner (Jacobson & Holburn, 2004). In the inaugural ment. This belief was implicit from the begin-
issue of the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, ning, but it was not until the 1960s that research
Baer, Wolf, and Risley (1968) described some of began which attempted to directly identify the
the defining features of ABA, distinct from the function of challenging behavior.
experimental analysis of behavior. They outlined An early study on the function of self-injuri-
seven dimensions of ABA: applied, behavioral, ous behavior (SIB) in a child with schizophrenia
analytic, technological, conceptually systematic, was conducted by Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, and
effective, and generalizable. Baer et al. (1968) Kassorla (1965). This study is noteworthy
described ABA as a discipline which “will make because it was the first to systematically investi-
obvious the importance of the behavior changed, gate a problem behavior wherein “the investiga-
the experimental manipulations which analyze tor has attempted control over self-destruction by
with clarity what was responsible for the change, systematically manipulating the variables of
the technologically exact description of all proce- which it might be a function” (Lovaas et al., 1965,
dures contributing to that change, the effectiveness p. 68). In the first phase of the study, the partici-
of those procedures in making sufficient change pant initially received social approval for appro-
for value, and the generality of that change” (Baer priate behaviors, and following a period of
et al., 1968, p. 97). Of particular relevance to the acquisition, the behavior was then extinguished
current chapter is the dimension of ABA referred by withholding the social reinforcers. The second
to as “analytic.” The analytic component stressed study occurred in a different experimental setting
the importance of using valid single-subject exper- than the first but procedurally was very similar.
imental designs to manipulate environmental vari- Social approval was delivered contingent upon
ables and observe the effects such variables have the subject pressing a lever, and once the lever-
on behavior, a practice at the very heart of experi- pressing rate had stabilized, the behavior was
mental functional analysis methodology. then again extinguished by withholding the social
attention. In the third phase, the authors demon-
strated that delivering verbal attention contingent
A Focus on the Function upon SIB resulted in an increase in the frequency
of Challenging Behaviors of the behavior (Lovaas, et al., 1965).
Carr (1977) was among the first researchers in
Early research in ABA attempted to reduce the ABA to promote a system in which intervention-
frequency and severity of challenging behaviors ists would develop hypotheses for conditions that
and facilitate the acquisition of adaptive skills maintain problem behavior and then develop
8 D.R. Dixon et al.

treatment strategies on the basis of those hypotheses. understaffed wards of mental hospitals, outbursts
Carr’s, 1977 review of the functions of self-injury of problem behavior were often the only way for
greatly influenced the development of a method individuals to attract a nurse’s attention.
of conducting functional analysis (Sturmey & Unfortunately, the more adaptive, socially accept-
Bernstein, 2004). His review concluded that self- able behaviors often went unnoticed or were even
injury could be maintained by three general punished by overworked hospital orderlies, a cir-
classes of environmental contingencies: positive cumstance that many clinicians sadly still observe
reinforcement, negative reinforcement, and sen- to this day in residential settings for individuals
sory or automatic consequences of the behavior with developmental and other disabilities.
(Carr, 1977). As noted by Sturmey and Bernstein Many studies in the 1960s applied this same
(2004), Carr and his colleagues conducted a series principle of adjusting the contingencies of adult
of important studies demonstrating functional attention in seeking to modify challenging behav-
relationships between the occurrence of certain iors of children. Hart, Allen, Buell, Harris, and
events and SIB, all of which developed experi- Wolf (1964) treated the frequent crying of two
mental conditions that could be used to experi- preschool children, starting with the hypothesis
mentally identify the functions of maladaptive that the behavior was reinforced by teacher atten-
behaviors (Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, 1980; tion. The authors successfully extinguished
Rincover, Newsom, & Carr, 1979). crying by training teachers to give no attention to
excessive crying and to provide immediate posi-
tive attention for more appropriate responses.
Early Research on Specific Functions Subsequent studies found the same mediating
of Challenging Behavior effects of attention on the presence of problem
behavior in both the classroom (Harris, Wolf, &
Before the landmark paper by Iwata et al. (1982) Baer, 1964; Thomas, Becker, & Armstrong,
was published, several early studies focused on 1968) and the home (Baskett & Johnson, 1982;
individual functions of behavior. These papers Budd, Green, & Baer, 1976; Hawkins, Peterson,
helped establish the foundations upon which Schweid, & Bijou, 1966). A substantial body of
more comprehensive experimental assessments literature published in the 1960s and 1970s sup-
were later developed. ported the positive reinforcement hypothesis,
indicating that the complete removal of social
consequences greatly reduced or eliminated SIB
Positive Reinforcement (Jones, Simmons, & Frankel, 1974; Lovaas &
Simmons, 1969; Tate & Baroff, 1966; Wolf,
One of the most prominent theories presented in Risley, Johnston, Harris, & Allen, 1967; Wolf,
the early literature on functions of aberrant Risley, & Mees, 1964).
behavior was that maladaptive behaviors were Lovaas et al. (1965) experimentally investi-
shaped and maintained by socially mediated pos- gated variables that controlled self-destructive
itive reinforcement (Carr, 1977). One of the first behavior in a child with schizophrenia. In a series
empirical demonstrations of how attention can of three studies, it was found that, following sev-
positively reinforce aberrant behavior was con- eral sessions of social extinction, reinstatement
ducted by Ayllon and Michael (1959). An infor- of social attention contingent on self-destructive
mal analysis led the authors to suggest that behavior produced the highest magnitude and
attention from nursing staff was inadvertently frequency of the problem behavior. A later study
positively reinforcing the psychotic speech of a by Lovaas and Simmons (1969) built upon this
psychiatric inpatient. When the nurses ceased work in an attempt to isolate some of the envi-
replying to the patient’s statements, the rate of ronmental conditions that controlled the self-
aberrant speech dropped dramatically. The destructive behavior of three children with severe
authors observed that within the overcrowded, developmental disabilities. Similar to Lovass and
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 9

colleagues’ earlier study, when the children were Other descriptive reports noted that demands
placed in a room where they were allowed to were very likely to increase SIB in children (e.g.,
engage in SIB, isolated from interpersonal contact Jones et al., 1974; Myers & Deibert, 1971; Wolf
and social consequences, they eventually ceased et al., 1967). In these early papers, it was observed
to hurt themselves (Lovaas & Simmons, 1969). that following such self-destructive behavior, the
While a significant amount of evidence sup- adults who were working with the children would
ported the concept of an attention function for often terminate the demands they were placing
challenging behavior, some early work also sug- upon the children. Taken together, these reports
gested that aberrant behavior may be maintained suggested that demands may constitute aversive
by other types of socially mediated positive rein- stimuli, and SIB functioned as an escape response
forcement. For example, Patterson, Littman, and maintained by the termination of demands; how-
Bricker (1967) conducted an early descriptive ever, no studies had yet attempted to experimen-
analysis of aggressive behavior and discussed the tally manipulate environmental variables to
possibility that access to food, candy, or preferred confirm this hypothesis.
toys may maintain it. They described cases in The first attempt at experimentally manipulat-
which children emitted aggressive behavior, ing the environment to evaluate the negative rein-
resulting in their victims relinquishing toys or forcement hypothesis appeared in Carr, Newsom,
other preferred materials. and Binkoff’s seminal paper (1976). Carr and
colleagues focused their experimental analysis
on the antecedent stimuli that would likely con-
Negative Reinforcement trol escape-maintained SIB. The study demon-
strated that levels of SIB were high in demand
In addition to investigating socially mediated situations (such as a classroom) and low in situa-
positive reinforcement as a potential function of tions that did not contain demands (such as
challenging behavior, early work on behavioral conversation or freeplay). In addition, they
function hypothesized that aberrant behavior may manipulated the occurrence of stimuli which had
be maintained by negative reinforcement. The historically been paired with removal of demands,
negative reinforcement hypothesis stated that hypothesizing that SIB should decrease upon the
aberrant behavior was learned behavior, rein- onset of the stimulus correlated with the termina-
forced by escape or avoidance of an aversive tion of demands. When the child, who was
stimulus or situation (Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, engaging in SIB, was presented with the vocal
1976). Much early work on the negative rein- prompt “O.K., let’s go,” a cue that normally ter-
forcement hypothesis focused on SIB. Early minated the demand period, the child abruptly
anecdotal reports noted that children who engaged stopped the self-destructive behavior. In contrast,
in SIB often did so in order to terminate an aver- when a neutral vocal stimulus was presented to
sive situation. For example, Freud and Burlingham the child, such as “The sky is blue” (a cue that
(1944) described a girl living in an institution had never been paired with the termination of
who would bang her head against her crib when demands), the child’s rate of SIB remained high.
she was put to bed, noting that this behavior By manipulating antecedent events that should
resulted in being removed from the crib. be correlated with escape-maintained behavior,
Ferster (1958) recognized that the problem and by demonstrating that the occurrence of the
behaviors of many psychiatric patients were behavior varied in a predictable way with the
inadvertently maintained by negative reinforce- manipulation, Carr provided a convincing early
ment. He noted that, rather than providing an experimental demonstration of maladaptive
individual with an adaptive behavioral repertoire, behavior with a negative reinforcement function,
attempts at punishment often reinforced aberrant albeit without yet manipulating the actual conse-
behaviors by allowing the individual to avoid or quences of which the behavior was thought to be
escape aversive consequences (Ferster, 1958). a function.
10 D.R. Dixon et al.

SIB was not the only behavior thought to serve pleasant odor that the behavior of smelling a
an escape function. Carr et al. (1980) sought to flower produces. It is also important to note that
identify the variables controlling severe aggres- stereotyped behaviors, behaviors which occur
sive behavior in two children. In a series of four repeatedly in the same manner, are often assumed
experiments, aggression was frequent in demand to be automatically reinforced. For this reason, it
conditions and rare in nondemand situations. Like is likely that stereotypy is often referred to as
SIB, aggression was shown to function sometimes “self-stimulatory” behavior, a term that assumes
as an escape response. While previous investiga- that the behavior is maintained by automatic rein-
tions had implied that aggression could serve the forcement. However, it is important to note that
function of an escape response to terminate the topography of a behavior (e.g., stereotypy)
demands or other aversive stimuli (Ludwig, Marx, does not necessarily indicate function (e.g., auto-
Hill, & Browning, 1969; Patterson et al., 1967), matic reinforcement). It is for this reason that the
Carr et al. (1980) was the first experimental study term stereotypy is generally preferred over “self-
to systematically investigate the possible role of stimulatory” or “self-stim” behavior.
escape factors in the maintenance of aggression. Early animal research on automatically rein-
Weeks and Gaylord-Ross (1981) examined forced behaviors addressed stereotypy across
the relationship between task characteristics and several species (Berkson, Mason, & Saxon,
problem behavior and provided additional exper- 1963). Experiments in the 1960s examined the
imental support for the negative reinforcement effects of environments that were “deprived” of
hypothesis of aberrant behaviors. It was concep- stimulation. In particular, social isolation was
tualized that severely handicapped children, when studied in primates because “it is difficult or
presented with a difficult task, would emit aber- impossible to study scientifically the impacts of
rant responses to terminate the aversive stimulus. culturally produced social isolation at the human
Three experimental conditions were created to level” (Harlow, Dodsworth, & Harlow, 1965).
test this possibility in which the students would Researchers noted that the same behavioral prin-
be presented with difficult tasks, easy tasks, and ciples seemed to operate for both humans and
no tasks. As predicted, the highest frequency of primates and that “social conditions which pro-
aberrant behavior was associated with difficult duce abnormality in one species will have com-
demands, and near-zero levels of problem behav- parable effects on the other” (Harlow et al., 1965,
ior occurred in settings that were free of demands, p. 90). Behaviors such as thumb sucking, unusual
thereby providing further support for the negative limb postures, and self-clasping were observed as
reinforcement hypothesis. common primate stereotypies.
A study by Berkson et al. (1963) examined
stimulus and situational factors affecting the ste-
Automatic Reinforcement reotyped behaviors characteristic of primates
raised without their mothers. They observed that
The concept of automatic reinforcement has early the level of stereotypy was highest in an environ-
roots in behavior analysis, dating back at least as ment in which vision, locomotion, and opportuni-
far as Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior ties to manipulate objects were restricted. The
(1957). Automatic reinforcement is reinforce- authors argued that it was very likely that the
ment that is produced automatically when a absence of environmental stimulation was an
behavior occurs. That is, it is reinforcement that important factor in the presence of such behavior,
is not dependent on the behavior of someone else as the stereotyped responses were reduced when
to deliver it. Automatic reinforcement is some- alternative activities were evoked (Berkson et al.,
times described as reinforcement that is inherent 1963). These early animal studies reflected a basic
in the behavior itself. Everyday examples include hypothesis that automatically reinforced behavior
the relief of the itching sensation that the behav- is more likely to occur when an organism is
ior of scratching an insect bite produces or the deprived of stimulation because the behavior may
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 11

be one of the only sources of environmental powerful positive reinforcer. An additional study
stimulation available to the organism. conducted by Rincover (1978) investigated the
Residential facilities were conceptualized as self-stimulatory behavior of psychotic children in
one type of setting in which individuals with dis- order to identify possible maintaining/supporting
abilities may be deprived of stimulation, and variables. The results showed that the self-
researchers began to conceptualize environmen- stimulatory behavior was “reliably decreased
tal deprivation as a factor that may contribute to when a certain sensory consequence was removed,
the maintenance of automatically reinforced mal- then increased when that consequence was per-
adaptive behaviors in humans (Green, 1967; mitted” (p. 307), thereby supporting the notion
McKinney, 1962; Murphy, 1982). For example, that the behavior was maintained by operant rein-
early researchers noted several cases of problem forcement in the form of sensory stimulation.
behavior among children who were restricted to Furthermore, Rincover stated that then-current
their cribs without toys (Dennis & Najarian, theories holding that stereotypy is a result of
1957). Collins (1965) treated head banging in an understimulation did “not easily account for these
isolated, severely intellectually disabled adult by data, primarily because the suppressive effect of
exposing the individual to a high level of sensory removing sensory consequences was specific to a
stimulation. The consequent decrease in SIB was particular sensory modality” (p. 307).
attributed to the increased tactile and kinesthetic
stimulation that was provided in treatment. Green
(1967) was another researcher to propose that the The First Comprehensive
stereotypy often seen in children living in institu- Experimental Functional Analysis
tional settings occurred as an adaptive response
to a decreased level of environmental stimula- The studies described thus far provided ample evi-
tion. He postulated that, in the overall absence dence for three possible functions of challenging
of stimulation, operant responses, such as self- behavior: (1) socially mediated positive reinforce-
destructive behaviors, develop into persistent ment (e.g., attention), (2) negative reinforcement
behavior as a function of the increased sensory (e.g., escape from nonpreferred tasks), and (3)
input they provide. automatic reinforcement. Substantial evidence
Experimental investigations of the effects of existed to support each hypothesis, but little or no
noncontingent stimuli on stereotypies provided research had been published on attempts at evalu-
additional foundation for the hypothesis that ating more than one potential function for the
some aberrant behaviors have an automatic rein- same behavior of one individual. In addition,
forcement function. Early studies demonstrated although a substantial amount of literature had
that vibration significantly decreased the stereo- been published which had implications for how to
typed behavior of intellectually disabled children identify the functions of challenging behavior, no
(Bailey & Meyerson, 1969, 1970; Meyerson, standard set of comprehensive functional analysis
Kerr, & Michael, 1967). Maisto, Baumeister, and procedures had yet been proposed, and several
Maisto (1978) noted that individuals who lacked researchers acknowledged the need for it (Carr,
adaptive modes of behavior or appropriate oppor- 1977; Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981). In particu-
tunities to increase stimulation often resorted to lar, Weeks and Gaylord-Ross (1981) wrote that “a
activities that involved direct contact or manipu- useful contribution to the burgeoning field of
lation of the body. behavioral assessment would be the development
Wolery (1978) assessed the effects of experi- of clear criteria for determining whether aberrant
menter-applied sensory stimulation that was behavior is maintained by positive reinforcement,
comparable to the child’s stereotypic behavior. negative reinforcement, or intrinsic reinforcement
Results from this study demonstrated that contin- (self-stimulation)” (p. 461).
gent trainer-applied sensory stimulation (which One year later, Iwata et al. published their
duplicated the child’s stereotypy) functioned as a seminal paper, “Toward a Functional Analysis of
12 D.R. Dixon et al.

Self-Injurious Behavior” (1982), which was therapy room that was equipped with a variety of
reprinted in 1994 in the Journal of Applied toys. The experimenter then told the participant
Behavior Analysis. The study included nine to play with the toys while the experimenter “did
children and adolescents with developmental some work.” Contingent on each occurrence of
disabilities and SIB. A randomized multielement SIB, or burst of occurrences, the experimenter
design (rapid alternation between 15-min ses- delivered brief statements of concern (e.g., “Don’t
sions of each condition) was used to compare the do that, you will hurt yourself”) while also deliv-
occurrence of SIB under four experimental ering brief physical attention (e.g., patting the
conditions: (1) academic, (2) alone, (3) social person on the shoulder). All other participant
disapproval, and (4) play. The first three condi- behaviors were ignored. The purpose of this
tions were selected to represent the three general condition was to test for a possible function of
functions of behavior that had been hypothesized positive reinforcement in the form of access to
up to that point (positive, negative, and automatic attention from others.
reinforcement) and the fourth served as a control In the play condition (sometimes referred to as
condition. the “toy play” or “control” condition), the therapy
In the academic condition (now usually room was equipped with a variety of toys, and no
referred to as the “demand” or “escape” condition), demands were placed on the child. The experi-
the experimenter and participant sat at a desk. menter delivered brief social and physical atten-
The experimenter presented tasks to the partici- tion to the participant, contingent on the absence
pant, using a graduated “three-step” prompting of SIB, at least every 30 s. SIB was ignored. This
sequence. The sequence began with the experi- condition was included to serve as a control con-
menter presenting the task vocally. If the partici- dition. It served as a suitable control condition for
pant did not respond within 5 s, the experimenter the attention condition because the antecedent
repeated the vocal instruction and provided a for attention-maintained behavior was absent
model prompt. If the participant did not respond (i.e., the participant was not deprived of attention),
appropriately after 5 s, the experimenter physi- and attention was not delivered as a consequence
cally guided the participant to respond, after of SIB. Similarly, the play condition served as a
which the next task was presented vocally. If the suitable control condition for escape-maintained
participant responded appropriately, the experi- behavior because the relevant antecedent was not
menter responded with brief praise. If, at any present (i.e., presentation of demands), nor was
time, the participant engaged in SIB, the experi- the putative maintaining consequence (escape
menter turned away from the participant and was not delivered contingent on SIB). It is difficult
ceased task demands for 30 s. This condition was to construct a control condition that withholds the
designed to test for a negative reinforcement consequence for automatically reinforced behav-
function because escape from demands was ior because the consequence is automatically
systematically presented, contingent on the produced, but the relevant antecedent (general
occurrence of the target behavior. deprivation of stimulation) is not present.
In the alone condition, the participant was The analysis continued to be conducted until
placed in a therapy room alone, with no toys or (1) stability in the level of SIB was observed,
items of any kind. This condition was designed to (2) unstable levels of SIB continued for 5 days,
test for automatic reinforcement and mimicked or (3) sessions had been conducted for 12 days.
the types of “deprived” environments hypothe- No assessment lasted longer than 11 days, and
sized to contribute to automatically reinforced the number of 15-min sessions that were required
behavior that were discussed in earlier research ranged from 24 to 53. The results demonstrated
on automatic reinforcement. significant variability in responding, both across
In the social disapproval condition (now various conditions within each participant and
usually referred to as the “attention” condition), across patterns of responding between partici-
the experimenter and the participant entered a pants. These results provided strong support for
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 13

the position that particular topographies of basic format used across a variety of other popu-
challenging behavior do not have singular causes lations, settings, and behaviors. In addition, alter-
but, rather, are learned behaviors that differ in ations to the basic format have been researched in
their relationship to environmental events, order to accommodate a variety of different
depending on the unique learning history of each behaviors, idiosyncratic environmental variables,
individual person. In particular, the results of and other behavioral functions, among others.
some participants strongly suggested that their A complete description of the history of experi-
SIB was maintained by attention, while others mental functional analysis research post-1982
appeared to be maintained by escape from would likely require several volumes in itself,
demands, and still others appeared to be main- and other chapters in the current volume provide
tained by automatic reinforcement. Another additional details in multiple areas. In what fol-
important finding was that the function of the lows, we provide descriptions of some of the
SIB did not appear to be correlated in any major highlights in terms of how the basic experi-
significant way with the rate or severity of the mental functional analysis format has been used
behavior. All of these results strongly supported to study many additional phenomena, as well as
the notion developed in the functional assess- how it has been modified to expand its scope
ment literature that the function and topography further, and we will conclude the chapter with a
of any given behavior are distinct and, more discussion of future directions for research.
importantly, that clinicians cannot, therefore, The “standard” functional analysis method-
assume a cause of behavior by simply looking at ology proposed by Iwata et al. (2000) has proven
the topography or severity. to be robust and widely applicable, as described
Another important contribution of the paper above. Indeed, reviews of functional analysis
by Iwata et al. (2000) was that it set forth a simple methodology have suggested that functional
format for experimentally investigating the func- analyses result in a determination of behavioral
tion of any behavior. By pitting one or more test function in about 94% of the cases in which they
conditions, each of which tests one potential con- are applied (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003;
sequence as a reinforcer, against a single control Iwata, Pace, et al., 1994). While some functional
condition that reverses or eliminates each of the analyses may result in undifferentiated or
contingencies being manipulated in the test ambiguous outcomes, this does not necessarily
conditions, multiple potential functions could be mean that the assessment process has failed or
assessed during the course of a single assessment. that the contingencies maintaining the problem
Furthermore, the variety of potential functions behavior cannot be understood (Tiger, Fisher,
that could be assessed within this format was lim- Toussaint, & Kodak, 2009). Many authors have
ited only by the imagination and ability of the noted that the traditional functional analysis
assessor to control environmental conditions. As methods developed by Iwata et al. (1982) occa-
we will see in the coming section of this chapter, sionally require modifications to assess behav-
this basic format has changed little in the last ioral function more accurately across distinct
30 years, but a considerable variety of different populations, response topographies, and set-
behaviors and functions have been assessed. tings, especially when an initial conventional
functional analysis proves to be inconclusive
(Bowman, Fisher, Thompson, & Piazza, 1997;
After 1982 Cooper, Wacker, Sasso, Reimers, & Donn, 1990;
Tiger et al., 2009). In what follows, we review
The seminal 1982 paper by Iwata et al. offered an some of the highlights of studies that have
elegant yet powerful format for conducting sought to modify some aspect of the basic
experimental assessments of the function of mal- approach in order to experimentally assess the
adaptive behaviors, and the three decades that function of behavior that may be difficult to
have passed since its publication have seen the assess using the standard approach.
14 D.R. Dixon et al.

Expansion Across Populations 1968; Thomas et al., 1968) to formulate the first
standardized functional analysis methodology.
The majority of functional analysis research has Although initially applied to the analysis of self-
been conducted with individuals with develop- injurious behavior, the methodology was quickly
mental disabilities, which is not surprising, given adapted to analyze environment–behavior rela-
the increased prevalence of challenging behav- tionships that maintained a wide array of problem
iors in this population (Hanley et al., 2003). behaviors, such as aggression (Day, Horner, &
Additionally, functional analysis methodology O’Neill, 1994; Lalli & Casey, 1996; Mace, Page,
has been expanded to young children with chal- Ivancic, & O’Brien, 1986; Wacker et al., 1990),
lenging behavior. For example, Wacker et al. destructive behavior (Bowman et al., 1997; Slifer,
(1998) trained parents to conduct functional Ivancic, Parrish, Page, & Burgio, 1986), stereo-
analyses and Functional Communication Training typy (Durand & Carr, 1987; Mace, Browder, &
(FCT) for 28 children, ages 1–6, with develop- Lin, 1987; Wacker et al., 1990), pica (Mace
mental disabilities who displayed aberrant behav- & Knight, 1986; Piazza et al., 1998), and
ior. Results indicated that problem behavior tantrums (Carr & Newsom, 1985).
served socially mediated functions for the major- The majority of functional analyses conducted
ity of children. Twenty-one percent of partici- in the 1980s and early 1990s focused on single
pants exhibited problem behavior maintained by functions that maintained one or more aberrant
positive reinforcement, 46% exhibited aberrant responses (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985; Iwata
behavior maintained by negative reinforcement, et al., 1982; Northup et al., 1991; Wacker et al.,
and 18% engaged in problem behavior that was 1990). However, some researchers noted that, on
multiply controlled. Kurtz et al. (2003) further occasion, multiple functions of distinct topogra-
extended functional analysis research by con- phies of aberrant behavior interfered with the
ducting functional analyses across 30 very young analysis of those behaviors (Derby et al., 1994).
children (ages 10 months to 4 years and 11 While many investigators noted the existence of
months) with SIB and other forms of challenging different topographies of behavior serving differ-
behaviors. The mean age of emergence of SIB ent functions for a single client (e.g., Durand,
was 17 months, and head banging was the most 1982; Mace et al., 1986; Slifer et al., 1986; Smith,
common topography. Functional analyses suc- Iwata, Vollmer, & Pace, 1992; Sturmey, Carlsen,
ceeded in identifying a function in 87.5% of Crisp, & Newton, 1988), the majority of research
cases, and successful function-based treatments conducted prior to 1994 combined separate
were implemented in most cases, as well. topographies of problem behavior and conducted
In addition to expansion of functional analysis one functional analysis on an aggregate class of
methodology to very young children with devel- target behavior (e.g., Durand & Carr, 1991;
opmental disabilities, research has demonstrated Wacker et al., 1990). Conducting separate func-
its efficacy with a variety of other populations, tional analyses for several distinct topographies
including children with attention deficit hyperac- presented huge time and cost constraints, leading
tivity disorder (DuPaul & Ervin, 1996) and typi- researchers to develop alternative strategies to
cally developing children with conduct disorders accurately and efficiently measure these separate
(Cooper et al., 1990). topographies. Derby et al. (1994) suggested
conducting a single functional analysis, initially
graphing all presenting target behaviors in an
Expansion Across Behaviors aggregate fashion and subsequently analyzing
each topography of behavior on separate graphs.
Multiple Topographies This approach offered investigators the practical
Iwata et al.’s, 1982 article was able to build upon advantage of conducting a single functional
previous theoretical papers (e.g., Carr, 1977) and analysis while still allowing them to separate
research methods (e.g., Bijou, Peterson, & Ault, each topography in order to generate specific
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 15

hypotheses about the function of each behavior analyses (Kahng, Abt, & Schonbachler, 2001). In
(Derby et al., 1994). The results of this study an early study on functional analysis of low-rate
demonstrated how separate functions could behavior, Kahng et al. (2001) conducted a
be hidden by an aggregate analysis but could be modified functional analysis of low-rate aggres-
accurately identified when the results for each sion, extending the session duration to 7 h per
target behavior were plotted separately. day and conducting sessions 5 days per week,
with each day representing a different analogue
Precursor Behaviors condition. The modified functional analysis pro-
Experimental Functional Analysis (EFA) of duced clear results, whereas a previous functional
behavior disorders often produces temporary analysis with standard session durations did not.
increases in problem behavior and has led to con- The Kahng et al. procedural modification suc-
cerns over the potentially reinforcing conse- ceeded in producing clear assessment results for
quences of evoking such behavior during the low-rate behavior, but two possible limitations
assessment procedures (Najdowski, Wallace, are of note. First, many may be uncomfortable
Ellsworth, MacAleese, & Cleveland, 2008). To with exposing participants to the experimental
reduce risk associated with an experimental func- conditions for such extended periods of time
tional analysis, some investigators have conducted (especially in the attention condition, which
a functional analysis of precursor behavior in essentially amounted to ignoring appropriate
order to indirectly infer the variables that maintain behavior all day), and second, some organiza-
problem behavior (Najdowski et al., 2008; Smith tions may have difficulty allocating the large
& Churchill, 2002). Lalli, Mace, Wohn, and number of highly trained staff that is needed for
Livezey (1995) provided a foundation for this such an extended functional analysis.
approach by showing that three problem behaviors Tarbox, Wallace, Tarbox, Landaburu, and
displayed by a 15-year-old girl typically occurred Williams (2004) evaluated an additional modi-
in a predictable sequence (referred to as a response fication to functional analysis methodology for
hierarchy), and when reinforcement contingencies assessing low-rate behavior. In particular, func-
were applied to the first of these responses, later tional analysis sessions were initiated contingent
ones in the sequence were suppressed. Based on upon the occurrence of challenging behavior. This
this information, subsequent studies demonstrated modification resulted in clear results in three adults
that it was possible to infer the maintaining vari- with intellectual disabilities and low-rate challeng-
ables for more severe behaviors based on the out- ing behaviors who had not engaged in a sufficient
comes of analyzing the more benign, precursor amount of challenging behavior during prior stan-
behavior (Smith & Churchill, 2002). Furthermore, dard functional analyses. Furthermore, treatment
interventions based on the outcomes of such anal- analyses were conducted on the basis of the func-
yses reduced precursor behavior and were associ- tional analysis outcomes for the two participants
ated with zero levels of severe problem behavior who were available for treatment, and both resulted
(Najdowski et al., 2008). These results represent a in successful reductions in challenging behavior.
promising alternative to assessing the operant
function of severe problem behavior directly.
Expansion Across Settings
Low-Rate Behaviors
Clinicians have often noted that high-frequency The immediate success of functional analysis
behavior appears to be easier to functionally within hospital settings led researchers to work
assess than low-frequency behavior (Sprague & on transferring the use of the technology across
Horner, 1999). Since low-rate behavior occurs other settings, and the results have been encour-
infrequently, it may be difficult to observe during aging. A common misconception seems to remain
descriptive analyses, and it may not occur at that successful experimental functional analyses
all during traditional experimental functional require a highly controlled hospital or university
16 D.R. Dixon et al.

setting, consisting of two or three staff members, developments leading up to it, could not possibly
laptop computers for data collection, and padded have identified all possible operant functions for
session rooms with one-way mirrors, but pub- challenging behavior. From a purely operant
lished research shows this is clearly not the case. standpoint, all functions can be classified under
A significant amount of research has demon- either positive or negative reinforcement and
strated that functional analysis techniques are either automatic or socially mediated. However, a
useful in less-controlled much shorter-term out- small but significant number of additional subdi-
patient clinical settings (Derby et al., 1992; visions of these categories have been identified in
Wacker et al., 1990). A still-common misconcep- the past 30 years, the highlights of which are
tion among educators is that functional analysis reviewed next.
methodology is not appropriate or not practical
for schools, and this notion is cited as justification Tangible
for often implementing only indirect and descrip- Perhaps the most substantial and enduring addi-
tive assessments in school settings (Weber, Killu, tion to the standard functional analysis methodol-
Derby, & Barretto, 2005). However, a very sub- ogy is the addition of a condition to assess for the
stantial amount of research has been published “tangible” function, that is, maintenance of behav-
on the successful application of functional analy- ior by forms of positive reinforcement, such as
sis methodology within schools. Hanley et al. food, toys, or other items or activities. The tangi-
(2003) found that just over 31% of published ble condition amounts to a minor modification to
research on experimental functional analyses was the attention condition. That is, positive reinforce-
conducted in school settings. In addition, a small ment is still addressed as a potential function of
but significant amount of research has demon- behavior, but the form of positive reinforcement
strated the use of functional analysis methodology delivered is some form of preferred item or activ-
in homes. For example, Arndorfer, Miltenberger, ity, and no physical or verbal attention is deliv-
Woster, Rortvedt, and Gaffaney (1994) conducted ered. Mace and West (1986) were the first to
brief experimental functional analyses with six include an experimental condition that assessed
children in their homes and found conclusive the effects of tangible reinforcement on problem
results in each case. Finally, the large amount of behavior. However, the condition which provided
research on functional analyses in schools and access to preferred items/activities contingent
homes described above clearly shows that con- upon challenging behavior also provided simulta-
trived settings are not necessary to produce clear neous escape from demands as a consequence.
results, but little research has directly compared The first study to isolate access to preferred items/
contrived and naturalistic settings. One recent activities as a consequence for behavior during an
study by Lang et al. (2008) directly compared the experimental functional analysis was conducted
results of functional analyses conducted in ther- by Day, Rea, Schussler, Larsen, & Johnson,
apy rooms versus classrooms for two children (1988). This study included a condition that pro-
with autism. The results were somewhat incon- vided only access to preferred items/activities for
sistent, but generally speaking, clearer results 20–30 s contingent on problem behavior and is,
were obtained in the therapy room. More research therefore, often cited as the earliest demonstra-
is needed which directly compares contrived tion of behavioral maintenance by access to tan-
versus natural settings. gible items (Hanley et al., 2003). However, the
Day et al. study also included participant peers
present in the condition, which represents another
Expansion to Other Functions possible antecedent variable which may set the
of Behavior occasion for challenging behavior. One of the ear-
liest studies to implement what is now considered
It should not be surprising that the original the “standard” tangible condition was conducted
article by Iwata et al. (2000), and the earlier by Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, and Roane (1995).
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 17

In this study, tangible sessions were preceded by commonly discussed but has thus far been the
a brief period of access to preferred items that subject of relatively little research. The general
caregivers reported were correlated with chal- idea is that maintaining consequence of some
lenging behavior. At the start of the session, the challenging behavior is access to the opportunity
preferred items were removed and returned to the to be in control. For example, a study by Bowman
participant for approximately 20 s, contingent on et al. (1997) demonstrated that the maintaining
each occurrence of the target behavior. consequence of challenging behavior for children
It is also interesting to note that, although the with developmental disabilities was the caregiver
term “tangible” implies that the reinforcer for complying with the requests (i.e., “mands”) of
problem behavior is a physical item that can be the participant, regardless of what those requests
grasped, this is not always the case. Significant were. This is to be distinguished from a standard
creativity and flexibility may be needed to accom- tangible condition in that the particular item or
modate the full range of items or activities that activity being requested was not relevant as long
may serve as positive reinforcers for challenging as the request was fulfilled. Similar findings were
behavior. For example, one recent study demon- replicated in a controlled case study by O’Connor,
strated that the opportunity to go for walks was Sorensen-Burnworth, Rush, and Eidman (2003)
the maintained reinforcer of behavior, a conse- in which the destructive behavior of a 14-year-
quence that was initially overlooked when more old boy with developmental disabilities was
conventional consequences were first evaluated found to be maintained by adult compliance with
(Ringdahl, Christensen, & Boelter, 2009). mands. More research is needed to identify
The tangible condition has become common exactly what “control” is from a behavioral
over the last two decades of functional analysis perspective and how it participates in the mainte-
research, and it is generally considered a “stan- nance of challenging behavior.
dard” condition. Indeed, in their review, Hanley
et al. (2003) found that 38% of articles included a Access to Stereotypy
tangible condition and that the problem behavior Preliminary research has documented challeng-
of 10% of participants was reported to have a tan- ing behaviors whose functions appear to be posi-
gible function (as opposed to 34% for escape and tive reinforcement in the form of the opportunity
25% for attention). However, many researchers to engage in ritualistic behavior, routines of some
caution against including a tangible condition in sort, or to engage in stereotyped behavior of some
a functional analysis unless caregivers report that kind. For example, Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson,
the target behavior is associated with a preferred and Thompson (1998) assessed the property
item or activity, because of the potential for destruction of two boys with intellectual disabili-
“shaping up” a false tangible function (Thompson ties and found that the behavior was maintained
& Iwata, 2001). Little research has suggested that by the opportunity which it afforded to engage in
functional analyses create false positive results, stereotypy with the destroyed property.
but Shirley, Iwata, and Kahng (1999) found that it Specifically, the participants destroyed plastic
is possible to unintentionally “create” or “shape items and then engaged in stereotypy with the
up” a false function for challenging behavior in broken pieces of the items. Treatments based on
the tangible condition. The findings of Shirley these results successfully reduced the property
and colleagues support the general practice of destruction.
excluding the tangible condition unless caregiv-
ers provide information that may suggest a
possible tangible function. Idiosyncratic Variables

Control Numerous studies have identified potential idio-


The role of “control” in the maintenance of chal- syncratic antecedent variables that can affect the
lenging behavior is a potential function that is outcomes of functional analyses. O’Reilly (1996)
18 D.R. Dixon et al.

evaluated the influence of the location where a Functional analysis, no matter how well developed,
participant resided the night previous to the day still requires the “analysis” component; it is not a
in which functional analysis sessions were con- universal cookbook approach to assessment, nor
ducted. The participant was a 25-year-old man was it ever intended to be.
with moderate intellectual disabilities who exhib-
ited intermittent SIB. The results of the analyses
demonstrated clearly that SIB occurred on days Experimental Designs
following nights spent with respite care and did
not occur on days following nights spent at home. The functional analyses in the 1982 paper by
A successful treatment for the behavior was Iwata et al. were carried out in the context of mul-
designed in which an alternative respite place- tielement experimental designs, and this design
ment was implemented. remains the most prevalent (Hanley et al., 2003).
Carr, Yarbrough, and Langdon (1997) evalu- However, several alternatives have been shown to
ated the effects of idiosyncratic variables on be effective. One such alternative is to match one
functional analysis outcomes in three individuals test condition with the control condition in a
with developmental disabilities. In each case, the “pair-wise” fashion in order to minimize poten-
presence or absence of highly idiosyncratic stim- tial carryover effects that may result from alter-
uli (large vs. small balls, magazines, and puzzles) nating multiple test conditions at once (Iwata,
determined whether the target behavior occurred Duncan, Zarcone, Lerman, & Shore, 1994). The
during assessment conditions, regardless of the reversal design has also been used in favor of the
particular programmed consequence for the multielement design in several studies, also with
condition. the intention of minimizing carryover effects
Ringdahl and Sellers (2000) examined the dif- (Vollmer et al., 1995). Finally, multiple studies
ferential effects on problem behavior of caregiv- have conducted what has come to be known as an
ers and inpatient staff members as therapists, “extended alone” condition in which multiple
finding that aberrant behavior varied not only as a successive sessions of the alone condition are
function of environmental contingencies but also conducted for behaviors that are suspected to be
as a function of therapist. Specifically, the aber- maintained by automatic reinforcement (Vollmer
rant behavior was more prevalent when the care- et al., 1995).
giver served as therapist during a functional
analysis than when a staff member implemented
the same procedures. Functional Analysis Duration
The effects of a multitude of other idiosyn-
cratic variables have been studied as well, includ- A commonly stated potential limitation to experi-
ing the number of therapists present during the mental functional analyses is that they are said to
attention condition (Taylor, Sisson, McKelvey, & require lengthy durations of time to complete.
Trefelner, 1993), quality of attention delivered There is little research to suggest that other
contingent on behavior (Fisher, Ninness, Piazza, equally reliable functional assessment options
& Owen-DeSchryver, 1996), and establishment exist that require less time; however, maximizing
of operations for escape-maintained behavior efficiency of any clinical procedure is always
(McComas, Hoch, Paone, & El-Roy, 2000), valuable. A significant amount of research has
among many others. Taken together, these studies been done on ways to shorten the overall duration
further reinforce the basic philosophical assump- of time required for functional analyses. One
tions behind functional analysis, specifically, that option for shortening the time required for a func-
it is a general format which is useful for identify- tional analysis is to shorten the duration of each
ing behavior/environment relations at the level of session, from the usual 10–15 to 1–5 min. Wallace
the individual client and that these relations are and Iwata (1999) retrospectively evaluated the
assumed to vary, as each individual is unique. effects of analyzing only the first 10 min, versus
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 19

the first 5 min, versus the entire 15-min duration consequences were delivered for challenging
of functional analysis sessions, and found that behavior in any conditions. That is, there was no
5-min session durations produced clear results in contingency between target behavior and conse-
a majority of cases. A 90-min assessment, based quence, and therefore the target behavior was
on Iwata et al.’s (1982) model, has been devel- putatively on extinction throughout the assess-
oped for use in brief outpatient clinic visits. In ment. Carr and Durand (1985) and many subse-
this model, usually referred to as the “brief func- quent studies have obtained clear results using this
tional analysis,” only one or two sessions of each methodology. Indeed, the review by Hanley et al.
condition are conducted, followed by a brief (2003) cited 56 studies which had been published
treatment assessment in which a replacement using this antecedent-only methodology. A major
communication response is usually trained. The potential limitation to this model is that, since
brief nature of the assessment does not always challenging behavior is on extinction during the
allow the target behavior to be observed success- assessment, it may decrease before clear results
fully, but the model can be useful nonetheless are obtained. However, a major potential strength
(Cooper et al., 1990; Derby et al., 1992; Northup of the approach is that many caregivers (e.g.,
et al., 1991). teachers, parents, etc.) who are not familiar with
standard functional analysis methodology may
not approve of the idea of intentionally reinforc-
Interpreting Functional Analysis Data ing challenging behavior during assessment. In
any case, the antecedent-only model is a significant
Iwata et al.’s original functional analysis paper contribution to the traditional model and should
(1982) utilized visual inspection, without any be considered as one possible alternative.
formal criterion, to interpret data produced by the
assessment. Visual inspection is the standard in
single-case experimental design (Kazdin, 2010) Training Others to Conduct
and continues to be adequate in the vast majority Functional Analyses
of functional analyses. However, some research-
ers have expressed concern about the potential One criticism of traditional functional analysis
for subjectivity in the interpretation of functional methodology is that the precision required to
analysis results, particularly in less-controlled conduct such an analysis necessitates extensive
settings. In order to increase both the validity and training and clinical expertise (Wallace, Doney,
reliability of visual inspection for interpreting Mintz-Resudek, & Tarbox, 2004). However,
functional analysis results, Hagopian et al. (1997) recent studies have demonstrated that individuals
developed a set of structured criteria and trained with no prior experience with functional analysis
individuals to apply these rules when analyzing procedures could be trained to implement func-
functional analysis data. The results suggested tional analyses with a high degree of fidelity
that, when applied correctly, the criteria improved (Iwata et al., 2000; Moore et al., 2002; Wallace
the reliability of interpretations of functional et al., 2004). With the use of a training package
analysis results. that included reading materials, watching a vid-
eotaped simulation, passing a written test, and
receiving feedback, Iwata et al. (2000) effectively
Antecedent-Only Functional Analysis trained undergraduate students to implement
three functional analysis conditions (attention,
An early study on experimental functional analy- demand, and play). Moore and colleagues (Moore
sis by Carr and Durand (1985) used a methodol- et al., 2002; Moore & Fisher, 2007) trained indi-
ogy in which only the antecedents of challenging viduals through video modeling. Recent studies
behavior were experimentally manipulated have demonstrated that, using video modeling,
between conditions, whereas no programmed undergraduate students and teachers could be
20 D.R. Dixon et al.

trained to implement functional analysis proce- they are rapid, easy to administer, and do not
dures with high fidelity (Iwata et al., 2000; Moore require direct observation of the target behavior.
et al., 2002; Wallace et al., 2004). Further, most require less than an hour to con-
duct. Descriptive assessments involve direct
observation and measurement of the target behav-
Nonexperimental Methods ior as well as environmental variables that are
for Functional Assessment presumed to be functionally related (Cooper
et al., 2007). In light of the resource-intensive
Experimental functional analysis has become one nature of EFA, it is not surprising that Desrochers
of the most widely used technologies in ABA et al. (1997) found that clinicians reported indi-
research on assessment and treatment of challeng- rect and descriptive assessments as more useful
ing behavior. However, the vast majority of clini- than experimental assessments. In cases where
cal and educational settings do not conduct resources (e.g., availability of trained staff) do
experimental analyses prior to treatment. Based not permit experimental analyses, or the low-rate
upon the most commonly described methods for of severe behavior requires an inordinate amount
conducting an EFA (e.g., Iwata, Dorsey et al., of time to observe the behavior within an ana-
1994), determining the clear function of a target logue setting, a significant amount of research
behavior may take several weeks to complete, has demonstrated that various indirect and
depending on how many sessions can be con- descriptive methods of functional assessment can
ducted per day. As noted previously, researchers yield useful information.
have made efforts to create abbreviated analyses A more thorough review of indirect and
(e.g., Northup et al., 1991). However, as discussed, descriptive methods is outside of the scope of
these procedures have a number of limitations this chapter. Readers are advised to see the
such as lower probability of observing the target chapter titled, “Scaling Methods of Functional
behavior during the shortened observation time. Assessment,” in this volume for a thorough treat-
Another limitation of EFA procedures is that sev- ment of the topic.
eral trained staff members are required to conduct
the analysis, again adding to the resources required
to conduct the analysis. Most clinical and educa- Concerns and Future Directions
tional settings simply do not have the staff avail-
able to conduct EFAs. In addition, many have Ecological Validity
concerns regarding the practice of intentionally
reinforcing challenging behavior during EFAs. Over the years, some studies have focused on a
Despite the large amount of research demon- descriptive analysis under natural settings (e.g.,
strating the reliability of EFAs, one must wonder Bijou et al., 1968), while others have emphasized
why the procedure has not been adopted on a the experimental analysis of aberrant behavior
wide scale in real-life clinical and educational within controlled laboratory conditions (e.g.,
settings. Although there is no simple answer to Iwata et al., 1982). Throughout this time,
this question, it seems likely that all the potential researchers have questioned the ecological valid-
limitations described above play a part. Based ity of the procedures employed in functional
upon these limitations, a large amount of research analyses, arguing that the results of such assess-
has been done on nonexperimental methods for ments are not reflective of the types of functional
functional assessment. These methods are com- relationships operating in an individual’s natural
monly classified as either indirect or descriptive setting (Emerson, 1992).
assessment (Tarbox et al., 2009). Indirect assess- While experimental functional analyses help
ments consist of interviewing caregivers who ensure careful control over the environment,
have previously observed the target behavior. critics have raised questions regarding the
Interviews may be open-ended or structured. ecological validity of the procedures employed
Major advantages of indirect assessments are that (Emerson, 1992). The requirement of demonstrating
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 21

experimental control over a problem behavior Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
may lead to artificial situations in which results problems through functional communication training.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126.
are not reflective of the types of functional Carr, E. G., & Newsom, C. (1985). Demand-related tan-
relationships operating in an individual’s natural trums: Conceptualization and treatment. Behavior
settings (Emerson, 1992). Modification, 9, 403–426.
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976).
Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psy-
chotic child. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
References 4, 139–153.
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980).
Arndorfer, R. E., Miltenberger, R. G., Woster, S. H., Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior of two
Rortvedt, A. K., & Gaffaney, T. (1994). Home-based retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior
descriptive and experimental analysis of problem Analysis, 13, 101–117.
behaviors in children. Topics in Early Childhood Carr, E. G., Yarbrough, S. C., & Langdon, N. A. (1997).
Special Education, 14, 64–87. Effects of idiosyncratic stimuli variables on functional
Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. (1959). The psychiatric nurse as analysis outcomes. Journal of Applied Behavior
a behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis, 30, 673–686.
Analysis of Behavior, 2, 323–334. Collins, D. T. (1965). Head-banging: Its meaning and
Baer, D. M. (1960). Escape and avoidance response of management in the severely retarded adult. Bulletin of
pre-school children to two schedules of reinforcement the Menninger Clinic, 4, 205–211.
withdrawal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Behavior, 3, 155–159. Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some River: Pearson Education.
still-current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Cooper, L. J., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G. M., Reimers, T. M.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97. & Donn, L. K. (1990). Using parents as therapists to
Bailey, J., & Meyerson, L. (1969). Vibration as a rein- evaluate appropriate behavior of their children:
forcer with a profoundly retarded child. Journal of Application to a tertiary diagnostic clinic. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 135–137. Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 285–296.
Bailey, J., & Meyerson, L. (1970). Effect of vibratory Day, H. M., Horner, R. H., & O’Neill, R. E. (1994).
stimulation on a retardate’s self-injurious behavior. Multiple functions of problem behaviors: Assessment
Psychological Aspects of Disability, 17, 133–137. and intervention. Journal of Applied Behavior
Baskett, L. M., & Johnson, S. M. (1982). The young child’s Analysis, 27, 279–289.
interactions with parents versus siblings: A behavioral Day, R. M., Rea, J. A., Schussler, N. G., Larsen, S. E., &
analysis. Child Development, 53, 643–650. Johnson, W. L. (1988). A functionally based approach
Berkson, G., Mason, W. A., & Saxon, S. V. (1963). to the treatment of self-injurious behavior. Behavior
Situation and stimulus effects on stereotyped behav- Modification, 12, 565–588.
iors of chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Dennis, W., & Najarian, P. (1957). Infant development
Physiological Psychology, 56, 786–792. under environmental handicap. Psychological
Bijou, S. W. (1955). A systematic approach to an experi- Monograph, 71(436), 1–13.
mental analysis of young children. Child Development, Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Peck, S., Sasso, G., DeRaad,
26, 161–168. A., Berg, W., et al. (1994). Functional analysis of sep-
Bijou, S. W. (1957). Methodology for an experimental arate topographies of aberrant behavior. Journal of
analysis of child behavior. Psychological Reports, 3, Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 267–278.
243–250. Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M.,
Bijou, S. W. (1958). A child study laboratory on wheels. Northup, J., Cigrand, K., et al. (1992). Brief functional
Child Development, 29, 425–427. assessment techniques to evaluate aberrant behavior in
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968). A an outpatient setting: A summary of 79 cases. Journal
method to integrate descriptive and experimental field of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 713–721.
studies at the level of data and empirical concepts. Desrochers, M. N., Hile, M. G., & Williams-Mosely, T.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191. (1997). A survey of functional assessment procedures
Bowman, L. G., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., & used with individuals with severe problem behaviors
Piazza, C. C. (1997). On the relation of mands and the and mental retardation. American Journal on Mental
function of destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Retardation, 101, 535–546.
Behavior Analysis, 30, 251–265. DuPaul, G. J., & Ervin, R. A. (1996). Functional
Budd, K. S., Green, D. R., & Baer, D. M. (1976). An analysis assessment of behaviors related to attention-deficit/
of multiple misplaced parental social contingencies. hyperactivity disorder: Linking assessment to inter-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 459–470. vention design. Behavior Therapy, 27, 601–622.
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious Durand, V. M. (1982). Analysis and intervention of self-
behavior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological injurious behavior. The Journal of the Association for
Bulletin, 84, 800–816. the Severely Handicapped, 7, 44–54.
22 D.R. Dixon et al.

Durand, V. M. (1987). “Look homeward angel”: A call to of problem parent-child relations with a parent in a
return to our (functional) roots. Behavior Analyst, 10, therapeutic role. Journal of Experimental Child
299–302. Psychology, 3, 99–107.
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
“self-stimulatory” behavior: Analysis and treatment Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional analysis of
application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental
119–132. Disabilities, 2, 3–20.
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional commu- Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
nication training to reduce challenging behavior: Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis
Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 251–264. 27, 197–209.
Emerson, E. (1992). Self-injurious behaviour: An over- Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman, D. C.,
view of recent trends in epidemiological and behav- & Shore, B. A. (1994). A sequential, test-control meth-
ioural research. Mental Handicap Research, 5, 49–81. odology for conducting functional analyses of self-
Ferster, C. B. (1958). Reinforcement and punishment in injurious behavior. Behavior Modification, 18, 289–306.
the control of human behavior of social agencies. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
Psychiatric Research Reports, 10, 101–118. Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994). The func-
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961). The development tions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-
of performances in autistic children in an automati- epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
cally controlled environment. Journal of Chronic Analysis, 27, 215–240.
Diseases, 13, 312–345. Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S.,
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1962). A method for the Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill acquisi-
experimental analysis of the behavior of autistic children. tion in the implementation of functional analysis
The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 32, 89–98. methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Fisher, W. W., Lindauer, S. E., Alterson, C. J., & 33, 181–194.
Thompson, R. H. (1998). Assessment and treatment of Jacobson, J. W., & Holburn, S. (2004). History and cur-
destructive behavior maintained by stereotypic object rent status of applied behavior analysis in develop-
manipulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, mental disabilities. In J. L. Matson, R. B. Laud, & M.
31, 513–527. L. Matson (Eds.), Behavior modification for persons
Fisher, W. W., Ninness, H. A., Piazza, C. C., & Owen- with developmental disabilities: Treatments and sup-
DeSchryver, J. S. (1996). On the reinforcing effects on ports (Vol. 1, pp. 1–32). Kingston: NADD Press.
the content of verbal attention. Journal of Applied James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1 & 2).
Behavior Analysis, 29, 235–238. New York: Dover.
Freud, A., & Burlingham, D. T. (1944). Infants without Jones, F. H., Simmons, J. Q., & Frankel, F. (1974). An
families. New York: International Universities Press. extinction procedure for eliminating self-destructive
Fuller, P. R. (1949). Operant conditioning of a vegetative behavior in a 9-year-old autistic girl. Journal of Autism
human organism. The American Journal of Psychology, and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 241–250.
62, 587–590. Kahng, S. W., Abt, K. A., & Schonbachler, H. E. (2001).
Green, A. H. (1967). Self-mutilation in schizophrenic chil- Assessment and treatment of low-rate high-intensity
dren. Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 234–244. problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Owen- 34, 225–228.
DeSchryver, J., Iwata, B. A., & Wacker, D. P. (1997). Kazdin, A. E. (2010). Single-case research designs:
Toward the development of structured criteria for Methods for clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.).
interpretation of functional analysis data. Journal of New York: Oxford University Press.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 313–326. Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F.,
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). O’Connor, J. T., Paclawskyj, T. R., et al. (2003).
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases.
Harlow, H. F., Dodsworth, R. O., & Harlow, M. K. (1965). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219.
Total social isolation in monkeys. Proceedings of the Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied
America, 54, 90–97. Behavior Analysis, 29, 391–395.
Harris, F. R., Wolf, M. M., & Baer, D. M. (1964). Effects Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livezey, K. (1995).
of adult social reinforcement on child behavior. Identification and modification of a response-class
Journal of Nursery Education, 20, 8–17. hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
Hart, B. M., Allen, E. K., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & 551–559.
Wolf, M. M. (1964). Effects of social reinforcement Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Machalicek, W., Lancioni, G.,
on operant crying. Journal of Experimental Child Rispoli, M., & Chan, J. M. (2008). A preliminary
Psychology, 1, 145–153. comparison of functional analysis results when con-
Hawkins, R. P., Peterson, R. F., Schweid, E., & Bijou, S. ducted in contrived versus natural settings. Journal of
W. (1966). Behavior therapy in the home: Amelioration Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 441–445.
2 A Brief History of Functional Analysis and Applied Behavior Analysis 23

Lindsley, O. R. (1956). Operant conditioning methods analysis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior
applied to research in chronic schizophrenia. Analysis, 40, 197–202.
Psychiatric Research Reports, 5, 118–139. Michael, J. (1980). Flight from behavior analysis.
Lindsley, O. R. (1960). Characteristics of the behavior of Behavior Analyst, 3, 1–22.
chronic psychotics as revealed by free-operant condi- Murphy, G. (1982). Sensory reinforcement in the mentally
tioning methods. Diseases of the Nervous System, 21, handicapped and autistic child: A review. Journal of
66–78. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12, 265–278.
Lindsley, O. R., Skinner, B. F., & Solomon, H. C. (1953). Myers, J. J., & Deibert, A. N. (1971). Reduction of self-
Studies in behavior therapy (Status Report I). abusive behavior in a blind child by using a feeding
Walthama: Metropolitan State Hospital. response. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C. Experimental Psychiatry, 2, 141–144.
(1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophre- Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Ellsworth, C. L.,
nia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal of MacAleese, A. N., & Cleveland, J. M. (2008).
Experimental Child Psychology, 2, 67–84. Functional analyses and treatment of precursor behav-
Lovaas, O. I., & Simmons, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation of iors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 97–105.
self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, K.,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 143–157. Cook, J., et al. (1991). A brief functional analysis of
Ludwig, A. M., Marx, A. J., Hill, P. A., & Browning, R. aggressive and alternative behavior in an outclinic
M. (1969). Control of violent behavior through faradic setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
shock: A case study. The Journal of Nervous and 509–522.
Mental Disease, 148, 624–637. O’Connor, J. T., Sorensen-Burnworth, R. J., Rush, K. S.,
Mace, F. C. (1994). The significance and future of func- & Eidman, S. L. (2003). A mand analysis and levels of
tional analysis methodologies. Journal of Applied treatment in an outpatient clinic. Behavioral
Behavior Analysis, 27, 385–392. Interventions, 18, 139–150.
Mace, F. C., Browder, D. M., & Lin, Y. (1987). Analysis O’Reilly, M. F. (1996). Assessment and treatment of epi-
of demand conditions associated with stereotypy. sodic self-injury: A case study. Research in
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Developmental Disabilities, 17, 349–361.
Psychiatry, 18, 25–31. Patterson, G. R., Littman, R. A., & Bricker, W. (1967).
Mace, F. C., & Knight, D. (1986). Functional analysis and Assertive behavior in children: A step toward a theory
the treatment of severe pica. Journal of Applied of aggression. Monographs of the Society for Research
Behavior Analysis, 19, 411–416. in Child Development, 32, iii–43.
Mace, F. C., Page, T. J., Ivancic, M. T., & O’Brien, S. Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A.,
(1986). Analysis of environmental determinants of Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., et al. (1998).
aggression and disruption in mentally retarded children. Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of its
Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 7, 203–221. reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior
Mace, F. C., & West, B. J. (1986). Analysis of demand Analysis, 31, 165–189.
conditions associated with reluctant speech. Journal of Rincover, A. (1978). Sensory extinction: A procedure for
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 17, eliminating self-stimulatory behavior in developmen-
285–294. tally disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child
Maisto, C. R., Baumeister, A. A., & Maisto, A. A. (1978). Psychology, 6, 299–310.
An analysis of variables related to self-injurious behav- Rincover, A., Newsom, C. D., & Carr, E. G. (1979). Using
iour among institutionalised retarded persons. Journal sensory extinction procedures in the treatment of com-
of Intellectual Disability Research, 22, 27–36. pulsive like behavior of developmentally disabled
McComas, J., Hoch, H., Paone, D., & El-Roy, D. (2000). children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Escape behavior during academic tasks: A preliminary Psychology, 47, 695–701.
analysis of idiosyncratic establishing operations. Ringdahl, J. E., Christensen, T. J., & Boelter, E. W. (2009).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 479–493. Further evaluation of idiosyncratic functions for severe
McKinney, J. P. (1962). A multidimensional study of the problem behavior: Aggression maintained by access
behavior of severely retarded boys. Child Development, to walks. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 275–283.
33, 923–938. Ringdahl, J. E., & Sellers, J. A. (2000). The effects of dif-
Meyerson, L., Kerr, N., & Michael, J. L. (1967). Behavior ferent adults as therapists during functional analyses.
modification in rehabilitation. In S. W. Bijou & D. M. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 33, 247–250.
Baer (Eds.), Child development: Readings in experi- Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. W. (1999). False-
mental analysis (pp. 214–239). New York: Appleton. positive maintenance of self-injurious behavior by
Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E., Riley, access to tangible reinforcers. Journal of Applied
J., DuBard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002). Teaching Behavior Analysis, 32, 201–204.
acquisition of functional analysis methodology. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 73–77. experimental analysis. Acton: Copley.
Moore, J. W., & Fisher, W. W. (2007). The effects of vid- Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
eotape modeling on staff acquisition of functional New York: The Macmillan Company.
24 D.R. Dixon et al.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton: Copley. behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
Skinner, B. F. (1974). Beyond behaviorism. New York: 169–178.
Vintage Books. Tiger, J. H., Fisher, W. W., Toussaint, K. A., & Kodak, T.
Slifer, K. J., Ivancic, M. T., Parrish, J. M., Page, T. J., & (2009). Progressing from initially ambiguous func-
Burgio, L. D. (1986). Assessment and treatment of tional analyses: Three case examples. Research in
multiple behavior problems exhibited by a profoundly Developmental Disabilities, 30, 910–926.
retarded adult. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
Experimental Psychiatry, 17, 203–213. H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification of extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
environmental determinants of behavior disorders aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
through functional analysis of precursor behaviors. Analysis, 28, 561–576.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–136. Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Harding, J. W., Derby, K. M.,
Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Pace, G. M. Asmus, J. M., & Healy, A. (1998). Evaluation and
(1992). On the relationship between self-injurious long-term treatment of aberrant behavior displayed by
behavior and self-restraint. Journal of Applied young children with disabilities. Developmental and
Behavior Analysis, 25, 433–445. Behavioral Pediatrics, 19, 260–266.
Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. (1999). Low-frequency Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G., Berg,
high-intensity problem behavior: Toward an applied W., Reimers, T., et al. (1990). A component analysis
technology of functional assessment and intervention. of functional communication training across three
In A. C. Repp & R. H. Horner (Eds.), Functional topographies of severe behavior problems. Journal of
analysis of problem behavior: From effective assess- Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
ment to effective support (pp. 98–116). Belmont: Wallace, M. D., Doney, J. K., Mintz-Resudek, C. M., &
Wadsworth. Tarbox, R. S. F. (2004). Training educators to imple-
Sturmey, P., & Bernstein, H. (2004). Functional analysis ment functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior
of maladaptive behaviors: Current status and future Analysis, 37, 89–92.
directions. In J. L. Matson, R. B. Laud, & M. L. Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session
Matson (Eds.), Behavior modification for persons with duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of
developmental disabilities: Treatments and supports Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175–183.
(Vol. 1, pp. 1–32). Kingston: NADD Press. Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views
Sturmey, P., Carlsen, A., Crisp, A. G., & Newton, J. T. it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.
(1988). A functional analysis of multiple aberrant Weber, K. P., Killu, K., Derby, K. M., & Barretto, A.
responses: A refinement and extension of Iwata et al.’s (2005). The status of functional behavioral assessment
(1982) methodology. Journal of Intellectual Disability (FBA): Adherence to standard practice in FBA meth-
Research, 32, 31–46. odology. Psychology in the Schools, 42, 737–744.
Tarbox, J., Wallace, M. D., Tarbox, R. S., Landaburu, H. J., Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task difficulty and
& Williams, W. L. (2004). Functional analysis and treat- aberrant behavior in severely handicapped students.
ment of low rate problem behavior in individuals with Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 449–463.
developmental disabilities. Behavioral Interventions, Wilkins, J., & Matson, J. L. (2009). History of treatment
19, 187–204. in children with developmental disabilities and psy-
Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles, chopathology. In J. L. Matson, F. Andrasik, & M. L.
R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009). Matson (Eds.), Treating Childhood Psychopathology
Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental and Developmental Disabilities (pp. 3–28). New York:
functional assessments of challenging behavior in Springer.
children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Wolery, M. (1978). Self-stimulatory behavior as a basis
Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514. for devising reinforcers. AAESPH Review, 3, 23–29.
Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. (1966). Aversive control of Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjec-
self-injurious behavior in a psychotic boy. Behaviour tive measurement or how applied behavior analysis is
Research and Therapy, 4, 281–287. finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Taylor, J. C., Sisson, L. A., McKelvey, J. L., & Trefelner, 11, 203–214.
M. F. (1993). Situation specificity in attention-seeking Wolf, M., Risley, T., Johnston, M., Harris, F., & Allen, E.
problem behavior: A case-study. Behavior Modification, (1967). Application of operant conditioning proce-
17, 474–497. dures to the behavior problems of an autistic child:
Thomas, D. R., Becker, W. C., & Armstrong, M. (1968). A follow-up and extension. Behaviour Research and
Production and elimination of disruptive classroom Therapy, 5, 103–111.
behavior by systematically varying teacher’s behavior. Wolf, M. M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1964). Application
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 35–45. of operant conditioning procedures to the behavior
Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2001). A descriptive problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research and
analysis of social consequences following problem Therapy, 1, 305–312.
Nature, Prevalence,
and Characteristics 3
of Challenging Behavior

Robert Didden, Peter Sturmey, Jeff Sigafoos,


Russell Lang, Mark F. O’Reilly, and Giulio E. Lancioni

Challenging behaviors are among the most into the nature, prevalence, and characteristics of
serious and studied problems in the field of challenging behaviors in this target group. Results
developmental disabilities (Matson, Kozlowski of these studies have shown that challenging
et al. 2011). An increasing number of studies are behaviors are common in children and adults
being published that have provided more insight with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and/or
intellectual disabilities (ID). Population studies
have shown that between 5 and 15% of individu-
als with ID show some type of challenging
R. Didden (*)
behavior, like self-injury, aggression, stereotypic
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University behavior, and other problem behaviors. Studies
Nijmegen, 6500 HE, P.O. Box 9104, Nijmegen, also show that rates of challenging behaviors are
The Netherlands increased if individuals also have ASD. For
e-mail: r.didden@pwo.ru.nl
example, Holden and Gitlesen (2006) found that
P. Sturmey among over 900 individuals with ID, 11% showed
Department of Psychology, Queens College,
City University of New York, A-315 Science Building,
one or more types of challenging behavior.
6530 Kissina Blvd, Flushing, NY 11367, USA Of the study sample, 6% had been diagnosed
e-mail: psturmey@gmail.com with autism, and 36% of this subgroup showed
J. Sigafoos challenging behavior. Rojahn, Matson, Lott,
School of Educational Psychology and Pedagogy, Esbensen, and Smalls (2001) employed the
Victoria University of Wellington, Karori Campus, Behavior Problem Inventory-01 (BPI-01) in a
PO Box 17-310, Wellington, New Zealand
e-mail: jeff.sigafoos@vuw.ac.nz
sample of individuals with ID (n = 432) who lived
in a residential facility and who were between 14
R. Lang
College of Education, Texas State University,
and 19 years old. Results showed that individuals
San Marcos, TX 78666, USA with ASD had higher rates of aggression, self-
e-mail: russlang@txstate.edu injury and stereotypy than those with ID without
M.F. O’Reilly ASD. These findings were corroborated by results
Department of Special Education, from a meta-analysis by McClintock, Hall, and
College of Education, The University of Texas at Austin, Oliver (2003) who have explored risk factors for
Austin, TX 78712, USA
e-mail: markoreilly@mail.utexas.edu
challenging behaviors in individuals with devel-
opmental disabilities. They have published a
G.E. Lancioni
Department of Psychology, University of Bari,
meta-analysis of 22 studies conducted over 30
Via QuintinoSella 268, Bary 70100, Italy years, and the results showed that children and
e-mail: g.lancioni@psico.uniba.it adults with ASD were more likely than other

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 25


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_3,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
26 R. Didden et al.

individuals with ID to exhibit challenging adaptive skills, and it leads to additional costs of
behaviors such as self-injury and aggression. specialized services.
These results indicate that ASD itself may be a In the last two decades, major advances have
risk factor for challenging behavior in individu- been made in the treatment of challenging behav-
als with ID. However, other researchers failed to iors in individuals with ASD and/or ID. Results
find an association between challenging behavior of numerous studies have shown that challenging
and ASD. For example, Tyrer et al. (2006) inves- behavior may be the result of a learning process
tigated prevalence rate of physical aggression in in which this behavior acquires an operant func-
over 3,000 adults with profound to mild ID, and tion through its interaction with environmental
they failed to find a relationship between chal- events. In this view, challenging behavior is not
lenging behavior and the presence of autism. conceptualized as a symptom of “underlying”
During the last decades, several definitions of psychopathology but as a response that has
challenging behavior have been proposed. For acquired one or more behavioral functions for the
example, Emerson (2005) defined challenging person. Effective behavioral intervention requires
behavior as the identification of functional properties of the
…culturally abnormal behaviour of such intensity, challenging behavior, a technology that is called
frequency or duration that the physical safety of functional assessment.
the person or others is placed in serious jeopardy, The most often addressed types of behavior in
or behaviour which is likely to seriously limit or functional assessment in individuals with ASD
deny access to the use of ordinary community
facilities… and/or ID are SIB and aggression, followed by
stereotypic behavior, tantrums, destruction, and,
In this definition, there is no reference to to a lesser extent, feeding problems, pica, and
specific topographies or causes of the challeng- rumination, among others (Matson, Sipes et al.,
ing behavior, but it is defined in terms of its 2011). This chapter considers the nature, preva-
effects on the person’s life. Other researchers lence, and characteristics of most of these chal-
have employed a definition of challenging behav- lenging behaviors among individuals with ASD
ior that distinguishes between more and less and/or ID. We focus on self-injury, stereotypy,
demanding behavior (e.g., Holden & Gitlesen, aggression, feeding disorders, pica, and rumina-
2006). The former means that the person shows tion/vomiting. Each section begins with a
challenging behavior on a daily basis, that this definition of one of these behaviors. Afterwards,
behavior usually prevents the person from taking forms of the behavior that have been observed
part in programs or activities, that more than one among individuals with ASD/ID are described
caregiver is needed to physically control the and classified. We then provide a selective review
behavior, and that the behavior causes major of studies that have explored the prevalence of
bodily injury to the person or others. Less challenging behavior. The final sections consider
demanding behavior includes attacking others, risk factors and adverse consequences of that
self-injurious behaviors, destruction, or other- type of challenging behavior.
wise problematic behavior that does not meet
these requirements.
Challenging behavior in individuals with ASD Self-Injurious Behavior
and/or ID has many adverse consequences for the
person involved and his or her family members, Definition
professional carers, and society at large. It may
result in rejection by peers and caregivers, exclu- Researchers have adopted two general approaches
sion from settings, restrictive and potential harm- to defining SIB. The first requires not only that
ful treatment practices (such as polypharmacy), the self-inflicted acts cause injury but also that
serious health risks, and distress to all involved. they occur with deliberate intent. Winchel and
Challenging behavior interferes with learning Stanley (1991) defined SIB as
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 27

…the commission of deliberate harm to one’s own SIB forms documented in the repertories of
body. The injury is done to oneself, without the aid individuals with ASD and other developmental
of another person, and the injury is severe enough
for tissue damage (such as scarring) to result. disabilities include head banging, head hitting,
(p. 306) hair pulling, eye poking, face slapping, and
biting, pinching, and scratching oneself (Fee &
In their elaboration of this definition, Winchel Matson, 1992; Matson, Fodstad, Mahan, &
and Stanley excluded acts that occurred with Rojahn, 2010; Sigafoos, Arthur, & O’Reilly,
“conscious suicidal intent or associated with sex- 2003; Smith et al., 2007). Many other forms of
ual arousal” (p. 306). Klonsky and Meuhlenkamp behavior can produce self-inflicted injury and/or
(2007) also emphasized underlying intent and serious health complications if they occur with
added reference to social norms when they defined sufficient frequency and/or severity (e.g., pica,
SIB as the “intentional destruction of body tissue rumination—also see section on “Pica, Vomiting,
without suicidal intention and for purposes not and Rumination”).
socially sanctioned” (p. 1045). In line with these Weiss (2003) reviewed several conceptually
definitions, behaviors associated with suicide, and empirically based schemes that have been
sexual arousal, or sociocultural practices are used to classify these SIB forms. Fee and Matson
excluded from our definition of SIB. (1992), for example, placed SIB behaviors on a
In contrast to the above approach, Rojahn, continuum of severity ranging from mild forms
Whittaker, Hoch, and Gonzáles (2007) argued for (e.g., head rubbing, finger sucking, nail licking,
defining SIB without reference to underlying inten- thigh slapping) to more severe forms (e.g., eye
tions so as to prevent the formation of potentially poking, self-scratching, chronic rumination, head
misleading inferences regarding the causes of SIB. banging). Consideration of severity would seem
They endorsed the approach of Tate and Baroff a useful type of classification when prioritizing
(1966), who defined SIB as repetitive acts directed behaviors for treatment, and this approach might
to one’s own body that cause tissue damage or also provide cues to the development of severe
physical harm. Similarly, Smith, Vollmer, and St. SIB. Along these lines, data from Murphy, Hall,
Peter Pipkin (2007) defined SIB as “Behavior that Oliver, and Kissi-Debra (1999) suggest that SIB
produces or has the capacity to produce tissue may emerge from early and less severe (or proto-
damage to the individual’s own body.” (p. 188). injurious) behaviors that are often present in the
For the purpose of this chapter, these two child’s repertoire before 2 years of age.
definitional approaches have been integrated. SIB In addition to severity, SIB could also be
is defined as behavior directed towards oneself viewed along a continuum of frequency as pro-
that causes—or has the potential to cause—tissue posed by Jones (1987). At one end of this con-
damage, exclusive of acts associated with sui- tinuum are high frequency/stereotyped acts that
cide, sexual arousal, or socially sanctioned prac- may cause injury over time, such as the almost
tices. This integrated definition reflects the constant hand rubbing seen in individuals with
common forms of SIB that have been observed Rett syndrome (Deb, 1998). At the other end are
among people with ASD and acknowledges that high intensity acts that occur less frequently, such
acts associated with suicide, sexual arousal, or as bursts of head banging that might occur once
socially sanctioned practices are often quite or twice per day.
distinct from the forms of SIB most commonly SIB has also been classified in terms of having
seen in individuals with ASD and other develop- a social versus nonsocial basis (Schroeder,
mental disabilities (Weiss, 2003). Mulick, & Rojahn, 1980; Sigafoos, Reichle, &
Light Shriner, 1994). Social SIB occurs because
Forms and Classification of past success in recruiting some type of social
response from other people. SIB forms reported
Numerous specific forms or topographies of to be associated with a social basis include head
behavior can meet the above definition of SIB. banging, self-biting, self-scratching, gouging
28 R. Didden et al.

oneself, pinching oneself, and pulling out one’s also assessed with the Childhood Autism Rating
own hair. Nonsocial SIB, in contrast, appears to Scale (CARS: Schloper, Reichler, DeVellis, &
produce direct sensory stimulation that is auto- Daly, 1988). The resulting mean total score for
matically reinforcing. Such acts may include the group was 36, which corresponds to a
stuffing orifices, mouthing objects, and finger classification of moderately autistic. Most of
sucking. Experimental data have accumulated to these 222 children also had an ID. In fact, 70% of
support this social/nonsocial distinction. the sample had severe ID, 5.9% had profound ID,
Specifically, it is now well established that for 20.3% had mild ID, and only 4% had no ID.
some individuals with ASD, SIB is often main- Other diagnoses included epilepsy (7.2%),
tained by positive or negative reinforcement that genetic syndromes (3.2%), and perinatal condi-
is socially mediated, such as attention from a tions (5%). The boy to girl ratio was 4.7:1, which
caregiver, access to preferred objects or activi- is consistent with reports that ASD is four to five
ties, and/or being allowed to escape from a non- times more common in boys than girls (American
preferred task. For other individuals, however, Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000).
SIB appears to be maintained by direct sensory To identify children with SIB, the researchers
consequences or automatic reinforcement (Iwata, had care staff to complete a questionnaire regard-
Pace et al., 1994). However, the social versus ing the presence or absence of SIB in each child’s
nonsocial distinction does not necessarily follow repertoire. When SIB was present, care staff also
precise topographical lines. Thus, some individu- rated its severity in a three-point scale (mild,
als could potentially develop seemingly nonso- moderate, severe). Assessment was also under-
cial forms of SIB that are in fact socially motivated taken of each child’s expressive speech and adap-
or that might acquire a social basis overtime and tive behavior functioning using the Autism
vice versa. Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Lord, Rutter, &
Lecoutteur, 1994) and the Vineland Adaptive
Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti,
Prevalence 1984), respectively. These additional measures
allowed the researchers to undertake analyses to
The prevalence of SIB has been more widely identify risk factors for SIB among these 222
studied among individuals with ID than individu- children. Based on the results of the care-staff
als with ASD. Overall, studies of individuals with questionnaire, the children were classified into
ID indicate that SIB occurs in approximately two groups based on presence or absence of SIB.
10–12% of this population (Bienstein & These two groups were then compared in terms
Nussbeck, 2009; Emerson et al., 2001; Holden & of gender, age, parental social class, level of
Gitlesen, 2006; Lowe et al., 2007; Murphy et al., speech/adaptive behavior, and perinatal and med-
1993; Oliver, Murphy, & Corbett, 1987; van ical conditions.
Ingen, Moore, Zaja, & Rojahn, 2010). The results of these analyses were remarkable
The 10–12% prevalence rate from studies of in terms of the relatively high percentage of
individuals with ID can be analyzed in relation to children who were identified as having SIB.
the results of three recent studies that have pro- Specifically, 53% (n = 109) of the sample was
vided data on the extent and circumstances of reported by care staff to engage in SIB, with
SIB among individuals with ASD. One of these 14.6% reported to engage in severe SIB.
three studies included 222 young children from Interestingly, there were no significant differ-
French service agencies for children with ASD ences between the SIB and non-SIB groups in
(Baghdadli, Pascal, Grisi, & Aussilloux, 2003). terms of age, gender, epilepsy, genetic syndromes,
These 222 children ranged from 2 to 7 years of or parental social class. The SIB group did,
age with a mean age of 5 years. The children met however, differ significantly from the non-SIB
the ICD-10 criteria for infantile autism (World group in terms of the severity of their autistic
Health Organization, 1992). The children were symptoms, their extent of speech and adaptive
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 29

behavior deficits, and their birth complications and four subscales: (a) Aggression/Destruction,
(i.e., perinatal conditions). Younger age, severe (b) Stereotypy, (c) SIB, and (d) Disruptive behavior.
autism, and greater daily living skill deficits were Items were rated as either 0 (not a problem, no
identified as increasing the likelihood that a child impairment) or 1 (problem, impairment). The SIB
would be rated as having SIB. subscale consisted of three items: (a) poking self
Compared to the consistently reported 10–12% in the eye; (b) harming self by hitting, pinching, or
prevalence rate of SIB among individuals with scratching, etc.; and (c) mouthing or swallowing
ID, the 53% prevalence rate reported by Baghdadli objects causing bodily harm. As in Baghdadli
et al. (2003) suggests that SIB could be up to five et al. (2003), direct care staff served as informants.
times more prevalent among individuals with Additional ratings using standardized scales were
ASD than for individuals with ID. However, the made of autistic symptoms and comorbid condi-
results of this study should be interpreted with tions to identify risk factors.
caution because critical psychometric properties The results of the Matson and Rivet (2008a)
of the care-staff questionnaire (e.g., inter-rater study showed that adults with autistic disorder
reliability, content validity) were lacking. In addi- had significantly higher mean total problem
tion, details on the definition and forms of SIB behavior scores (4.39) followed by the PDD-
assessed were lacking, and descriptive anchors NOS (2.92), and ID-only (1.41) groups. Mean
for the rating of SIB severity were not provided. SIB ratings were also significantly different for
The high prevalence rate might also reflect the the autism (0.61), PDD-NOS (0.40), and ID-only
fact that 70% of these children had ASD and (0.13) groups. As is often found in studies of
severe ID. Among individuals with ID, SIB is problem behavior among individuals with devel-
more prevalent among those with severe, as com- opmental disabilities, most individuals in this
pared to mild/moderate, ID (Schroeder, Tessel, sample engaged in more than one type of prob-
Loupe, & Stodgell, 1997). lem behavior. For example, SIB and stereotypy
In a second relevant study, Matson and Rivet were significantly correlated (r = 0.48). Consistent
(2008a) studied 320 adults from two residential with Baghdadli et al. (2003), Matson and
centers in Louisiana, USA. Participants ranged Rivet also found that the frequency of problem
from 20 to 88 years of age with a mean age of 52 behavior, including SIB, increased with the sever-
years. The sample included more males (56%) ity of autistic symptoms, suggesting that severe
than females (44%). Most of these adults (75.5%) autism is a risk factor for SIB and other problem
were reported to have a profound level of ID. The behaviors. However, this conclusion should be
sample was divided into an ASD group, a PDD- interpreted with caution because the sample con-
NOS group, and an ID-only group. Sixty-two sisted only of adults living in residential facili-
individuals (19%) had a diagnosis of autistic dis- ties. There are data suggesting that individuals
order, and 99 individuals (31%) were diagnosed with severe behavior problems, including SIB,
with PDD-NOS. The remaining participants had are more likely to end up in more restrictive
ID only. The three groups did not differ in terms settings, such as residential centers (see Sigafoos
of gender, ethnicity, or length of time living in the et al., 2003, pp. 42–43 for a review). The Matson
centers, but they did differ in terms of their age and Rivet (2008a) study is limited by the inclu-
and level of ID. Specifically the autistic and PDD- sion of only adults most of whom had profound
NOS groups had fewer individuals over 60 years ID. Still, the results point to a significantly higher
of age, and the autistic group had more individu- prevalence of SIB among the autism group com-
als with profound ID than the ID-only group. pared to the PDD-NOS and ID-only groups.
In this study, Matson and Rivet (2008a) used a Murphy, Healy, and Leader (2009) studied
reliable and valid measure of problem behavior: 157 children with ASD. The children were
the Autism Spectrum Disorders-Behavior recruited from Irish educational units. Their ages
Problems for Adults (ASD-BPA; Matson, Terlonge, ranged from 3 to 14.2 years with a mean of 8.5
& Gonzáles, 2006). The ASD-BPA has 19 items years. As is typical of the ASD population, the
30 R. Didden et al.

sample included more boys (82.8%) than girls behaviors among individuals with developmental
(17.2%). The level of ID was reported for only disabilities. Based on this review, and the results
69% (n = 109) of the children. Of these 109, 20% of Baghdadli et al. (2003), Matson and Rivet
had normal intelligence, 37.6% had mild ID, (2008a), and Murphy et al. (2009), a number of
23.8% had moderate ID, 17.4% had severe ID, factors might be pointed out as responsible for
and only one child had profound ID. increasing the risk of SIB among individuals with
Data were provided by informants with a mini- ASD. These are:
mum of 1 year experience in working with the
child. The researcher interviewed informants using Severity of Intellectual Disability
the Behavior Problems Inventory (BPI-01; Rojahn, SIB is more prevalent among those with ASD
Matson, Lott, Esbensen, & Smalls, 2001). The BPI and severe ID, as compared to those with ASD
has 52 items covering various types of problem and either mild/moderate levels of ID or no ID.
behaviors (e.g., aggression, stereotypy), including
14 SIB-specific items. Items are rated for frequency Severity of Autistic Symptoms
on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 The prevalence of SIB is higher among individu-
(hourly) and for severity on a 4-point scale ranging als with more severe symptoms of autism when
from 0 (no problem) to 3 (severe problem). compared to individuals with less severe autism
The resulting prevalence figure for SIB was symptoms.
lower than reported by Baghdadli et al. (2003),
but roughly three times higher than the 10–12% ASD Diagnosis
reported in studies of individuals with ID. An ASD diagnosis increases the risk of SIB
Specifically, 32.5% of the children were rated as among individuals with ID.
having SIB, aggression, and stereotyped behav-
ior, whereas 11% showed SIB and stereotyped Comorbid Conditions
behavior, and 4% showed SIB and aggression. SIB among individuals with ASD, severe ID, and
Thus, most children showed more than one type comorbid psychopathology (e.g., depression,
of problem behavior. The most common form of bipolar disorder) appears to be higher than for any
SIB was self-biting, which was present in 22% of other clinical group (Matson & LoVullo, 2008).
the children with SIB. There were no significant
relations between age or gender and problem Placement
behavior. Children with severe ID had a higher SIB is more prevalent among people with ID in
median rating for severity of SIB. This is an institutional versus community settings (Holden
important study due to the fact that there was a & Gitlesen, 2006; Lowe et al., 2007). This could
greater distribution of levels of ID among these reflect a tendency for people with SIB to be
157 children when compared to the Baghdadli placed in institutions, although Oliver et al.
et al. (2003) and Matson and Rivet (2008a) stud- (1987) noted that the prevalence of SIB is more
ies. However, the study is limited by a relatively difficult to estimate among community samples.
modest sample size and restricted age range. Still, There is insufficient evidence regarding the rela-
the results are consistent with a general conclu- tion between placement and SIB among individ-
sion that SIB is more prevalent among individuals uals with ASD.
with ASD and severe ID than for individuals with
less severe ID and less severe autistic symptoms. Genetic Syndromes
Certain genetic syndromes appear to predispose
the individual to SIB (Percy et al., 2007).
Risk Factors Lesch-Nyhan and Cornelia de Lange syn-
dromes, for example, are associated with an
In a review chapter, Sigafoos et al. (2003) delineated increased risk of SIB, which often takes a pre-
a number of risk factors for SIB and other problem cise, syndrome-specific form (Winchel &
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 31

Stanley, 1991 ) . There also appears to be a restraints and other intrusive treatments for SIB
relatively high prevalence of SIB among indi- raises ethical concerns (National Institutes of
viduals with Fragile X syndrome, and many Health, 1989).
such individuals also meet diagnostic criteria The National Institutes of Health (1989) noted
for autism. However, Hall, Lighthouse, and the cost of SIB at up to $US100,000.00 per year
Reiss (2008) reported that individuals with for a person with severe SIB. In addition to the
Fragile X and autism were no more likely to financial costs, there are likely to be significant
show SIB than individuals with Fragile X social costs, such as institutionalization and
alone. reduced opportunities for social inclusion
(Winchel & Stanley, 1991).
Age
There is conflicting evidence regarding the rela-
tion between age and SIB. Baghdadli et al. (2003) Stereotypic Behavior
reported that younger children were more at risk
for SIB, but Murphy et al. (2009) found no such Definition
relation. Currently, there is insufficient evidence
on the influence of chronological age on SIB in The APA (2000) defined Stereotyped Movement
persons with ASD. Disorder (SMD) as “… motor behavior that is
repetitive, often seemingly driven, and nonfunc-
Gender tional (Criterion A) … markedly interferes with
Gender does not appear to be a risk factor for SIB normal activities … (Criterion B) … sufficiently
among children with ASD. severe to be a focus of treatment (Criterion C) no
better accounted for by compulsion … a tic … a
stereotypy that is part of a Pervasive Developmental
Adaptive Behavior Functioning
Disorder, or hair pulling (Criterion D) … is not
Baghdadli et al. (2003) found that greater deficits
due to the direct effects of a substance or general
in daily living skills were risk factors for SIB, but
medical condition (Criterion E) … [and] must
the evidence base is too meager at present to draw
persist for at least 4 weeks (Criterion F).”
any firm conclusions.
In addition to SMD, DSM-IV’s diagnostic
criteria for Autistic Disorder include “restricted,
repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior,
Adverse Consequences interests, and activities as manifested by at least
one of the following: (a) encompassing preoc-
SIB is a major problem because of the associ- cupation with one or more stereotyped and
ated injury and related health problems that can restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal
arise from this behavior. SIB can lead to long- either in intensity or focus (b) apparently
term negative health consequences. Mandell inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional
(2008) noted that SIB is a significant antecedent routines or rituals (c) stereotyped and repetitive
to the hospitalization of children with ASD. In motor mannerisms (e.g., hand or finger flapping
addition to serious injury, long-term health prob- or twisting or complex whole-body movements)
lems, and hospitalization risk, SIB is associated (d) persistent preoccupation with parts of
with a number of other side effects, such as objects.” Thus, there is overlap between the
increased risk of being placed on powerful med- diagnostic criteria for SMD and Autistic Disorder
ications that can cause serious side effects. SIB related to repetitive motor behavior; however,
may also lead to social and physical isolation of the diagnostic criteria for Autistic Disorder
the individuals. This in turn might restrict oppor- placed stereotyped and repetitive motor manner-
tunities for learning, social development, and isms as only one of four kinds of restrictive,
community participation. The use of mechanical repetitive, and stereotyped behavior. (The
32 R. Didden et al.

DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for Pervasive Prevalence


Developmental Disorders other than Autistic
Disorder also reference repetitive motor behavior). SMD is relatively common among people with
Thus, when someone presents with repetitive developmental disabilities with perhaps as many
motor behavior that is a focus of clinical atten- of half of those diagnosed with ID displaying
tion and the person does not also meet criteria for some form of stereotypy (Bodfish et al., 1995;
Autistic Disorder, then a diagnosis of SDM may Rojahn, 1986). People with ASD show even
be made; however, if the person also displays higher prevalence rates of SMD. A study by
other behavior that meets criteria for Autistic Goldman et al. (2009) illustrates this observation.
Disorder, then that diagnosis is appropriate. Goldman et al. compared the prevalence of ste-
The APA (2000) distinguishes SIB (see previous reotypies in four groups of children: high or low
section) as one subtype of SMD. Although many functioning children with autism and high and
forms of SIB are indeed repetitive, such as some low functioning children with other developmen-
examples of repeated head banging, face slapping, tal disorders. Two hundred and seventy-seven
and hand mouthing, not all examples of SIB are children participated. They used an IQ cutoff of
indeed repetitive or appear to be stereotypical in 80 to operationalize high and low functioning
topography. For example, in response to apparently and assessed stereotypies with a checklist that the
minor social provocations, a person may slam their authors developed themselves for this study. The
fingers into the wheels of their wheelchair resulting prevalence of stereotypies in the entire sample
in tissue damage. Although such SIB is indeed was 44%. However, the prevalence of stereotyp-
clinically significant strictly speaking, it does not ies was 71 and 64% in the low and high function-
meet the criteria for SMD, self-injurious type, since ing group of children with autism, respectively,
the behavior is not repetitive. and 31 and 18% for the low and high functioning
non-autistic groups. Thus, the prevalence of
stereotypies was a function of both the presence
Forms and Classification of autism and degree of ID.

Some of the most common topographies of SMD


include body rocking, repetitive hand move- Adverse Consequences
ments, finger flicking, spinning, twirling and
mouthing objects, and repetitive posturing SMD has a number of adverse consequences,
(Bodfish et al., 1995). Some common forms of some of which are minor and some of which may
SIB include head banging, face and head slap- be quite significant. Repetitive behaviors are
ping, and self-biting. often socially stigmatizing; thus, RMDs are often
Typically developing infants show many targeted in intervention studies, and reduction in
repetitive motor behaviors such as arm waving, stereotypic behavior had been used to validate
kicking legs, and repetitive manipulation of staff-training procedures (Dib & Sturmey, 2007).
objects (Thelen, 1981). Indeed, some typically SMD may also have adverse consequences as it
developing infants also display SIB such as head may be related to other more directly harmful
banging (Sallustro & Atwell, 1978; Thelen, behavior, for example, it might function as a
1981). Typical adults also often engage in repeti- member of a response chain that terminates in a
tive behavior such as hair twirling, body rocking, dangerous behavior such as elopement (Falcomata,
and repetitive object manipulation. These repeti- Roane, Feeney, & Stephenson, 2010). The pres-
tive movements can be distinguished from SMD ence of SMD is also a risk marker for several
and those associated with ASDs because they do other more serious challenging behaviors such as
not come to dominate the child’s behavioral SIB and multiple challenging behaviors (Oliver,
repertoire, do not persist over time, and do not Petty, Ruddick, & Bacarese-Hamilton, 2011).
interfere with learning and daily functioning. Perhaps the most significant and insidious risk
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 33

associated with SMD is that it may inhibit the consensus about how to measure attribution of
expression of adaptive behavior and the acquisi- intent, and secondly, this term raises questions
tion of new skills. when applied to individuals with very limited
cognitive skills or deficits in the ability to take the
perspective of another person which is often the
Risk Factors case in individuals with ASD and/or ID.
Individuals with ASD and/or ID may engage in
SMD is often more prevalent in people with aggressive behavior despite having severe deficits
autism, more severe ID, with sensory impair- in these areas. For our purposes, we “define”
ments, such as visual impairments, and limited aggressive behavior as behavior that (potentially)
mobility skills. Certain medical problems, such results in injury or harm in another person or in
as otitis media, are also risk factors for head property destruction without consideration of
banging. Certain forms of stereotypy are also whether the aggressive behavior is “deliberately”
associated with genetic syndromes, for example, exhibited or not.
repetitive hand movements are associated with
Rett disorder. Finally, a number of environmental
variables also place an individual at greater risk, Forms and Classification
such as barren environments. Such environments
may fail to provide the opportunities and contin- The term “aggression” refers to a wide range of
gencies to support acquisition and expression of behavioral topographies. For example, Matson
adaptive behavior that may compete with the and Rivet (2008a) used the ASD-BPA, an infor-
development and/or persistence of SMD and SIB. mant-based 19-item rating scale consisting of
Additionally, some barren environments may four subscales derived from factor analysis. The
inadvertently reinforce and shape repetitive and Aggression/Destruction subscale contains seven
self-injurious behaviors or shape repetitive behavioral topographies of aggression: kicking
behavior into SIB (Guess & Carr, 1991). objects, throwing objects at others, banging on
objects, aggression towards others, ripping
clothes, yelling or shouting at others, and prop-
Aggressive Behavior erty destruction.
Usually, a distinction is made between aggres-
Definition sive behavior directed at objects (property
destruction) and aggressive behavior directed at
There is no single accepted definition of aggres- other people. Examples of topographies of
sion. Farmer and Aman (2011) review problems aggression directed at objects are throwing, delib-
with defining aggression. In the psychology lit- erately breaking, and hitting objects. Subcategories
erature, aggression is defined as something along of aggression towards other people are physical
the lines of “intentional harm doing” and “behav- aggression (e.g., hitting, kicking) and verbal
ior that is harming or injuring another person.” In aggression (e.g., yelling at someone, threatening
these “definitions,” one implies that a certain someone). Some researchers distinguish a third
behavior is directed to another person. Vitiello category of aggression, namely, aggression
and Stoff (1997) broadened the definition to directed towards the self or auto-aggression; this
include also behavior directed at objects. They is also called SIB (see above section on SIB).
defined aggression as behavior deliberately aimed Aggressive behavior is often classified as a
at inflicting damage to persons or property. An type of externalizing behavior as opposed to
important element in these definitions is that rumination, fearful behavior, and SIB that are
there is an “attribution of intent” (Farmer & examples of internalizing behaviors. Externalizing
Aman, 2011; p. 318). This is problematic for use behaviors are directed towards the environment
in people with ASD and/or ID. First, there is no of the person, including other people, and these
34 R. Didden et al.

behaviors usually interfere with the behavior of that aggression may also have one or more other
others. By contrast, internalizing behaviors are functions for any given individual. For example,
directed towards the person himself and usually O’Reilly et al. (2010) have conducted functional
do not interfere with the behaviors of others. The analysis of challenging behavior in 10 children
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & with ASD or PDD-NOS who were between 4 and
Rescorla, 2000) is a well-researched measure of 8 years old. Results showed that challenging
challenging behavior that addresses externalizing behaviors, including aggression and SIB, were
and internalizing behaviors. Scores are summed maintained by automatic reinforcement for 8 out
to form seven syndrome scales including the of the 10 children and by multiple sources of rein-
scale on aggression. forcement for the other 2 children. No data were
Another classification of aggressive behavior presented for only aggressive behavior as each
was proposed by Farmer and Aman (2011) who child engaged in a variety of challenging behav-
distinguish between “proactive” and “reactive” iors during functional analysis.
aggression. Proactive or instrumental aggression
is characterized by instrumental goals, or the per-
ception of a link between aggressive behavior Prevalence
and a desired outcome. Reactive aggression is
characterized by behavior that results from exter- Several studies have reported high prevalence
nal provocation or anger without thought of per- rates of aggressive behaviors in samples of chil-
sonal gain. The authors state that this distinction dren and adults with ASD. For example, Ming,
is clinically important because each subtype may Brimacombe, Chaaban, Zimmerman-Bier, and
have a different etiology. Farmer and Aman have Wagner (2008) have used retrospective chart
developed the Children’s Scale of Hostility and reviews to assess prevalence of aggressive behav-
Aggression: Reactive/Proactive (C-SHARP) and iors in 160 children and youngsters who were
recently used this scale with a sample of 121 consecutively admitted to a university-based
children with ASD. center. All had received a diagnosis of ASD.
Another way to classify aggressive behavior is Specifically, 48% had autism, 45% had PDD-
to look at the type of reinforcement that maintains NOS and the remaining 7% had Asperger
this type of challenging behavior. For example, disorder. Participants were between 2 and 18
Matson, Sipes et al. (2011) reviewed 173 studies years old. Results showed that 32% of the sample
that used functional assessment with respect to engaged in some type of aggressive behavior, and
type of challenging behavior and function(s) this behavior was significantly associated with
identified that maintained those behaviors. The mood disorder.
most frequently studied type of challenging Hartley et al. (2008) investigated prevalence
behavior after SIB was aggression. The most of challenging behaviors in a subgroup of chil-
often reported function of aggression was avoid- dren with ASD, namely, children with autism.
ance of or escape from task demands, followed Parents of 169 children who were between 1.5
by access to tangibles and, to a lesser degree, and 6 years of age completed the CBCL
nonsocial or sensory reinforcement. Results of (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000), of which aggres-
this review suggest that aggressive behavior may sion is one of seven syndromes. Each syndrome
be classified according to the type of environmen- scale has a criterion above which a behavior is
tal event that maintains it. While there are five considered clinically significant or severe enough
functions for challenging behavior, aggression to warrant intervention. Among this sample of
may primarily be maintained by escape from children, 22.5% showed clinically significant lev-
demand. So aggression may primarily be moti- els of aggression.
vated by social events, while other types of chal- Several studies have assessed rates of chal-
lenging behavior may primarily be maintained by lenging behavior such as aggression in samples
nonsocial ones. It should be noticed, however, of individuals with ASD and ID. For example, in
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 35

a large sample (n = 3,065) of adults with profound of adults with autism/ID (n = 62) to rates among
to mild ID, which in 68 cases was combined with individuals with PDD-NOS/ID (n = 99) and indi-
autism, aggression was found in 29% of the viduals with ID only (controls; n = 159) of whom
subsample (i.e., cases with ID and autism) (Tyrer most had profound ID. Data on aggression/
et al., 2006). In Kanne and Mazurek’s (2011) destruction were collected using the ASD-BPA.
study, 1,380 children between 4 and 17 years par- Prevalence rates across various topographies of
ticipated, all of whom had some type of ASD. aggression/destruction ranged from 11% (ripping
Total IQ ranged between 13 and 167, and data clothes) to 29% (yelling or shouting at others).
were collected on ASD symptomatology, cogni- Comparative analyses revealed that participants
tive and adaptive functioning, and aggressive with autism had significantly higher rates of
behavior. Results showed that 56% of the sample aggression/destruction than both the PDD-NOS
engaged in some form of aggressive behavior, group and the ID only group. These results indi-
ranging from mild (24%) to severe physical cate that among adults with profound ID who live
aggression or violence (32%), towards in a facility aggression/destruction is associated
caregivers. with severity of ASD symptoms.
Matson and Rivet (2008b) have investigated Elevated rates of aggression have been found
prevalence rates of several forms of aggression in in individuals with ID who also have genetic dis-
298 adults with ASD and ID who lived in two orders, which are frequently associated with ASD
residential facilities. It was found that among symptomatology. For example, Arron, Oliver,
participants with autism, 7% engaged in throw- Moss, Berg, and Burbridge (2011) have investi-
ing objects at others, 15% engaged in aggression gated prevalence rates of physical aggression in
towards people, and 14% engaged in property several syndrome groups. Physical aggression
destruction. was most common in Smith-Magenis syndrome
Dominick, Ornstein Davis, Lainhart, Tager- (74%) and in Angelman syndrome (73%) and, to
Flusberg, and Folstein (2007) assessed various a lesser extent, in Cri du Chat, Fragile X, and
topographies of aggression (e.g., hitting, biting Prader–Willi syndrome (43%), with rates of 70,
others) in a sample of 54 children with ASD and 52, and 43%, respectively. Compared to a control
compared outcomes to 38 children with language group (46%), aggression was significantly higher
impairment. Children in both groups were between for Angelman and Smith-Magenis syndromes.
4 and 14 years old. Groups were matched on age
and nonverbal IQ. Aggression occurred more fre-
quently in children with ASD (i.e., 33%) than in Risk Factors
those without ASD (i.e., 21%), but the difference
was not statistically significant. Over half of the Among 298 adults with ASD and severe to pro-
children had the onset of their aggressive behav- found ID who were living in a residential facility,
ior during the toddler years. However, the preva- Matson and Rivet (2008b) found that aggression/
lence of temper tantrums was significantly higher destruction (as measured by the ASD-BPA) was
in children with ASD (i.e., 71%) than in children not related to severity of ASD symptomatology.
with language impairment (i.e., 23%). A temper However, a closer look revealed that communica-
tantrum was operationally defined as crying, tion impairment was a significant predictor of
flailing, and yelling usually in response to some aggression/destruction.
aversive stimulus, such as change in activity. For In Kanne and Mazurek’s (2011) sample of
60% of these children, temper tantrums occurred children and youngsters (4–17 years old), a rela-
on a daily basis, and 20% of children with ASD tionship was found between aggressive behavior
had the onset of tantrums by 1 year of age. and age, and social and communication deficits.
Matson and Rivet (2008a) compared preva- The likelihood of aggression decreased with
lence rates of various topographies of challeng- increasing age, while it increased with ASD-
ing behavior (including aggression/destruction) related social and communication skill deficits.
36 R. Didden et al.

IQ and gender and severity of ASD symptoms and 52 years of age, and data were collected
were not related to aggression, while self-injurious through a range of questionnaires. Results showed
behavior, ritualistic behaviors, and resistance to that aggressive behavior was significantly more
change were predictive of aggression. common in Angelman syndrome and in Smith-
Hartley et al. (2008) showed that among 169 Magenis syndrome. Across all syndrome groups,
young children with autism, severity of aggres- impulsivity and overactivity were positively cor-
sive behavior was negatively correlated with related with aggression. Physical aggression was
nonverbal cognitive functioning, expressive lan- more likely in younger individuals with Cri du
guage, and adaptive skills. No associations were Chat, Fragile X, and Prader–Willi syndrome.
found between externalizing behaviors and age, Aggression was associated with being male in
gender, or severity of autistic symptoms. These Prader–Willi syndrome and with being of lower
results were largely corroborated by those from a ability in Prader–Willi and Cornelia de Lange
study by Dominick et al. (2007) who found that syndromes. The latter results imply that there
aggression in children with ASD was negatively may be syndrome-specific associations between
correlated (albeit moderately) with cognitive and physical aggression and certain risk factors.
language measures such as verbal and nonverbal
IQ, and level of receptive and expressive lan-
guage. By contrast, in Hartley et al.’s study, Adverse Consequences
severity of autistic symptoms was positively
associated with challenging behavior. Aggression Kanne and Mazurek (2011) have reviewed stud-
was also positively associated with self-injury ies on adverse consequences of aggression in
and temper tantrums. Other studies have not people with ID and/or ASD and concluded that
found such a relationship. For example, Matson, aggression is one of the strongest predictors of
Boisjoli, and Mahan (2009) investigated relation- crisis intervention re-referrals, admission to resi-
ships between challenging behavior and expres- dential facilities, and use of psychotropic medi-
sive and receptive communication skills in 168 cation. It may lead to increased levels of stress
toddlers who met DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) cri- among parents and out-of-home placements of
teria for ASD and who were enrolled in an early children and it is associated with an increased
intervention program. Data on ASD symptoms risk of physical abuse from caregivers. Aggression
and challenging behavior (e.g., aggression) were in children with ASD and ID is significantly
collected using the Baby and Infant Screen for associated with burnout and emotional exhaus-
Children with aUtism Traits (BISCUIT). A range tion among teachers and special education sup-
of developmental skills were measured using the port staff. In this way, this behavior has negative
Battelle Developmental Inventory-2nd edition effects on teachers’ instructional efforts, and it
(BDI-2). Results showed that in this group of tod- interferes with instruction. Related to this, Tyrer
dlers low levels of receptive and expressive com- et al. (2006) have found that almost half of carers
munication were associated with low levels of of adults with ID who engaged in aggression
aggression. These results suggest that communi- reported that they were unable to cope with the
cation deficits may not be a risk factor for aggres- aggressive behavior compared to 10% of those
sion in infants and toddlers with ASD. Differences caring for adults without aggression.
in sample characteristics and type of measure-
ment may also account for the discrepancies in
outcomes among the aforementioned studies. Feeding Disorders
Recent studies have shown that certain genetic
disorders may also be a risk factor for aggression. Definition
For example, Arron et al. (2011) have compared
prevalence rates of aggressive behavior (physical The APA (2000) defined Feeding Disorder of
aggression) among several syndrome groups and Infancy and Early Childhood (hereafter “Feeding
one control group. Participants were between 4 Disorders”) as
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 37

“ … persistent failure to eat adequately, as reflected meet this criterion, but still may have significant
in persistent failure to gain weight or significant behavior management issues regarding food
weight loss over at least one month (Criterion A)
… “ (p. 98) and this disorder is not better accounted selectivity or refusal.
for by other disorders such as general medical con-
ditions (Criterion B), another mental disorder,
including other disorders of eating such as Prevalence
Rumination Disorder or lack of food (Criterion C),
and the disorder must onset before the age of 6 Recently, research has paid more attention to
years (Criterion D.)
feeding disorders, especially in the context of
Feeding Disorders are difficult to diagnose for early intervention for young children with ASD.
several reasons. Food refusal lies along a contin- Williams, Seiverling, and Field (in press) pro-
uum in terms of both its severity and duration, vided a partial review of the epidemiology of food
and it may wax and wane in severity over time. refusal and concluded that the prevalence of food
The presence of medical problems, which is com- refusal was unclear. For example, Field, Garland,
mon, can also distract family members and pro- and Williams (2003) reported a prevalence of
fessionals away from psychological aspects of 34% of children with special needs rising to 69%
food refusal, which may coexist with relevant of children with gastrointestinal reflux. Similarly,
medical problems and which may or may not be Fodstad and Matson (2008) reported that 25% of
related to a Feeding disorder. Additionally, paren- adults with disabilities in an institutional setting
tal tolerance and societal norms for children’s have some form of a feeding disorder as assessed
eating behavior have probably changed dramati- by a psychometric screening measure. Both of
cally over time and vary tremendously over cul- these studies, however, were samples of conve-
tures. Thus, diagnosis of feeding disorders can be nience rather than random samples from some
problematic. population of interest, and there are simply too
few studies to make any strong conclusions
regarding the prevalence of feeding disorders.
Even less is known about the prevalence of
Forms and Classification
food selectivity (Williams & Seiverling, 2010).
Recently, however, Raspa, Bailey, Bishop,
Feeding disorders can take several forms. This
Holiday, and Olmsted (2010) surveyed a sample
can include food refusal and food selectivity. In
of 1,075 children with Fragile X syndrome, a
food refusal, a child may refuse all or many foods
condition associated with ASD symptoms.
such that the child does not consume sufficient
Questions related to food selectivity included
calories and has other nutritional deficiencies
items related to selectivity by texture, color,
and/or associated medical problems, such as mal-
smell, and type of food. Selectivity by texture
nutrition or chronic gastrointestinal feeding. Food
was most common and ranged from 63% of boys
selectivity refers to eating a very narrow range of
aged 6–10 years to 19% of girls aged 11–15
foods and/or being very sensitive to apparently
years. Food selectivity was generally less preva-
minor differences in foods presentation or other
lent in adults. For example, food selectivity was
food-related stimuli. For example, a child may
reported in only 39% of men and 19% of women.
eat only milk from a preferred bottle and cream
These results suggest that food selectivity is
cheese, but refuse to drink milk from any other
common in people with Fragile X syndrome;
container, refuse to consume similar foods such
however, the validity of the data, which were
as yogurt, and refuse to consume cream cheese
reported by family members, is unclear.
on a cracker. Such children might also be highly
sensitive to with whom and where they eat, refus-
ing to eat with certain caretakers or only eating a Adverse Consequences
limited range of foods in certain settings.
Although the APA (2000) requires failure to Food Refusal may be associated with various
gain weight or low weight, some children do not medical and social problems. These may include
38 R. Didden et al.

gastrointestinal reflux, esophagitis, vomiting and (i.e., food presented), while food selectivity may
regurgitation, painful or otherwise difficult swal- be maintained by access to preferred food items.
lowing and chronic gastrointestinal tube feeding, Some empirical evidence for risk factors
etc. It is difficult to disentangle cause and effect comes from Field et al. (2003) who reported on
here. Food refusal may begin after the occurrence risk factors for five feeding problems: food
of one or more painful or invasive medical or refusal, food selectivity by type, food selectivity
other procedures related to swallowing or food by texture, oral motor delays, or dysphagia in
consumption. Chronic gastrointestinal feeding children with autism, Down syndrome, and cere-
may begin as a feeding strategy but then contrib- bral palsy. Esophageal reflux was the most com-
ute to the maintenance of food refusal by failing mon problem and predicted food refusal, whereas
to provide opportunities to relearn how to eat or neuroanatomical abnormalities were associated
by providing an easy alternative to feeding a child with skill deficits. Although Raspa et al. (2010)
who is difficult to feed. reported age- and gender-related differences in
Food selectivity can be associated with a the prevalence of food selectivity in people with
number of adverse consequences. For example, Fragile X syndrome, these data do not shed any
food selectivity may make meal times difficult light on what the mechanisms might be that place
for families as meal consumption may take certain demographic groups at greater or lesser
extensive lengths of time to complete and family risk. Thus, although there is a limited quantity of
members may need to engage in a range of data on risk factors for feeding disorders, the data
behavior management strategies to persuade that are available suggest the notion of classical
their child to eat. Sometimes the family mem- conditioning and difficulties in acquisition of
bers’ behavior management strategies fail, pro- adaptive feeding responses as plausible mecha-
voking distressing and/or dangerous challenging nisms for explaining the onset of some Feeding
behavior and perhaps even maintaining food disorders.
refusal and inadvertently shaping even more
difficult challenging behavior. Sometimes, chil-
dren with high food selectivity also refuse to eat Pica, Vomiting, and Rumination
any new food for prolonged periods of time.
This can be highly distressing to family mem- Definition
bers and perhaps place the child at risk for medi-
cal problems. Pica is the ingestion of nonnutritive or inedible
objects (Matson, Belva, Hattier, & Matson, 2011).
Pica can be food-based (e.g., eating rotten food,
Risk Factors frozen food, or scavenging for food in the trash)
or nonfood-based (e.g., ingesting cigarette butts,
Due to the limited epidemiology for food refusal feces, hair, or paint). The individual who engages
and selectivity, it is difficult to identify risk fac- in pica seeks out inedible objects in their environ-
tors with certainty. Children who display food ment, places the object in their mouth, and then
refusal and selectivity often present with a his- either chews the object or immediately swallows
tory of medical problems that could function as it. Some people will engage in pica with several
classical and operant conditioning events that different types of objects, and other people will
might result in the onset and maintenance of food only ingest one specific type of object (e.g., only
refusal (Williams, Seiverling et al., 2010). It is cigarette butts) (Matson et al., 2011). When an
easy to speculate as to how parental management individual ingests only a specific type of non-
strategies might inadvertently shape food refusal, edible material, a more specific term may be used
especially in the case of children who have weak to describe the pica (Stiegler, 2005). In order to
or absent adaptive feeding behavior. The function diagnose pica as a stand-alone disorder, the
of food refusal may be that of escaping demands behavior (a) must persist for longer than 1 month,
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 39

(b) must be developmentally inappropriate, and prevalence of pica among individuals with ID liv-
(c) cannot be part of cultural or religious practice ing in the community to be 0.3%. This large dis-
(APA [DSM-IV-TR], 2000). However, pica is not crepancy in prevalence data may be due to
typically diagnosed separately in individuals with different estimation methods (e.g., direct obser-
autism due to the prevalence of other problem vation vs. review of patient records) and the
behaviors within the autism population. different definitions of pica used across the prev-
Rumination is the deliberate regurgitation, alence studies (Ali, 2001; Danford & Huber,
chewing, and swallowing of stomach contents, 1982). For example, although all definitions of
and vomiting (or emesis) is the expulsion of pica have included the ingestion of inedible
regurgitated stomach contents from the mouth objects, some studies have also included compul-
(Lang et al., 2011; Starin & Fuqua, 1987). A com- sive eating of food (e.g., Danford & Huber, 1982),
bination of these regurgitation behaviors may be mouthing but not swallowing of objects (e.g.,
called ruminative vomiting (e.g., Mulick, McAlpine & Singh, 1986), and eating frozen
Schroeder, & Rojahn, 1980). Individuals who foods that should be heated (e.g., Tewari,
engage in ruminative vomiting often obtain Krishnan, Valsalan, & Ashok, 1995). Danford
access to vomitus by tilting back their head, arch- and Huber compared case records to direct obser-
ing their torso, and creating suction with their vations and found that pica was often not reported
tongue on the roof of the mouth or by thrusting in patients’ records. Therefore, studies relying
their fingers down their throat (Tierney & Jackson, solely on reviews of case records may underesti-
1984). In order for these regurgitation behaviors mate prevalence (Danford & Huber, 1982).
to be considered ruminative vomiting, the regur- Data on the prevalence of ruminative vomit-
gitation cannot be due to physiological issues, ing among individuals with autism is sparse and
such as illness, drug side effects, or gastroesoph- no large-scale studies have been published (Lang
ageal abnormality (APA, 2000). Like pica, rumi- et al., 2011). However, the available data sug-
nation may be diagnosed as a stand-alone gests that approximately 5–10% of individuals
disorder. However, this diagnosis is not typically with ID may engage in these behaviors (Danford
made with individuals with autism due to the & Huber, 1981; Gravestock, 2000; Tewari et al.,
common association between the behavior and 1995). One potential reason for the lack of preva-
autism (i.e., other problem behaviors). lence data for ruminative vomiting may be due to
difficulties distinguishing between regurgitation
caused by medical issues and psychogenic or
Prevalence operant regurgitation (i.e., regurgitation main-
tained by automatic and social reinforcement
Estimates of the prevalence of pica among people contingencies) (Lang et al., 2011). For example,
with autism are rare (Myles, Simpson, & Hirsch, Rogers, Stratton, Victor, Kennedy, and Andres
1997). However, Kinnell (1985) found that 60% (1992) found gastroesophageal abnormalities
of a sample of 70 people with severe autism that could cause chronic regurgitation in 91% of
engaged in pica. Several studies have estimated the people identified by hospital staff as engaging
the prevalence of pica among groups of people in ruminative vomiting.
with ID, without, however, specifying how many
of those people also had autism (c.f., Ashworth,
Martin, & Hirdes, 2008). In these studies, preva- Adverse Consequences
lence estimates of pica among people living in
institutions ranged from 9.2 to 25.8% (Matson, Pica is associated with a variety of serious health
Sipes et al., 2011). Studies providing estimations risks including vomiting, malnutrition, poisoning,
of prevalence outside institutional settings are anemia, parasitic infection, gastroesophageal
rare. Rojahn (1986) conducted a postal survey of trauma, and death (e.g., Ali, 2001; Ashworth et al.,
over 25,000 people in Germany and estimated the 2008; Decker, 1993; Matson, Sipes, et al., 2011;
40 R. Didden et al.

Matson & Bamburg, 1999; Stiegler, 2005). Matson, Sipes et al., 2011; McAlpine & Singh,
Decker (1993) reviewed the treatment history of 1986). Pica also appears to be more common in
35 patients that engaged in pica at a community individuals with autism than in individuals with
hospital over a period of 15 years and found that other developmental disabilities (Ashworth et al.,
75% of the patients had required surgery. For 2008; Myles et al., 1997; Stiegler, 2005). For
these individuals, the complication rate for sur- example, Kinnell (1985) compared an autism
gery was 30%, and the death rate was 11% group to a Down syndrome group and found pica
(Decker, 1993). Even in cases in which behav- in 60% of the autism group and only 4% of the
ioral treatments are utilized, the ingestion of a Down syndrome group. Several studies have
single object (e.g., a safety pin) may result in reported that pica appears to decrease with age
life-threatening complications (e.g., Falcomata, (e.g., Ali, 2001), and other studies have reported
Roane, & Pabico, 2007). For some people with that pica persists over time (e.g., Matson &
autism, pica is also associated with aggressive Bamburg, 1999). One reason for the reduced rates
behavior that occurs when their attempts to obtain of pica in older groups of people with ID may be
specific inedible objects for the purpose of inges- the shorter life expectancy of individuals with
tion are impeded (Danford & Huber, 1982; Matson profound ID (Danford & Huber, 1981).
& Bamburg, 1999). However, other people with Ruminative vomiting can occur in typically
autism who engage in pica are not aggressive and developing children but is more common among
may be notably withdrawn and submissive (Tewari people with autism and severe to profound ID
et al., 1995). (Gravestock, 2000; Tierney & Jackson, 1984).
Chronic ruminative vomiting may lead to seri- When rumination occurs in typically developing
ous health risks including malnutrition, weight children, it often begins and ends early in life;
loss, dehydration, increased susceptibility to dis- however, in individuals with autism and ID, it
ease, tooth decay, choking, gastrointestinal bleed- often begins later in life and persists over time
ing, and death (Fredericks, Carr, & Williams, (Winton & Singh, 1983). For people with autism,
1998; Singh, 1981; Starin & Fuqua, 1987). the risk of developing ruminative vomiting may
Rumination is the primary cause of death in 5 to be equal across ages and level of intellectual
10% of people who ruminate (Fredericks et al., functioning (Parry-Jones, 1994; Rastam, 2008).
1998). In addition to adverse health effects, vom- Lang et al. (2011) reviewed the intervention
iting and rumination may also exacerbate the research targeting rumination and operant vomit-
social deficits associated with autism. For exam- ing and found that many of the participants
ple, frequent contact with vomitus may hinder a receiving treatment had a comorbid visual impair-
person’s appearance and cause foul odors leading ment suggesting that visual impairment may be a
to social isolation and reduced educational or risk factor. Johnston and Greene (1992) reviewed
vocational opportunities (Starin & Fuqua, 1987). data on 10 patients across 10 years and found that
the quantity of food consumed during meals is
related to frequency of rumination. Interventions
Risk Factors in which more food is provided to people who
ruminate often result in reductions in ruminative
Dementia, pregnancy, and sickle cell anemia are vomiting (Lang et al., 2011).
risk factors for the development of pica in indi-
viduals with and without ID (Ali, 2001; Ashworth
et al., 2008). For individuals with autism, pica Conclusion
appears to be more common among individuals
with the most severe to profound ID, suggesting In this chapter, we have provided a selective
that pica is more common in individuals with review of studies on the nature, prevalence, and
autism than in people with Asperger’s syndrome characteristics of challenging behaviors in indi-
(Ashworth et al., 2008; Danford & Huber, 1982; viduals with ASD and/or ID. A relatively large
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 41

number of studies have been published on the self-injurious and aggressive behaviour in genetic
prevalence and associated factors of challenging syndromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,
55, 109–120.
behavior in children and adults with ASD and/or Ashworth, M., Martin, L., & Hirdes, J. P. (2008).
ID. Overall, these rates may vary as a result of Prevalence and correlates of pica among adults with
differences in operational definitions, ASD diag- intellectual disability in institutions. Journal of Mental
nosis, measurement instrument, and sample char- Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 1,
176–190.
acteristics. Despite this, it is now well established Baghdadli, A., Pascal, C., Grisi, S., & Aussilloux, C.
that individuals with ASD and/or ID are at risk (2003). Risk factors for self-injurious behaviors among
for various types of challenging behavior and 222 children with autistic disorders. Journal of
that these behaviors add to the “severity” of ASD Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 622–627.
Bienstein, P., & Nussbeck, S. (2009). Reliability of a
condition (Matson & Rivet, 2008b). German version of the Questions About Behavioral
An increasing number of studies have Function (QABF) Scale for self-injurious behavior in
identified risk factors for challenging behavior in individuals with intellectual disabilities. Journal of
individuals with ASD and/or ID. Risk factors Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2,
249–260.
may be biological, for example, in case the chal- Bodfish, J. W., Crawford, T. W., Powell, S. B., Parker, D.
lenging behavior is part of the behavioral pheno- E., Golden, R. N., & Lewis, M. H. (1995). Compulsion
type of the genetic disorder. It should be noted, in adults with mental retardation: Prevalence, phe-
however, that results of studies on risk factors are nomenology, and comorbidity with stereotypy and
self-injury. American Journal on Mental Retardation,
correlational and it is often unclear to what extent 100, 183–192.
a risk factor is also a cause of challenging Danford, D. E., & Huber, A. M. (1981). Eating dysfunc-
behavior (Matson, Kozlowski et al., 2011). For tions in an institutionalized mentally retarded popula-
example, several studies have shown that also tion. Appetite, 2, 281–292.
Danford, D. E., & Huber, A. M. (1982). Pica among men-
phenotypic behaviors may be influenced by envi- tally retarded adults. American Journal on Intellectual
ronmental events and that such behaviors may be and Developmental Disabilities, 87, 141–146.
reduced by procedures based on functional anal- Deb, S. (1998). Self-injurious behaviour as part of genetic
ysis (see, e.g., Radstaake, Didden, Oliver, Allen syndromes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172,
385–388.
& Curfs, in press). Other risk factors are related Decker, C. J. (1993). Pica in the mentally handicapped:
to skill deficits, such as deficits in the area of A 15 year surgical perspective. Canadian Journal of
communication and adaptive skills that may be Surgery, 36, 551–554.
targeted for intervention. For example, training Dib, N., & Sturmey, P. (2007). Reducing student stereo-
typy by improving teachers’ implementation of dis-
of cognitive and adaptive skills has shown to crete-trial teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior
result in improved functioning in children with Analysis, 40, 339–343.
ASD and/or ID (see results of a recent meta-anal- Dominick, K., Ornstein Davis, N., Lainhart, J., Tager-
ysis by Peters-Scheffer, Didden, Korzilius, & Flusberg, H., & Folstein, S. (2007). Atypical behav-
iors in children with autism and children with a history
Sturmey, 2011). of language impairment. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 28, 145–162.
Emerson, E. (2005). Challenging behaviour: Analysis and
intervention in people with severe intellectual disabili-
References ties. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Emerson, E., Kiernan, C., Alborz, A., Reeves, D., Mason,
Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2000). Child Behavior H., & Swarbrick, R., et al. (2001). The prevalence of
Checklist. Burlington: ASEBA. challenging behaviors: A total population study.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22, 77–93.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (Revised) Falcomata, T., Roane, H., & Pabico, R. (2007).
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Unintentional stimulus control during the treatment of
Ali, Z. (2001). Pica in people with intellectual disability: pica displayed by a young man with autism. Research
A literature review of aetiology, epidemiology and in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 350–359.
complications. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Feeney, B. J., & Stephenson,
Disability, 26, 205–215. K. M. (2010). Assessment and treatment of elopement
Arron, K., Oliver, C., Moss, J., Berg, K., & Burbidge, C. maintained by access to stereotypy. Journal of Applied
(2011). The prevalence and phenomenology of Behavior Analysis, 43, 513–517.
42 R. Didden et al.

Farmer, C., & Aman, M. (2011). Aggressive behavior in Kinnell, H. G. (1985). Pica as a feature of autism. British
a sample of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 80–82.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, Klonsky, E. D., & Meuhlenkamp, J. J. (2007). Self-injury:
317–323. A research review for the practitioner. Journal of
Fee, V. E., & Matson, J. L. (1992). Definition, classification, Clinical Psychology: In Session, 63, 1045–1056.
and taxonomy. In J. K. Luiselli, J. L. Matson, & N. N. Lang, R., Mulloy, A., Giesbers, S., Pfeiffer, B., Delaune,
Singh (Eds.), Self-injurious behavior: Analysis, E., Didden, R., et al. (2011). Behavioral interventions
assessment, and treatment (pp. 3–20). New York: for rumination and operant vomiting in individuals
Springer. with intellectual disabilities: A systematic review.
Field, D., Garland, M., & Williams, K. (2003). Correlates Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32,
of specific childhood feeding problems. Journal of 2193–2205.
Pediatrics and Child Health, 39, 299–304. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecoutteur, A. (1994). Autism
Fodstad, J. C., & Matson, J. L. (2008). A comparison of Diagnostic Interview Revised: A revised version of a
feeding and mealtime problems in adults with intel- diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with
lectual disabilities with and without autism. Journal of possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 20, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24,
541–550. 659–683.
Fredericks, D. W., Carr, J. E., & Williams, W. L. (1998). Lowe, K., Allen, D., Jones, E., Brophy, S., Moore, K., &
Overview of the treatment of rumination disorder for James, W. (2007). Challenging behaviours: Prevalence
adults in a residential setting. Journal of Behavior and topographies. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 29, 31–40. Research, 51, 625–636.
Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M., Greene, P., Kim, M., Mandell, D. S. (2008). Psychiatric hospitalization among
& Rapin, I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in children children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
with autism and other developmental disorders. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1059–1065.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51, Matson, J. L., & Bamburg, J. W. (1999). A descriptive
30–38. study of pica in persons with mental retardation.
Gravestock, S. (2000). Eating disorders in adults with Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability 11, 353–361.
Research, 44, 625–637. Matson, J. L., Belva, B., Hattier, M. A., & Matson, M. L.
Guess, D., & Carr, E. (1991). Emergence and maintenance (2011). Pica in persons with developmental disabili-
of stereotypy and self-injury. American Journal on ties: Characteristics, diagnosis, and assessment.
Mental Retardation, 96, 299–319. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
Hall, S. S., Lighthouse, A. A., & Reiss, A. L. (2008). 1459–1464.
Compulsive, self-injurious, and autistic behavior in Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., & Mahan, S. (2009). The rela-
children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome. tion of communication and challenging behaviors in
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 113, infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.
44–53. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
Hartley, S., Sikora, D., & McCoy, R. (2008). Prevalence 21, 253–261.
and risk factors of maladaptive behaviour in young Matson, J. L., Fostad, J. C., Mahan, S., & Rojahn, J.
children with autistic disorder. Journal of Intellectual (2010). Cut-offs, norms and patterns of problem
Disability Research, 52, 819–829. behaviours in children with developmental disabilities
Holden, B., & Gitlesen, J. P. (2006). A total population on the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with
study of challenging behavior in the county of autism (BISCUIT-Part 3). Developmental
Hedmark, Norway: Prevalence and risk markers. Neurorehabilitation, 13, 3–9.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 456–465. Matson, J. L., Kozlowski, A. M., Worley, J. A., Shoemaker,
Iwata, B., Pace, G., Dorsey, M., Zarcone, J., Vollmer, T., M. E., Sipes, M., & Horowitz, M. (2011). What is the
Smith, R., et al. (1994). The functions of self-injurious evidence for environmental causes of challenging
behavior: An experimental-epidemiological analysis. behaviors in persons with intellectual disabilities and
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240. autism spectrum disorders? Research in Developmental
Johnston, J. M., & Greene, K. S. (1992). Relation between Disabilities, 32, 693–698.
ruminating and quantity of food consumed. Mental Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2008). A review of behav-
Retardation, 30, 7–11. ioral treatments for self-injurious behaviors of persons
Jones, R. S. (1987). The relationship between stereotyped with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Modification,
and self-injurious behaviour. British Journal of 32, 61–76.
Medical Psychology, 60, 287–289. Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008a). Characteristics of
Kanne, S., & Mazurek, M. (2011). Aggression in children challenging behaviours in adults with autistic disorder,
and adolescents with ASD: Prevalence and risk fac- PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal of
tors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 33,
41, 926–937. 323–329.
3 Nature, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Challenging Behavior 43

Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008b). The effects of severity A total population study. Journal of Mental Deficiency
of autism and PDD-NOS symptoms on challenging Research, 31, 147–162.
behaviors in adults with intellectual disabilities. Oliver, C., Petty, J., Ruddick, L., & Bacarese-Hamilton,
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, M. (2011). The association between repetitive, self-inju-
20, 41–51. rious and aggressive behavior in children with severe
Matson, J. L., Sipes, M., Horovitz, M., Worley, S., intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and
Shoemaker, M., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). Behaviors Developmental Disorders [Epub ahead of print].
and corresponding functions addressed via functional Parry-Jones, B. (1994). Merycism or rumination disorder:
assessment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, A historical investigation and current assessment.
32, 625–629. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 303–314.
Matson, J. L., Terlonge, C., & Gonzáles, M. L. (2006). Percy, M., Cheetham, T., Gitta, M., Morrison, B.,
Autism Spectrum Disorders—Diagnosis—Adult Machalek, K., Bega, S., et al. (2007). Other syndromes
Version. Baton Rouge: Disability Consultants. and disorders associated with intellectual and devel-
McAlpine, C., & Singh, N. N. (1986). Pica in institution- opmental disabilities. In I. Brown & M. Percy (Eds.),
alized mentally retarded persons. Journal of Mental A comprehensive guide to intellectual and develop-
Deficiency Research, 30, 171–178. mental disabilities (pp. 229–267). Baltimore: Paul H.
McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark- Brookes.
ers associated with challenging behaviours in people Peters-Scheffer, N., Didden, R., Korzilius, H., & Sturmey,
with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study. P. (2011). A meta-analytic study on the effectiveness
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, of comprehensive ABA-based early intervention pro-
405–416. grams for children with autism spectrum disorders.
Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., Chaaban, J., Zimmerman- Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 60–69.
Bier, B., & Wagner, G. (2008). Autism spectrum dis- Radstaake, M., Didden, R., Oliver, C., Allen, D., & Curfs,
orders: Concurrent clinical disorders. Journal of Child L. (In press). Functional analysis and functional com-
Neurology, 23, 6–13. munication training in individuals with Angelman
Mulick, J. A., Schroeder, S. R., & Rojahn, J. (1980). syndrome. Developmental Neurorehabilitation.
Chronic ruminative vomiting: A comparison of four Raspa, M., Bailey, D. B., Bishop, E., Holiday, D., &
treatment procedures. Journal of Autism and Olmsted, M. (2010). Obesity, food selectivity, and
Developmental Disorders, 10, 203–213. physical activity in individuals with fragile-X
Murphy, G. H., Oliver, C., Corbett, J., Crayton, L., Hales, Syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation,
J., Head, D., et al. (1993). Epidemiology of self-injury, 115, 482–495.
characteristics of people with severe self-injury and Rastam, M. (2008). Eating disturbances in autism spec-
initial treatment outcome. In C. Kiernan (Ed.), trum disorders with focus on adolescent and adult
Research to practice? Implications of research on years. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 5, 31–42.
challenging behaviour of people with learning disabil- Rogers, B., Stratton, P., Victor, J., Kennedy, B., & Andres,
ity (pp. 1–35). Avon: British Institute of Learning M. (1992). Chronic regurgitation among persons with
Disabilities. mental retardation: A need for combined medical and
Murphy, G., Hall, S., Oliver, C., & Kissi-Debra, R. (1999). interdisciplinary strategies. American Journal on
Identification of early self-injurious behaviour in Mental Retardation, 96, 522–527.
young children with intellectual disability. Journal of Rojahn, J. (1986). Self-injurious stereotypic behavior of
Intellectual Disability Research, 43, 149–163. noninstitutionalized mentally retarded people:
Murphy, O., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2009). Risk factors Prevalence and classification. American Journal of
for challenging behaviors among 157 children with mental Deficiency, 91, 268–276.
autism spectrum disorders in Ireland. Research in Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Lott, D., Esbensen, A. J., &
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 474–482. Smalls, Y. (2001). The Behavior Problems Inventory:
Myles, B. S., Simpson, R. L., & Hirsch, N. C. (1997). An instrument for the assessment of self-injury, ste-
A review of literature on interventions to reduce pica reotyped behavior and aggression/destruction in indi-
in individuals with developmental disabilities. Autism, viduals with developmental disabilities. Journal of
1, 77–95. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 577–588.
National Institutes of Health. (1989). Treatment of destruc- Rojahn, J., Whittaker, K., Hoch, T. A., & Gonzáles, M. L.
tive behaviors in persons with developmental disabili- (2007). Assessment of self-injurious and aggressive
ties (Consensus Development Conference Statement, behavior. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Handbook of assess-
Vol. 7, No. 9). Bethesda: Author. ment in persons with intellectual disability (pp. 281–
O’Reilly, M. F., Rispoli, M., Davis, T., Machalicek, W., 319). San Diego: Academic.
Lang, R., Sigafoos, J., et al. (2010). Functional analy- Sallustro, F., & Atwell, C. W. (1978). Body rocking, head
sis of challenging behavior in children with autism banging, and head rolling in normal children. The
spectrum disorder: A summary of 10 cases. Research Journal of Pediatrics, 93, 704–708.
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 1–10. Schloper, E., Reichler, R. J., & Rochen Renner, B. (1988).
Oliver, C., Murphy, G. H., & Corbett, J. A. (1987). Self- The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Los Angeles:
injurious behaviour in people with mental handicap: Western Psychological Services.
44 R. Didden et al.

Schroeder, S. R., Mulick, J. A., & Rojahn, J. (1980). The cal survey. British Journal of Developmental
definition, taxonomy, epidemiology and ecology of Disabilities, 41, 13–22.
self-injurious behavior. Journal of Autism and Thelen, E. (1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An
Developmental Disorders, 10, 417–432. ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology,
Schroeder, S. R., Tessel, R. E., Loupe, P. S., & Stodgell, 17, 237–257.
C. J. (1997). Severe behavior problems among people Tierney, D., & Jackson, H. J. (1984). Psychosocial treat-
with developmental disabilities. In W. E. MacLean Jr. ments of rumination disorder: A review of the litera-
(Ed.), Ellis’ handbook of mental deficiency, psycho- ture. Australia and New Zealand Journal of
logical theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 439–464). Developmental Disabilities, 10, 81–112.
Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Tyrer, F., McGrother, C., Thorp, C., Donaldson, M.,
Sigafoos, J., Arthur, M., & O’Reilly, M. (2003). Bhaumik, S., Watson, J., et al. (2006). Physical aggres-
Challenging behavior and developmental disability. sion towards others in adults with learning disabilities:
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Prevalence and associated factors. Journal of
Sigafoos, J., Reichle, J., & Light Shriner, C. (1994). Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 295–304.
Distinguishing between socially and nonsocially moti- Van Ingen, D. J., Moore, L. L., Zaja, R. H., & Rojahn, J.
vated challenging behavior: Implications for the selec- (2010). The Behavior Problems Inventory (BPI-01)
tion of intervention strategies. In M. F. Hayden & B. in community-based adults with intellectual disabili-
H. Abery (Eds.), Challenges for a service system in ties: Reliability and concurrent validity vis-à-vis the
transition: Ensuring quality community experiences Inventory for Client and Agency Planning (ICAP).
for persons with developmental disabilities (pp. 147– Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31,
169). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. 97–107.
Singh, N. N. (1981). Rumination. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Vitiello, B., & Stoff, D. M. (1997). Subtypes of aggres-
International review of research in mental retardation sion and their relevance to child psychiatry. Journal of
(Vol. 10, pp. 139–182). New York: Academic. the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Smith, R. T., Vollmer, T. R., & St. Peter Pipkin, C. (2007). Psychiatry, 36, 307–315.
Functional approaches to assessment and treatment of Weiss, J. (2003). Self-injurious behaviours in autism: A
problem behavior in persons with autism and related literature review. Journal on Developmental
disabilities. In P. Sturmey & A. Fitzer (Eds.), Autism Disabilities, 9, 129–143.
spectrum disorders: Applied behavior analysis, evi- Williams, K. E., & Seiverling, L. J. (2010). Eating prob-
dence, and practice (pp. 187–234). Austin: Pro-Ed. lems in children with autism spectrum disorders.
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Topics in Clinical Nutrition, 25, 27–37.
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Interviewth ed.). Williams, K.E., Seiverling, L.J., & Field, D.G. (in press).
Circle Pines: American Guidance Service. Feeding problems. In P. Sturmey & R. Didden (Eds.),
Starin, S. P., & Fuqua, W. (1987). Rumination and vomit- Evidence-base practice in intellectual disabilities.
ing in the developmentally disabled: A critical review Chichester: Wiley UK.
of the behavioral, medical, and psychiatric treatment Winchel, R. M., & Stanley, M. (1991). Self-injurious
research. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 8, behavior: A review of the behavior and biology of self-
575–605. mutilation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,
Stiegler, L. N. (2005). Understanding pica behavior: A 306–317.
review for clinical and educational professionals. Winton, A., & Singh, N. (1983). Rumination in pediatric
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, populations: A behavioral analysis. Journal of the
20, 27–38. American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. (1966). Aversive control of Psychiatry, 22, 269–275.
self-injurious behavior in a psychotic boy. Behavior World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10
Research and Therapy, 4, 281–287. classification of mental and behavioural disorders:
Tewari, S., Krishnan, V. H. R., Valsalan, V. C., & Ashok, Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines.
R. (1995). Pica in learning disability hospital: A criti- Geneva: Author.
Function of Challenging Behaviors
4
Giulio E. Lancioni, Nirbhay N. Singh, Mark F. O’Reilly,
Jeff Sigafoos, and Robert Didden

McLaughlin et al., 2003; Stewart & Alderman,


Introduction 2010; Wood, Blair, & Ferro, 2009). In particular,
challenging behaviors include a variety of
Challenging behavior is a label/definition self-injurious stereotypies (e.g., hitting, scratch-
normally used to identify a variety of perfor- ing, biting one’s own body parts), other appar-
mance expressions that (a) can be dangerous to ently nonself-injurious stereotypies (e.g., flapping
the person’s physical safety or to the safety of own hands, body rocking, and repeated verbal-
others sharing the person’s context, and/or (b) izations), aggression toward others, and property
can seriously interfere with the person’s access to destruction (Lancioni, Singh, O’Reilly, &
the typical community facilities (Carr, Dozier, Sigafoos, 2009; Matson & LoVullo, 2008;
Patel, Adams, & Martin, 2002; Emerson, 1995; Neidert, Dozier, Iwata, & Hafen, 2010; Stokes &
Luiselli, 2009; Watson & Watson, 2009; Wilder,
Kellum, & Carr, 2000).
G.E. Lancioni (*) Challenging behaviors are highly likely
Department of Psychology, University of Bari,
among persons with general developmental dis-
Via QuintinoSella 268, Bary 70100, Italy
e-mail: g.lancioni@psico.uniba.it orders, autism, severe and profound intellectual
disabilities, and multiple disabilities (Kurtz
N.N. Singh
American Health and Wellness Institute, et al., 2003; Lancioni et al., 2009). The first con-
P.O. Box 80466, Raleigh, NC 27623, USA sideration about challenging behaviors is that
e-mail: nirbsingh52@aol.com they have a definitely negative impact on the
M.F. O’Reilly person’s developmental and adaptive opportuni-
Department of Special Education, College of Education, ties and on his or her context and therefore their
The University of Texas at Austin, Austin,
occurrence needs to be reduced through appro-
TX 78712, USA
e-mail: markoreilly@mail.utexas.edu priate intervention strategies (Singh et al., 2009;
Tarbox et al., 2009). The second consideration is
J. Sigafoos
School of Educational Psychology and Pedagogy, that challenging behaviors may have their emer-
Victoria University of Wellington, Karori Campus, gence and continuous occurrence justified by
PO Box 17-310, Wellington, New Zealand the functions (consequences) that they have for
e-mail: jeff.sigafoos@vuw.ac.nz
the person. In other words, they may have an
R. Didden important role for the person (e.g., in terms of
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University
stimulation and reinforcement), irrespective of
Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9104, Nijmegen 6500 HE,
The Netherlands whether an external observer could attribute
e-mail: r.didden@pwo.ru.nl them an immediate/obvious meaning or not

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 45


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_4,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
46 G.E. Lancioni et al.

(LaBelle & Charlop-Christy, 2002; Najdowski, more likely to occur (e.g., in situations in which
Wallace, Ellsworth, MacAleese, & Cleveland, the person is alone, in difficult task situations, in
2008; Noell & Gansle, 2009). The third consider- situations where attention is diverted or when
ation is that any attempt to intervene to reduce the environmental events not scheduled are likely to
occurrence of challenging behaviors should rely occur). In essence, the scale is provided with
on (a) an understanding of the possible functions many of the questions that functional analysis
of such behaviors and (b) intervention strategies procedures pose and attempt to resolve through
that are matched to those functions (Matson & manipulation of the situations. Through those
Minshawi, 2007; Neidert et al., 2010; Singh et al., questions, the scale attempts to evaluate four dif-
2009; Tarbox et al., 2009). ferent functions as potentially responsible for the
These three considerations have over time challenging behavior, that is, access to specific
become a widely agreed way of thinking and the items/activities, attention, escape from demands,
common approach is that an assessment of the per- and sensory or automatic consequences.
son’s challenging behavior is a preliminary condi- The QABF was originally conceived as a
tion for the development of the intervention 25-item questionnaire and evaluated over a large
strategy. Assessment can occur in different ways. number of participants with highly encouraging
The three most common forms may involve the results in terms of its ability to identify the func-
use of rating scales, observational descriptive strat- tion of the challenging behavior for a large per-
egies, and functional analysis procedures (Borrero centage of participants (Matson, Bamburg, Cherry,
& Borrero, 2008; Harvey, Luiselli, & Wong, 2009; & Paclawskyj, 1999; Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush,
Kates-McElrath, Agnew, Axelrod, & Bloh, 2007; Smalls, & Vollmer, 2000). Recently, research has
Pence, Roscoe, Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009). shown that a 15-item version may preserve the
same potential as the longer version (Singh et al.,
2009). One possible difference of this scale com-
Assessment Strategies pared to the MAS is that the questions are even
more explicit as to the target of their inquiry (e.g.,
Rating Scales engages in the behavior to get attention, engages
in the behavior as a form of self-stimulation, and
The use of rating scales relies on the notion that engages in the behavior when he/she does not
staff/caregivers, who know the person with chal- want to do something). Through those questions,
lenging behavior, can provide relevant informa- the scale attempts to evaluate five different func-
tion when faced with structured questions about tions, which add physical discomfort to those
the behavior and its possible functions within a already contemplated in the MAS.
reasonably short amount of time (Carter, Devlin,
Doggett, Harber, & Barr, 2004; Singh et al., 2009;
Watson & Watson, 2009). Two of the rating scales Descriptive Methods
may be considered particularly useful and practi-
cal, that is, the Motivation Assessment Scale The use of observational, descriptive strategies
(MAS) (Durand & Crimmins, 1988) and the involves direct observation and recording of the
Questions about Behavioral Function (QABF) challenging behavior as well as of the environ-
(Matson et al., 2005; Matson & Wilkins, 2009; mental variables that might be relevant for such
Singh et al., 2009). The MAS includes 16 ques- behavior. The most common form of descriptive
tions, through which it attempts to identify the strategy consists of recording the antecedent
functions of the person’s challenging behavior. event and the consequence for each occurrence of
The specificity of the questions with the clear the target behavior (Noell & Gansle, 2009; Tarbox
definition of the challenging behavior are sup- et al., 2009). This strategy, which is also known
posed to increase the probability of providing as the ABC (antecedent–behavior–consequence)
useful information as to when the behavior is approach, requires the person’s behavior to be
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 47

recorded in combination with what occurs order to identify or exclude functional relations
immediately before its emission and what follows between the behavior and the specific environ-
it. For example, one may record that a student’s mental variables being manipulated. The stan-
screaming and self-hitting behavior in the class- dard functional analysis approach described by
room is generally preceded by the teacher asking Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman
him or her specific (task-related) questions and (1982/1994) involves four specific assessment
followed by the teacher’s silence or gentle physi- conditions across which the challenging behavior
cal interaction directed at calming the student. In is measured and compared, that is, attention,
that case, one could hypothesize that the student’s demand, play, and alone. These conditions are
challenging behavior has the function of avoiding presented several times non-consecutively,
task-related questions and possibly obtaining according to a multielement baseline design. The
attention and physical contact (Borrero & Borrero, attention condition involves the use of some
2008; Harvey et al., 2009; Pence et al., 2009). forms of reprimand or concern (e.g., Don’t do
Another direct observation strategy involves that or you should not hurt yourself) delivered
the use of time charts, the most popular of which contingent on the occurrence of the challenging
is the scatter plot (Noell & Gansle, 2009; behavior. The demand condition involves the pre-
Touchette, MacDonald, & Langer, 1985). The sentation of fairly difficult tasks (e.g., educational
first step in this type of strategies is to develop a or occupational tasks, such as coloring or arrang-
grid that divides the time of observation into ing objects, that are considered demanding for
specific periods (e.g., half hours, quarter hours or the participant) or of questions waiting to be
any other time unit that might seem appropriate). answered. The task or questions would be
Each time unit is then filled with the frequencies momentarily interrupted/halted by the occurrence
of the behavior under observation. After several of the challenging behavior. The play condition
observation days, a pattern may emerge with the involves the availability of preferred material and
problem behavior concentrated in certain periods activities and the positive interaction of the thera-
of the day and virtually absent in others. The dif- pist in charge of the session, who delivers atten-
ferences in the occurrence of the behavior might tion with overall regularity except when the
then be correlated to the presence or absence of challenging behavior occurs. The alone condition
certain staff/caregivers, the presence or absence involves the request that the participant stays in a
of certain activities, the presence or absence of room with no stimuli or persons present.
task-related demands, the presence or absence of To establish whether attention is playing a role
certain groups of stimuli and reinforcers, as well in maintaining the challenging behavior, the data
as the presence or absence of other recognizable obtained in this condition are compared with the
variables. This type of picture/evidence may not data resulting from the play and alone conditions.
be sufficient to identify the function of the behav- Relatively high levels of challenging behavior
ior and its controlling variables. Nonetheless, it during the attention condition and low levels of
may facilitate the efforts to discover the relation- the behavior during the latter two conditions
ship between the challenging behavior and one or would strongly suggest that the behavior is
more environmental variables. The manipulation directed at obtaining environmental attention
of some of those variables may then help reduce (i.e., social positive reinforcement). Relatively
or eliminate the problem (Touchette et al., 1985). high levels of challenging behavior in the demand
condition and lower levels in the other conditions
would suggest that the behavior is directed at
Functional Analysis escaping difficult demand situations (i.e., tasks or
questions), thus obtaining negative reinforce-
The functional analysis is an experimental method ment. Relatively high levels of challenging
that involves the manipulation of antecedents and behavior during the alone situation, as compared
consequences of the challenging behavior in to the other situations, may suggest that the
48 G.E. Lancioni et al.

behavior provides a form of reinforcement not number of studies have reported additional
mediated by environmental variables (i.e., auto- conditions compared to those included in the
matic reinforcement). classical model. For example, a number of stud-
ies have added the tangible condition, that is, a
condition within which a toy or a food item is
Functional Analysis and Possible visible but out of reach and its delivery is arranged
(Desirable or Necessary) Adaptations contingent on the occurrence of the challenging
behavior (e.g., Asmus, Franzese, Conroy, &
There is broad consensus as to the fact that func- Dozier, 2003; Kuhn, Hardesty, & Sweeney, 2009;
tional analysis represents the best, more powerful Kurtz, Chin, Rush, & Dixon, 2008; Lancioni,
approach to determine the functions of challeng- Walraven, O’Reilly, & Singh, 2002; Tarbox et al.,
ing behaviors. Indeed, most of the functional 2009). Some studies have divided attention into
analysis studies published in scientific journals social and physical attention components so as to
apparently reported interpretable data as to the determine whether one or the other of these com-
possible behavioral function with important prac- ponents could be motivating for the challenging
tical implications for intervention (Hanley, Iwata, behavior (e.g., Britton, Carr, Kellum, Dozier, &
& McCord, 2003; Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, & Weil, 2000). Other studies have replaced or sup-
Maglieri, 2005). Notwithstanding the positive plemented some of the traditional conditions with
results and utility of functional analysis, a num- new ones such as response blocking and response
ber of issues have been raised that call for possi- blocking with attention so as to represent daily
ble adaptations/changes of the classical Iwata program arrangements available for the partici-
et al.’s (1982/1994) model or conventional ver- pants (Hagopian & Toole, 2009).
sions following the original model (e.g., Buchanan
& Fisher, 2002; Carter et al., 2004; Tarbox et al.,
2009). Those issues, which may also present Designing Short Versions
some level of contradiction with one another, of Functional Analysis
concern (a) the occasional need of extending the
number of assessment conditions available so While the utility of functional analysis proce-
that the analysis becomes more representative of dures is not in discussion, their practicality
complex daily environments and ultimately more (affordability) in terms of time costs may raise
reliable, (b) the practical importance of designing some questions (Matson & Minshawi, 2007;
short versions of the assessment procedure, Moore, Fisher, & Pennington, 2004; Northup
(c) the opportunity of drawing different assess- et al., 1991; Perrin, Perrin, Hill, & DiNovi, 2008;
ment sequences in cases of inconclusive data, and Wallace & Knights, 2003). Iwata et al. (1982/1994)
(d) the importance of carrying out the assessment reported that the mean number of sessions
within the same educational/rehabilitation con- included in early functional analysis studies was
text in which the person spends his or her time 26 with an average time cost of about 6.5 h (seem-
(and of maintaining a number of basic elements ingly high in many daily contexts). In response
of that context) (Carter et al., 2004; LaBelle & to this question, various attempts were made to
Charlop-Christy, 2002; Lang et al., 2008; 2009; design and evaluate shorter, more affordable
Wallace & Knights, 2003; White et al., 2011). procedures. For example, Northup et al. (1991)
successfully designed a functional analysis pro-
cedure that lasted a 90-min period (i.e., a period
Extending the Range (Number) typically reserved for outpatient psychological
of Assessment Conditions evaluations) and was carried out within a single
day. Kahng and Iwata (1999) compared a full-
The desirability (need) of extending the assess- length functional analysis with a one-session-
ment range has frequently been addressed and a per-condition functional analysis with 50
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 49

participants with developmental disabilities and (i.e., change the position of the door). Healey
self-injurious or aggressive behavior and found et al. (2001) suggested that lack of response dif-
that the outcomes corresponded in 66% of the ferentiation under multielement designs (with
cases. Wallace and Knights (2003) reported com- quickly alternating conditions) may advise the
parable results with brief and extended functional use of blocked phases, that is, phases in which
analysis procedures applied with three adults the conditions are repeated for some time rather
with developmental disabilities and disruptive than being regularly rotated.
behavior. The brief analysis was based on 2-min
sessions and the extended analysis on 10-min
sessions. The time durations of the two were 36 Carrying Out the Assessment within
min and 310 min, respectively. Perrin et al. (2008) the Educational/Rehabilitation Context
carried out a brief functional analysis of elope- (or Maintaining Similar Task
ment with two 3-year-old children with a diagno- or Attention Condition)
sis of autism. The functional analysis included
five conditions each of which was presented over The fact that functional analysis can be arranged
four 5-min sessions. The results of the functional in different settings and carried out by different
analysis were differentiated and the intervention persons can be considered a practical advantage,
strategies based on the identified behavioral func- but can also present serious risks in terms of out-
tions were effective. come dependability (Lang et al., 2008; 2009;
McAdam, DiCesare, Murphy, & Marshall, 2004;
Tiger, Fisher, Toussaint, & Kodak, 2009). For
Drawing Different (Special) Assessment example, Lang et al. (2008) showed that the
Conditions or Sequences results of the functional analysis carried out with
two children of 12 and 7 years of age with autism
The results of the functional analysis can occa- and related communication and behavioral disor-
sionally be largely undifferentiated. This out- ders corresponded across settings (i.e., a therapy
come may be an indication that (a) the challenging room and a classroom) only for one of the chil-
behavior is idiosyncratic and requires very dren. Lang et al. (2009) replicated the earlier
specific assessment conditions (different from findings with a 4-year-old child whose challeng-
those usually included in the functional assess- ing behaviors included screaming and aggres-
ment procedure) or (b) the multielement method- sion. Indeed, the functional analysis carried out
ology used to alternate the conditions is less than in the playground seemed to indicate that the
optimal to differentiate those conditions and help child’s behaviors were mainly maintained by
clarify among them (DeLeon, Kahng, Rodriguez- adult attention. The functional analysis carried
Catter, Sveinsdóttir, & Sadler, 2003; Hausman, out in the classroom, however, seemed to indicate
Kahng, Farrell, & Mongeon, 2009; Healey, that the behaviors were maintained prevalently
Ahearn, Graff, & Libby, 2001; Ringdahl, by access to toys. Intervention procedures based
Christensen, & Boelter, 2009). For example, on non-contingent use of attention and toys indi-
DeLeon et al. (2003) reported that the level of cated that the former was more effective in the
challenging behavior was undifferentiated in con- playground and the latter more effective in the
ventional conditions but clearly dominant in a classroom. Tiger et al. (2009) found discrepan-
specially designed condition called “contingent cies between the problems identified in the daily
wheelchair movement.” Hausman et al. (2009) program and the outcome of their early functional
reported low levels of self-injurious and aggres- analyses. For example, the early functional analy-
sive behavior in a 9-year-old girl under conven- sis carried out with a 10-year-old child with intel-
tional conditions. Yet, her challenging behavior lectual disability, hydrocephalus, and mild visual
increased quite drastically when the assessment impairment did not evidence the high rates of
allowed the girl to access a ritualistic behavior aggressive behavior for which the child had been
50 G.E. Lancioni et al.

referred. In an attempt to extend the assessment Attention Alone or in Combination


and understand the reasons of the discrepancies,
it was discovered that the therapist in charge of Lindauer, Zarcone, Richman, and Schroeder
the functional analysis used a graduated- (2002) carried out a functional analysis with a
prompting procedure to help the child during 25-year-old man who had a diagnosis of pro-
academic tasks while the classroom teacher did found intellectual disability, Lennox Gastaut
not apparently do so and resorted to repeating the syndrome, and autism. The man presented self-
verbal instructions multiple times. This differ- injurious behaviors, such as hitting his head, bit-
ence was finally found to be responsible for the ing his hands, and banging his head, as well as
discrepancies. aggression. The functional analysis assessment
was based on 10-min sessions carried out in the
man’s bedroom and included five conditions, that
Selected Studies of Functional is, attention, demands, ignore, tangible items,
Analysis Divided by Identified and free play. During the attention condition, the
Functions therapist intervened following the instances of
self-injurious behavior for about 5 s. During the
The number of studies reported on functional tangible items condition, the man had access to
analysis is quite vast and involves a wide range of a preferred item (a tape recorder) for 30 s contin-
participants, behaviors, environments, and assess- gent on the occurrence of challenging behavior.
ment conditions. The aim of this section is to Similarly, in the demands condition, the occur-
present a small selection of studies (see list in rence of the behavior interrupted any demand
Table 4.1) serving as descriptive examples of pro- for 30 s. The man displayed relatively high rates
cedural approaches and of outcomes, that is, of of challenging behavior in the attention condi-
identified functions. These examples may serve tion suggesting that this was the main function
as guidelines for setting up the assessment and of the behavior. In a subsequent intervention
using its results for designing a matching inter- program, the authors combined a functional
vention. The first group of studies presented communication training (FCT) program and
below concerns research reports, which identified extinction for the challenging behavior with
attention either alone or in combination with other highly satisfactory results.
variables as the behavioral function (e.g., Fyffe, Kahng, Fittro, and Russell (2004) con-
Lancaster et al., 2004; Roane & Kelly, 2008; ducted a functional analysis with a 9-year-old
Stokes & Luiselli, 2009). The second group of boy who had a diagnosis of traumatic brain injury
studies concerns research reports, which identified and seizure disorder. The boy presented inappro-
tangible items as the behavioral function (e.g., priate sexual behavior, which consisted of touch-
Ingvarsson, Kahng, & Hausman, 2008; Lang ing or attempting to touch others in the area of the
et al., 2010). The third group of studies concerns groin, buttocks, or breast. The functional analysis
research reports, which pointed out escape from procedure was carried out in a therapy room
demand as the behavioral function (e.g., O’Reilly through 20-min sessions and included three
& Lancioni, 2000; Rooker & Roscoe, 2005). The assessment conditions, that is, demand, social
fourth group of studies concerns research reports, attention, and toy play. During the demand condi-
which suggested automatic reinforcement as the tion, there was a 30-s break in the task contingent
behavioral function (e.g., Falcomata, Roane, on the challenging behavior while praise was
Hovanetz, Kettering, & Keeney, 2004; Piazza, scheduled for compliance. In the social attention
Adelinis, Hanley, Goh, & Delia, 2000; Wilder, condition, the boy received a 5-s reprimand at the
Register, Register, Bajagic, & Neidert, 2009). The occurrence of the behavior. In the toy play condi-
final group of studies includes research reports, tion, the boy had access to preferred items and
which pointed out idiosyncratic events such as received attention at 30-s intervals. The task anal-
door manipulation as the behavioral function ysis showed that the behavior was exhibited only
(e.g., DeLeon et al., 2003; Hausman et al., 2009). in the social attention condition. Based on these
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 51

Table 4.1 List of studies divided by identified functions


Functions/authors Participants (No.) Age Challenging behaviors
Attention
Lindauer et al. (2002) 1 25 Self-injurious behavior
Fyffe et al. (2004) 1 9 Inappropriate sexual behavior
Lancaster et al. (2004) 4 34–56 Bizarre speech
Borrero et al. (2005) 3 7–12 Aggression and disruption
Dwyer-Moore and Dixon (2007) 3 70–90 Disruptive vocalizations or wandering and
attempts to exit place
Roane and Kelley (2008) 1 16 Self-injurious behavior
Stokes and Luiselli (2009) 1 4 Elopement
Tangible items
Hagopian et al. (2001) 1 6 Aggression/disruption and self-injurious behavior
Tarbox et al. (2003) 3 6–39 Elopement
Ingvarsson et al. (2008) 1 8 Aggression/disruption and self-injurious behavior
Reed et al. (2009) 1 16 Destruction and self-injurious behavior
Lang et al. (2010) 1 3 Elopement
Demand escape
O’Reilly and Lancioni (2000) 1 4 Aggression and self-injurious behaviors
Peyton et al. (2005) 1 10 Noncompliant vocal behavior
Rooker and Roscoe (2005) 1 5 Self-injurious behavior
Wilder et al. (2005) 1 40 Self-injurious behavior
Butler and Luiselli (2007) 1 13 Aggression, tantrums, and self-injurious behavior
Alone (automatic reinforcement)
Richman et al. (1998) 1 27 Disruption and self-injurious behavior
Roscoe et al. (1998) 3 20–38 Self-injurious behavior
Kuhn et al. (1999) 1 35 Self-injurious behavior
Piazza et al. (2000) 3 6–17 Disruption and self-injurious behavior
Wilder et al. (2000) 1 30 Head rocking
Tang et al. (2003) 6 4–17 Disruption, stereotypies, and self-injurious
behaviors
Falcomata et al. (2004) 1 18 Inappropriate verbalizations
Moore et al. (2004) 1 12 Self-injurious behavior
Wilder et al. (2009) 1 37 Rumination
Idiosyncratic events
DeLeon et al. (2003) 1 14 Aggression
Ringdahl et al. (2009) 1 18 Aggression and attempts to leave the room

data, an intervention strategy was implemented out in a therapy room over 10-min sessions and
that involved FCT (i.e., the boy was taught to included the standard four conditions suggested
request for attention via a card) and momentary by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) for one of the partici-
blocking followed by extinction/ignoring for the pants and several condition and design variations
challenging behavior. The intervention impact for the other participants. The outcome of the
was positive and the availability of the communi- assessment showed that the bizarre speech of two
cation card was gradually reduced. of the participants was clearly higher in the atten-
Lancaster et al. (2004) studied four adults of tion condition. In relation to this outcome, non-
34–56 years of age, who had dual (i.e., intellec- contingent attention was subsequently scheduled
tual and psychiatric) diagnoses and presented for both these participants with encouraging
with bizarre speech. The assessment was carried results. Both participants had clear declines
52 G.E. Lancioni et al.

(but not the elimination) of the challenging the therapist sat far from the patient and intervened
behavior. For one of them, the decline in bizarre with 5–10 s of attention (i.e., a number of sen-
speech was accompanied by an increase in appro- tences frequently used within the facility) only in
priate speech. relation to the problematic behaviors. In the
Borrero, Vollmer, Borrero, and Bourret (2005) demand condition, the therapist required the
reported the evaluation of three children of 12, 8, patients to perform simple motor activities or
and 7 years of age who presented with mild intel- computation tasks, which were involved in the
lectual disability, autism and moderate intellec- patients’ exercise programs. Compliance resulted
tual disability, and mild intellectual disability and in praise. No response or incorrect responding
seizure disorder, respectively. The participants’ resulted in prompting. Challenging behavior
challenging behaviors consisted of aggression resulted in interruption of any demands for 30 s.
towards other persons and disruption. The func- In the leisure/play condition, leisure items such
tional analysis procedure was based on sessions as magazines and television were available and
of 10 or 5 min and included four conditions, that the therapist provided social attention at intervals
is, attention, demands (instructions), tangible, of about 30 s. In the alone condition, the patients
and play. During the attention condition, the par- were alone in the assessment room and had no
ticipants received a brief reprimand in relation to leisure items available while the therapist watched
the challenging behavior. During the demand them unobtrusively from outside the room. The
condition (which included task instructions pre- results of the functional analysis showed that the
sented at intervals of about 10 s and supplemented disruptive verbalizations of one of the two
with prompts), the occurrence of the challenging patients with such a challenging behavior and the
behavior caused a 30-s interruption of all instruc- wandering of the third patient were clearly higher
tions and prompts. During the tangible condition, during the attention condition. The disruptive
the behavior ensured a 30-s access to preferred verbalization of the other patient seemed to be
items. During the play condition, the participants higher during the demand condition. In order to
had free access to preferred items and received determine the dependability of the assessment
attention from the therapist at 30-s intervals. The data, intervention conditions matching these data
first participant showed a clear increase in disrup- were organized. Attention contingent on appro-
tive behavior during the attention condition (sug- priate verbalizations was used for the first person.
gesting that attention was the specific variable Noncontingent attention was arranged for the
that maintained the behavior). The third partici- patient with the wandering problem, and FCT and
pant had an increase in aggression and disruptive extinction were used for the last participant. The
behavior during attention, demand, and tangible results of the intervention supported the findings
conditions. The second participant, to the con- of the functional analysis with all three patients
trary, showed a behavior increase only in the tan- and improved their general performance.
gible condition. Roane and Kelley (2008) investigated the case
Dwyer-Moore and Dixon (2007) studied three of a 16-year-old girl who had a diagnosis of mod-
elderly persons of 70–90 years of age who were erate intellectual disability, generalized anxiety
affected by dementia and resided in a care facil- disorder, and cerebral degenerative chorea. The
ity. Their challenging behaviors consisted of dis- girl’s challenging behavior consisted of hitting
ruptive vocalizations or wandering and attempting herself with her hands and banging her head. The
to exit the locked facility. The functional analysis functional analysis was conducted in a fully pad-
procedure was carried out in a room of the care ded room over sessions of 10 min. The two
facility and consisted of the rapid alternation of assessment conditions consisted of continuous
four standard conditions (i.e., attention, demand, and contingent physical attention. In the first of
alone, and leisure/play). The conditions were these conditions (continuous attention), a thera-
implemented over sessions of 10 min separated pist sat next to the girl on the floor so that the girl
by intervals of 5 min. In the attention condition, could wrap her arms around the therapist or the
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 53

therapist held hands with the girl throughout the well as by sensory consequences (i.e., automatic
length of the session. During the second (contin- reinforcement; Lancioni et al., 2009). Although,
gent attention) condition, the two forms of physi- the man did not show the skin picking behavior
cal contact/attention were the same as those during the demand condition, staff hypothesized
mentioned above. They were applied only in con- that rectal picking could also be motivated by
nection with the occurrence of the challenging task escape (as it often resulted in the interruption
behavior when the therapist moved close to the of all he was doing and visits to the hospital). The
girl for 20 s. The levels of challenging behavior intervention program, which was matched to the
seemed definitely higher in the contingent condi- assessment, included reduced toilet times, FCT,
tion suggesting that this form of attention was and differential social reinforcement of positive
responsible for it. Based on these data, the authors toilet behavior. The program outcome showed a
applied continuous physical attention as the inter- successful elimination of rectal picking and the
vention variable to reduce foot withdrawal and maintenance of the positive effects over time.
promote correct step performance during walking
sessions. Consistent with the results of the func-
tional analysis, the walking data showed that con- Access to Tangible Items
tinuous physical attention reduced foot withdrawal
and increased step performance. By contrast, Hagopian, Wilson, and Wilder (2001) followed
attention contingent on foot withdrawal increased the case of a 6-year-old boy with autism and mild
this behavior and reduced step performance. intellectual disability who presented with aggres-
Stokes and Luiselli (2009) reported the assess- sion, disruption, and self-injurious behaviors.
ment and treatment of a 26-year-old man with The functional analysis was eventually concen-
Prader–Willi syndrome who attended a commu- trated on social attention, task demand, social
nity-based vocational training program and lived demand, and tangible conditions through 10-min
in a supervised group-home. The man’s main sessions. The social demand condition involved
challenging behavior consisted of rectal picking, the therapist talking to the boy and commenting
which had a long history and was health threaten- on his play. This stopped for 30 s in connection
ing. He also presented with skin picking, which with the boy’s challenging behavior. Similarly,
consisted of excoriating the skin around his the challenging behavior allowed 30-s access to a
fingernails, arms, and lips. Following the applica- preferred toy in the tangible condition. The
tion of functional assessment scales such as the assessment results indicated that the boy had high
MAS and the Functional Analysis Screening Tool levels of challenging behavior in the tangible and
(Iwata & DeLeon, 1996), the authors also con- social demand conditions. In the subsequent
ducted a specific functional analysis assessment intervention, the use of FCT (to require objects)
using skin picking as target behavior (rather than was effective to reduce the challenging behavior
rectal picking as they wanted to avoid undue risks to virtually zero levels.
in relation to this particular behavior). The func- Tarbox, Wallace, and Williams (2003) carried
tional analysis was based on four standard condi- out an investigation with three participants of 6,
tions (i.e., attention, demand/escape, play, and 28, and 39 years of age who had diagnoses of
alone) implemented in a small room at the voca- Asperger’s syndrome, profound intellectual
tional training context. Each condition was imple- disability and seizure disorder, and severe intel-
mented over three 5-min sessions that were lectual disability, respectively. The challenging
carried out by a specific staff person (i.e., differ- behavior of each of the participants was elope-
ent from the persons involved in the other ses- ment. The functional analysis was carried out by
sions/conditions). The results of the assessment regular caregivers in situations that resembled the
showed that the behavior had high frequency in participants’ typical settings (i.e., those in which
the attention and alone conditions suggesting the challenging behaviors occurred). The condi-
that it could be motivated by staff attention as tions implemented over 10-min sessions included
54 G.E. Lancioni et al.

attention, tangible, demand, and play. The results its implementation. Moreover, two condition
of the functional analysis showed that the first variations were realized. One of these new condi-
two participants had high levels of elopement tions was preceded by the participant’s choice of
responses during the tangible condition (i.e., the tangible item that he would have during the
when they could reach a toy store and obtain a sessions (i.e., contingent on the occurrence of the
potato chip, respectively). In the subsequent, challenging behaviors). The other condition
matched intervention, the use of FCT to enable involved the same item across all sessions. The
the two participants to formulate item requests results showed that the frequency of the challeng-
combined with elopement blocking produced ing behaviors was high only in the tangible
largely satisfactory results. condition, in which the participant was allowed
Ingvarsson et al. (2008) reported the case of to choose the preferred item to use.
an 8-year-old girl who had been diagnosed with Lang et al. (2010) conducted an assessment
autism, mild cerebral palsy, moderate intellectual with a 4-year-old boy who had a diagnosis of
disability, and obsessive–compulsive disorders. Asperger’s syndrome and presented the problem
She also presented aggression, disruption and of elopement. Separate functional analyses were
self-injurious behaviors. The assessment was car- carried out in the boy’s typical classroom and in a
ried out by a therapist in a bedroom of the facility resource room in which the boy received indi-
in which the girl had been admitted through vidual instruction. The analyses were based on
10-min sessions. The conditions included atten- 5-min sessions covering attention, demand, play,
tion, demand, toy play, ignore, and tangible items. and tangible conditions. In the tangible condi-
In the tangible condition, the girl obtained her tion, a television and a DVD player were avail-
preferred food contingent on her challenging able. The child was shown his preferred DVD for
behavior. High rates of challenging behavior 10 s prior to the session and contingent on each
were consistently observed only in the tangible elopement episode during the session. The results
condition. A subsequent intervention provided of the two analyses differed. The rates of elope-
preferred food items noncontingently within a ment were higher during the attention condition
task-like condition with positive results, irrespec- in the resource room and during the tangible con-
tive of the density of the delivery. dition in the classroom. Intervention strategies
Reed, Pace, and Luiselli (2009) studied a based on the noncontingent use of attention and
16-year-old boy with a diagnosis of pervasive tangible items validated the results of the func-
developmental disorder with seizure and mood tional analyses. In fact, the attention-based inter-
problems, and a possible hemispheric brain vention was effective in the resource room while
lesion. The boy attended a school for students the tangible-based intervention was effective in
with brain injury and showed multiple forms of the classroom.
self-injury and destructive behaviors. The func-
tional analysis was carried out within a special
(assessment) room over 10-min sessions. The Demand Escape
procedure involved three common conditions
(i.e., demand, attention, and play), which were O’Reilly and Lancioni (2000) examined the situ-
carried out according to standard rules. The only ation of a 4-year-old girl with moderate level of
specificities were (a) the use of educational tasks intellectual disability who presented with self-
of the participant’s individualized program for injurious and aggressive behavior. The girl was
the demand condition, (b) the availability of five residing at home with her family and the func-
preferred stimuli relevant for the participant tional analysis and intervention were conducted
engagement during the play condition, and (c) the in that context with the mother acting as thera-
use of different staff persons for the implementa- pist. The functional analysis included the stan-
tion of the diverse conditions. The tangible dard attention, demand, play, and alone conditions
condition involved a new (fourth) staff person for and was implemented through 15-min sessions.
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 55

During the demand condition, the girl was functional analysis was carried out in a special
presented with two tasks that she had difficulty room through 10-min sessions covering the four
carrying out. Both tasks were available within standard (i.e., attention, demand, play, and alone)
each session and the therapist/mother maintained conditions. During the demand condition, a
her requests on them expect in case of challeng- difficult task was used and the challenging behav-
ing behaviors. Contingent on these behaviors, the ior allowed a 15-s interruption of such a task. The
task was removed and the mother stepped away results of the analysis indicated that the boy
for a minimum of 10 s starting from the cessation exhibited clearly higher levels of the challenging
of those behaviors. The results showed high behavior in the demand condition and suggested
levels of challenging behaviors during the demand that such behavior served to escape the difficult
condition and virtually no challenging behaviors task situation. Repetitions of functional analysis
during the other conditions. The authors also with noncontingent access to forms of self-
found out that the level of challenging behaviors restraint or contingent access to self-restraint
co-varied with the level of sleep deprivation. indicated that the challenging behavior occurred
That is, more severe sleep deprivation tended to only when self-restraint was contingent. This lat-
increase the challenging behavior. ter data would seem to modify the early sugges-
Peyton, Lindauer, and Richman (2005) tion that challenging behavior served as escape.
reported a study with a 10-year-old girl who had Wilder, Normand, and Atwell (2005) reported
a diagnosis of autism and developmental delays. the evaluation of a 40-year-old woman with a
The girl’s challenging behavior consisted of non- diagnosis of autism, gastro-esophageal reflux and
compliant vocal behavior (e.g., the girl declaring food allergies. She presented with food refusal
her inability to do what she was asked to do). The and exhibited various forms of severe self-injuri-
assessment was carried out at home with the girl’s ous behavior. The functional analysis was carried
mother acting as therapist and video-recording out in a therapy room through 10-min sessions
the sessions for subsequent expert scoring. that covered the four standard conditions. During
The functional analysis included the attention, the attention condition, the therapist followed
demand, play, and alone conditions. During the every instance of challenging behavior with a
demand condition, the mother prompted her to verbal reprimand and a brief physical touch.
complete tasks dealing with number, letter, and During the play condition, the woman had free
picture identifications. Compliance led her to access to preferred items and activities and
receive praise and to be presented with additional received the therapist’s attention every 30 s, while
instructions/prompts. Noncompliance led to task no consequences were available for her challeng-
interruption and material removal for about 20 s. ing behavior. During the demand condition, the
Data showed high levels of noncompliant vocal therapist presented a bite of food on a spoon
behavior only in the demand condition with the every 30 s. Brief praise was used if the woman
suggestion that the behavior served to escape the accepted the bite. If the woman did not accept the
demand situation. Subsequent investigations in bite but did not show the challenging behavior,
which the instructions were provided in an indi- the spoon remained at her lips for 30 s at which
rect way demonstrated that the noncompliant time a new bite was presented. In case of chal-
vocal behavior decreased to virtually zero levels lenging behavior, the spoon was removed and the
(thus suggesting that the instruction mode rather therapist took a distance from the woman for
than the task demand was responsible for the about 15 s. The woman was reported to show the
challenging behavior) while task performance challenging behavior almost exclusively during
was very high. the demand condition and it was suggested that
Rooker and Roscoe (2005) carried out an such behavior served to escape the feeding situa-
assessment with a 5-year-old boy with a diagno- tion. In a subsequent intervention effort, the
sis of autism and a serious self-injurious behavior authors tested the effects of continuous stimulation
consisting of chin-to-shoulder hitting. The initial (children’s video), which was briefly interrupted
56 G.E. Lancioni et al.

(i.e., for 15 s) only in connection with the condition, in which basic motor activities (least
occurrence of the challenging behavior. Data demanding forms of engagement) were used. The
showed that the level of challenging behavior results of the third phase of the analysis showed
dropped drastically and the level of bite accep- that the highest levels of challenging behavior
tance increased to about 90% of the total. occurred with the therapist in charge of the previ-
Butler and Luiselli (2007) reported an elabo- ous assessment phases and the classroom teacher
rate three-phase functional analysis with a (i.e., the two persons with whom the girl had a
13-year-old girl who had a diagnosis of autism longer learning history, that is, the longest practice
and presented with self-injurious behavior as well time for consolidating the relationship between
as aggression and tantrums. The analysis was car- demands, challenging behavior, and escape
ried out within the girl’s daily educational context opportunities). Based on the aforementioned
through 10-min sessions. The first phase of the results, the intervention program was directed at
functional analysis involved the four standard (a) smoothing (attenuating the aversiveness/
(attention, demand, play, and alone) conditions. difficulties of) the instructions so as to reduce the
During the demand condition, the therapist pre- motivation to engage in challenging behavior to
sented academic tasks that were part of the girl’s escape them, (b) implementing noncontingent
educational plan. For each task, the initial instruc- escape occasions to allow the girl multiple break
tion would be followed by prompting if the girl (reinforcement) occasions, and (c) avoiding task
failed to respond. Every correct response was fol- removal and therapist distancing in relation to
lowed by praise. Challenging behavior led to the challenging behavior. Those basic principles
removal of the task and distancing of the therapist were combined with fading strategies concerning
for 30 s. The second phase of the functional anal- the number of instructions presented within each
ysis was directed at assessing different demand session and the interval between the occasions of
conditions. One of these conditions involved the noncontingent reinforcement. This intervention
same requests/tasks used during the first phase package seemed to be very effective with an
of the functional analysis. Another condition increased number of instructions available and a
involved requests concerning the simple manipu- virtual disappearance of the challenging behavior.
lation of objects such as puzzles, beads, clay, and
sensory toys already familiar to the girl. The final
condition involved requests concerning basic Alone (Automatic Reinforcement)
motor activities such as sitting down and putting
something away. The third phase of the functional Richman, Wacker, Asmus, and Casey (1998)
analysis served to assess the impact of five differ- carried out an assessment with a 27-year-old
ent therapists within a demand condition that woman who had a diagnosis of profound intel-
matched the one used during the first phase of the lectual disability and autism, and presented with
analysis. The five therapists were known to the disruptive behavior as well as finger picking.
girl and included the one responsible for the first Functional analysis, which included the standard
two phases of the analysis, the classroom teacher, four conditions applied over 5-min sessions,
and three classroom assistants. showed a different picture for the two forms of
The results of the first phase of the analysis challenging behavior. In essence, disruptive
showed that the girl had low or zero levels of behavior seemed to occur (almost) exclusively
challenging behavior except in the demand con- within the demand condition, while finger picking
dition, in which those levels were very high. The was consistently present through all conditions.
results of the second phase of the analysis indi- Based on these data, the hypothesis was that dis-
cated that the levels of challenging behavior were ruptive behavior was maintained by escape and
highest in the first demand condition, in which finger picking was due to the sensory input it
the educational (most demanding/difficult) tasks produced (i.e., was maintained by automatic rein-
were presented and lowest in the third demand forcement; see Kenzer & Wallace, 2007; Lyons,
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 57

Rue, Luiselli, & DiGennaro, 2007; Vollmer, and obsessive–compulsive disorder and displayed
1994). The intervention program for finger pick- severe face hitting and head banging. The func-
ing consisted of (a) a combination of response tional analysis was carried out through 10-min
blocking applied to prevent its occurrence and sessions and included the four standard condi-
effects and prompting aimed at directing the tions. Given the severity of the challenging
woman’s hand to a toy, and (b) praise contingent behaviors, the therapist tried to block their occur-
on independent toy play. The positive effects of rence throughout the sessions. The behavior lev-
this package, which were maintained over time, els seemed to be highest in the alone and demand
could not be replicated through the use of the conditions, suggesting that automatic reinforce-
reinforcement (praise) alone. ment, escape or both could be responsible for the
Roscoe, Iwata, and Goh (1998) studied three outcome. To determine which hypothesis was
adults of 20–38 years of age who were diagnosed more realistic, intervention strategies involving
with profound or moderate intellectual disability sensory extinction, escape extinction or both
and displayed self-injurious behaviors such as were arranged. The intervention effects were
arm rubbing, object hitting, hand mouthing, and highly positive when the two forms of extinction
body picking and rubbing. The functional analy- together and when sensory extinction alone were
sis included the four standard conditions, which used and largely unsatisfactory when escape
were implemented by four different therapists extinction was applied. On this basis, it was con-
over four different rooms through 15-min ses- cluded that the challenging behaviors were main-
sions. One participant showed high levels of chal- tained by sensory reinforcement.
lenging behavior in the demand, play and alone Piazza et al. (2000) conducted their investiga-
conditions, and somewhat lower levels in the tions with three participants of 6–17 years of age
attention condition. The second participant had who had a diagnosis of severe to profound intel-
comparably high levels across all conditions. The lectual disability and multiple challenging behav-
third participant had high levels only in the alone iors. Dangerous behavior (e.g., climbing on
condition. Based on these data, the view was that furniture), saliva play, and hand mouthing were
the challenging behaviors did not have a social exposed to functional analysis, which included
function and were probably maintained by auto- the four standard conditions for one participant
matic reinforcement. In line with this hypothesis, and a fifth (tangible) condition for the other two.
the intervention strategies carried out with the Sessions were 10-min long and were carried out
participants consisted of stimulation and sensory in a specially equipped context. The outcome of
extinction. The two strategies were compared the analysis indicated that the dangerous behavior
through 10-min sessions implemented alterna- occurred at the highest level in the alone condi-
tively. The stimulation condition consisted of pro- tion. The other two behaviors had high frequen-
viding the participants with preferred objects cies in all conditions, including the alone condition.
(e.g., a dumbbell massager or a plastic ring) that Therefore, the hypothesis was that the behaviors
they could manipulate without incurring the chal- were maintained by their sensory consequences
lenging behaviors. The sensory extinction con- (automatic reinforcement). On this basis, objects
sisted of using devices (e.g., foam sleeves or latex were selected that could produce sensory conse-
gloves) that would mask the sensory effects of the quences similar to those related to the challenging
challenging behaviors. The results were eventu- behaviors, and the hope was that the participants
ally positive with both intervention strategies. could play with these objects and, thereby, pro-
However, the effects of stimulation were appar- duce sufficient sensory input to replace the need
ently more rapid and accompanied by partici- for the challenging behaviors. The intervention
pants’ object engagement. data comparing the effects of those objects
Kuhn, DeLeon, Fisher, and Wilke (1999) with those of unmatched ones (i.e., producing
reported the study of a 35-year-old man who had stimulation unrelated to the challenging behav-
a diagnosis of autism, severe intellectual disability, iors) supported the aforementioned anticipation.
58 G.E. Lancioni et al.

All three participants had drastic reductions of visual masking occurred through goggles or a
their challenging behaviors when allowed to dark screen; tactile masking occurred through
engage in activity with matched material. gloves. Preliminary findings indicated some
Wilder et al. (2000) carried out a study with a effects of one form of masking or another with
30-year-old woman who had a diagnosis of pro- three of the participants. In a subsequent effort to
found intellectual disability and visual impair- investigate masking and competing (substitutive)
ment and presented with persistent bilateral head sensory stimulation, the authors reported encour-
rocking. The functional analysis involving the aging results with at least two of the three partici-
four standard conditions, which were imple- pants involved. One of these two responded
mented over 10-min sessions, showed that the equally positively (with near zero levels of chal-
behavior was present at very high levels across lenging behavior) to the masking and the compet-
all conditions. This outcome seemed to indicate ing stimulation and the other showing a very
that the behavior did not have any social function, obvious reduction of the challenging behavior
but was rather maintained by automatic rein- during the competing sensory stimulation.
forcement. Intervention strategies based on this Falcomata et al. (2004) examined an 18-year-
notion were subsequently examined to determine old man who had a diagnosis of autism and
the reliability of the functional analysis conclu- presented with problem behaviors including inap-
sions. An assessment comparing the use of vari- propriate verbalizations. The first phase of the
ous forms of stimulation intervention through the functional analysis involved five conditions,
sessions showed that a vibrating massage reduced namely, the four standard ones plus a tangible
the rocking behavior drastically, while illumina- condition. The second phase involved only the
tion changes and increased noise inputs did not alone condition. Sessions lasted 10 min.
produce effects. A subsequent assessment involved Inappropriate verbalizations were consistently
the comparison of two environmental enrichment present during the alone, attention and demand
strategies, which included multiple stimuli, and conditions of the first assessment phase and
varied from one another for the presence or remained very high during the second phase. The
absence of vibratory stimuli in the package. In suggestion was therefore that they were main-
line with the previous data, the package including tained by automatic reinforcement. In a related
vibratory stimuli was more effective in reducing treatment evaluation, two intervention strategies
head rocking. were compared. One included the use of noncon-
Tang, Patterson, and Kennedy (2003) carried tingent reinforcement, that is, the participant had
out functional analyses with six participants of continuous access to a radio and no consequences
4–17 years of age who were diagnosed with pro- occurred for the inappropriate verbalizations.
found intellectual or multiple disabilities and pre- The other strategy differed from the previous one
sented with one or two challenging behaviors only in that the radio was removed for 5 s at the
such as hand mouthing and head shaking. The occurrence of inappropriate verbalizations. The
functional analyses involved the four standard first intervention strategy succeeded in reducing
conditions, which were applied over 5-min ses- the inappropriate verbalizations to about 50% of
sions. The results of the analyses indicated that the baseline level. The second intervention strat-
the challenging behaviors were showing high lev- egy reduced them to a virtually zero level.
els of occurrence across all conditions or during Moore et al. (2004) conducted a study with a
the alone condition, therefore suggesting that 12-year-old girl who had a diagnosis of autism
automatic reinforcement was largely (or totally) and presented with a multitude of severe chal-
responsible for their maintenance. In a related lenging behaviors, which included hand, head,
intervention assessment, masking (sensory-attenu- shoulder, foot and leg. The study was organized
ation) strategies were implemented with five of the into two different phases. The first phase was
original participants. Auditory masking occurred directed at assessing the occurrence or absence of
through the use of safety plugs or headphones; the specific behaviors in relation to the presence
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 59

or absence of protective equipment. The partici- intervention phases, the rates decreased to around
pant was alone within a padded room through all 1 per min. Eventually, the participant was able to
the assessment sessions. The results showed that maintain such a level by self-administering the
the behaviors were at a virtually zero level when spray at intervals of about 10 s.
protective equipment was present on all relevant
parts of the body. Shoulder-related behaviors
occurred when this body part was freed from pro- Access to Idiosyncratic Events
tective material. A similar outcome occurred with
hand-related behaviors, but not with foot/leg- DeLeon et al. (2003) reported the case of a
related behaviors. The second phase of the study 14-year-old boy who was diagnosed with pro-
was a functional analysis (with the four standard found intellectual disabilities, cerebral palsy, and
conditions) applied to the hand-related behaviors. visual impairment, and showed a multitude of
The outcome of this phase showed that the occur- challenging behaviors including aggression (e.g.,
rence of these behaviors increased drastically in hitting, pinching, and grabbing others). The func-
each of the assessment conditions when the pro- tional analysis, which was carried out through
tective equipment was removed. On the basis of 10-min sessions, included seven conditions.
both sets of data, the authors’ conclusion was that Three of them (i.e., attention, play, and alone)
the participant’s challenging behaviors were represented standard conditions. The other four
maintained by automatic reinforcement. (i.e., task demand, daily living activities, social
Wilder et al. (2009) conducted a study with a demand, and contingent wheelchair movement)
37-year-old man who had a diagnosis of profound were arranged according to the participant’s gen-
intellectual disability and autism and presented eral characteristics/peculiarities. During task
with rumination (i.e., regurgitation, chewing and demand, the therapist required the performance
re-swallowing of food previously ingested). The of academic tasks but supplemented the requests
functional analysis involved the four standard with guidance/orientation toward the material
conditions, which were presented by four differ- and adapted prompting sequences. During daily
ent therapists wearing shirts of different colors. living activities, the therapist performed hygiene
Each condition involved two sessions per day, one activities for the boy and interrupted those activi-
prior to the meal and one after the meal. Sessions ties for 30 s in concomitance with the challenging
were 10-min long. The participant showed no behaviors. During the social demand, the thera-
rumination during the sessions occurring prior to pist provided continuous verbal and physical
the meal. By contrast, he had high levels of rumi- attention and interrupted it all for 30 s following
nation during all sessions carried out after the challenging behaviors. Finally, during the contin-
meal, irrespective of the conditions under which gent wheelchair movement, the therapist resumed
they occurred. In light of these findings, the sug- wheelchair pushing for 30 s after each instance of
gestion was that the behavior did not have a social challenging behavior. The findings showed that
function, but was maintained by automatic rein- challenging behavior was highest within the last
forcement. Although the source of this reinforce- assessment condition. Based on these findings,
ment was not known, it was assumed that it would the intervention included FCT (i.e., technology-
be related to oral stimulation. On this basis, an assisted request of wheelchair movement), which
intervention strategy involving the use of preferred allowed him 30 s of wheelchair pushing, and
oral stimulation on a noncontingent schedule was extinction for the challenging behavior. The boy
implemented. The oral stimulation consisted of increased his requests and reduced the challeng-
applying pie spray (i.e., a form of stimulation ing behavior to near zero levels.
which was considered easy to use and advanta- Ringdahl et al. (2009) studied an 18-year-old
geous on the long term because calorie-free). The woman who had a diagnosis of severe to profound
rate of rumination during the baseline phases intellectual disability and presented with chal-
varied between 2.8 and 3.7 per min. During the lenging behaviors, which consisted of aggression
60 G.E. Lancioni et al.

and attempts to leave the room. The initial The third group of studies identified demand
functional analysis included fairly standard con- escape (e.g., O’Reilly & Lancioni, 2000; Rooker
ditions concerning attention, demand, and play. & Roscoe, 2005). The fourth group of studies
The outcome of this analysis showed that the identified automatic reinforcement (e.g., Piazza
challenging behaviors were largely distributed et al., 2000; Wilder et al., 2009). The fifth group
across all conditions and thus no conclusions of studies identified idiosyncratic events (e.g.,
could be drawn as to their possible social func- Ringdahl et al., 2009).
tion. The subsequent functional analysis com- The intervention strategies developed on the
pared two conditions involving walking in the basis of the results of the functional analyses
hallway. In one condition, the woman had con- were generally satisfactory thus providing sup-
tinuous access to walking in the hallway with the port for the reliability of those results and the
therapist providing regular attention. In the other notion that the intervention should be planned in
condition, the woman had continuous access to accordance with the function of the challenging
leisure items as well as attention from the thera- behaviors. Three questions would seem to be
pist. Her access to walking (i.e., 30-s walking open to additional research in this area. The first
periods), however, was contingent on the occur- question pertains to the functional analysis’
rence of aggression. The woman showed aggres- model, implementation procedures, and time
sion only in the latter condition. On the basis of requirements/costs. With regard to the model,
this outcome, intervention strategies were suc- suggestions were made about the occasional
cessfully implemented, which involved FCT necessity of contemplating additional or alterna-
(to request walks), use of walks as reinforcement tive conditions compared to those available in the
of simple activities, and use of walks on a non- standard format, that is, the one provided by
contingent schedule. Iwata et al. (1982/1994) and followed by most of
the studies in the field. With regard to the imple-
mentation procedures, evidence was presented
Conclusion about the possible relevance of (a) carrying out
the assessment in the same contexts in which the
Challenging behaviors are fairly common among person normally is and shows his or her challeng-
persons with developmental disorders, autism, ing behavior and (b) involving in the assessment
severe and profound intellectual disabilities, and the personnel responsible of the daily programs.
multiple disabilities (Lancioni et al., 2009; Rehfeldt With regard to the assessment duration (time
& Chambers, 2003; Stewart & Alderman, 2010). costs), the point was made that the length of many
The fact that these behaviors have a negative impact of the published analyses might be excessive (too
on the person’s developmental and adaptive oppor- costly) for several daily contexts. Indications also
tunities calls for the application of appropriate exist that shorter versions of those analyses might
intervention strategies (Singh et al., 2009; Tarbox be sufficient to provide usable information (e.g.,
et al., 2009). Intervention is more likely to succeed Northup et al., 1991; Perrin et al., 2008; Wallace
if it is matched to the function of the person’s chal- & Knights, 2003).
lenging behavior (Borrero & Borrero, 2008; The second question concerns the fact that
Matson & Minshawi, 2007; Neidert et al., 2010). intervention may need to reconcile the elimina-
In this chapter, five groups of studies were reviewed tion of the challenging behavior with the estab-
to illustrate different outcomes of the functional lishment of adaptive skills, which are essential to
analysis, that is, the identification of five different foster development (Lancioni et al., 2009). For
functions for challenging behaviors. The first group example, the effectiveness of a noncontingent
of studies identified attention (e.g., Fyffe et al., strategy to reduce challenging behaviors main-
2004; Roane & Kelley, 2008). The second group tained by access to attention or tangible items
of studies identified access to tangible items would need to be combined with efforts to develop
(e.g., Hagopian et al., 2001; Reed et al., 2009). useful skills, through which the participant can
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 61

have new opportunities of engagement and of disruptive vocalization in elderly dementia patients.
stimulation (Kazdin, 2001; Lancioni et al., 2009). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 99–103.
Butler, L. R., & Luiselli, J. K. (2007). Escape-maintained
The third question concerns the notion of idio- problem behavior in a child with autism. Journal of
syncratic situations and functions. In essence, Positive Behavior Interventions, 9, 195–202.
one could argue that idiosyncrasy concerned the Carr, J. E., Dozier, C. L., Patel, M. R., Adams, A. N., &
forms of reinforcement that worked for the per- Martin, N. (2002). Treatment of automatically rein-
forced object mouthing with noncontingent reinforce-
sons involved in the studies, which differed from ment and response blocking: Experimental analysis
the attention and tangible conditions/reinforcers and social validation. Research in Developmental
generally proposed within standard functional Disabilities, 23, 37–44.
analyses (e.g., DeLeon et al., 2003; Ringdahl Carter, S. L., Devlin, S., Doggett, R. A., Harber, M. M., &
Barr, C. (2004). Determining the influence of tangible
et al., 2009). The evidence available suggests that items on screaming and handmouthing following and
one cannot design a functional analysis assess- inconclusive functional analysis. Behavioral
ment independent of the participants’ daily expe- Interventions, 19, 51–58.
rience and possible reinforcement sources. DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S. W., Rodriguez-Catter, V.,
Sveinsdóttir, I., & Sadler, C. (2003). Assessment of
In conclusion, the vast literature on functional aberrant behavior maintained by wheelchair move-
analysis of challenging behaviors has produced a ment in a child with developmental disabilities.
large body of evidence on the possibility of iden- Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24, 381–390.
tifying the functions of those behaviors and there- Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988). Identifying
the variables maintaining self-injurious behavior.
fore of designing more respondent treatment Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18,
programs. At present, there is wide agreement on 99–117.
the need of carrying out the functional analysis of Dwyer-Moore, K. J., & Dixon, M. R. (2007). Functional
challenging behaviors and of using the results of analysis and treatment of problem behavior of elderly
adults in long-term care. Journal of Applied Behavior
such analysis to design the intervention. There is Analysis, 40, 679–683.
also a clear understanding that future research Emerson, E. (1995). Challenging behaviour: Analysis and
would need to concentrate on and clarify several intervention with people with learning difficulties.
aspects of both the functional analysis and the Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Hovanetz, A. N., Kettering,
subsequent intervention processes (e.g., Healey T. L., & Keeney, K. M. (2004). An evaluation of
et al., 2001; Lang et al., 2010; Perrin et al., 2008; response cost in the treatment of inappropriate vocal-
Wilder et al., 2009). izations maintained by automatic reinforcement.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 83–87.
Fyffe, C. E., Kahng, S. W., Fittro, E., & Russell, D. (2004).
Functional analysis and treatment of inappropriate
References sexual behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
37, 401–404.
Asmus, J. M., Franzese, J. C., Conroy, M. A., & Dozier, C. Hagopian, L. P., & Toole, L. M. (2009). Effects of response
L. (2003). Clarifying functional analysis outcomes for blocking and competing stimuli on stereotypic behav-
disruptive behaviors by controlling consequence ior. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 117–125.
delivery for stereotypy. School Psychology Review, 32, Hagopian, L. P., Wilson, D. M., & Wilder, D. A. (2001).
624–630. Assessment and treatment of problem behavior main-
Borrero, C. S. W., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive tained by escape from attention and access to tangible
and experimental analyses of potential precursors to items. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior 229–232.
Analysis, 41, 83–96. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Borrero, C. S. W., Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., & Bourret, Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
J. (2005). A method for evaluating parameters of rein- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
forcement during parent-child interactions. Research Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K. A.
in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 577–592. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for pun-
Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Kellum, K. K., Dozier, C. L., & ishment and extinction components of function-based
Weil, T. M. (2000). A variation of noncontingent rein- interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
forcement in the treatment of aberrant behavior. 51–65.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 425–435. Harvey, M. T., Luiselli, J. K., & Wong, S. E. (2009).
Buchanan, J. A., & Fisher, J. E. (2002). Functional assess- Application of applied behavior analysis to mental
ment and noncontingent reinforcement in the treatment health issues. Psychological Services, 6, 212–222.
62 G.E. Lancioni et al.

Hausman, N., Kahng, S. W., Farrell, E., & Mongeon, C. and treatment of the bizarre speech of dually diag-
(2009). Idiosyncratic functions: Severe problem nosed adults. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
behavior maintained by access to ritualistic behaviors. 37, 395–399.
Education and Treatment of Children, 32, 77–87. Lancioni, G. E., Singh, N. N., O’Reilly, M. F., & Sigafoos,
Healey, J. J., Ahearn, W. H., Graff, R. B., & Libby, M. E. J. (2009). An overview of behavioral strategies for
(2001). Extended analysis and treatment of self-injuri- reducing hand-related stereotypies of persons with
ous behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 16, 181–195. severe to profound intellectual and multiple disabilities:
Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S. W., & Hausman, N. L. (2008). 1995-2007. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Some effects of noncontingent positive reinforcement 30, 20–43.
on multiply controlled problem behavior and compli- Lancioni, G. E., Walraven, M., O’Reilly, M. F., & Singh,
ance in a demand context. Journal of Applied Behavior N. N. (2002). Persistent humming by a man with mul-
Analysis, 41, 435–440. tiple disabilities: Evaluating function and treatment
Iwata, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G. (1996). Functional analysis opportunities. European Journal of Behavior Analysis,
screening tool (FAST). Gainesville: The Florida Center 3, 75–80.
on Self-Injury, University of Florida. Lang, R., Davis, T., O’Reilly, M., Machalicek, W., Rispoli,
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & M., Sigafoos, J., et al. (2010). Functional analysis and
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis treatment of elopement across two school settings.
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 113–118.
27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Lancioni, G., Rispoli, M.,
in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). Machalicek, W., Chan, J. M., et al. (2009). Discrepancy
Kahng, S. W., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Correspondence in functional analysis results across two applied set-
between outcomes of brief and extended functional tings: Implications for intervention design. Journal of
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 393–398.
149–159. Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Machalicek, W., Lancioni, G.,
Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied Rispoli, M., & Chan, J. M. (2008). A preliminary
settings (6th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth. comparison of functional analysis results when con-
Kates-McElrath, K., Agnew, M., Axelrod, S., & Bloh, C. ducted in contrived versus natural settings. Journal of
L. (2007). Identification of behavioral function in pub- Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 441–445.
lic schools and clarification of terms. Behavioral Lindauer, S. E., Zarcone, J. R., Richman, D. M., &
Interventions, 22, 47–56. Schroeder, S. R. (2002). A comparison of multiple
Kenzer, A. L., & Wallace, M. D. (2007). Treatment of reinforcer assessments to identify the function of mal-
rumination maintained by automatic reinforcement: adaptive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
A comparison of extra portions during a meal and Analysis, 35, 299–303.
supplemental post-meal feedings. Behavioral Lyons, E. A., Rue, H. C., Luiselli, J. K., & DiGennaro, F. D.
Interventions, 22, 297–304. (2007). Brief functional analysis and supplemental
Kuhn, D. E., DeLeon, I. G., Fisher, W. W., & Wilke, A. E. feeding for postmeal rumination in children with
(1999). Clarifying an ambiguous functional analysis developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied
with matched and mismatched extinction procedures. Behavior Analysis, 40, 743–747.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 99–102. Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj,
Kuhn, D. E., Hardesty, S. L., & Sweeney, N. M. (2009). T. R. (1999). A validity study on the Questions About
Assessment and treatment of excessive straightening Behavior Function (QABF) scale: Predicting treat-
and destructive behavior in an adolescent diagnosed ment success for self-injury, aggression, and stereo-
with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, typies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20,
42, 355–360. 163–175.
Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F., Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2008). A review of behav-
O’Connor, J. T., Paclawskyj, T. R., et al. (2003). ioral treatments for self-injurious behaviors of persons
Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Modification,
behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases. 32, 61–76.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219. Matson, J. L., Mayville, S. B., Kuhn, D. E., Sturmey, P.,
Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Rush, K. S., & Dixon, D. R. Laud, R., & Cooper, C. (2005). The behavioral func-
(2008). Treatment of challenging behavior exhibited tion of feeding problems as assessed by the questions
by children with prenatal drug exposure. Research in about behavioral function (QABF). Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 29, 582–594. Developmental Disabilities, 26, 399–408.
LaBelle, C. A., & Charlop-Christy, M. H. (2002). Matson, J. L., & Minshawi, N. F. (2007). Functional
Individualizing functional analysis to assess multiple assessment of challenging behavior: Toward a strategy
and changing functions of severe behavior problems in for applied settings. Research in Developmental
children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Disabilities, 28, 353–361.
Interventions, 4, 231–241. Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Factors associated
Lancaster, B. M., LeBlanc, L. A., Carr, J. E., Brenske, S., with the questions about behavior function for func-
Peet, M. M., & Culver, S. J. (2004). Functional analysis tional assessment of low and high rate challenging
4 Function of Challenging Behaviors 63

behaviors in adults with intellectual disability. reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Behavior Modification, 33, 207–219. 33, 13–27.
McAdam, D. B., DiCesare, A., Murphy, S., & Marshall, Reed, D. D., Pace, G. M., & Luiselli, J. K. (2009). An inves-
B. (2004). The influence of different therapists on tigation into the provision of choice in tangible condi-
functional analysis outcomes. Behavioral Interventions, tions of a functional analysis. Journal of Developmental
19, 39–44. and Physical Disabilities, 21, 485–491.
McLaughlin, T. F., Derby, K. M., Gwinn, M., Taitch, H., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Chambers, M. R. (2003). Functional
Bolich, B., Weber, K., et al. (2003). The effects of analysis and treatment of verbal perseverations dis-
active and violent play activities on brief functional played by an adult with autism. Journal of Applied
analysis outcomes. Journal of Developmental and Behavior Analysis, 36, 259–261.
Physical Disabilities, 15, 93–99. Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., Asmus, J. M., & Casey, S. D.
Moore, J. W., Fisher, W. W., & Pennington, A. (2004). (1998). Functional analysis and extinction of different
Systematic application and removal of protective behavior problems exhibited by the same individual.
equipment in the assessment of multiple topographies Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 475–478.
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Ringdahl, J. E., Christensen, T. J., & Boelter, E. W. (2009).
37, 73–77. Further evaluation of idiosyncratic functions for severe
Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Ellsworth, C. L., problem behavior: Aggression maintained by access
MacAleese, A. N., & Cleveland, J. M. (2008). to walks. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 275–283.
Functional analyses and treatment of precursor Roane, H. S., & Kelley, M. E. (2008). Decreasing problem
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, behavior associated with a walking program for an
97–105. individual with developmental and physical disabili-
Neidert, P. L., Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., & Hafen, M. ties. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,
(2010). Behavior analysis in intellectual and develop- 423–428.
mental disabilities. Psychological Services, 7, 103–113. Rooker, G. W., & Roscoe, E. M. (2005). Functional analy-
Noell, G. H., & Gansle, K. A. (2009). Introduction to sis of self-injurious behavior and its relation to self-
functional assessment. In A. Akin-Little, S. G. Little, restraint. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
M. A. Bray, & T. J. Kehle (Eds.), Behavioral interven- 537–542.
tions in schools: Evidence-based positive strategies Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Goh, H. L. (1998). A com-
(pp. 43–58). Washington, DC: American Psychological parison of noncontingent reinforcement and sensory
Association. extinction as treatments for self-injurious behavior.
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 635–646.
K., Cook, J., et al. (1991). A brief functional analysis Singh, A. N., Matson, J. L., Mouttapa, M., Pella, R. D.,
of aggressive and alternative behavior in an outclinic Hill, B. D., & Thorson, R. (2009). A critical item anal-
setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, ysis of the QABF: Development of a short form assess-
509–522. ment instrument. Research in Developmental
O’Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, G. (2000). Response covaria- Disabilities, 30, 782–792.
tion of escape-maintained aberrant behavior correlated Stewart, I., & Alderman, N. (2010). Active versus passive
with sleep deprivation. Research in Developmental management of post-acquired brain injury challenging
Disabilities, 21, 125–136. behaviour: A case study analysis of multiple operant
Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y., procedures in the treatment of challenging behaviour
& Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about behavioral maintained by negative reinforcement. Brain Injury,
functions (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func- 24, 1616–1627.
tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in Stokes, J. V., & Luiselli, J. K. (2009). Applied behavior
Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229. analysis assessment and intervention for health:
Pence, S. T., Roscoe, E. M., Bourret, J. C., & Ahearn, W. Threatening self-injury (rectal picking) in an adult
H. (2009). Relative contributions of three descriptive with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clinical Case Studies, 8,
methods: Implications for behavioral assessment. 38–47.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 425–446. Tang, J.-C., Patterson, T. G., & Kennedy, C. H. (2003).
Perrin, C. J., Perrin, S. H., Hill, E. A., & DiNovi, K. Identifying specific sensory modalities maintaining
(2008). Brief functional analysis and treatment of the stereotypy of students with multiple profound dis-
elopement in preschoolers with autism. Behavioral abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24,
Interventions, 23, 87–95. 433–451.
Peyton, R. T., Lindauer, S. E., & Richman, D. M. (2005). Tarbox, R. S. F., Wallace, M. D., & Williams, L. (2003).
The effects of directive and nondirective prompts on Assessment and treatment of elopement: A replication
noncompliant vocal behavior exhibited by a child with and extension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 36, 239–244.
251–255. Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H. L., & R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009).
Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental
matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic functional assessments of challenging behavior in
64 G.E. Lancioni et al.

children with autism. Journal of Developmental and (pp. 27–41). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514. Association.
Tiger, J. H., Fisher, W. W., Toussaint, K. A., & Kodak, T. White, P., O’Reilly, M., Fragale, C., Kang, S., Muhich, K.,
(2009). Progressing from initially ambiguous func- & Falcomata, T. (2011). An extended functional analy-
tional analyses: Three case examples. Research in sis protocol assesses the role of stereotypy in aggression
Developmental Disabilities, 30, 910–926. in two young children with autism spectrum disorder.
Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 784–789.
(1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control Wilder, D. A., Kellum, K. K., & Carr, J. E. (2000).
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Evaluation of satiation-resistant head rocking.
Analysis, 18, 343–351. Behavioral Interventions, 15, 71–78.
Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforce- Wilder, D. A., Normand, M., & Atwell, J. (2005).
ment: Implications for behavioral research in develop- Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for food
mental disabilities. Research in Developmental refusal and associated self-injury. Journal of Applied
Disabilities, 15, 187–207. Behavior Analysis, 38, 549–553.
Wallace, M. D., & Knights, D. J. (2003). An evaluation of Wilder, D. A., Register, M., Register, S., Bajagic, V., &
a brief functional analysis format within a vocational Neidert, P. L. (2009). Functional analysis and treatment
setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, of rumination using fixed-time delivery of a flavor spray.
125–128. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 877–882.
Watson, T. S., & Watson, T. S. (2009). Behavioral assess- Wood, B. K., Blair, K.-S. C., & Ferro, J. B. (2009). Young
ment in the schools. In A. Akin-Little, S. G. Little, children with challenging behavior: Function-based
M. A. Bray, & T. J. Kehle (Eds.), Behavioral interven- assessment and intervention. Topics in Early Childhood
tions in schools: Evidence-based positive strategies Special Education, 29, 68–78.
Populations and Problems
Evaluated with Functional 5
Assessment

John M. Huete, Patricia F. Kurtz, and R. Justin Boyd

B.F. Skinner (1953) first emphasized that a 2000; Singh, Donatelli, Best, & Williams, 1993).
scientific analysis of behavior requires an under- Direct methods of functional assessment are pro-
standing of the cause–effect relationship between cedures that utilize direct observation or experi-
behavior and the environmental conditions that mental manipulation for assessing the function of
are associated with its occurrence. Skinner termed behavior (Miltenberger, 1999). Included in direct
the understanding of this cause–effect relation- observational methods are assessment techniques
ship “a functional analysis of behavior.” As meth- that result in correlational outcomes, such as
ods for examining these relationships have gathering Antecedent–Behavior–Consequence
expanded, the analyses of behavioral contingen- (A–B–C) data (Bijou, Peterson, & Ault, 1968)
cies have come to be known more generally as and structured descriptive assessments (Anderson
functional assessment. The terms functional & Long, 2002), but also include experimental
assessment and functional analysis are frequently methods that result in the demonstration of
used interchangeably; however, it is useful to dis- cause–effect relationships (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer,
tinguish between these two terms, as they often Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994). The term
connote different meanings. Functional assess- functional analysis often is used to refer to more
ment typically is conceptualized as a broad cate- direct and experimental assessment procedures.
gory of procedures used to assess the function of The selection of procedures often is dependent
behavior. These include indirect methods and on a number of factors, including the individual
direct methods (Miltenberger, 1999). Indirect being assessed, the behavior of interest, resources
methods assess the function of behavior without available for assessment, context of the assess-
requiring any direct observation or experimental ment, and the cost–benefit analysis of conducting
manipulation of the behavior, such as structured the assessment (Johnston & O’Neill, 2001; Scott,
interviews (O’Neill, Horner, Sprague, Storey, & Anderson, Mancil, & Alter, 2009). Vollmer,
Newton, 1997) and questionnaires/checklists Marcus, Ringdahl, and Roane (1995) have pro-
(Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush, Smalls, & Vollmer, vided practical guidance on making decisions
about and progressing through functional assess-
ment procedures, suggesting that practitioners
J.M. Huete (*) • P.F. Kurtz • R.J. Boyd begin with more brief assessment procedures and
Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy proceed to more time-consuming, costly extended
Krieger Institute and The Johns Hopkins assessments as needed.
University School of Medicine, 707 N. Broadway,
Importantly, functional assessment should be
Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
e-mail: Huete@kennedykrieger.org; understood not as a specific tool, but rather as a
Kurtz@kennedykrieger.org; boydj@kennedykrieger.org broad approach to understanding the relationship

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 65


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_5,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
66 J.M. Huete et al.

of behavior to the factors associated with its functional assessment procedures were confined
occurrence. As such, the numerous functional to research study or specialized settings, such as
assessment procedures that have been developed, hospitals or residential homes. However, over the
ranging from caregiver interviews (O’Neill et al., past 25 years, interest in and use of functional
1997) to direct experimental testing of functional assessment has grown exponentially (Hanley,
hypotheses (Iwata et al., 1982/1994), are a testa- Iwata, & McCord, 2003). As a result, numerous
ment to the flexibility and applicability of the functional assessment procedures have been uti-
approach to understanding a growing list of lized with a variety of challenging behaviors
behaviors. Therefore, no specific procedure has demonstrated by individuals with IDD and
ownership over the term functional assessment Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). The purpose
nor is necessarily a better approach to understand- of this chapter is to succinctly describe the range
ing the function of behavior. Optimally, informa- of challenging behaviors and populations for
tion from a variety of functional assessment whom functional assessment has been used. The
procedures (e.g., interview, questionnaires, and primary focus will be on the application of func-
direct observation) is combined to best understand tional assessment to individuals diagnosed with
the function of behavior (Vollmer et al., 1995). ASD and IDD. The first part of this chapter briefly
While Skinner’s initial emphasis was to under- will review the variety of populations diagnosed
stand all behaviors of the organism in terms of a with ASD or IDD with whom functional assess-
functional analysis, specific technologies of func- ments have been used. Then, research on the use
tional assessment have evolved and grown mostly of functional assessment with common challeng-
in an attempt to understand problem and chal- ing behaviors most associated with individuals
lenging behaviors. Indeed, the flexibility of func- diagnosed with ASD and IDD will be reviewed.
tional assessment procedures has spurred its
application to the assessment and understanding
of a variety of problems, including the treatment Populations Evaluated
of substance use disorders (Budney & Higgins, with Functional Assessment
1998; Tuten, Jones, Ertel, Jakubowski, &
Sperlein, 2006), depression (Dougher & Hackbert, The specific populations of persons diagnosed
1994; Martell, 2008), and anxiety disorders with ASD and IDD for whom functional assess-
(Kearney, Cook, Wechsler, Haight, & Stowman, ment has been used are numerous and varied, and
2008; Virues-Ortega & Haynes, 2005). The hall- the groups themselves are not necessarily mutu-
mark of functional assessment, and what has ally exclusive of one another (Sturmey, 1996). A
separated it from other assessment approaches, is comprehensive review of each IDD population
its ability to lead to more effective interventions evaluated with functional assessment is beyond
for behavior as a result of identifying the function the scope of this chapter. Therefore, the popula-
of the behavior more precisely (Carr et al., 1999; tions discussed will be delineated based primar-
Didden, Duker, & Korzilius, 1997; Scotti, Evans, ily on the most common developmental diagnostic
Meyer, & Walker, 1991). categories for which functional assessments have
Even though functional assessment has dem- been used.
onstrated broad applications to a variety of prob- At first glance, it may appear that functional
lems, its history is most associated with assessment procedures are utilized almost exclu-
challenging behaviors of individuals with intel- sively with individuals diagnosed with intellectual
lectual and developmental disabilities (IDD). disability or mental retardation. This group cer-
From its beginnings as a conceptual framework to tainly is well represented in the functional assess-
understand the function of self-injurious behavior ment literature likely because they are at higher risk
(Carr, 1977), functional assessment has become for exhibiting more severe challenging behaviors
the benchmark approach to assessment of chal- (Matson & Rivet, 2008; McClintock, Hall, & Oliver,
lenging behavior for persons with IDD. Initially, 2003). Previous epidemiological studies have
5 Populations and Problems 67

supported that increased severity of intellectual erroneous, though, to conclude that all challenging
disability, such as a diagnosis of severe and behaviors of autistic individuals are nonsocially
profound mental retardation, is correlated with a maintained or the result of automatic reinforce-
higher incidence of certain severe challenging ment. Functional assessment procedures have
behaviors (Berkson, 1983; McClintock et al., been integral in demonstrating social operant
2003). However, not all individuals diagnosed functions for challenging behaviors of autistic
with ASD have a diagnosis of intellectual disabil- individuals often dismissed as nonsocial behav-
ity (Edelson, 2006; Hurley & Levitas, 2007; iors (Franco et al., 2009; Hausman, Kahng,
Matson & Rivet, 2008). Furthermore, functional Farrell, & Mongeon, 2009; Kuhn, Hardesty, &
assessments are commonly used to assess chal- Sweeney, 2009). For example, Franco et al.
lenging behaviors of individuals with mild to (2009) identified escape and access to tangible
moderate intellectual disabilities (Crockett & functions for vocal stereotypy of a 7-year-old boy
Hagopian, 2006; Hanley et al., 2003), as well as diagnosed with autism. Recent studies have fur-
typically developing individuals, such as children ther demonstrated social operant functions for
diagnosed with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity challenging behaviors that are associated with
Disorder (Northup & Gulley, 2001; Stahr, nonsocial behaviors, such as self-injurious behav-
Cushing, Lane, & Fox, 2006) or Emotional or ior or aggression functioning to allow access to
Behavioral Disorders (EBD: Nahgahgwon, ritualistic behaviors (Hausman et al., 2009;
Umbreit, Liaupsin, & Turton, 2010; Rasmussen Murphy, Macdonald, Hall, & Oliver, 2000).
& O’Neill, 2006; Restori, Gresham, Chang, Lee, These studies highlight the important contribu-
& Laija-Rodriquez, 2007). tions that functional assessment has made to the
In terms of specific developmental diagnoses, understanding of behavior in autism.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) repre- While there are several reported examples of
sents one of the larger diagnostic groupings of functional assessment with persons diagnosed
individuals for whom functional assessments are with autism, there are considerably fewer with
conducted. This is due largely to the various chal- other PDDs. Only a handful of published reports
lenging behaviors which are observed in many exist where functional assessment procedures are
individuals diagnosed with PDD (Weiss, Fiske, used to evaluate challenging behaviors of indi-
& Ferraioli, 2009). PDD, however, is a broad viduals diagnosed with Asperger’s Disorder
diagnostic category and includes specific diagno- (Clarke, Dunlap, & Vaughn, 1999; Tarbox,
ses of Autistic Disorder, Rett’s Disorder, Wallace, & William, 2003; Arvans & LeBlanc,
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, Asperger’s 2009; Lang, Didden et al., 2009; Lang, Didden
Disorder, and PDD Not Otherwise Specified et al., 2010), Rett’s Disorder (Oliver, Murphy,
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Much Crayton, & Corbett, 1993), and Childhood
of the functional assessment literature has been Disintegrative Disorder (Carter & Wheeler,
devoted to the challenging behaviors of individu- 2007). It may be that there are fewer reports of
als with autism that interfere with adaptive and functional assessments with these disorders due
daily living skills (LaBelle & Charlop-Christy, to their smaller prevalence compared to autism.
2002; Tarbox et al., 2009; Weiss et al., 2009). However, to the extent that individuals diagnosed
Specifically, severe challenging behaviors often with PDDs share similar challenging behaviors
are the primary targets of these assessments (e.g., stereotypies, self-injurious behaviors,
(O’Reilly et al., 2010). Many challenging behav- aggression, elopement), there is a robust litera-
iors of individuals diagnosed with autism, such ture supporting the efficacy of functional assess-
as inappropriate vocalizations, stereotyped move- ment of these challenging behaviors.
ments, and self-injury, are thought to be main- Down’s Syndrome is the most common chro-
tained by automatic reinforcement. Functional mosomal disorder associated with intellectual
assessments of these behaviors often support this disability (Deitz & Repp, 1989). While challeng-
conclusion (O’Reilly et al., 2010). It would be ing behaviors are not a necessary characteristic of
68 J.M. Huete et al.

Down’s Syndrome, recent evidence suggests a Reiss (2006) used experimental functional analy-
behavioral phenotype associated with avoidance ses to demonstrate social escape as a function for
behavior that increases the likelihood of chal- several challenging behaviors of 114 children
lenging behaviors (Feeley & Jones, 2006; Patti & diagnosed with Fragile X. Likewise, several stud-
Tsiouris, 2006). Several studies have documented ies have examined operant mechanisms for chal-
the utility of functional assessments for individu- lenging behaviors using functional analysis
als with Down’s Syndrome, addressing such technology in persons diagnosed with Cornelia
challenging behaviors as self-injurious behavior de Lange syndrome (Kern, Mauk, Marder, &
(O’Reilly, Murray, Lancioni, Sigafoos, & Lacey, Mace, 1995; Moss et al., 2005; Oliver et al.,
2003), aggression (McComas, Thompson, & 2006). Oliver et al. (2006) conducted experimen-
Johnson, 2003), and vocal stereotypy (Athens, tal functional analyses with 18 children ages 1 to
Vollmer, Sloman, & Pipkin, 2008). In one unique 16 years diagnosed with Cornelia de Lange,
application of functional assessment, Millichap where the influence of varying adult interaction
et al. (2003) utilized descriptive assessments to on subject social avoidance and self-injury was
demonstrate that certain behavioral excesses evaluated. For six participants, level of adult
exhibited by elderly persons diagnosed with attention was associated with social initiation and
Down’s Syndrome and dementia were function- termination behaviors, while for three others,
ally associated with environmental events, rather self-injury was associated with adult attention.
than randomly occurring. Using A–B–C data col- Collectively, these studies have demonstrated
lection techniques, the authors examined the rela- that challenging behaviors thought to be part of
tionship of certain behavioral excesses (e.g., the syndrome’s behavioral phenotype, including
crying, shouting, stereotypies, inappropriate self-injurious behavior, were sensitive to social
vocalizations, self-injury) to environmental contingencies particularly attention from others.
events for four adults ages 42 to 56 years living in Lesch–Nyhan syndrome is a genetic disorder
group or nursing homes. The behavioral excesses of purine metabolism in which severe self-injuri-
observed of these participants were significantly ous self-biting is a hallmark characteristic
correlated with increases in social contact from (Nyhan, 2002). Research using functional assess-
peers and staff. ment has demonstrated social attention functions
In addition to PDDs and Down’s syndrome, for self-injurious behaviors in individuals diag-
there are several genetic disorders that are associ- nosed with Lesch–Nyhan syndrome (Bergen,
ated with challenging behaviors for which func- Holbern, & Scott-Huyghebaert, 2002; Hall,
tional assessment has proved useful. Some of the Oliver, & Murphy, 2001). These findings have
most notable disorders include Fragile X, been further supported with evidence that inter-
Cornelia de Lange, Lesch–Nyhan, Prader–Willi, ventions using manipulations of social attention,
Smith–Magenis, and Angelman syndromes. such as extinction and differential attention, have
Although as a whole these genetic disorders are decreased the occurrence of self-injury in indi-
relatively rare, it is surprising that few published viduals diagnosed with Lesch–Nyhan syndrome
reports exist regarding the use of functional (Olson & Houlihan, 2000).
assessments for these populations. This is unfor- Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) is a congenital
tunate given the relationship of these genetic dis- disorder in which obsession with food is one key
orders to challenging behaviors. As previously behavioral characteristic, as well as several other
stated, it may be falsely assumed that challenging challenging behaviors, including skin picking and
behaviors in these populations are inherently temper tantrums (Dykens, Cassidy, & DeVries,
associated with the syndrome. However, several 2011). Research examining skin picking in PWS
functional assessment studies have demonstrated using functional assessments has shown that the
that many of these challenging behaviors are sen- behavior is associated with self-stimulation
sitive to and vary as a function of environmental (Didden, Korzilius, Curfs, 2007; Lang et al.,
changes. For example, Hall, DeBernardis, and 2010; Radstaake et al., 2011; Slifer, Iwata, &
5 Populations and Problems 69

Dorsey, 1984), arousal reduction (Radstaake non-IDD populations, usually individuals with
et al., 2011), as well as attention from others and severe psychiatric disorder, such as Borderline
escape from work (Stokes & Luiselli, 2009). Personality Disorder (Andover & Gibb, 2010;
Woodcock, Oliver, and Humphreys (2009) used Kerr & Muehlenkamp, 2010). However, SIB is
interviews and questionnaires to assess the func- most notable as a challenging behavior for per-
tion of temper tantrums in persons diagnosed sons with IDD, where prevalence recently has
with PWS. Temper tantrums were more likely to been estimated at 4.9% in adults (Cooper et al.,
occur following a change in routine. 2009), but likely is higher for individuals diag-
Finally, functional assessments have been nosed with ASD (Oliver & Richards, 2010).
used to evaluate self-injurious and aggressive Baghdadli, Pascal, Grisi, and Aussiloux (2003)
behaviors of individuals diagnosed with Smith– found a SIB prevalence rate of 53% in a sample
Magenis syndrome (SMS) and Angelman syn- of 222 children under the age of 7 years diag-
drome (Bass & Speak, 2005; Strachan et al., nosed with autism.
2009; Taylor & Oliver, 2008). In each of these SIB often is discussed as a class of behaviors,
studies, functional assessments identified social which may falsely suggest homogeneity of these
operants for the challenging behaviors, including behaviors. The topographies of SIB in individu-
attention from others and escape from demands. als with IDD, and ultimately their behavioral
In summary, functional assessment is applica- functions, actually are varied and include head
ble to a broad range of individuals diagnosed with banging, head hitting, body hitting, self-scratch-
ASD and IDD. The key feature of functional ing, self-biting, eye poking, and hair pulling.
assessment is that the results more directly suggest These individual topography categories can be
intervention for specific target challenging behav- delineated even further by identifying specific
iors (Carr et al., 1999; Didden et al., 1997; Scotti behaviors within a category. For example, head
et al., 1991). One important and prominent theme hitting can be defined as face slapping with an
from the functional assessment literature with open hand, head punching with a closed fist, or
ASD and IDD populations is that social operant head hitting with an object.
functions have been demonstrated for certain chal- The personal and societal costs of SIB in indi-
lenging behaviors, when perhaps previously they viduals with IDD prompted researchers in the
were dismissed as nonsocial or nonfunctional. As 1960s and 1970s to develop better interventions
a result of identifying social functions for these and treatments (Carr, 1977). An outgrowth of this
challenging behaviors, more acceptable, rein- push was an emphasis on identifying the function
forcement-based interventions, rather than pun- of SIB, which would suggest and lead to better
ishment-based interventions, tend to be selected interventions. Carr (1977) initially summarized
(Pelios, Morren, Tesch, & Axelrod, 1999). several functional hypotheses for SIB, noting that
more effective interventions were needed and
dependent on understanding the motivational
aspects of SIB. These hypotheses included SIB as
Challenging Behaviors Evaluated a learned operant, maintained by positive social
with Functional Assessment reinforcement or negative reinforcement; a means
of providing sensory stimulation; or the product
Self-Injurious Behavior of aberrant physiological processes (Carr, 1977).
Drawing from these hypotheses, Iwata and
Perhaps no other challenging behavior was more colleagues (1982/1994) described the first exper-
responsible for ushering in the use of functional imental procedure for evaluating the operant
assessment than self-injurious behavior (SIB). functions of self-injury, demonstrating that for
SIB is defined as any behavior directed towards six of the nine participants, SIB was associated
self that results in tissue damage or injury to the with a specific stimulus condition. The initial
person (Tate & Baroff, 1966). SIB is observed in formulation of functional assessment for SIB
70 J.M. Huete et al.

(Iwata et al., 1982/1994) consisted of using history of SIB to be established in demand or


analog conditions to experimentally evaluate the instructional situations, because these situations
effects of contingent delivery of attention, escape are not as prevalent for this age group. Similarly,
from academic demands, and being alone on Didden et al. (2007) found that self-injurious skin
the occurrence of SIB. These conditions separately picking in persons diagnosed with PWS is pre-
tested if SIB was functionally related to social dominantly maintained by automatic reinforce-
positive reinforcement, social negative reinforce- ment and likely associated with the compulsive
ment, or automatic reinforcement, respectively. behaviors typical of Prader–Willi.
Specifically, rates of SIB which were consistently The single-subject, multielement experimen-
elevated when compared to the other conditions in tal design used by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) is one
the assessment were considered to be demonstra- of the more common methods for assessment of
tions of a causal, functional relationship between SIB, but is just one of many approaches to func-
SIB and that consequence. For example, if SIB tional assessment. Additionally, this method tests
occurred at higher rates in the social disapproval, more general hypotheses regarding maintaining
or attention, condition, then a conclusion of the variables for SIB. The methods and sophistica-
functional analysis would be that SIB was associ- tion with which the function of SIB is assessed
ated with periods of low attention and reinforced have greatly expanded over the past 25 years. In
by attention from others as a consequence. particular, functional assessments are not confined
Functional assessment has broadened the to evaluating general hypotheses regarding the
understanding of SIB by demonstrating that many function of challenging behavior (i.e., attention,
forms of the behavior were maintained entirely, escape, tangible objects) and are increasingly
or in part, by social consequences. To illustrate guided by more specific hypotheses, which pro-
this, Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, and Zarcone (1994) vide improved information regarding the func-
conducted experimental functional analyses with tion of challenging behavior and thus appropriate
152 subjects exhibiting SIB in order to under- intervention. For example, following initial
stand the epidemiology of SIB functions. Results inconclusive functional analysis results, Hausman
showed that SIB was maintained by social posi- et al. (2009) utilized an A-B-A-B reversal design
tive reinforcement for 26.3% of cases, social neg- to demonstrate that a child’s SIB and other chal-
ative reinforcement for 38.1% of cases, automatic lenging behaviors were maintained by access to
reinforcement for 25.7% of cases, and multiple rituals, specifically opening and closing cabinet
factors (e.g., social positive and social negative doors. Similarly, Hagopian, Wilson, and Wilder
reinforcement) for 5.3% of cases; results were (2001), after observing that SIB and other chal-
undifferentiated for the remaining 4.6% of cases. lenging behaviors for a 6-year-old child with
While the Iwata et al. (1994) sample was hetero- autism occurred at highest rates during the toy
geneous with respect to age and diagnoses, addi- play control condition, included an escape from
tional similar epidemiological evaluations provide attention condition during subsequent functional
valuable information about the distribution of analysis. The function of the child’s challenging
SIB functions for specific populations (Didden behavior was maintained, in part, by social avoid-
et al., 2007; Kurtz et al., 2003). For example, ance. In a couple of other examples, additional
Kurtz et al. (2003) reviewed functional analysis modified functional analyses have demonstrated
results for 30 children under the age of 5 years SIB to be maintained by increased compliance
who exhibited SIB and found a considerably with an individual’s demands, or requests
lower percentage of cases where SIB was main- (Bowman, Fisher, Thompson, & Piazza, 1997),
tained by social negative reinforcement (3.4%) as well as by a specific quality of attention, such
and a higher percentage where SIB was main- as physical contact (Richman & Hagopian, 1999).
tained by social positive reinforcement (37.9%). In each of these cases, evaluating more specific
The authors reasoned that for very young chil- hypotheses about SIB resulted in improved
dren, there likely is not as much opportunity for a assessment outcomes and better interventions.
5 Populations and Problems 71

One issue that often arises in the functional identifying the most effective interventions for
assessment of SIB is the time and cost of con- SIB (Carr et al., 1999; Didden et al., 1997; Scotti
ducting experimental functional analyses et al., 1991), there continues to be concern regard-
(Axelrod, 1987; Oswald, Ellis, Singh, & Singh, ing the use of procedures that evoke SIB (Hastings
1994). In many settings, such as schools, & Noone, 2005). Direct functional assessment of
resources may not be readily available to conduct SIB, whether from observation or experimental
these analyses. Increasingly, indirect functional manipulation, involves allowing a potentially
assessment measures are used in these settings to harmful behavior to occur and in the case of
offset the time and cost of conducting direct func- structured descriptive and experimental analyses,
tional assessment. Several instruments, including purposely introducing conditions that set the
the Functional Assessment Interview (FAI: occasion for SIB. Iwata et al. (1982/1994) set a
O’Neill et al., 1997) and Questions About standard for ethically conducting direct func-
Behavioral Function questionnaire (QABF: tional analysis of SIB by stipulating termination
Paclawskyj et al., 2000), have been used exten- criteria for the assessment and having medical
sively and possess good psychometric properties personnel involved, examining each subject prior
(Freeman, Walker, & Kaufman, 2007; Nicholson, to and during each assessment. The authors’
Konstantinidi, & Furniss, 2006; Paclawskyj, focus was to ensure that each subject was not
Matson, Rush, Smalls, & Vollmer, 2001; Stage exposed to any greater risk than would occur in
et al., 2006). However, indirect functional assess- the natural environment.
ment measures do not always correlate with the One method to reduce the risk of injury while
results of experimental functional analyses. As conducting direct functional assessment of SIB is
such, some researchers have concluded that indi- to utilize protective equipment, such as padded
rect functional assessment measures should be helmets or arm splints, in the assessment.
used as a preliminary step to direct functional However, protective equipment itself can either
analysis procedures (Alter, Conroy, Mancil, & inhibit the occurrence of SIB or reduce its occur-
Haydon, 2008; Paclawskyj et al., 2001; Stage rence, thus resulting in inconclusive functional
et al., 2006). analysis results and inhibiting evaluation of the
Brief functional analysis methods also are operant function of SIB (Borrero, Vollmer,
available (Kahng & Iwata, 1999; Northup, Wacker, Wright, Lerman, & Kelley, 2002; Le & Smith,
Sasso, & Steege, 1991), but still require some 2002). Another method for conducting functional
time and expertise to conduct. Additionally, non- analyses of SIB safely involves the assessment of
experimental functional assessments have been precursor behaviors (Bergen et al., 2002;
used, particularly structured descriptive assess- Langdon, Carr, & Owen-DeSchryver, 2008;
ments, which generally produce similar results as Najdowski, Wallace, Ellsworth, MacAleese, &
analog functional analyses (Anderson & Long, Cleveland, 2008). Specifically, precursor behav-
2002; Sasso, Reimers, Cooper, & Wacker, 1992). iors are behaviors that reliably precede and serve
However, similar to indirect functional assess- the same function as the challenging behavior,
ment methods, brief functional assessment results such as SIB. For example, a less severe behavior
do not always correlate with the results of such as screaming or stomping of a foot may
extended experimental analyses and may tend to typically precede head banging. In this scenario,
overestimate attention as a function of challeng- the dependent variable is screaming or foot
ing behavior (Kahng & Iwata, 1999; Lerman & stomping rather than head banging. Functional
Iwata, 1993; Thompson & Iwata, 2007). analyses that make precursor behaviors the
Another related issue in the functional assess- dependent variable can assess the function of SIB
ment of SIB is how to ethically and safely con- without observing its occurrence by instead
duct assessment, particularly experimental assessing the function of the precursor behavior.
analyses. Although there is little disagreement In this way, the functional analysis may be con-
regarding the utility of functional assessments in ducted more safely for the individual; however,
72 J.M. Huete et al.

limitations of this approach exist as identifying objects, jumping, and inappropriate vocalizations
reliable precursors that are in the same response (Cunningham & Schreibman, 2008; Goldman
class as SIB can be difficult. et al., 2009). Although most stereotypies are con-
In summary, SIB is common and one of the sidered innocuous, in many instances, stereotypy
more severe challenging behaviors exhibited by can significantly interfere with skill development,
individuals diagnosed with ASD and IDD. can be self-directed and classified as self-injuri-
Functional assessment procedures have aided in ous, and can result in negative social conse-
the understanding of SIB, which has lead to quences for the individual, such as social
improved interventions. While direct functional avoidance and isolation (Jones, 1991; Matson
assessment, particularly experimental functional et al., 2009; Matson, Kiely, & Bamburg, 1997;
analyses, provides the most valid assessment Matson & Rivet, 2008). As a result, stereotypies
results for SIB, there are several factors that should often have been targets of functional assessments
be considered in determining an assessment (Kennedy, 2007; Patel, Carr, Kim, Robles, &
approach. Ultimately, practitioners should make Eastridge, 2000; Vollmer et al., 1995).
decisions based on a cost–benefit analysis, which The initial and prevailing conceptualization of
includes weighing the advantages of conducting stereotyped movements is that these behaviors
direct assessment of SIB function (i.e., more accu- serve an automatic function for the individual,
rate and thorough assessment of function) with usually in the form of sensory stimulation
the cost of conducting the assessment (i.e., safety (Lovaas, Newsom, & Hickman, 1987; Rapp &
concerns, staff time). If direct functional assess- Vollmer, 2005). There exists a considerable
ment is likely to result in improved intervention research literature utilizing functional assess-
that can reduce or eliminate SIB, the short-term ments with stereotypies that supports the theory
costs of the procedures may be outweighed by the that stereotypies are maintained by sensory con-
longer-term benefits for the individual. sequences or self-stimulation (Dawson, Matson,
& Cherry, 1998; Piazza, Adelinis, Hanley, Goh,
& Delia, 2000; Rapp, 2006). The sensory func-
Stereotyped Behaviors tion of stereotypy often is demonstrated by pro-
viding the individual with items or activities that
Stereotyped behaviors, or stereotypies, are produce a similar sensory consequence as the ste-
defined as repetitive, rhythmic, and seemingly reotypy, which results in reductions in stereotypy
purposeless behaviors that tend to be inappropri- (Piazza et al., 2000). Additionally, research has
ate (Goldman et al., 2009; Turner, 1999). shown that other severe challenging behaviors
Stereotyped movements and interests are a core are maintained by an individual’s access to ste-
diagnostic feature of ASD (American Psychiatric reotypy, suggesting that stereotypy is a reinforcer
Association, 2000), as well as a common behav- itself (Falcomata, Roane, Feeney, & Stephenson,
ioral feature of many IDDs (Dominick, Davis, 2010; Hanley, Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg,
Lainhart, Tager-Flusberg, & Folstein, 2007; Hill 2000; Hausman et al., 2009).
& Furniss, 2006). Prevalence estimates vary, but However, it should be noted that there are sev-
suggest that as many as two-thirds of individuals eral examples of stereotypies being multiply
diagnosed with IDDs exhibit some form of ste- maintained and varying as a function of socially
reotypy, with estimates for individuals diagnosed mediated consequences (Ahearn, Clark, Gardener,
with ASD perhaps higher (Berkson, 1983; Chung, & Dube, 2003; Durand & Carr, 1987).
Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000; Durand and Carr (1987) examined the relation-
Goldman et al., 2009; Matson, Dempsey, & ship of stereotypies (i.e., hand flapping and body
Fodstad, 2009; Matson & Rivet, 2008). Some rocking) to instructional situations in three chil-
common stereotypies include body rocking, dren diagnosed with PDDs. Stereotypy increased
head shaking, hand and arm flapping, finger with the presentation of difficult tasks and
tapping, clapping, spinning objects, shaking decreased with the termination of these tasks.
5 Populations and Problems 73

These results demonstrated that stereotyped Ringdahl, 2001). Dawson et al. (1998) examined
movements, in part, functioned as a means of the functions of various challenging behaviors
escape from non-preferred tasks. In these exam- via QABF (Paclawskyj et al., 2000) in a sample
ples, stereotypy originally may have served a self- of 36 adults diagnosed with autism, PDD-NOS,
stimulatory function, but obtained socially or mental retardation. Results showed that aggres-
mediated functions after a history was established sion was primarily endorsed as maintained by
of the behavior resulting in social consequences, social consequences, such as access to attention
such as escape from non-preferred situations or or tangibles. Marcus et al. (2001), using experi-
attention from others (Durand & Carr, 1987). For mental functional analyses in a sample of 8 chil-
example, Goh, Iwata, Shore, and DeLeon (1995) dren and adolescents diagnosed with
conducted experimental functional analyses with developmental disabilities, found aggression to
12 adults diagnosed with developmental disabili- be maintained by social positive and social nega-
ties who engaged in stereotypic hand mouthing. tive consequences for each individual.
For 2 of the participants, hand mouthing was Much of the recent functional assessment lit-
maintained by social positive reinforcement in the erature on aggression has focused on idiosyncratic
form of attention and access to preferred items. variables associated with its occurrence. For
example, aggression has been demonstrated to be
maintained by such specific consequences as
Aggression access to certain conversation topics (Roscoe,
Kindle, & Pence, 2010), access to walks (Ringdahl,
Aggressive behaviors include inappropriate Christensen, & Boelter, 2009), and wheelchair
physical contact directed towards another person movement (DeLeon, Kahng, Rodriguez-Catter,
that may result in harm or injury (Luiselli, 2009). Sveinsdóttir, & Sadler, 2003). Adelinis and
Common aggressive behaviors observed in ASD Hagopian (1999) conducted a functional analysis
and IDD populations include hitting, kicking, bit- with a 27-year-old male diagnosed with autism
ing, pinching, scratching, hair pulling, grabbing, and moderate mental retardation and found that
and choking (Luiselli, 2009). Aggression is esti- his aggressive behaviors were associated with the
mated to occur in up to 22% of individuals diag- use of “don’t” instructions, especially those that
nosed with IDD (Gardner & Cole, 1990; Jacobson, interrupted preferred activities. Kahng, Leak, Vu,
1982) and can have significant adverse conse- and Mishler (2008) demonstrated that the aggres-
quences for individuals with ASD and IDD, often sion of a 16-year-old boy occurred when mechan-
interfering with learning and skill acquisition, ical arm splints, which were an intervention for
increasing the likelihood of social isolation, and SIB, were removed. Functional analysis indicated
often leading to more restrictive school and home that the participant’s aggression was maintained
placements (Luiselli, 2009). Because of the seri- by access to his arm splints. Similarly, aggression
ous consequences of aggression, it is not surpris- has been shown to follow blocking of and be
ing that much of the functional assessment maintained by access to stereotyped movements
literature has been devoted to this challenging (Hagopian & Toole, 2009).
behavior. In a review of 277 studies using func- While most functional assessments of aggres-
tional analysis methodology to evaluate challeng- sive behaviors identify social operant functions
ing behavior, Hanley et al. (2003) found that for the behaviors, there have been examples of
aggression was second only to SIB as the most aggression maintained by automatic reinforce-
evaluated challenging behavior, occurring in ment (Luiselli, 2009; Thompson, Fisher, Piazza,
40.8% of the sample. & Kuhn, 1998). Thompson et al. (1998) described
Unlike SIB, functional assessments of aggres- the functional analysis of a 7-year-old boy diag-
sive behaviors primarily have identified social nosed with pervasive developmental disorder
factors mediating their occurrence (Hanley et al., and severe mental retardation, whose aggressive
2003; Marcus, Vollmer, Swanson, Roane, & chin pressing was maintained independent of
74 J.M. Huete et al.

social consequences. Subsequent assessment of demonstrated that property destruction was part
this behavior suggested that it was maintained by of a response chain that produced broken object
tactile stimulation (i.e., the sensation of rubbing pieces with which subjects engaged in automati-
his chin). cally maintained stereotypy (i.e., tapping the bro-
ken pieces). Intervention that provided the
subjects with already broken materials or pre-
Property Destruction ferred toys resulted in significant reductions in
property destruction.
Property destruction generally is defined as any
behavior that results in damage to or destruction
of the physical environment and includes such Tantrums
behaviors as throwing objects, hitting or kicking
objects, ripping or tearing paper/books, slam- Tantrum behaviors can diminish the positive
ming doors, and banging on furniture, to name a interactions between children and their parents
few (Ebanks & Fisher, 2003; Fisher, Lindauer, (Vollmer, Northup, Ringdahl, LeBlanc, &
Alterson, & Thompson, 1998; Petursdottir, Esch, Chauvin, 1996). Such behaviors can place a strain
Sautter, & Stewart, 2010; Schieltz et al., 2010). on parents and caregivers by causing them to
Property destruction is a noted common chal- avoid community or other social outings and
lenging behavior of persons diagnosed with ASD activities with their child (Patterson, Chamberlain,
and IDD (Smith & Matson, 2010). Although it & Reid, 1982). Although tantrum behavior may
has been evaluated in numerous functional assess- include a constellation of disruptive and destruc-
ment studies (Hanley et al., 2003), property tive behaviors, it usually is operationally defined
destruction seldom has been the sole subject of as periods of screaming and/or crying with or
assessment and more often is included with other without other problem behaviors. Current litera-
severe challenging behaviors often under the ture on tantrum behavior suggests that tantrums
general category of destructive behaviors most commonly co-occur with other disruptive or
(Harding et al., 2001; Schieltz et al., 2010; destructive behaviors in children with ASD
Tarbox, Wallace, Tarbox, Landaburu, & Williams, (Baghdadli et al., 2003; Fox, Keller, Grede, &
2004) and occasionally under the category of dis- Bartosz, 2007; Petursdottir et al., 2010) and that
ruptions (Kuhn et al., 2009). This is likely due to a particularly strong relation exists between
the presumption that property destruction is in aggressive behavior and tantrums (Matson, 2009).
the same response class or is part of a similar This point is evident in the literature as few stud-
response chain as other challenging behaviors ies have focused on tantrum behavior alone, but
(Fisher et al., 1998; Harding et al., 2001). in conjunction with other destructive or socially
In general, results of functional assessments disruptive behaviors (e.g., Campbell & Lutzker,
with property destruction have identified social 1993; Luiselli & Murbach, 2002).
operant functions for the behavior, including In terms of assessment, variations of func-
escape from aversive tasks (Ebanks & Fisher, tional analysis procedures have been used to eval-
2003), access to preferred items (Reed, Pace, & uate tantrums. For example, Wilder, Chen, Atwell,
Luiselli, 2009), and attention (Harding et al., Pritchard, and Weinstein (2006) evaluated tan-
2001). Some unique findings, though, regarding trums associated with transitions. In addition,
property destruction have been made via func- descriptive functional assessments of tantrum
tional assessment. For example, Bowman et al. behaviors (Campbell & Lutzker, 1993; Luiselli &
(1997) found that property destruction, along Murbach, 2002) and discrete screaming behavior
with other challenging behaviors, was associated (Graff, Lineman, Libby, & Ahearn, 1999) in nat-
with increased compliance with the demands uralistic settings have been described in the liter-
of two children diagnosed with developmental ature. Campbell and Lutzker (1993) utilized
disabilities. Additionally, Fisher et al. (1998) descriptive functional assessment procedures to
5 Populations and Problems 75

evaluate the environmental determinants of severe clothing, etc., occurs in up to 25% of individuals
tantrum behaviors which often co-occurred with with IDD (Danford & Huber, 1982), but is one of
property destruction for an 8-year-old boy with the most difficult problem behaviors to assess
ASD who was reported to have no existing func- and effectively treat (Favell, McGimsey, & Schell,
tional communication skills. Given that the par- 1982; Madden, Russo, & Cataldo, 1980). Pica
ticipant’s challenging behaviors were most places the individual at risk of illness, medical
problematic at home, the researchers trained the complications, or injury due to choking, intesti-
child’s mother to collect A–B–C data and subse- nal obstruction, bowel perforation, or parasitic
quently developed functional hypotheses from infection (Fisher et al., 1994; Foxx & Martin,
those data. For this participant, tantrum behavior 1975). Pica presents significant challenges for
was hypothesized to be maintained by both nega- caregivers and service providers, often necessi-
tive reinforcement (i.e., escape from demand sit- tating the provision of continuous supervision,
uations) and positive reinforcement (i.e., access use of protective equipment and/or punishment
to tangible items). Following functional commu- procedures, and a more secure residential place-
nication training and an activity planning compo- ment (Mace & Knight, 1986).
nent, significant reductions in both the frequency The application of functional analysis meth-
and duration of this child’s tantrum behavior were ods to this serious problem has led to new
achieved. approaches to both assessment and treatment. In
In addressing discrete screaming behavior, an early study, Mace and Knight (1986) exam-
Graff et al. (1999) used descriptive functional ined both the amount of social interaction pro-
assessment procedures with a young girl with vided and the types of protective equipment used
ASD and IDD in her school setting. After exam- with an adult with severe pica (i.e., ingestion of
ining A–B–C data collected by the participant’s ripped/shredded clothing or other materials), who
teacher and determining that no positive correla- resided in an institution and was prescribed a hel-
tion existed between screaming and any specific met with face shield to prevent pica. In the first
antecedent, the researchers hypothesized that analysis, three levels of staff–client social inter-
screaming was maintained by automatic rein- action during vocational work were assessed: fre-
forcement. This information was also consistent quent interaction (close, continuous staff
with informal functional assessment interviews supervision), limited interaction (staff physically
conducted with the participant’s mother. Upon distant, attention provided on fixed time, 3-min
conclusion of these methods and a functional schedule), and no interaction (staff distant, turned
analysis which corroborated the descriptive away, no contact). In the second analysis, three
assessment hypothesis, the researchers imple- forms of protective equipment were assessed:
mented a function-based intervention which helmet with face shield, helmet without face
was effective in reducing screaming for this shield, and no helmet. The two sets of assessment
participant. results were used to develop a comprehensive
intervention that surprisingly involved limited
levels of staff interaction and no protective hel-
Pica met; this intervention was effective in achieving
substantial reductions in pica.
Although the majority of studies using functional Functional analysis procedures have been
assessment methods have targeted severe chal- modified to permit safe occurrence and direct
lenging behaviors such as self-injury, aggression, observation of pica (Piazza, Hanley & Fisher,
and disruptive behavior (Hanley et al., 2003), the 1996; Piazza, Fisher, Hanley, & Lindauer, 1998;
procedures have been modified or refined in vari- Piazza, Roane, Keeney, Boney, & Abt, 2002). For
ous ways to assess other challenging behaviors. example, Piazza et al. (1998) conducted func-
For example, pica, the ingestion of inedible tional analyses of pica for 4 children admitted to
objects such as rocks, paper, glass, cigarette butts, an inpatient unit. Sessions were conducted in
76 J.M. Huete et al.

experimental rooms where the individual was reported in individuals with IDDs (Jacobson,
permitted to engage in pica within a “baited” 1982; Lowe et al., 2007). Elopement not only
environment; that is, the session room was baited may interfere with participation in educational,
with pica items deemed medically safe and/or social, or vocational activities but may place the
items that simulated pica items (e.g., bits of flour individual at great risk for injury (e.g., running
tortilla to simulate paint chips) that the individual into traffic) (Piazza et al., 1997). In cases where
would typically seek to ingest in home or com- elopement persists or is dangerous to the indi-
munity settings. Then, analog functional analysis vidual, additional staff support or restrictive resi-
conditions as described by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) dential care may be required to minimize risk.
were conducted. As expected, pica was main- Similar to the assessment of pica, research in this
tained by automatic reinforcement for two of four area has been limited. It is difficult to assess and
participants. Interestingly, for two participants, understand the contextual variables surrounding
pica was maintained at least in part by social con- the occurrence of elopement (i.e., what is the
sequences, specifically access to adult attention individual running to or from), and there are
and tangibles. significant challenges in safely assessing the
The baited environment approach has also behavior as permitting even one occurrence can
been utilized with individuals displaying ciga- be dangerous. In the first application of functional
rette butt pica (Piazza et al., 1996; Goh, Iwata, & analysis procedures to this problem, Piazza et al.
Kahng, 1999). In most of these cases, functional (1997) assessed elopement in three children with
analysis outcomes indicated pica was maintained intellectual disabilities admitted to an inpatient
by automatic reinforcement, and reinforcement- unit. Functional analysis procedures (Iwata et al.,
based treatments were effective in reducing pica. 1982/1994) were modified such that attention,
However, Goh et al. (1999) noted that in one case, tangible, and demand sessions were conducted in
despite clear functional assessment findings, the hospital, in settings which simulated the natu-
multiple attempts at intervention were unsuccess- ral environment where elopement was likely to
ful, necessitating use of preventive measures for occur for each participant, but were safe for
pica. More recently, blocking the individual’s repeated sessions. For example, elopement for
attempts at pica has been assessed within the two participants was assessed in a restricted two-
baited environment (Hagopian & Adelinis, 2001; room area, where the participant could leave one
McCord, Grosser, Iwata, & Powers, 2005). Thus, room and enter the other to access consequences
assessment of pica via functional analysis has specified for that condition. For a third partici-
been critical in moving researchers and practitio- pant, elopement was assessed in a large common
ners away from use of protective equipment and area where exit was not available, as he report-
punishment to development of reinforcement- edly engaged in elopement in open spaces. Across
based, less restrictive treatments, such as provi- all conditions, elopement was allowed to occur,
sion of stimuli matched to reinforcing properties corresponding contingencies were applied, and
of pica items, or provision of noncontingent rein- the individual was then brought back to the start-
forcement, for both automatically and socially ing location with minimal attention. In cases
maintained pica. where functional analysis results were unclear,
reinforcer assessments were conducted to clarify
behavioral function. Assessment results indicated
Elopement that for two children, elopement was maintained
by access to tangible items, and for a third par-
Functional analysis procedures have also been ticipant, elopement was maintained by access to
successfully applied to elopement, i.e., leaving adult attention. While effective treatments were
an area without supervision or caregiver consent developed for these participants, generality of
(Tarbox et al., 2003). Although prevalence esti- results was limited, as assessment and treatment
mates are lacking, this behavior is frequently were conducted in an inpatient setting.
5 Populations and Problems 77

In an effort to improve external validity of this Feeding Problems


assessment, more recent studies have focused on
conducting functional analyses of elopement in Functional analyses have also been successfully
natural settings while maintaining a high level of applied to the assessment of feeding problems.
safety and experimental control. For example, Feeding difficulties can take a variety of forms,
Tarbox et al. (2003) replicated Piazza et al.’s including food refusal (refusal to eat all or most
(1997) procedures but conducted all sessions in foods presented), food selectivity (consumption
an enclosed mall and in a vocational day pro- of select foods or food groups and refusal of oth-
gram, using a confederate to monitor the partici- ers), and mealtime problem behaviors (turning
pant and maintain safety. Additionally, to further head away from spoon, crying, throwing food,
simulate conditions in the natural environment, vomiting) (Piazza & Roane, 2009). The preva-
parents were trained to serve as therapists during lence of feeding problems among children with
all assessment and treatment sessions. Results of intellectual and developmental disabilities is esti-
the three functional analyses indicated that elope- mated at 35% (Dahl & Sundelin, 1986); however,
ment was maintained by positive reinforcement a very high prevalence, as high as 90%, is reported
in the form of access to a toy store, access to tan- in children with autism (Kodak & Piazza, 2008).
gible items, and access to adult attention for the It is important to note that for many children
three respective participants. Similarly, Kodak, exhibiting food refusal, there is a comorbid medi-
Grow, and Northup (2004) conducted a func- cal condition such as gastroesophageal reflux,
tional analysis of elopement outdoors on a kick- cardiopulmonary condition, neurological condi-
ball field with a 5-year-old girl diagnosed with tion, food allergy, or anatomical anomaly
ADHD. The participant’s elopement was shown (Williams, Field, & Sieverling, 2010). Feeding
to be sensitive to adult attention and was treated problems can have a serious impact on child
effectively with noncontingent reinforcement and health; typically reported complications include
time-out. Most recently, Lang, Davis et al., (2010) malnourishment, lack of growth, failure to thrive,
assessed a student’s elopement in two school set- and reliance on supplemental feeding via gastros-
tings, the student’s classroom and a resource tomy tube (Williams et al., 2010).
room where discrete trial training was conducted, One focus of the research on feeding problems
using procedures similar to those described in children with intellectual disabilities has been
by Piazza et al. (1997) and Tarbox et al. (2003). on functional analysis of inappropriate mealtime
Interestingly, the two functional analyses behaviors. In the first such study, Piazza et al.
identified different maintaining variables for (2003) initially conducted descriptive assessments
elopement across the two settings: elopement at home with six parents and their children to iden-
within the classroom was maintained by access to tify potential reinforcers for inappropriate meal-
a preferred activity (watching videos), while time behaviors such as head turning, batting at
elopement from the resource room was main- the spoon, throwing food/utensils, crying, expel-
tained by access to adult attention. ling food, vomiting, self-injury, and aggression.
Taken together, the above studies illustrate the Descriptive assessment results indicated that all
utility and flexibility of functional analysis proce- parents provided attention, escape, and/or tangi-
dures in identifying behavioral function for differ- bles contingent upon food refusal behaviors.
ent forms of problem behavior, Indeed, in a recent Additionally, following inappropriate behavior, all
review of the treatment literature on elopement, parents provided attention and removed food, and
Lang, Rispoli et al., (2009) noted that not only was half of the parents provided access to a tangible
elopement usually maintained by operant contin- item. Based on these data, modified functional
gencies but that function-based interventions (i.e., analysis procedures described by Iwata et al.
those based on functional analysis results) were (1982/1994) were used to assess food refusal
most effective in reducing elopement. behaviors for 15 children. Brief meal sessions were
78 J.M. Huete et al.

conducted in experimental rooms by behavior Research on functional assessment and treat-


therapists wherein a bite of food and the instruc- ment of feeding difficulties has expanded to
tion “Take a bite” were presented. Inappropriate include such topics as mealtime behavior prob-
mealtime behaviors resulted in one of three types lems that are maintained by multiple sources of
of consequences: removal of the spoon of food reinforcement (Bachmeyer et al., 2009), feeding
(escape condition), provision of verbal coaxing or problems specific to children with autism (cf.
statements of concern (attention condition), or pro- review by Volkert & Vaz, 2010), and parent-con-
vision of preferred toy, food, or drink (tangible ducted functional analyses of inappropriate meal-
condition). A play/control condition was also con- time behaviors displayed by children with autism
ducted. Functional analysis results indicated that spectrum disorders (Najdowski, Wallace, Penrod
for 12 children, inappropriate mealtime behaviors et al., 2008). Finally, modifications to functional
were sensitive to social consequences (three chil- analysis methodology have also been useful in
dren displayed few problem behaviors during the assessing the feeding-related, post-meal problem
assessment). Indeed, 60% of children exhibited behavior of rumination. Rumination is the regur-
problem behavior sensitive to negative reinforce- gitation, chewing, and re-swallowing of previ-
ment, suggesting that escape plays a primary role ously ingested food; this behavior can produce
in maintenance of mealtime behavior problems. esophagitis, dehydration, and other medical prob-
Also, more than 50% of children exhibited meal- lems. Two studies (Lyons, Rue, Luiselli, &
time problem behaviors maintained by positive DiGennaro, 2007; Wilder, Register, Register,
reinforcement. Importantly, as seen with studies of Bajacic, & Neidert, 2009) have reported on func-
SIB and other challenging behaviors exhibited by tional analyses with children with developmental
children with intellectual disabilities, effective delay or autism who displayed rumination; in all
function-based interventions were derived from cases, rumination was maintained by automatic
functional analysis results. reinforcement, and effective treatments were
More recent studies have replicated and identified for all participants. Thus, as demon-
extended Piazza et al.’s (2003) research identify- strated by research on self-injury and other
ing social contingencies responsible for mainte- behavior problems, functional analysis results
nance of mealtime behavior problems. For have been useful in guiding development of
example, Borrero, Woods, Borrero, Masler, and highly specific and effective interventions (i.e.,
Lesser (2010) conducted descriptive analyses of those based on the function of the behavior) for a
feeding problems with 25 children and their par- range of inappropriate mealtime or food-related
ents. They found that social reinforcement, pri- behaviors.
marily in the form of attention and escape, was
most commonly provided by parents following
mealtime problem behavior, which supports the Noncompliance
validity of a functional analysis approach to
assessment of feeding problems. In another study, Noncompliant behavior can be observed in many
Wilder, Normand, and Atwell (2005) assessed different forms and may often co-occur with
the food selectivity and SIB of a 3-year-old girl other topographies of challenging behavior (e.g.,
diagnosed with autism. They utilized a brief aggressive and disruptive tantrum behavior).
functional analysis methodology described by Among childhood problems, noncompliance has
Northup et al. (1991) where each test and control been noted as one of the most concerning prob-
condition consisted of single data points within a lems for parents and teachers (Miles & Wilder,
reversal design. This modified assessment was 2009). For children with ASD and IDD who often
effective in identifying escape as a maintaining benefit from highly specialized academic pro-
variable for the child’s meal-related problem gramming and instruction, compliance with
behaviors and in guiding development of an instructions may be essential in order to produce
effective reinforcement-based intervention. meaningful gains in academic and social skills.
5 Populations and Problems 79

For observational or data collection purposes, given the presence or absence of individually
noncompliant behavior often is operationally designed antecedent interventions (e.g., partici-
defined by defining its inverse (i.e., compliance). pant choice of task sequence), interventions were
For example, when providing discrete trial-based implemented which produced near-zero levels of
instruction using a least-to-most prompting problem behavior.
sequence, compliance may be defined as “begin- In a second study conducted by Peyton et al.
ning or completing the request within 5 s of the (2005), the type of prompting used, independent
verbal or gestural prompt.” Noncompliance usu- of the task type, was found to set the occasion for
ally is defined as the child doing anything other challenging behaviors associated with noncom-
than complying. pliant vocal behavior (NVB), but not actual non-
Not unlike other topographies of challenging compliance. In their study, modified functional
behavior, researchers have utilized functional analysis procedures were employed to examine
assessment methodologies to identify putative the NVB of a 15-year-old girl with ASD who was
discriminative stimuli and establishing opera- referred for the assessment and treatment of dis-
tions associated with noncompliance (Rortvedt & ruptive and inappropriate vocalizations. For this
Miltenberger, 1994; Roscoe, Rooker, Pence, & individual, NVB was defined as “any vocal behav-
Longworth, 2009), as well as the consequent ior involving a refusal to comply with a request”
environmental events maintaining these behav- (e.g., “I can’t ever do that,” “I won’t do it”).
iors (Peyton, Lindauer, & Richman, 2005; Interestingly, researchers found that although
Rodriguez, Thompson, & Baynham, 2010). high levels of NVB were observed during demand
Specifically, task features such as degree of conditions conducted as part of a standard func-
difficulty or task presentation style (e.g., type of tional analysis (i.e., Iwata et al., 1982/1994), only
prompting or rate of prompt delivery) have been moderate levels of noncompliance were observed,
found to be particularly relevant. causing them to question the validity of a negative
McComas, Hoch, Paone, and El-Roy (2000) reinforcement hypothesis. Next, a follow-up func-
utilized multiple methods of functional assess- tional analysis comparing two demand conditions
ment to identify idiosyncratic establishing condi- where NVB resulted in either task removal (i.e.,
tions which contributed to the occurrence of escape) or no task removal demonstrated that
problematic behaviors (i.e., aggression, disrup- NVB was not sensitive to escape, but was consis-
tion, and noncompliance) reported to occur dur- tently occasioned by directive prompts. Finally,
ing academic times for three children with ASD an intervention analysis comparing the use of
attending a small private school. Following func- directive (e.g., “show me the circle”) versus non-
tional analyses procedures (Iwata et al., directive prompts (e.g., “I wonder where the cir-
1982/1994) for each participant, it was hypothe- cle is”) was conducted which demonstrated that
sized that problem behaviors and noncompliance nondirective prompts reduced NVB to near zero.
were maintained by negative reinforcement in the Although noncompliant behavior may be
form of escape from task demands. Following the common during demand contexts and particu-
functional analyses, descriptive assessments com- larly when aversive stimuli, such as difficult
prised of informal teacher interviews and A–B–C tasks, are present, some functional assessments
observations were conducted during academic have also found noncompliance to be sensitive to
work times in each participant’s classroom. From positive reinforcement. Based on teacher con-
these observations, different task features specific cerns and reports of noncompliant behavior,
to each participant were hypothesized to be idio- Rodriguez et al. (2010) examined the noncompli-
syncratic establishing operations, such as difficult ant behavior of three preschool-aged children
tasks, adult-selected task sequences, and repeti- who attended a university-affiliated early child-
tive tasks, and were associated with problem hood program. During their functional analysis
behavior and noncompliance. Finally, upon exam- procedures, the degree to which noncompliant
ining levels of problem behavior and compliance behavior was maintained by either escape or
80 J.M. Huete et al.

attention was examined by alternating attention preferred activity to a low preferred activity, from
and escape conditions in a multielement design low to high, from low to low, and from high to
where the antecedent conditions in each test high (McCord et al., 2001; Wilder et al., 2006).
remained constant, yet the contingencies for McCord et al. (2001) conducted a functional
compliance or noncompliance were programmed analysis to examine the self-injurious behavior of
based on a contingency reversal strategy. That is, two adults with ID by arranging activity initiation
during the escape condition, noncompliance transitions (i.e., changes from no activity to a low
resulted in escape, while compliant behavior or high preferred activity) as well as activity ter-
resulted in attention, and during the attention mination transitions (i.e., changes from an ongo-
condition, the contingencies were reversed. ing high or low preferred activity to no activity).
Noncompliance was highest for all three partici- Furthermore, each condition was evaluated when
pants when noncompliant behavior resulted in participants either were or were not required to
positive reinforcement in the form of attention make a physical change in location. The results
(e.g., “come on, you know you can do this”) but of their analysis suggested that for both individu-
not escape, as may have been indicated by more als, SIB was maintained by negative reinforce-
naturalistic or descriptive methods. ment in the form of avoiding physical location
changes and that one of the participant’s SIB was
also maintained by escape from ongoing tasks
Problems with Transitions and avoidance of task initiations.
Wilder, Harris, Reagan, and Rasey (2007) uti-
A transition can be defined as changing from one lized methods similar to those used by McCord
activity to a different activity which may or may et al. (2001) to examine the tantrum behavior of
not include a change in physical location (e.g., typically developing children in preschool and
transitioning from recess to the classroom). Some identified different maintaining consequences for
authors have estimated that up to 35% of a child’s each. For one participant, problem behaviors
waking hours may include transitions (Sainato, were maintained by positive reinforcement in the
Strain, Lefebvre, & Rapp, 1987). For individuals form of gaining access to the previously inter-
with ASD and IDD, transitions may prove rupted activity. For the second participant, prob-
uniquely problematic for a number of reasons lem behaviors were maintained by negative
and have long been associated with occasion- reinforcement in the form of task avoidance. The
ing problem behavior within this population use of a DRO plus extinction procedure resulted
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). While in reducing tantrum behaviors to near zero for
intervention research has suggested the role of both of these participants.
environmental predictability as being a particu- Although these examples provide evidence for
larly relevant variable when considering aberrant the utility of functional analysis methodology to
behaviors associated with transitions (Flannery evaluate challenging behaviors associated with
& Horner, 1994), the application of functional transitions, when transitions occur in the home,
assessment methodologies to evaluate challeng- school, or community, they may involve multiple
ing behaviors associated with transitions remains contextual variables which are difficult to repli-
limited. cate using analog arrangements (Kern &
In natural settings, transitions often involve mul- Vorndran, 2000). Furthermore, the use of descrip-
tiple stimulus changes, such as the removal or loss tive assessments in naturalistic settings may
of multiple types of preferred stimuli, and func- enhance the treatment utility of the assessment
tional assessment methods have been applied to (English & Anderson, 2006).
examine these events (McCord, Thomson, & Iwata, Kern and Vorndran (2000) provide an exam-
2001). In particular, examination of the following ple of a less intensive functional assessment
types of transitions has been suggested: from a high methods used to evaluate problems associated
5 Populations and Problems 81

with transitions. These researchers examined the validity of this measure is promising for use with
“flopping” behavior (i.e., dropping to the ground typically developing children (Kearney &
and refusing to move during transitions) of an Silverman, 1999), the SRAS has not been evalu-
11-year-old girl diagnosed with IDD, ADHD, ated with children with ASD or IDD. Fortunately,
and moderate obesity in her school setting. although scarce, examples of individualized
Specifically, data were collected by coupling functional assessments of SRB within this popu-
informal functional assessment interviews with lation are available and have shown promise
naturalistic A–B–C observations conducted dur- (Meyer, Hagopian, & Paclawsky, 1999; Arvans
ing transitions (e.g., from recess to the classroom) & LeBlanc, 2009).
throughout the child’s school day. After examin- Meyer et al. (1999) used functional assess-
ing their functional assessment information, it ment methods to examine the SRB of an 18-year-
was hypothesized that challenging behaviors old male diagnosed with moderate mental
were maintained by avoidance of low preferred retardation, mild cerebral palsy, and Anxiety
environments or activities, such as activities Disorder NOS who also engaged in physical and
involving high rates of demands. verbal aggression and disruptive behaviors.
Although multiple phases of assessment and
treatment were conducted as part of their study,
School Refusal the use of teacher and school attendance reports,
informal interviews with the participant’s teach-
As the preceding examples have demonstrated, ers and parent, and unstructured descriptive
noncompliance with immediate academic, daily observations during parent–child interactions
living, or other tasks can be evaluated using func- allowed the researchers to hypothesize that school
tional assessment methods successfully. However, refusal was maintained by positive reinforcement
a related concern with respect to noncompliance in the form of access to preferred activities and
is school refusal behavior (SRB). SRB has been social attention from his mother. Upon imple-
defined as “child-motivated refusal to attend menting a function-based intervention, an
school and/or problems with remaining in classes increase in school attendance and decreases in
for an entire day” and should be distinguished SRB and other noncompliant behaviors were
from absenteeism which is generally used to refer observed.
to child absences from school that are not due to In a second study, Arvans and LeBlanc (2009)
their overt refusal (Kearney, 2008). SRB in typi- evaluated the relation between migraine reports
cally developing children has been estimated to and SRB for a 15-year-old boy diagnosed with
occur in 1–10% of the school-age population Asperger’s Disorder. In this case, the authors
(Kearney, 2008; Kearney & Silverman, 1999; used the Functional Assessment Interview
King & Bernstein, 2001), and others have sug- (FAI; O’Neill et al., 1997) and the Functional
gested that the prevalence of this behavior in chil- Assessment Screening Tool (FAST; Iwata &
dren with ASD and IDD may be comparable DeLeon, 1995) to examine the antecedents and
(Kurita, 1991). consequences associated with the boy’s migraine
To better understand the function of SRB, reports. Upon conclusion, the maintaining conse-
Kearney and Silverman (1993) developed the quence most reliably following migraine reports
School Refusal Assessment Scale (SRAS), which was determined to be escape from school or indi-
is designed to identify one of the following four vidual class periods during school. Subsequently,
forms of hypothesized reinforcement for school rather than treating migraines via medication, a
refusal: avoidance of stimuli provoking negative function-based intervention consisting of a token
affectivity, escape from aversive social or evalua- economy and escape extinction were found to
tive situations, attention seeking, or positive tan- decrease migraine reports and increase school
gible reinforcement. However, while the treatment and class attendance.
82 J.M. Huete et al.

This will lead to the development and refinement


Summary of more effective and efficient functional assess-
ment procedures and to the expansion and adapta-
Functional assessment is comprised of a broad
tion of procedures to more populations and
range of indirect and direct procedures for exam-
challenging behaviors.
ining the function, or purpose, of challenging
behaviors. Functional assessment procedures have
been applied to a variety of populations, but are
most notably used with individuals diagnosed
References
with IDD and ASD, likely due to the increased Adelinis, J. D., & Hagopian, L. P. (1999). The use of sym-
prevalence of challenging behaviors in these pop- metrical ‘do’ and ‘don’t’ requests to interrupt ongoing
ulations. While SIB has been most associated activities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,
with the evolution of functional assessment and 519–523.
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., Gardener, N. C., Chung, B.
functional analysis methods, adaptations to proce- I., & Dube, W. V. (2003). Persistence of stereotypic
dures have allowed for successful assessment of behavior: Examining the effects of external reinforc-
numerous challenging behaviors, including ste- ers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36,
reotypies, aggression, property destruction, and 439–448.
Alter, P. J., Conroy, M. A., Mancil, G., & Haydon, T.
feeding problems, to name a few. The most impor- (2008). A comparison of functional behavior assess-
tant element of functional assessment is that the ment methodologies with young children: Descriptive
results typically inform regarding intervention. methods and functional analysis. Journal of Behavioral
For example, identification of SIB maintained by Education, 17, 200–219.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
attention from others can be addressed with inter- and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-
ventions such as noncontingent delivery of atten- IV-TR). Washington, DC: Author.
tion on a fixed-time schedule or differential Anderson, C. M., & Long, E. S. (2002). Use of a struc-
reinforcement procedures. As a result of this link tured descriptive assessment methodology to identify
variables affecting problem behavior. Journal of
between functional assessment results and treat- Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 137–154.
ment, interventions have progressively become Andover, M. S., & Gibb, B. E. (2010). Non-suicidal self-
less punitive and more reinforcement-based and injury, attempted suicide, and suicidal intent among
have resulted in less restrictive placements for psychiatric inpatients. Psychiatry Research, 178,
101–105.
numerous individuals with IDD or ASD. One Athens, E. S., Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & Pipkin, C.
other related and prominent effect of the increased (2008). An analysis of vocal stereotypy and therapist
use of functional assessments is a better under- fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,
standing of the behavioral presentations for 291–297.
Arvans, R. K., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2009). Functional
numerous populations of individuals with IDDs. assessment and treatment of migraine reports and
Specifically, functional assessments have demon- school absences in an adolescent with Asperger’s dis-
strated that challenging behaviors once thought to order. Education & Treatment of Children, 32,
be an inherent characteristic of particular diagno- 151–166.
Axelrod, S. (1987). Functional and structural analyses of
ses, and thus automatically maintained, may actu- behavior: Approaches leading to reduced use of pun-
ally be regulated by, or at least sensitive to, social ishment procedures. Research in Developmental
or environmental factors. This has allowed practi- Disabilities, 8, 165–178.
tioners to develop interventions for challenging Bachmeyer, M. H., Piazza, C. C., Fredrick, L. D., Reed,
G. K., Rivas, K. D., & Kadey, H. J. (2009). Functional
behaviors of individuals where it was once thought analysis and treatment of multiply controlled inappro-
interventions would not be effective. As more priate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
research is conducted, and as more practitioners Analysis, 42, 641–658.
adopt functional assessment procedures, the Baghdadli, A., Pascal, C., Grisi, S., & Aussilloux, C.
(2003). Risk factors for self-injurious behaviours
already growing body of support for functional among 222 young children with autistic disorders.
assessment as the most effective means of assess- Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47,
ing challenging behaviors should expand greatly. 622–627.
5 Populations and Problems 83

Bass, M. N., & Speak, B. L. (2005). A behavioral approach Cooper, S. A., Smiley, E. E., Allan, L. M., Jackson, A. A.,
to the assessment and treatment of severe self-injury in Finlayson, J. J., Mantry, D. D., et al. (2009). Adults
a woman with Smith-Magenis syndrome: A single with intellectual disabilities: Prevalence, incidence
case study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, and remission of self-injurious behaviour, and related
33(3), 361–368. doi:10.1017/S1352465805002110. factors. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,
Bergen, A. E., Holbern, S. W., & Scott-Huyghebaert, V. C. 53, 200–216.
(2002). Functional analysis of self-injurious behavior Crockett, J. L., & Hagopian, L. P. (2006). Prompting pro-
in an adult with Lesch-Nyhan syndrome. Behavior cedures as establishing operations for escape-main-
Modification, 26, 187–204. tained behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 65–71.
Berkson, G. (1983). Repetitive stereotyped behaviors. Cunningham, A. B., & Schreibman, L. (2008). Stereotypy
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 88, 239–246. in autism: The importance of function. Research in
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968). A Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 469–479.
method to integrate descriptive and experimental field Dahl, M., & Sundelin, C. (1986). Early feeding problems
studies at the level of data and empirical concepts. in an affluent society. Acta Paediatrica, 75, 370–379.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191. Danford, D. E., & Huber, A. M. (1982). Pica among men-
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. tally retarded adults. American Journal of Mental
H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: Deficiency, 87, 141–146.
Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism Dawson, J. E., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. (1998). An
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237–243. analysis of maladaptive behaviors in persons with
Borrero, J. C., Vollmer, T. R., Wright, C. S., Lerman, D. autism, PDD-NOS, and mental retardation. Research
C., & Kelley, M. E. (2002). Further evaluation of the in Developmental Disabilities, 19, 439–448.
role of protective equipment in the functional analysis Deitz, D. D., & Repp, A. C. (1989). Mental retardation. In
of self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior T. H. Ollendick, M. Hersen, T. H. Ollendick, & M.
Analysis, 35, 69–72. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychopathology
Borrero, C. S., Woods, J. N., Borrero, J. C., Masler, E. A., (2nd ed., pp. 75–91). New York: Plenum Press.
& Lesser, A. D. (2010). Descriptive analyses of pedi- DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S., Rodriguez-Catter, V.,
atric food refusal and acceptance. Journal of Applied Sveinsdóttir, I., & Sadler, C. (2003). Assessment of
Behavior Analysis, 43, 71–88. aberrant behavior maintained by wheelchair move-
Bowman, L. G., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., & ment in a child with developmental disabilities.
Piazza, C. C. (1997). On the relation of mands and the Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24, 381–390.
function of destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Didden, R., Duker, P. C., & Korzilius, H. (1997). Meta-
Behavior Analysis, 30, 251–265. analytic study on treatment effectiveness for problem
Budney, A. J., & Higgins, S. T. (1998). Community rein- behaviors with individuals who have mental retardation.
forcement plus vouchers approach: Manual 2: American Journal on Mental Retardation, 101, 387–399.
National Institute on Drug Abuse. Therapy manuals Didden, R., Korzilius, H., & Curfs, L. G. (2007). Skin-
for drug addiction. (NIH Publication Number picking in individuals with Prader-Willi Syndrome:
98-4309). Rockville: National Institute on Drug Prevalence, functional Assessment, and its comorbid-
Abuse. ity with compulsive and self-injurious behaviours.
Campbell, R. V., & Lutzker, J. R. (1993). Using functional Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
equivalence training to reduce severe challenging 20, 409–419.
behavior: A case study. Journal of Developmental and Dominick, K. C., Davis, W. O., Lainhart, J., Tager-
Physical Disabilities, 5, 203–216. Flusberg, H., & Folstein, S. (2007). Atypical behav-
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious behav- iors in children with autism and children with a history
ior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological of language impairment. Research in Developmental
Bulletin, 84, 800–816. Disabilities, 28, 145–162.
Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Turnbull, A. P., Marquis, J. G., Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (1994). A behavior-ana-
McLaughlin, D., McAtee, M. L., et al. (Eds.). (1999). lytic account of depression and a case report using
Positive behavior support for people with develop- acceptance-based procedures. The Behavior Analyst,
mental disabilities: A research synthesis. Washington, 17, 231–333.
DC: American Association on Mental Retardation. Durand, V., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on
Carter, S. L., & Wheeler, J. J. (2007). Analysis of behav- ‘self-stimulatory’ behavior: Analysis and treatment
ioural responding across multiple instructional condi- application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20,
tions for a child with childhood disintegrative disorder. 119–132.
Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 7, Dykens, E. M., Cassidy, S. B., & DeVries, M. L. (2011).
137–141. Prader-Willi syndrome. In S. Goldstein, C. R.
Clarke, S., Dunlap, G., & Vaughn, B. (1999). Family- Reynolds, S. Goldstein, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.),
centered, assessment-based intervention to improve Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disor-
behavior during an early morning routine. Journal of ders in children (2nd ed., pp. 484–511). New York:
Positive Behavior Interventions, 1, 235–241. Guilford.
84 J.M. Huete et al.

Ebanks, M. E., & Fisher, W. W. (2003). Altering the tim- Goh, H.-L., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. W. (1999).
ing of academic prompts to treat destructive behavior Multicomponent assessment and treatment of cigarette
maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,
Analysis, 36, 355–359. 297–316.
Edelson, M. (2006). Are the majority of children with Goh, H., Iwata, B. A., Shore, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G.
autism mentally retarded? A systematic evaluation of (1995). An analysis of the reinforcing properties of
the data. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental hand mouthing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Disabilities, 21, 66–83. 28, 269–283.
English, C. L., & Anderson, C. M. (2006). Evaluation of Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M. W., Greene, P. E.,
the treatment utility of the analog functional analysis Kim, M., & Rapin, I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in
and the structured descriptive assessment. Journal of children with autism and other developmental disor-
Positive Behavior Interventions, 8, 212–229. ders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51,
Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Feeney, B. J., & Stephenson, 30–38.
K. M. (2010). Assessment and treatment of elopement Graff, R. B., Lineman, G. T., Libby, M. E., & Ahearn, W.
maintained by access to stereotypy. Journal of Applied H. (1999). Functional analysis and treatment of
Behavior Analysis, 43, 513–517. screaming in a young girl with severe disabilities.
Favell, J. E., McGimsey, J. F., & Schell, R. M. (1982). Behavioral Interventions, 14, 233–239.
Treatment of self-injury by providing alternate sen- Hagopian, L. P., & Adelinis, J. D. (2001). Response blocking
sory activities. Analysis & Intervention in with and without redirection for the treatment of pica.
Developmental Disabilities, 2, 83–104. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 527–530.
Feeley, K. M., & Jones, E. A. (2006). Addressing chal- Hagopian, L. P., & Toole, L. M. (2009). Effects of response
lenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome: blocking and competing stimuli on stereotypic behav-
The use of applied behaviour analysis for assessment ior. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 117–125.
and intervention. Down Syndrome: Research & Hagopian, L. P., Wilson, D. M., & Wilder, D. A. (2001).
Practice, 11, 64–77. Assessment and treatment of problem behavior main-
Fisher, W. W., Lindauer, S. E., Alterson, C. J., & tained by escape from attention and access to tangible
Thompson, R. H. (1998). Assessment and treatment of items. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
destructive behavior maintained by stereotypic object 229–232.
manipulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Hall, S., DeBernardis, M., & Reiss, A. (2006). Social
31, 513–527. escape behaviors in children with Fragile-X syndrome.
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Kurtz, P. F., Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36,
Sherer, M. R., & Lachman, S. R. (1994). A prelimi- 935–947.
nary evaluation of empirically derived consequences Hall, S., Oliver, C., & Murphy, G. (2001). Self-injurious
for the treatment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior behaviour in young children with Lesch-Nyhan syn-
Analysis, 27, 447–457. drome. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,
Flannery, K. B., & Horner, R. H. (1994). The relationship 43, 745–749.
between predictability and problem behavior for stu- Hanley, G., Iwata, B., & McCord, B. (2003). Functional
dents with severe disabilities. Journal of Behavioral analysis of problem behavior: A review. Journal of
Education, 4, 157–176. Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
Fox, R. A., Keller, K. M., Grede, P. L., & Bartosz, A. M. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Thompson, R. H., & Lindberg,
(2007). A mental health clinic for toddlers with devel- J. S. (2000). A component analysis of “stereotypy as
opmental delays and behavior problems. Research in reinforcement” for alternative behavior. Journal of
Developmental Disabilities, 28, 119–129. Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 285–297.
Foxx, R. M., & Martin, E. D. (1975). Treatment of scav- Harding, J. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Barretto, A.,
enging behavior (coprophagy and pica) by overcorrec- Winborn, L., & Gardner, A. (2001). Analysis of
tion. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 13, 153–162. response class hierarchies with attention-maintained
Franco, J. H., Lang, R. L., O’Reilly, M. F., Chan, J. M., problem behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior
Sigafoos, J., & Rispoli, M. (2009). Functional analysis Analysis, 34, 61–64.
and treatment of inappropriate vocalizations using a Hastings, R. P., & Noone, S. J. (2005). Self-injurious
speech-generating device for a child with autism. behavior and functional analysis: Ethics and evidence.
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities,
24, 146–155. 40, 335–342.
Freeman, K. A., Walker, M., & Kaufman, J. (2007). Hausman, N., Kahng, S., Farrell, E., & Mongeon, C.
Psychometric properties of the Questions About (2009). Idiosyncratic functions: Severe problem
Behavioral Function Scale in a child sample. American behavior maintained by access to ritualistic behaviors.
Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 122–129. Education & Treatment of Children, 32, 77–87.
Gardner, W. I., & Cole, C. L. (1990). Aggression and Hill, J., & Furniss, F. (2006). Patterns of emotional and
related conduct difficulties. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), behavioural disturbance associated with autistic traits
Handbook of behavior modification with the mentally in young people with severe intellectual disabilities
retarded (2nd ed., pp. 225–248). New York: Plenum and challenging behaviours. Research in Developmental
Press. Disabilities, 27, 517–528.
5 Populations and Problems 85

Hurley, A., & Levitas, A. S. (2007). The importance of Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
recognizing autism spectrum disorders in intellectual Handicaps, 25, 212–216.
disability. Mental Health Aspects of Developmental Kerr, P. L., & Muehlenkamp, J. J. (2010). Features of psy-
Disabilities, 10, 157–161. chopathology in self-injuring female college students.
Iwata, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G. (1995). The functional anal- Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 32, 290–308.
ysis screening tool (FAST). Unpublished manuscript, King, N. J., & Bernstein, G. A. (2001). School refusal in
University of Florida. children and adolescents: A review of the past 10 years.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., & Bauman, K. E. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
(1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 197–205.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209 Kodak, T., Grow, L., & Northup, J. (2004). Functional
(Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in analysis and treatment of elopement for a child with
Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal
Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., & Zarcone, J. R. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 229–232.
(1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior: An Kodak, T., & Piazza, C. C. (2008). Assessment and behav-
experimental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of ioral treatment of feeding and sleeping disorders in
Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240. children with autism spectrum disorders. Child and
Jacobson, J. W. (1982). Problem behavior and psychiatric Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17,
impairment within a developmentally disabled popu- 887–905.
lation: I. Behavior frequency. Applied Research in Kuhn, D. E., Hardesty, S. L., & Sweeney, N. M. (2009).
Mental Retardation, 3, 121–139. Assessment and treatment of excessive straightening
Johnston, S. S., & O’Neill, R. E. (2001). Searching for and destructive behavior in an adolescent diagnosed
effectiveness and efficiency in conducting functional with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
assessments: A review and proposed process for teach- 42, 355–360.
ers and other practitioners. Focus on Autism & Other Kurita, H. (1991). School refusal in pervasive develop-
Developmental Disabilities, 16, 205. mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Jones, R. S. (1991). Stereotypy: Challenging behaviour or Disorders, 21, 1–15.
adaptive response. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. F.,
Therapy, 20, 25–40. O’Connor, J. T., Paclawskyj, T. R., et al. (2003).
Kahng, S., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Correspondence Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious
between outcomes of brief and extended functional behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases.
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219.
149–159. LaBelle, C. A., & Charlop-Christy, M. H. (2002).
Kahng, S., Leak, J., Vu, C., & Mishler, B. (2008). Individualizing functional analysis to assess multiple
Mechanical restraints as positive reinforcers for and changing functions of severe behavior problems in
aggression. Behavioral Interventions, 23, 137–142. children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
Kearney, C. A. (2008). School absenteeism and school Interventions, 4, 231–241.
refusal behavior in youth: A contemporary review. Lang, R., Davis, T., O’Reilly, M., Machalicek, W., Rispoli,
Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 451–471. M., Sigafoos, J., et al. (2010). Functional analysis and
Kearney, C. A., Cook, L., Wechsler, A., Haight, C. M., & treatment of elopement across two school settings.
Stowman, S. (2008). Behavioral assessment. In M. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 113–118.
Hersen, A. M. Gross, M. Hersen, & A. M. Gross Lang, R., Didden, R., Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M.,
(Eds.), Handbook of clinical psychology (Children and Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., et al. (2010). Behavioral
adolescents, Vol. 2, pp. 551–574). Hoboken: Wiley. treatment of chronic skin-picking in individuals with
Kearney, C. A., & Silverman, W. K. (1993). Measuring developmental disabilities: A systematic review.
the function of school refusal behavior: The School Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 304–315.
Assessment Scale. Journal of Clinical Child Lang, R., Didden, R., Sigafoos, J., Rispoli, M., Regester,
Psychology, 22, 85–96. A., & Lancioni, G. E. (2009). Treatment of chronic
Kearney, C. A., & Silverman, W. K. (1999). Functionally skin-picking in an adolescent with Asperger syndrome
based prescriptive and nonprescriptive treatment for and borderline intellectual disability. Clinical Case
children and adolescents with school refusal behavior. Studies, 8, 317–325.
Behavior Therapy, 30, 673–695. Lang, R., Rispoli, M., Machalicek, W., White, P. J., Kang,
Kennedy, C. H. (2007). Stereotypic movement disorder. S., Pierce, N., et al. (2009). Treatment of elopement in
In P. Sturmey (Ed.), Functional analysis in clinical individuals with developmental disabilities: A system-
treatment (pp. 193–209). San Diego: Elsevier atic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Academic Press. 30, 670–681.
Kern, L., Mauk, J. E., Marder, T. J., & Mace, F. (1995). Langdon, N. A., Carr, E. G., & Owen-DeSchryver, J. S.
Functional analysis and intervention for breath hold- (2008). Functional analysis of precursors for serious
ing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, problem behavior and related intervention. Behavior
339–340. Modification, 32, 804–827.
Kern, L., & Vorndran, C. M. (2000). Functional assess- Le, D. D., & Smith, R. G. (2002). Functional analysis of
ment and intervention for transition difficulties. self-injury with and without protective equipment.
86 J.M. Huete et al.

Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, McClintock, K. K., Hall, S. S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk
14, 277–290. markers associated with challenging behaviours in
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and people with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic
experimental analyses of variables maintaining self- study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47,
injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior 405–416.
Analysis, 26, 293–319. McComas, J., Hoch, H., Paone, D., & El-Roy, D. (2000).
Lovaas, O. I., Newsom, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). Self- Escape behavior during academic tasks: A preliminary
stimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement. analysis of idiosyncratic establishing conditions.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 45–68. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 479–493.
Lowe, K. K., Allen, D. D., Jones, E. E., Brophy, S. S., McComas, J. J., Thompson, A., & Johnson, L. (2003). The
Moore, K. K., & James, W. W. (2007). Challenging effects of presession attention on problem behavior
behaviours: Prevalence and topographies. Journal of maintained by different reinforcers. Journal of Applied
Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 625–636. Behavior Analysis, 36, 297–307.
Luiselli, J. K. (2009). Aggression and noncompliance. In McCord, B. E., Grosser, J. W., Iwata, B. A., & Powers, L.
J. L. Matson & J. L. Matson (Eds.), Applied behavior A. (2005). An analysis of response-blocking parame-
analysis for children with autism spectrum disorders ters in the prevention of pica. Journal of Applied
(pp. 175–187). New York: Springer. Behavior Analysis, 38, 391–394.
Luiselli, J. K., & Murbach, L. (2002). Providing instruc- McCord, B. E., Thomson, R. J., & Iwata, B. A. (2001).
tion from novel staff as an antecedent intervention for Functional analysis and treatment of self-injury asso-
child tantrum behavior in a public school classroom. ciated with transitions. Journal of Applied Behavior
Education & Treatment of Children, 25, 356–365. Analysis, 34, 195–210.
Lyons, E. A., Rue, H. C., Luiselli, J. K., & DiGennaro, F. Meyer, E. A., Hagopian, L. P., & Paclawskyj, T. R. (1999).
D. (2007). Brief functional analysis and supplemental A function-based treatment for school refusal behav-
feeding for postmeal rumination in children with ior using shaping and fading. Research in
developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Developmental Disabilities, 20, 401–410.
Behavior Analysis, 40, 743–747. Miles, N. I., & Wilder, D. A. (2009). The effects of behav-
Mace, F. C., & Knight, D. (1986). Functional analysis and ioral skills training on caregiver implementation of
treatment of severe pica. Journal of Applied Behavior guided compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 19, 411–416. Analysis, 42, 405–410.
Madden, N. A., Russo, D. C., & Cataldo, M. F. (1980). Millichap, D., Oliver, C., McQuillan, S., Kalsy, S., Lloyd,
Environmental influences on mouthing in children V., & Hall, S. (2003). Descriptive functional analysis
with lead intoxication. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, of behavioral excesses shown by adults with Down
5, 207–216. syndrome and dementia. International Journal of
Marcus, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Swanson, V., Roane, H. R., Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 844–854.
& Ringdahl, J. E. (2001). An experimental analysis of Miltenberger, R. (1999). Understanding problem behav-
aggression. Behavior Modification, 25, 189–213. iors through functional assessment. In N. A. Wieseler
Martell, C. R. (2008). Behavioral activation for depres- & R. H. Hanson (Eds.), Challenging behavior of per-
sion. In W. T. O’Donohue, J. E. Fisher, W. T. sons with mental health disorders and severe develop-
O’Donohue, & J. E. Fisher (Eds.), Cognitive behavior mental disabilities (pp. 215–235). Washington, DC:
therapy: Applying empirically supported techniques in American Association on Mental Retardation.
your practice (2nd ed., pp. 40–45). Hoboken: Wiley. Moss, J. J., Oliver, C., Hall, S. S., Arron, K. K., Sloneem,
Matson, J. (2009). Aggression and tantrums in children J. J., & Petty, J. J. (2005). The association between
with autism: A review of behavioral treatments and environmental events and self-injurious behaviour in
maintaining variables. Journal of Mental Health Cornelia de Lange syndrome. Journal of Intellectual
Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2, 169–187. Disability Research, 49, 269–277.
Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009). Murphy, G., Macdonald, S., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2000).
Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in Aggression and the termination of “rituals”: A new
infants with autism and pervasive developmental dis- variant of the escape function for challenging behav-
order. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, ior? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21,
122–127. 43–5.
Matson, J. L., Kiely, S. L., & Bamburg, J. W. (1997). The Nahgahgwon, K. N., Umbreit, J., Liaupsin, C. J., &
effect of stereotypies on adaptive skills as assessed Turton, A. M. (2010). Function-based planning for
with the DASH-II and Vineland Adaptive Behavior young children at-risk for emotional and behavioral
Scales. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, disorders. Education & Treatment of Children, 33,
471–476. 537–559.
Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008). Characteristics of Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Ellsworth, C. L.,
challenging behaviours in adults with autistic disorder, MacAleese, A. N., & Cleveland, J. M. (2008).
PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal of Functional analyses and treatment of precursor behav-
Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 33, ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,
323–329. 97–105.
5 Populations and Problems 87

Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Penrod, B., Tarbox, J., Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.,
Reagon, K., & Higher, T. S. (2008). Caregiver con- & Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions About Behavioral
ducted experimental functional analyses of inappro- Function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func-
priate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in
Analysis, 41, 459–465. Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229.
Nicholson, J., Konstantinidi, E., & Furniss, F. (2006). On Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.
some psychometric properties of the questions about Y., & Vollmer, T. R. (2001). Assessment of the conver-
behavioral function (QABF) scale. Research in gent validity of the Questions About Behavioral
Developmental Disabilities, 27, 337–352. Function scale with analogue functional analysis and
Northup, J., & Gulley, V. (2001). Some contributions of the Motivation Assessment Scale. Journal of
functional analysis to the assessment of behaviors Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 484–494.
associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Patel, M. R., Carr, J. E., Kim, C., Robles, A., & Eastridge,
and the effects of stimulant medication. School D. (2000). Functional analysis of aberrant behavior
Psychology Review, 30, 227–238. maintained by automatic reinforcement: Assessments
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., & Steege, M. (1991). of specific sensory reinforcers. Research in
A brief functional analysis of aggressive and alterna- Developmental Disabilities, 21, 393–407.
tive behavior in an outclinic setting. Journal of Applied Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1982).
Behavior Analysis, 24, 509–522. A comparative evaluation of a parent-training program.
Nyhan, W. L. (2002). Lessons from Lesch-Nyhan syn- Behavior Therapy, 13, 638–650.
drome. In S. R. Schroeder, M. Oster-Granite, T. Patti, P. J., & Tsiouris, J. A. (2006). Psychopathology in
Thompson, S. R. Schroeder, M. Oster-Granite, & T. adults with Down syndrome: Clinical findings from an
Thompson (Eds.), Self-injurious behavior: Gene- outpatient clinic. International Journal on Disability
brain-behavior relationships (pp. 251–267). and Human Development, 5, 357–364.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Pelios, L., Morren, J., Tesch, D., & Axelrod, S. (1999).
Association. The impact of functional analysis methodology on
Oliver, C., Arron, K., Hall, S., Sloneem, J., Forman, D., & treatment choice for self-injurious and aggressive
McClintock, K. (2006). Effects of social context on behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,
social interaction and self-injurious behavior in 185–195.
Cornelia de Lange syndrome. American Journal on Petursdottir, A. I., Esch, J. W., Sautter, R. A., & Stewart,
Mental Retardation, 111, 184–192. K. K. (2010). Characteristics and hypothesized func-
Oliver, C., Murphy, G. H., Crayton, L., & Corbett, J. A. tions of challenging behavior in a community-based
(1993). Self-injurious behavior in Rett syndrome: sample. Education and Training in Autism and
Interactions between features of Rett syndrome and Developmental Disabilities, 45, 81–93.
operant conditioning. Journal of Autism and Peyton, R. T., Lindauer, S. E., & Richman, D. M. (2005).
Developmental Disorders, 23, 91–109. The effects of directive and nondirective prompts on
Oliver, C., & Richards, C. (2010). Self-injurious behav- noncompliant vocal behavior exhibited by a child with
iour in people with intellectual disability. Current autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
Opinion in Psychiatry, 23, 412–416. 251–255.
Olson, L., & Houlihan, D. (2000). A review of behavioral Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H., &
treatments used for Lesch-Nyhan syndrome. Behavior Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of
Modification, 24, 202–222. matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic
O’Neill, R. E., Horner, R. H., Sprague, J. R., Storey, K., & reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Newton, J. S. (1997). Functional assessment and pro- 33, 13–27.
gram development for problem behavior: A practical Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Brown, K. A., Shore, B. A.,
handbook (2nd ed.). Florence: Brookes/Cole. Patel, M. R., Katz, R., et al. (2003). Functional analy-
O’Reilly, M. F., Murray, N., Lancioni, G. E., Sigafoos, J., sis of inappropriate mealtime behaviors. Journal of
& Lacey, C. (2003). Functional analysis and interven- Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 187–204.
tion to reduce self-injurious and agitated behavior Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., & Lindauer, S.
when removing protective equipment for brief time E. (1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses
periods. Behavior Modification, 27, 538–559. of its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied
O’Reilly, M., Rispoli, M., Davis, T., Machalicek, W., Behavior Analysis, 31, 165–189.
Lang, R., Sigafoos, J., et al. (2010). Functional analy- Piazza, C. C., Hanley, G. P., Bowman, L. G., Ruyter, J.
sis of challenging behavior in children with autism M., Lindauer, S. E., & Saiontz, D. M. (1997).
spectrum disorders: A summary of 10 cases. Research Functional analysis and treatment of elopement.
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 1–10. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 653–672.
Oswald, D. P., Ellis, C. R., Singh, N. N., & Singh, Y. N. Piazza, C. C., Hanley, G. P., & Fisher, W. W. (1996).
(1994). Self-injury. In J. L. Matson & J. L. Matson Functional analysis and treatment of cigarette pica.
(Eds.), Autism in children and adults: Etiology, assess- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 437–450.
ment, and intervention (pp. 147–164). Belmont: Piazza, C. C., & Roane, H. S. (2009). Assessment of pedi-
Thomson Brooks/Cole. atric feeding disorders. In J. L. Matson, F. Andrasik,
88 J.M. Huete et al.

M. L. Matson, J. L. Matson, F. Andrasik, & M. L. Sainato, D. M., Strain, P. S., Lefebvre, D., & Rapp, N.
Matson (Eds.), Assessing childhood psychopathology (1987). Facilitating transition times with handi-
and developmental disabilities (pp. 471–490). capped preschool children: A comparison between
New York: Springer. peer-mediated and antecedent prompt procedures.
Piazza, C. C., Roane, H. S., Keeney, K. M., Boney, B. R., Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 285–291.
& Abt, K. A. (2002). Varying response effort in the Sasso, G. M., Reimers, T. M., Cooper, L. J., & Wacker, D.
treatment of pica maintained by automatic reinforce- P. (1992). Use of descriptive and experimental analy-
ment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, ses to identify the functional properties of aberrant
233–246. behavior in school settings. Journal of Applied
Radstaake, M., Didden, R., Bolio, M., Lang, R., Lancioni, Behavior Analysis, 25, 809–821.
G. E., & Curfs, L. G. (2011). Functional assessment Schieltz, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Harding, J. W., Berg, W.
and behavioral treatment of skin picking in a teenage K., Lee, J. F., & Dalmau, Y. (2010). An evaluation of
girl with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clinical Case Studies, manding across functions prior to functional commu-
10, 67–78. nication training. Journal of Developmental and
Rapp, J. T. (2006). Toward an empirical method for iden- Physical Disabilities, 22, 131–147.
tifying matched stimulation for automatically rein- Scott, T. M., Anderson, C., Mancil, R., & Alter, P. (2009).
forced behavior: A preliminary investigation. Journal Function-based supports for individual students in
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 137–140. school settings. In W. Sailor, G. Dunlop, G. Sugai, &
Rapp, J. T., & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A R. Horner (Eds.), Handbook of positive behavior sup-
review of behavioral assessment and treatment. port (pp. 421–441). New York: Springer.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 527–547. Scotti, J. R., Evans, I. M., Meyer, L. H., & Walker, P.
Rasmussen, K., & O’Neill, R. E. (2006). The effects of (1991). A meta-analysis of intervention research with
fixed-time reinforcement schedules on problem behav- problem behavior: Treatment validity and standards of
ior of children with emotional and behavioral disor- practice. American Journal on Mental Retardation,
ders in a day-treatment classroom setting. Journal of 96, 233–256.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 453–457. Singh, N. N., Donatelli, L. S., Best, A. A., & Williams, D.
Reed, D. D., Pace, G. M., & Luiselli, J. K. (2009). An E. (1993). Factor structure of the Motivation
investigation into the provision of choice in tangible Assessment Scale. Journal of Intellectual Disability
conditions of a functional analysis. Journal of Research, 37, 65–74.
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 21, Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
485–491. Oxford, England: Macmillan.
Restori, A. F., Gresham, F. M., Chang, T., Lee, H. B., & Slifer, K., Iwata, B. A., & Dorsey, M. F. (1984). Reduction
Laija-Rodriquez, W. (2007). Functional assessment- of eye gouging using a response interruption proce-
based interventions for children at-risk for emotional dure. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
and behavioral disorders. California School Psychiatry, 15, 369–375.
Psychologist, 12, 9–30. Smith, K. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Behavior problems:
Richman, D. M., & Hagopian, L. P. (1999). On the effects Differences among intellectually disabled adults with
of ‘quality’ of attention in the functional analysis of co-morbid autism spectrum disorders and epilepsy.
destructive behavior. Research in Developmental Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31,
Disabilities, 20, 51–62. 1062–1069.
Ringdahl, J. E., Christensen, T. J., & Boelter, E. W. (2009). Stahr, B., Cushing, D., Lane, K., & Fox, J. (2006).
Further evaluation of idiosyncratic functions for severe Efficacy of a function-based intervention in decreas-
problem behavior: Aggression maintained by access ing off-task behavior exhibited by a student with
to walks. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 275–283. ADHD. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 8,
Rodriguez, N. M., Thompson, R. H., & Baynham, T. Y. 201–211.
(2010). Assessment of the relative effects of attention Stage, S. A., Jackson, H. G., Moscovitz, K., Erickson, M.,
and escape on noncompliance. Journal of Applied Thurman, S., et al. (2006). Using multimethod-multi-
Behavior Analysis, 43, 143–147. source functional behavioral assessment for students
Rortvedt, A. K., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1994). Analysis of with behavioral disabilities. School Psychology
a high-probability instructional sequence and time-out Review, 35, 451–471.
in the treatment of child noncompliance. Journal of Stokes, J. V., & Luiselli, J. K. (2009). Applied behavior
Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 327–330. analysis assessment and intervention for health:
Roscoe, E. M., Kindle, A. E., & Pence, S. T. (2010). Threatening self-injury (rectal picking) in an adult
Functional analysis and treatment of aggression main- with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clinical Case Studies, 8,
tained by preferred conversational topics. Journal of 38–47.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 723–727. Strachan, R., Shaw, R., Burrow, C., Horsler, K., Allen, D.,
Roscoe, E. M., Rooker, G. W., Pence, S. T., & Longworth, & Oliver, C. (2009). Experimental functional analysis
L. J. (2009). Assessing the utility of a demand assess- of aggression in children with Angelman syndrome.
ment for functional analysis. Journal of Applied Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
Behavior Analysis, 42, 819–825. 1095–1106.
5 Populations and Problems 89

Sturmey, P. (1996). Functional analysis in clinical psy- Volkert, V. M., & Vaz, P. C. M. (2010). Recent studies on
chology. Oxford, England: Wiley. feeding problems in children with autism. Journal of
Tarbox, J., Wallace, M. D., Tarbox, R. F., Landaburu, H. Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 155–159.
J., & Williams, W. (2004). Functional analysis and Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
treatment of low-rate problem behavior in individuals H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
with developmental disabilities. Behavioral extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
Interventions, 19, 73–90. aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Tarbox, R. S. F., Wallace, M. D., & Williams, L. (2003). Analysis, 28, 561–576.
Assessment and treatment of elopement: A replication Vollmer, T. R., Northup, J., Ringdahl, J. E., LeBlanc, L.
and extension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, A., & Chauvin, T. M. (1996). Functional analysis of
36, 239–244. severe tantrums displayed by children with language
Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles, delays: An outclinic assessment. Behavior
R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009). Modification, 20, 97–115.
Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental Weiss, M., Fiske, K., & Ferraioli, S. (2009). Treatment of
functional assessments of challenging behavior in autism spectrum disorders. In J. L. Matson, F.
children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Andrasik, & M. L. Matson (Eds.), Treating childhood
Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514. psychopathology and developmental disabilities (pp.
Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. (1966). Aversive control of 287–332). New York: Springer.
self-injurious behavior in a psychotic boy. Behavior Wilder, D. A., Chen, L., Atwell, J., Pritchard, J., &
Research and Therapy, 4, 281–287. Weinstein, P. (2006). Brief functional analysis and
Taylor, L. L., & Oliver, C. C. (2008). The behavioural treatment of tantrums associated with transitions in
phenotype of Smith-Magenis syndrome: Evidence for preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior
a gene-environment interaction. Journal of Intellectual Analysis, 39, 103–107.
Disability Research, 52, 830–841. Wilder, D. A., Harris, C., Reagan, R., & Rasey, A. (2007).
Thompson, R. H., Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., & Kuhn, Functional analysis and treatment of noncompliance
D. E. (1998). The evaluation and treatment of aggres- by preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior
sion maintained by attention and automatic reinforce- Analysis, 40, 173–177.
ment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, Wilder, D. A., Normand, M., & Atwell, J. (2005).
103–116. Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for food
Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2007). A comparison of refusal and associated self-injury. Journal of Applied
outcomes from descriptive and functional analyses of Behavior Analysis, 38, 549–553.
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Wilder, D. A., Register, M., Register, S., Bajagic, V., &
Analysis, 40, 333–338. Neidert, P. L. (2009). Functional analysis and treat-
Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behavior in ment of rumination using fixed-time delivery of a
autism: A review of psychological research. Journal of flavor spray. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 839–849. 877–882.
Tuten, M., Jones, H. E., Ertel, J., Jakubowski, J., & Williams, K. E., Field, D. G., & Sieverling, L. (2010).
Sperlein, J. (2006, June). Reinforcement-based treat- Food refusal in children: A review of the literature.
ment: A novel approach to treating substance abuse Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 625–628.
during pregnancy. Counselor Magazine, 22–29. Woodcock, K. K., Oliver, C. C., & Humphreys, G. G.
Virues-Ortega, J., & Haynes, S. N. (2005). Functional (2009). Associations between repetitive questioning,
analysis in behavior therapy: Behavioral foundations resistance to change, temper outbursts and anxiety in
and clinical application. International Journal of Prader–Willi and Fragile-X syndromes. Journal of
Clinical and Healthy Psychology, 5, 567–587. Intellectual Disability Research, 53, 265–278.
Methods of Defining and Observing
Behaviors 6
Sarah Hurwitz and Noha F. Minshawi

Clear definitions of behaviors also serve to


Introduction enhance communication between practitioners,
caregivers, and other individuals involved in
In functional analysis, the ability to track the
research or clinical care. Ensuring that all parties
occurrence of a target behavior is essential to
are talking about the same behavior allows for
being able to identify what factors maintain that
valid and reliable data to be collected by multiple
behavior. This is so fundamental that to overlook
people and in different settings. In addition,
the crucial step of defining and observing behav-
Haynes and O’Brien (2000) state that the most
iors in designing an intervention or research study
important reason to create operational definitions
is to open the door to a large number of reliability
of behaviors is to help the clinician and the client
and validity issues in our research and integrity
carefully consider the behavior and clarify the
and generalization problems in our treatments.
way that they are thinking about this behavior.
Therefore, taking the time a priori to carefully
This point can be readily transferred to Applied
consider the behaviors to be targeted and how
Behavior Analysis (ABA) and other fields where
best to measure these behaviors over time is the
multiple individuals are typically part of the
first step to sound intervention and research.
assessment and treatment process, and all team
There are several reasons why creating opera-
members must be in agreement with the deci-
tional definitions and recording data on behaviors
sions being made.
is so important to social and behavioral sciences
The focus of this chapter is threefold: (1) to
as a whole and to functional analysis in particular
identify target behaviors for research and inter-
(Haynes & O’Brien, 2000). Defining behaviors
vention, (2) to define these target behaviors so
accurately is necessary in order to make a diag-
that they can be identified by multiple observers
nosis, and many psychological diagnoses require
across settings, and (3) to illuminate methods to
the presence of specific, observable behaviors.
observe and measure the target behaviors. To be
more specific, there are several factors to con-
S. Hurwitz (*) • N.F. Minshawi sider when identifying and defining a behavior
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School for research or intervention. First, one must con-
of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA sider whether the behavior is clinically or socially
Christian Sarkine Autism Treatment Center, significant enough to warrant further attention.
James Whitcomb Riley Hospital for Children, While this is a point that is often overlooked or
Indiana University School of Medicine,
Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA taken for granted, the decision of which behav-
e-mail: sarahbethhurwitz@gmail.com; iors to study is just as important as the manner in
nminshaw@iupui.edu which we go about studying the behaviors.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 91


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_6,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
92 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

The second goal of this chapter is to provide appropriate target, including whether the behavior
guidelines for defining these target behaviors. in question is socially significant, dangerous,
It is important to determine which aspects of the results in restrictions being placed on the individ-
behavior are most important in order to ensure ual, and whether the behavior interferes with the
that the targets of the study have ecological valid- acquisition of other skills (Hanley, Iwata, &
ity and clinical significance. Once this has been McCord, 2003). In addition, it is important to keep
determined, the behavior must be operationally in mind that we often want to focus our attention
defined with sufficient detail that interrater reli- on the promotion of new, adaptive skills in addi-
ability can be obtained. Operational definitions tion to decreasing less desirable behaviors.
for a number of commonly observed problem
behaviors are included in this chapter to provide
the reader with examples of different ways in Social Significance
which these behaviors have been defined in the
recent literature. The social significance of a target behavior is one
The final goal of this chapter is to present important factor to consider in evaluating a
information on different methods of data collec- potential target behavior. Behaviors can be
tion that are of particular relevance to functional socially significant for a number of reasons. The
analysis. Once it has been determined that a behavior may be significant in that it impairs an
behavior is of significant interest to warrant data individual’s ability to interact with others in his
collection and the behavior has been operation- or her community. For example, if a child engages
ally defined, the next step is to determine the in severe tantrum behaviors when his parents
appropriate data collection methodology. It is take him to church, his parents may begin to limit
important to make a good match between the the amount of time the child attends church or
target behavior and the method that will best may leave the child at home while the rest of the
capture the occurrences of that behavior. Proper family attends church. Another way that a behav-
consideration of the strengths and weaknesses of ior can be socially significant is if the behavior
different data collection systems is vital to ensur- results in a negative appraisal of the individual by
ing that the appropriate aspects of the behavior other people. This can frequently be the case with
will be captured and the data collected can be repetitive behaviors, such as hand flapping, in
used meaningfully. individuals with Autistic Disorder. Because hand
flapping is not a behavior observed in typical
social interactions, the fact that an individual is
Identifying Target Behaviors flapping her hands may result in other people
viewing her as “different.” These negative
The first step in identifying target behaviors is to appraisals can lead to decreased attempts by
determine if a behavior is of significant enough others to interact with the person with socially
concern to merit intervention. In other words, one significant behavior issues and thereby reduce
must ask: Is this behavior important? Sometimes opportunities for socialization.
this important question is overlooked for a num-
ber of reasons. For example, some might think
that if one person finds the behavior to be prob- Dangerous Behaviors/Risk for Harm
lematic, then that is sufficient reason to intervene.
Others might think that if a behavior is “interest- A behavior that places an individual and those
ing” or “unique,” then the behavior must be a around him or her at risk for bodily harm may be
worthwhile research target. While these may be reason for intervention or study. The behaviors
appropriate rationales at times, they typically are most commonly associated with risk for harm are
not able to stand alone. Instead, one must consider physical aggression and self-injurious behaviors
other aspects of a behavior that may make it an (SIB). Physical aggression and SIB are among the
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 93

most common reasons for inpatient admissions severe enough that the parents eventually terminate
for individuals with developmental disabilities the demand and allow the child to avoid sitting
(Mandell, 2008) due to the potential for injury. on the toilet. The tantrum behaviors that are ini-
As a result, these behaviors often come to the tially not presented as a target for intervention
attention of researchers and clinicians so they can clearly become more clinically relevant when
be addressed before they cause severe injury. one considers that these behaviors are interfering
Behaviors that currently cause injury or have the with the child’s ability to learn a new, more adap-
potential to cause injury without intervention tive skill. Careful consideration of the anteced-
often become high priority research and interven- ents to behavior problems and the consequences
tion targets. evoked by the behavior will assist in determining
whether the behavior is potentially interfering
with acquisition or demonstration of more appro-
Restrictive Behaviors priate skills.

Many behavior problems are significant because


of the degree of restriction these behaviors can Adaptive Skills
place on the living, educational placements, and
quality of life of individuals with developmental While the majority of behavioral research and
disabilities. Individuals with severe behavior clinical intervention is focused on maladaptive or
problems are more likely to be placed in more inappropriate behaviors, such as aggression and
restrictive living environments, such as group SIB, it is important that clinicians and research-
homes and residential facilities (Lakin, Hill, ers not forget that adaptive and appropriate
Hauber, Bruininks, & Heal, 1983). In addition, behaviors should also be considered as behav-
behavior problems can greatly limit the degree to ioral targets. The prior discussion regarding
which children with developmental disabilities socially significant behaviors can be applied to
and other psychiatric conditions can be integrated determining which adaptive skills that may be
into classroom environments with their neuro- targeted in research or clinical intervention.
typically developing peers. As was previously While we may consider a behavior problematic if
mentioned, some behavior problems can limit the it limits opportunities for social interaction, an
amount of social interactions and social opportu- adaptive behavior may be targeted because of its
nities available as well. ability to facilitate interactions with others. For
example, teaching a young child to say “Hi” or
“Do you want to play?” are useful adaptive skills
Interfering Behaviors because these two behaviors may lead to further
socialization opportunities, such as a conversa-
Finally, when considering whether a specific tion or play. Furthermore, if a problem behavior
behavior problem should become the focus of an leads to negative appraisals from others, then we
intervention or a research study, one should also may consider teaching adaptive behaviors that
consider the degree to which the behavior in can lead to positive appraisals. Teaching appro-
question interferes with the ability to engage in or priate hygiene skills may help to avoid negative
learn more appropriate behaviors. For example, appraisals, while teaching workplace etiquette
parents may be concerned that their 5-year-old such as how to engage in small talk in the break
daughter is not yet toilet trained. But when ques- room may lead to coworkers viewing an individ-
tioned about prior toilet training attempts, the ual in a more positive manner. Regardless of the
parents may report that whenever they attempt to skill, it is important that researchers and clini-
seat the child on the toilet, she engages in tantrum cians carefully consider the adaptive behaviors
behaviors that consist of crying, screaming, and that can and should be taught to individuals with
dropping to the ground. These behaviors are developmental disabilities.
94 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

Table 6.1 Examples of ambiguous terms vs. quantifiable


Creating Operational Definitions descriptions of behaviors
of Target Behaviors Ambiguous term Quantifiable description
Disruptive Child throws his work on the floor
Once a target behavior has been selected based behavior and overturns his chair
on the criteria detailed above, the prescribed pro- Tantrum Child lays on the floor, yells, and
cess for conducting a functional assessment typi- kicks her feet
cally begins with the operational definition of the Aggression Child hits his brother with an open
hand
target behavior (Tassé, 2006). This step is crucial
Being mean Child curses at her classmates
and without a clearly defined target behavior it is
almost impossible to ensure the reliability of the
data being recorded. individuals, such as parents or teachers, to take
Kazdin (2001) recommends three criteria for behavioral data. There are many colloquial terms
creating operational definitions of target behav- for problem behaviors that may have different
iors. First, the behavior should be defined objec- meanings in different geographical areas and
tively. An objective definition is one that is based cultures. For example, one parent may say that
on the characteristics of the behavior that can be her child has a “meltdown” when she screams
observed. Therefore, the behavior should not be and cries, while the teacher may consider these
something that is unobservable, such as a thought, behaviors to be a “fit” or “tantrum.” If both of
opinion, or motivation. In addition, one should these individuals are responsible for taking data
avoid placing subjective, moral, or social judg- on the screaming and crying behavior of the child
ments into the behavioral definitions. For exam- but terms are not clearly and quantifiably defined,
ple, stating that a behavior is “mean” or “cruel” then the data will lack reliability and potentially
might imply that the individual engaging in the validity. Table 6.1 provides examples of ambigu-
behavior has malicious intent, which may not ous terms for common problem behaviors and
always be the case. This type of subjectivity in how these behaviors can be described in a
describing behaviors can also negatively influence quantifiable way.
the individuals collecting data. Kazdin (2001) makes the final point that the
Second, the definition should be clear and operational definition of a behavior should be
unambiguous. The rule of thumb that Kazdin complete. All of the responses that fall within
(2001) provides is that an observer should be able the definition should be clearly outlined, and the
to read the behavioral definition, repeat it, and responses that should be included and excluded
paraphrase it with little explanation from the should be defined and enumerated. The com-
individual who wrote the definition. Unfortunately, pleteness of a definition is important when one
many terms used to describe behavior are ambig- considers the complexity of the behaviors that
uous. For example, a teacher might report that a are the focus of functional behavioral assess-
child is “difficult” or “out of control.” Although ments (FBA). One way in which one can ensure
this is type of statement might be compelling, it is that a definition is complete is to consider includ-
not measurable. By eliminating the subjectivity ing all possible topographies of the behavior. The
in definitions, one can ensure that all individuals topography of a behavior is what the behavior
involved in data collection will be taking data on looks like in objective terms. For example,
the same behavior and not their subjective opin- “physical aggression” can include topographies
ion of the behavior. Converting ambiguous terms such as hitting with an open hand, biting, kick-
into clear statements that can be observed and ing, head-butting, grabbing, and any number of
measured is a crucial component to a good opera- other related behaviors. In order for an opera-
tional definition. tional definition to be complete, it should include
This is an especially important point to con- all of the topographies of the behavior that are of
sider when a clinician has chosen to rely on other concern and explicitly exclude topographies that
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 95

are not relevant to the target behavior. While we one can develop a greater appreciation for the
know that topography does not provide informa- fact that the definitions of these behaviors can
tion as to the function of a target behavior, topog- vary greatly, and without proper definitions, we
raphy can be important in creating an operational can never be entirely sure that we are all speaking
definition that is clear, concise, and complete. the same language. In reviewing the definitions
provided, it is significant to note that the
definitions are clear, succinct, and complete and
Examples of Operationally Defined
all provide sufficient detail that another individ-
Target Behaviors
ual could read the definitions and begin taking
data on the behaviors without need for further
In contemplating how to go about operationally
clarification or explanation.
defining a behavior of interest, it is sometimes
helpful to consider the way that the behavior has
been defined by other researchers. Table 6.2 pres- Observation Methods
ents examples of how several common problem
behaviors have been defined in the recent litera- Once it has been determined that a behavior is
ture. While many of these examples come from significant enough to target with an intervention
the field of ABA, operational definitions were and the behavior has been operationally defined,
also obtained from other child-focused research. the next step is to select an appropriate observa-
By studying the definitions of problem behaviors tion measurement system for use throughout the
utilized by researchers across several disciplines, intervention or research study. There are several

Table 6.2 Examples of operationally defined target behaviors


Target behavior Operational definition References
Tantrum “…saying ‘I don’t want to’ or ‘no’ at a volume above Wilder, Chen, Atwell, Pritchard,
normal conversational level, and whining, defined as a and Weinstein (2006)
high-pitched, unintelligible cry”
Tantrum “…excessive tantrums without aggression (tantrums that Belden, Thomson,
do not include aggression or violence but that included and Luby (2008)
shouting, crying, and/or nondirected flailing
movements)”
Noncompliance “…Noncompliance was defined as refusing to carry out Singh et al. (2006)
instructions or requests made by the mother”
Noncompliance “Verbally skilled noncompliance was defined as the child Johnston, Murray, & Ng (2007)
not following directions, but instead trying to talk things
out reasonably and calmly with the mother”
Self-injury “SIB was defined as using any part of the body to hit Kerth, Progar, and Morales (2009)
another part of the body, using any part of the body to hit
a surface, biting self and kicking self”
Self-injury “SIB was defined as any open- or closed-fist hand-to- Magnusson and Gould (2007)
head or fist-to-body contact; knee-to-head contact from
any distance; or leg-to-leg contact”
Stereotypy “Holding toy near eyes and repeatedly swinging it back Lang et al. (2010)
and forth”
Stereotypy “Vocal stereotypy was defined as acontextual audible Lanovaz, Fletcher,
sounds or words produced with an open or closed and Rapp (2009)
mouth”
Physical aggression “Aggression consisted of hitting, kicking, punching, Kahng, Abt,
grabbing, pulling hair, and throwing objects from less and Schonbachler (2001)
than 0.3 m from another person”
Physical aggression “…defined as hitting, slapping, and sitting on a therapist” Roane, Fisher, Sgro, Falcomata,
and Pabico (2004)
96 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

ways to generate information about problem likely to be seen. For example, if the target
behaviors including parent or teacher interviews, behavior is related disruptive behavior in the
questionnaires, standardized assessments, and classroom, naturally, the observation must take
direct observation. An interview or other assess- place in the classroom setting. On the other hand,
ment tool can provide important information there are times when observations are not practi-
about which behaviors to examine and the cal in the child’s natural environment. For exam-
context(s) in which these behaviors occur. ple, if the targeted behavior may result in harm to
Although interviews and standardized assess- others, such as aggressive behavior, then the
ments play a role in functional analysis, their util- classroom, where other children may be placed
ity is somewhat limited because interviews and in a dangerous position, may not be an appropri-
questionnaires rely on anecdotal reports (Hanley ate setting. In these cases, a simulation of the
et al., 2003), and standardized assessments cannot situation in which the behavior is most com-
be repeated frequently. On the other hand, making monly exhibited can be created in the clinic or
observations directly provides clear, replicable, laboratory instead. In the clinic or lab, the most
and reliable data about behavior. Additionally, probable antecedents and consequences of the
direct observation can be conducted in the child’s target behavior should be employed to elicit
natural environment and provides a concrete the behavior. For whatever setting is selected,
record of what is observed there (Rogers, 2001). the option exists to record the data live (as it
Using direct observation to collect data about happens) or to videotape the session and code the
behavior is a key component to functional data later.
analysis. “One advantage of the direct method is
that the observer is recording the antecedents,
behaviors, and consequences as they occur rather Selecting a Measurement System
than reporting them from someone’s memory.
This information is likely to be more objective The goal of an observation session is to system-
and accurate when it is derived from direct obser- atically record the occurrences of the target
vation” (p. 139, Barnhill, 2005). One downside to behavior. In functional assessment, these obser-
direct observation is that it can be quite time- vations will be repeated frequently and compared
consuming and labor intensive. The time and over time and across social or environmental
effort involved provides important information, conditions to determine the function of the
but it may not be practical in all circumstances. behavior (Herzinger & Campbell, 2007). In order
Since direct observation is the method used most for observation sessions to be comparable, the
commonly in functional assessment (Hanley data must be recorded in the same way each time.
et al., 2003), the remainder of this chapter will If the data collection system, targeted behavior,
focus on developing measurement systems for or operational definition of the behavior change
direct observation of behavior. between observations, then the data can no longer
be compared, and vast amounts of time, effort,
and other resources may be lost. Therefore,
Location before any direct observations begin, a system-
atic plan should be developed to ensure that the
One of the first issues to be addressed prior to target behavior can be recorded accurately and
selecting a measurement system is determining efficiently over time, in all applicable settings,
the location(s) in which behaviors will be and by different observers.
observed. Data can be taken in the child’s natural The first step in choosing a measurement
environment (i.e., in his/her classroom or home, system is to start with a clear definition of the
or in the community) or in a clinic or laboratory target behavior. The definition should include
setting. The choice of location is dependent on important parameters of the behavior, such as
the conditions under which the behavior is most what the behavior looks like, when it begins and
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 97

Table 6.3 Characteristics of behavioral measurement systems


Measurement system Behavioral features Example target behavior
Event recording High or low frequency; discrete Aggression
Partial-interval recording High frequency; can be difficult to count Hand flapping
Whole-interval recording High frequency; continuous Being on-task in classroom
Time sampling Low frequency; continuous Wandering around classroom
Duration Lasts a relatively long time; behavior has a clear Tantrums
beginning and end

ends, if it continues for a long time, and if it Types of Measurement Systems


occurs frequently or not. Once there is an opera-
tional definition, the system that measures the Event Recording
behavior most effectively and usefully should Event recording provides a frequency count of
be chosen. Frequency and duration of the behav- a behavior over an allotted amount of time.
ior are the two most important factors to con- This means that for each observation session,
sider when choosing a recording system. one can know the exact number of times that
Behaviors that occur frequently may occur the target behavior occurred. Event recording is
quickly and then be over (e.g., calling out an best used to track discrete behaviors (behaviors
answer or hitting a sibling), or they may last for a that have a clear beginning and a clear end).
longer time (e.g., wandering around the room These can be positive or negative behaviors,
unengaged or throwing a tantrum). Behaviors can such as correctly using a target word, making
also be very discrete, meaning they have easily inappropriate vocalizations during class, hitting
recognizable beginnings and ends (like raising a another child, or throwing learning materials on
hand to answer a question), or they can be more the floor.
difficult to measure precisely (as in repetitive The first step in event recording is to deter-
hand flapping behavior). All of these factors of mine how long an observation period will be
the behavior should be thought about as the match (e.g., 10 min, an hour, the entire day, or across a
between the target behavior and the measurement week). For high frequency behaviors, a sample of
system is made. time can give sufficient information, particularly
Depending on the type of behavior being if you choose the time of day that the behavior is
examined, the data will be recorded as a fre- most frequently observed or most problematic.
quency count, a rate of occurrence, or duration of Good times to observe might include 20 min of
the behavior. For behaviors that are frequent, it reading time at school, an hour session of speech
may be important to record each incident of the therapy, or during a trip to the grocery store. For
behavior during a specific period of time, or it low-frequency behaviors, it may be necessary to
may be more important to examine the length of record every instance that the behavior occurs.
time that the behavior lasts. For less frequently For example, it might be important to document
occurring behavior, it may be impractical to have each instance of aggression toward a classmate,
a dedicated observer wait all day to see the behav- even if the aggression happens only once every
ior so a sampling procedure employed by an other day.
observer who is also doing other things might be During the observation, a notation is made for
more useful. There are several coding systems each time a behavior occurs. A data sheet should
commonly used for behavioral observations; four include a column to record each instance of the
of the most commonly used systems are (1) event target behavior, the start and the end time for the
coding, (2) interval coding (partial and whole observation session, as well as who is observing
intervals), (3) time sampling, and (4) duration and the date. See Table 6.4 for a sample event
coding (Table 6.3). recording coding sheet.
98 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

Table 6.4 Sample event recording data sheet Table 6.5 Levels of intensity for self-injurious behavior
(SIB)
Child’s name: Jamie
Target behavior: Yells out answer without being called on Level 1 intensity = SIB with no visible damage
by the teacher Level 2 intensity = SIB which results in bruise
Level 3 intensity = SIB which results in breaking of skin
Start End Number Observer
Date time time of vocalizations initials Level 4 intensity = SIB which results in the need for
medical intervention
1/21/2011 9:15 9:45  EMG
1/25/2011 9:18 9:50  EMG
1/26/2011 9:20 9:50  SBH Intensity
There are also scenarios where the frequency of
the behavior is not as important as the severity or
Rate
intensity of each instance. This can be especially
There are times when observation sessions are of
valuable information for those behaviors that
different lengths. In this case, two approaches
may occur at a low frequency but may cause
can be taken. First, the observation could be lim-
varying degrees of harm or damage each time. In
ited so that each session is always the same length
these situations, it may be useful to know the
of time. This can be done by ending observations
level of intensity of each episode of a behavior.
at a specified time even if the activity is not com-
How intense a tantrum is or how severe an aggres-
plete or the behavior is still occurring. For exam-
sive event was may be just as important to track
ple, if the frequency of tantrums during reading
as how often the events occur. If this is a critical
time is being targeted, only observe the first
factor, recording the intensity for each episode of
10 min of reading time regardless of how long
the behavior can provide illuminating informa-
the reading lesson continues.
tion. In order to record intensity, first, develop a
The other way to compare sessions of differ-
rubric by defining observable parameters for each
ent lengths is to calculate the rate that of the
level of the behavior. There can be two levels of
behavior. Rate is the frequency of the behavior
intensity (e.g., high and low) or many levels
expressed as a ratio with time (Alberto &
scored on a likert-type scale (e.g., a score of 1 to
Troutman, 1995). For example, if reading time
5, with 1 being none and 5 being many). Each
lasts for 20 min on some days and 30 min on
level should be well defined so that it can be used
other days, the observation would continue for
across observers. An example of four levels of
the full length of reading time each day, and then,
intensity for episodes of SIB is in Table 6.5. To
a rate of tantrums can be obtained by dividing the
include intensity in data recording, add a column
frequency by the number of minutes. This would
for intensity on to the recording sheet so that for
create a rate of tantrums per minute and allow
each time the behavior occurs, you note what
data to be compared across sessions.
level of intensity was observed.
To calculate the rate, divide the number occur-
rences of the behavior by the length of the ses-
sion. The time element can be per minute, per
Interval Recording
In interval recording, observation sessions are
hour, or some other ratio of time depending on
divided into brief time intervals of several sec-
how often the behavior occurs and how long
onds each (e.g., 10- or 15-s intervals, usually
observations are. For example, for a 30-min
less than 30 s), and the observer records whether
observation, if there are 15 episodes of the behav-
the target behavior occurs in each interval
ior, calculate the per-minute rate by dividing 15
(Barnhill, 2005). This is different from event
by 30 and obtain the rate of 0.5 episodes per min-
recording because only some incidents of the
ute. For observation sessions that are of the same
behavior will be identified, but an estimate of the
length, the rate per activity can also be deter-
behavior is still obtained. Interval recording is
mined. For example, during 10 min of reading
especially useful for behaviors that occur very
time, if five tantrums are seen, the rate of five tan-
frequently or behaviors that do not have obvious
trums per reading session can be used to compare
beginnings or ends. There are three main types
to the next 10-min observation.
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 99

Table 6.6 Interval coding for a 5-min observation session of 20-s intervals

Mark each aspect of tantrum behavior seen during each interval


• Behavior 1: whining/crying
• Behavior 2: yelling
• Behavior 3: hitting self on head
Behavior 20 40 60 20 40 60 20 40 60 20 40 60 20 40 60
1     
2   
3 

of interval recording, partial-interval recording, Behaviors that are continuous, like on-task
whole-interval recording, and momentary time behavior or being out of seat in class, are very
sampling (see next section below). appropriate for whole-interval recording. Whole-
In partial-interval recording, a notation is interval recording is especially useful when there
made if the behavior is seen at all during the is a behavioral goal of increased sustained behav-
interval. Often, this method is chosen when the ior (Steege, Watson, & Gresham, 2009). Whole-
target behavior occurs frequently or for behav- interval recording produces a conservative
iors that are not easily counted. For example, if a estimate of the behavior because if the behavior
child is engaging in a repetitive behavior like does not persist for the entire interval, it is not
hand flapping or rocking, it may difficult to count recorded as present.
how many times the behavior is repeated. Instead, Table 6.6 is a sample data sheet for partial-
the child is observed for a short interval, and a interval recording for three aspects of tantrum
notation is made regardless if the behavior is seen behavior. In this example, the overall tantrum is
once or many times during the interval. divided into three related target behaviors to help
In order to aid data collection, the observer identify how extreme the tantrum becomes. If a
may set aside a few seconds after each interval tantrum occurs during an interval, from one to
to write down what happened so that they do not three, check marks can be made depending on
miss occurrences in the next interval. For exam- how many aspects of the behavior are seen. In
ple, the observer can watch the child for a stan- this example, tantrum behavior was seen during
dard interval of time, say 10 s, and then take the 33% of the observation, and it escalated to self-
next 5 s to record what happened, return to injury for only one interval, about 7% of the time.
observation for 10 s, and again record for 5 s A column that is left blank indicates that the
until the observation session is complete. target behavior was not seen during that interval.
Alternatively, if coding via video, the video can
be watched for 10 s, paused as notations are Momentary Time Sampling
recorded, and then play can be resumed to view Momentary time sampling is commonly used
the next 10-s interval. when more frequent observation is not practical
Interval recording is summarized by the per- (Barnhill, 2005). Momentary time sampling is a
centage of total intervals in which the behavior subset of interval recording and is best used for
was recorded (Furniss, 2009). For example, if the behaviors that are not very frequently occurring.
behavior was noted during 10 out of 30 intervals, These behaviors can be discrete or last for longer
there was 30% occurrence of the behavior. This periods of time. In momentary time sampling,
provides an estimate of the total number of times data is taken at predetermined intervals of time.
the behavior was seen rather than an exact count. At a specific time, the observer looks up, records
For many purposes, an estimate provides enough if the behavior is happening at that instant, and
information, and an exact count is not necessary. then does not make a record again until the pre-
Whole-interval recording uses the same sys- scribed moment of the next interval. This method
tem as partial-interval recording; only the behav- provides only an estimate of the rate of the target
ior must be present during the entire interval. behavior, but it can be very useful if the person
100 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

recording the data is a teacher or other adult who Table 6.7 Duration recording data sheet
is doing several things at once. Child’s name: Jamie
As an example, a teacher in her classroom is Target behavior: Amount of time child gets out of seat
concerned that a child is constantly getting up until she is seated again
from his desk and wandering around the room Date: 1/11/2011
Observation start time: 11:00 am Observation end time:
during math lessons. In this case, an interval of 12:30 pm = Total (mins): 90
five minutes might be used. At the very begin-
Episode Start time End time Duration
ning (or end) of the interval, the teacher looks at
1 11:03 11:08 5 min
the child, records if the child is sitting in his/her
2 11:42 11:50 7 min
seat at that moment, and then continues with her
3 12:20 12:24 4 min
teaching. When another five minutes pass, the Total Duration: 16 min
teacher again looks at the child and repeats the
observation and recording. During a 30-min
lesson, the teacher would have six data points tion recording would be the measurement system
recorded about the child’s in-seat behavior. of choice.
However, if the child left his or her seat between Duration can be measured in seconds, min-
observations and then sat back down, this infor- utes, or even hours. A stopwatch or a clock with
mation would be missed. On the other hand, there a second hand is a good tool for measuring dura-
is enough data to have a good sense of how dis- tion. As soon as the behavior begins, the stop-
ruptive the out-of-seat behavior was during the watch is clicked on, or the start time is recorded.
lesson, and the data could be collected without Time then passes, and when the behavior ends,
the expense of a dedicated observer. The benefits the stopwatch is clicked off or the end time is
of time sampling can often outweigh the poten- recorded. This is repeated for every time the
tial loss of information in settings or with observ- behavior is seen during the observation session.
ers who are not able to devote all of their time to If using the stopwatch, the total cumulative
collecting data or when data must be collected on amount of time spent in the behavior will be
multiple individuals at the same time. apparent at the end of the session. If using a clock
or watch to record the duration of each episode of
Duration Coding the behavior, the total time spent can be added up
The duration of a behavior refers to how long the to give you the duration for the session. A sample
behavior lasts. This is important for behaviors recording sheet for duration can be seen in
that take a long time to resolve, like screaming Table 6.7.
and crying during an elongated tantrum, or In Table 6.7, each episode of the behavior is
behaviors that are not lasting long enough, like recorded on a separate line. Using this type of
staying on-task during homework time. Some of recording sheet also provides a count of fre-
the same behaviors that can be measured using quency because for each duration that is recorded,
event recording can also be measured using dura- one occurrence of the behavior is also noted
tion recording. Therefore, a choice of priority (Glasberg, 2006). Using a stopwatch to time
must be made: is it more important to know how duration does not supply the frequency count, but
many times a child engages in the behavior or it is more parsimonious.
how long they are spending engaging in the
behavior. For example, a child may rock and flap Rate
his hands in front of his eyes on four distinct The length of time that a child engages in a
occasions in an hour observation session, but behavior can be measured in two ways for dura-
each episode may last for 3 or 4 min. In this case, tion coding: the average duration and the total
the duration of time spent during the hour (i.e., duration (Alberto & Troutman, 1995). The aver-
15 min) may be more valuable to know than the age duration is the average time that the child
number of episodes (i.e., four times), so a dura- engages in a behavior across the course of a day
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 101

(or other period of time). For example, if the child provoking and maintaining the target behavior
cries three times in a day for 15, 5, and 10 min (Gresham, Watson & Skinner, 2001). If the goal
each, then the average crying duration for that of functional analysis is to identify a causal rela-
day would be 10 min. The total duration gives the tionship between environmental factors and the
total amount of time a child engages in a behav- target behavior (Asmus, Franzese, Conroy &
ior over the course of an observation session. For Dozier, 2003), having a record of this series of
example, if the child wanders around the room events sheds light on the situation in which the
unengaged for 2-, 3-, and 5-min episodes during behavior occurs.
an hour of observation, the total duration would Recording so much information can be quite
be 10 min of aimless wandering per hour. time-consuming, so the A–B–C analysis can be
used in conjunction with the other measurement
Latency systems discussed earlier in this chapter to pro-
Latency is another time-based system that mea- vide context to support the data taken on duration
sures the interim between behaviors. Latency is or rate of occurrence of the target behavior. That
the length of time between an environmental is, the A–B–C data can be taken for enough time
event and the start of a behavior. For example, to clarify causes of the behavior, and the other
how long it takes a child to get started on a task methods can be used to track the behavior over
after being told to start or how much time elapses time. Table 6.8 is a sample A–B–C recording
between when the child is asked to sit down and form for a child who talks to himself, loudly and
the child actually sits in the seat. Latency is off-topic, during a therapeutic social-skills
useful to know when the goal is to reduce the group.
amount of time between a request and the When planning intervention, the A–B–C data
behavior. can then be quite useful. The context provided by
this observation suggests the antecedent or con-
sequent events that appear to trigger the behavior.
A–B–C Analysis These environmental factors can be manipulated
and data on the occurrence of the target behavior
Another way to collect direct observation data is taken. This systematic manipulation of anteced-
by using the Antecedent–Behavior–Consequence, ents or consequences can be continued until a
or A–B–C, analysis. In this system, when the reduction in the challenging behavior and/or an
behavior is observed, the events occurring imme- increase in an appropriate replacement behavior
diately prior to and following the behavior are is seen (Stichter, 2001). Additional information
recorded. This information can then be used to on intervention will be provided in another
form hypotheses about the events that are chapter of this book.

Table 6.8 Sample A–B–C recording form

Date: January 12, 2011


Behavior: Child talks aloud to himself about movies or TV shows during group activities during social-skills group
Time Antecedent Behavior Consequence
4:05 Therapist says, “It’s time to put toys Child starts talking to himself Therapist asks him to stop talking
away and gather for circle time” in agitated voice about or go to the break area until he can
Transformers be quiet; child goes to break area
4:15 Therapist asks children to take turns Child mumbles to himself Therapist says he does not have to
reading and looks upset read aloud
4:40 Children are eating snack together, Child talks for 5 min about Therapist finally asks him who he
they are prompted to engage Pokemon without asking is talking to and child is quiet for
in conversation with peers questions or pausing to allow the rest of snack
others to comment
102 S. Hurwitz and N.F. Minshawi

Haynes, S. N., & O’Brien, W. H. (2000). Principles and


Conclusion practice of behavioral assessment. New York:
Springer.
Direct observation of behavior is a valuable, Herzinger, C. V., & Campbell, J. M. (2007). Comparing
functional assessment methodologies: A quantitative
although time-consuming, step in providing synthesis. Journal of Autism and Developmental
important insights about the function of challeng- Disorders, 37, 1430–1445.
ing behavior. A well-defined behavior can be Johnston, C., Murray, C., & Ng, L. (2007). Types of non-
reliably measured and tracked over time. By compliance in boys with Attention-Deficit/
Hyperactivity Disorder with and without oppositional
determining the measurement system that pro- behavior. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 29,
vides the most accurate information about a tar- 1–20.
get behavior, data can be systematically collected Kahng, S., Abt, K. A., & Schonbachler, H. E. (2001).
and used for several purposes in functional anal- Assessment and treatment of low-rate high-intensity
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
ysis. Behavioral data can be used to identify the Analysis, 34, 225–228.
cause(s) that maintain a behavior, to develop an Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied
intervention that will address the behavior, and to settings (6th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing.
track the occurrence of that behavior over time to Kerth, D. M., Progar, P. R., & Morales, S. (2009). The
effects of non-contingent self-restraint on self-injury.
determine if the intervention is successful. When Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
carefully considered, this methodological step of 22, 187–193.
observing behavior can provide insights and util- Lakin, K., Hill, B., Hauber, F., Bruininks, R., & Heal, L.
ity across many settings for many individuals (1983). New admissions and readmissions to a national
sample of public residential facilities. American
involved. Journal on Mental Deficiency, 88, 13–20.
Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Machalicek, W.,
Rispoli, M., Lancioni, G., et al. (2010). The effects of
an abolishing operation intervention component on
References play skills, challenging behavior, and stereotypy.
Behavior Modification, 34, 267–289.
Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (1995). Applied behav- Lanovaz, M. J., Fletcher, S. E., & Rapp, J. T. (2009).
ior analysis for teachers. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Identifying stimuli that alter immediate and subse-
Hall. quent levels of vocal stereotypy: A further analysis of
Asmus, J. M., Franzese, J. C., Conroy, M. A., & Dozier, C. functionally matched stimulation. Behavior
L. (2003). Clarifying functional analysis outcomes for Modification, 33, 682–704.
disruptive behaviors by controlling consequence Magnusson, A. F., & Gould, D. D. (2007). Reduction of
delivery for stereotypy. School Psychology Review, automatically-maintained self-injury using contingent
32, 624–630. equipment removal. Behavioral Interventions, 22,
Barnhill, G. P. (2005). Functional behavioral assessment 57–68.
in schools. Intervention in School and Clinic, 40, Mandell, D. S. (2008). Psychiatric hospitalization among
131–143. children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Belden, A. C., Thomson, N. R., & Luby, J. L. (2008). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1059–1065.
Temper tantrums in healthy versus depressed and dis- Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata, T. S.,
ruptive preschoolers: Defining tantrum behaviors & Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method of thin-
associated with clinical problems. The Journal of ning reinforcer delivery during differential reinforce-
Pediatrics, 152, 117–122. ment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,
Furniss, F. (2009). Assessment Methods. In J. L. Matson 213–218.
(Ed.), Applied behavior analysis for children with Rogers, E. (2001). Functional behavioral assessment and
autism spectrum disorders. New York: Springer. children with autism: Working as a team. Focus on
Glasberg, B. A. (2006). Functional behavior assessments Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 16,
for people with autism: Making sense of seemingly 228–231.
senseless behavior. Bethesda: Woodbine House. Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Winton, A. S. W., Fisher, B.
Gresham, F. M., Watson, T. S., & Skinner, C. H. (2001). C., Wahler, R. G., Mcaleavey, K., et al. (2006). Mindful
Functional behavioral assessment: Principles, proce- parenting decreases aggression, noncompliance, and
dures, and future directions. School Psychology self-injury in children with autism. Journal of
Review, 30, 156–172. Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 14, 169–177.
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Steege, M. W., Watson, T. S., & Gresham, F. M. (2009).
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Conducting school-based functional behavioral
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. assessments. New York: Guilford.
6 Methods of Defining and Observing Behaviors 103

Stichter, J. P. (2001). Functional analysis: The use of ana- Wilder, D. A., Chen, L., Atwell, J., Pritchard, J., &
logues in applied settings. Focus on Autism and other Weinstein, P. (2006). Brief functional analysis and
Developmental Disabilities, 16, 232–239. treatment of tantrums associated with transition in pre-
Tassé, M. J. (2006). Functional behavioural assessment in school children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
people with intellectual disabilities. Current Opinion 39, 103–107.
in Psychiatry, 19, 475–480.
Interview and Observation Methods
in Functional Assessment 7
Alison M. Kozlowski and Johnny L. Matson

Act Amendments of, 1997, 20 U.S.C. Section


Introduction 1400 et seq, 1997; Individuals with Disabilities
Education Act Amendments of, 2004, 11 Stat. 37
Challenging behaviors are evinced by individuals
U.S.C. Section 1401, 2004). However, the meth-
who have a variety of disabilities including those
ods of conducting a functional behavioral assess-
with intellectual disability (ID; McClintock, Hall,
ment need not be identical across cases.
& Oliver, 2003; Poppes, van der Putten, &
Techniques are often individualized depending
Vlaskamp, 2010) and/or autism spectrum disor-
on the frequency and severity of the challenging
ders (ASD; Matson, Wilkins, & Macken, 2009;
behavior, availability of resources, and informa-
Mudford et al., 2008; Murphy, Healy, & Leader,
tion that has already been acquired regarding the
2009), as well as those individuals who have mild
challenging behavior and its function(s).
disabilities or do not possess any documented
Functional behavioral assessment, in general,
disabilities (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2006; Kinch,
refers to methods of ascertaining the maintaining
Lewis-Palmer, Hagan-Burke, & Sugai, 2001).
variables of challenging behaviors through both
When formulating treatment plans for these indi-
experimental and nonexperimental means, and it
viduals, clinicians often state that conducting a
comprises three main categories: indirect or anec-
functional behavioral assessment is an integral
dotal assessments, descriptive or naturalistic
part of the process and assists in treatment plan-
observational assessments, and experimental
ning. In fact, federal law in the USA currently
functional analysis (EFA) (Iwata, Vollmer,
mandates that treatment of all challenging behav-
Zarcone, & Rodgers, 1993; Neidert, Dozier,
iors is based on the results of a functional behav-
Iwata, & Hafen, 2010). Although only the former
ioral assessment as stated in the Individuals with
two is discussed in this chapter, it is first critical
Disabilities Education Act Amendments of, 1997
to understand the difference between functional
and 2004 (Individuals with Disabilities Education
behavioral assessment in general and EFA. While
these terms may seem synonymous and are often
confused as such, they are not and should not be
A.M. Kozlowski (*) used interchangeably. Functional behavioral
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, assessment includes a group of possible strategies
LA 70803, USA
used to determine the function(s) of challenging
e-mail: alikoz@gmail.com
behavior, whereas EFA, which is one type of
J.L. Matson
functional behavioral assessment, refers solely to
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA the experimental manipulation of environmen-
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com tal variables to achieve this same information.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 105


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_7,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
106 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

The remaining functional behavioral assessment assessment methods tend to be necessary and/or
techniques do not incorporate experimental preferred in many cases.
manipulation of variables. The focus of this chapter is on interview and
EFA is commonly viewed as the hallmark of observations methods that may be completed as a
functional behavioral assessment (Hanley, Iwata, part of the functional behavioral assessment pro-
McCord, 2003; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & cess. First, overall interview methods is addressed
Richman, 1982/1994; Neidert et al., 2010). This with descriptions of some of the most commonly
is largely in part due to the fact that EFA is the used and most researched interviews currently
only established way in which a causal relation- available being provided. Next, a similar over-
ship can be determined between different func- view is given for direct observation methods and
tions and behaviors, while other functional their examples. Then, since many studies explor-
behavioral assessment strategies only indicate ing the psychometrics of both interviews and
which functions and behaviors correlate with one direct observation methods are in comparison to
another. An earlier chapter of this book provides one another as well as other functional behavioral
an in depth review of EFA and its components, so assessment methods, psychometric data, advan-
it will not be discussed thoroughly here. However, tages, and disadvantages of these tools are offered
it is important to note here that, despite its elite and compared in subsequent sections.
status, EFA is not always practical or safe to
employ. In these circumstances, other functional
behavioral assessment methods, such as those Interview Methods
that will be reviewed here, are necessary.
Instances in which EFA would not be deemed Interviews are among the most common func-
appropriate include when the behavior is not tional behavioral assessment strategies employed
occurring frequently enough to adequately assess (Ellingson, Miltenberger, & Long, 1999; Rojahn,
it in such a setting (Matson & Minshawi, 2007). Whittaker, Hoch, & González, 2007). Use of
If the challenging behavior is occurring only interviews allows clinicians to collect a variety of
rarely, the chances of it occurring within a con- information regarding the challenging behavior(s)
trived setting are also low. The safety of the indi- and bypasses many of the concerns with EFA.
vidual and others also needs to be given Such methodology does not require the target
consideration when conducting a functional behavior to be exhibited during the assessment
behavioral assessment. Severe behaviors that process, which permits assessment of less fre-
may cause injury to the self or others are not ide- quently occurring behaviors and those behaviors
ally assessed through EFA. This is because an that pose serious danger or risk to the self or oth-
EFA requires that the challenging behavior occurs ers. Furthermore, an interview of this kind could
without interruption. Therefore, safety parame- be viewed as a broadband functional assessment
ters frequently employed in the naturalistic set- measure. In contrast to EFA and many scaling
ting would actually interfere with identifying the methods such as the Questions About Behavioral
function of the behavior. Another concern with Function (QABF; Matson & Vollmer, 1995) and
EFA is that factors related to the challenging Motivation Assessment Scale (MAS; Durand &
behavior may be unable to be integrated into the Crimmins, 1992), all of which are thoroughly
assessment process, such as the behavior occur- reviewed in other chapters of this book, the results
ring with specific caregivers (English & Anderson, provide clinicians with comprehensive informa-
2004). Furthermore, EFA typically requires large tion surrounding the target behavior that may
amounts of resources including trained staff, otherwise not be considered. Responses are typi-
significant periods of time, reinforcers, and work cally open-ended and are, therefore, not limited
space that is not always readily available to clini- or restricted by confounding variables or specific
cians or facilities (Matson & Minshawi, 2007). categories of functions. However, as will be dis-
Therefore, alternative functional behavioral cussed later on with respect to the interviews
7 Interview and Observation Methods 107

reviewed herein, there are also drawbacks with The interview was created by modifying the FAIF
functional behavioral assessment interviews, as (O’Neill et al., 1997) and is administered in a
with any other assessment strategy. similar fashion. However, rather than requiring
45–90 min to complete, the FACTS only requires
10–25 min, with administration time dependent
Teacher/Caregiver Interviews on how knowledgeable the informant is with
respect to the student being assessed and the
Functional Analysis Interview Form number and complexity of challenging behaviors
The Functional Analysis Interview Form (FAIF) in question (McIntosh et al., 2008). The interview
is one of the most popular and frequently used comprises two parts: Part A begins by collecting
interview measures for assessing the function(s) narrative information regarding strengths of the
of challenging behaviors. The interview is admin- individual, identifying problem behaviors, and
istered to someone familiar with the individual identifying routines during which the behaviors
being assessed (e.g., parent and caregiver) and most commonly occur (e.g., when, where, and
takes approximately 45–90 min to complete with whom). The last section is completed by
(O’Neill et al., 1997). The FAIF is a paper-and- asking the respondent to provide the interviewer
pencil interview and primarily elicits information with the student’s daily schedule including activi-
through open-ended questions related to the ties, individuals present during different activi-
behaviors in question. It comprises 11 sections ties, the specific problem behaviors elicited at
which probe for information regarding the fol- different times, and the likelihood of these behav-
lowing: (1) descriptions of the behaviors, (2) iors occurring during these times, which is rated
potential bioenvironmental events that may affect on a scale from 1 (low) to 6 (high). Up to three
the behaviors, (3) events and situations that pre- routines are then selected for further assessment
dict the presence of the behaviors, (4) the func- in Part B based on similar behaviors being likely
tions or consequences maintaining the behaviors, to occur during certain conditions.
(5) the efficiency of the behaviors, (6) functional During Part B, each routine identified during
alternative behaviors the individual already dis- Part A is examined separately. Therefore, up to
plays, (7) the individual’s communicative abili- three Part B assessments may be conducted for
ties, (8) things to do and avoid when working the individual. After identifying which routine
with the person to increase their success, (9) rein- will be examined during each specific Part B
forcing items, activities, or events for the indi- assessment, more details regarding the problem
vidual, (10) behavior and treatment history, and behavior are gather through open-ended ques-
(11) a diagram to summarize the information col- tions (e.g., operationally defining the behavior,
lected regarding predictors and/or consequences frequency, duration, and intensity). Next, predic-
of challenging behaviors. Interviewers pose the tors/antecedents and then consequences are
questions to respondents and record the respon- explored with many options being made avail-
dent’s answers in the appropriate space. Follow-up able as well as giving the respondent the opportu-
questions may be asked as needed throughout the nity to include self-identified predictors and/or
interview. consequences. A summary of the behavior is then
compiled, which integrates the antecedents,
Functional Assessment Checklist: behavior, and consequences. This information is
Teachers and Staff later used for development of a treatment plan.
Although its name may imply otherwise, the The respondent rates their confidence in the com-
Functional Assessment Checklist: Teachers and piled summary statement on a scale of 1 (not very
Staff (FACTS) (March et al., 2000) is a semi- confident) to 6 (very confident). Lastly, strategies
structured interview to be used for functional previously and/or currently used for preventing
behavioral assessments with student populations. and treating the problem behavior are named.
108 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Functional Assessment and Intervention Student-Assisted Functional


Program Assessment Interview
The Functional Assessment and Intervention The Student-Assisted Functional Assessment
Program (FAIP) is a computer-based functional Interview (Kern, Dunlap, Clarke, & Childs, 1994)
behavioral interview program originally devel- was the first interview of its kind. The interview
oped for use in school settings (University of is divided into four sections and takes approxi-
Utah, Utah State University, & Utah State mately 20–30 min to administer. The first section
Department of Education, 1999 as cited in contains 12 questions regarding the student’s
Hartwig, Tuesday Heathfield, & Jenson, 2004). schoolwork and classroom to which the student
The program guides the interviewee through five can respond “always,” “sometimes,” or “never.”
sections pertaining to a specific individual and In the second section, open-ended questions are
his/her targeted challenging behavior. In the first posed to the student to assess why and when the
three sections, the interviewee is asked to provide targeted challenging behavior occurs, what
information regarding identifying and setting changes could be made within the school setting
information, antecedents, and consequences. to alleviate the student’s difficulties and to iden-
Prior to continuing, the interviewee is then tify rewards/activities that the student enjoys.
prompted to confirm or disconfirm all anteced- Next, the student is asked to rate all of their
ents and consequences the program has identified classes in terms of how much they enjoy the sub-
based on the information provided. In the fourth ject using a Likert scale with ratings 1–5 where 1
section, the program integrates the identified indicates “not at all” and 5 corresponds to “very
antecedents and consequences to formulate much.” In the final section, what the student likes
hypothesized functions. Up to four possible func- and dislikes about each subject is explored
tions may be elicited from the program: gain through a series of open-ended questions.
attention, obtain access to tangibles, escape/avoid
demands, and sensory stimulation. At this time, Student-Guided Functional
the interviewee either confirms or disconfirms Assessment Interview
each hypothesized function. In the last section, The Student-Guided Functional Assessment
the interviewee is given function-based and Interview (Reed, Thomas, Sprague, & Horner,
research-supported interventions that are specific 1997) was developed for use in school settings
to the individual based on identifying informa- when children are engaging in challenging behav-
tion provided earlier. The interviewee is then able iors within the classroom, mainly talking out of
to choose from these options. turn, teasing/bullying, not following directions,
and not completing work. The interview is bro-
ken down into multiple sections and typically
Student Interviews administered to both the student and teacher.
First, the individual is asked to define the target
Although the majority of functional behavioral behaviors. Next, problematic settings and/or
assessment interviews rely on parents, caregiv- classes are noted by instructing the individual to
ers, or teachers as informants, a more recent complete a daily schedule. The schedule contains
development in the field has incorporated gather- each class or other activity the student partici-
ing information from students/individuals engag- pates in throughout the day as well as the instruc-
ing in challenging behaviors. Being able to derive tor for that class or activity. The individual is then
information from this source allows clinicians asked to rate the likelihood and intensity of the
better insight into challenging behaviors includ- student engaging in the target behavior during
ing the potential for a wider breadth of data. To that class or activity on a scale of 1 (least difficult)
date, several variations of a student-guided func- to 6 (most difficult). A blank diagram is then pre-
tional assessment interview exist which are com- sented to be completed by the individual with
monly adapted from one another. respect to events surrounding the target behavior
7 Interview and Observation Methods 109

(e.g., class demands, teacher demands, receiving ment. While conducting this observation, real-time
attention, and noise/distractions). Events are doc- data is recorded, thereby eliminating the biased
umented in the order in which they occur before effects of retrospective report. Contingency event
or after the target behavior. Lastly, a support plan recording was originally developed to be com-
is developed in a similar diagram where setting pleted in an unstructured format where data is
events and predictors are manipulated, replace- collected by documenting the date, time, anteced-
ment behaviors are contrived, and consequences ent event(s), target behavior (i.e., the challenging
are given for engagement in the challenging behavior), and consequent event(s) in separate
behavior versus the desired behavior. columns. Antecedents refer to the events occur-
ring prior to the display of the target behavior,
while consequent events are those occurring after
Direct Observation Methods the individual has already begun engaging in the
target behavior. The data is descriptive in nature
Although interviews provide clinicians with a detailing the sequence of events in the observer’s
wealth of information, best practice suggests that own words; therefore, it is commonly dubbed the
multiple methods of functional behavioral assess- descriptive or narrative recording format (Cooper,
ment be integrated to determine the function(s) of Heron, & Heward, 2007; Miltenberger, 2001).
challenging behaviors. Observation methods are Additional columns may be included depending
yet another option frequently incorporated into on the goal of the assessment with data in other
comprehensive functional behavioral assess- categories related to the challenging behavior,
ments. While observations certainly involve such as other individuals present at the time of the
direct examination of the individual, it is impor- challenging behavior or the location in which the
tant to understand that observation methods are behavior occurred, being recorded (Rojahn,
not synonymous with EFA. However, in contrast Schroeder, & Hoch, 2008). For an example of a
to EFA, direct observations occur within the natu- blank A–B–C recording sheet of this kind, refer
ral environment of the individual thus allowing to Appendix A.
clinicians the ability to assess situations in which Subsequently, contingency event recording
challenging behaviors actually occur. Furthermore, was expanded to include A–B–C checklists, also
unlike alternative methods of functional behav- known as structured A–B–C data collection. The
ioral assessment, direct observations do not rely premise of the data collection is synonymous
on retrospective report or memory, thereby elimi- with the original A–B–C recording sheets except
nating confounds associated with such reports. that now narrative report for the antecedent
event(s), target behavior, and consequent event(s)
is replaced with options for the observer to sim-
Contingency Event Recording ply check-off based on occurrence. An example
(A–B–C Data/Recording) of an A–B–C checklist is presented in Appendix
B. One such checklist was developed by O’Neill
Contingency event recording, more commonly and colleagues (1997), named the Functional
referred to as Antecedent–Behavior–Consequence Assessment Observation Form (FAOF). This
(A–B–C) data/recording, is by far the most preva- specific observation form contains eight sections.
lent form of nonexperimental observation meth- First, the individual being observed and dates of
ods used in functional behavioral assessment. observation are noted, with the possibility of
This method was actually one of the first func- observations spanning over more than just 1 day.
tional behavioral assessment techniques intro- Next, predetermined time intervals are decided
duced in applied settings and was developed by upon and labeled on the form. These time inter-
Bijou, Peterson, and Ault (1968). Contingency vals are dependent on the individual being
event recording involves direct observation of the observed and may coincide with specific activi-
individual being assessed in their natural environ- ties throughout the day, similar to scatter plot
110 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

data collection which will be discussed shortly. on A–B–C checklists, and it also provides the
This form actually differs from traditional contin- observer with the ability to describe all of the
gency event recordings by including this compo- events regardless of their perceived effect on
nent. The next sections (i.e., behaviors, predictors, the target behavior. However, structured check-
perceived functions, and actual consequences) lists may cue the observer to notice specific ante-
are presented in a checklist format. All targeted cedents or consequences that they may have
challenging behaviors are listed followed by pre- otherwise overlooked or considered irrelevant to
dictors, also known as antecedents. The perceived the situation. Another clearly positive property of
functions section, which also differs from typical checklists is that they are easy and quick to com-
contingency event recordings, prompts the plete. If an observer collecting A–B–C data is also
observer to endorse which listed function he/she working with the individual who is exhibiting the
believes to have brought about the behavior. challenging behavior, which is quite common
Finally, in the actual consequences section, the since parents, teachers, and therapists are often
observer checks the column aptly describing what those collecting the data, it may not be feasible to
occurred following the individual engaging in the expect the observer to provide a narrative on the
targeted challenging behavior. The authors events. This would be especially true if there is a
encourage clinicians to first conduct an interview greater frequency of the challenging behavior.
to choose which behaviors, predictors, perceived Despite the advantages and disadvantages of
functions, and actual consequences should be each A–B–C data collection method, very little
displayed as options on the data collection form. research has yet to examine the differences
With more recent advances in technology, addi- between structured and unstructured A–B–C data.
tional contingency event recording strategies Based on the results of one study assessing the
using a structured format have become available. accuracy and preference of both formats among
For example, similar to A–B–C checklists docu- 16 special education teachers and paraprofession-
menting information on paper, personal data als, the structured format yielded slightly greater
assistants have been used to electronically collect accuracy and was more preferred among teachers
observational data (Tarbox et al., 2009). When (Lerman, Hovanetz, Strobel, & Tetreault, 2009).
using these devices, antecedents, behaviors, and Overall, the accuracy of data collected across
consequences are documented similar to a paper- both methods was only modest due to the teach-
and-pencil checklist format. ers’ lack of knowledge regarding functional
Both contingency event recording formats behavioral assessment. Therefore, further train-
have advantages and disadvantages that clinicians ing in functional behavioral assessment is neces-
should be aware of prior to choosing which sary for teachers, especially those working with
specific data collection method to utilize. Ideally, children who display behavioral difficulties.
individuals with a strong background in functional Contingency event recording data is com-
behavioral assessment will be called upon for the monly interpreted in one of two ways. The sim-
collection of behavioral data. Unfortunately, this plest method is based on a correlational visual
is seldom possible in practice, and parents and inspection of the frequencies of the antecedents
teachers are commonly required to collect the and consequences related to specific challenging
appropriate data. Therefore, one of the more behaviors (Tarbox et al., 2009). If the antecedents
immediate considerations should be the compe- and consequences serve the same function for the
tency of the observer who will be collecting the same behavior, it is sufficient to say that the occur-
data. When using the unstructured, narrative for- rence of the behavior served that single function.
mat, observers are able to describe in their own Then, the most frequently occurring of those
words what events occurred prior to and follow- functions for that specific behavior may be
ing the target challenging behavior. This elimi- hypothesized to maintain the behavior. If anteced-
nates the confound of the observer not ents and consequences do not coincide during a
understanding specific terms commonly located single occurrence of a behavior, the interpretation
7 Interview and Observation Methods 111

becomes more complicated. In these cases, the Once again, the 30-s window is a suggestion
behavior may be maintained by multiple func- which may be modified. As was discussed with
tions or irrelevant correlating antecedents and respect to interpretation of contingency event
consequences may be coinciding with the behav- recording data, conditional probabilities may also
ior. As such, interpretations should be made with be calculated for continuous event recordings.
caution. On the other hand, contingency event Also, due to the nature of continuous event
recording data may also be analyzed by calculat- recording data collection allowing for additional
ing conditional probabilities (Lerman & Iwata, variables to be collected, other calculations may
1993; Mace & Lalli, 1991). First, the proportion also be possible. For example, intervals during
of times the target behavior followed each ante- which a specific antecedent preceded the target
cedent out of all of the times the target behavior behavior divided by the number of intervals con-
occurred is calculated. In addition, the percentage taining that specific antecedent can also be calcu-
of times each consequence followed the target lated since all antecedents are documented
behavior is also calculated. As will be discussed regardless of whether they are antecedents to the
in the next section, conditional probabilities may target behavior. Therefore, this data allows the
also be calculated for continuous event recording clinician to determine how often the target behav-
with additional calculations possible. ior actually followed the antecedent—perhaps
the antecedent occurred frequently without a sub-
sequent occurrence of the target behavior.
Continuous Event Recording Information of this kind can be quite valuable.

Based on Bijou and colleagues’ (1968) original


work on contingency event recording, continuous Scatter Plot Analysis
event recording was subsequently developed
(Mace, Lalli, & Lalli, 1991). Data collection While slightly less direct than data collected
begins by an individual first compiling a list of through contingency or continuous event record-
possible antecedents, challenging behaviors, and ings, scatter plot analysis is yet another observa-
consequences during observation periods. All tional method of collecting data related to the
categories may be broad or narrow depending on function(s) of challenging behavior. To collect
the specific individual being assessed. Then pre- this type of data, predetermined time intervals are
determined time intervals for data collection are decided upon before beginning data collection
established and divided into equal time segments (Touchette, MacDonald, & Langer, 1985).
for data collection. Mace and colleagues suggest Although these time intervals can be as simple as
that 15–60-min observation periods be used with hour or half-hour blocks of time throughout the
10-s time intervals. Therefore, if the designated day, it is strongly suggested that the time periods
observation period is 15 min, the entire period represent different activities occurring during the
can be divided into 90 10-s time intervals. During day or even other changes in the environment,
the direct observation periods, observers use a such as staff shift changes. Recording data
partial-interval recording procedure. If any of the according to differing environmental aspects will
antecedents or behaviors occur during a 10-s allow for easier interpretation of the data. Scatter
period, the appropriate box is marked. This is the plot data is simpler to collect in comparison to
distinguishing difference between contingency contingency or continuous event recording data
and continuous event recording – all antecedents because its collection only requires that an indi-
are recorded regardless of if they are following vidual denote whether the target behavior
by engagement in the target behavior. occurred during the specified time interval rather
Consequences are documented somewhat differ- than supply a descriptive narrative account or
ently; only consequences occurring up to 30 s determine the antecedents or consequences of the
following challenging behaviors are documented. behavior. Data collection can be implemented in
112 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

two ways: either frequency data can be collected conclusions cannot be derived from scatter plot
with a tally mark being placed in the time period data. When Kahng et al. were unable to decipher a
during which the target behavior occurred or data temporal pattern from several visual displays of
can be plotted on a grid during the observation scatter plot data, the authors constructed a control
period. If the tally mark method is chosen, the chart (Pfadt & Wheeler, 1995) for each scatter plot
data is later compiled into a graph with the time to statistically interpret the data. Control charts are
period along the X-axis and the frequency of the commonly used as one of many statistical proce-
challenging behavior along the Y-axis similar to dures to improve industrial organization production.
the visual presentation of the grid data collection However, Pfadt and Wheeler suggest that these
method. This method may also be more feasible statistical procedures may also be applied to the
in settings where training in data collection is behavioral sciences to analyze behavior patterns.
limited since frequency data is often collected to The statistical analysis aims to identify patterns of
monitor progress regardless of its inclusion in a variability that are considered “out of statistical
scatter plot. control.” That is, they are statistically sufficiently
Utilizing the grid option eliminates the need deviant from the mean so as to be significantly
for two steps in the scatter plot process; however, different. Applying this statistical analysis to the
it requires greater time investment during actual same 15 sets of data which had been impervious to
data collection. On the grid, successive days are scatter plot analyses resulted in a temporal pattern
presented along the X-axis, while the time peri- being identified for 12 of the 15 data sets. Although
ods are displayed on the Y-axis. Then for each Kahng et al. state that needing to apply this statisti-
time period over the course of each day, the cal analysis to scatter plot data compromises one
appropriate block is shaded accordingly. of the main advantages of scatter plot analysis (i.e.,
Typically, an empty cell indicates that the target being able to visually interpret the data with ease),
behavior was absent, while a filled cell marks its addition still allows clinicians to identify tem-
presence of the behavior. However, depending on poral patterns of behavior, which is the goal of
the frequency of the target behavior, variations of scatter plot analysis.
this method can be used (Kahng et al., 1998;
Touchette et al., 1985). For example, a blank cell
may represent an absence of the behavior, while a Psychometric Properties
shaded cell denotes low frequencies of the behav- of Interview and Direct Observation
ior and a filled cell indicates high frequencies of Methods
the behavior. The difference between low and
high frequencies of the behavior would be based Since the current chapter focuses on two func-
on predetermined criteria. Although more than tional behavioral assessment methods (i.e., inter-
three different codes can be used, some have views and direct observations), and these two
found this to compromise the interpretability of methods are often compared to one another in
the data (Touchette et al.). studies, data regarding the reliability and validity
Interpretation of scatter plot data involves of the aforementioned methods will be discussed
inspection of the visual display to determine time in a similar fashion. First, some examples of stud-
periods, which correlate with specific events, dur- ies only addressing one form of functional behav-
ing which the target behavior is more likely to ioral assessment will be presented. Subsequently,
occur. Although some researchers find scatter plots examples of studies examining multiple func-
to be advantageous in that they are easily inter- tional behavioral assessment strategies will be
preted visually, simple visual interpretation of reviewed. Please note that the review of psycho-
scatter plot data does not always arrive at a conclu- metric properties presented is not an all inclusive
sion regarding temporal periods during which the compilation of studies regarding the specific
target behavior is most likely to occur (Kahng assessment method, but rather a demonstration of
et al., 1998). However, this is not to say that these recent research.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 113

Interviews students. Inter-rater reliability for the entire FAIP


averaged 63.9% agreement, while inter-rater reli-
FACTS ability for the derived functions averaged 70.96%
An excellent review completed by McIntosh et al. agreement across participants. Test–retest reli-
(2008) provides a wealth of information on the ability was calculated by having one set of 19
psychometric properties of the FACTS. The participants complete the FAIP for a second time,
review aggregated the results of nine separate approximately 30 days following its first admin-
studies assessing the properties of the FACTS in istration. Test–retest reliability averaged 72.66%
a total of 41 children attending public preschools, for the entire FAIP and 81.4% agreement for the
elementary schools, and middle schools. The derived functions. Concurrent validity was
test–retest reliability was found to be strong with assessed by having multiple respondents com-
respect to antecedents, functions, and total behav- plete the FAIP, MAS, and FAIF. There was
ioral hypotheses, while the test–retest reliability 69.44% agreement between the FAIP and MAS,
for setting events was moderate. Inter-rater reli- and 76.34% agreement between the FAIP and
ability was also moderate across respondents. In FAIF. The clinical utility of all three assessments
terms of validity, convergent validity has been was also measured, with results indicating that
most commonly explored by comparing the professionals most preferred the FAIP overall
FACTS to either direct observations or an EFA. when compared with the MAS and FAIF.
Complete agreement between the FACTS and
direct observations reached 90%, while the Student-Guided Functional
FACTS and EFA agreed on functions for 53% of Assessment Interview
the cases. However, it should be noted that there Reed and colleagues (1997) assessed the inter-
were some instances in which there was partial rater reliability of the Student-Guided Functional
agreement between assessment methods. For Assessment Interview by administering the inter-
example, for 5% of validation cases between the view to ten students in the fifth through eighth
FACTS and direct observations, the direct obser- grades, and their corresponding teachers, who
vations pointed toward multiple functions, one of had a history of exhibiting challenging behaviors
which was consistent with the function identified within the school setting. All interviews were
by the FACTS. Similarly, for 24% of the valida- administered first to teachers and then to the cor-
tion cases between the FACTS and EFA, the EFA responding students within 3 days of the original
indicated multiple functions, one of which was interview. When conducting interviews with the
also indicated by the FACTS. Validity based on students, prompting questions were frequently
treatment utility was also explored for 15 stu- incorporated as students often needed guidance
dents. All treatment plans developed based on the throughout the assessment. These were used as
identified function from the FACTS resulted in a follow-up questions to the main questions asked
decrease in targeted challenging behaviors. The during the interview and were standard for all
majority of students experienced at least a 50% interviews with students.
reduction in problem behaviors. Taken collectively, there was 60% teacher–stu-
dent agreement on the entire functional behavioral
FAIP assessment portion of the interview. When break-
A sample of 59 school psychologists, social ing down the results according to the different
workers, and teachers participated in the stan- aspects of the functional behavioral assessment
dardization of the FAIP using a sample of chil- section, agreement was variable. Teachers and
dren in the third through sixth grades who students demonstrated agreement on 81.5% of
engaged in challenging behaviors within the challenging behaviors, with students identifying
classroom setting (Hartwig et al., 2004). For more behaviors than did teachers. The behaviors
inter-rater reliability, 19 pairs of participants that were reported only by the students and not the
were asked to complete the FAIP on 19 separate teachers appear to be those that were not necessarily
114 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

observable to teachers within the classroom possible that the results derived through the EFAs
setting (e.g., possession of inappropriate items), were similarly invalid.
thus at least minimally explaining the discrepancy.
While there was 77% agreement for predictors Scatter Plot Analysis
and consequences of challenging behaviors, there Touchette and colleagues (1985) reported excel-
was only 23% agreement on setting events. lent inter-rater reliability between observers in
Overall, there was 38% agreement on the support collection of data that was used in scatter plot
plan portion of the interview, with agreement analyses for three individuals with ASD. For two
varying between 25% and 48% across prevention of the three children assessed, functions main-
strategies, teaching strategies, consequences, and taining the challenging behavior were identified
setting changes. However, consistency between through scatter plot analysis, thereby causing
the functional assessment and support plan por- function-based interventions to be implemented.
tions for teacher and student interviews was 78% A subsequent reduction in challenging behaviors
and 70%, respectively, suggesting that there was was observed for all clients.
moderate to good ability on behalf of the infor- Symons, McDonald, and Wehby (1998) used
mants to develop treatment plans consistent with scatter plot analysis in two behavior management
their hypothesized functions. Taken collectively, classrooms in Canada for two boys who engaged
there was a 22% agreement across the entire inter- in challenging behaviors frequently throughout
view between teachers and students. the day (i.e., more than 10 times per day). Each of
the two classroom teachers was instructed to col-
lect frequency data for each 30-min interval
Direct Observations throughout the school day. During the study, the
first author (Symons) collected interobserver
Continuous Event Recording agreement data with each teacher for a minimum
Lerman and Iwata (1993) investigated the ability of 20% of school days to ensure inter-rater reli-
of continuous event recordings to identify the ability; the average agreement was 93.0%. The
function of self-injurious behaviors in six adults first author then made a scatter plot visual display
with profound intellectual disability. For five of of each student’s behavior data using symbols to
the individuals, continuous event recordings were denote low, medium, and high frequencies of the
completed for a total of 24 h. For one individual, behavior based on preestablished criteria for each
assessment was conducted for a total of 48 h to individual student. These scatter plots were
determine whether a lengthier assessment period updated on a weekly basis. To assess the validity
would clarify the results. In addition, EFAs were of scatter plot data, team meetings were held
completed for all participants independent of the approximately once each week with the first
continuous event recording results. While EFAs author, teacher, and teacher’s aide present to ana-
were found to identify the maintaining variables lyze the data and identify time periods of con-
of self-injurious behavior in all of the partici- cern, if any. Once one or more time periods of
pants, continuous event recordings appeared to concern were noted, hypotheses regarding the
be successful only in differentiating social versus elevation in the presence of the target behavior
nonsocial functions. Whether attention, escape, during these time periods were proposed, and an
or another social contingency maintained the appropriate intervention was then implemented
behavior could not be discerned through the for one of the time periods based on this hypoth-
descriptive assessment. Additionally, a lengthier esis. For both students, implementation of an
assessment period did not prove effective in fur- intervention based on scatter plot analysis resulted
ther clarifying the results of a descriptive assess- in a moderate decrease in challenging behaviors,
ment. However, it should be noted that EFA thereby supporting the effectiveness of scatter
was held as the gold standard in this assessment plot analysis in functional behavioral assessment
and its results were not validated. Therefore, it is within the classroom setting.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 115

Maas, Didden, Bouts, Smits, and Curfs (2009) Comparisons of Multiple Assessment
used scatter plot analysis to determine the tempo- Methods
ral characteristics of excessive daytime sleepi-
ness and disruptive behaviors in seven adults with Arndorfer, Miltenberger, Woster, Rortvedt, and
Prader-Willi Syndrome. Frequency data were Gaffaney (1994) used a multi-assessment method
collected by parents and/or caregivers across a to assess the maintaining variables of challenging
4-week period between normal waking hours. behaviors in five children ages 2–13 years who
Time periods were broken down into 2-h inter- had varying levels of intellectual impairment,
vals, and within the 2-h time period the presence developmental delays, and/or other psychologi-
of behaviors was rated across two separate situa- cal disorders. The first phase of the study was
tions—activities versus no activities. Each behav- termed the “descriptive assessment” and included
ior received one of three scores; 0 indicated not assessment methods such as administration of the
sleepy/no disruptive behavior, 1 indicated some- MAS and FAIF, as well as contingency event
what sleepy/somewhat disruptive behavior, and 2 recording data collected through direct observa-
indicated very sleepy or asleep/severe disruptive tions independently by the parents and research-
behavior. All codes were operational defined for ers. For one child, all four assessments arrived at
the observers. Interobserver agreement for data the same function. For the remaining four chil-
collection was deemed good. Separate scatter dren, the FAIF and A–B–C data indicated identi-
plots for excessive daytime sleepiness and dis- cal functions while the MAS was inconsistent.
ruptive behaviors were then constructed for each The descriptive assessment data was then com-
participant during activities and during the piled for each child, so that hypotheses regarding
absence of activities. The time intervals were the function(s) of the challenging behaviors could
segmented vertically with activity and non-activ- be made, with the hypothesized function being
ity periods separated, and successive days were chosen as the one supported by the most assess-
segmented horizontally. Scatter plot analysis ments. As such, the FAIF’s and A–B–C data’s
indicated that individuals with Prader-Willi identified function was always the one chosen for
Syndrome exhibited excessive daytime sleepi- manipulation. EFAs were then completed to
ness more commonly during the late afternoon assess the validity of the hypothesized functions
and evening hours, especially when no activities derived through the descriptive assessments. All
were planned. Excessive daytime sleepiness was children’s descriptive assessment results were
also more common on Saturdays, also increasing validated through 90–120 min EFAs. Functional
when there was no activity involvement. A less Communication Training (FCT), a treatment pro-
distinct pattern emerged for disruptive behaviors; tocol frequently implemented to teach individu-
engagement in disruptive behavior was relatively als to appropriately communicate to achieve the
consistent across days and activity involvement. same function their challenging behavior had
However, there was a slight elevation during been maintained by, was then implemented for
weekends when no activities were provided. The two of the children according to the validated
results of this study have many implications for function. Teaching these children to verbally
the use of scatter plot analysis. First, as the request the attention or tangible they desired
authors themselves point out, more concrete significantly decreased the rate of their challeng-
results may have been obtained through the use ing behaviors. Thus, implementation of FCT fur-
of shorter time intervals (e.g., Touchette et al., ther validated the results of both these children’s
1985). Secondly, since the authors based much of descriptive assessments and EFAs. Therefore,
their analysis on the hypothesis that the targeted both the results of the FAIF and contingency
behaviors would increase during periods of inac- event recordings were validated through the
tivity, the information obtained through the anal- findings in this study.
ysis was somewhat limited. Specific activities Cunningham and O’Neill (2000) conducted a
were not considered nor were other possible similar study with three boys aged 3–5 years who
influential factors, such as staff preference. were diagnosed with an ASD. Each child engaged
116 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

in challenging behaviors to include biting self, services for a behavior disorder. The teacher and
physical aggression, and tantrums. Four func- student interviews were developed specifically
tional behavioral assessment techniques were for the study, while the FAOF was used to collect
compared: EFA, an interview (FAIF), contin- contingency event recording data. The interviews
gency event recording (FAOF), and a scaling were administered separately to groups of teach-
method (MAS). While multiple functions were ers and individual students, and they solicited
identified for each child, all four assessment information regarding the behaviors, their ante-
methods arrived at the same primary function for cedents, and their consequences. Summary state-
two of the children. For the third child, the EFA ments were then derived based on the information
and FAIF arrived at the same primary function, collected, and teachers and students were then
while contingency event recording and the MAS individually asked to rank these statements as to
arrived at another identical primary function. In which scenarios were the most problematic, thus
this example, the secondary function identified the most likely to be maintaining the challenging
by the EFA and FAIF was also the same and behavior. With respect to data collected through
served as the primary function identified by con- the FAOF, rankings were made similarly by cal-
tingency event recording and the MAS, and vice culating the percent occurrence of each function
versa. Therefore, although the sample size within across all observations. Interobserver agreement
the study was quite small, a limitation that will be was also calculated for the direct observations,
discussed later, these findings suggest that the with an average agreement of 80%. Results indi-
aforementioned functional behavioral assessment cated a significant discrepancy between the chal-
methods were able to reliably identify the same lenging behaviors identified by teachers and
function albeit at different rankings. students with only a 30% rate of agreement.
Alter, Conroy, Mancil, and Haydon (2008) Much like the study conducted by Reed and col-
implemented four different functional behavioral leagues (1997), teachers were less likely to iden-
assessment techniques with four children who tify behaviors that were not easily observable
were at risk for emotional and behavioral disor- within the classroom setting. Overall, there was a
ders. The FAIF, MAS, and A–B–C recordings 64% agreement across all three functional behav-
were all compared to EFA, which was designated ioral assessment methods (i.e., teacher interview,
as the most valid method of assessment. When student interview, and contingency event record-
compared, the FAIF, MAS, and A–B–C recording ing) with respect to accuracy of identified func-
methods all demonstrated low agreement with one tion as well as rank order of that function with
another. Furthermore, the FAIF and MAS also respect to other noted functions. The remaining
demonstrated low consistency with EFA. 36% of cases displayed agreement between
Therefore, within this sample, the FAIF was not teacher interviews and contingency event record-
deemed a valid assessment of maintaining vari- ing data but not with student interviews.
ables of challenging behaviors. On the other hand, Newcomer and Lewis (2004) investigated the
A–B–C recordings were designated as the only validity of descriptive assessment methods (i.e.,
assessment method which corroborated the teacher interviews, student interviews, scatter
findings of an EFA for all four children. Therefore, plots, and A–B–C recordings) in three children
although contingency event recording does not ages 9–11 years old attending public elementary
involve experimental manipulation of variables schools who were displaying behavioral
present within the individual’s immediate envi- difficulties putting them at risk for failure that
ronment as does EFA, this study indicated that the school year. Assessment occurred in three
results of these two assessments are quite similar. phases—the first phase explored functions utiliz-
Murdock, O’Neill, and Cunningham (2005) ing the aforementioned descriptive assessment
assessed the reliability and validity of teacher methods, the second phase generated hypotheses
interviews, student interviews, and contingency based on the descriptive assessment methods,
event recordings. Eight boys ages 12–15 partici- and an EFA was conducted during the third phase
pated in the study, all of which were receiving to confirm the hypotheses generated. Across all
7 Interview and Observation Methods 117

three children, the A–B–C recordings, scatter nonsocial variables. No discernable pattern of
plots, student interviews, and EFA demonstrated object mouthing was seen across conditions,
convergent validity. The teacher interview, which thereby confirming the results of contingency
was conducted using an adapted FAIF, corrobo- event recordings in that the behavior was main-
rated the findings for two of the three children, tained by nonsocial variables.
while the third child’s FAIF indicated the child’s Ervin, DuPaul, Kern, and Friman (1998) uti-
primary function as his secondary one. Therefore, lized a teacher interview, student interview
taken together, it appears that all of the descrip- (Student Assisted Functional Assessment
tive assessment methods were valid in identify- Interview), and direct observations to formulate
ing the maintaining variable of these three hypotheses regarding the function of two teenage
children’s challenging behaviors. Based on these boys’ off-task behavior within the classroom.
maintaining variables, function-based treatments Both boys met diagnostic criteria for attention-
and nonfunction-based treatments were imple- deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional
mented. For all children, function-based treat- defiant disorder at the time of the study. Based on
ments resulted in a significant decrease in the cumulative results of the descriptive assess-
challenging behaviors when compared with base- ments, which all pointed toward identical func-
line. Nonfunction-based treatments were met tions, function-based intervention plans were
with increases and significant variability in chal- implemented for both of the boys. A significant
lenging behaviors for two of the students, and a decrease was seen in the off-task behavior of both
slight decrease in one student. However, for the boys during intervention phases with an increase
student who experienced decreases in challeng- in the behaviors occurring during reversal proce-
ing behaviors both during function- and nonfunc- dures. Therefore, the cumulative results of the
tion-based treatments, the gains were greater with comprehensive functional behavioral assessment
the former. appeared valid in identifying the variables main-
Mueller and Kafka (2006) completed a com- taining both of the boys’ off-task behavior, allow-
prehensive functional behavioral assessment for ing for appropriate interventions to be put in
a 4-year-old girl who engaged in object mouthing place.
within the classroom setting. Techniques
employed included parent and teacher interviews,
contingency event recording, and EFA. The inter- Overview of Interviews in Functional
views conducted were not according to a specified Behavioral Assessment
protocol, but did elicit information regarding
antecedents and consequences of object mouth- Given that interviews are among the most popular
ing. Taken together, the parent and teacher inter- method of functional behavioral assessment,
views were relatively inconclusive in identifying attention needs to be given to their potential use in
specific antecedents likely to precede object identifying maintaining variables of challenging
mouthing. However, an attention function was behaviors and aiding in implementation of appro-
hypothesized based on information acquired priate interventions. To date, minimal research has
regarding consequences since the consequence to been conducted on the psychometric properties of
mouthing was always a verbal reprimand. Based various interviews, and the results of studies that
on contingency event recordings, mouthing was have been completed are relatively inconsistent
only potentially maintained by attention in the with one another. While some have found parent,
form of verbal reprimands, but most likely main- caregiver, and/or teacher interviews to be quite
tained by a nonsocial function as it occurred beneficial in identifying the function of challeng-
across various situations without discrimination. ing behaviors (e.g., Cunningham & O’Neill, 2000;
Finally, an EFA was conducted with attention and McIntosh et al., 2008; Newcomer & Lewis, 2004),
alone conditions to distinguish whether object others tend to find that these assessment methods
mouthing was maintained by attention or are invalid (e.g., Alter et al., 2008).
118 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Many variables may play a role in the differ- challenging behavior most commonly occurred,
ences found between studies. O’Neill and col- and self-assessed experience with functional
leagues (1997) assert that the FAIF should be behavioral assessment. At the conclusion of the
administered by a professional with training in interview, the teachers and staff were asked to rate
functional assessment. Although this is a rela- how confident they were that their interview had
tively undisputable claim with respect to all func- identified the correct function on a 6-point Likert
tional assessment methods, it appears appropriate scale ranging from 1 (not confident) to 6 (very
to say that this may hold even more truth for func- confident). Then, an EFA was conducted to
tional assessment interviews, as opposed to rating identify the function of the behavior.
scales, due to the unstructured nature of the assess- Although the vast majority of informants
ment process as well as the clinical judgment reported possessing little to no experience with
needed to interpret the results. While the FAIF conducting a functional behavioral assessment or
and other interview methods are exceedingly thor- developing a treatment plan, it was found that
ough, they produces a much more complex set of 91.4% of respondents rated their confidence as a
data when compared with rating scales due to the 4 or higher. However, the only significant finding
open-ended format of the interview as well as the was that those individuals who were highly
lack of a scoring algorithm (Sturmey, 1994). confident and identified the correct function had
Therefore, it is possible that the findings of stud- significantly more exposure to the student both
ies differed based on the training of those admin- throughout the school day and during times in
istering the interviews. In fact, despite its popular which the student engaged in the targeted behav-
use, research has also identified interviews to be ior. Therefore, when choosing a respondent for a
the assessment method with which clinicians have functional behavioral assessment interview, it
had the least amount of training (Ellingson et al., seems appropriate that those being interviewed
1999). Furthermore, when asked to rate how easy should be individuals who have considerable
different functional behavioral assessment strate- exposure to the student. However, exposure is not
gies were to use, interviews were rated as being sufficient. The respondent must also indicate that
more difficult to use than scaling methods and they are confident in their ratings. Unfortunately,
direct observations. Interviews were also rated confidence ratings cannot be obtained until the
less effective in determining the function(s) of interview is complete. Though this is without
behavior and less useful when compared with question a limitation of this finding, this informa-
EFA and direct observation, but to be more effec- tion is still valuable in determining if the already
tive and more useful in comparison to rating scales. administered interview is likely valid. Clearly,
In addition to these concerns about interviews more research needs to be conducted to investi-
overall, another major limitation of interviews is gate this relationship. In the meantime, it is sug-
that they rely on retrospective report. gested that informants be those who are familiar
The utility of interviews in functional behav- with the individual both during and outside of
ioral assessment does not solely rely on the specific behavioral challenges. Furthermore, if confidence
interview administered nor its psychometrics but ratings are later found to be weak, those conduct-
also the respondent participating in the interview ing the interview are advised to interpret the
process. Borgmeier and Horner (2006) investi- results with caution or to weigh the results of
gated the predictive validity of confidence ratings other interviews more heavily.
made by the respondents. A total of 63 teachers Experience with functional behavioral assess-
and staff participated in completing the FACTS ment is another factor that may affect the validity
for nine students. Five to eight teachers or staff of interview results. Although Borgmeier and
completed the interview for each of the nine Horner (2006) did not find a correlation between
students, all of whom varied in their exposure to the validity of interview results and experience
the student during the school day, exposure to with functional behavioral assessment, a
the student during periods when the targeted significant flaw with this finding is that the
7 Interview and Observation Methods 119

experience of functional behavioral assessment type of functional behavioral assessment to be


was self-assessed on a Likert scale. Therefore, it deemed inappropriate. Therefore, it is highly
may be that participants rated their experience suggested that although the effectiveness of stu-
and knowledge with functional behavioral assess- dent interviews should continue to be explored,
ment based on different factors. Other research they should not be used in isolation, even with
has found that training informants on aspects of individuals without intellectual impairments.
functional behavioral assessment actually does With respect to standard parent, teacher, and
lead to an increased ability to accurately identify caregiver interviews, the FAI and FACTS both
the functions of challenging behaviors (McNeill, have moderate to strong research support with
Watson, Henington, & Meeks, 2002). Therefore, respect to their reliability and validity. At the
conducting interviews with informants who have same time, this is not to say that some research
at least some background in functional behav- has not suggested otherwise or that a sufficient
ioral assessment would prove to be beneficial. amount of research has been conducted as of yet.
The strong suggestion to choose informants For example, all studies exploring these inter-
who have knowledge of functional behavioral views have only included a small number of par-
assessment clearly speaks against the idea of ticipants. Therefore, reliability and validity
including students or individuals engaging in the findings need to be interpreted with caution.
challenging behaviors in the interview process. However, although these interviews may be less
However, this is not necessarily the case. systematic and have less research to support their
Collecting information from those engaging in psychometric reliability and validity, they are not
challenging behaviors may be quite beneficial, without their advantages. As was previously
with students having the ability to identify inter- stated, interviews do not require the individual in
vention strategies that may assist them person- question to be present nor that the targeted behav-
ally. Furthermore, as was commonly seen when ior, which may pose danger to the individual and/
conducting student interviews, students are likely or others, be exhibited. Interviews also require
to identify more behavior problems than teachers significantly less time to complete than EFAs.
due to exposure limitations on the part of teach- The majority of functional behavioral assess-
ers (Murdock et al., 2005; Reed et al., 1997). Yet, ments likely include at least a minor interview
there was some discrepancy between studies with regardless of its effectiveness in identifying func-
respect to teacher and student interviews corrob- tions. This is because interviews prompt infor-
orating each other’s findings (Ervin et al., 1998; mants to supply basic information that EFA,
Murdock et al.; Newcomer & Lewis, 2004; Reed direct observations, and scaling methods do not.
et al.). Additionally, despite the possible advan- Most interviews, such as the FAIF and FACTS,
tages of including the student or individual require the respondent to operationally define the
engaging in the challenging behavior in the func- targeted behavior, a critical piece of information
tional behavioral assessment interview process, that other functional behavioral assessment
this assessment method may not be appropriate methods do not incorporate. It is not that these
for all populations. At present, research has only alternative methods are overlooking the importance
documented its use among individuals, primarily of this information, but rather that it is assumed
students, who have either a mild disability or no that this information has already been gathered
diagnostic label. Therefore, future research needs through an interview. Therefore, at least a brief
to explore whether individuals with intellectual interview should be mandatory when beginning a
and/or developmental disabilities would benefit functional behavioral assessment as it may be
from participation in this form of functional seen as a starting point to any functional behav-
behavioral assessment. It is likely that deficits ioral assessment, especially when the function(s)
associated with intellectual and/or developmental of behavior are elusive to the assessor.
disabilities may hinder the individual’s ability to Although very little research has been
provide accurate information, thus causing this conducted on the FAIP to date, the results of the
120 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

preliminary study are exciting and provide initial their status as correlational assessment methods.
evidence that the FAIP may be a useful interview In comparison to EFA, these methods require
assessment for use when conducting functional considerably less resources. Relatively little
behavioral assessments. It differs from the major- training is required to collect and interpret the
ity of other interviews by interpreting the narra- data, and supplies needed are of little or no cost.
tive reports from respondents, thus formulating Furthermore, since direct observation data is col-
hypotheses regarding the function(s) of behav- lected during the individual’s regular activities, it
iors. Since the lack of a scoring algorithm is one does not often require extra personnel or time to
of the more prominent disadvantages to func- complete. Although direct observations are cri-
tional behavioral assessment interviews, the tiqued for only being able to identify correlations
FAIP’s built in scoring program is a major advan- between behaviors and antecedents/conse-
tage. Not only does it sidestep the difficulty in quences, a major limitation of EFAs, the fact that
interpretation of the results, but also it does not the behaviors are not occurring in the natural
require significant expertise in the area of func- environment, is overcome by this method of
tional behavioral assessment. Furthermore, since assessment.
the FAIP is administered by a computer as Despite the similarity in the overall method of
opposed to a professional, its use significantly collecting direct observation data, there are
reduces the need for personnel resources that significant differences between the three methods
may not be available. The cost of administering discussed within this chapter that warrant consid-
the FAIP is also another likely advantage of the eration. Direct observations vary in their simplic-
interview due to less resources being needed and ity, range of data, and validity. While scatter plot
because a one-time fee would be in place as data is the simplest to collect, its results are more
opposed to purchasing of multiple assessment ambiguous than either contingency or continuous
measures. However, this is not to say that the event recordings since the presence of targeted
FAIP is not without its disadvantages. Clearly, a behaviors is merely correlated with different time
great deal of more research is needed. Also, in periods, which is in turn correlated with different
contrast to other interviews which appear to be activities occurring throughout the day. Though
more broad based in nature, as they will probe some have found the results of such analyses to
about many different factors within the individual’s be beneficial and helpful in formulating treatment
environment that may be affecting his/her behav- plans, results are somewhat speculative in nature.
ior, the FAIP narrows its results down to four On the other hand, contingency and continuous
general hypotheses. It should also be noted that event recordings provide significantly more infor-
although the FAIP makes a significant contribu- mation to the clinician and can also actually inte-
tion to treatment by providing function- and grate some aspects of a scatter plot analysis since
research-based interventions, a professional is the time of day can similarly be documented.
needed to implement and monitor the effective- However, contingency and continuous event
ness of these treatments. recordings also require more training in the area
of functional behavioral assessment to ensure
that accurate data is being collected. Furthermore,
Overview of Direct Observation these methods require more time and attention be
Methods in Functional Behavioral given to data collection while conducting obser-
Assessment vations since significantly more documentation is
required. Although continuous event recordings
Direct observation data, including contingency would appear to be superior over contingency
event recordings, continuous event recordings, event recordings, the former may be just as unrea-
and scatter plots, can greatly assist in the func- sonable to conduct as EFAs since it requires con-
tional behavioral assessment process despite stant documentation throughout a predetermined
7 Interview and Observation Methods 121

observation period. In fact, it is rather unlikely alternative functional behavioral assessment


that staff working with an individual to be techniques exist, none are without their flaws.
observed would be capable of completing a con- While each of these overall methods and their
tinuous event recording while working with the specific strategies have different advantages and
individual. disadvantages to consider prior to beginning any
Based on the advantages and disadvantages of functional behavioral assessment, the key to a
the three direct observation methods discussed comprehensive functional behavioral assessment
here in comparison to one another, it is not sur- does not rely on only one method but rather a col-
prising that contingency event recordings are laboration of different methodologies to assist in
often chosen in lieu of either of the other two the treatment planning process. Furthermore,
methods. This, of course, means that there has functional behavioral assessments should be indi-
also been less research conducted on either of the vidualized so that one set protocol is unlikely to
other two methods. The research that has been be appropriate for all cases.
conducted on scatter plot analyses and continu- Didden (2007) suggests a seven-step plan for
ous event recordings to date is at best inconsis- conducting a thorough functional behavioral
tent. However, the most common of the direct assessment: (1) identify and operationally defined
observation methods, contingency event record- the targeted challenging behaviors, (2) utilize
ing, is also the direct observation method with the direct observation methods such as contingency
most evidence to support its use in functional event recordings and scatter plots, (3) administer
behavioral assessment (e.g., Alter et al., 2008; interviews and scales to those familiar with the
Newcomer & Lewis, 2004). Although contin- individual, (4) complete an EFA, (5) integrate
gency event recording cannot be considered syn- results from functional behavioral assessment to
onymous with EFA by any means, the results formulate hypotheses regarding the function(s)
have repeatedly been found to corroborate those of targeted challenging behaviors, (6) develop a
found through an EFA. Therefore, contingency treatment plan based on the derived function
event recordings should routinely be completed maintaining the targeted challenging behavior(s),
when conducting a functional behavioral assess- and (7) monitor effectiveness of treatment inter-
ment, especially if resources do not permit that ventions. Although this seven-step plan appears
an EFA or more comprehensive assessment be to be without question the ideal assessment, in
conducted. most cases it is not practical. Therefore, in cases
in which a comprehensive functional behavioral
assessment involving an EFA or solely an EFA
Conclusion cannot be conducted, it is proposed that alterna-
tive, brief functional behavioral assessment strat-
EFA, which is often deemed the gold standard of egies be used initially with a progression to more
functional behavioral assessment methods, is not time-consuming and labor-intensive methods
always practical, safe, or even possible. Therefore, as deemed necessary (Vollmer et al., 1995).
alternative functional behavioral assessment With this progression as the basis to functional
techniques are often deemed necessary. Within behavioral assessment, a brief interview and
this chapter, various interview and direct obser- some form of direct observation, preferably
vation methods commonly used to aid in func- contingency event recordings, should undoubt-
tional behavioral assessment have been reviewed. edly be included with more intense methods
All of these assessments have their own strengths being incorporated when needed. In this manner,
and weaknesses, which have been addressed the most parsimonious way of identifying func-
accordingly. Based on the information presented tions maintaining challenging behaviors can be
herein, it should be apparent that although many accomplished.
122 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Appendix A References1
Alter, P. J., Conroy, M. A., Mancil, G. R., & Haydon, T.
Antecedents Consequence (2008). A comparison of functional behavior assess-
Date/ (what happened Target (what happened ment methodologies with young children: Descriptive
time right before?) behavior right after?) methods and functional analysis. Journal of Behavioral
Education, 17, 200–219.
Arndorfer, R. E., Miltenberger, R. G., Woster, S. H.,
Rortvedt, A. K., & Gaffaney, T. (1994). Home-based
descriptive and experimental analysis of problem
behaviors in children. Topics in Early Childhood
Special Education, 14, 64–87.
*Bergstrom, M. K. (2003). Efficacy of school-based teams
conducting functional behavioral assessment in the
general education environment. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Oregon.
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968).
A method to integrate descriptive and experimental
field studies at the level of data and empirical concepts.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191.
*Borgmeier, C. & Horner, R. H. (2006). An evaluation of
Appendix B the predictive validity of confidence ratings in identi-
fying accurate functional behavioral assessment
hypothesis statements. Journal of Positive Behavior
Interventions, 8, 100–105.
Date/time *Carter, D. L. & Horner, R. H. (2007). Adding functional
Staff initials behavioral assessment to First Step to Success: A case
Behavior study. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9,
Physical aggression 229–238.
Out of seat Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
Location
River: Pearson.
Classroom Cunningham, E., & O’Neill, R. E. (2000). Comparison of
Hallway results of functional assessment and analysis methods
Bathroom with young children with autism. Education and
Cafeteria Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental
Disabilities, 35, 406–414.
Playground
Didden, R. (2007). Functional analysis methodology in
Antecedents developmental disabilities. In P. Sturmey (Ed.),
Direction given Functional analysis in clinical treatment (pp. 65–86).
Preferred item removed Burlington: Academic.
Transition Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1992). The Motivation
Denied request Assessment Scale administrative guide. Topeka:
Monaco & Associates.
No staff attention
Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., & Long, E. S.
Other (write on back) (1999). A survey of the use of functional assessment
Consequences procedures in agencies serving individuals with devel-
Verbal reprimand opmental disabilities. Behavioral Interventions, 14,
Redirection to current task 187–198.
Ignored
Given tangible 1
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies
Allowed to escape activity
included in the McIntosh et al. (2008) review paper dis-
Other (write on back) cussed within.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 123

English, C. L., & Anderson, C. M. (2004). Effects of injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
familiar versus unfamiliar therapists on responding in Analysis, 26, 293–319.
the analog functional analysis. Research in Maas, A. P. H. M., Didden, R., Bouts, L., Smits, M. G., &
Developmental Disabilities, 25, 39–55. Curfs, L. M. G. (2009). Scatter plot analysis of exces-
Ervin, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., Kern, L., & Friman, P. C. sive daytime sleepiness and severe disruptive behav-
(1998). Classroom-based functional and adjunctive ior in adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: A pilot
assessments: Proactive approaches to intervention study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
selection for adolescents with attention deficit hyper- 529–537.
activity disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Mace, F. C., & Lalli, J. S. (1991). Linking descriptive and
31, 65–78. experimental analyses in the treatment of bizarre
*Filter, K. J., & Horner, R. H. (2009). Function-based aca- speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
demic interventions for problem behavior. Education 553–562.
and Treatment of Children, 11, 222–234. Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., & Lalli, E. P. (1991). Functional
Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. C. (2006). Functional assess- analysis and treatment of aberrant behavior. Research
ment, collaboration, and evidence-based treatment: in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 155–180.
Analysis of a team approach for addressing challeng- *March, R. E. & Horner, R. H. (2002). Feasibility and
ing behaviors in young children. Journal of School contributions of functional behavioral assessment in
Psychology, 44, 231–252. schools. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Disorders, 10, 158–170.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. March, R. E., Horner, R. H., Lewis-Palmer, T., Brown, D.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. Crone, D., Todd, A. W., et al. (2000). Functional
Hartwig, L., Tuesday Heathfield, L., & Jenson, W. R. Assessment Checklist: Teachers and Staff (FACTS).
(2004). Standardization of the Functional Assessment Eugene: Educational and Community Supports.
and Intervention Program (FAIP) with children who Matson, J. L., & Minshawi, N. F. (2007). Functional
have externalizing behaviors. School Psychology assessment of challenging behavior: Toward a strategy
Quarterly, 19, 272–287. for applied settings. Research in Developmental
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments Disabilities, 28, 353–361.
of 1997, 20 U.S.C. Section 1400 et seq. (1997). Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. (1995). Questions About
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments Behavioral Function (QABF). Baton Rouge: Disability
of 2004, 11 Stat. 37 U.S.C. Section 1401. (2004). Consultants, LLC.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Macken, J. (2009). The rela-
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis tionship of challenging behaviors to severity and
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2,
in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). 29–44.
Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Rodgers, T. McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark-
A. (1993). Treatment classification and selection based ers associated with challenging behaviours in people
on behavioral function. In R. Van Houten & S. Axelrod with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study.
(Eds.), Behavior analysis and treatment (pp. 102–125). Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47,
New York: Plenum. 405–416.
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., Fischer, S. M., Page, T. J., McIntosh, K., Borgmeier, C., Anderson, C. M., Horner, R.
Treadwell, K. R. H., Williams, D. E., et al. (1998). H., Rodriguez, B. J., & Tobin, T. J. (2008). Technical
Temporal distributions of problem behavior based on adequacy of the Functional Assessment Checklist:
scatter plot analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Teachers and Staff (FACTS) FBA interview measure.
Analysis, 31, 593–604. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 10,
Kern, L., Dunlap, G., Clarke, S., & Childs, K. E. (1994). 33–45.
Student-assisted functional assessment interview. *McKenna, M. K. (2006). The role of function-based
Diagnostique, 19, 29–39. academic and behavior supports to improve reading
Kinch, C., Lewis-Palmer, T., Hagan-Burke, S., & Sugai, achievement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
G. (2001). A comparison of teacher and student func- University of Oregon.
tional behavior assessment interview information from McNeill, S. L., Watson, T. S., Henington, C., & Meeks, C.
low-risk and high-risk classrooms. Education and (2002). The effects of training parents in functional
Treatment of Children, 24, 480–494. behavior assessment on problem identification, prob-
Lerman, D. C., Hovanetz, A., Strobel, M., & Tetreault, A. lem analysis, and intervention design. Behavior
(2009). Accuracy of teacher-collected descriptive Modification, 26, 499–515.
analysis data: A comparison of narrative and struc- Miltenberger, R. G. (2001). Behavior Modification: prin-
tured recording formats. Journal of Behavioral ciples and procedures (2nd ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth/
Education, 18, 157–172. Thomson Learning.
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and Mudford, O. C., Arnold-Saritepe, A. M., Phillips, K. J.,
experimental analyses of variables maintaining self- Locke, J. M., Ho, I. C. S., & Taylor, S. A. (2008).
124 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Challenging behaviors. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Clinical Rojahn, J., Schroeder, S. R., & Hoch, T. A. (2008). Self-
assessment and intervention for autism spectrum dis- injurious behavior in intellectual disabilities.
orders (pp. 267–297). London: Elsevier. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Mueller, M. M., & Kafka, C. (2006). Assessment and Rojahn, J., Whittaker, K., Hoch, T., & González, M. L.
treatment of object mouthing in a public school class- (2007). Assessment of self-injurious and aggressive
room. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 137–154. behavior. In L. Glidden & J. L. Matson (Eds.),
Murdock, S. G., O’Neill, R. E., & Cunningham, E. (2005). International review of research in mental retardation
A comparison of results and acceptability of functional (Handbook of assessment in persons with intellectual
behavioral assessment procedures with a group of mid- disability, Vol. 34, pp. 281–319). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
dle school students with emotional/behavioral disorders *Salentine, S. P. (2003, May). The impact of “contextual fit”
(E/BD). Journal of Behavioral Education, 14, 5–18. on the selection of behavior support plan procedures. In
Murphy, O., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2009). Risk factors R. H. Horner (Chair), Moving from functional assess-
for challenging behaviors among 157 children with ment to the design of behavior support. Symposium
autism spectrum disorder in Ireland. Research in conducted at the conference of the International
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 474–482. Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco.
Neidert, P. L., Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., & Hafen, M. *Schindler, H. R. & Horner, R. H. (2005). Generalized
(2010). Behavior analysis in intellectual and develop- reduction of problem behavior of young children with
mental disabilities. Psycholgoical Services, 7, autism: Building transsituational interventions.
103–113. American Joumal of Mental Retardation, 110, 36–47.
Newcomer, L. L., & Lewis, T. J. (2004). Functional Sturmey, P. (1994). Assessing the functions of aberrant
behavioral assessment: An investigation of assessment behaviors: A review of psychotropic instruments.
reliability and effectiveness of function-based inter- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24,
ventions. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral 293–304.
Disorders, 12, 168–181. Symons, F. J., McDonald, L. M., & Wehby, J. H. (1998).
O’Neill, R. E., Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Sprague, J. R., Functional assessment and teacher collected data.
Storey, K., & Newton, J. S. (1997). Functional assess- Education and Treatment of Children, 21, 135–159.
ment and program development for problem behavior: Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
A practical handbook. Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole. R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009).
Pfadt, A., & Wheeler, D. J. (1995). Using statistical pro- Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental
cess control to make data-based clinical decisions. functional assessments of challenging behavior in
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 349–370. children with autism. Journal of Developmental and
Poppes, P., van der Putten, A. J. J., & Vlaskamp, C. (2010). Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514.
Frequency and severity of challenging behaviour in Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N.
people with profound intellectual and multiple dis- (1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control
abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
1269–1275. Analysis, 18, 343–351.
*Preciado, J. A. (2006). Using a function-based approach University of Utah, Utah State University, & Utah State
to decrease problem behavior and increase reading Department of Education. (1999). Functional
academic engagement for Latino English language Assessment and Intervention Program. Longmont:
learners. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University Sopris West.
of Oregon. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
Reed, H., Thomas, E., Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
(1997). The Student Guided Functional Assessment extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
Interview: An analysis student and teacher agreement. aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Behavioral Education, 7, 33–49. Analysis, 28, 561–576.
Experimental Functional Analysis
8
Timothy R. Vollmer, Henry S. Roane,
and Amanda B. Rone

There are some redundancies in the terminol-


Introduction ogy. “Experimental” is used to indicate that inde-
pendent variables are introduced and withdrawn
Terminology to evaluate the effects on behavior (a commonly
cited analogy is the allergy test, during which the
Experimental functional analysis, as it relates to
patient is intentionally exposed to hypothesized
the assessment of severe behavior disorders,
allergens). “Functional” is used to indicate that
refers to behavioral assessment procedures that
the goal is to identify changes in behavior as a
involve manipulation of variables hypothesized
function of the variables introduced; however,
to maintain problematic behavior (e.g., Iwata,
“functional” has also come to refer to the operant
Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994).
function (roughly akin to “purpose of” or “reason
For example, the assessment could involve inten-
for” behavior). To identify the operant function of
tionally providing attention as a consequence to
behavior means to identify the variables, such as
problematic behavior in one test condition to test
reinforcers, responsible for its occurrence.
whether such attention increases (i.e., reinforces)
“Analysis” is used in the sense of Baer, Wolf, and
the problem behavior. By testing for sensitivity to
Risley (1968) to indicate that the procedure shows
attention (and other consequences) as reinforce-
a reasonable demonstration of events responsible
ment, an experimental functional analysis has
for the occurrence or nonoccurrence of behavior.
utility distinct from other forms of behavioral
Thus, it is difficult to see how an analysis (in this
assessment insofar as the variables hypothesized
restricted sense of the term) could be nonexperi-
to maintain behavior are directly manipulated
mental or nonfunctional. Further, some people
rather than inferred.
are confused by the use of the term “experimen-
tal” because of the term’s association with
research studies. Although an experimental func-
tional analysis may, in fact, be a part of a research
T.R. Vollmer (*) • A.B. Rone study, it is also a standard clinical procedure for
Psychology Department, University of Florida, the assessment of severe behavior disorders and
Gainesville, FL 32611, USA is not experimental in the sense of “undergoing
e-mail: vollmera@ufl.edu;
preliminary testing.” This confusion may be prob-
arone@heartlandforchildren.org
lematic because, for example, insurance compa-
H.S. Roane
nies may be disinclined to pay for an assessment
State University of New York Upstate
Medical University, Syracuse, NY, USA if it is considered experimental rather than rou-
e-mail: roaneh@upstate.edu tine, and a university institutional review board

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 125


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_8,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
126 T.R. Vollmer et al.

may require further elaboration of procedures if engaged in differentially higher levels of SIB in
the methods are viewed as anything other than the social disapproval (attention) condition, two
commonly accepted practice (as may be implied engaged in differentially higher levels of SIB in
by the term experimental). Thus, although it prob- the academic demand (escape) condition, and
ably would suffice to say that the procedure to three engaged in differentially higher levels of
which we refer represents an “analysis” of behav- SIB in the alone (automatic reinforcement) con-
ior, we will use the term “functional analysis” dition. In total, six of the nine participants
hereafter both to maintain convention and to engaged in differentially higher levels of SIB in
avoid unnecessary redundancy and a possible one of the test conditions relative to the other test
misinterpretation of the term “experimental.” conditions. In addition, some participants engaged
in differentially high levels of SIB in all condi-
tions, an outcome that now usually leads to a con-
Historical Overview clusion that the behavior is maintained by
automatic reinforcement.
The functional analysis approach stems origi- Shortly after the original publication of Iwata
nally from behavioral assessments and treatments et al. (1982/1994), Carr and Durand published a
of self-injurious behavior (SIB). Carr (1977) series of studies using logic similar to that of
reviewed studies suggesting that SIB was some- Iwata et al. but with a different experimental
times maintained by positive reinforcement in the approach (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985; Durand &
form of attention or other tangible items (e.g., Carr, 1987). Whereas Iwata et al. manipulated
Lovaas & Simmons, 1969), negative reinforce- both antecedents to and consequences for the
ment in the form of escape from instructional occurrence of problem behavior, Carr and Durand
activity (e.g., Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, 1976), manipulated antecedents only. If the target behav-
or (what is now called) automatic reinforcement ior was hypothesized to be reinforced by atten-
in the form of sensory stimulation (e.g., tion (i.e., if problem behavior occurred during a
Baumeister & Forehand, 1973). Automatic rein- period of attention deprivation), the participants
forcement merely refers to behavior that is rein- were taught appropriate ways to gain attention
forced independent of social mediation (i.e., through functional communication training
produces its own source of reinforcement). (FCT). If the target behavior was hypothesized to
The literature review by Carr (1977), coupled be reinforced by escape (i.e., if problem behavior
with clinical exigencies, seems to have set the occurred following an instructional demand), the
occasion for Iwata et al. (1982/1994) to test the participants were taught appropriate ways to gain
social positive, social negative, and automatic assistance in completing instructional activities,
reinforcement hypotheses with individual partici- also via FCT. The experimenters did not manipu-
pants who were hospitalized for severe SIB. Nine late any consequences following the occurrence
participants were exposed to each of four condi- of problem behavior, resulting in a less convinc-
tions: social disapproval (to test for positive rein- ing demonstration of the operant function of the
forcement in the form of attention), academic problem behavior. For this reason, the procedures
demand (to test for negative reinforcement in the used by Carr and Durand (i.e., antecedent-only
form of escape), alone (to test for automatic rein- manipulations) are not commonly employed in
forcement), and toy play (to serve as a control for current assessments; however, these studies are
the test conditions). The format and logic behind of historical significance because they were
these conditions will be discussed in additional among the first to adopt the logic of linking
detail later. The four conditions were presented in assessment to treatment.
a multielement format, in which a separation in The true utility and versatility of the functional
data paths by condition indicates a reinforcement analysis approach became increasingly clear over
effect in that condition, and session duration was the past two decades, when literally hundreds of
15 min. Results showed that one participant treatment studies and methodological refinements
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 127

(e.g., Kahng, Iwata, DeLeon, & Worsdell, 1997) For example, the therapist might say, “I need to
were published. Treatment studies routinely do some work,” and then begin shuffling through
used functional analysis as a pretreatment screen- papers or a book while “ignoring” the participant.
ing method, demonstrating its utility for both The participant receives no attention from the
research and clinical assessment. Methodological therapist unless a target response occurs. If a tar-
refinements have included evaluating brief ass- get response occurs, the therapist delivers atten-
essments (e.g., Bloom, Iwata, Fritz, Roscoe, & tion, usually in the form of a reprimand, comfort
Carreau, 2011; Northup et al., 1991), modified statements, physical blocking, or some combina-
experimental designs (e.g., Iwata, Duncan, tion thereof, immediately following the target
Zarcone, Lerman, & Shore, 1994), and inclusion response. Therefore, in this context, the partici-
of idiosyncratic test conditions (e.g., Vollmer pant is deprived of attention until he/she engages
et al., 1998), among others. in a targeted problem behavior. Usually, the
specific form of attention delivered is designed to
mimic the type of attention the participant was
Common Test and Control Conditions observed to receive during natural interactions
(e.g., Piazza et al., 1999), and although the atten-
Functional analyses are usually conducted as a tion usually takes the form of a brief statement or
series of sessions, comprising experimental con- interaction, the duration of attention is sometimes
ditions. In some cases, the conditions are pre- designed to match the duration of reinforcement
sented in a planned order, but in other cases they in other conditions (e.g., Fisher, Piazza, &
are presented randomly or in alternating order Chiang, 1996).
(Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003). The logic of a
planned sequence is that one session can serve as
an establishing operation for the subsequent ses- Demand
sion in the series. For example, in an alone ses-
sion, the participant receives no attention, so The demand condition is designed to test for
attention should have high value in a subsequent socially mediated negative reinforcement in the
attention session. Most commonly, the functional form of escape (Iwata et al., 1982/1994). This
analyses are conducted using a multielement for- condition attempts to address the question, “Does
mat wherein conditions are alternated and ses- the participant engage in the behavior because it
sions usually last anywhere from 5 to 15 min. produces escape from or avoidance of non-
The most common conditions of a functional preferred tasks?” During this condition, a thera-
analysis are attention, demand, alone, tangible, pist presents some type of instructional demand
and play. Each will be discussed briefly below. to the participant. The form of the demand can be
academic, self-care, or any instruction that has
been observed to be potentially non-preferred to
Attention the participant; however, potentially non-pre-
ferred demands should be restricted to those
The attention condition is designed to test for commonly or necessarily presented to the partici-
socially mediated positive reinforcement in the pant in their natural environment (e.g., teeth
form of attention (Iwata et al., 1982/1994). This brushing, completing a math worksheet). This
condition attempts to address the question, “Does restriction is important to ensure that demands
the participant engage in the behavior because it are constrained to those that are socially valid
produces attention from others?”During this con- instructional activities, and not just demands that
dition, a therapist is in the room with the partici- the individual may find to be extremely aversive.
pant but directs his or her attention away from the The instructional demands are the establishing
participant. The absence of attention is the estab- operation for escape as reinforcement. The thera-
lishing operation for attention as reinforcement. pist presents instructions either continuously or
128 T.R. Vollmer et al.

on a set schedule (such as once every 30 s) and Typically, the participant is allowed access to
usually follows a three-step sequence involving a preferred items or activities, which are then
verbal instruction, a model or gestural prompt, removed when the session begins. Contingent on
and physical guidance to complete the instruc- occurrences of the target behavior, the items or
tion. The instructional sequence is always com- activities are returned to the participant for a
pleted unless a target response occurs, at which scheduled period of time, such as 20 or 30 s. The
time the therapist terminates the instructional tangible condition is usually used only when
sequence until the next scheduled demand or after there is evidence to suggest that a target behavior
a programmed break interval, such as 20 or 30 s. commonly produces such a consequence, other-
wise there is potential for false-positive outcomes
(Shirley, Iwata, & Kahng, 1999).
Alone

The alone condition is designed to test whether Control


behavior persists in the absence of social rein-
forcement (i.e., is automatically reinforced). This The control condition, described as “toy play” in
condition attempts to address the question, “Does the Iwata et al. (1982/1994) study, is designed to
the participant engage in the behavior if there are eliminate establishing operations and reinforce-
no social consequences for the behavior?” The ment contingencies used in the test conditions.
participant is placed in a room alone with no toys To control for the effects of positive reinforce-
or preferred activities and is ideally observed ment, the participant is provided with continuous
through a one-way window (Iwata et al., attention, attention delivered on a dense schedule
1982/1994). The general absence of stimulation (e.g., one statement delivered every 30 s), or
is considered to be an establishing operation for attention provided on a brief differential rein-
automatic reinforcement. In some school and forcement schedule (e.g., attention provided if
home settings, a one-way window arrangement is the participant has not engaged in a target behav-
not available, so experimenters have modified the ior for 5 s). In addition, the participant is provided
condition to be a “no consequence” condition. with ample stimulation in the form of preferred
During a no consequence condition, an observer tangible items. To control for the effects of
is present in the room with the participant, but the escape, the participant is not presented with any
target behavior produces no social consequences. demands. Characteristically, low rates of the
When behavior persists in an alone condition, target behavior are obtained in the control
there is strong evidence to conclude that the tar- condition.
get behavior is operant behavior maintained by
automatic reinforcement (for a more detailed dis-
cussion, see Vollmer, 1994). Sample Outcomes

Figure 8.1 is adapted from Vollmer, Iwata, Smith,


Tangible Zarcone, and Mazaleski (1993).In this study,
each data point represents a single session lasting
A tangible condition was not included in the orig- 15 min. Note that the behavior rates are elevated
inal Iwata et al. (1982/1994) study. The tangible in the attention condition relative to all other con-
condition is designed to test for socially mediated ditions, providing an example of SIB reinforced
positive reinforcement in the form of contingent by attention (positive reinforcement).
access to preferred items or activities. This con- Figure 8.2 is adapted from Vollmer, Marcus,
dition attempts to address the question, “Does the and Ringdahl (1995a). In this study, each data
participant engage in the behavior because it pro- point represents a single session lasting either 10
duces access to a preferred item or activity?” (upper panel) or 5 min (lower panel). In the upper
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 129

Fig. 8.1 An example of a functional analysis showing attention as reinforcement for SIB. Brenda was an adult woman
living in a residential facility. Adapted from Vollmer, Iwata, Smith, et al. (1993)

Fig. 8.2 Example of functional analyses showing escape as reinforcement for SIB. Kevin was an adolescent and Mark
was a preschooler. Adapted from Vollmer, Marcus, and Ringdahl (1995a)

panel, the participant was allowed brief breaks breaks from table work contingent on SIB. At the
from walking contingent on SIB. Note that the point in the assessment where table work started
behavior rates are elevated in the escape (demand) (denoted by an arrow), SIB occurred at differen-
condition relative to all other conditions, provid- tially higher rates during the escape condition,
ing one example of SIB reinforced by escape. In providing a second example of SIB reinforced by
the lower panel, the participant was allowed brief escape (negative reinforcement).
130 T.R. Vollmer et al.

Figure 8.3 is adapted from Ringdahl, Vollmer, conditions were conducted, and in the lower panel,
Marcus, and Roane (1997). In this study, each data a second topography of SIB (hand and body hit-
point represents the level of responding during a ting) occurred only during the no interaction con-
single 10-min session, and both assessments are ditions. Both of these outcomes provide examples
for the same child. In the upper panel, one topog- of SIB reinforced by automatic reinforcement.
raphy of SIB (hand banging) occurred in all condi- Figure 8.4 is adapted from Athens and Vollmer
tions and persisted when consecutive no interaction (2010). In this study, each data point represents

Fig. 8.3 Examples of functional analyses showing SIB the lower panel, hand and body hitting occurred exclu-
maintained by automatic reinforcement. Barry was a pre- sively in the no interaction condition. Both outcomes are
schooler. In the upper panel, hand banging occurred in all indicative of an automatic reinforcement effect. Adapted
conditions and persisted in the no interaction condition. In from Ringdahl et al. (1997)

Fig. 8.4 An example of a functional analysis showing an outcome where tangible items (toys) reinforced Corey’s dis-
ruptive behavior. Adapted from Athens and Vollmer (2010)
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 131

the level of responding in a single 10-min session. should not be viewed as a treatment for a particular
Note that the behavior rates are differentially form of behavior (e.g., hand biting); rather, it
higher in the tangible condition when compared should be viewed as a treatment for behavior
with the other conditions, providing an example maintained by a particular source of reinforce-
of disruptive behavior reinforced by the delivery ment (e.g., social positive reinforcement). A sec-
of tangible items. ond clinical premise is that behavior disorders are
best classified according to behavioral functions.
For example, one individual might engage in
The Utility of Functional Analysis property destruction maintained by negative rein-
forcement and another person might engage in
Functional Analysis as Clinical SIB maintained by negative reinforcement. The
Assessment treatment for those two individuals could be more
similar than a treatment for two individuals who
Dozens of the leading behavioral treatment insti- engage in exactly the same form of SIB (Vollmer
tutes and programs do currently, or have histori- & Smith, 1996), because an emphasis is placed on
cally, used functional analysis as an initial the function rather than the form of the behavior.
assessment for the purposes of treatment devel- The clinical goal of a functional analysis is to
opment. To list just a few examples, functional match the treatment to the assessment outcome.
analysis is or was a requisite assessment in the For example, if behavior is found to be main-
following behavior analytic programs: Kennedy tained by positive reinforcement in the form of
Krieger Institute/Johns Hopkins School of attention, attention can either be withheld (extinc-
Medicine, Marcus Autism Center/Children’s tion), provided contingent on some alternative
Healthcare of Atlanta, Monroe-Meyer Institute/ behavior (differential reinforcement), provided
University of Nebraska Medical Center, noncontingently, or some combination, to reduce
University of Iowa Children’s Hospital, Florida motivation to engage in problem behavior and to
Center for Self-Injury, Children’s Seashore break the contingency between problem behavior
House/University of Pennsylvania School of and attention (e.g., Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone,
Medicine, Family Behavior Analysis Clinic/ Smith, & Mazaleski, 1993). Similarly, if behavior
Upstate Medical University among many others. is maintained by negative reinforcement in the
The typical model is that once an individual is form of escape, escape can either be withheld
admitted to the program, they participate in a during extinction (e.g., Iwata et al., 1990), pro-
functional analysis prior to the development of a vided contingent on some alternative behavior
treatment program (Iwata, Pace et al., 1994). during differential reinforcement (e.g., Marcus &
Functional analysis is so commonly used by Vollmer, 1995), provided noncontingently (e.g.,
expert practitioners because it directly prescribes Vollmer, Marcus, & Ringdahl, 1995a), or some
treatment. A clinical premise of functional analy- combination.
sis is that behavior disorders are not best evalu- When behavior is maintained by automatic
ated solely on the basis of response topography. reinforcement, the treatment prognosis is not as
For example, two different clients might engage good as when behavior is maintained by social
in exactly the same form of behavior, but the reinforcement because the therapist often cannot
behavior might be maintained by negative rein- directly control the automatic reinforcer.
forcement for one person and maintained by posi- Nonetheless, when behavior is maintained by
tive reinforcement for the other. Therefore, even automatic reinforcement, treatment could involve
though these individuals may engage in topo- identifying substitutable reinforcers to provide
graphically identical behavior, the treatments for either contingently or noncontingently (e.g.,
each individual may differ significantly because Vollmer, Marcus, & LeBlanc, 1994). Further, the
of the functional, rather than topographical, treat- automatic reinforcement produced by the behav-
ment prescription. The treatment classification ior can be blocked, either by equipment or by
132 T.R. Vollmer et al.

physical means, as a form of extinction (e.g., 1995), breath holding (Kern, Mauk, Marder, &
Lerman & Iwata, 1996). Mace, 1995), pica (Mace & Knight, 1986),
If a functional analysis is not conducted, there schizophrenic vocalizations (Wilder, Masuda,
is a degree of guesswork involved in treatment O’Conner, & Baham, 2001), and coprophagia
development. For example, if the reinforcer(s) (Ing, Roane, & Veenstra, 2011) just to name of
maintaining problematic behavior are not few. Thus, the use of functional analysis in
identified, a therapist might prescribe an irrele- research may provide new information on previ-
vant treatment or, even worse, a “treatment” that ously misunderstood or little understood behav-
actually exacerbates the behavior. An example of ior disorders.
an irrelevant treatment would be ignoring behav- However, the systematic replication of func-
ior that is maintained by automatic reinforce- tional analysis methods to identify the function
ment. Even if the prescribed treatment is of other diverse forms of behavior should pro-
implemented with complete integrity, the behav- ceed with caution. Recall that the functional anal-
ior will not extinguish because the reinforcer that ysis of SIB was based on decades of prior research
is maintaining the behavior has not been with- pointing to attention, escape, and automatic rein-
held. Yet, professional therapists might be forcement as specific sources of reinforcement
inclined to ignore the behavior based on their for the disorder (Carr, 1977). However, these
experience with extinction being effective with specific sources of reinforcement may need to be
socially reinforced behavior. An example of a refined in some cases (such as when peer rather
treatment that might exacerbate problematic than adult attention might function as reinforce-
behavior could be timeout for behavior that is ment; e.g., Northup et al., 1995) or when phylo-
maintained by escape from instructional demands. genic variables possibly play a role. For example,
In this situation, it is likely that the behavior it is now widely known that aggression serves
would get worse because timeout might actually operant functions (e.g., Marcus, Vollmer,
serve as negative reinforcement (Iwata, Pace, Swanson, Roane, & Ringdahl, 2001); however,
Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994). Yet, profes- there is evidence that animals (presumably
sional therapists might be inclined to implement including humans) will behave aggressively in
timeout based on their experience with timeout response to extreme aversive stimulation (e.g.,
being an effective punishment procedure. Ulrich & Azrin, 1962) and there is clear evidence
of a role of imitation and modeling in aggression
(e.g., Bandura & Waiters 1963). Thus, the expan-
Functional Analysis as a Research sion of the functional analysis approach to topog-
Method raphies other than SIB should progress as
cautiously and systematically as possible, taking
Hundreds of published studies have demonstrated into account a range of potential influences.
the utility of functional analysis as a research
method. Vollmer and Smith (1996) discussed Translational Research
three distinct contributions of the approach as a Prior to the development of functional analysis
research method, and these contributions are assessment methodology, there was a degree of
summarized below. guesswork involved in identifying putative rein-
forcers maintaining problematic behavior. With
Understanding Diverse Response the advent of functional analysis procedures,
Topographies reinforcers can be identified explicitly. By so
Since the publication of Iwata et al. (1982/1994), doing, principles of reinforcement that have been
dozens of studies have systematically replicated widely studied in basic research can be usefully
the approach to identify the operant function of applied to the human situation. Although it is
seemingly inexplicable problematic behavior, beyond the scope of this chapter to detail the
including classroom disruption (Northup et al., range of translational research made possible by
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 133

functional analysis, we will highlight one area to for individuals whose behavioral functions are
demonstrate the role of functional analysis in appropriate for the research question at hand. Part
translational research: the matching law. one of behavioral studies commonly involves
The matching law is a widely accepted prin- showing via functional analysis that behavior is
ciple of reinforcement that posits proportional reinforced by a particular consequence. Part two
behavior rates on two or more alternatives will of the study (if a treatment study) then goes on to
match proportional reinforcement rates on those evaluate the efficacy of one or more treatments for
alternatives. For example, if you had two buttons behavior given a particular operant function.
to press and one of the buttons produced money A prototype for this approach was published by
twice as often as the other (on average), after a Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, and Cataldo
period of time you will tend to press that button (1990). First, Iwata et al. showed that the SIB of
almost twice as often as the other button. We seven individuals was reinforced by escape from
have found that the same was true of children instructional activity. Next, the experimenters
allocating problematic and appropriate behavior implemented escape extinction (i.e., eliminated
(Borrero & Vollmer 2002; Borrero et al., 2010). escape as a consequence by guiding the partici-
In one study (Borrero & Vollmer, 2002), we pants through the instructed task even when SIB
first observed children interacting with their par- occurred) and showed that the SIB was reduced in
ents and teachers in natural settings such as the six of seven cases. It was important to first show
home or classroom. Observers scored whether that the behavior in question was in fact main-
certain events, such as attention, access to tangi- tained by escape, otherwise the subsequent extinc-
bles, or escape from instructions, tended to fol- tion procedure would not have been a logical next
low either problematic behavior or appropriate step. For example, if the behavior had been rein-
behavior. Next, we conducted a functional analy- forced by attention or physical contact, escape
sis to identify specific sources of reinforcement extinction in the form of guided compliance might
for the problematic behavior. Finally, we revis- have actually strengthened the behavior.
ited our observational data and scored the fre-
quency with which reinforcers identified during
the functional analysis following appropriate ver- Preassessment Considerations
sus problematic behavior during the naturalistic
observations. We found that if the problematic Informed Consent
behavior produced greater frequencies of rein-
forcement, the participants tended to engage in The Iwata et al. (1982/1994) paper provided
proportionally more problematic behavior. This detailed information on informed consent and
finding has clinical value insofar as the goal of medical protection for the participants who par-
parent or teacher training then becomes one of ticipated. In recent years, such detail is often
reversing the reinforcement contingencies to omitted from functional analysis studies; how-
favor appropriate behavior. In a subsequent study ever, we recommend that informed consent
(Borrero et al., 2010), we found that problematic should always be obtained before beginning any
and appropriate behavior rates essentially functional analysis involving dangerous behav-
reversed as contingencies of reinforcement were ior. One reason for this suggestion is that to
reversed. In both of these matching law studies, it nonprofessionals (and in some cases other pro-
would have been impossible to know for certain fessionals), a functional analysis may be counter-
that particular events functioned as reinforcers if intuitive unless it is carefully explained. For
we had not conducted a functional analysis. example, it may not be immediately clear why it
is important to intentionally reinforce dangerous
Participant Screening behavior before commencing with treatment. An
Many behavioral treatment studies are preceded additional reason for obtaining informed consent
by a pretreatment functional analysis to screen is that the parents or guardians should be aware
134 T.R. Vollmer et al.

of the potential risks and should have knowledge Iwata, Reid, & Davis, 1982; Mazaleski, Iwata,
of the assessment procedures. For example, the Rodgers, Vollmer, & Zarcone, 1994). In addition,
alone condition of a functional analysis involves physical contact in the form of blocking can
having an individual alone in a room to test for potentially produce an extinction effect (Lerman
problem behavior maintained by automatic rein- & Iwata, 1996), a punishment effect (Smith,
forcement, and the rationale for the condition Russo, & Le, 1999), or even a reinforcement
may not be readily apparent to an untrained effect (Vollmer, Iwata, Smith, & Rodgers, 1992).
individual. Access to self-restraining materials can also
influence the outcome of a functional analysis
(Smith, Iwata, Vollmer, & Pace, 1992; Smith,
Medical Monitoring and Protection Lerman, & Iwata, 1996). In short, although pro-
tection in the form of equipment or blocking may
In order for a functional analysis to identify the be medically necessary, the potential confounds
variables maintaining problem behavior, it is nec- should be recognized.
essary for problem behavior to occur during one During functional analyses of aggression, it is
or more of the assessment conditions. With SIB, common for therapists to wear protective equip-
in particular, such situations may place the par- ment, including shin guards, long sleeves, arm
ticipant at a risk for harm, and it is important to pads, etc. (Marcus et al., 2001). We are aware of
obtain a medical opinion and to collaborate with no studies to date that have shown protective
medical personnel to determine the extent to equipment to alter the outcome of functional anal-
which the SIB is allowable. For some individuals, yses of aggression. Thus, it is considered standard
a single blow to the head or eyes may be deemed best practice to ensure protection of the therapist
unacceptable, in which case they would likely be conducting the assessment, and we recommend
excluded from a functional analysis unless pro- the use of protective equipment when necessary.
tective equipment is used; however, for other
individuals, what might appear to be severe and
dangerous may actually be judged by medical Session Duration
personnel to be relatively harmless in the short
term. For example, face slapping may be loud In the Iwata et al. (1982/1994) study, sessions were
and cause redness, but observing this behavior 15 min in duration; however, Wallace and Iwata
for relatively short periods of time, such as the (1999) demonstrated that assessment outcomes for
5–15 min typical of a functional analysis session, 10-min sessions were nearly identical to session
is often deemed by medical personnel as not outcomes for 15-min sessions (the 10-min “ses-
overly dangerous, especially when weighed sions” were actually the first 10 min of the 15-min
against the possibility of successful treatment for sessions). Interestingly, even 5-min sessions were
the behavior. When conducting a functional anal- shown to yield very few discrepancies when com-
ysis of SIB, it is important to weigh the “pros” of pared with the results of the 15-min sessions. Thus,
the assessment (i.e., identifying a treatment that most current applications of functional analyses
will ultimately reduce the occurrence of the involve relatively brief, usually 10-min, sessions.
behavior) to the “cons” (i.e., permitting the The duration of the session should be determined
repeated occurrence of SIB). To date, there have prior to beginning the analysis and should remain
been few attempts to standardize protective pro- constant unless a participant reaches some prede-
cedures for functional analyses (see Weeden, termined termination criterion.
Mahoney, & Poling, 2010 for a discussion).
When protective equipment or response block-
ing is required, it is important to recognize the Response Topography
potential confounding influence of those vari-
ables. For example, protective equipment can Many individuals with severe behavior disorders
potentially extinguish or punish SIB (e.g., Dorsey, display multiple topographies of problematic
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 135

behavior, and it is best to preselect the specific


form or forms of behavior that will produce the
test consequences during the functional analysis.
Researchers have shown that reinforcing one
topography of behavior can greatly alter the prob-
ability of another topography occurring (e.g.,
Lalli, Mace, Wohn, & Livezy, 1995; Richman,
Wacker, Asmus, Casey, & Andelman, 1999);
thus, reinforcing too many topographies of behav-
ior might in fact “disguise” the function of the
most problematic form of behavior. If an indi-
vidual engages in several topographies of serious
problem behavior, multiple (i.e., separate) func-
tional analyses can be conducted to help ensure
the accurate identification of the variables main-
taining each specific problem behavior. For
example, if an individual engages in SIB and
aggression, a separate functional analysis should
be conducted for each behavior rather than com-
bining the topographies.

Experimental Design
Fig. 8.5 A model for progressing from brief to extended
Most commonly, a multielement design is used in functional analyses. Figure adapted from Vollmer, Marcus,
the context of a functional analysis to demon- Ringdahl, et al. (1995)
strate experimental control. Some researchers
have used the logic of a multielement design but response rates are plotted to more rapidly detect
in a much briefer format, either by using brief response patterns. If the within-session response
reversals (e.g., Northup et al., 1991) or by using rates are differentiated by condition, the partici-
within-session data analyses (e.g., Vollmer, Iwata, pant then moves on to treatment. If they are not
Zarcone, et al., 1993). When the multielement differentiated by condition, the assessment
approach yields undifferentiated outcomes, some becomes a standard multielement assessment as
researchers have used either a sequential test- described by Iwata et al. (1982/1994). If the mul-
control format in which a single test condition is tielement assessment is differentiated, the partici-
juxtaposed against the control condition (e.g., pant then moves on to treatment; however, if the
Iwata, Duncan, et al., 1994) or a reversal design assessment is undifferentiated, the participant is
to isolate the test conditions and minimize multiple- then exposed to several consecutive alone or no
condition interference (e.g., Vollmer, Iwata, consequence sessions to determine whether the
Duncan, & Lerman, 1993). behavior will persist in the absence of social rein-
Previously we have described a model for pro- forcement. If the behavior does maintain in the
gressing from brief assessments to extended alone or no interaction condition, the assessment
experimental analyses to clarify outcomes in the is concluded and behavior is determined to be
most efficient manner possible without sacrificing maintained by automatic reinforcement; how-
clear outcomes (Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, & ever, if the behavior extinguishes in the alone or
Roane, 1995b). In that model, the assessment no interaction condition, the social reinforcement
begins with a brief evaluation in which the condi- test conditions (attention, demand, and tangible)
tions are rapidly alternated in a multielement for- are presented in isolation in a reversal design
mat, but the (within session) minute-by-minute format. The progression is summarized in Fig. 8.5,
136 T.R. Vollmer et al.

which is adapted from the Vollmer, Marcus, sure to each test condition fewer than a dozen or
Ringdahl, et al. (1995) paper. so times, residual target behavior could occur in
some or all conditions even though the maintain-
ing reinforcer is not being presented. It is also
Limitations and Future Directions possible that unidentified sources of automatic
reinforcement contribute to the maintenance of
Inconclusive Outcomes behavior (Vollmer, 1994).

Iwata, Pace et al. (1994) reported that inconclu-


sive outcomes were obtained in 4.6% of their Difficult Treatment Implementation
functional analyses of SIB. Although not a high
percentage, it can be expected that almost 1 in 20 At times the outcome of a functional analysis
assessments will not yield useful outcomes. For prescribes a behavioral treatment that is difficult,
these cases, it is possible that extraneous or if not impossible, to implement successfully. For
uncontrolled sources of reinforcement influence example, if a functional analysis shows that
the behavior. It may also be possible that the vari- severe aggression displayed by an adult, very
ables maintaining the behavior are highly specific. strong man is reinforced by escape from instruc-
For example, it is possible that behavior main- tional activity, it will likely be difficult to imple-
tained by social positive reinforcement in the ment an extinction component while maintaining
form of attention is only reinforced by the atten- the safety of both the individual and the therapist.
tion of a particular person (and not the therapist), Another example might involve severe SIB that is
making it unlikely that the problem behavior will reinforced by physical contact in the form of
maintain during the assessment. Epidemiological blocking. If the SIB is sufficiently dangerous, it is
data have not been reported on the function for difficult to develop a treatment involving extinc-
behavior disorders other than SIB, such as aggres- tion (i.e., the removal of blocking) because the
sion; however, it stands to reason that assess- person might get severely hurt if blocking proce-
ments of other topographies of problem behavior dures were to be removed.
are also sometimes inconclusive. Future research Although such situations do arise, we contend
should be aimed at clarifying inconclusive out- that there is nothing about the functional analysis
comes, possibly taking into account other factors per se that creates the problem of difficult treat-
such as elicitation. For example, basic research ment implementation. Irrespective of the method
has shown that humans will bite down reflexively used to prescribe or develop treatment, the situa-
in response to pain (Hutchinson, 1977), so some tion would be difficult given the types of exam-
self-biting might occur in response to undetected ples provided above. In fact, it is possible that by
aversive stimulation. Also, basic research has shedding light on the variables maintaining prob-
shown that organisms will behave aggressively in lem behavior, a functional analysis provides
response to reinforcer loss or aversive stimula- information for treatment development that could
tion (Ulrich & Azrin, 1962); thus, it is possible circumvent some of the problems associated with
that some human aggression is elicited in that an extinction component. For example, Lalli et al.
fashion. (1999) showed that the use of positive reinforce-
Apart from elicitation, it is unknown whether ment for compliance successfully competed with
a history of reinforcement, even when the rein- escape for severe aggression and SIB, such that
forcement is no longer in place, might influence escape extinction was unnecessary. Similarly,
the current occurrence of some forms of aberrant Athens and Vollmer (2010) manipulated several
behavior, in a manner similar to when an organ- reinforcement parameters such that, even when
ism’s behavior does not quickly extinguish problematic behavior was still being reinforced,
despite no longer producing reinforcement. the “pay off” for engaging in appropriate behav-
Because most functional analyses involve expo- ior was greater along the dimensions of reinforcer
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 137

immediacy, duration, and quality. Accordingly, 95% accuracy. Although it is true that in some
response allocation shifted toward appropriate cases sophisticated decision making is required
behavior despite the absence of an extinction when analyzing data in a functional analysis, the
component. same is true for virtually any type of psychology
assessment or medical assessment.
Does a functional analysis require high num-
Time and Resource Constraints bers of staff? Usually a functional analysis
requires one therapist and one observer (with a
Some have argued that a functional analysis is second observer for about 20% of the sessions).
time consuming and cumbersome, requiring too Some variations of functional analysis have used
much expertise and personnel (e.g., Sturmey, a parent or teachers as the therapist or observer
1995). In one sense, this criticism is most unfor- (e.g., Cooper, Wacker, Sasso, Reimers, & Donn,
tunate because individuals with or without dis- 1990). Also, because only a few general classes
abilities spend incredible resources on other of reinforcement maintain a high percentage of
life-threatening illnesses such as cancer or heart behavior disorders, it is possible that future
disease. It is unlikely that anyone would ever research could involve functional analyses during
question the resource requirements to treat such ongoing activities. For example, a parent or
illness, and we would argue that when SIB or teacher could be taught the rule: Do not change
aggression is life threatening, resource limita- what you are doing if the problem behavior
tions should also not govern decision making. occurs. Given that reinforcement requires stimu-
When the target behavior is not life threatening, lus change, at a minimum, social sources of rein-
but greatly reduces the quality of life for the indi- forcement could be ruled in or out with essentially
vidual or the individual’s family, perhaps the no additional personnel costs simply by having a
issue is a bit more complicated, and the following parent or teacher follow the rule.
bear consideration. Are there alternatives available? If some briefer
Is a functional analysis time consuming? If an form of assessment was shown to be as reliable
assessment requires (for example) six exposures and as convincing as a functional analysis, most
to each condition, and there are four conditions, a would agree that a functional analysis would not
total of 24 sessions would be required to com- always be required. Indirect forms of assessment
plete the assessment. Suppose there is equal time are reviewed elsewhere in this volume, but in
in set up, breaks between sessions, and so on, the short, although moving in a favorable direction,
total (and we would consider this to be liberal no indirect assessment methods to date have been
time consumption) is 480 min or 8 h of assess- conclusively shown to identify the operant func-
ment time. If an effective treatment can be tion of behavior with consistency and reliability.
identified in 8 h, this time allocation should be
contrasted with the time allocation spent on inad-
vertently reinforcing the behavior, repairing/ Covert Behavior
replacing destroyed items, the cost of purchasing
new items, treating injuries, and so on. Further, Some serious behavior problems displayed by
some research has shown that clear functional individuals with intellectual disabilities occur
analyses can take as little as 90 total minutes either especially or exclusively when they are
(e.g., Northup et al., 1991; Vollmer, Marcus, alone (or when they believe that they are alone;
Ringdahl & Roane, 1995b). Chapman, Fisher, Piazza, & Kurtz, 1993). The
Is a functional analysis cumbersome and does covert nature of the behavior can make an assess-
it require extensive expertise? Iwata et al. (2000) ment difficult to complete in some settings; how-
showed that undergraduate psychology students, ever, a functional analysis can be accomplished
given approximately 2 h of training, implemented when a true alone condition can be implemented
functional analysis procedures with greater than (i.e., using a one-way window). An example can
138 T.R. Vollmer et al.

be seen in Cowdery, Iwata, and Pace (1990), in analyst might need to restrict the assessment to
which the researchers showed that the severe SIB an interview format or records review to develop
(skin picking and scratching) displayed by a hypotheses about the function of the behavior.
young boy occurred exclusively when he was Another approach that has emerged in recent
alone. They observed the behavior through a one- research involves the functional analysis of “pre-
way window and, during treatment, provided cursor” behavior, or behavior that reliably pre-
feedback based on his wounds (although they had dicts the onset of more dangerous behavior
actually conducted direct behavioral observa- (Borrero & Borrero, 2008; Herscovitch, Roscoe,
tions). A differential reinforcement schedule was Libby, Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009; Najdowski,
successfully implemented to treat the behavior. Wallace, Ellsworth, MacAleese, & Cleveland,
2008; Smith & Churchill, 2002). The notion
behind assessing precursor behavior is that if an
Low Frequency Behavior earlier response form is reinforced, a later
response in a hierarchy need not occur
A related problem occurs when the target behav-
ior occurs at such a low frequency that it is rarely
observed. However, it is possible that the behav- Conclusions
ior occurs rarely because people in the environ-
ment make sure that the establishing operation is The functional analysis approach was based on
avoided. For example, staff might learn to avoid decades of research showing that severe behavior
asking a resident to brush her teeth because oral disorders are sensitive to operant contingencies of
hygiene sessions produce high rates of problem- reinforcement. The assessment method developed
atic behavior (e.g., Shore, Iwata, Vollmer, Lerman, by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) has come to be known
& Zarcone, 1995). If so, merely conducting a as an “experimental functional analysis.” We have
functional analysis may increase the probability argued that the term “functional” analysis is a
of observing the behavior in context because the sufficient descriptor and is in keeping with cur-
establishing operation is intentionally presented. rent usage. The functional analysis approach is
Kahng, Abt, and Schonbachler (2001) reported ideally suited to identify cause and effect rela-
another interesting approach. Because aggression tions between environmental events and behavior.
was not observed during typical 10-min func- As a result, this type of assessment directly pre-
tional analysis sessions, they increased the obser- scribes treatments and serves as a useful research
vation period and functional analysis test method. Although several potential limitations of
contingencies to a 7-h period wherein one test the approach have been cited, recent research has
condition was conducted per day. By so doing, addressed many of them. We conclude that the
the experimenters were able to identify the oper- functional analysis approach does as should
ant function of aggression insofar as it clearly remain as a standard for behavioral assessment.
occurred most frequently during the protracted
attention condition. Thus, although low-rate
behavior is somewhat problematic for a func- References
tional analysis approach, the procedures can be
adapted to accommodate the difficulty. Athens, E., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An evaluation of dif-
ferential reinforcement of alternative behavior without
extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43,
569–589.
High-Intensity Behavior Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97.
At times, the target behavior is so intense and
Bandura, A., & Waiters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and
dangerous that not even a single response can be personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
allowed to occur. In these cases, the behavior Winston.
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 139

Baumeister, A., & Forehand, R. (1973). Stereotyped acts. Herscovitch, B., Roscoe, E. M., Libby, M. E., Bourret, J.
In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in C., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). A procedure for identify-
mental retardation (Vol. 6, pp. 55–96). New York: ing precursors to problem behavior. Journal of Applied
Academic. Behavior Analysis, 42, 697–702.
Bloom, S. E., Iwata, B. A., Fritz, J. N., Roscoe, E. M., & Hutchinson, R. R. (1977). By-products of aversive con-
Carreau, A. B. (2011). Classroom application of a trol. In W. K. Honig & L. L. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook
trial-based functional analysis. Journal of Applied of operant behavior (pp. 415–431). Englewood Cliffs:
Behavior Analysis, 44, 19–31. Prentice-Hall.
Borrero, C. S. W., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive Ing, A. D., Roane, H. S., & Veenstra, R. A. (2011).
and experimental analyses of potential precursors to Functional analysis and treatment of coprophagia.
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 151–155.
Analysis, 41, 83–96. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
Borrero, J. C., & Vollmer, T. R. (2002). An application of Richman, G. S. (1994). Journal of Applied Behavior
the matching law to severe problem behavior. Journal Analysis, 27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 13–27. Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20,
Borrero, C. S. W., Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Bourret, 1982).
J. C., Sloman, K. N., Samaha, A. L., et al. (2010). Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman, D.
Concurrent reinforcement schedules for problem C., & Shore, B. A. (1994). A sequential, test-control
behavior and appropriate behavior: Experimental methodology for conducting functional analyses of
applications of the matching law. Journal of the self-injurious behavior. Behavior Modification, 18,
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 455–469. 289–306.
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious behav- Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
ior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An analy-
Bulletin, 84, 800–816. sis of procedural form and function. Journal of Applied
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144.
problems through functional communication training. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994). The func-
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976). tions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-
Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psy- epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
chotic child. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Analysis, 27, 215–240.
4, 139–153. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E.,
Chapman, S., Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., & Kurtz, P. F. & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental analysis and
(1993). Functional assessment and treatment of life- extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of
threatening drug ingestion in a dually diagnosed youth. Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 11–27.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 255–256. Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S.,
Cooper, L., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Reimers, T., & Donn, Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill acquisi-
L. (1990). Using parents as therapists to evaluate tion in the implementation of functional analysis
appropriate behavior of their children: Application to methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
a tertiary diagnostic clinic. Journal of Applied Behavior 33, 181–194.
Analysis, 23, 285–296. Kahng, S., Abt, K., & Schonbachler, H. (2001). Assessment
Cowdery, G. E., Iwata, B. A., & Pace, G. M. (1990). and treatment of low-rate high-intensity problem
Effects and side effects of DRO as treatment for self- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior 225–228.
Analysis, 23, 497–506. Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Worsdell, A. S.
Dorsey, M. F., Iwata, B. A., Reid, D. H., & Davis, P. A. (1997). Evaluation of the “control over reinforcement”
(1982). Protective equipment: Continuous and contin- component in functional communication training.
gent application in the treatment of self-injurious Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 267–277.
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, Kern, L., Mauk, J. E., Marder, T. J., & Mace, E. C. (1995).
217–230. Functional analysis and intervention for breath-
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on holding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
“self-stimulatory” behavior: Analysis and treatment 339–340.
application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livezy, K. (1995).
119–132. Identification and modification of a response-class
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., & Chiang, C. L. (1996). hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
Effects of equal and unequal reinforcer duration dur- 551–559.
ing functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C.,
Analysis, 29, 117–120. Borrero, J., Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). between positive and negative reinforcement in the
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–295.
140 T.R. Vollmer et al.

Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology for Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification of
distinguishing between extinction and punishment environmental determinants of behavior disorders
effects associated with response blocking. Journal of through functional analysis of precursor behaviors.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 231–234. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–136.
Lovaas, O. I., & Simmons, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation of Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Pace, G. M.
self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of (1992). On the relationship between self-injurious
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 143–157. behavior and self-restraint. Journal of Applied
Mace, F. C., & Knight, D. (1986). Functional analysis and Behavior Analysis, 25, 433–445.
treatment of severe pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Smith, R. G., Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Self-
Analysis, 19, 411–416. restraint as positive reinforcement for self-injurious
Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R. (1995). Effects of differ- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,
ential negative reinforcement on disruption and com- 99–102.
pliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, Smith, R. G., Russo, L., & Le, D. D. (1999). Distinguishing
229–230. between extinction and punishment effects of response
Marcus, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Swanson, V., Roane, H. R., blocking: A replication. Journal of Applied Behavior
& Ringdahl, J. E. (2001). An experimental analysis of Analysis, 32, 367–370.
aggression. Behavior Modification, 25, 189–213. Sturmey, P. (1995). Analog baselines: A critical review of
Mazaleski, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Rodgers, T. A., Vollmer, T. the methodology. Research in Developmental
R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1994). Protective equipment as Disabilities, 16, 269–284.
treatment for stereotypic hand mouthing: Sensory Ulrich, R. E., & Azrin, N. H. (1962). Reflexive fighting in
extinction or punishment effects? Journal of Applied response to aversive stimulation. Journal of the
Behavior Analysis, 27, 345–355. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 511–520.
Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Ellsworth, C. L., Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforce-
MacAleese, A. N., & Cleveland, J. M. (2008). ment: Implications for behavioral research in develop-
Functional analyses and treatment of precursor behav- mental disabilities. Research in Developmental
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 97–105. Disabilities, 15, 187–207.
Northup, J., Broussard, C., Jones, K., George, T., Vollmer, Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., & Lerman, D.
T. R., & Herring, M. (1995). The differential effects of C. (1993). Extensions of multi element functional
teacher and peer attention on the disruptive classroom analyses using reversal-type designs. Journal of
behavior of three children with a diagnosis of attention Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 311–325.
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Applied Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., & Rodgers, T.
Behavior Analysis, 28, 227–228. A. (1992). Reduction of multiple aberrant behaviors
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, and concurrent development of self-care skills with
K., Cook, J., et al. (1991). A brief functional analysis differential reinforcement. Research in Developmental
of aggressive and alternative behavior in an outclinic Disabilities, 13, 287–299.
setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., Zarcone, J. R.,
509–522. & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in the
Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Contrucci, S. A., Delia, M. treatment of attention-maintained self-injurious behav-
D., Adelinis, J. D., & Goh, H. (1999). An evaluation of ior: Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) and differen-
the properties of attention as reinforcement for destruc- tial reinforcement of other behavior (DRO). Journal of
tive and appropriate behavior. Journal of Applied Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21.
Behavior Analysis, 32, 437–449. Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. S.,
Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., Asmus, J. M., Casey, S. & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). Within-session patterns of
D., & Andelman, M. (1999). Further analysis of prob- self-injury as indicators of behavioral function.
lem behavior in response class hierarchies. Journal of Research in Developmental Disabilities, 14, 479–492.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 269–283. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994).
Ringdahl, J. E., Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Roane, Treatment of self-injury and handmouthing following
H. S. (1997). An analog evaluation of environmental inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of Applied
enrichment as treatment for aberrant behavior. Journal Behavior Analysis, 27, 331–344.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 203–216. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. (1995a).
Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. (1999). False- Noncontingent escape as treatment for self-injury
positive maintenance of self-injurious behavior by maintained by negative reinforcement. Journal of
access to tangible reinforcers. Journal of Applied Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15–26.
Behavior Analysis, 32, 201–204. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
Shore, B. A., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Lerman, D. C., H. S. (1995b). Progressing from brief assessments to
& Zarcone, J. R. (1995). Pyramidal staff training in the extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
extension of treatment for severe behavior disorders. aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 323–332. Analysis, 28, 561–576.
8 Experimental Functional Analysis 141

Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Lalli, J. S., VanCamp, C. M., the descriptions of participant protections? Research
Sierp, B. J., Wright, C. S., et al. (1998). Fixed-time in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 299–303.
schedules attenuate extinction-induced phenomena in Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session
the treatment of severe aberrant behavior. Journal of duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 529–542. Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175–183.
Vollmer, T. R., & Smith, R. G. (1996). Some current Wilder, D. A., Masuda, A., O’Conner, C., & Baham, M.
themes in functional analysis research. Research in (2001). Brief functional analysis and treatment of
Developmental Disabilities, 17, 229–249. aberrant vocalizations in an adult with schizophre-
Weeden, M., Mahoney, A., & Poling, A. (2010). Self- nia. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
injurious behavior and functional analysis: Where are 65–68.
In Vivo Assessment: Issues
of Real-Time Data 9
Max Horovitz and Johnny L. Matson

One of the fundamental elements of functional Therefore, in vivo data collection has its primary
assessment for challenging behaviors is the col- advantage in its ability to be collected in the natu-
lection of data. Data are used to help determine ral environment in which the behavior typically
the nature of the problem, create a case formula- occurs. It does not require experimental manipu-
tion, carry out the functional assessment, and lation of the environment, which can create an
monitor the progress of interventions (Hartmann, artificial setting and reduce the external validity
Barrios, & Wood, 2004). All decisions made dur- of the data (Martens, DiGennaro, Reed, Szczech,
ing the course of a functional assessment are & Rosenthal, 2008).
based off of data. Most data collected during this In vivo data collection also allows for greater
process are collected in vivo. That is to say, most specificity of data, as definitions of challenging
data are collected through real-time observations behavior can be modified to fit each individual’s
of individuals in their natural environment. unique pattern of behavior (Matson & Nebel-
There are a number of advantages to in vivo Schwalm, 2007). This makes in vivo data collec-
data collection, primarily that observations are tion procedures very flexible, as they can be
carried out in the same setting in which the behavior adapted to target a variety of behaviors in a vari-
naturally occurs (Gardner, 2000). Take, for exam- ety of settings (Hartmann et al., 2004; Martens
ple, an individual who engages in self-injurious et al., 2008). Due to this specificity and flexibility,
behavior when he is hungry. In vivo assessment real-time data are more sensitive to treatment
allows for data to be collected in this individual’s effects than data collected via scaling methods
home, where he or she typically engages in the (Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007).
behavior. Were the individual brought into a Another advantage is that observers are able to
clinic for observation, the properties of the behav- directly see interactions between an individual
ior, such as frequency, intensity, and duration, and his or her environment (Gardner, 2000; Iwata,
may differ due to the novel environment. Vollmer, & Zarcone, 1990). Indirect assess-
ments from outside informants, such as parent
reports, may be susceptible to personal biases,
such as expectations, attributions, and mood
M. Horovitz (*) (Eddy, Dishion, & Stoolmiller, 1998; Fergusson,
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Lynskey, & Horwood, 1993). In contrast, real-
e-mail: maxh13@gmail.com
time data collection allows for direct observa-
J.L. Matson
tions, thereby bypassing the need for an informant.
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA In vivo data collection, in principle, allows for
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com objective evaluation of the effects of treatment

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 143


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_9,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
144 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

(Iwata et al., 1990; Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). those who will be using it for observation. Finally,
However, as we will see later in this chapter, that if the definition is found to be appropriate and
is not always the case. clear, it should then be field tested by two observ-
While in vivo data collection has a number of ers who only have the definition. If high agree-
advantages and clear utility, there are a number of ment is found, the definition is ready to be used;
problems that must be considered. This chapter if not, additional revisions may be required.
discusses a variety of problems associated with Finally, it is necessary that the criteria used to
the collection and use of real-time data that must operationally define the behavior easily distin-
be addressed. These problems are broken into five guish the target behavior from similar behaviors.
general categories: Defining the behavior, collect- For example, if the target behavior is hitting, the
ing the data, reliability, validity, and the interpre- criteria must clearly distinguish this from other
tation and use of the data. Problems common to similar behaviors, such as pushing (Bijou et al.,
each of these areas are discussed in depth. 1968). A lack of clarity in definitions will often
lead to a decrease in reliability, as will be dis-
cussed later (Bijou et al., 1968).
Defining the Behavior

The first step in data collection is to determine Collecting the Data


and define on what behavior data will be col-
lected. In order for this to be done, an accurate, After defining the target behavior, the next step is
operational definition must be established for the to determine how data will be collected. There are
behavior. For the purposes of in vivo data collec- many ways of classifying data collection tech-
tion, the behavior must be defined in clearly niques. Two primary categories of in vivo data
observable terms (Bijou, Peterson, & Ault, 1968). collection procedures will be discussed in this
Hawkins and Dobes (1977) suggest three charac- chapter: event recording and time sampling. There
teristics of a well-formed operational definition: are a number of methods of collecting data within
(1) the definition should include objective terms each of these categories, each of which will be
and refer only to observable characteristics of the discussed along with associated problems.
behavior; (2) the definition should be unambigu-
ous and clear to experienced observers; and (3)
the definition should be complete, defining what Event Recording
should be included and what should be excluded,
thereby reducing inference on the part of the Event recording involves counting the number of
observer. Additionally, they suggest that the times a specific behavior occurs during an inter-
definition be explicitly stated to data collectors, val (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). This is most
as implicit definitions are more prone to error. appropriate for collecting data on discrete behav-
These guidelines help prevent multiple observers iors that have clear beginning and end points
from using varying definitions and allow for rep- (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). The most basic
lication of data collection. type of event recording is to simply write down
Barrios (1993) provides a four-step process what behavior is occurring during certain periods
for creating such an operational definition. The of times (Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). This method
first step is to research how the behavior has pre- involves writing a descriptive account of every-
viously been operationalized, as it may be possi- thing relevant that is occurring during an obser-
ble to adopt a similar definition. The second step vation. Although this method can provide a very
is to construct a definition appropriate for the cur- thorough description of what has occurred, there
rent behavior in the current setting. Next, the are a number of problems with its use. Firstly, it
definition should be reviewed by people with can be very difficult to complete such recordings,
knowledge on the subject matter, as well as by as the observer is required to accurately note
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 145

everything that is occurring. This requires the tantrum behavior, defined as crying and pounding
observer to direct the majority of their attention fists, the observer could easily record how long
toward actually recording the behavior, rather the behavior lasts and how much time lapses
than toward the individual’s behavior (Lipinski & between occurrences. Additionally, in compari-
Nelson, 1974). This can result in the observer son to the previous method, much less attention
failing to fully observe the behavior, leading to needs to be directed toward the actual recording,
inaccuracies in the recording. Additionally, as the allowing the data collector to more accurately
recording is a narrative account of the behavior, it and thoroughly observe the behavior.
is difficult to compare findings with other Despite these improvements, there are still a
researchers and clinicians (Bijou et al., 1968). As number of problems associated with this method-
described above, a properly operationalized tar- ology. Firstly, it requires prior selection of behav-
get behavior is necessary for communication iors to be recorded (Goldfried, 1982; Lipinski &
between researchers and clinicians. A narrative Nelson, 1974). Any other behaviors that occur
summary does not allow for such communica- during the observation are not recorded. For
tion. Moreover, as there is no clear structure, dif- example, if tantrums are selected as the target
ferent observers might record different behavior, the occurrences of other challenging
information. For example, one observer might behaviors during the observation are not recorded.
record the location of the observation, whereas Potentially valuable information can be lost
another may omit this information. Finally, it can because of this. Challenging behaviors that occur
be difficult, if not impossible, to objectively at low frequencies can also be problematic for
determine the duration, latency, or intensity of a this type of event recording (Singh et al., 2006;
behavior from a narrative account (Bijou et al., Tarbox et al., 2009). If observations are carried
1968). Without the use of standardized language out at specific times for data collection, the
and recording procedures, there is too much room behavior must occur during the observation for
for subjective interpretation. any information to be recorded. Take, for exam-
To combat many of these weaknesses, one can ple, an individual who engages in self-injury
add behavioral codes to the above method. when hungry. If he or she is not hungry during
Behavioral codes identify target behaviors that the observation period, it is unlikely the behavior
can either be specific or general (Bijou et al., will occur, and therefore no information can be
1968). A specific code lists specific, operation- gathered on the behavior. This has been noted to
ally defined behaviors to be observed. In contrast, be especially problematic in mental illness, as
a general code lists a class of behaviors, allowing challenging behaviors typically occur at low fre-
for the recording of multiple behaviors (e.g., ver- quencies but high intensities in this population
bal responses). Specific symbols can be used to (Singh et al., 2006). A final problem, especially
represent the operationally defined target behav- with respect to functional assessment, is that
ior, making the recording process much simpler event recording says nothing about the function
(Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). Additionally, the fre- of the behavior (Bijou et al., 1968). No informa-
quency of target behaviors can be recorded with a tion is provided about the antecedents and conse-
checklist, hand counters, or electronic counters quences that may be influencing the behavior.
(Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). This methodol- This final limitation can be addressed through
ogy improves upon the previous one, primarily the use of Antecedent–Behavior–Consequence
by standardizing the definitions and procedures. (ABC) cards or sequence analysis (Bijou et al.,
This reduces the subjectivity involved, thereby 1968; Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). In addi-
allowing for the communication and comparison tion to recording the occurrence of a behavior,
of results. As the behaviors are discretely and the events that occur immediately before and
operationally defined, constructs such as dura- after the behavior are recorded. Similar to the
tion, latency, and intensity can be objectively recording of the behavior itself, the antecedents
recorded. For example, if the target behavior is and consequences can either be recorded in a
146 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

narrative fashion, or target antecedents and unlike event recording, time sampling allows for
consequences can be selected beforehand and quantification of data (Lerman & Iwata, 1993).
included on a checklist. This provides consider- For example, a 30-min data collection session on
ably more information than the previous meth- self-injury could be broken into 30, 1-min inter-
ods, as it begins to provide information about vals. If self-injury occurs during 15 intervals, this
possible functions of the behavior. However, one can be quantified, allowing one to say that the
should keep in mind that this method only behavior occurred during 50% of the intervals.
describes interactions between behaviors and This number can then be compared to subsequent
environmental events; this does not, in and of observations to determine if there is an increase
itself, establish a functional relationship (Bijou or decrease in the behavior. Given these advan-
et al., 1968). An additional problem with this tages, time sampling has frequently been used in
method is that it may difficult to quantify the data a variety of settings with a variety of challenging
obtained through this method (Lerman & Iwata, behaviors (Lerman & Iwata, 1993)
1993). It is difficult to determine if the antecedent Similar to event recording, there are a number
events are similarly correlated with nonoccur- of methods of collecting time-sampling data. The
rences of the target behavior. For example, sup- three primary methods of time sampling are
pose being asked to complete a task is identified as whole-interval time sampling, partial-interval time
an antecedent. While this may frequently occur sampling, and momentary time sampling (Sulzer-
before the target behavior, it may just as frequently, Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). Whole-interval time
or more frequently, occur without being followed sampling requires the behavior to occur through-
by the target behavior. As data are only collected out the interval. In the above example, the indi-
on occurrences, it is not possible to make this vidual must engage in self-injury for the entire
comparison. Finally, while some strategies for minute for it to be scored. Conversely, in partial-
examining sequential data have been proposed interval time sampling, only one instance of the
(Martens et al., 2008), there is no consensus within behavior must occur during the interval. In the
the field on how such data should be analyzed above example, if the individual engages in self-
(Tarbox et al., 2009). Additional problems with injury, even for just 1 s during the interval, it is
the use of ABC data will be discussed below in the scored. Finally, momentary time sampling requires
section discussing use and interpretation of data. the behavior to occur at the end of the interval.
While time sampling clearly has some advan-
tages, as with event recording, there are a number
Time Sampling of problems that must be considered. Firstly, time
sampling is not practical for infrequent behaviors
A second category of in vivo data collection is (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). For example,
termed time sampling (Bijou et al., 1968; Sulzer- take an individual who engages in self-injury
Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). Time sampling involves approximately once a week. While this behavior
recording the occurrence or nonoccurrence of a can be very serious, time-sampling procedures
behavior in a specified time interval. Time sam- will provide little to no information on the behav-
pling, in contrast to event recording, can be ior. Again, as this is often case in those with men-
appropriate for both discrete and nondiscrete tal illness, the effectiveness of this approach is
behaviors (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). For limited (Singh et al., 2006). Additionally, it is
example, take an individual who engages in self- much more difficult to identify antecedents and
injury by hitting his or her head. If this individual consequences with time-sampling procedures.
hits his or her head twice, pauses for 20 s, and then Specific incidences of the behavior are not
hits his or her head again two more times should recorded, thus it is not feasible to record anteced-
this be counted as one or two occurrences? While ent and consequences to the behavior. This limits
this is a problem for event recording, it is not at all the utility of this method for establishing func-
problematic for time sampling. Additionally, tional relationships.
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 147

Another problem is that time-sampling proce- became more accurate. A follow-up study
dures do not record all behaviors that occur dur- (Powell, Martindale, Kulp, Martindale, &
ing an observation (Johnston & Pennypacker, Bauman, 1977) similarly found that partial-
1993). For example, in momentary time sam- interval time sampling overestimated the fre-
pling, no recording occurs until the end of the quency of behavior, while whole-interval time
interval. However, this results in a large period of sampling underestimated the frequency of behav-
nonobservation time. The data collected will, ior. As error occurred in only one direction for each
therefore, typically be under- or over-representa- method, conducting a large number of observa-
tive of the true behavior. There is no way to assess tions could not control for this error. Additionally,
the extent to which the data is inaccurately repre- error remained large even when intervals lasted
senting the data (Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993). only 30 s and was not directly related to either the
Johnston and Pennypacker (1993), therefore, frequency or duration of the behavior. The authors
suggest limiting the amount of nonobservation suggest that this may lead researchers and clini-
time that occurs during a session. They also rec- cians to inaccurately interpret changes in behav-
ommend limiting the interpretation of data col- ior due to treatment. Conversely, momentary time
lected through time sampling, periodically sampling was fairly accurate when observations
assessing for accuracy of data, and matching pro- were conducted at 5, 10, 20, or 60-s intervals.
cedures with the distribution of responding. However, when intervals went beyond this length,
A considerable amount of research has exam- error began to increase. Finally, momentary time
ined the various methods of time sampling, high- sampling was found superior to both types of
lighting problems inherent with each. One set of interval time sampling for estimating the dura-
researchers compared frequency recording (i.e., tion a behavior occurred.
event recording), interval recording (i.e., partial- Another study compared momentary time
interval time sampling), and time sampling (i.e., sampling and partial-interval time sampling in
momentary time sampling) for use with different measuring behavior change, both absolute and
rates of behavior (Repp, Roberts, Slack, Repp, & relative (Harrop & Daniels, 1986). Both methods
Berkler, 1976). Results showed that momentary tended to overestimate the absolute rate of behav-
time sampling did not produce representative iors. Additionally, partial-interval time sampling
data, particularly when the behavior did not occur overestimated the absolute duration of behaviors,
at a constant rate and was frequently occurring. especially when behaviors occurred at lower rates
Partial-interval recording was more accurate for and shorter durations. Conversely, momentary
behaviors that occurred at low and medium rates; time sampling did not produce such errors. Based
however, it underestimated behaviors that on these findings, the authors suggested that dura-
occurred at high rates. tion, not rate, should be the dependent measure
Powell, Martindale, and Kulp (1975) com- when using momentary time sampling. However,
pared all three methods of time sampling with when measuring relative changes, the authors
frequency recording for measuring in-seat behav- found partial-interval time sampling to be more
ior. For frequency recording, the behavior was sensitive than momentary time sampling. Despite
continuously measured over the course of the this superiority, partial-interval time sampling
session. Whole-interval time sampling was found underestimated the change in a behavior if it was
to consistently underestimate the frequency of of high frequency and short duration.
the behavior, partial-interval time sampling was One shortcoming of these studies was their
found to consistently overestimate the frequency use of simulated or computer-simulated behav-
of the behavior, and momentary time sampling iors. Therefore, it is unclear to what extent they
both over-and underestimated the frequency of apply to behavioral observations in naturally set-
the behavior. However, it was noted that as the tings. Unfortunately, few studies have been con-
intervals were made shorter (i.e., more observa- ducted that examine these methods with naturally
tions were made), the time sampling methods occurring challenging behaviors (Matson &
148 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

Nebel-Schwalm, 2007). One of the only such et al., 2010). Kahng and Iwata (1998) conducted
studies, by Gardenier, MacDonald, and Green a review of 15 computerized systems for collect-
(2004), compared partial-interval time sampling ing real-time data. Although the reviewers were
and momentary time sampling for recording ste- unable to systematically analyze these systems,
reotypies in children with autism spectrum disor- they provided descriptive reports of the various
ders. In this study, partial-interval time sampling systems. Most of the systems reviewed included
was found to consistently overestimate the dura- software for analyzing data and many could be
tion of stereotypies. Momentary time sampling used on handheld devices. Systems ranged from
was found to both overestimate and underesti- free in price to over $1,500.
mate duration, but to a lesser extent than partial- Unfortunately, there has been little empirical
interval time sampling. Across all samples, research comparing electronic data collection
partial-interval time sampling overestimated the with hand-collected data. One such study com-
duration by an average of 164%, whereas momen- pared the two methods for recording responses of
tary time sampling over- and underestimated the children with autism during discrete trial training
duration by an average of 12–28% (depending on (Tarbox et al., 2010). Results found that elec-
the interval length). The authors concluded that tronic data collection required more time than
momentary time sampling should, therefore, be pen and paper collection for all four participants.
used for duration recording of stereotypy. There Accuracy of data was similar for both methods,
is clearly a need for additional research examin- with the average accuracy of electronic data rang-
ing the use of these methods with challenging ing from 83.75% to 95% and the average accu-
behaviors. racy of pen and paper data ranging from 98.13%
As a whole, these studies suggest a number of to 100%. Graphing data was accomplished faster
strengths and limitations inherent in each method via electronic data collection for all participants.
of time sampling. It does not appear that any form Although this study employed a small sample
of time sampling provides a true representation size, it is one of the only to empirically examine
of the frequency at which behaviors occur, electronic data collection. The researchers sum-
although partial-interval may be more represen- marize that although electronic data collection
tative for behaviors that do not occur at high fre- may save time outside of therapy sessions, it may
quencies (Harrop & Daniels, 1986; Repp et al., require more time during actual sessions.
1976). Conversely, momentary time sampling
may be less susceptible to error when recording
the duration of behaviors (Gardenier et al., 2004; General Problems with Data Collection
Harrop & Daniels, 1986; Powell et al., 1975,
1977). The limitations of these methods must be In addition to the problems discussed thus far,
taken into account when considering how data there are additional problems general to all
will be collected. in vivo data collection procedures. In vivo data
collection procedures can be very time consum-
ing, especially if narrative accounts are required
Electronic Data Collection (Arndorfer & Miltenberger, 1993; Iwata et al.,
1990). This decreases the likelihood of compli-
Although most data are collected by hand (i.e., ance with data collection procedures. For exam-
using pen and paper), electronic equipment is ple, teachers may not have time to complete ABC
being increasingly used to collect real-time data or time-sampling data, which require their frequent
(Tarbox, Wilke, Findel-Pyles, Bergstrom, & attention (Arndorfer & Miltenberger, 1993). On
Granpeesheh, 2010). Potential advantages of the other hand, they may be much more likely to
electronic data collection include simplicity, complete interviews and indirect assessments,
electronic storage of data, the ability to electroni- which can be conducted in one session. An addi-
cally analyze data, and simpler recording (Tarbox tional problem is that most of these techniques
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 149

require extensive training (Gardner, 2000; may have no relationship to the target behavior;
Hartmann et al., 2004; Tarbox et al., 2009). however, its presence may deter the observer from
Inadequate training may lead to decreases in the detecting other important, but less frequently
reliability of collected data, as will be discussed occurring, antecedents.
later (Bijou et al., 1968). A final problem is that some behaviors and
It is important that the data collected be repre- stimuli are difficult, if not impossible, to quantify
sentative of the individual’s typical behavior. (Bijou et al., 1968). This is especially true of bio-
However, how does one determine when enough logical or internal stimuli. For example, how does
data has been collected? How does one know that one quantify feelings of anxiousness through
the data is now representative? If the behavior observation? Challenging behaviors may serve
frequently occurs, one may need to collect many physical functions (Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush,
observations in many settings to get a full picture Smalls, & Vollmer, 2000), such as being uncom-
(Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). Unfortunately, there fortable or feeling ill. However, how can one
is no objective criterion for making this determi- record the frequency, duration, or intensity of
nation (Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). This is espe- these feelings through in vivo data collection?
cially problematic, considering the time and Similarly, social stimuli can be very difficult to
training needed to implement these techniques. objectively quantify. However, Bijou et al. (1968)
In order for accurate in vivo data collection to stress that such specific biological and social
occur, the observer must maintain contact with variables must be assessed for a thorough func-
the subject of data collection. A number of fac- tional assessment to take place. Additional prob-
tors, such as movement during the observation lems that may occur while collecting data, such
session, may interfere with this contact or make as reactivity and observer effects will be dis-
observation of the target behavior more difficult cussed more in depth below.
(Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993). For example, if
the target behavior is biting one’s hands, the
observer must be able to see the individual’s hands Reliability of Data
and mouth. If the individual moves during the
observation so that these are no longer visible, the One of the most important factors to consider
observation can no longer occur. Other behaviors when examining real-time data is the reliability
by the individual, or other individuals in the envi- of the data. It is extremely important that the data
ronment, may similarly make data collection that is collected be both consistent and accurate.
difficult, if not impossible (Barrios, 1993; Johnston It should be noted that agreement and accuracy
& Pennypacker, 1993). For example, a noisy envi- are not synonymous (Kazdin, 1977). Agreement
ronment may make it difficult to record instances exists when multiple raters make similar record-
of cursing. To counter such problems, one may ings, regardless of if these recordings are correct.
need to manipulate the environment to restrict For example, if both raters record that a behavior
these possibilities (Johnston & Pennypacker, occurs 10 times, agreement is 100%. This is true
1993). However, this adds a possible confounding regardless of how often the behavior actually
variable, as the setting is no longer truly the natu- occurred. In contrast, accuracy reflects if raters
ral setting in which the behavior typically occurs. record how often the behavior truly occurs.
Another problem with in vivo data collection is Typically, interobserver agreement is calculated
the presence of frequent variables unrelated to the and agreement is assumed to reflect accuracy
target behavior. These unrelated events might (Kazdin, 1977). However, agreement alone may
overshadow or mask relevant variables that occur not be enough to ensure the quality of data; accu-
less frequently (Iwata et al., 1990). Consider an racy and generalizability should also be reported
observation in which another individual is con- when possible (Mitchell, 1979).
stantly yelling and screaming next to the subject of While on the surface it seems that reliability
the observation. This variable (i.e., others yelling) should be easy to achieve, there are a number of
150 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

factors that can affect the achieved reliability. merely important to understand that there is more
Firstly, as discussed above, the definition of the than one method for calculating the reliability of
behavior itself can affect reliability (Bijou et al., data. It is important to understand the advantages
1968). If there is room for subjective interpreta- and disadvantages of each, so that the results can
tion, two observers may define the behavior dif- be interpreted correctly. Incorrectly used meth-
ferently. The two observers may, therefore, be ods may inflate or deflate the perceived reliability
recording two different behaviors, thereby affect- of the data, leading to incorrect interpretations.
ing the reliability of the data. Additional factors
that will be discussed below include the method
of calculating reliability, the coding system Coding Systems
employed, inadequate training, reactivity to the
reliability assessment, observer drift, and charac- There are many different ways to code behavior,
teristics of the observers (Bijou et al., 1968; each of which can impact the reliability. As dis-
Kazdin, 1977; Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). cussed previously, one decision that must be
made is whether to use specific or general codes
(Bijou et al., 1968). General codes allow for more
Calculating Reliability complex behavioral patterns to be recorded; how-
ever, general codes allow more room for interpre-
There are a number of methods for calculating tation. This, in turn, can lead to a decrease in
the reliability of data, each with its advantages reliability. The more comprehensive and specific
and disadvantages. While a full discussion of the code is, the higher reliability will be (Bijou
reliability techniques is beyond the scope of this et al., 1968).
chapter, a brief discussion about the importance A second reliability factor related to coding is
of selecting an appropriate technique is given. the complexity of the coding system. Complexity
This is extremely important, as selecting an inap- can be defined as the number of categories in the
propriate method for calculating reliability may coding system, the number of behaviors being
be one reason that inadequate reliability of data is observed, or the number of individuals being
found (Bijou et al., 1968). observed (Kazdin, 1977). As these numbers
Interobserver agreement is one of the most increase, the complexity of the coding system
common methods by which researchers calculate increases. The question then becomes what
reliability of data (Hartmann, 1977). Interobserver impact does increasing complexity have on reli-
agreement refers to the extent to which data ability? A number of researchers have sought to
between observers agree with one another answer this question. Mash and McElwee (1974)
(Mudford, Martin, Hui, & Taylor, 2009). There examined the effects of complexity, defined as
are a number of ways to calculate interobserver the number of categories, on both accuracy and
agreement, and the limitations of each method agreement. They compared the use of two coding
should be known before selecting one for use. systems, one with four behavior categories and
For example, one can divide the number of ses- the other with eight. Additionally, the eight-cate-
sions in which the two observers agreed by the gory system required the observer to make more
number of total sessions and multiply by 100. complex discriminations between categories. The
This method, while commonly used, is very strin- authors found an inverse relationship between
gent and does not use all of the information avail- complexity and reliability. They found that agree-
able (Hartmann, 1977). For a more detailed ment increased over time in the more complex
review of methods of calculating interobserver group, to the point that significant differences
agreement, the reader is directed elsewhere in the were no longer found after the fourth trial.
literature (e.g., Hopkins & Hermann, 1977; Accuracy similarly increased over time, although
House, House, & Campbell, 1981; Mudford it remained significantly lower in the more com-
et al., 2009). For the purposes of this chapter, it is plex group throughout the study. This was despite
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 151

the fact that both groups showed mastery of the warnings against potential sources of error,
coding systems during training. The predictabil- including biases, observer drift, and reactivity (as
ity of the behavior did not have any effect on discussed below). The second step is to educate
accuracy. observers about the operational definition that
Taplin and Reid (1973) conducted a study on will be used and how data will be recorded. This
the effects of instructions and experimenters on may be accomplished through written materials,
observer reliability. While not the primary aim of filmed instructions, or in-person demonstrations.
the study, the researchers also examined the effect The third step is to evaluate the observer’s train-
of complexity, defined as the number of different ing. Observers are assessed to ensure that they
codes used, on reliability. They found a moderate have an adequate understanding of the operational
negative correlation (r = −0.52) between com- definition and coding system. Feedback and cor-
plexity and reliability. rections are given at this step, until the observer
Kazdin (1977) provides a number of implica- has mastered the system. Additionally, the opera-
tions of these findings. Firstly, estimates of reli- tional definition and coding system may be altered
ability must be interpreted with respect to at this step if they are found to be inadequate. The
complexity. Additionally, the complexity of the fourth step is application; observers begin using
data collection system may change over time. For the data collection system, first in analog situa-
example, if interventions are successful, the num- tions and then in real situations. Observers must
ber of behaviors being recorded may decrease. attain sufficient agreement and accuracy to prog-
Therefore, calculations of reliability may not be ress. This ensures that observers have mastered
comparable across different phases. the system before collecting data in the situation
Barrios (1993) calls for a rational appraisal of of interest. Observers are gradually introduced to
the demands of the coding system. This involves data collection in the setting of interest, as mas-
having the system evaluated by those creating tery in analog sessions does not ensure mastery in
the system, colleagues, and potential observers. actual sessions (as discussed further below).
If the demands are found to be too high, one Additionally, observers are continually provided
may need to decrease the number of behaviors feedback concerning their reliability and reminded
being tracked, simplify the nature of the behav- that reliability will be periodically checked. The
ior being tracked, or decrease the duration of fifth step is recalibration. This is where reliability
observations. of data collection is assessed in the actual situa-
tion of interest. The final step in training and
monitoring is termination. After data collection is
Training completed, observers are asked for feedback on
the data collection system, provided information
Another factor that can influence the reliability of on what was found and how it will be used,
data is the training of observers. If observers are not reminded of confidentiality, and thanked for their
properly trained, they may inaccurately collect data assistance. Hartmann and Wood (1982) provide a
and fail to control their own behaviors (Bijou et al., similar seven-step model.
1968). Bijou et al., (1968) provide some recom- An additional aspect of training is the type of
mendations for ensuring adequate training, such as behavior that is trained. Mash and McElwee
familiarizing observers with recording tools and (1974) found that observers who were trained to
employing a second observer during training. code unpredictable behavior had better accuracy
Barrios (1993) provides a six-step model for in novel situations than those trained to code pre-
training and monitoring observers. The first step dictable behavior sequences. This is critical as
is orientation. This involves conveying the impor- in vivo data collection necessitates that observa-
tance of objective data collection to observers. In tions occur in varying settings and situations.
this step, observers are also told what they will be As mentioned above, even if observers achieve
doing and what is expected of them. This includes mastery in training, this does not necessarily
152 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

mean that they have been adequately trained. believed that no reliability assessment would
Mash and McElwee (1974) found that the reli- take place, that about 20% of their data would
ability of data collected by observers using a randomly be checked for reliability after observa-
complex coding system was inadequate, despite tions were completed, or that periodic overt
the fact that they had achieved mastery of the cod- checks would take place. Results showed that
ing system during training. Similarly, Taplin and reliability during spot-checks was significantly
Reid (1973) found that observers never attained higher than both before and after the checks. No
the same level of performance during data collec- significant differences were found between
tion that they achieved in training. In fact, reli- the groups with respect to reliability. This is
ability decreased, on average, 15% once training likely because reliability for all groups dropped
was completed and data collection began. Thus, significantly after training.
the reliability achieved during training is likely to Romanczyk, Kent, Diament, and O’Leary,
be an overestimate of the reliability that will be (1973) conducted a similar experiment to exam-
achieved during actual data collection. The steps ine the differential affects of overt and covert reli-
outlined above by Barrios (1993) are intended to ability assessment. Two assessors accompanied
help protect against this drop in reliability. observers during all observations. In the overt
condition, observers were led to believe that
either one assessor was computing reliability or
Reactivity to the Assessment both were computing reliability. In reality, both
of Reliability assessors were computing reliability in all con-
ditions each using a unique code. This allowed
As reliability of data is crucial, researchers and the researchers to compare reliability with an
clinicians will often compute reliability as data identified assessor and reliability with an
are being collected. However, knowledge of this unidentified assessor. In the covert condition,
reliability assessment can, in and of itself, affect observers were not told that reliability was being
reliability. Reid (1970) conducted an experiment assessed. Reliability during covert assessment
in an analog setting to examine this effect. was consistently lower than reliability during
Participants were instructed to code behavioral overt assessment. Disruptive behaviors were
data in a lab, believing that the reliability was reported at lower rates (approximately 25%
being assessed only during the first two sessions lower) when observers did not believe that reli-
(reliability was covertly assessed during the ability was being checked. Additionally, reliabil-
remaining three sessions). The reliability of ity was consistently higher with the identified
observers significantly decreased when they did assessor than with the unidentified assessor. This
not believe that reliability as being checked. suggested that, when observers knew who was
Reliability dropped by an average of approxi- computing reliability, they shifted their observa-
mately 25% when reliability was covertly tional criteria to be more like the assessor.
checked. This change was not gradual, occurring These findings strongly suggest that observers
immediately upon switching conditions. Although are reactive to knowledge of reliability assess-
a complex coding system was used in this study, ments. Observers are more likely to record reli-
it should be noted that in the previously discussed able data when they believe that reliability is
study by Mash and McElwee (1974), reliability being assessed. Unfortunately, it is not typically
increased over time in the complex condition. possible to continuously assess for reliability.
The opposite was found in the current study due Similarly, it may be difficult to convince observ-
to the covert reliability assessment. ers that their reliability is constantly being
A follow-up study by Taplin and Reid (1973) checked (Kazdin, 1977). Taplin and Reid (1973)
compared three conditions: covert reliability suggest that it may be necessary to overtrain
assessment, random overt reliability assessment, observers, so that reliability remains adequate
and spot-check assessment. That is to say participants even if it decreases after training.
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 153

Observer Drift Kazdin, 1977). Observer drift is, therefore,


another reason that in vivo data assessment can
One possible explanation for decreases in reli- require extensive training and costs.
ability after training is known as observer drift.
While different researchers have defined the term
observer drift differently, the term chiefly refers Observer Bias, Distraction,
to changes in an observer’s definition of a behav- and Discontent
ior over time (Kazdin, 1977; Smith, 1986). For
example, observers may be trained to collect data Characteristics of the observer can also threaten
on physical aggression, defined as hitting, push- the reliability of real-time data. Both implicit and
ing, and pinching. If pinching is low in frequency explicit biases by the observer, distractions to the
or intensity, the observer may no longer include observer, and discontent can all affect the reli-
this in their definition of physical aggression. So ability of collected data. Additionally, the man-
while data would include pinching at first, later ner in which data is presented by observers can
data would no longer include this aspect of the affect the way in which the data is interpreted.
behavior. As pinching should be recorded based Observer expectancies and biases can influence
on the behavior’s definition, reliability will the reliability of collected data. Such biases
decrease over time. include hypotheses about the purpose of the data
Kent, O’Leary, Diament, and Dietz (1974) collection, hypotheses as to how the subject of
examined the variance in behavioral recordings data collection should behave, and beliefs about
accounted for by observer. Observer pairs what the data should look like (Hartmann &
accounted for a total of about 17% of the variance Wood, 1982). Biases can also be developed based
in recordings of disruptive behavior, with about on subject characteristics and information
5% representing consistent differences between expressed by the primary investigator (Hartmann
pairs of observers throughout the experiment. & Wood, 1982).
Additionally, about 12% of the total variance in A study by Kent et al. (1974) examined the
disruptive behavior recordings were accounted for effect of expectation biases on behavior record-
by interactions of observer pair with other factors. ing. Observers were either told to predict changes
Observer drift may be difficult to detect, as in behavior or to predict no change. They found
agreement between observers may remain high, that the evaluations of the treatment effects were
while accuracy decreases (Kazdin, 1977). significantly affected by predictions. Those told
Observers who work together closely may make to predict changes in behavior reported seeing a
similar changes to their definitions, thereby main- global change in behavior. However, actual
taining agreement and losing accuracy. This behavior recordings did not significantly differ
effect is known as consensual observer drift between the groups. Those who believed the
(Johnson & Bolstad, 1973; Power, Paul, Licht, & behavior would change were just as accurate as
Engel, 1982; Smith, 1986). Observer drift may those who did not believe the behavior [would]
also affect within subjects research designs, as change. This would seem to suggest that expec-
the observer may change his or her definition tancies do not bias in vivo data collection,
over the course of the study (Lipinski & Nelson, although they may affect global evaluations.
1974). Taking the above example of pinching, A follow-up study (O’Leary, Kent, &
data might show a decrease in physical aggres- Kanowitz, 1975) examined the influence of both
sion over time. However, this decrease would be instructions and feedback on data collection.
confounded by the observer’s modification of the Observers were told that two behaviors were
definition of physical aggression. The best way to expected to decrease, whereas two other behav-
combat this problem is likely to periodically iors would experience no change. Positive feed-
retrain observers, ensuring they are applying the back was given when observers reported decreases
definition correctly (Hartmann et al., 2004; in the behavior that was expected to change,
154 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

whereas negative feedback was provided when is that it can occur in the natural environment,
they reported no change or increases in the target increasing the external validity of data.
behaviors. After feedback, observers recorded Unfortunately, there are still a number of extrane-
the target behaviors significantly less frequently, ous factors that may influence in vivo data collec-
suggesting that significant bias had occurred. No tion, threatening the validity of the data. In vivo
changes were found with respect to the control data collection is often used to measure the fre-
behaviors. These findings suggest that while quency, intensity, and duration of a target challeng-
expectancies may not bias data collection, a com- ing behavior. Extraneous variables, such as observer
bination of both expectancies and feedback can effects and reactivity, can alter these target vari-
have a significant impact. ables during data collection sessions, making the
Another possible source of bias involves the data no longer representative of the behavior.
presentation and analysis of data. Although often
overlooked as a source of bias, inappropriate
analysis and misleading presentation of data can Observer Effects and Reactivity
be another form of bias (Mcnamara &
MacDonough, 1972). For example, data may be As discussed, one of the primary advantages of
biased if no statistical analyses are conducted. in vivo data collection is that it can be conducted
Similarly, graphical data can be misleading if not in the natural environment in which the behavior
displayed accurately. It is critical that data be occurs (Gardner, 2000). This assumes, however,
reported as unambiguously as possible, so that that the presence of an observer does not affect
anyone who uses the data can come to similar this environment. Research has shown that this is
conclusions (Mcnamara & MacDonough, 1972). not necessarily the case (Hartmann & Wood,
If observers are distracted, either externally or 1982; Lipinski & Nelson, 1974; Repp, Nieminen,
internally, collected data may not be reliable Olinger, & Brusca, 1988). Those being observed
(Barrios, 1993). For example, if there is a lot of may even be hostile about the fact that they are
noise in the environment, the observer may be being observed (Lipinski & Nelson, 1974).
unfocused and unable to record all relevant behav- Conversely, they may try to impress observers or
iors. Similarly, worries or preoccupations by the reduce challenging behaviors in the presence of
observer may distract him or her from accurately observers (Lipinski & Nelson, 1974). Such
recording data. Barrios (1993) also discusses dis- changes in behavior during observations are
content as a type of internal distractor that may be often termed reactive effects or reactivity
especially problematic. If observers are treated in (Hartmann & Wood, 1982). A number of factors
a disrespectful or harsh manner, discontent may can contribute to reactive effects, such the child’s
arise. Discontent may also arise from unpleasant gender and age, the gender of the observer, the
interactions between observers and others familiarity of the participant with the observer,
involved in the data collection process. While and the observation setting (Gardner, 2000).
steps can be taken to reduce possible external dis- The issue of reactivity is a complex one and its
tractors, they are likely to reduce the external effects on the validity of data are unclear (Goldfried,
validity of data. Barrios (1993) suggests monitor- 1982; Hartmann & Wood, 1982). Hartmann and
ing observers for signs of internal distractions and Wood (1982) outlined five factors that may con-
intervening if necessary. However, it may not be tribute to reactivity: social desirability, subject
possible to detect all internal distractions. characteristics, conspicuousness of observation,
observer attributes, and the rational for observa-
tion. Individuals may try to suppress undesirable
Validity of Data behaviors or engage in more socially appropriate
behaviors when being observed. Therefore, an
In addition to ensuring accuracy and agreement, individual may be less likely to engage in the chal-
it is critical that data collection remain valid. One lenging behavior during observations, as they are
of the main advantages of in vivo data collection not socially desirable behaviors. Characteristics of
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 155

those being observed may also contribute to reac- conditions, likely accounting for the changes in
tivity. Hartmann and Wood (1982) suggest that compliance.
those who are more sensitive, less confident, and Taken together, these findings are quite
older than 6 years old may be more reactive than significant. Suppose parents wants to ensure that
others during observations. Additionally, the more their children will receive treatment. Parents
obtrusive the data collection is, the more likely could manipulate their behavior to make the chil-
reactive effects will occur. However, findings on dren’s behavior appear worse than it typically is
obtrusiveness are not consistent and do not guar- during an observation. The data collected in this
antee that the data will be invalid. The fourth fac- observation would not be representative of the
tor outlined by Hartmann and Wood, 1982 is true behavior and thus no longer valid (Harris &
characteristics of the observer. Attributes of the Lahey, 1982). However, it may be difficult to
observer, such as race and gender, may influence detect if this is occurring. While certain parental
reactive effects. Finally, the rationale for observa- behaviors were associated with changes in behav-
tion is potentially influential factor. If there is not a ior (e.g., commanding), it is unlikely that the
good rationale for the presence of the observer, observer will know if this behavior is typical of
reactive effects may be more likely. the parent.
Unfortunately, there is a general lack of
research on the effects of such characteristics,
particularly with respect to challenging behaviors Interpretation and Use
(Goldfried, 1982; Harris & Lahey, 1982). If sub-
stantial changes in behavior occur, reactivity can The use of real-time data for functional assess-
affect the external and internal validity of col- ment is described in greater detail elsewhere in
lected data. It is not possible to separate the this text. However, a brief consideration should
effects of reactivity and one cannot be sure the be given to how in vivo data is used and inter-
data are similar to what would be found without preted. When in vivo data is used to examine the
the effect of reactivity (Repp et al., 1988). maintaining variable of a challenging behavior, it
Researchers have also found that children can is often termed descriptive analysis (Iwata et al.,
be reactive to parents’ behavior during observa- 1990; Lerman & Iwata, 1993). There are many
tions (Harris & Lahey, 1982). Lobitz and Johnson ways to conduct descriptive analyses, many of
(1975) examined the effects of parental manipu- which are slight adaptations of others. A brief
lation on their children’s behaviors. Parents were description of two of the more basic methods will
asked to present their child as bad, good, or nor- be given, along with associated problems. The
mal. Significantly more challenging behaviors reader is directed to Chap. 8 for a more in-depth
were found under the bad condition, when com- discussion of these methods.
pared with the other two. This was true for both The first method, as discussed above, is the
children with a history of challenging behaviors use of ABC cards, also known as sequential anal-
and those without such a history. No significant ysis (Bijou et al., 1968; Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer,
differences were found between the good and 1977). As data are quantified, one can calculate
normal conditions, suggesting that parents could the probability that a target behavior will follow a
not make their children look better, only worse. specific antecedent or be followed by a specific
In a similar study examining compliance, par- consequence (Iwata et al., 1990). However, as
ents were asked to make their children appear mentioned previously, there are no standards for
obedient and later disobedient in a clinic play- interpreting this data (Tarbox et al., 2009).
room (Green, Forehand, & McMahon, 1979). Additionally, the results from such an analysis are
Significant changes in compliance were found in often inconsistent; the function suggested by the
children with a history of challenging behaviors antecedent may be inconsistent with the function
and those without such a history. Parental behav- suggested by the consequence (Tarbox et al., 2009).
iors, such as use of rewards and questioning ver- A second method, developed by Touchette,
sus commanding, differed between the two MacDonald, and Langer (1985), is known as a
156 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

scatter plot analysis. Data is graphed on a scatter attention as the maintaining variable, when it is,
plot, with levels of a behavior observed during in fact, escape.
specified time intervals being recorded through-
out the day. The main purpose of this method is
to see if there is a pattern of distribution of behav- Conclusions
iors throughout the day. Thus the scatter plot
shows when a behavior typically occurs and The collection of data is one of the primary
when it rarely occurs. This allows for aspects of conducting a functional assessment.
identification of possible temporal variables that Data are used to help understand the nature of the
may be affecting the target variable. The authors challenging behavior, including characteristics
suggest that the scatter plot analysis be used such as the frequency, duration, intensity, and
when a behavior is frequently occurring, as infor- function of the behavior. The primary method in
mal observation may not suggest a reliable func- which this data is collected in vivo. In vivo data
tional relationship. The main problem with has its primary advantage in that it is collected in
scatter plot analyses is that the scatter plot only the natural environment in which the behavior
provides information on environmental variables occurs, bypassing the sources of bias and error
related to the time of day (Axelrod, 1987; Iwata that may come from indirect data.
et al., 1990). For example, suppose an individual While in vivo data no doubt has its advantages,
engages in self-injury when he or she sees many potential problems with such data have
another individual with a preferred item. As this been discussed. There are a number of factors
could occur at any point during the day, the scat- that can influence the reliability and validity of
ter plot is unlikely to reveal information about in vivo data. Careful consideration should be
this relationship. Thus, the scatter plot will only given to these factors at each stage of data collec-
identify antecedents and consequences that are tion, from defining the behavior to using the data.
relate to the behavior on a fixed, regular basis While the problems of in vivo data have been dis-
(Axelrod, 1987). cussed at length in this chapter, in vivo data are
A major problem for all methods of descrip- not without utility. Several researchers have pro-
tive analyses is that established relationships are vided recommendations on how to minimize
correlational (Bijou et al., 1968; Iwata et al., many of these sources of error (e.g., Barrios,
1990; Lerman & Iwata, 1993; Tarbox et al., 1993; Repp et al., 1988). Additionally, while
2009). Just because a relationship has been estab- research does not support conducting functional
lished, it does not necessarily reflect a functional assessments based solely on in vivo data (Iwata
relationship. For example, as described above, et al., 1990; Lerman & Iwata, 1993; Tarbox et al.,
the target behavior may be highly correlated with 2009), there may be value in combining this
frequently occurring, but unrelated events (Iwata approach with others.
et al., 1990). Conversely, the true functional vari- While a great deal of research has been con-
able may be one that is only reinforced intermit- ducted on this subject matter, there is still a need
tently. For example, take a child who engages in for more. Much of the research that has been con-
tantrums in an attempt to escape hygiene-related ducted on in vivo data collection predates the
tasks. This may serve as the function even if the popularization of modern functional assessment.
probability of escape is very low. Therefore, a Thus little research has been conducted on these
very low correlation would be found between the sources of error with respect to their use in the
behavior and the consequence of escaping. functional assessment process. While much of
Conversely, if the mother of the child provides the findings are likely to hold true, there is a need
the child with attention during the tantrum, there for more research examining this empirically.
would be a high correlation found between the Additional research in this area will help to ensure
behavior and the consequence of attention. Thus the reliability and validity of data, so that more
descriptive analysis may incorrectly identify meaningful interpretations can be made.
9 In Vivo Assessment: Issues of Real-Time Data 157

Hartmann, D. P., & Wood, D. D. (1982). Observational


References methods. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. E. Kazdin
(Eds.), International handbook of behavior
Arndorfer, R. E., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1993). Functional modification and therapy (pp. 109–138). New York:
assessment and treatment of challenging behavior: A Plenum.
review with implications for early childhood. Topics in Hawkins, R. P., & Dobes, W. (1977). Behavioral definitions
Early Childhood Special Education, 13, 82–105. in applied behavior analysis. In B. C. Etzel, J. M.
Axelrod, S. (1987). Functional and structural analyses of LeBlanc, & D. M. Baer (Eds.), New developments in
behavior: Approaches leading to reduced use of pun- behavioral research: Theory, method and application.
ishment procedures? Research in Developmental Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Disabilities, 8, 165–178. Hopkins, B. L., & Hermann, J. A. (1977). Evaluating
Barrios, B. A. (1993). Direct observation. In T. H. interobserver reliability of interval data. Journal of
Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of child and Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 121–126.
adolescent assessment (pp. 140–164). Boston: Allyn House, A. E., House, B. J., & Campbell, M. B. (1981).
& Bacon. Measures of interobserver agreement: Calculation
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968). A formulas and distribution effects. Journal of
method to integrate descriptive and experimental field Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 3,
studies at the level of data and empirical concepts. 37–57.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191. Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1990). The
Eddy, J. M., Dishion, T., & Stoolmiller, M. (1998). The experimental (functional) analysis of behavior disor-
analysis of intervention change in children and fami- ders: Methodology, applications, and limitations. In A.
lies: Methodological and conceptual issues embedded C. Repp & N. N. Singh (Eds.), Perspectives on the use
in intervention studies. Journal of Abnormal Child of nonaversive and aversive interventions for persons
Psychology, 26, 53–69. with developmental disabilities (pp. 301–330).
Fergusson, D., Lynskey, M., & Horwood, L. (1993). The Sycamore: Sycamore Publishing.
effect of maternal depression on maternal ratings of Johnson, S. M., & Bolstad, O. D. (1973). Methodological
child behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, issues in naturalistic observation: Some problems and
21, 245–269. solutions for field research. In L. A. Hamerlynck, L. C.
Gardenier, N. C., MacDonald, R., & Green, G. (2004). Handy, & E. J. Mash (Eds.), Behavior change:
Comparison of direct observational methods for mea- Methodology, concepts, and practice. Champaign:
suring stereotypic behavior in children with autism Research Press.
spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (1993). Strategies
Disabilities, 25, 99–118. and tactics of behavioral research (2nd ed.). Hillsdale:
Gardner, F. (2000). Methodological issues in the direct Erlbaum.
observation of parent-child interaction: Do observa- Kahng, S. W., & Iwata, B. A. (1998). Computerized sys-
tional findings reflect the natural behavior of partici- tems for collecting real-time observational data.
pants? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 253–261.
3, 185–198. Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and complexity of
Goldfried, M. R. (1982). Behavioral assessment. In A. S. assessment: The ABCs of reliability. Journal of
Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 141–150.
International handbook of behavior modification and Kent, R. N., O’Leary, K. D., Diament, C., & Dietz, A.
therapy. New York: Plenum. (1974). Expectation biases in observational evaluation
Green, K. D., Forehand, R., & McMahon, R. J. (1979). of therapeutic change. Journal of Consulting and
Parental manipulation of compliance and noncompli- Clinical Psychology, 42, 774–780.
ance in normal and deviant children. Behavior Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and
Modification, 3, 245–266. experimental analyses of variables maintaining self-
Harris, F. C., & Lahey, B. B. (1982). Subject reactivity in injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
direct observational assessment: A review and critical Analysis, 26, 293–319.
analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 2, 523–538. Lipinski, D., & Nelson, R. (1974). Problems in the use of
Harrop, A., & Daniels, M. (1986). Methods of time sam- naturalistic observation as a means of behavioral
pling: A reappraisal of momentary time sampling and assessment. Behavior Therapy, 5, 341–351.
partial interval recording. Journal of Applied Behavior Lobitz, W. C., & Johnson, S. M. (1975). Parental manipu-
Analysis, 19, 73–77. lation of the behavior of normal and deviant children.
Hartmann, D. P. (1977). Considerations in the choice of Child Development, 46, 719–726.
interobserver reliability estimates. Journal of Applied Martens, B. K., DiGennaro, F. D., Reed, D. D., Szczech,
Behavior Analysis, 10, 103–116. F. M., & Rosenthal, B. D. (2008). Contingency space
Hartmann, D. P., Barrios, B. A., & Wood, D. D. (2004). analysis: An alternative method for identifying contin-
Principles of behavioral observation. In S. N. Haynes gent relations from observational data. Journal of
& E. M. Heiby (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 69–81.
psychological assessment: Behavioral assessment Mash, E. J., & McElwee, J. D. (1974). Situational effects
(Vol. 3, pp. 108–127). Hoboken: Wiley. on observer accuracy: Behavioral predictability, prior
158 M. Horovitz and J.L. Matson

experience, and complexity of coding categories. Repp, A. C., Nieminen, G. S., Olinger, E., & Brusca, R.
Child Development, 45, 367–377. (1988). Direct observations: Factors affecting the
Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. (2007). Assessing accuracy of observers. Exceptional Children, 55,
challenging behaviors in children with autism spec- 29–36.
trum disorders: A review. Research in Developmental Repp, A. C., Roberts, D. M., Slack, D. J., Repp, C. F., &
Disabilities, 28, 567–579. Berkler, M. S. (1976). A comparison of frequency,
McNamara, J. R., & MacDonough, T. S. (1972). Some interval and time-sampling methods of data collection.
methodological considerations in the design and Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 501–508.
implementation of behavior therapy research. Behavior Romanczyk, R. G., Kent, R. N., Diament, C., & O’Leary,
Therapy, 3, 361–378. K. D. (1973). Measuring the reliability of observa-
Mitchell, S. (1979). Interobserver agreement, reliability, tional data: A reactive process. Journal of Applied
and generalizability of data collected in observational Behavior Analysis, 6, 175–184.
studies. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 376–390. Singh, N. N., Matson, J. L., Lancioni, G. E., Singh, A. N.,
Mudford, O. C., Martin, N. T., Hui, J. K. Y., & Taylor, S. Adkins, A. D., McKeegan, G. F., et al. (2006).
A. (2009). Assessing observer accuracy in continuous Questions about behavioral function in mental illness
recording of rate and duration: Three algorithms com- (QABF-MI): A behavior checklist for functional
pared. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, assessment of maladaptive behavior exhibited by indi-
527–539. viduals with mental illness. Behavior Modification,
O’Leary, K. D., Kent, R. N., & Kanowitz, J. (1975). 30, 739–751.
Shaping data collection congruent with experimental Smith, G. A. (1986). Observer drift—A drifting definition.
hypotheses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, Behavior Analyst, 9, 127–128.
43–51. Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying
Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y., behavior analysis procedures with children and youth.
& Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about behavioral New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func- Taplin, P. S., & Reid, J. B. (1973). Effects of instructional
tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in set and experimenter influence on observer reliability.
Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229. Child Development, 44, 547–554.
Powell, J., Martindale, A., & Kulp, S. (1975). An evalua- Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Findel-Pyles, R. S., Bergstrom,
tion of time-sample measures of behavior. Journal of R. M., & Granpeesheh, D. (2010). A comparison of
Applied Behavioral Analysis, 8, 463–469. electronic to traditional pen-and-paper data collection
Powell, J., Martindale, B., Kulp, S., Martindale, A., & in discrete trial training for children with autism.
Bauman, R. (1977). Taking a closer look: Time sam- Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 65–75.
pling and measurement error. Journal of Applied Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
Behavior Analysis, 10, 325–332. R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009).
Power, C. T., Paul, G. L., Licht, M. H., & Engel, K. L. Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental
(1982). Evaluation of self-contained training proce- functional assessments of challenging behavior in
dures for the Time-Sample Behavioral Checklist. children with autism. Journal of Developmental and
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514.
Assessment, 4, 223–261. Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N.
Reid, J. B. (1970). Reliability assessment of observation (1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control
data: A possible methodological problem. Child of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Development, 41, 1143–1150. Analysis, 18, 343–351.
Scaling Methods
of Functional Assessment 10
Megan Sipes and Johnny L. Matson

Challenging behaviors are common in those with Gettinger & Callan Stoiber, 2006). As such,
developmental disabilities (DD) and intellectual functional assessments have grown in popularity.
disabilities (ID) (Matson, Cooper, Malone, & One review found that previous to 1985, approxi-
Moskow, 2008; Matson, Kiely, & Bamburg, mately 35% of studies which used behavioral
1997; Murphy et al., 2005) and also in typically intervention for aggressive behavior in those with
developing children with emotional and behav- DD or ID used some form of functional assess-
ioral problems (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998). ment. However, this percentage increased to 71%
Estimates suggest that in those with DD, such as for more recent studies which used functional
Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), 8–17% of assessment (Hile & Desrochers, 1993). Functional
individuals exhibit challenging behaviors assessments allow for the identification of the
(Emerson & Bromley, 1995; Kiernan et al., 1997; maintaining factor of the behavior so that preven-
Lowe et al., 2007). These behaviors range from tion and intervention strategies can be added to a
physical aggression (i.e., hitting, kicking, and bit- treatment program, as well as replacement behav-
ing others) to self-injurious behavior (SIB; i.e., iors which allow the person to achieve the goal of
hitting self, head banging, skin picking) to prop- the challenging behavior by more appropriate
erty destruction. While severe challenging behav- means. Furthermore, functional assessment for
iors are common in those with DD and ID, a each challenging behavior is required as topo-
similar number of typically developing children graphically similar behaviors may serve drasti-
also exhibit challenging behaviors that may lead cally different functions (Iwata et al., 1994).
to emotional and behavioral disorders later on
(Webster-Stratton, 1997). It has been well estab-
lished in the literature that the most effective Functional Assessment Methods
treatments are interventions based on the func-
tions of the behavior (DuPaul & Ervin, 1996; A variety of methods of functional assessment
are commonly used in the literature. Just some of
the methods include direct observation (i.e., scat-
ter plots and ABC charts), analogue methodolo-
M. Sipes (*) gies (i.e., experimental functional analysis; EFA),
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
and indirect methods (i.e., scales and interviews).
e-mail: msipes1@gmail.com
Conroy, Davis, Fox, and Brown (2002) proposed
J.L. Matson
a three-tier model which suggests the path for
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA functional assessments. The first tier includes
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com environmental assessment and intervention.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 159


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_10,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
160 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

These changes are global and are put in place for about the function of the problematic behavior.
everyone in that particular setting. For example, One other possible problem with direct assess-
there may be physical changes to the environ- ment methods may occur when the problem
ment which may include the seating arrangement behaviors occur at low frequencies. As a result,
or materials available. The first tier also includes many hours of direct observation could be needed
instructional changes, such as how individuals to gain the needed information and this may not
are directed and expectations. The second tier is be feasible. Examples of direct functional assess-
used for children who are at a higher risk for ment methods include scatter plots and ABC
developing problem behaviors. During this level charts.
of assessment, children might require behavioral
observation or interviews with teachers to gain Scatter plots
more information about the behavior. At the final Originally developed by Touchette, MacDonald,
level of assessment, tier three, assessment and and Langer (1985), the scatter plot method aims
intervention is individualized. For this level, tech- to correlate events in the natural environment
niques may include use of scaling methods or with certain times of day. The data is collected by
EFAs to gain information about antecedents and defining a specific interval and then obtaining
consequences of behavior. The scaling methods information on the frequency and duration of
which are in tier three will be the main focus of behaviors. For example, the time interval may be
this chapter. defined as 1 min and then the observer is to record
However, to fully evaluate the functional if the targeted challenging behavior occurred at
assessment scales, it is important to consider any time during that interval. The percent of
other methods of determining the functions of intervals that the behavior occurred can be calcu-
behaviors to examine the pros and cons of these lated and patterns about when throughout the day
methods. Different methods of functional assess- the behavior is most likely to occur can be
ment are briefly described here and the strengths deduced. However, this information is limited
and weaknesses of each method are highlighted. because little can be gathered about the anteced-
Then, specific scaling methods are reviewed in ents and consequences, though some have sug-
greater detail. gested that this data can also be collected (Bosma
& Mulick, 1990). Furthermore, a benefit of using
scatter plots is that data is relatively simple to
Direct Methods collect, and as a result, it is easily used in applied
settings. Because of the simplicity of this method,
The first set of functional assessment methods scatter plots have been used often in schools and
are referred to as the direct methods. This is inpatient facilities for those with ID and DD.
because the behaviors are observed in their natu- Unfortunately, a lack of research exists which
ral environment and no manipulation of the actually examines the psychometric properties of
behavior is typically taking place. Due to the this method (Matson & Minshawi, 2007).
natural observations which are used in direct
methods, this form of functional assessment pos- ABC Charts
sesses the benefit of higher ecological validity A second functional assessment method that uses
(Hall, 2005). This allows for greater generaliza- direct observation is ABC charting. In this
tion of the results into real-world settings. While method, the observer collects data on A (the ante-
direct assessment of challenging behaviors is cedent), B (the behavior), and C (the conse-
advantageous because the observer does not need quence). The antecedent includes events that
to rely on secondary information from a caregiver occurred before the target behavior, while the
or teacher, one can only glean correlational data behavior is the target action of concern. The con-
(Hall). This is because the environment is not sequence includes anything that happens after the
controlled, and thus, no causation can be implied behavior occurs, whether they are an action of the
10 Scaling Methods 161

individual or other people in the environment. controlled environments which are very different
The more detailed this information will be, the than the individuals’ typical environment (Hall,
more helpful it will be in hypothesizing the func- 2005). Furthermore, several other shortcomings
tion of the behavior (Joyce, 2006). Unlike the of EFAs exist. For example, the nature of this
scatter plot, the ABC charting method provides method requires the behavior to be elicited. This
much information about events occurring around brings up ethical considerations when dealing
the behavior of interest, but less temporal infor- with behaviors that are harmful to the individual
mation is gained (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). as is often the case with SIB. On a similar note,
In addition, this method can be more time con- new functions of the behaviors may develop
suming, especially if detailed information is through the manipulations that occur during trails
recorded. Finally, some subjective information which could make treatment more difficult and
may be included in the reports which can con- complex (Matson & Minshawi, 2007). In addi-
taminate the findings. tion, certain conditions may be difficult to repli-
cate in the assessment trials. Overall, it has been
noted that while EFAs are useful in research set-
Analogue Methods tings, these assessments are not often feasible in
applied settings (Matson & Minshawi).
A second class of functional assessments is
referred to as analogue assessments. The most
common method of analogue assessment is Indirect Methods
referred to as EFA. EFA is considered an ana-
logue assessment because the assessments are The final class of functional assessments is indi-
conducted in a controlled environment in which rect assessments. These methods are referred to
trained staff serve as therapist. The experiments as indirect because no direct observation or inter-
consist of a multielement design in which several action with the behavior of interest occurs.
conditions, including social positive reinforce- Instead information is obtained through a third
ment (attention condition), social negative rein- party such as a parent, caregiver, or teacher. These
forcement (demand/escape condition), and indirect methods have several benefits. First,
automatic reinforcement (alone condition) as because there is no direct contact with the indi-
well as a control condition (play), are presented vidual, these methods offer a less intrusive way to
in a random order (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, gain information about the problem behavior
& Richman, 1982; Iwata et al., 1994). Each of the (Floyd, Phaneuf, & Wilczynski, 2005). Secondly,
conditions may require several trials which can respondents can report about behaviors over a
lead to a relatively high number of total trails. longer period of time, which could be helpful for
Some studies report 30–50 trials for one child less frequent behaviors. This is opposed to the
(Derby et al., 2000) with one to eight trials occur- direct and EFA methods which only take into
ring per day (Rojahn, Whittaker, Hoch, & account the behaviors that are being exhibited in
Gonzalez, 2007). The behavioral responses in the moment. In addition, indirect methods may
these trials are then analyzed to hypothesize the be preferable when it may be unethical to recre-
maintaining function(s). When compared with ate the behavior in an analogue setting (O’Neill
the other methods of functional assessment, EFAs et al., 1997). However when EFAs are required,
offer a greater ability to infer causation about the indirect methods can be used previous to EFAs to
function of the behavior because of the con- limit the number of conditions that need to be
trolled, experimental nature of the design included and thereby making the EFA more
(Anderson, Freeman, & Scotti, 1999). However, efficient (Floyd et al.). While this class of mea-
while this method reaps the benefits of being able sures presents many benefits, there are several
to imply causality, ecological validity and gener- limitations. For example, the interviewer must
alizability are compromised due to the highly depend on the third party to report accurately
162 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

which can become problematic if there is over- or The remainder of this chapter reviews several
underreporting (Durand & Crimmins, 1990). of the informant-based functional assessment
Furthermore, as was the case with direct func- scales which are commonly used in the literature.
tional assessment methods, the cause of the func- Reviews include studies available on reliability,
tion cannot be determine and only correlational validity, factor structure, and other notable
information can be obtained (Hall, 2005). The research. The two most commonly used scales,
two more common types of indirect functional the MAS and QABF, are reviewed first, then other
assessments include interviews and rating scales. less commonly used and researched scales are
discussed. Finally, several studies are discussed
Functional Assessment Interviews which compare different scales and methods.
Functional assessment interviews collect informa-
tion on a number of variables that could affect the
targeted problematic behavior. For example, infor- Scaling Methods
mation is often collected on the topography of the
behavior, setting events, other events surrounding Motivation Assessment Scale
the behaviors, communication skills, previous (Durand, 1988)
treatment attempts, etc. One of the most com-
monly used and most comprehensive interviews is Description
the Functional Assessment Interview (FAI; The MAS is one of the more highly cited and
O’Neill et al., 1997). The FAI requires approxi- used informant-based functional assessment
mately 45–90 min to administer (Cunningham & scales available in the literature (Sigafoos, Kerr,
O’Neill, 2000). The open-ended questions in these & Roberts, 1994; Toogood & Timlin, 1996). This
interviews allow for further probing when addi- measure is composed of 16 items which are rated
tional information is needed. Unfortunately, these by the informant on a seven-point Likert scale.
types of interviews have not been studied thor- As with all informant-based measures, the
oughly and thus, their psychometric properties respondent should know the individual well and
can be questioned (Floyd et al., 2005). should also have experience with the behavior of
interest. The most common informants are teach-
Informant-Based Scales ers and parents, and these respondents should
The second type of indirect functional assess- answer questions based on the setting in which
ment is informant-based scales. These measures they know the individual best. The four subscales
typically have respondents answer questions which represent motivating factors/functions
regarding the functions of behavior on a Likert- were created based on face validity by the scale’s
type scale. Two of the most common functional developers, but have since been investigated sta-
assessment scales that are used include the tistically. The original four factors included
Questions About Behavior Function (QABF; escape from aversive events, gaining access to
Matson & Vollmer, 1995) and the Motivation social attention, gaining access to tangible
Assessment Scale (MAS; Durand, 1988). One of rewards, and sensory reinforcement. Items on
the main benefits of scales compared to the other each of the subscales are totaled and ranked.
functional assessment methods is its short admin-
istration time. This is important as functions of Reliability
behaviors can change over time (Bodfish, 2004; Inter-rater Reliability
Guess & Carr, 1991). Therefore, scaling methods The first examination of the reliability for the
are beneficial because they can easily be admin- MAS was conducted by Durand and Crimmins
istered at regular intervals or when the function (1988), the developers of the scale. In this study,
of the behavior is thought to have changed. In teachers of 50 children with DD who exhibited
addition, scales are easily scored and interpreted SIB were administered the MAS. In addition,
so that many professionals can be trained and uti- assistant teachers were administered the MAS to
lize them. obtain inter-rater reliability. The authors first
10 Scaling Methods 163

examined the correlations between informants on for the main function determined was only found
each item of the MAS. All items were reported to for 29.1% of participants. These percentages
be significant with Pearson correlations ranging were similar in both samples. The authors stated
from 0.66 to 0.92. Similarly, the authors looked that with these findings being so different from
at function categories and found all correlations Durand and Crimmins’ study that the utility of
to be significant at p < 0.001 (range 0.80–0.95). the MAS is brought into question. Zarcone and
Finally, the rank of functions was compared colleagues hypothesized several reasons for the
between respondents, meaning looking at if the discrepancies in findings. These reasons included
two respondents had a specific function, such as sample characteristics, ambiguity of items (e.g.,
escape, ranked first, then looking at the second, “Does it seem that he/she enjoys performing the
third, etc., ranks. Spearman rank-order correla- behavior?”), and training of informant (e.g., mas-
tions were all found to be significant at p < 0.001 ter’s level psychologist versus high-school edu-
with a range from 0.66 to 0.81. Based on the cation). Other characteristics of the behavior,
results from this sample, it can be concluded that such as behavior frequency, may also contribute
the MAS as good inter-rater reliability. to the differences in reliability found.
While the developers of the scale found inter- In addition to the study by Zarcone et al.
rater reliability to be good, other researchers have (1991), Sigafoos and colleagues (1994) found
not replicated these results. Zarcone, Rodgers, inter-rater reliability that differed from the origi-
Iwata, Rourke, and Dorsey (1991) found the mea- nal study for the MAS. Their sample was smaller
sure to be less reliable with their samples. In this and was composed of 18 adolescents and adults
study, two different samples were examined: one with severe to profound ID who also exhibited
composed of direct care staff for individuals with challenging behaviors. Using Pearson correla-
severe to profound ID who lived at a residential tions for the overall score on the measure, agree-
and another of teachers of students who attended ment between two raters ranged from −0.667 to
a school for those with autism and moderate to 0.722 with an overall correlation of 0.034.
severe ID who also exhibited severe behavior Correlations were significant for 12 of the 18
problems. Once again, second raters, either a sec- pairs of raters. Conversely, when examining indi-
ondary teacher or second direct care staff, were vidual items, none of the correlations were
obtained for each individual. The same correla- significant with the range from −0.337 to 0.425,
tions as in the Durand and Crimmins (1988) study and none of the correlations for each of the sub-
were calculated. Item-by-item correlations ranged scales was significant with a range from −0.093 to
from −0.30 to 0.81 (M = 0.27). Correlations for 0.168. Similar findings were gained when using
the mean scores of each function were −0.80 to percent agreement as opposed to the Pearson cor-
0.99 (M = 0.41), and when using Spearman rank relations. For 8 of the 18 rater pairs (44.4%),
order correlations the range was from −0.80 to agreement on the top-ranked function was
1.0 (M = 0.41). As can be seen by comparing achieved. Sigafoos et al. also bring into question
these correlations to the numbers above, there is the reliability and utility of the MAS, even though
certainly a discrepancy. In addition to correla- this measure is used often in applied settings.
tions, Zarcone et al. used percent agreement, both In 1996, Conroy, Fox, Bucklin, and Good
exact and adjacent, for each item. Exact agree- administered the MAS to 14 teachers of adoles-
ment was achieved when the Likert ranking on an cents and 6 caregivers for adult men in a residen-
item was identical across informants. On the tial setting all of whom had ID and a challenging
other hand, adjacent agreement, which is less behavior. Each person was rated by two people as
restrictive, was achieved if the ranking between in the previously discussed studies. The exact
informants was within one Likert rank. With the match agreement was calculated for each item
exact agreement method, agreement on items and was found to be 0.27 (range = 0.06–0.56).
ranged from 0 to 63% (M = 20%) and as expected Using the adjacent method, the mean percent
adjacent agreement was somewhat higher with a agreement was 0.56 (range = 0.25–0.78). The
range from 0 to 88% (M = 48%). Exact agreement reliability of the function identified was also
164 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

examined, but results were not particularly strong. Test–Retest Reliability


Overall, the correlation between informant-rated Some of the studies discussed above also exam-
functions was r = 0.66 and only 28% of the cor- ined test–retest reliability to examine the stability
relations between raters were significant. of MAS scores across time. The first study of this
The next study by Duker and Sigafoos (1998) type of reliability was by the developers of the
added to the previous literature on the MAS be test, Durand and Crimmins (1988). By adminis-
examining the reliability of the individual factors tering the MAS to the teachers 30 days apart, the
which make up the scale. In this sample, which researchers calculated the correlations between
consisted of caregivers of 86 individuals with ID items, functional categories, and also examined
who exhibited 90 challenging behaviors in all, the Spearman rank-order coefficients. Based on
Pearson correlations for items averaged 0.415 their sample, the item correlations ranged from
with a range from 0.225 to 0.62, which is similar 0.89 to 0.99, the function categories ranged from
to the previous three studies discussed. In regard 0.92 to 0.98, and the Spearman coefficient was
to the subscales, the Pearson correlations for items 0.82–0.89. All of these were found to be
were 0.510 for Sensory, 0.369 for Escape, 0.115 significant at the 0.001 level.
for Attention, and 0.494 for Tangible. All of these Other researchers have also examined test–
correlations were significant accept for the retest reliability over a 1-month interval. Conroy
Attention subscale. Percent agreements, exact and and colleagues (1996) administered the MAS
adjacent, were also calculated to examine inter- four times over a 1-month period. Percent agree-
rater reliability. For exact agreement, the average ment for items requiring an exact match had a
agreement was 0.31 (range from 0.23 to 0.40). As large range from 0.0 to 0.88 with a mean of 0.40.
expected, adjacent agreement was higher with the In addition, the adjacent agreement, or what the
average being 0.63 (range 0.54–0.71). Percent authors referred to as plus/minus-one agreement
agreement for the individual subscales was 0.16 was higher and ranged from 0.12 to 1.0 with a
for Sensory, 0.89 for Escape, 0.12 for Attention, mean of 0.76.
and 0.12 for Tangible when using the exact agree- In a similar study, Barton-Arwood, Wehby,
ment and 0.33 for Sensory, 0.30 for Escape, 0.25 Gunter, and Lane (2003) examined test–retest
for Attention, and 0.25 for Tangible. As can be reliability of the MAS using 10 teachers who
seen by the reported numbers on the subscales, rated 30 students with challenging behaviors at
certain factors seem to have greater inter-rater three different times: the first administration, 1
reliability. One final study to note was conducted week later, and 4 weeks after that. On the MAS
by Shogren and Rojahn (2003) which briefly over all three administrations, item-by-item cor-
examined several types of reliability for the MAS. relations were significant for 88% of items across
This study found inter-rater reliability to be good all three administrations. Seventy percent of
based on the guidelines from Cicchetti (1994) functions were stable from time 1 to time 2, 1
with a range from 0.35 to 0.73. week later, but this percentage dropped during
As can be seen from the studies reviewed, the the last administration. For the one-third of cases
inter-rater reliability of the MAS has been exam- which the function was not consistently deter-
ined quite extensively, but mixed results have mined, 63% of cases had the top-ranked function
been found. By far, the highest inter-rater reli- and second-ranked function switched.
ability was found by the developers of the mea- Additionally, the authors noted that the primary
sures who found reliability to be in the fair to function of behavior remained fairly consistent
excellent range based on Cicchetti (1994) even if item-by-item was not consistent which
definitions (Durand & Crimmins, 1988). may indicate that the item by item reliability is
However, when looking at the studies in total, it not as important. Also in 2003, Shogren and
is difficult to get a good picture of the inter-rater Rojahn (2003) also reported good test–retest reli-
reliability. ability of 0.71–0.89.
10 Scaling Methods 165

In sum, it seems that the MAS demonstrates the QABF, another informant-based functional
good test–retest reliability. However, it is impor- assessment measure. The agreement between
tant to remember that the function of a challeng- these two informant-based measures was even
ing behavior can fluctuate over time (Joyce, higher than what was found for the EFA method
2006). And thus, if assessments are not consis- with agreement in 61.5% of the cases.
tent over a period of time, it may be a product of
a newly developed behavioral function as opposed Factor Structure
to a flaw in the test. While the developers of the MAS created the
subscales of the MAS based on face validity,
Internal Consistency other researchers have examined statistically if
The final type of reliability which has been exam- these factors are appropriate. Among these stud-
ined for the MAS is internal consistency of the ies, there have been mixed results regarding the
items within each factor of the MAS. The first of factor structure of the MAS. The first of these
two studies found that the internal consistency to studies was conducted by Singh and colleagues
be 0.684 for Sensory, 0.738 for Escape, 0.759 for (1993) who used two separate samples. The first
Attention, and 0.867 for Tangible (Duker & included direct care staff of individuals with DD
Sigafoos, 1998). Shogren and Rojahn (2003) and high-frequency (>15 times per hour) SIB in
found comparable results with Cronbach’s alpha residential facilities, while the second sample
ranging from 0.80 to 0.96 on each of the sub- was teachers of students with mild to moderate
scales. Overall, the internal consistencies of the ID who exhibited lower rates (<15 times per
subscales which make up the MAS are fair to hour) of SIB. For the residential sample, a factor
excellent (Cicchetti, 1994). structure was found that was consistent with the
factors based on face validity. However, the
Validity authors found no meaningful factor structure for
While a larger amount of research exists regard- the sample of students, even though they exam-
ing the reliability of the MAS, less research has ined structures with three to five factors. The
examined the validity of this measure. The devel- authors cite the differences in frequency of the
opers of the MAS were the first to examine the challenging behavior, the level of ID, and train-
validity by comparing results of functional assess- ing of the informants as possible reasons that the
ments using the MAS to the results of experimen- results of the factor analyses differed. They con-
tal analyses (Durand & Crimmins, 1988). Using clude that the MAS may be a useful clinical tool
a sample of eight children with DD, five experi- for those with more severe ID who exhibit higher
mental conditions (i.e., baseline, attention, frequency problem behaviors as opposed to
escape, tangible, and unstructured) were con- higher functioning individuals with lower rates of
ducted and results were compared to those of the challenging behaviors.
MAS. The authors examined the correlation of The second attempt analyze the factors of the
ranks of certain functions and found that there MAS was by Bihm, Kienlen, Ness, and Poindexter
was high agreement between the two measures (1991). These researchers used principle axis fac-
(r = 0.99 and p < 0.001). toring analysis with varimax rotation to deter-
A second study by Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush, mine the factors of the MAS. Their findings more
Smalls, and Vollmer (2001) also examined the closely supported the original subscale proposed
convergent validity of the MAS with EFAs (as by the authors of the MAS. There were, however,
described by Iwata et al., 1982). The percent two items that different. The first “Does the
agreement between the two methods was 43.8%. behavior occur when you stop attending?” was
In addition to examining the agreement of the originally on the attention scale, but may be more
MAS with the EFA, the authors also investi- appropriate on the sensory scales. And the second
gated the agreement was between the MAS and item “Does the person seem to do the behavior to
166 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

upset or annoy you when you are getting him to unidimensional construct since previous studies
do something else?” might fit better with the on factor analysis were inconsistent. Their
attention scale though it was originally on the findings neither supported the unidimensional
escape subscale. In sum, Bihm and colleagues hypothesis or the four factors as described by
found support for the factors proposed by Durand Durand (1988). However, this study only exam-
and Crimmins (1988). ined the behaviors of stereotypies and other
A third study by Duker and Sigafoos (1998), repetitive behaviors. The authors suggest that
however, did not find statistical support for the these types of behaviors may be different from
four factors of the MAS. Using a sample of care- some of the other maladaptive behaviors such as
givers of individuals with mental retardation and aggression.
challenging behaviors, the MAS was adminis- As can be seen from the results above, there
tered and principal components analysis with has not been a consensus from research about the
varimax rotation was conducted. In addition to factors of the MAS. While a handful of studies
the previously conducted factor analysis, Duker have been conducted, researchers have not yet
and Sigafoos also divided behavior into three dif- suggested what to do regarding the measure and
ferent topographies, maladaptive, destructive, if the original factors should continue to be used
and disruptive, to see if the factors found differed. or if the factors should be revised somehow. This
For maladaptive, disruptive, and destructive would be helpful to increase the confidence that
behaviors, the four factors found explained 69.6, can be put behind the MAS.
83.5, and 76.4% of the variance, respectively.
However, while each of the behavior topogra-
phies indicated four factors, the individual items Questions About Behavior Function
that loaded onto each scale were not consistent (Matson & Vollmer, 1995)
with the original proposed factor structure.
Interestingly, these factor structure studies Description
used exploratory methods as opposed to The (QABF) is a functional assessment scale
confirmatory methods, even though factors for designed to aid in determining the function of
measure were already proposed. Therefore, challenging behaviors in those with DD by gain-
Kearney, Cook, Chapman, and Bensaheb (2006) ing information from informants, such as parents
chose to use confirmatory methods to see if the or caregivers. The 25-items are rated by infor-
face valid scales would be supported. Using a mants on a four-point Likert scale as Never,
sample of adults with severe to profound ID who Rarely, Some, or Often. Total administration time
exhibited moderate levels of maladaptive behav- is approximately 20 min and the measure is eas-
ior, the authors examined three indices for good- ily scored and interpreted. When interpreting
ness of fit which is recommended in the literature results, a clear function is considered an endorse-
(Kline, 2005). When examining all of the items, ment of four or five of the items endorsed within
the original factor structure found by Durand and one subscale with no other subscales containing
Crimmins (1988) was not supported by all of the significant endorsements. Since behaviors may
indices. However, the authors then deleted sev- have multiple or secondary functions, elevations
eral of the items which lowered the internal con- of one or more scales need to be interpreted
sistency (four out of seven of these items were in accordingly.
the sensory reinforcement function). Once sev-
eral items were deleted, the new CFA with the Reliability
select items did support the proposed factor Inter-rater Reliability
analysis. The first study which examined the inter-rater
In the most recent examination of the factor reliability of the QABF used a sample of 57
structure of the MAS, Joosten and Bundy (2008) adults with severe to profound ID and challeng-
examined if the MAS was actually measuring a ing behaviors (Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush, Smalls,
10 Scaling Methods 167

& Vollmer, 2000). Inter-rater reliability was subscale with no other subscales containing
found to be good with Spearman rank-order cor- significant endorsements), agreement was
relations for the primary function ranging from achieved on 63.6% of participants with aggres-
−0.095 to 1.0 with 52% of the items exceeding sion, and 50% of those exhibiting SIB. For behav-
0.80. Total percent agreement for each of the iors with multiple functions, reliability decreased
items ranged from 69.57 to 95.65% with 56% of as the rank of the function decreased, with the
the items exceeding 80% agreement. Finally, agreement ranging from 57.7% for the first ranked
Pearson product-moment for each subscale and function down to 0% for the fifth ranked func-
the total score between the two informants were tion. While the lower functions did not demon-
0.79–0.98, p < 0.01. strate good inter-rater reliability, this may be less
To replicate these results, Shogren and Rojahn important in relation to having good reliability
(2003) examined the reliability of the QABF with for the stronger maintaining functions.
a sample of 20 adults diagnosed with mental A second variable that has been explored with
retardation who required a behavior treatment the QABF is the effect of frequency of the
plan to target challenging behaviors such as challenging behavior. Matson and Wilkins
aggression, self-injury, and property destruction. (2009) examined how reliability varied with low-
The results demonstrated the inter-rater reliabil- and high-frequency behaviors. They defined
ity to be fair to good (r = 0.46–0.60) which was low-frequency behaviors as those that occurred
lower than what Paclawskyj and colleagues only on a monthly or weekly basis, while those
(2000) found. The authors cite the smaller sample that occurred daily or hourly were defined as
size as a possible reason for the difference in high frequency. For low-frequency behaviors,
findings. Spearman correlations ranged from 0.162 to 0.429
In another study, Nicholson, Konstantinidi, with four of the scales having significant correla-
and Furniss (2006) looked at inter-rater reliability tions (i.e., attention, nonsocial, physical, and tan-
for items, scales, and overall total with both exact gible) and for high-frequency behaviors Spearman
and adjacent agreement as was done with the correlations ranged from 0.069 to 0.477 and three
MAS. Exact agreement for items ranged from of the subscales reached significance (i.e., escape,
32.2 to 61.86% with a mean agreement of 43.22%. physical, and tangible). Overall, while both
As expected, adjacent agreement was higher with low- and high-frequency behaviors were reliabil-
a range from 69.49 to 84.75% with a mean of ity identified, high-frequency behaviors had
77.97%. Pearson correlations for each of the sub- higher reliability on average. This may be because
scales ranged from 0.421 to 0.623. informants are more familiar with the behavior
While the previous three studies examined since it happens more often and there have been
inter-rater reliability for the measure generally, more experiences to based responses on.
others have looked at other variables which may Based on the studies that have been conducted
affect psychometrics such as number of main- with the QABF, the measure seems to exhibit
taining variables and low- versus high-frequency adequate inter-rater reliability. Furthermore, the
behaviors. Matson and Boisjoli (2007) examined findings seem to more consistent and favorable
inter-rater reliability for behaviors that had more when compared with the inter-rater reliability of
than one function. Agreement was achieved when the MAS. It should be noted that the samples
both raters ranked the functions of the same from these studies have been limited to primarily
severity. For example, if rater 1 found attention to caregivers of adults with ID; thus, it would be
be the strongest maintaining variable and tangible useful to expand the samples examined to see if
as the second, then to be considered agreement the inter-rater reliability remains strong.
rater 2 would have to have both function severi-
ties ranked the same as well. For those challeng- Test–Retest Reliability
ing behaviors only maintained by one variable Several studies have also examined the test–retest
(i.e., four or five of the items endorsed within one reliability of the QABF. In the first study by
168 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

Paclawskyj and colleagues (2000), the QABF function, environmental enrichment, and social
was administered to the caregiver two times with skills training would be included in the treatment
an interval of 3 weeks. Spearman rank-order cor- plan. These functional assessment-based plans
relations were high with 76% of the item statis- were compared to general plans which used tech-
tics exceeding 0.80. Total percent agreement was niques such as blocking, redirecting and inter-
also good with 96% of items exceeding 80% rupting, not based on the outcome of the QABF.
agreement. The only other study noted to exam- For the behavior examined, SIB, aggression, and
ine test–retest reliability also found test–retest stereotypies, significant decreases were seen
reliability to be good with Pearson-r for the sub- when using the function-based treatment plan.
scales ranging from 0.62 to 0.93 (Shogren & For example, using the QABF-based plan, SIB
Rojahn, 2003). decreased 66%, whereas it only decreased by
21% with the generic plan. These findings speak
Internal Consistency to the validity and utility of the QABF, especially
Three studies have reported investigated the in applied setting in which the findings from the
internal consistency of the QABF. The first by functional assessment would be used to deter-
Paclawskyj and colleagues (2000) found high mine a treatment plan.
internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha on In a second study, Pacwalskyj and colleagues
the subscales ranging from 0.900 to 0.928. The (2001) examined the convergent validity of the
second study, while still finding good internal QABF with EFA (Iwata et al., 1982) just as they
consistency, did not find coefficient alphas quite had compared to the MAS to EFA. The QABF
as high (Shogren & Rojahn, 2003). The authors and experimental method showed an agreement
point out that for the subscales that have equiva- on the maintaining function 56.3% of the time.
lents on the MAS, the alphas ranged from 0.82 to This was slightly higher than what was found
0.88; however, for the physical subscale which when examining the MAS and EFA. In addition
has no MAS equivalent the Cronbach’s alpha was to examining the agreement of the QABF with
only 0.24. the EFA, the authors also investigated the agree-
The last study by Nicholson and colleagues ment between the QABF and MAS. Sixty-one
(2006) replicated previous results, and also took percent of the time the MAS and QABF exhib-
their examination of internal consistency a step ited agreement which was an improvement when
further. Similar to previous findings, the alphas compared with their agreements with the EFA
for the subscales ranged from 0.785 to 0.922. method.
Next the authors looked at internal consistency in
regards to how it varied with high- and low-fre- Factor Structure
quency behaviors as well as different behavioral Matson and colleagues (1996) presented data
topographies such as disruptive, maladaptive, and from exploratory factor analysis which used a
destructive behaviors. Internal consistency sample of 462 individuals, ages 13–86 years with
remained high regardless of these variables. ID who exhibited the challenging behaviors of
SIB, aggression, or property destruction. These
Validity data, which were later published by Paclawskyj
While the QABF has been shown to have good and colleagues (2000), supported five factors to
reliability, in order for it to be a strong measure, the measure including Attention, Escape,
its validity must also be established. Matson, Physical, Tangible, and Nonsocial. These factors
Bamburg, Cherry, and Paclawskyj (1999) exam- were found to explain 74.5% of the variance.
ined the predictive validity of the QABF by More recently, Nicholson and colleagues (2006)
examining outcomes of functional based treat- replicated factor analysis on the severity scores
ment plans which used the QABF to determine of the QABF using a younger sample (N = 40)
the function. For example, if the behavior was with ID who exhibited a total of 118 challenging
determined to be maintained by a nonsocial behaviors. Their finding also highly supported
10 Scaling Methods 169

the original factors proposed by the authors of the such as attention, while the other behaviors served
measure. The first four factors were commensu- more of a nonsocial function. Other studies have
rate with the physical, attention, tangible, and revealed similar trends in the maintaining func-
escape subscales. The fifth factor found included tions of certain challenging behaviors. Matson
four of the five nonsocial items, but the last of and colleagues (1999) found that with the QABF
these nonsocial items (i.e., engages in the behav- as the tool for functional assessment, SIB was
ior in a highly repetitive manner, ignoring his/her most often the function of nonsocial and also
surroundings) loaded onto a separate sixth factor. escape. Aggression most often served the func-
Overall, their factor structure explained 73% of tion of gaining access to attention and escape from
the variance. A last study by Singh and colleagues demands, while stereotypies were once again
(2009) also replicated the factor analysis of the found to serve a nonsocial function. Matson
QABF when the attempted to shorten the mea- and Boisjoli (2007) also found aggression was
sure (to be discussed later). Their findings were most often maintained by escape and SIB by
similar to the previous studies in which five fac- nonsocial.
tors were found which explained 73.9% of the Based on these results, there are implications
total variance. With these three studies, there is for treatment. While each individual needs a
good evidence that the five factors of the QABF functional assessment, in certain circumstances
are supported statistically as well as rationally. knowing the typical functions of challenging
behaviors could aid in developing a treatment
Profiles of Challenging Behaviors plan. In addition, prevention strategies may want
While there is a wide range of challenging behav- to be included for certain behaviors even if that
iors that can be investigated using functional particular function was not found to be the pri-
assessment, certain behaviors are fairly common mary-maintaining function (Tarbox et al., 2009).
in the literature, such as physical aggression, SIB, For example, even though aggression may be
and stereotypies (Poppes, van der Putten, & found to be maintained by gaining access to a
Vlaskamp, 2010). Because some of these behav- tangible, it may be worthwhile to include preven-
iors are common, especially in those with ID or tion strategies that target attention. This may
DD, researchers have begun to study if certain decrease the chances that this behavior would
functions are more likely to maintain certain later be maintained by attention because the per-
behaviors. That is, are there common functional son could be getting attention through appropri-
profiles for specific problematic behaviors? ate means.
Several studies have examined the results of
QABF to look for trends in functions. One such Multiple Functions
study by Dawson, Matson, and Cherry (1998) The QABF has been shown to be able to clearly
examined the three challenging behaviors of SIB, identify at least one function in 84% of cases
aggression, and stereotypies to determine if cer- (Matson et al., 1999). However, as mentioned
tain functions were more commonly identified previously, some challenging behaviors are main-
based on behavior topography. The findings sup- tained by more than one function (Matson &
ported possible function profiles for certain behav- Boisjoli, 2007). Matson and Boisjoli, using the
iors. For example, aggression was most likely QABF, discovered that multiple functions of
maintained by attention, while stereotypies were behavior are fairly common with approximately
more likely maintained by a nonsocial function. half of the behaviors examined being maintained
SIB was not supported by a particular function. In by multiple functions. Multiple functions were
a similar study, Applegate, Matson, and Cherry defined by having a severity of score that was
(1999) examined the behaviors of SIB, stereotyp- greater than six. Twenty-two percent were main-
ies, aggression, pica, or rumination in adults with tained by two functions, 18% by three functions,
ID. In this case, aggression was once again found 7% by four functions, and 7% met criteria
to be maintained by more environmental variables for being maintained by all five functions.
170 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

These findings have implications for treatment Results showed the correlations ranged from
planning specifically, so that all maintain func- 0.098 to 0.836, p < 0.01, for each of the subscales.
tions are being addressed properly. However, as Finally, inter-rater reliabilities for each of the
noted in the reliability section for the QABF, the subscales were all significant at the 0.01 level
reliability decreases for the lower-rank functions with correlations of 0.932 for attention, 0.933 for
and this should be considered when examining escape, 0.955 for nonsocial, 0.927 for physical,
lower-rank functions (Matson & Boisjoli). and 0.815 for tangible. Based on this primary
study of the QABF-SF, it seems that this measure
would be useful as a functional assessment
Questions About Behavior measure; however, to further prove the utility of
Function—Short Form this measure, validity studies need to also be
(Singh et al., 2009) conducted.

The original QABF consisted of 25 items.


However, if the number of items was decreased, Questions About Behavior
the efficiency of the functional assessment would Function—Mental Illness
be increased. Singh and colleagues (2009) con- (Singh et al., 2006)
ducted exploratory and confirmatory factor anal-
ysis to determine if the full QABF could be While the original QABF was developed for
shortened to create a briefer measure which still those with developmental disabilities, this mea-
possessed adequate psychometrics. First, the sure has been extended for those with severe
authors attempted to replicate the factor structure mental illness as well as challenging behaviors
that Paclawskyj and colleagues (2000) found for are also observed with this population (Serper
the full-length measure. Using exploratory factor et al., 2005). Thus far, limited information is
analysis with varimax rotation, the same five fac- available on this measure. However, Singh et al.
tors (i.e., Attention, Escape, Physical, Tangible, (2006) did examine the factors of the newly
and Nonsocial) were found which explained adapted measure by administering the Questions
73.9% of the total item variance. Secondly, to About Behavior Function—Mental Illness
determine which items should be retained for the (QABF-MI) to 135 adults with serious mental ill-
shortened version of the QABF, a priori calcula- ness from three inpatient psychiatric hospitals.
tions of reliability and validity (see Singh et al., Their finding supported the factor structure which
2009 for full description of criteria to maintain or was found by Paclawskyj et al. (2000). Across the
eliminate items) were examined. Based on these five factors of physical discomfort, social atten-
calculations, ten items from the original measure tion, tangible reinforcement, escape, and nonso-
were eliminated. Thus, the new Questions About cial, inter-rater reliability ranged from 0.96 to
Behavior Function—Short Form (QABF-SF) 0.98, and Pearson r test–retest reliability ranged
consisted of 15 items. A second factor analysis from 0.86 to 0.99. Coefficient alpha suggested
was used to confirm that the factor structure of acceptable internal consistency ranging from 0.84
the original measure was retained for the new ver- to 0.92 for the target behaviors of aggression and
sion. Results showed that the five factors in the property destruction. The authors also pointed
original full-length QABF were also found in the out that while analogue functional analyses have
QABF-SF. The authors also examined the reli- considerable utility for persons with developmen-
ability of the QABF-SF. Cronbach’s alpha showed tal disabilities, it would not be possible to use this
high-internal consistency for each of the sub- method for individuals with normal intellectual
scales: 0.92 for attention, 0.91 for escape, 0.84 functioning and mental illness on practical
for nonsocial, 0.94 for physical, and 0.80 for tan- grounds. Thus, a measure such as the QABF-MI
gible. To examine test–retest reliability, Pearson would be beneficial for this population so that
product moment correlations were examined. functions of challenging behaviors can be
10 Scaling Methods 171

identified and appropriate treatments can be put significant item-by-item correlations across all
in place. Additionally, by using functional assess- three administrations of the PBQ, only 20% were
ment for challenging behaviors in a typical popu- significant. In addition to item-by-item consis-
lation, environmental causes could be identified tency, the consistency of the function identified
which could reduce the chance of unwarranted was also explored. When examining exact agree-
psychotropic medications. ment, the functions remained stable 59–81% of
the time across administration periods. In 50% of
cases when the function was not stable, the first
Problem Behavior Questionnaire and second ranked function switched some time
during the three administrations. The authors
While some of the other scaling methods dis- noted that while item-by-item consistent may not
cussed in this chapter were created for those with have been high, the primary function of behav-
more severe behavior problems and impairments, iors remained fairly over time which may be
the Problem Behavior Questionnaire (PBQ) was more meaningful.
developed out of a need to functional-based
assessment for those with milder behavior prob-
lems in a general education setting (Barton- Functional Assessment for Multiple
Arwood et al., 2003). The PBQ consists of 15 CausaliTy (Matson et al., 2003)
items which describe situations in which the
behavior is likely to occur. For example, “Does The purpose of the Functional Assessment for
the problem behavior occur in the presence of Multiple CausaliTy (FACT) is to determine the
specific peers?” These items are then rated based hierarchy of functions for challenging behaviors
on how often each situation occurs by the infor- that have multiple functions. Researchers have
mant on a seven-point Likert scale with the noted that approximately up to half of challeng-
anchors of never, 10, 25, 50, 75, 90%, or always. ing behaviors may serve multiple functions
Within the 15 items, there are five main functions (Matson & Boisjoli, 2007). Therefore, in some
being addressed: access to peer attention, access cases, it would be beneficial to determine which
to teacher attention, escape from peer attention, function is more prominent to aid in treatment
escape from teacher attention, and setting events. development. While other measures rank func-
Those items with a score of three or above (i.e., tions, the FACT more directly tests the different
occurring 50% or more of the time) are consid- functions against one another.
ered as a potential hypothesis for problem behav- The FACT is composed of 35 items that are
ior and those functions with two or more items presented in a forced-choice format in which two
scored at a three or above are considered to be possible functions are presented and the respon-
primary hypotheses. dent must decide which is more applicable to the
In terms of psychometrics, only one study was individual. While the original measure was com-
found which examined reliability of this measure posed of 50 items each with three possible
(Barton-Arwood et al., 2003). In this study, which responses, due to the lengthy time to administer
also examined the MAS, there were 10 teachers the measure, some items were eliminated and
who rated 30 students’ behaviors across three only two response options were used to result in
separate times: the first administration, 1 week a 35-item measure. An example item from the
later, and 4 weeks after that. The results showed FACT is “Engages in the behavior more (A) to
that the percent of significant item-by-item cor- get attention, or more (P) because he/she is
relations between any of the two administration in pain, or (N) neither?” Based on a factor analy-
periods ranged from 47% to 74%. As might be sis which was conducted, a five-factor model
expected, the lowest amount of significant corre- was suggested (Matson et al., 2003). The five
lations occurred from the first and third adminis- factors found were consistent with the five sub-
tration periods. When examining the percent of scales of the QABF: Tangible, Physical, Attention,
172 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

Escape, and Nonsocial. In regard to reliability,


the internal consistency was computed with the
Comparison of Functional
Kuder-Richardson formula 20 (KR-20). All sub-
Assessment Methods
scales of the FACT yielded good to high esti-
With such a variety of methods available to con-
mates of reliability (0.88–0.92). At this time, no
duct functional assessments, the question arises
other psychometric studies have been completed.
as to which of these methods may be the best or
More research is needed to determine the validity
even if these methods are comparable. While the
and general utility of the FACT.
strengths and weaknesses of these different meth-
ods, including ABC charts, scatter plots, EFAs,
interviews, and rating scales, were reviewed at
Motivation Analysis Rating Scale the beginning of this chapter, some researchers
(Wieseler, Hanson, Chamberlain, have compared methods in more controlled
& Thompson, 1985) settings.
One study conducted by Lerman and Iwata
The Motivation Analysis Rating Scale (MARS), (1993) compared experimental and descriptive
also referred to as the Contingency Analysis methods of functional assessment in six adults
Questionnaire (CAQ) was one of the first func- with ID, all of whom exhibited the challenging
tional assessment scales developed. The measure behavior of SIB. Descriptive analysis used par-
assessed the following functions of behavior: tial-interval recording in which the occurrence of
social and tangible positive reinforcement, social the behavior, as well as subject and staff responses
and situational escape, and self-stimulation. The and other environmental factors were recorded in
six items that make up the scale are answered on 10-s intervals. EFA methods were similar to those
a five-point Likert scale ranging from “never” to in Iwata et al. (1982). On five of six trials, results
“almost always.” While this measure was one of on the functions obtained using descriptive meth-
the first to be developed, little research has been ods were not consistent with those using the
conducted to investigate its psychometric proper- experimental methods. The authors concluded
ties. As a result, little can be said about the utility that due to the correlational nature of descriptive
of this measure. methods that these methods are not sufficient and
may not be necessary in determining the function
of challenging behaviors.
Functional Analysis Screening While Lerman and Iwata (1993) only exam-
Tool (Iwata & DeLeon, 1995) ined experimental and descriptive methods, sev-
eral other studies have also included scaling
The Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST) methods to compare results. One of the first was
is a 27-item informant-based functional assess- by Crawford, Brockel, Schauss, and Miltenberger
ment measure. This measure is composed of three (1992). In their study, the authors first completed
factors: (1) social influences on behavior, (2) the MAS, then the EFA followed by the ABC
social reinforcement, and (3) nonsocial (auto- charts for two men and two women with severe to
matic) reinforcement. Once again there has been profound ID who exhibited high rates of stereo-
very limited research on this measure in terms of typic behaviors. The MAS and ABC charts were
psychometric properties. The only information conducted with both home and vocational staff.
found reported that the inter-rater reliability was However, there were some discrepancies between
found to be 67%. As a result of the limited these informants which may have been due to
research on this measure, it is not reportedly behaviors actually differing in various settings.
used in the literature and requires more investi- The findings showed that the MAS reliability
gation into its psychometrics if it is going to identified the function of behaviors as being sen-
be used. sory most often. The next assessment, the EFA,
10 Scaling Methods 173

reportedly obtained inconsistent results. On the (Hall, 2005). In this study, the sample consisted
other hand, of all three assessment methods, the of four individuals with severe to profound ID
ABC chart most clearly determined the function and who also exhibited a challenging behavior.
of the challenging behaviors to be sensory. The The three methods examined were descriptive
authors note that the ABC charting and MAS are methods (i.e., time-based lag sequential analysis
most useful in applied settings due to their rela- (Sackett, 1987), Iwata and colleagues (1994)
tive accuracy and brevity. In addition, in analyz- experimental methods, and the QABF (Matson &
ing the structure of the study, one should consider Vollmer, 1995). Each of these methods was
that the ABC chart was completed after the EFA conducted for the sample and their challeng-
was conducted. This is a concern as some ing behavior so that agreement rates on the
researchers have noted that EFAs can change and determined function of the behavior could be
create new functions to challenging behaviors compared. Based on the authors’ findings, exper-
(Matson & Minshawi, 2007). imental and informant-based assessments were
In a similar study, Toogood and Timlin (1996) concordant for three of four participants, whereas
examined 92 individuals with severe ID and chal- the descriptive assessment was only concordant
lenging behaviors. The methods compared in this with the experimental results in one of four trials.
study included informant-based interview, infor- Furthermore, descriptive methods took approxi-
mant-based rating scale (MAS), ABC chart, scat- mately 10 h, the experimental method took 2 h,
ter plot analysis, and EFA. It should be noted that and while the QAFB took 15 min. Thus, the time
two versions of the MAS were used. The first was benefits of certain methods are evident. Clearly, a
the original version and the second had four addi- limitation of this study is the very small sample
tional items added to differentiate between social size which makes it difficult to put forth strong
avoidance and task avoidance. Authors examined conclusions based on these results.
the percent of behaviors that were ascribed func- One study by Paclawskyj et al. (2001) com-
tions. The method that predicted clear functions pared the QABF and MAS more directly by
the most often was clinical interview, followed administering both measures and also conducting
by the MAS (both versions), scatter plot analysis, an EFA. Between the three types of functional
EFA, and ABC charts. The agreement rate over- assessment, the QABF and MAS demonstrated
all five methods was extremely low at only 2.5% the greatest concordance rate by identifying the
(i.e., 3 of the 121 behaviors assessed). When same function 61.5% of the time. This would
including all assessed behaviors (i.e., including make sense since both measures use the same
those behaviors that no function was determined), format of asking informants to provide informa-
agreement rates between any two methods ranged tion about the behavior and each is composed of
from 10 to 62%. When only behaviors for which similar subscales. However, when compared with
a function was determined was included, agree- the EFA, the QABF had greater agreement than
ment rates rose to 44–89%. In either case, the the MAS with agreement rates being 56.3 and
methods which resulted in the highest agreement 43.8%, respectively. However, as can be seen by
were between MAS and interview. When exam- the percentage, the QABF and MAS seem to be
ining these results, the reader should note that the quite comparable.
function rank was not examined, and thus, behav- The next more recent study by Tarbox and col-
iors which identified multiple functions, it did not leagues (2009) compared different functional
matter if the function was first or second ranked. assessment methods for seven children with
If the level of agreement had been considered, autism diagnoses. The three methods examined
these rates would have been much lower. included ABC data and specific coding for ante-
While Toogood and Timlin (1996) found the cedents and consequences as the descriptive
MAS to have good concordance with informant- method, the QABF as indirect method, and Iwata
based interview, the QABF has been shown and colleagues’ (1982) EFA method as the
to have good agreement with EFA methods analogue method. Once again, each method was
174 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

conducted for each child’s challenging behavior determined. For example, one may begin with a
so that concordance rates could be obtained. The scaling method, such as the QABF or MAS,
results indicated that the QABF (indirect method) because these methods are time and cost efficient.
and EFA (experimental method) had the greatest At this point, no further assessment may be
concordance rates. Exact agreement was achieved needed. However, if it was felt necessary, other
for three of seven participants, and partial agree- more time-consuming methods, such as ABC
ment (at least one of the same functions were charts or EFA, could be used to glean additional
concordant) was achieved on six of the seven par- information.
ticipants. On the other hand, partial and/or exact
agreement was only obtained for two of seven
between indirect and descriptive and one of seven General Conclusions
between descriptive and EFA. Interestingly, when
examining the concordance across all three meth- When working with individuals with challenging
ods, exact agreement was only found for one of behaviors, specifically those individuals with ID
the seven cases examined. While this study used or DD, completing a functional assessment for
a small sample size, it highlights the discrepan- challenging behaviors is of the utmost impor-
cies in results across different functional assess- tance to develop appropriate behavior treatment
ment methods. In the discussion, the authors state plans. As can be seen from the review above,
that the QABF identified all of the same func- there are several methods of functional assess-
tions as the EFA except on one occasion, but in ment which each offer their own strengths and
addition the QABF also identified some functions weaknesses. It can be argued that each of these
of behavior that were not found with EFA. The methods has its place in certain situations.
authors state that the QABF found “false posi- Scaling methods are beneficial because a
tives.” However, this statement assumes that the larger amount of information can be gathered
EFA is correct in its functions identified and that from caregivers or direct care staff in a short
the QABF is identifying functions that are not amount of time. These measures are also easy to
valid. However, one could also argue that the score and interpret. This allows for the readminis-
QABF is more sensitive because it samples a tration of the measure over periods of time which
large range of behavior that the EFA was actually is important as new behaviors may develop or
missing secondary functions of the challenging functions of behaviors may change. While there
behavior. In either case, the authors state that it are a handful of scaling measures that have been
may not be detrimental to obtain false positives created, many have not been thoroughly research.
on the assessment. If functions were identified However, the QABF and MAS have been investi-
that are not actually maintaining the challenging gated thoroughly enough to state that they have
behavior, then the treatment plan would have adequate psychometric properties and should be
extra prevention and intervention strategies. This top choices for applied settings. These scales,
would be preferable to a functional assessment along with other forms of functional assessment,
which did not detect a maintaining function and should be used often to ensure the best treatment
resulted in an inadequate treatment plan that of individuals with challenging behaviors.
could worsen behaviors.
When examining the studies above, one can
see that there is not always good agreement References
between different methods of functional assess-
ment. However, it is also important to remember Anderson, C. M., Freeman, K. A., & Scotti, S. R. (1999).
that each method has its own strengths and weak- Evaluation of the generalizability (reliability and
nesses, and these should be considered when validity) of analog functional assessment methodol-
ogy. Behavior Therapy, 30, 31–50.
choosing a method. In many cases, a clinician
Applegate, H., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. (1999). An
may want to progress from one method to another evaluation of functional variables affecting severe
until they feel an accurate function has been problem behaviors in adults with mental retardation
10 Scaling Methods 175

using the Questions About Behavioral Function Scale DuPaul, G. J., & Ervin, R. A. (1996). Functional assess-
(QABF). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, ment of behavior related to attention deficit/hyperac-
229–238. tivity disorder: Linking assessment to intervention
Barton-Arwood, S. M., Wehby, J. H., Gunter, P. L., & Lane, design. Behavior Therapy, 27, 601–622.
K. L. (2003). Functional behavior assessment rating Durand, V. M. (1988). The motivation assessment scale.
scales: Intrarater reliability with students with emo- In M. Hersen & A. Bellack (Eds.), Dictionary of
tional or behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders, behavioral assessment techniques (pp. 309–310).
28, 386–400. Tarrytown: Pergamon.
Bihm, E. M., Kienlen, T. L., Ness, M. E., & Poindexter, A. Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988). Identifying the
R. (1991). Factor structure of the Motivation variables maintaining self-injurious behavior. Journal
Assessment Scale for persons with mental retardation. of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 17, 17–28.
Psychological Reports, 68, 1235–1238. Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. (1990). Chapter 3
Bodfish, J. W. (2004). Treating the core features of autism: Assessment. In V. M. Durand (Ed.), Severe behaviour
Are we there yet? Mental Retardation and problems: A functional communication training
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, approach. New York: Guilford.
318–326. Emerson, E., & Bromley, J. (1995). The form and function
Bosma, A., & Mulick, J. A. (1990). Brief report: of challenging behaviours. Journal of Intellectual
Ecobehavioral assessment using transparent scatter Disability Research, 39, 388–398.
plots. Behavioral Residential Treatment, 5, 137–140. Floyd, R. G., Phaneuf, R. L., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2005).
Brestan, E. V., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psycho- Measurement properties of indirect assessment
social treatments of conduct disordered children and methods for functional behavioral assessment: A review
adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids. of research. School Psychology Review, 34, 58–73.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 180–189. Gettinger, M., & Callan Stoiber, K. (2006). Functional
Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of assessment, collaboration, and evidence-based treat-
thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assess- ment: Analysis of a team approach for addressing
ment instruments in psychology. Psychological challenging behaviors in young children. Journal of
Assessment, 6, 284–290. School Psychology, 44, 231–252.
Conroy, M. A., Davis, C. A., Fox, J. J., & Brown, W. H. Guess, D., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Emergence and mainte-
(2002). Functional assessment of behavior and effec- nance of stereotypy and self-injury. American Journal
tive supports for young children with challenging on Mental Retardation, 96, 299–319.
behaviors. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 27, Hall, S. S. (2005). Comparing descriptive, experimental and
35–47. informant-based assessments of problem behaviors.
Conroy, M. A., Fox, J. J., Bucklin, A., & Good, W. (1996). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 514–526.
An analysis of the reliability and stability of the Hile, M. G., & Desrochers, M. N. (1993). The relationship
Motivation Assessment Scale in assessing the chal- between functional assessment and treatment selec-
lenging behaviors of persons with developmental dis- tion for aggressive behaviors. Research in
abilities. Education & Training in Mental Retardation Developmental Disabilities, 14, 265–274.
& Developmental Disabilities, 31, 243–250. Iwata, B. A. & DeLeon, I. G. (1995). The functional anal-
Crawford, J., Brockel, B., Schauss, S., & Miltenberger, R. ysis screening tool (FAST). Unpublished manuscript,
G. (1992). A comparison of methods for the functional Gainesville: University of Florida.
assessment of stereotypic behavior in persons with Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. E., Bauman, K. E.,
mental retardation. Journal of the Association for & Richman, G. S. (1982). Towards a functional analy-
Persons with Severe Handicaps, 17, 77–86. sis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in
Cunningham, E., & O’Neill, R. (2000). Comparison of Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20.
results of functional assessment and analysis methods Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
with young children with autism. Education and Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R., et al. (1994). The functions
Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-epidemio-
Disabilities, 35, 406–414. logical analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Dawson, J. E., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. (1998). An 27, 215–240.
analysis of maladaptive behaviors in persons with Joosten, A. V., & Bundy, A. C. (2008). The motivation of
autism, PDD-NOS, and mental retardation. Research stereotypic and repetitive behavior: Examination of
in Developmental Disabilities, 19, 439–448. construct validity of the Motivation Assessment Scale.
Derby, K. M., Hagopian, L., Fisher, W. W., Richman, D., Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38,
Augustine, M., Fahs, A., et al. (2000). Functional anal- 1341–1348.
ysis of aberrant behavior through measurement of Joyce, T. (2006). Functional assessment and challenging
separate response topographies. Journal of Applied behaviour. Psychiatry, 5, 312–315.
Behavior Analysis, 33, 113–117. Kearney, C. A., Cook, L. C., Chapman, G., & Bensaheb,
Duker, P. C., & Sigafoos, J. (1998). The Motivation A. (2006). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analy-
Assessment Scale: Reliability and construct validity ses of the Motivation Assessment Scale and Resident
across three topographies of behaviour. Research in Choice Assessment Scale. Journal of Developmental
Developmental Disabilities, 19, 131–141. and Physical Disabilities, 18, 1–11.
176 M. Sipes and J.L. Matson

Kiernan, C., Reeves, D., Hatton, C., Alborz, A., Emerson, high rate challenging behaviors in adults with intellectual
E., Mason, H., et al. (1997). The HARC challenging disability. Behavior Modification, 33, 207–219.
behavior project report 1: Persistence and change in Murphy, G., Beadle-Brown, J., Wing, L., Gould, J., Shah,
the challenging behavior of people with a learning A., & Holmes, N. (2005). Chronicity of challenging
disability. Manchester: Hester Adrian Research behaviors in people with severe intellectual disabilities
Centre, University of Manchester. and/or autism: A total population sample. Journal of
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 405–418.
equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. Nicholson, J., Konstantinidi, E., & Furniss, F. (2006). On
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and some psychometric properties of the Questions About
experimental analyses of variables maintaining self Behavior Function (QABF). Research in Developmental
injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Disabilities, 27, 337–352.
Analysis, 26, 293–319. O’Neill, R. E., Homer, R. H., Ablin, R. W., Sprague, J. R.,
Lowe, K., Allen, D., Jones, E., Brophy, S., Moore, K., & Storey, K., & Newton, J. S. (1997). Functional assess-
James, W. (2007). Challenging behaviours: Prevalence ment and program development for problem behav-
and topographies. Journal of Intellectual Disability iors: A practical handbook. New York: Brooks/Cole.
Research, 51, 625–636. Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.,
Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj, & Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about Behavioral
T. R. (1999). A validity study on the Questions About Function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func-
Behavioral Function (QABF) Scale: Predicting treat- tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in
ment success for self-injury, aggression, and stereo- Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229.
typies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.,
163–176. & Vollmer, T. R. (2001). Assessment of the convergent
Matson, J. L., & Boisjoli, J. (2007). Multiple versus single validity of the Questions about Behavioral Function
maintaining factors of challenging behaviors as Scale with analogue functional analysis and the moti-
assessed by the QABF for adults with intellectual dis- vation assessment scale. Journal of Intellectual
ability (ID). Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability Research, 45, 484–494.
Disability, 32, 39–44. Poppes, P., van der Putten, A. J. J., & Vlaskamp, C. (2010).
Matson, J. L., Cooper, C., Malone, C. J., & Moskow, S. L. Frequency and severity of challenging behaviour in
(2008). The relationship of self-injurious behaviors people with profound intellectual and multiple dis-
and other maladaptive behaviors among individuals abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31,
with severe and profound intellectual disability. 1269–1275.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 141–148. Rojahn, J., Whittaker, K., Hoch, T. A., & Gonzalez, M. L.
Matson, J. L., Kiely, S. L., & Bamburg, J. W. (1997). The (2007). Assessment of self-injurious and aggressive
effect of stereotypies on adaptive skills as assessed behavior. International Review of Research in Mental
with the DASH-II and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Retardation, 34, 281–319.
Scales. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, Sackett, G. P. (1987). Analysis of sequential social inter-
471–476. action data: Some issues, recent developments, and a
Matson, J. L., Kuhn, D. E., Dixon, D. R., Mayville, S. B., causal inference model. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.),
Laud, R. B., Cooper, C. L., et al. (2003). The develop- Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 855–
ment and factor structure of the functional assessment 878). New York: Wiley.
for multiple causaliTy (FACT). Research in Serper, M. R., Goldberg, B. R., Herman, K. G., Richarme,
Developmental Disabilities, 24, 485–495. D., Chou, J., Dill, C. A., et al. (2005). Predictors of
Matson, J. L., & Minshawi, N. F. (2007). Functional aggression on the psychiatric inpatient service.
assessment of challenging behavior: Toward a strategy Comprehensive Psychiatry, 46, 121–127.
for applied settings. Research in Developmental Shogren, K. A., & Rojahn, J. (2003). Convergent reliabil-
Disabilities, 28, 353–361. ity and validity of the questions about behavioral func-
Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. (1995). Questions About tion and the motivation assessment scale: A replication
Behavioral Function (QABF). Baton Rouge: Disability study. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Consultants, LLC.. Translated into Italian, German, Disabilities, 15, 367–375.
and Korean. Sigafoos, J., Kerr, M., & Roberts, D. (1994). Inter-rater
Matson, J. L., Vollmer, T. R., Paclawskyj, T. R., Smiroldo, reliability of the Motivation Assessment Scale: Failure
B. B., Applegate, H. R., & Stallings, S. (1996). to replicate with aggressive behavior. Research in
Questions About Behavioral Function (QABF): An Developmental Disabilities, 15, 333–342.
instrument to assess functional properties of problem Singh, N. N., Donatelli, L. S., Best, A., Williams, D. E.,
behaviors. Poster presented at the 22nd Annual Barrera, F. J., Lenz, M. W., et al. (1993). Factor struc-
Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, ture of the Motivation Assessment Scale. Journal of
San Francisco, CA. Intellectual Disability Research, 37, 65–74.
Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Factors associated with Singh, N. N., Matson, J. L., Lancing, G. E., Singh, A. N.,
the QABF for functional assessment of low rate and Adkins, A. D., McKeegan, G. F., et al. (2006). Questions
10 Scaling Methods 177

About Behavior Function in Mental Illness (QABF-MI): informant-based, experimental and descriptive
A behavior checklist for functional assessment of mal- methods. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual
adaptive behavior exhibited by individuals with mental Disabilities, 9, 206–222.
illness. Behavior Modification, 30, 739–751. Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N.
Singh, A. N., Matson, J. L., Mouttapa, M., Pella, R. D., (1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control
Hill, B. D., & Thorson, R. (2009). A critical item anal- of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
ysis of the QABF; Development of a short form assess- Analysis, 18, 343–351.
ment instrument. Research in Developmental Webster-Stratton, C. (1997). Early intervention for
Disabilities, 30, 782–792. families of preschool children with conduct prob-
Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying lems. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), The effectiveness of
behavior-analysis procedures with children and youth. early intervention (pp. 429–453). Baltimore: Paul H.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Brookes.
Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles, Wieseler, N. A., Hanson, R. H., Chamberlain, T. P., &
R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009). Thompson, T. (1985). Functional taxonomy of stereo-
Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental typic and self-injurious behavior. Mental Retardation,
functional assessments of challenging behavior in 23, 230–234.
children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Zarcone, J. R., Rodgers, T. A., Iwata, B. A., Rourke, D. A.,
Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514. & Dorsey, M. F. (1991). Reliability analysis of the
Toogood, S., & Timlin, K. (1996). The functional assess- Motivation Assessment Scale: A failure to replicate.
ment of challenging behaviour: A comparison of Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 349–360.
Treatment Methods Commonly
Used in Conjunction with Functional 11
Assessment

Nicole M. Rodriguez, Wayne W. Fisher,


and Michael E. Kelley

Prior to this point, behavioral interventions more


Treatment Methods Commonly Used commonly involved the application of potential
in Conjunction with Functional reinforcers and punishers generally based on
Assessment reports in the literature or the clinician’s experi-
ence with prior cases. In some cases, these alter-
Treatments designed to address the function of
native reinforcers and punishers were sufficient
behavior have become a hallmark of behavior
to override existing contingencies of reinforce-
analysis. To identify environmental variables
ment for problem behavior (Carr, Taylor, Carlson,
maintaining problem behavior, a functional anal-
& Robinson, 1989; Cataldo, 1989). Functional
ysis is typically conducted prior to developing
analysis has provided a means of taking into
treatment. This process allows the clinician or
account the variables responsible for the mainte-
researcher to understand why the problem behav-
nance of problem behavior when developing a
ior occurs such that the treatment can be tailored
treatment, and this methodology has been dem-
to address those variables (e.g., by rearranging
onstrated to have strong predictive validity of
reinforcement contingencies or addressing the
successful treatment outcomes. Further, treat-
motivating operation).
ments based on the results of a functional analy-
Environmental variables responsible for
sis are less likely to involve punishment or
maintaining operant behavior fall into three
contain superfluous treatment components
broad categories: social-positive reinforcement,
(Pelios, Morren, Tesch, & Axelrod, 1999).
social-negative reinforcement, and automatic
This chapter focuses on function-based treat-
reinforcement. It was not until Iwata, Dorsey,
ments for challenging behavior such as self-
Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994) that
injurious behavior (SIB), aggression, or disruptive
behavioral sensitivity to reinforcers falling
behavior. Because procedures may vary depend-
within each of these categories was assessed
ing on the function of behavior, treatments for
within one comprehensive functional analysis.
social-positive reinforcement, social-negative
reinforcement, and automatic reinforcement are
N.M. Rodriguez (*) • W.W. Fisher • M.E. Kelley discussed separately. When discussing reinforce-
Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, ment-based procedures, we refer to the use of the
Munroe-Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska functional reinforcer identified via a functional
Medical Center, 985450 Nebraska Medical Center,
analysis. We refer the reader to Chap. 7 in this
Omaha, NE 68198-5450, USA
e-mail: nicole.rodriguez@unmc.edu; wfisher@unmc.edu; book for a discussion of how to identify the func-
michael.kelley@unmc.edu tion of behavior.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 179


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_11,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
180 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

Vollmer, Zarcone, & Smith, 1993; Shirley, Iwata,


Operant Mechanisms Kahng, Mazaleski, & Lerman, 1997; Zarcone,
of Reinforcement Contingencies Iwata, Hughes, & Vollmer, 1993).
A second functional component of a reinforce-
Iwata and colleagues (Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, &
ment contingency is its motivating operation.
Miltenberger, 1994; Iwata, Pace, & Dorsey et al.,
A motivating operation is an environmental vari-
1994) described three operant mechanisms
able that alters the efficacy of a reinforcer as well
related to the function of problem behavior—
as the current frequency of all behavior under
reinforcer, motivating operation, and discrimina-
control of that reinforcer (Laraway, Snycerski,
tive stimulus. Because of their relevance to the
Michael, & Poling, 2003). In social-positive rein-
arrangement of the conditions of a functional
forcement contingencies, periods of time without
analysis and the development of function-based
access to the reinforcer (e.g., caregiver attention
treatments, we will review the components of a
and preferred materials) establish or increase
reinforcement contingency briefly here.
the value of the reinforcer, whereas periods of
One functional component of a reinforcement
time with access to the reinforcer abolish or
contingency is the reinforcing consequence deliv-
decrease the value of the reinforcer. In social-
ered after emission of the target response. A rein-
negative reinforcement contingencies, the presen-
forcer is defined by the effect it has on behavior;
tation of the aversive stimulus (e.g., academic
when applied contingent on a response class, the
tasks and medical procedures) establishes or
probability of responses that belong to that class
increases the value of avoidance or escape (the
occurring again in the future increases (Catania,
reinforcer), whereas the absence of the aversive
1992). The reinforcer can either involve the pre-
stimuli abolishes or decreases the value of avoid-
sentation of an appetitive event (positive reinforce-
ance or escape. Other variables, such as the delivery
ment) or the prevention or termination of an
of appetitive stimuli (e.g., time-based delivery of
aversive event (negative reinforcement).1 In addi-
highly preferred edibles) during aversive events
tion, the reinforcer can be social in that its delivery
can also serve to reduce the aversive nature of an
is mediated by another person or automatic in that
event and abolish the effectiveness of escape as a
the behavior directly produces the reinforcer (e.g.,
reinforcer (Lomas, Fisher, & Kelley, 2010). An
rubbing one’s own temple to alleviate a headache;
understanding of how motivating operations
scratching an itch). An understanding of the rein-
influence the probability of a target response
forcers responsible for the maintenance of behav-
facilitates the use and manipulation of those
ior is integral for developing function-based
motivating operations during treatment of prob-
treatments. In fact, several studies have shown that
lem behavior. In fact, one of the advantages of
in many cases it may be necessary to disrupt the
reinforcement-based procedures (e.g., differen-
response-reinforcer relation (i.e., extinction) to
tial reinforcement of alternative behavior [DRA],
ensure successful treatment outcomes (e.g., Iwata,
differential reinforcement of other behavior
Pace, & Cowdery et al., 1994; Mazaleski, Iwata,
[DRO], noncontingent reinforcement [NCR]) is
that the functional reinforcer continues to be
1
Whereas some researchers have proposed that the dis-
available, which can serve to reduce the estab-
tinction between positive and negative reinforcement lishing operation for problem behavior.
should be abandoned on account that the distinction is A third functional component of a reinforce-
ambiguous and without functional significance (e.g. Baron ment contingency is its discriminative stimulus.
& Galizio, 2005; Michael, 1975), others argue that the dis-
tinction is both useful and sufficiently engrained in our
A discriminative stimulus is an antecedent stimu-
terminology (e.g., Iwata, 2006; Lattal & Lattal, 2006; lus whose presence signals the availability of a
Sidman, 2006). We maintain the distinction here to particular reinforcer. A discriminative stimulus
describe whether the stimulus change involved the intro- enters a reinforcement contingency through
duction or withdrawal of a stimulus following the target
behavior and stress the importance of describing and ana-
repeated exposure to a functional stimulus–
lyzing both sides of the stimulus change, including the behavior–consequence relation, which can
relevant motivating operations and discriminative stimuli. vary depending on an individual’s history of
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 181

reinforcement. For example, under naturally The relevance of the three operant mechanisms
occurring conditions, it is possible for a rein- of reinforcement contingencies to the develop-
forcement history to develop in which the pres- ment of function-based treatments will be dis-
ence of a child’s mother but not the father may cussed further in the remainder of this chapter.
signal the availability of physical attention (e.g.,
hugging and rocking the child) following SIB if
the mother frequently delivered this consequence Functioned-Based Treatments
for SIB in the past and the father did not. In a for Behavior Maintained
functional analysis, discriminative stimuli help to by Social-Positive Reinforcement
signal the consequence that is available within
each condition. Such discriminative stimuli can A wide variety of problem behavior (e.g., SIB,
be naturalistic like the presence of academic aggression, property destruction, elopement, and
materials signaling the availability of escape for pica) has been shown to be maintained by social-
SIB, contrived like different colored t-shirts or positive reinforcement. For example, in their review
poster boards being associated with different of 152 single-subject functional analyses, Iwata,
conditions, or both. Conners et al. (2000) demon- Pace, & Dorsey, et al. (1994) found that the SIB of
strated that associating salient discriminative 26.3% (40) of the participants included in their
stimuli with each condition of the functional analysis was maintained by social-positive rein-
analysis can reduce carry-over effects between forcement. Examples of potential social-positive
conditions, thereby improving the efficiency or reinforcers that may be responsible for the mainte-
clarity of a functional analysis. nance of problem behavior include caregiver atten-
An understanding of the relation between a tion such as verbal reprimands, physical consoling
target response and these three operant mecha- (e.g., rubbing back), physical restraint or access to
nisms is important not only for understanding the preferred items such as food, toys, or TV.
principles that underlie a functional analysis but
also for developing effective function-based
interventions for problem behaviors. That is, Extinction
function-based treatments for problem behavior
generally involve manipulation of one or more of When contingencies are arranged such that prob-
these mechanisms (i.e., the consequence for lem behavior no longer produces reinforcement,
problem behavior, the motivating operation that extinction is in effect. For social-positive contin-
establishes the effectiveness of that consequence gencies, this involves withholding delivery of the
as a reinforcer for problem behavior, and/or the reinforcer following problem behavior (e.g.,
discriminative stimulus that signals that problem Fisher et al., 1993). For example, if problem
behavior will produce that reinforcer). When behavior is maintained by caregiver attention,
developing function-based treatments, a fruitful then extinction would involve assuring that atten-
place to start is to ask a series of questions related tion is no longer provided contingent on the
to these three components: (1) How can the rein- undesirable behavior (i.e., planned ignoring;
forcing consequence maintaining problem behav- Iwata, Pace, & Cowdery et al., 1994).
ior be altered to decrease problem behavior and As previously noted, results of several studies
increase appropriate behavior? (2) How can the (e.g., Mazaleski et al., 1993; Shirley et al., 1997)
relevant motivating operation be altered to suggest that extinction is an integral component
decrease the probability of problem behavior? of successful treatment packages. That is, prob-
and (3) How can discriminative stimuli be lem behavior may not decrease if it continues to
arranged to signal changes in the availability of produce reinforcement, even if the functional
reinforcement for appropriate behavior and the reinforcer is also provided contingent on, for
continued unavailability of reinforcement for example, an appropriate alternative response. For
problem behavior? this reason, extinction is commonly included as at
182 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

least one component of a larger treatment package during FCT to assure that problem behavior no
(e.g., functional communication training [FCT] longer produces reinforcement.
plus extinction). Based on their review of the literature on FCT,
Despite the role extinction plays in ensuring Tiger et al. (2008) provided several guidelines for
successful treatment outcomes, however, extinc- selecting and teaching a functional communica-
tion is rarely prescribed in isolation. This recom- tion response. Because individuals with develop-
mendation is due in large part to the negative side mental disabilities may have limited verbal
effects sometimes associated with extinction repertoires, the target topography of the commu-
(e.g., extinction bursts, aggression, or emotional nicative response may include any one of a variety
behavior) as well as the dangers associated with of modalities such as vocal responses, picture
implementing extinction at less than perfect exchanges, sign language or gestures, or use of
integrity (thereby producing an intermittent voice or text output devices. Tiger et al. identified
schedule of reinforcement that strengthens the three areas for consideration when selecting the
behavior). Ensuring the continued availability of topography of the communicative response: (a)
the functional reinforcer but independent of prob- the effort associated with emitting the response
lem behavior is thought to attenuate some of the should be less than that of emitting problem
negative side effects associated with extinction behavior (Horner & Day, 1991), (b) the response
(Lerman & Iwata, 1995; Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, should be easily recognizable by others, including
Smith, & Mazaleski, 1993; Vollmer et al., 1998). adults that are not familiar with the individual’s
Combining extinction with reinforcement-based behavioral treatment (to increase the likelihood
procedures allows for both the response–rein- that novel individuals will provide reinforcement
forcer relation and the motivating operation to be contingent on the response; Durand, 1999; Durand
addressed. & Carr, 1992), and (c) the response should be
simple enough (based on the individual’s current
repertoire) to be acquired quickly. For example,
Functional Communication Training for children with limited vocal repertoires (e.g.,
who can speak in one- to two-word sentences),
FCT, a form of DRA, is one of the most com- teaching the vocal response, “play” would be
monly used function-based treatments for prob- more appropriate than, “play with me please.” In
lem behavior (Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek, 2008). In general, vocal responses may be better than non-
FCT, the reinforcer found to maintain problem vocal responses as they are more likely to recruit
behavior is provided contingent on an alternative reinforcement from others but would not be ideal
communicative response. If problem behavior is if the response was not recognizable (e.g., due to
found to be maintained by access to preferred poor enunciation). In this case or in the case of an
materials, then FCT would involve teaching the individual who does not speak, exchanging a card
individual an appropriate means of obtaining that (e.g., that has a picture of two people playing and/
reinforcer (e.g., handing over a card that says, or that has the words “please play with me” writ-
“snack, please.”). The aim is to teach and rein- ten on it) may be appropriate as it is simple and
force an alternate means of obtaining the func- easily recognizable by others.
tional reinforcer. This allows the individual Despite the fact that FCT is one of the most
control over when the reinforcer is obtained, researched function-based treatments, few studies
which may also serve to reduce the motivating have provided clear, replicable procedural details
operation that evokes problem behavior. However, for the training of a functional communication
because contact with the new reinforcement con- response (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985; Fisher et al.,
tingency (i.e., reinforcement for the functional 1993; Shirley et al., 1997; Wacker et al., 1990).
communication response) may not be sufficient There are two general strategies that have been
to shift responding from problem behavior to reported: graduated prompts (e.g., most-to-least
appropriate behavior, extinction is often used prompts, least-to-most promps) and time-delay
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 183

prompts. In most-to-least prompting procedures, reinforcement is available and does not occur at
a physical or vocal prompt to engage in the target times when reinforcement is not available (Fisher,
communicative response is provided immediately Kuhn, & Thompson, 1998; Hanley, Iwata, &
upon the removal of the reinforcer and then grad- Thompson, 2001). The use of schedule-correlated
ually faded in intensity (e.g., after a number of stimuli (multiple schedules) can help ensure that
trials involving full physical guidance, the thera- the communicative response is not weakened via
pist might guide the individual’ hand such that it extinction during those periods in which rein-
is hovering over the card). Prompted responses forcement is not available. To teach discriminate
are reinforced to ensure contact with the contin- responding, the individual is typically provided
gencies and to prevent exposure to deprivation with repeated exposure to the schedule-correlated
from the functional reinforcer (e.g., attention), stimuli (e.g., different-colored cards for reinforce-
which helps to decrease the probability of evok- ment and extinction periods) and the respective
ing problem behavior during training. The inten- contingencies operating when those stimuli are
sity of the prompt is faded, generally according to present. Hanley et al. (2001) began with brief
some predetermined criteria (e.g., two consecu- periods of extinction (S−) and then gradually
tive sessions without problem behavior) until the increased the length of the time that the S− was
response is being emitted consistently in the present and decreased the length of the time the
absence of any prompts. With least-to-most S+ (indicating reinforcement was available) was
prompting, the therapist proceeds from the least present. When the communicative response
intense to the most intense prompt sequentially involves a picture exchange card system, restrict-
within each trial (e.g., progressing from vocal, ing access to the picture card may serve as a prac-
model, to physical prompts, as necessary). tical approach to reinforcer-schedule thinning
In time-delay prompting procedures, the rele- (Roane, Fisher, Sgro, Falcomata, & Pabico, 2004).
vant establishing operation is provided (e.g., atten- In addition to the absence of the FCT, card serv-
tion is removed or a demand is presented) and ing as a discriminative stimulus that signals the
then the individual is provided with a short period unavailability of reinforcement, restricting access
of time (e.g., 1 s) to respond before being vocally to the materials necessary to emit the response
or physically prompted to engage in the commu- makes it impossible for the individual to emit the
nicative response. The period of time before the response at times when it will not produce rein-
prompt is delivered is gradually extended (e.g., in forcement (e.g., when the parent is occupied
1-s intervals) to transfer control from the thera- changing the diaper of an infant sibling).
pist’s prompt to the evocative event. Regardless of
the strategy used to train the response, reinforce-
ment should be delivered immediately and follow- Differential Reinforcement
ing each occurrence of the communicative response of Other Behavior
(i.e., an FR 1 schedule; Horner & Day, 1991). We
refer the reader to Tiger et al. (2008) for additional Reinforcement can also be provided contingent
guidelines for developing FCT interventions. on the nonoccurrence of the problem behavior
One criticism of FCT as a treatment for prob- (DRO). This can involve programming the deliv-
lem behavior is that the communicative response ery of the reinforcer based on the absence of prob-
often requires an FR 1 schedule of reinforcement lem behavior either (a) throughout the duration of
to assure that problem behavior decreases to near- the DRO interval (e.g., 30 s; whole-interval DRO;
zero levels of responding. However, it is often see Repp, Barton, & Brulle, 1983) or (b) dur-
impractical or even impossible for caregivers to ing a brief momentary observation at the end of
maintain such a dense schedule of reinforcement the DRO interval (momentary DRO; Lindberg,
in the natural environment (Fisher et al., 1993). Iwata, Kahng, & DeLeon, 1999). This latter pro-
One approach to addressing this issue is to bring cedure serves as a practical alternative to whole-
the functional communicative response under interval DRO schedules as it requires considerably
stimulus control such that it occurs at times when less vigilance on the part of the observer.
184 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

Varying the interval lengths of momentary reinforcement independent of all behavior, on a


DRO schedules may be important for obscuring time-based schedule (Fisher, DeLeon, Rodriguez-
the potential discrimination of the point in time at Catter, & Keeney, 2004; Vollmer et al., 1993).
which the individual is being observed (Lindberg, This approach is commonly referred to as NCR.2
Iwata, & Kahng, et al., 1999). If the end of the For behavior maintained by attention, this could
DRO interval is signaled (e.g., a timer beeps) or involve providing a brief form of attention such
predictable (e.g., occurs at fixed intervals), then it as verbal praise every 10 s or more extended
is possible for the individual to engage in inappro- forms of attention wherein attention is provided
priate behavior throughout the remainder of the continuously within a play context (e.g., 15 min
interval and still receive the reinforcer. Lindberg, of play).
Iwata, Kahng, et al. suggested that the predictabil- Kahng, Iwata, Thompson, and Hanley (2000)
ity of the observation period in momentary DRO noted that reductions in problem behavior during
schedules when the intervals are fixed may have NCR can be attributed either to a reduction in the
contributed to the ineffectiveness of such sched- establishing operation for problem behavior or the
ules in previous studies comparing whole-interval disruption of the response–reinforcer contingency.
and momentary DRO schedules (e.g., Repp et al., The large and immediate reduction in problem
1983; cf., Derwas & Jones, 1993). behavior commonly observed when reinforce-
Another important consideration for DRO ment is delivered on a dense schedule or in large
schedules is the length of the interval (or in the magnitudes (e.g., Hagopian, Fisher, & Legacy,
case of variable schedules, the average length of 1994; Roscoe, Iwata, & Rand, 2003) suggests that
the interval). The interval length should be short the effects of NCR are largely attributable to a
enough to prevent periods of deprivation due to reduction in the establishing operation for prob-
low rates of reinforcement. This is particularly lem behavior. This pattern of responding is in
important for whole-interval schedules in which contrast to that observed when extinction is imple-
the criteria for reinforcement is much more strin- mented in isolation (e.g., Fisher et al., 2004).
gent (one instance of problem behavior resets the One way to assure that the establishing opera-
DRO interval). Based on a formula described by tion for problem behavior has been eliminated or
Poling and Ryan (1982), Vollmer et al. (1993) reduced is to provide reinforcement continuously
used the mean interresponse time from the prior (Vollmer et al., 1993). Alternatively, the schedule
n sessions as the DRO interval. The interval of reinforcement can be based on either the mean
length increased based on decreases in SIB, but if latency to the first instance of problem behavior
performance worsened (SIB increased) the inter- during baseline (Lalli, Casey, & Kates, 1997) or
val length was never shortened. In this way, the the mean interresponse time of problem behavior
interval length was gradually extended such that during baseline (Kahng et al., 2000). In such cases,
longer periods of time without problem behavior it is recommended that the density of the rein-
were required for reinforcement to be delivered. forcement schedule be accommodated to prevent
Similar procedures for selecting and modifying the development of adventitious reinforcement by
the DRO interval length were used for other pro- making it either noticeably more or less dense
cedural variations of DRO schedules (including
the variable-momentary DRO schedule) in
2
Lindberg, Iwata, & Kahng, et al., 1999. The term, NCR, has been criticized because the intended
(and often observed) effect is the weakening of the target
response; yet, reinforcement is defined as an increase in
responding to the contingent delivery of a reinforcer (Poling
Response-Independent Delivery & Normand, 1999). The term, fixed-time (FT) schedule,
of Positive Reinforcement has been offered as an alternative but this label does not
acknowledge the previous functional relation between the
target response and the stimulus delivered on the time-
Another reinforcement-based approach to the based schedule. We use the term NCR in this chapter to
treatment of problem behavior is to deliver maintain contact with the relevant applied literature.
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 185

than the mean interresponse time. Alternatively, a behavior no longer results in the prevention or
DRO contingency could be added to the NCR termination of the aversive stimulus or event. To
schedule in which the delivery of a scheduled assure the continued presentation of the stimulus,
reinforcer in the NCR schedule would be delayed researchers have used instructional sequences
(e.g., by 5 s), if problem behavior occurs immedi- such as three-step guided compliance (e.g., Iwata
ately prior to the scheduled delivery (e.g., Britton, et al., 1990). This procedure involves a series of
Carr, Kellum, Dozier, & Weil, 2000). three types of prompts: verbal, gestural/model,
To increase the practicality of NCR schedules, and physical prompt (hand-over-hand guidance),
the density of the reinforcement schedule can be in which the child is provided with approximately
gradually thinned (Hagopian et al., 1994). 5 s between each prompt to comply before the
Alternatively, it has been shown that the availabil- therapist moves onto the next prompt in the three-
ity of other, albeit nonfunctional, reinforcers such step series. If the child complies with the demand
as toys can increase function-based treatments for following the verbal or gestural/model prompt,
behavior maintained by attention, if the items that verbal praise (e.g., “Nice job!” or “That’s right!”)
are made available compete with the attention is delivered. If problem behavior occurs at any
provided by caregivers (e.g., Hanley, Piazza, & point throughout the three-step sequence, the cli-
Fisher, 1997). A competing items assessment nician or researcher either immediately physically
(described below under treatments for automati- guides the correct response (e.g., Iwata et al.,
cally maintained problem behavior; Piazza et al., 1990) or continues the three-step sequence (e.g.,
1998) can be adapted to identify items likely to Kuhn, DeLeon, Fisher, & Wilke, 1999). When
compete with problem behavior maintained by physical guidance is impossible or near impossi-
attention or other social contingencies (Fisher, ble (e.g., due to the size of the individual), a par-
O’Connor, Kurtz, DeLeon, & Gotjen, 2000). tial physical prompt followed by the representation
of the demand may be sufficient to produce an
escape extinction effect. Although procedural
Functioned-Based Treatments variations in escape extinction are commonly
for Behavior Maintained implemented in clinical practice, additional
by Social-Negative Reinforcement research is needed on the conditions under which
such procedural variations would or would not be
Social-negative reinforcement, particularly effective. Finally, because escape extinction may
escape from demands, is one of the most com- be difficult to implement (e.g., physical guidance
mon maintaining variables for problem behavior. with large, strong clients), it may be necessary to
In fact, Iwata, Pace, Dorsey et al. (1994) found explore other function-based treatments that can
that the SIB of 38.1% (58) of the 152 participants be used either in conjunction with or in place of
in their analysis was maintained by social- escape extinction.
negative reinforcement. Social-negative rein-
forcement can take the form of avoidance or
escape from academic demands, medical proce- Differential Negative Reinforcement
dures (Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, of Alternative Behavior (DNRA)
1990), noise (McCord, Iwata, Galensky,
Ellingson, & Thomson, 2001), or social interac- Functional Communication Training
tion (Hall, DeBernardis, & Reiss, 2006). FCT for social-negative reinforcement involves
training and reinforcing a communicative
response that results in breaks from (e.g., Horner
Extinction & Day, 1991) or assistance with (e.g., Carr &
Durand, 1985) nonpreferred or aversive stimuli
To assure that the response–reinforcer relation is or tasks (e.g., academic demands). Examples of
disrupted in social-negative contingencies, con- communicative responses include teaching the
tingencies must be rearranged such that problem individual to say, “Break, please” to receive a
186 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

break from the task or “I don’t understand” to Another approach to treating problem behav-
receive assistance with a task. Other modalities ior maintained by escape is to deliver positive
such as sign language, gestures, or the handing reinforcement (e.g., a highly preferred edible) for
over of a picture card with picture and/or tex- compliance. Positive reinforcement for compli-
tual stimuli depicting the reinforcer can also be ance has been shown to be effective even when
used as the communicative response. As previ- problem behavior continues to result in escape
ously noted, FCT is commonly combined with (e.g., DeLeon, Neidert, Anders, & Rodriguez-
extinction (e.g., Durand & Merges, 2001) or Catter, 2001; Kodak, Lerman, Volkert, &
punishment (e.g., Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, & Trosclair, 2007; Lalli et al., 1999). Lalli et al.
Maglieri, 2005) to ensure successful treatment suggested two operant mechanisms that may
outcomes. explain the efficacy of differential positive rein-
One limitation of FCT for behavior main- forcement of compliance in treating behavior
tained by escape is that the individual can, and maintained by negative reinforcement: (a) that
usually does, emit the communicative response the relative value of the positive reinforcer is
frequently enough to escape all or almost all the greater than that of escape (DeLeon et al., 2001)
instructional demands. In such circumstances, and/or (b) that the availability of the positive
the individual is not learning or benefiting from reinforcer serves as an abolishing operation that
the instruction (Fisher et al., 1993). Instructional lessens the aversive properties of the demand
fading (also known as demand or stimulus fad- context. Two recent studies have provided strong
ing) involves gradually increasing the number of support for this latter hypothesized mechanism
demands presented per session, and it is one by demonstrating that the provision of positive
approach that can be used to assure the continued reinforcers (i.e., food plus praise), either on a
presentation of instructional demands (Pace, time-based schedule or contingent on compli-
Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, 1993). ance, can produce marked reductions in problem
One of the advantages of instructional fading is behavior reinforced by escape even when the
that it may increase tolerance of instructional escape contingency remains in effect (Lomas
tasks (e.g., through processes akin to that of sys- et al., 2010; Lomas, Fisher, Kelley, & Fredrick,
tematic desensitization). in press).

Differential Reinforcement of Compliance


Another approach to decreasing problem behav- Differential Negative Reinforcement
ior while assuring the continued presentation of of Other Behavior
academic demands is to provide escape contin-
gent on compliance (e.g., Marcus & Vollmer, Differential negative reinforcement of other
1995; Vollmer, Roane, Ringdahl, & Marcus, behavior (DNRO) involves providing escape con-
1999). Differential reinforcement of compliance tingent on zero levels of responding throughout
can result in concomitant decreases in problem the DNRO interval (Buckley & Newchok, 2006;
behavior, even when the consequences for prob- Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). Because the timing of
lem behavior remain unchanged (Parrish, Cataldo, reinforcement in DNRO schedules is under con-
Kolko, Neef, & Egel, 1986; Russo, Cataldo, & trol of the clinician or researcher, DNRO may be
Cushing, 1981). Similarly, escape extinction can particularly useful when it is important for the
result in concomitant increases in compliance. As clinician or researcher to maintain control over
suggested by Cataldo and colleagues, it is possi- when ongoing activities are disrupted (e.g., dur-
ble that such indirect effects of treatments are ing medical procedures). DNRO also involves the
observed when compliance and problem behav- continued presentation of the aversive stimulus,
ior are inverse members of an overarching which may produce habituation and make their
response class call “instruction following.” subsequent presentation less aversive.
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 187

Response-Independent Delivery
Functioned-Based Treatments for
of Negative Reinforcement
Behavior Maintained by Automatic
Reinforcement
Like positive reinforcers, escape can be delivered
independent of problem behavior, on a time-
A third potential source of reinforcement
based schedule (Kodak, Miltenberger, &
for problem behavior has been referred to as
Romanuik, 2003; Vollmer, Marcus, & Ringdahl,
automatic reinforcement because the reinforcing
1995). This procedure has also been referred to
consequence is one that is a direct or “automatic”
as noncontingent escape (NCE). The delivery of
result of the target response (Skinner, 1953;
escape on a dense fixed-time schedule can pro-
Vaughn & Michael, 1982; Vollmer, 1994).
duce immediate and large reductions in problem
Because the direct consequences of automatically
behavior, after which the schedule can be leaned
maintained behavior are often difficult to manip-
(e.g., from 10 s to 2.5 or 10 min; Vollmer et al.,
ulate, automatic reinforcement is inferred in a
1995). Further, there is some evidence to suggest
functional analysis based on the persistence of
that NCE may be effective even when problem
behavior in the absence of social contingencies
behavior continues to result in escape (e.g., Lalli
(e.g., when the individual is alone). The over-
et al., 1997), which is important when escape
whelming majority of the literature on the assess-
extinction cannot be implemented with perfect
ment and treatment of stereotypy suggests that
procedural integrity (e.g., the individual is large
most stereotypy is maintained by automatic rein-
and difficult to physically guide).
forcement (Rapp & Vollmer, 2005). Potential
An alternative method of addressing the moti-
examples of behavior maintained by automatic
vating operation is to identify the specific features
reinforcement include body rocking that per-
of a demand context that makes it aversive and
sists due to favorable kinesthetic sensations, or
then to modify procedures in ways that lessen
the repeated waving of colorful objects in front
those aversive properties. Examples of properties
of the eyes that persists due to favorable visual
of demand contexts that have been found to estab-
sensations.
lish the efficacy of escape as reinforcement for
problem behavior include the inclusion of difficult
(Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981), less preferred
Extinction
(Dunlap, Kern-Dunlap, Clarke, & Robbins,
1991), or novel tasks (Mace, Browder, & Lin,
One approach to decreasing automatically main-
1987; Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995). The
tained problem behavior is to disrupt or attenuate
duration of an instructional session, the rate of
the response–reinforcer relation (also referred to
instructions, massed trials, certain prompting
as sensory extinction, e.g., Kuhn et al., 1999;
strategies, and the cancelation of a planned and
Rincover, Cook, Peoples, & Packard, 1979).
preferred activity prior to the instructional ses-
Perhaps the most common method for doing so
sion have also been shown to function as motivat-
is to block the response from occurring.3 For
ing operations for escape (Dunlap et al., 1991;
example, if self-injury in the form of eye poking
Horner, Day, & Day, 1997; Munk & Repp, 1994;
is found to be maintained by automatic reinforce-
Smith et al., 1995). To abolish or lessen the value
ment, then one might program extinction by
of escape as reinforcement for problem behavior,
researchers have used strategies such as inter-
spersing less aversive tasks (Ebanks & Fisher, 3
In some cases, the effects of response blocking on prob-
2003; Horner, Day, Sprague, O’Brien, & lem behavior may be more appropriately attributed to the
Heathfield, 1991), gradually increasing the rate or effects of punishment. We refer the reader to Lerman and
aversiveness of tasks (Pace, Ivancic, & Jefferson, Iwata (1996) for an example of a procedure that can be
used to identify the processes (automatic extinction versus
1994; Pace et al., 1993), and providing choices punishment) responsible for decreased responding when
regarding the order or timing in which tasks are response blocking is applied contingent on problem
completed (Dyer, Dunlap, & Winterling, 1990). behavior.
188 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

having someone physically block the individual’s extinction procedures as a treatment (e.g., extinc-
finger from making contact with his or her eye tion for two participants involved limiting the
(e.g., by placing their hand between the individu- putative visual stimulation by either dimming
al’s hand and eye). Sensory extinction could also overhead lights or applying a blindfold).
take different forms such as having the individual One potential limitation of sensory extinction
wear eye goggles for eye poking (Kennedy & procedures that involve innovative manipulations
Souza, 1995), gloves for hand mouthing or self- such as the ones described above is that the clini-
scratching (Reid, Parsons, Phillips, & Green, cian must first identify the specific source of
1993; Roscoe, Iwata, & Goh, 1998), or a protec- automatic reinforcement, which is often difficult.
tive helmet for head hitting (Silverman, Watanabe, In contrast, response blocking or interruption
Marshall, & Baer, 1984). If, for example, eye (which prevents the completion of the problem
poking was maintained by the visual-like sensa- behavior) is often sufficient to disrupt the
tions it produces, then eye goggles, which pre- response–reinforcer relation regardless of the
vent the finger from making contact with the eye, specific source of automatic reinforcement. On
would assure that this behavior no longer con- the other hand, one disadvantage of response
tacts this form of automatic reinforcement. blocking is that it often requires constant moni-
In one of the first applications of function- toring to assure that the individual’s behavior
based procedures for automatically reinforced does not contact the automatic reinforcer on an
behavior, Rincover et al. (1979) demonstrated the intermittent reinforcement schedule. If eye-poking
application of sensory extinction and matched is maintained by the visual stimulation it produces,
stimuli to the assessment and treatment of auto- the ability to use goggles to prevent access to this
matically reinforced problem behavior. Based on reinforcer (and thereby increase this potentially
teacher interviews and casual observations, dangerous form of SIB) serves as a practical alter-
Rincover et al. developed hypotheses as to the native to response blocking because the former
sensory consequences maintaining each of four procedure requires much less monitoring of the
participants’ problem behavior (object spinning, individual’s behavior than does the latter
hand flapping, hand waving, or finger flapping). procedure.
In the first phase, sensory extinction was applied
in a reversal design to isolate the specific source
of sensory stimulation. For example, during Response-Independent Delivery
casual observations, one participant appeared to of Alternate Sources of Automatic
be listening to the noise produced by a plate spin- Reinforcement
ning. Assuming that auditory stimulation was the
reinforcing consequence for object spinning, sen- An alternate approach commonly used to treat
sory extinction for this participant was introduced behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement
by installing carpet on top of the table, which is to provide alternate sources of stimulation that
markedly reduced the volume and quality of the compete with or substitute for the automatic
sound produced by object spinning. Sensory reinforcer for problem behavior (e.g., Goh et al.,
extinction decreased levels of problem behav- 1995; Piazza, Adelinis, Hanley, Goh, & Delia,
ior across all participants, suggesting that the 2000; Piazza et al., 1998; Rincover et al., 1979;
specific source of automatic reinforcement had Roscoe et al., 1998; Shore, Iwata, DeLeon,
been identified. Rather than using sensory extinc- Kahng, & Smith, 1997; Wilder, Draper, Williams,
tion as the treatment for the problem behavior & Higbee, 1997). This approach to intervention
exhibited by the individuals in their study, has also been referred to as an enriched environ-
Rincover et al. went on to assess the effects of ment (e.g., Horner, 1980; Vollmer, Marcus, &
providing noncontingent access to matched stim- LeBlanc, 1994).
uli (described below), perhaps due to the practi- In the second phase of Rincover et al. (1979),
cal limitations of using some of their sensory stimuli matching the sensory consequences
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 189

produced by problem behavior were provided Clark, DeBar, & Florentino, 2005; Vollmer et al.,
noncontingently and continuously and compared 1994), particularly if the problem behavior pri-
to unmatched stimuli (recall that the specific marily occurs when little to no alternate sources
source of automatic reinforcement was identified of stimulation are available (e.g., an austere insti-
through the systematic application of sensory tutional environment). For example, Vollmer
extinction in the first phases of their study). When et al. demonstrated that the use of highly pre-
matched and unmatched stimuli were concur- ferred toys or leisure items identified via a paired-
rently available, all participants (a) exclusively or choice preference assessment (Fisher et al., 1992)
primarily engaged with matched stimuli and (b) was sufficient to reduce problem behavior, and
engaged in zero or near-zero levels of problem Ahearn et al. showed that unmatched but highly
behavior. These results demonstrated that pro- preferred stimuli were more effective than
viding access to matched stimuli may produce matched stimuli at producing desirable treatment
both an increase in item engagement and a outcomes.
decrease in problem behavior. One of the advantages of providing access to
Piazza and colleagues presented an alternate alternate sources of stimulation as treatment for
method for empirically deriving potentially sub- automatically maintained problem behavior is
stitutable or competing stimuli (e.g., Piazza et al., that it provides an opportunity to engage in an
1998, 2000). Prior to assessing the relative effects appropriate alternative response. However, sim-
of matched versus unmatched stimuli, Piazza ply providing access to alternate stimuli may not
et al. conducted a preference assessment to deter- be sufficient to decrease automatically main-
mine stimuli associated with high levels of item tained problem behavior or increase item engage-
engagement and low levels of problem behavior. ment (Lindberg, Iwata, & Kahng, 1999; e.g.,
Stimuli included in the competing-stimulus Hanley, Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg, 2000;
assessment were selected based on the topogra- Piazza et al., 1998). In such cases, additional
phy of the behavior (climbing, saliva play, and strategies such as prompts, response blocking, or
hand mouthing), observations, and experimenters’ reinforcement may be necessary to increase con-
hypotheses with respect to the source of auto- tact with the alternate source of stimulation or
matic reinforcement. For example, for the partici- prevent access to the stimulation produced by
pant who engaged in dangerous behavior such as problem behavior.
climbing on furniture and jumping out of win-
dows, stimuli hypothesized to match the putative
automatic reinforcer—kinesthetic stimulation Differential Reinforcement
from jumping or bouncing—included a green of Alternative Behavior
bouncy ball, an air mattress for bouncing, and a
balance board. The preference assessment served Several researchers have shown that access to the
as a basis from which to identify stimuli likely to opportunity to engage in automatically reinforc-
substitute for, or compete with, the automatic ing problem behavior (e.g., stereotypy) can be
reinforcement produced by problem behavior. In used to increase an alternative response such as
addition, Piazza et al. demonstrated that providing appropriate engagement with leisure items or
access to stimuli hypothesized to match the correct responding on academic or prevocational
automatic reinforcement was generally more tasks (e.g., Charlop, Kurtz, & Casey, 1990;
effective than unmatched stimuli in reducing Charlop-Christy & Haymes, 1996; Hanley et al.,
automatically reinforced problem behavior. 2000; Sugai & White, 1986; Wolery, Kirk, &
However, it should be noted that there are studies Gast, 1985). DRA for automatically maintained
that suggest that simply providing access to alter- problem behavior may be desirable, if it is socially
nate activities (regardless of whether they are acceptable to allow some of the problem behavior
matched) may be sufficient to compete with the to occur (e.g., the target behavior is considered a
reinforcer maintaining problem behavior (Ahearn, problem because it occurs at times that interfere
190 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

with other activities such as work). It should be prior functional analysis tend to be more effective
noted, however, that to arrange problem behavior and less likely to include a punishment compo-
as a reinforcer other strategies such as response nent than ones not based on a functional analysis.
blocking will likely be needed to prevent access
to the problem behavior when the contingency
for reinforcement has not yet been met. References
As an example of the application of contingent
access to automatically maintained problem Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R., & Florentino, C.
behavior as a reinforcer for alternate behavior, (2005). On the role of preference in response competi-
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
Hanley et al. (2000) measured the additive effects
247–250.
of several treatment components on the effects of Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (2005). Positive and negative
the automatically maintained problem behavior reinforcement: Should the distinction be preserved?
(e.g., skin pressing and hand mouthing) and item The Behavior Analyst, 28, 85–98.
Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Kellum, K. K., Dozier, C. L., &
engagement of three individuals with develop-
Weil, T. M. (2000). A variation of noncontingent rein-
mental disabilities. The treatment components forcement in the treatment of aberrant behavior.
that were evaluated were: (a) continuous access to Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 425–435.
alternate stimulation, (b) prompts to engage with Buckley, S. D., & Newchok, D. K. (2006). Analysis and
treatment of problem behavior evoked by music.
materials, (c) response blocking, and (d) access to
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 141–144.
stereotypy as reinforcement for item engagement. Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
Prompts to engage with materials were insufficient problems through functional communication training.
for decreasing problem behavior or increasing the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126.
Carr, E. G., Taylor, J. C., Carlson, J. I., & Robinson, S.
appropriate alternative for all three participants.
(1989). Reinforcement and stimulus-based treatments
When response blocking was added to prompts, for severe behavior problems in developmental dis-
stereotypy decreased for all three participants and abilities. Background paper prepared for the National
appropriate item engagement increased for two Institutes of Health Consensus Development Panel on
the Treatment of Destructive Behavior. Bethesda, MD:
participants. The addition of contingent access to
National Institutes of Health.
stereotypy was necessary to increase appropriate Cataldo, M. F. (1989). The effects of punishment and other
item engagement for the third participant. behavior reducing procedures on the destructive
behaviors of persons with developmental disabilities.
Background paper prepared for the National Institutes
of Health Consensus Development Panel on the
Concluding Remarks Treatment of Destructive Behavior. Bethesda, MD:
National Institutes of Health.
The process of identifying a treatment based on Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning. Englewood Cliffs:
the results of a functional analysis has been asso- Prentice Hall.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1996). Using
ciated with a number of advantages, including
obsessions as reinforcers with and without mild reduc-
increasing the likelihood that the treatment will tive procedures to decrease inappropriate behaviors of
produce rapid and clinically significant reductions children with autism. Journal of Autism and
in problem behavior. By identifying the variables Developmental Disorders, 26, 527–546.
Charlop, M. H., Kurtz, P. F., & Casey, F. G. (1990). Using
that maintain problem behavior, clinicians and
aberrant behaviors as reinforcers for autistic children.
researchers can manipulate those variables to shift Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 163–181.
responding from problem behavior to appropriate Conners, J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S. W., Hanley, G. P.,
behavior. Function-based interventions tend to be Worsdell, A. S., & Thompson, R. H. (2000).
Differential responding in the presence and absence of
more efficient because the results of the functional
discriminative stimuli during multielement functional
analysis direct the behavior analyst to focus on a analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33,
small number of environmental variables that 299–308.
have a large influence on problem behavior, and DeLeon, I. G., Neidert, P. L., Anders, B. M., & Rodriguez-
Catter, V. (2001). Choices between positive and
thus function-based treatments are generally free
negative reinforcement during treatment for escape-
of superfluous intervention components. Finally, maintained behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
research has shown that interventions based on a Analysis, 34, 521–525.
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 191

Derwas, H., & Jones, R. S. P. (1993). Reducing stereo- Hall, S. S., DeBernardis, G. M., & Reiss, A. L. (2006).
typed behavior using momentary DRO: An experi- Social escape behaviors in children with fragile X syn-
mental analysis. Behavioral Interventions, 8, 45–53. drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Dunlap, G., Kern-Dunlap, L., Clarke, S., & Robbins, F. R. Disorders, 36, 935–947.
(1991). Functional assessment, curricular revision, Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001).
and severe behavior problems. Journal of Applied Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment
Behavior Analysis, 24, 387–397. with functional communication training. Journal of
Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication training Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38.
using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communi- Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Thompson, R. H., & Lindberg,
ties of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior J. S. (2000). A component analysis of “stereotypy as
Analysis, 32, 247–267. reinforcement” for alternative behavior. Journal of
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1992). An analysis of main- Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 285–297.
tenance following functional communication training. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 777–794. Noncontingent presentation of attention and alterna-
Durand, V. M., & Merges, E. (2001). Functional commu- tive stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained
nication training: A contemporary behavior analytic destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
intervention for problem behaviors. Focus on Autism Analysis, 30, 229–237.
and Other Developmental Disorders, 16, 110–119. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K.
Dyer, K., Dunlap, G., & Winterling, V. (1990). Effects of A. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for
choice making on the serious problem behaviors of punishment and extinction components of function-
students with severe handicaps. Journal of Applied based interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior
Behavior Analysis, 23, 515–524. Analysis, 38, 51–65.
Ebanks, M. E., & Fisher, W. W. (2003). Altering the tim- Horner, R. D. (1980). The effects of an environmental
ing of academic prompts to treat destructive behavior “enrichment” program on the behavior of institution-
maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior alized profoundly retarded children. Journal of Applied
Analysis, 36, 355–359. Behavior Analysis, 13, 473–491.
Fisher, W. W., DeLeon, I. G., Rodriguez-Catter, V., & Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of
Keeney, K. M. (2004). Enhancing the effects of extinc- response efficiency on functionally equivalent com-
tion on attention-maintained behavior through non- peting behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior
contingent delivery of attention or stimuli identified Analysis, 24, 719–732.
via a competing stimulus assessment. Journal of Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., & Day, J. R. (1997). Using
Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 171–184. neutralizing routines to reduce problem behaviors.
Fisher, W. W., Kuhn, D. E., & Thompson, R. H. (1998). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 601–614.
Establishing discriminative control of responding Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., Sprague, J. R., O’Brien, M., &
using functional and alternative reinforcers during Heathfield, L. T. (1991). Interspersed requests: a nona-
functional communication training. Journal of Applied versive procedure for reducing aggression and self-
Behavior Analysis, 31, 543–560. injury during instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior
Fisher, W. W., O’Connor, J. T., Kurtz, P. F., DeLeon, I. G., Analysis, 24, 265–278.
& Gotjen, D. L. (2000). The effects of noncontingent Iwata, B. A. (2006). On the distinction between positive
delivery of high- and low-preference stimuli on atten- and negative reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst, 29,
tion-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of 121–123.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 79–83. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis
L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for per- 27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention
sons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982).
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: an analysis
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- of procedural form and function. Journal of Applied
munication training with and without extinction and Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144.
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
26, 23–36. Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994). The func-
Goh, H., Iwata, B. A., Shore, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., Lerman, tions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-epi-
D. C., Ulrich, S. M., et al. (1995). An analysis of the demiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
reinforcing properties of hand mouthing. Journal of Analysis, 27, 215–240.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 269–283. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E.,
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., & Legacy, S. M. (1994). & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental analysis and
Schedule effects of noncontingent reinforcement on extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of
attention-maintained destructive behavior in identical Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 11–27.
quadruplets. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., Thompson, R. H., & Hanley, G. P.
27, 317–325. (2000). A method for identifying satiation versus
192 N.M. Rodriguez et al.

extinction effects under noncontingent reinforcement Mace, F. C., Browder, D., & Lin, Y. (1987). Analysis of
schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, demand conditions associated with stereotypy. Journal
419–432. of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18,
Kennedy, C. H., & Souza, G. (1995). Functional analysis 25–31.
and treatment of eye poking. Journal of Applied Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R. (1995). Effects of differ-
Behavior Analysis, 28, 27–37. ential negative reinforcement on disruption and com-
Kuhn, D. E., DeLeon, I. G., Fisher, W. W., & Wilke, A. E. pliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
(1999). Clarifying an ambiguous functional analysis 229–230.
with matched and mismatched extinction procedures. Mazaleski, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 99–102. J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce-
Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., Volkert, V. M., & Trosclair, N. ment and extinction components in DRO contingen-
(2007). Further examination of factors that influence cies with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
preference for positive versus negative reinforcement. Analysis, 26, 143–156.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 25–44. McCord, B. E., Iwata, B. A., Galensky, T. L., Ellingson, S.
Kodak, T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Romanuik, C. (2003). A., & Thomson, R. J. (2001). Functional analysis and
The effects of differential negative reinforcement of treatment of problem behavior evoked by noise.
other behavior and noncontingent escape on compli- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 447–462.
ance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, Michael, J. (1975). Positive and negative reinforcement:
379–382. A distinction that is no longer necessary; or a better
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent way to talk about bad things. Behaviorism, 3, 33–44.
reinforcement as treatment for severe problem behav- Munk, D. D., & Repp, A. C. (1994). The relationship
ior: Some procedural variations. Journal of Applied between instructional variable and problem behavior:
Behavior Analysis, 30, 127–137. A review. Exceptional Children, 60, 390–401.
Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C., Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., & Jefferson, G. (1994).
Borrero, J., Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition Stimulus fading as treatment for obscenity in a brain-
between positive and negative reinforcement in the injured adult. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied 27, 301–305.
Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–296. Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Cowdery, G. E., Andree, P. J., &
Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. McIntyre, T. (1993). Stimulus (instructional) fading
(2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe during extinction of self-injurious escape behavior.
them: some further refinements. Journal of Applied Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 205–212.
Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414. Pelios, L., Morren, J., Tesch, D., & Axelrod, S. (1999).
Lattal, A. K., & Lattal, A. D. (2006). And yet …: Further The impact of functional analysis methodology on
comments on distinguishing between positive and treatment choice for self-injurious and aggressive
negative reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst, 29, behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,
129–134. 185–195.
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1995). Prevalence of the Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H. L., &
extinction burst and its attenuation during treatment. Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 93–94. matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology for reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
distinguishing between extinction and punishment 33, 13–27.
effects associated with response blocking. Journal of Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A.,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 231–233. Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., et al. (1998).
Lindberg, J. S., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. W. (1999). On Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of its
the relation between object manipulation and stereo- reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior
typic self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Analysis, 31, 165–189.
Behavior Analysis, 32, 51–62. Parrish, J. M., Cataldo, M. F., Kolko, D. J., Neef, N. A., &
Lindberg, J. S., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S.W., & DeLeon, Egel, A. L. (1986). Experimental analysis of response
I. G. (1999). DRO contingencies: An analysis of vari- covariation among compliant and inappropriate behav-
able-momentary schedules. Journal of Applied iors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 241–254.
Behavior Analysis, 32, 123–136. Poling, A., & Normand, M. (1999). Noncontingent rein-
Lomas, L. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The forcement: An inappropriate description of time-based
effects of variable-time delivery of food items and schedules that reduce behavior. Journal of Applied
praise on problem behavior reinforced by escape. Behavior Analysis, 32, 237–238.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425–435. Poling, A., & Ryan, C. (1982). Differential-reinforcement-
Lomas-Mevers, J., Fisher, W. W., Kelley, M. E., & of-other-behavior schedules. Behavior Modification,
Fredrick, L. (in press). The Effects of variable-time 6, 3–21.
versus contingent reinforcement delivery on problem Rapp, J. T., & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A
behavior maintained by escape. Journal of Applied review of behavioral assessment and treatment.
Behavior Analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 527–547.
11 Treatment Methods Commonly Used 193

Reid, D. H., Parsons, M. B., Phillips, J. F., & Green, C. W. Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional
(1993). Reduction of self-injurious hand mouthing communication training: A review and practical guide.
using response blocking. Journal of Applied Behavior Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23.
Analysis, 26, 139–140. Vaughn, M. E., & Michael, J. L. (1982). Automatic rein-
Repp, A. C., Barton, L. E., & Brulle, A. R. (1983). A com- forcement: An important but ignored concept.
parison of two procedures for programming the dif- Behaviorism, 10, 217–228.
ferential reinforcement of other behaviors. Journal of Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforce-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 435–445. ment: Implications for behavioral research in develop-
Rincover, A., Cook, R., Peoples, A., & Packard, D. (1979). mental disabilities. Research in Developmental
Sensory extinction and sensory reinforcement princi- Disabilities, 15, 187–207.
ples for programming multiple adaptive behavior Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1992). Differential rein-
change. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, forcement as treatment for severe behavior disorders:
221–233. Procedural and functional variations. Research in
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata, T. S., & Developmental Disabilities, 13, 393–417.
Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method of thinning Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G.,
reinforcer delivery during differential reinforcement. & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in the
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 213–218. treatment of attention-maintained self-injurious behav-
Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Goh, H. (1998). A com- ior: noncontingent reinforcement and differential rein-
parison of noncontingent reinforcement and sensory forcement of other behavior. Journal of Applied
extinction as treatments for self-injurious behavior. Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 635–646. Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Lalli, J. S., Van Camp, C. M.,
Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Rand, M. S. (2003). Effects Sierp, B. J., Wright, C. S., et al. (1998). Fixed-time
of reinforcer consumption and magnitude on response schedules attenuate extinction-induced phenomena in
rates during noncontingent reinforcement. Journal of the treatment of severe aberrant behavior. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 525–539. Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 529–542.
Russo, D. C., Cataldo, M. F., & Cushing, P. J. (1981). Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994).
Compliance training and behavioral covariation in the Treatment of self-injury and hand mouthing following
treatment of multiple behavior problems. Journal of inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of Applied
Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 209–222. Behavior Analysis, 27, 331–344.
Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S., Mazaleski, J. L., & Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. (1995).
Lerman, D. C. (1997). Does functional communica- Noncontingent escape as treatment for self-injurious
tion training compete with ongoing contingencies of behavior maintained by negative reinforcement.
reinforcement? An analysis during response acquisi- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15–26.
tion and maintenance. Journal of Applied Behavior Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B.
Analysis, 30, 93–104. A. (1999). Evaluating treatment challenges with dif-
Shore, B. A., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S., & ferential reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal
Smith, R. G. (1997). An analysis of reinforcer substi- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 9–23.
tutability using object manipulation and self-injury as Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G., Berg, W.,
competing responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Reimers, T., et al. (1990). A component analysis of
Analysis, 30, 21–40. functional communication training across three topog-
Sidman, M. (2006). The distinction between positive and raphies of severe behavior problems. Journal of
negative reinforcement: Some additional consider- Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
ations. The Behavior Analyst, 29, 135–139. Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task difficulty and
Silverman, K., Watanabe, K., Marshall, A. M., & Baer, D. M. aberrant behavior in severely handicapped students.
(1984). Reducing self-injury and corresponding self- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 449–463.
restraint through the strategic use of protective Wilder, D. A., Draper, R., Williams, W. L., & Higbee, T. S.
clothing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, (1997). A comparison of noncontingent reinforce-
545–552. ment, other competing stimulation, and liquid resched-
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New uling for the treatment of rumination. Behavioral
York: The Free Press. Interventions, 12, 55–64.
Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Goh, H., & Shore, B. A. (1995). Wolery, M., Kirk, K., & Gast, D. L. (1985). Stereotypic
Analysis of establishing operations for self-injury behavior as a reinforcer: Effects and side-effects.
maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15,
Analysis, 28, 515–535. 149–161.
Sugai, G., & White, W. J. (1986). Effects of using object Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Hughes, C. E., & Vollmer, T.
self-stimulation as a reinforcer on the prevocational R. (1993). Momentum versus extinction effects in the
work rates of an autistic child. Journal of Autism and treatment of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of
Developmental Disorders, 16, 459–470. Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 135–136.
The Role of Functional Assessment
in Treatment Planning 12
Deborah A. Napolitano, Vicki Madaus Knapp,
Elizabeth Speares, David B. McAdam,
and Holly Brown

concern, and is empirically supported. This is


Functional Assessment especially important when treating individuals
and the Treatment Planning Process who display challenging behavior which fre-
quently interfere with their opportunities to learn
Treatment planning to reduce challenging behav-
and to live and be educated in less-restrictive set-
ior (e.g., aggression and property destruction)
tings (Matson, Mayville, & Laud, 2003). The
can be complex and requires a prescriptive inter-
utility of functional behavior assessments (FBAs)
professional team-based approach. As with other
and functional analyses (FAs) in the identification
problems that interfere with an individual’s qual-
of the specific function or purpose of challenging
ity of life, such as health issues, the best course of
behavior has been embraced commonly by per-
treatment is one that accurately assesses the prob-
sons charged with supporting individuals who
lem, leads to effective treatment for the issue of
display challenging behavior (e.g., behavior ana-
lysts and educators). This is demonstrated by the
laws requiring FBAs be conducted when indi-
viduals are struggling in educational environ-
D.A. Napolitano (*) • D.B. McAdam
University of Rochester School of Medicine, ments (Individuals with Disabilities Education
601 Elmwood Avenue, Box 671, Rochester, Improvement Act, 2004) and the belief that
NY 14642, USA the assessments conducted should result in the
e-mail: deborah_napolitano@urmc.rochester.edu;
development of interventions that help preserve
david_mcadam@urmc.rochester.edu
the child’s placement in the least restrictive
V.M. Knapp
appropriate educational setting. The utility of
Summit Educational Resources, 150 Stahl Road,
Getzville, NY 14068, USA these assessments has been repeatedly demon-
e-mail: vmknapp@summited.org strated in the published literature (e.g., Derby
E. Speares et al., 1992; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, &
Hillside Children’s Center, 1183 Monroe Avenue, Richman, 1994; Kennedy & Souza, 1995;
Rochester, NY 14620, USA Kern, Childs, Dunlap, Clarke & Falk, 1994;
e-mail: espeares@hillside.com
McCord, Thompson, & Iwata, 2001; Ellingson,
H. Brown Miltenberger, Stricker, Galensky, & Garlinghouse,
Hillside Children’s Center, 1183 Monroe Avenue,
2000). Several excellent reviews of this extensive
Rochester, NY 14620, USA
literature have been conducted and have indepen-
University of Rochester School of Nursing,
dently identified functional analysis and assess-
601 Elmwood Avenue, Box SON,
Rochester, NY 14642, USA ment as best practice (e.g., Carr, 1994; Hanley,
e-mail: holly_brown@urmc.rochester.edu Iwata, & McCord, 2003).

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 195


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_12,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
196 D.A. Napolitano et al.

Research using functional analyses and assess- strategies, behavior teaching, and consequence
ments has demonstrated that challenging behav- strategies such as DRO. Whereas, the nonfunc-
ior is maintained by both social and nonsocial tion-based plan included strategies not linked to
factors (Hanley et al., 2003). In the majority of the function of a participant’s challenging behav-
cases, the challenging behavior of persons have ior. Both plans were then implemented by the stu-
proven functional for the individual by allowing dent’s teachers within the framework of an
them to obtain desired social reinforcers (positive ABCBC reversal design. Results showed that the
reinforcement) such as toys or attention (e.g., function-based plans produced decreases in chal-
Kodak, Northup, & Kelly, 2007), to avoid or lenging behavior, whereas the implementation of
eliminate aversive events (negative reinforce- the nonfunction-based plans did not result in a
ment) such as instructional demands or proximity clinically significant change in behavior.
to other people (e.g., Moore & Edwards, 2003), In a related study, Newcomer and Lewis
and to identify internally occurring (automatic) (2004) compared function-based interventions to
reinforcers (e.g., Kuhn, DeLeon, Fisher, & Wilke, general classroom procedures. Using a multiple
1999). Thus, an assessment process that success- baseline across three participants, the researchers
fully identifies these factors is a powerful tool in demonstrated that function-based interventions
providing the members of an individual’s treat- produced significant reductions in challenging
ment team with the necessary information to behavior when compared to baseline and non-
choose among potentially effective evidence- function-based interventions. Although teachers
based strategies to reduce the individual’s chal- expressed some concern about the time involved
lenging behavior (Horner, 1994). in the assessment process, they rated the use of
function-based interventions very positively.
Other researchers have demonstrated high-social
Why Choose a Function-Based acceptability (social validity) of and a clear pref-
Intervention? erence by practitioners (e.g., school teachers) for
function-based interventions (Ervin, DuPaul,
There have been many demonstrations that the Kern, & Friman, 1998; McLaren & Nelson, 2009;
identification of function of a person’s challenging Nahgahgwon, Umbreit, Liaupsin, & Turton,
behavior can help researchers develop an effective 2010). Given this information, it seems that the
function-based intervention (Kurtz et al., 2003). use of functional assessment and function-based
In addition to functional assessment, federal intervention is the key component in successful
law (Individuals with Disabilities Education treatment planning for the reduction of challeng-
Improvement Act, 2004) also mandates that for ing behavior.
individuals demonstrating challenging behavior, Despite the superiority of function-based
the results should be used to identify potentially interventions to nonfunction-based interventions,
effective, function-based interventions [e.g., func- the understanding of the necessity and/or the skill
tional communication training (FCT), differential of how to move from functional assessment to
reinforcement of other behavior (DRO), or extinc- function-based treatment is still lacking. Several
tion]. This seems logical given that researchers researchers have examined whether function-
have demonstrated that function-based interven- based interventions are typically used by non-
tions are more effective than nonfunction-based or researchers. They also have examined whether
generic interventions. Ingram, Lewis-Palmer, and practitioners who routinely develop and write
Sugai (2005), for example, conducted a compre- behavioral intervention and support plans know
hensive functional assessment for two boys in how to correctly develop and implement function-
sixth grade. Based on the results of the FBA, they based interventions. Two published studies have
then compared the effectiveness of a function- examined whether, given the correct function of
based and a nonfunction-based intervention plan. challenging behavior, teachers would choose
The function-based intervention plan included set- function-based strategies (Scott et al., 2005;
ting event manipulations, preventative antecedent Sugai, Lewis-Palmer, & Hagan, 1998). In both
12 Treatment Planning 197

cases, teachers tended to choose nonfunction- function-based interventions across several


based, contraindicated interventions, such as populations (e.g., Nahgahgwon, et al., 2010;
time-out when behavior was described as Underwood, Umbreit, & Liaupsin, 2009; Wood,
being maintained by escape or avoidance of low- Ferro, Umbreit, & Liaupsin, 2011). The model is
preference activities (e.g., math and activities of designed to facilitate the correct identification of
daily living). In an interesting study, Scott et al. function-based replacement behaviors (e.g.,
(2005) trained key school team members (e.g., appropriately requesting a break), antecedent
school psychologists and teachers) to rigorous arrangements (e.g., visual supports such as a pic-
criteria on functional assessment and function- ture activity schedule), and extinction. Underwood
based intervention planning. Areas trained et al. (2009) for example, examined the efficacy
included restructuring antecedent conditions, of the model for the development of a function-
instructional techniques, consequences for posi- based intervention for three adults with intellec-
tive behavior, and consequences for negative tual disabilities. The function-based interventions
behavior. Thirty-one students were chosen of selected using the decision model was effective
those referred to the school treatment team due to in reducing the challenging behavior of all three
the display of recurring challenging behavior. participants. Wood et al. (2011) examined the
With the guidance of a trained facilitator, the efficacy of the decision model for three preschool
school teams determined the function of the chal- students with intellectual and developmental dis-
lenging behavior based on available assessment abilities. In this study, the student’s teachers
information. The teams then created behavior actively participated in the development of the
intervention plans based on the hypothesized intervention; however, the researchers took the
function of problem behavior for all 31 students. lead on the development. For all three partici-
Experts (i.e., persons who had published in the pants, interventions chosen using the decision
area of functional assessment and intervention) model were very effective in reducing the tar-
also chose interventions based on the same geted challenging behavior. Despite the demon-
hypothesized functions. Experts were more likely strated effectiveness of the decision model,
to identify teaching strategies (e.g., teaching an teachers and family members did not take the
alternative communication response) and were lead in the development of the interventions.
less likely to identify the need to use reductive This, combined with the results of the Scott et al.
strategies (e.g., response cost). School teams (2005) study, suggests the further need for
included an exclusionary strategy such as time- increased education and training for practitioners
out in 70% of the interventions developed. In in function-based intervention planning.
contrast, the experts identified no students who
required the use of exclusionary strategies.
Despite being trained to rigorous criteria, school The Need for Interprofessional
personnel continued to select nonfunction-based Team Planning
reductive interventions. There were some limita-
tions to this study such as discrepancy between In addition to training, effective treatment plan-
choices of interventions by the experts. Despite ning begins with the selection of key team mem-
these limitations, however, this study highlights bers or stakeholders. In an editorial in the Journal
the concern over FBAs leading to well-designed, of Intellectual Disability Research, Holland (2011)
function-based interventions in the “real world.” described a changing landscape in the develop-
These results provide strong support for the need ment of interventions for individuals with intel-
to increase practitioner’s skills in developing and lectual disabilities. Practitioners and researchers
implementing function-based interventions. are increasingly recognizing the need for assess-
Umbreit and his colleagues provide several ment and intervention that are designed by teams
examples of the use of a decision model (see which include professionals from multiple disci-
Umbreit, Ferro, Liaupsin, & Lane, 2007 for com- plines. Although Holland states that there is a
plete description of the model) for developing great need for multidisciplinary evaluations, he
198 D.A. Napolitano et al.

also acknowledges the challenges involved. Given events). Additionally, a thorough understanding of
that both biological and environmental variables positive and negative reinforcement is critical to
may contribute to an individual’s challenging the designing of an effective intervention. As a
behavior, an appropriate, interprofessional team general rule, interventions should target each criti-
should include experts from the fields of behavior cal variable.
analysis, education (both general and special edu- The competing behavior model (O’Neill et al.,
cation teachers), medicine (e.g., nursing, physi- 1997) is a helpful tool to guide the interprofes-
cian, and psychiatry), psychology, support staff sional team in the development of a function-
(e.g., one-to-one paraprofessionals), family mem- based behavior intervention plan (see http://www.
bers, and the individual whose challenging behav- pbis.org/common/pbisresources/tools/BSP_
ior is being targeted for reduction whenever Template.doc).
possible. It provides a nice visual representation of the
An interprofessional team approach to treat- critical variables related to treatment planning.
ment planning can lead to improved outcomes This model identifies (1) setting events, (2) ante-
through systematic evaluation of all variables cedents, (3) replacement behavior, and (4) con-
impacting the individual and a coordinated deliv- sequences as the primary targets for intervention.
ery of care. The work of an interprofessional Interventions should be considered based on the
team is focused on the individual and there is a factors contributing to the challenging behavior,
sense of shared responsibility in the decision- both distal events and immediate contingencies.
making process. This opportunity for shared The interprofessional team should consider inter-
decision making allows for increased focus on ventions to (1) minimize the impact of setting
the integration of disciplines and can lead to events, (2) minimize the impact of antecedents,
innovative interventions for complex problems (3) teach replacement skills, and (4) target the
(Patel, Pratt, & Patel, 2008). consequence events by increasing reinforcement
An interprofessional team is in a unique posi- for appropriate behavior (e.g., differential rein-
tion to evaluate and interpret assessment results forcement) and decreasing reinforcement for tar-
and to use the information obtained to develop geted challenging behavior (i.e., extinction). By
function-based interventions. All decisions identifying these variables, the interprofessional
regarding intervention should be based on the team can focus on conducting problem solving
most comprehensive data available (Matson to identify function-based interventions that
et al., 2003). After all contributing factors have matched the function and the contributing vari-
been identified, the team can start problem solv- ables of the person’s challenging behavior. For
ing to identify an appropriate function-based example, a substitute teacher (setting event) gave
intervention. Generally, the team begins by iden- the student a worksheet to complete (antecedent
tifying effective interventions for setting events stimulus), which resulted in flopping to the floor
or establishing operations. That is, the events (problem behavior), which allowed the student
which increase the likelihood that the reinforcer to successfully escape from having to do the
for challenging behavior will either be more or math (maintaining consequence). From this sce-
less desirable or potent to the individual. nario, the team then determines the desired
behavior, or the behavior in which you would
prefer this student to engage in and its conse-
Treatment Planning quence. For example, the desired behavior is
completing a worksheet and the current conse-
In considering how to develop a function-based quence for that is earning a sticker which is then
intervention, it is important to consider all the fac- placed on their sticker chart. Finally, the team
tors serving to maintain the challenging behavior identifies a behavior that is an adaptive alterna-
as well as factors that may make the reinforcer tive to the problem behavior that will result in the
more or less potent (establishing operations/setting same maintaining consequence as engaging in
12 Treatment Planning 199

the problem behavior. For example, requesting a above. Setting events, however, are often events
break from math (alternative adaptive behavior) that are related to complex situations (e.g., family
will result in a brief escape from the instructional instability) or medical concerns (e.g., mental
demand (maintaining consequence). health challenges or an illness such as chronic ear
Essential to the competing behavior model is infections). When setting events are successfully
to identify strategies that make the challenging identified, they may be the most difficult factors
behavior “irrelevant, ineffective, and inefficient” for the interprofessional team to address success-
(O’Neill et al., 1997, p. 66). Identifying and fully. The primary goal when designing an inter-
arranging setting events and antecedents so that vention for a setting event is to minimize the
the challenging behavior would not occur is key impact it may have on the antecedent and conse-
in making the problem behavior irrelevant. Any quence to the challenging behavior. This is often
situational and contextual rearrangements might difficult to do (e.g., changing family interaction
result in a relatively artificial environment for the styles) and is best accomplished when an inter-
individual, but helpful in reducing challenging professional team including members with vary-
behavior, so it is important to gradually reinsti- ing expertise are involved in the problem-solving
tute any changes that were made gradually and process related to treatment plan development.
systematically over time. Providing an adaptive For example, in the case of increased family
alternative to the challenging behavior that is stressors, such as lack of health insurance and a
easier to obtain or requires less response effort is family member with high medical needs, a social
key in making the challenging behavior inefficient. worker might be prepared to assist the family to
An adaptive alternative might include one rudi- identify solutions. In another example, in the case
mentary task or gesture which immediately of a child experiencing mental health concerns,
results in the same maintaining consequence as having a psychiatrist on the interprofessional
the display of the challenging behavior. Over team may be critical given their expertise in
time the adaptive alternative behavior should be understanding the effects of medication and the
systematically shaped to become more complex empirical research which suggests that for some
and effortful. Withholding the maintaining con- individuals the combination of a medication and
sequence or reinforcer after the display of chal- a behavioral intervention might be more effective
lenging behavior is key to making the behavior than either alone (Napolitano et al., 1999). Other
ineffective. The challenging behavior is placed on examples of setting events might be specific
extinction. The challenging behavior should no genetic conditions (e.g., Prader–Willi syndrome
longer result in the maintaining consequence and and Smith–Magenis syndrome). For genetic con-
the individual should be provided with multiple ditions such as these the inclusion of an expert on
opportunities to engage in the adaptive alterna- the specific condition might be invaluable.
tive behavior, which is easier and more quickly Additionally related to behavioral needs and edu-
results in gaining access to the maintaining con- cational difficulties, an expert may have particu-
sequence. Strategies to ensure that the challeng- larly important contributions in the assessment of
ing behavior becomes irrelevant, inefficient, and learning needs related the genetic condition (e.g.,
ineffective should be listed for each of the four persons with Down syndrome generally show
major areas of the competing behavior pathways higher levels of adaptive behavior in comparison
(setting event, antecedent event, problem behav- to intellectual ability). While we may not be able
ior, and maintaining consequence). to totally eliminate a setting event, understanding
the impact it may have on challenging behavior is
extremely valuable in planning an effective inter-
Choosing a Setting Event Intervention vention. Additionally, minimizing its impact may
make a significant difference in the ability of the
Setting events may sometimes be as simple as a interpersonal team to develop an intervention
child having a substitute teacher, as described which produces a clinically significant effect.
200 D.A. Napolitano et al.

Many researchers have described or investi- care giver for her when her mother was away on
gated the impact of setting events and establish- frequent business trips. Additionally, Matilda’s
grandmother was a great support for her, especially
ing operations on challenging behavior and with school and often supervised her homework
effective interventions to eliminate or reduce the completion. She also was able to help provide
impact on the challenging behavior. Specific tutoring of sorts for math, a subject that Matilda
examples include constipation (e.g., Christensen has not always been as successful in as her other
subjects. Likely related to the recent family stres-
et al., 2009), menstrual pain (Carr & Smith, sors with Matilda’s grandmother passing away, her
1995), psychiatric disorders (e.g., Baker, parents have decided to divorce. This is an addi-
Blumberg, Freeman, & Wieseler, 2002), medica- tional loss for Matilda during an already difficult
tion (e.g., DiCesare, McAdam, Toner, & Varrell, time. Finally, Matilda’s father reported that several
nights a week Matilda has had difficulty sleeping
2005; Northup, Fussilier, Swanson & Borrero, and has been getting up and watching TV in the
1997), and social reinforcers such as attention middle of the night. On the days she has not slept
(e.g., Edrisinha, O’Reilly, Sigafoos, Lancioni, & well, data indicate that she is most likely to engage
Choi, 2010). in the challenging behavior.
O’Reilly, Lacey, and Lancioni (2000) evalu- The example above highlights how complex
ated the degree to which noise affected challeng- challenging behavior can be. The setting events
ing behavior of a child with Williams Syndrome. identified by the school team are significant and
The authors evaluated the effects of noise using a appear to be very difficult, if not impossible to
functional analysis. Three conditions (noise in fully eliminate. This is a great example of when
the background, no noise, and noise plus ear- an interprofessional team is necessary to treat
plugs) were tested. During the demand condition, challenging behavior. Upon review, the interpro-
when noise was present but no earplugs, the child fessional team determined the following inter-
engaged in aggression to avoid instructional vention strategies: The social worker determined
demands. When there was no background noise that it was appropriate to provide some in-home
or noise was present, but the child wore earplugs, family therapy. This would focus on minimizing
he engaged in no or low rates of aggression. the impact of the stressors and specific ways to
Based on the assessment results, the authors rec- support Matilda to better cope when she is expe-
ommended a quiet instructional environment for riencing difficulties. Matilda’s father said that the
the child and guidelines for the use of earplugs to family were very interested and in full support of
attenuate the effect of environmental noise. this idea. The psychiatrist determined that it
Consider the following example: would be appropriate for Matilda to be evaluated
Matilda, a 9-year-old girl, has always enjoyed for depression and ideally receive counseling
school and generally received good grades. focusing on how to better cope with her feelings
Recently, however, she has struggled in most
classes and particularly in math. Although she had
of loss. The teacher identified opportunities for
no history of being “rude” to adults, over the last Matilda to receive tutoring in math at least twice
few months she has been sent to the principal’s per week from a student teacher. Matilda’s father
office (often during math) quite frequently for what identified several consistent times that she could
teachers describe as being “rude, defiant, and
unwilling to listen.” Some school personnel are
do her homework completion when he could pro-
even beginning to wonder if she has Oppositional vide her with encouragement and tutoring. The
Defiant Disorder (ODD). She was referred to the nurse identified that Matilda may be in need to
school’s assessment team to do a functional assess- have a sleep study, but the team determined that
ment and develop an intervention. Based on a func-
tional assessment, which included an analysis of
the behavior analyst should help the family with
the setting events, antecedents, and consequences develop and evaluate a behavioral sleep interven-
maintaining the challenging behavior, it was deter- tion first.
mined that there were several possible setting While these interventions cannot eliminate all
events. First, Matilda’s grandmother, who had
lived with her family, recently passed away sud-
concerns (i.e., grandmother passing away and
denly. Matilda was very close with her grand- parents divorcing), they are designed to minimize
mother and her grandmother had been a secondary the impact of these events on Matilda’s active
12 Treatment Planning 201

participation in her educational program. Through Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, Smith, and Mazaleski
the active participation of a carefully constructed (1993) demonstrated the efficacy of another ante-
interprofessional team Matilda is much better cedent intervention, noncontingent reinforcement
able to cope and more actively participate in (NCR) on the self-injurious behavior (SIB) (e.g.,
her life. head hitting and head banging) of three adult
women with intellectual disabilities. A pretreat-
ment functional analysis was conducted for each
Choosing an Antecedent Intervention of the participants and identified attention as a
factor-maintaining SIB. Two interventions were
Similar to the goal for setting event intervention, then compared, attention provided noncontin-
our goal for antecedent intervention is to mini- gently (NCR) and attention provided according
mize their impact, modify them, or eliminate to a DRO schedule. The results obtained showed
them altogether. They also can provide increased that both interventions were equally successful in
structure in the environment (Kern & Clemens, reducing SIB. However, the authors argue that
2007). It is critical that the event preceding or set- NCR was the better intervention option due to a
ting the occasion for the challenging behavior reduced likelihood that the individual will have
have been correctly identified to develop a suc- an extinction burst and the potential ease of
cessful antecedent intervention. Many research- implementation.
ers have demonstrated methods for identifying The clinical efficacy of many other antecedent
effective antecedent interventions to decrease the function-based interventions has been demon-
likelihood that challenging behavior will occur. strated in the published literature including
For example if the antecedent is deprivation from matched stimuli, demand fading, and choice.
attention, how might a situation in which minimal Piazza, Adelinis, Hanley, Goh, and Delia, (2000),
attention is available be made less problematic for example, demonstrated that matched stimuli
for the individual? If the antecedent is academic was effective in reducing automatically main-
demands (tasks which require hand writing), is tained challenging behavior (climbing, saliva
there a way to decrease how aversive these are to manipulation, and hand mouthing) for three indi-
the individual? A number of researchers have viduals with severe developmental disabilities. In
identified interventions that, when the function of another study, Demand fading, an antecedent
the challenging behavior is correctly identified, intervention for escape maintained challenging
can be very successful in minimizing the impact behavior was demonstrated to be effective in
of the antecedent on the challenging behavior. reducing destructive behavior when combined
Much research has been conducted on the with escape extinction and differential reinforce-
efficacy of antecedent manipulations on the ment (Piazza, Moes, & Fisher, 1996). Choice was
reduction of challenging behavior. In one exam- demonstrated to be an effective intervention on
ple, Mace and Belfiore (1990) demonstrated the increasing assignment completion and decreasing
efficacy of behavioral momentum to increase noncompliance to complete school assignments
compliance and decrease escape-maintained ste- (Stenhoff, Davey, & Linugaris/Kraft, 2008).
reotypy in a woman with an intellectual disability. All of these interventions demonstrate that
A functional analysis indicated that the stereotypy manipulation of the antecedent variables can be an
was maintained by escape from demands. The effective interventions both alone and in combina-
authors used a high-probability demand sequence tion with other interventions in the reduction of
followed by a low-probability demand with challenging behavior. Now consider our example
extinction to increase compliance to demands and of Matilda, described above. The antecedent condi-
decrease stereotypy. This intervention was suc- tion which routinely set the occasion for her prob-
cessful and demonstrated how manipulating ante- lem behavior was instructional demands to complete
cedent conditions can reduce repetitive escape academic assignments, particularly math. The
maintained stereotypic behavior. function identified by Matilda’s interprofessional
202 D.A. Napolitano et al.

team was social-negative reinforcement in the form deadline) or earn (e.g., a monetary bonus). The
of escape or avoidance of academic demands. same concept applies to challenging behavior. An
Based on this information, several of the anteced- individual who can access what they want through
ent interventions described above might be appro- the display of challenging behavior will not typi-
priate for Matilda’s interdisciplinary team to cally exert greater effort to use a new skill, despite
evaluate. Demand fading, for example, might be an it yielding the same result as their challenging
appropriate intervention to consider. Using this behavior. Therefore, we must take this into con-
approach, Matilda is initially asked to complete a sideration when writing a treatment plan.
smaller number of tasks (e.g., single-digit addition Sufficient reinforcement must also be avail-
problems), then is slowly prompted to complete an able for the replacement behavior and at a greater
increased number of math problems contingent on rate than for challenging behavior, in the natural
success. Providing Matilda with the opportunity to environment. An individual may attempt to use a
choose among several math worksheets might also replacement behavior if the reinforcer associated
be effective in providing her with the opportunity it is more potent than the reinforcer associated
to have more control over the aversive instructional with the replacement skill.
situation. Take the following example into consideration:
A child runs out of the classroom for a break. The
child is taught to raise his hand and ask for a break
Choosing Replacement/Teaching when he wants one. He raises his hand appropri-
Strategy/Strategies ately and asks for a break; however, the teacher
rarely calls on him. The child gives up on raising
his hand and goes back to running out of the
While antecedent interventions are very impor- classroom.
tant and effective, they are typically not highly
effective in the absence of teaching the individual Although the replacement behavior may not
a new skill. It is important that the new skill be available in all situations and at all times, it is
taught allows the individual to obtain the same important to teach something that will be rein-
reinforcer which they receive through the display forced frequently during the learning phase of
of challenging behavior to promote their long- implementation. From there, programming for
term success (Carr & Durand, 1985). Additionally, generalization and thinning of the reinforcement
the challenging behavior likely has a history of schedule can occur after the individual is using
being extremely functional for the individual. the replacement behavior consistently.
That is, it has served them well to this point; FCT is one of the most common replacement
therefore, a replacement behavior that allows the behavior interventions. The alternative behavior
individual to gain access to the same or similar that is reinforced during FCT is a communication
reinforcer has the greatest likelihood of working. behavior designed to allow the individual to more
For example, a child that throws a pencil at the efficiently access the reinforcer maintaining their
teacher to get her attention can be reinforced for challenging behavior. An individual can commu-
the absence of throwing pencils; however, what nicate using a variety of modulates including sign
adaptive alternative will the student use to get the language, pictures communication system such
teacher’s attention now? Without a better option as The Picture Exchange Communication System
or skill to communicate their need the child may (PECS), and vocal statements (e.g., talk to me
resort to another challenging behavior. please). Durand and Carr (1991) evaluated the
Some important considerations in teaching effects of FCT on reducing the challenging
replacement skills are first that it must require behavior (e.g., aggression and SIB) of three boys
less response effort than the challenging behav- between 9 and 12 years of age. All three partici-
ior. Most people would not work an additional pants had an intellectual disability and engaged
40 h in a week unless there was something they in frequent challenging behavior. For two partici-
were trying to avoid (e.g., missing a big payroll pants, a functional analysis indicated that the
12 Treatment Planning 203

reinforcer was to escape demands, while the Choosing Consequence Interventions


results of the analysis for the third participant
identified both escape from demands and atten- To complete the treatment, based on the results
tion as reinforcers for problem behavior. After of the functional assessment or analysis, it is
baseline observation in the natural environment, important to consider how to reduce the likeli-
participants were taught to either request help or hood the reinforcer for challenging behavior is
attention, based on the results of the functional available and how to motivate the individual to
analysis. The results indicated clinically want to engage in the desired and replacement
significant reductions in challenging behavior for behaviors.
all three participants. Two years post the initial
intervention, observation indicated that one par- Extinction
ticipant’s challenging behavior had returned to After the interprofessional team has identified the
the baseline rate. When a booster session was function of the individual’s challenging behavior,
conducted to help this participant more clearly they should include extinction in the plan.
communicate his requests, challenging behavior Extinction is the process of withholding the rein-
again reduced significantly. forcer maintaining a student’s challenging behav-
Many researchers have examined FCT since ior. To be successfully implemented, this
the initial studies by Carr and Durand (1985) and intervention requires an educational team to con-
Durand and Carr (1991). Fisher, et al. (1993), for duct a comprehensive functional assessment or
example, examined the necessity of extinction analysis. If you do not have a clear understanding
and a punishment procedure combined with FCT of why the person is displaying challenging
to reduce challenging behavior. Results were behavior, you cannot successfully terminate the
mixed, indicating that some participants were relationship between the student’s behavior and
successful with FCT alone, some with what he or she gets out of the behavior. For exam-
FCT + extinction, and for some participants the ple, if an individual’s challenging behavior occurs
inclusion of a punishment component was a nec- to obtain attention (positive reinforcement) from
essary to produce a clinically significant reduc- their teacher, extinction would involve withhold-
tion. More recently, Worsdell, Iwata, Hanley, ing attention contingent on challenging behavior.
Thompson, and Kahng (2000) determined that In contrast, if a student displays challenging
errors in the FCT response might result in its behavior to get out of difficult academic work
ineffectiveness as an intervention; however, the (negative reinforcement), extinction would
results also suggested that small errors in the involve preventing them from using their chal-
implementation of extinction (e.g., occasional lenging behavior to get out of their work. This
reinforcement of an individual’s challenging might involve continuing to prompt a person to
behavior) may not compromise the effectiveness engage in work despite his or her continued resis-
of FCT. tance or attempts to get out of the activity. The
Consider again our example of Matilda. It was main advantage of extinction is that it is an inter-
determined that in the context of numerous set- vention that is based on the specific reason why
ting events, given the demand to complete assign- an individual displays his or her challenging
ments, particularly math, she was likely to engage behavior. Extinction also has several possible
in inappropriate comments (e.g., being “rude”) limitations. First, an individual may experience
for negative reinforcement (i.e., to avoid her an extinction burst. These bursts are com-
work). If we were to develop an intervention for monly occurring phenomena in which the rate or
this using FCT we might teach her to request help intensity of a person’s challenging behavior may
when she is struggling with a particular problem initially increase before decreasing (Lerman,
or to request a short break when she is feeling the Iwata, & Wallace, 1999). Such behavior can be
task is too difficult. very problematic for parents or teachers because
204 D.A. Napolitano et al.

they may not be able to successfully work with a The use of extinction for our example, Matilda,
student during this difficult time. This may be while difficult, is likely an important component
especially true if the individual’s increase in chal- of an effective intervention. The interprofessional
lenging behavior jeopardizes their educational or team and especially the educational professionals
residential placement or occurs in public places on the team need to determine whether the possi-
such as the grocery store or on field trips. ble extinction burst from not allowing her to leave
Additionally, placing one challenging behavior the classroom might be able to be tolerated by the
on extinction sometime results in an individual educational team members who work with her in
displaying forms of challenging of behavior the classroom. The implementation of extinction
that they have not typically displayed in the past might be necessary to promote Matilda’s under-
(i.e., extinction-induced resurgence; Lieving, standing that making inappropriate statements to
Hagopian, Long, & O’Connor, 2004). Research- her teacher will no longer be effective in allowing
ers, for example, have shown that placing SIB her to leave the classroom. Extinction, combined
such as hand biting on extinction can result in an with the initial lessening of demands and com-
increase in aggression (Magee & Ellis, 2000). munication instruction to obtain help and/or a
Finally, it might not be realistic to expect people break should be effective in eliminating the
to withhold the reinforcer for a behavior. For extinction burst. If the team determines that a
example, it might not be possible to physically potential extinction burst cannot be handled safely
prompt a child who is very resistant and strong to in the classroom, alternative strategies should be
complete their math and not get out of their chair. identified prior to implementation of the plan. For
The failure to successfully implement extinction example, since math is the primary work task
can result in a student’s challenging behavior Matilda is trying to avoid, can she go to a room
being strengthened. This process of accidentally with few distractions (e.g., less people, no mate-
strengthening behavior is referred to as intermit- rials associated with high-preference activities
tent reinforcement by behavior analysts (Kendall, in clear view) to complete her work contingent on
1974). Due to the concerns described above, challenging behavior, rather than to the office?
extinction in real-life settings is very seldom used
in isolation; instead it should be used in combina- Differential Reinforcement
tion with other behavioral treatment strategies Differential reinforcement involves providing
often differential reinforcement of other or alter- reinforcement for a functionally alternative behav-
native behavior (Lerman & Iwata, 1995). Iwata, ior or the absence of a challenging behavior for a
Pace, Cowdery, and Milternberger (1994) con- predetermined interval of time while minimizing
ducted a study which illustrated both the clinical reinforcement for the challenging behavior of
usefulness of extinction and the need to match concern. Clinical experience suggests that differ-
the extinction procedure used to the function of ential reinforcement may be particularly useful
the individual’s challenging behavior. Three per- when people cannot implement extinction per-
sons with developmental disabilities who engaged fectly. There are two commonly used differential
in head banging participated in the study. A pre- reinforcement procedures. Differential reinforce-
treatment functional analysis demonstrated that ment of other behavior (DRO) involves the
the head banging of the three participants was delivery of a positive consequence (reinforcer)
maintained by different consequences (i.e., social contingent on the absence of a challenging behav-
attention from other people, escape or avoidance ior during a predetermined period of time (e.g.,
behavior, or automatic reinforcement). Two 15 min, 1 h). Differential reinforcement of alter-
forms of extinction were implemented for each native behavior (DRA) involves the delivery of a
participant’s head banging and only the form that positive consequence contingent on the display of
matched the results of the behavioral function an alternative response such as compliance or
identified in the functional analysis was effective appropriate communication (e.g., asking your
in reducing their SIB. teacher to talk to you about a topic in which you
12 Treatment Planning 205

have lots of interest, such as baseball). Differential be instances where the use of punishment-based
reinforcement procedures should be designed to intervention components may be necessary com-
address the function of an individual’s challeng- ponents of successful interventions (van Houten
ing behavior. That is, the reinforcer identified et al., 1988). Interventions may be more effective
through the functional assessment process to be with the inclusion of punishment procedures
maintaining the individual’s challenging behavior and may even be preferred to non-punishment-
should be used to reinforce either the absence of based interventions by the individual participat-
challenging behavior or the display of an alterna- ing in the intervention (Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, &
tive behavior. For example, in the case of a child Maglieri, 2005). Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan,
who becomes upset when he or she is asked to do Acquisito, and LeBlanc (1998) summarized the
their spelling, a low-preference activity, DRA use of FCT to increase replacement skills for
might consist of having his one-on-one parapro- challenging behavior for 21 individuals.
fessional staff provide him with token each time Functional analyses were conducted for each par-
he spells a word without throwing his pencil or ticipant. Ten participants engaged in challenging
hitting his desks. After earning the required behavior for positive reinforcement in the form of
tokens, the child then would have the opportunity attention (nine) or tangible items (one), seven
to exchange his tokens for the opportunity for a participants engaged in challenging behaviors to
break and to engage in a high-preference activity, escape demands (negative reinforcement), four
preferably one identified as high-preference by a had mixed results (problem behavior was main-
preference assessment. tained by both positive and negative reinforce-
ment). While the intervention was very successful
Punishment for all participants, without the use of extinction
Punishment involves the contingent delivery of an or punishment, FCT was not successful for any
item or event that a student find to be at least participant. FCT with extinction was imple-
mildly aversive or the removal of a preferred item mented 25 times across 19 participants (some
or event (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). participants challenging behavior was maintained
Punishment procedures such as time out and by more than one consequence). FCT combined
response cost, while widely used in natural envi- with extinction initially reduced challenging
ronments (Peterson & Martens, 1995), can be con- behavior by 90% for 11 of the 25 participants.
troversial particularly with individuals perceived The addition of a punishment procedure was nec-
as being highly vulnerable (e.g., persons with essary for 14 participants to reduce the challeng-
intellectual disabilities). Accurate identification of ing behavior by 90%. This result was particularly
the function of challenging behavior increases the evident when demands were increased and rein-
likelihood that effective, non-punishment-based forcement was delayed.
interventions should be used to decrease the likeli- When an interprofessional team is considering
hood that a punishment procedure is necessary the use of punishment-based interventions some
(Pelios, Morren, Tesch, & Axelrod, 1999). precautions should be taken. First, the interven-
Unfortunately, recent research (i.e., Scott et al., tion should not be contraindicated to the function
2005 described above) has indicated that despite of the challenging behavior. That is, if the chal-
identification of function, educators still chose lenging behavior is maintained by negative rein-
punishment-based strategies. Additionally, these forcement (e.g., escaping instructional demands)
strategies were often contraindicated of the behav- the intervention chosen should not also remove
ioral function (e.g., time out for negatively rein- the individual from an environment in which the
forced challenging behavior). demands are provided. This would likely compete
While caution should be used when consider- with the other interventions being implemented.
ing a punishment-based strategy, they should not Additionally, adequate reinforcement should be
be excluded when appropriate. Interprofessional available to maintain the desired or competing
team members should recognize that there may behaviors (Lerman & Vorndran, 2002).
206 D.A. Napolitano et al.

(e.g., the individual is displaying problem behav-


Developing a Function-Based ior to make other people mad). This can lead to
Treatment Plan caregivers wanting to implement nonfunctional,
consequence-based interventions to establish
Once the interprofessional team has determined
“control.” Second, people might believe that ran-
the interventions to be implemented, a treatment
dom events that are correlated with the occur-
plan must be developed. When developing an
rence of challenging behavior even when the
intervention plan, the interprofessional team must
results of functional assessment or analysis has
give careful thought to who will be using the plan
determined there is no relationship between these
and implementing the strategies outlined. Is the
events and an individual’s problem behavior. For
plan to be carried out by parents or professionals
example, an individual who displays challenging
with training in applied behavior analysis, with
behavior often receives attention from others in
little or no training or both? The interprofessional
the form of a shocked or angry verbal reaction or
team must carefully tailor the strategies described
reprimand. These forms of contingent verbal
to match the level of knowledge of the people
attention might result in people believing that
who will be implementing them to ensure ease of
challenging behavior that is actually maintained
implementation. Functional treatment plans
by social-negative reinforcement in the form of
should be easy to read yet include all the key
escape or avoidance behavior is maintained by
components described above. This includes
social-positive reinforcement in the form of atten-
highlighting important aspects, pointing out
tion from other people (Thompson & Iwata,
details in a concise manner, and using a consis-
2007). Finally, some people might believe in
tent familiar format (see http://www.pbis.org/
faulty or nonevidence-based conceptualization of
common/pbisresources/tools/BSP_Template.doc
problem behavior (e.g., that escape-maintained
for one example of a suggested format for a
behavior is due to a child’s deficits related to the
behavior intervention and support plans).
processing of information, stereotypic behavior is
Comprehensive intervention plans should
due to sensory disregulation). Understanding why
include the following components:
the behavior is occurring, or what the function of
What is contributing to this behavior? (Setting
the behavior is, may help the caregiver intervene
events)
more objectively. For example, if a child yells at
What happens leading up to this behavior, and
his teacher to get out of science work specifically,
what can I do to help with that? (Antecedent
giving the child a math paper to do in place of sci-
interventions)
ence will not be appropriate. Understandably, this
What does the behavior look like? (Behavioral
might be very frustrating for the science teacher
definition)
though, who might interpret the behavior as aimed
How do I best respond? (Consequence inter-
at him, rather than occasioned by the class topic.
ventions)
This again is an additional rationale for inclusion
of all members of the interprofessional team in
assessment and treatment planning.
Importance of Clarifying Function The professionals and caregivers that imple-
and Procedures ment a plan might not have the same knowledge
about variables affecting the challenging behavior
Often the function of problem behavior is misun- as the person writing the plan. This factor illus-
derstood by professionals, parents, and support trates why it is important to state other setting
staff who interact with the person on a daily basis. events that may be affecting the behavior, in accor-
There are several common misinterpretations of dance with the interprofessional team perspectives.
problem behavior. First, people might believe Medication side effects, physical pains, trauma, or
that the individual displaying problem behavior other nonsocial factors affecting the behavior may
is intentionally attempting to manipulate them provide the caregiver with additional understanding
12 Treatment Planning 207

of the factors which set the occasion for a par- and teachers, should be encouraged to be active
ticipant’s problem behavior. This may lead to participants in the intervention planning process.
decreased frustration and increased compliance
with the implementation of the intervention.
Stating this briefly in the beginning of the func- Behavioral Skills Training
tional treatment plan will set the stage for the rest for Implementation
of the plan, much as the setting events of the indi-
vidual set the stage for the problem behavior. Following caregiver feedback to the clinician
writing the plan, and revising when appropriate,
teaching can begin. Plan implementation can be
Defining and Measuring Behavior broken down into the steps used in Behavioral
Skills Training (BST) (see Miltenberger, 2008
During the assessment process, a definition of the for a complete description). These steps include
challenging behavior was developed and methods modeling, instruction, rehearsal, and feedback.
for measurement created (see Chap. 7). The goal BST has been demonstrated to be an effective
of a functional treatment plan is for all caregivers strategy in teaching others to implement a new
to respond in a standardized way and to modify skill. Some examples of skills taught using BST
the behavior. As such, all persons implementing include safety skills (Himle, Miltenberger,
the plan should be able to identify when a behav- Gatheridge, & Flessner, 2004), and preventing
ior is occurring and when it is not. Defining gun play (Miltenberger et al., 2004), instructional
behavior in an observable way assists in reaching skills at a community setting (Wood, Luiselli, &
this goal of treatment planning. This should be Harchik, 2007), and discrete trial teaching by
stated clearly in the beginning of the plan to focus staff (Sarakoff & Sturmey, 2004). Using a multi-
the attention of the behavioral intervention. ple-baseline design, Sarakoff and Sturmey (2004)
It is important that data are collected prior to demonstrated that BST was a useful strategy for
the intervention (baseline) and during the inter- teaching staff to implement discrete trial teach-
vention. This is to determine whether the inter- ing. Three staff members providing support in the
vention is successfully reducing the challenging participant’s group home were trained to imple-
behavior. It is also ideal to empirically evaluate ment ten key components of discrete trial teach-
the intervention by briefly withdrawing the inter- ing (e.g., eye contact, providing immediate
vention then reinstating it to determine whether reinforcement for correct responding) to 100%
the intervention is what is causing behavior accuracy. All four components of BST were used,
change. and the discrete-trial teaching skills were acquired
quickly. Sarakoff and Sturmey note that the com-
ponents of the BST intervention package have
Implementation not been evaluated separately to determine
whether it is necessary to use all of the compo-
It is important in beginning a new plan that the nents; however, given the data on the effective-
person(s) responsible for the daily implementa- ness of the intervention package and responsible
tion have received some training on basic behav- costs associated with it makes good sense to use
ioral principles, factors that maintain challenging all the components when teaching people how to
behavior, and the purpose of function-based inter- implement a behavior intervention plan.
vention. Additionally, clinical experience sug- To use BST to train staff to implement a
gests that the more professionals and stakeholders behavior intervention plan, the staff should first
who can participate in the assessment and treat- have an opportunity to read the description of
ment planning process, the more likely the inter- function, the rationale for writing the plan, and
professional team will be invested in the success the intervention steps. Modeling the intervention
of the plan. Team members, including parents components, by the clinician writing the plan,
208 D.A. Napolitano et al.

other members of the interprofessional team or parents and teachers are trained to preestablished
another highly trained staff member is the next criteria, they can use the process of modeling,
step. This step is essential to further problem instruction, rehearsal, and feedback to teach sec-
solve any idiosyncratic aspects of the plan. During ondary or new caregivers (line staff, grandpar-
the modeling phase, it is essential that the learner ents, or support staff). This “train the trainer”
(caregiver) is fully dedicated to learning the plan model is often necessary when a large number of
and is not distracted by other factors (e.g., work persons will be implementing the intervention.
responsibilities). Once the opportunity for initial Consistency of implementation is critical and all
observational learning has occurred, additional persons charged with supporting the individual
modeling may need to occur numerous times, should be trained to the same criteria. Additionally,
until the key stakeholders can implement the plan it is important to have the primary trainers or cli-
fluently with few errors. The clinician must be nicians do fidelity checks with all persons imple-
aware throughout the modeling process that both menting the intervention. This may prevent
correct and incorrect procedures can be learned problems with the plan due to lack of fidelity.
by the caregiver, and therefore there must be a
high degree of consistency and accuracy in
implementation. Modifications
Continuing to have the written plan to refer to
while the modeling is taking place will help with The need to modify a behavior plan should not be
the instruction. By giving the caregiver a visual thought of as a setback, but rather an opportunity
representation of the intervention while modeling to clarify the details of intervention components
(written instruction), as well as providing instruc- and to conduct additional problem solving by the
tion and feedback after modeling has occurred, the interprofessional team. First, a check for fidelity
caregiver will have multiple opportunities to ask of implementation should be conducted. If the
questions about how to implement the plan cor- plan is being implemented correctly, without
rectly. The caregiver should be encouraged to anticipated results, the potency of the reinforcers
rehearse implementation of the plan soon after the being delivered should be tested (e.g., has the
modeling has occurred. The clinician should be individual become satiated with the reinforcers
present at the time of rehearsal, to provide feed- used?). This is particularly true if a plan initially
back immediately. Feedback, including praise for reduced challenging behavior, but is no longer as
correct implementation and correcting errors, effective. In the absence of problems with fidelity
should occur immediately after rehearsal has and reinforcers, the interprofessional team should
occurred. Predetermined criteria for successful problem solve reasons for the lack of success (e.g.,
implementation should be determined prior to has there been a change in important variables,
BST and all steps should continue to be used until such as change in family situation?) and consider
the criteria are met. Finally, BST should also be whether additional assessment is necessary.
implemented in the natural environment, when- The published behavioral literature has repeat-
ever possible, to provide opportunities for model- edly demonstrated the clinical efficacy of a func-
ing, instruction, rehearsal, and feedback in the tion-based approach to reduce challenging
environment in which the challenging behavior behavior. Crafting a successful, individualized
routinely occurs. intervention is best accomplished through an
interprofessional team problem-solving model
which uses function-based intervention planning.
Additional Caregiver Training Through careful problem solving, the interpro-
fessional team may successfully address the vari-
A similar process can be used to train multiple ous factors which occasion an individual’s
caregivers in implementing behavior intervention problem behavior such as setting events and
plans. Once lead or primary caregivers such as maintaining consequences.
12 Treatment Planning 209

Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto,


References J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of func-
tional communication training with and without
Baker, D. J., Blumberg, E. R., Freeman, R., & Wieseler, extinction and punishment. Journal of Applied
N. A. (2002). Can psychiatric disorders be seen as Behavior Analysis, 31, 211–235.
establishing operations? Integrating applied behavior Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
analysis and psychiatry. Mental Health Aspects of Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
Developmental Disabilities, 5, 118–124. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
Carr, E. G. (1994). Emerging themes in the functional Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K.
analysis of problem behavior. Journal of Applied A. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for
Behavior Analysis, 27, 393–399. punishment and extinction components of function-
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior based interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior
problems through functional communication training. Analysis, 38, 51–65.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Himle, M. B., Miltenberger, R. G., Flessner, C., &
Carr, E. G., & Smith, C. E. (1995). Biological setting events Gatheridge, B. (2004). Teaching safety skills to chil-
for self-injury. Mental Retardation and Developmental dren to prevent gun play. Journal of Applied Behavior
Disabilities Research Reviews, 1, 94–98. Analysis, 37, 1–9.
Christensen, T. J., Ringdahl, J. E., Bosch, J. J., Falcomata, Holland, T. (2011). The future for research in intellectual
T. S., Luke, J. R., & Andelman, M. S. (2009). disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,
Constipaiton associated with self-injurious and aggres- 55, 1–3.
sive behavior exhibited by a child diagnosed with Horner, R. H. (1994). Functional assessment: Contributions
autism. Education and Treatment of Children, 32, and future directions. Journal of Applied Behavior
89–103. Analysis, 27, 401–404.
Cooper, J0., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Ingram, K., Lewis-Palmer, T., & Sugai, G. (2005).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle Function-based intervention planning: Comparing the
River: Pearson Education. effectiveness of FBA function-based and non-func-
Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M., tion-based intervention plans. Journal of Positive
Northup, J., Cigrand, K., et al. (1992). Brief functional Behavior Interventions, 7, 224–236.
assessment techniques to evaluate aberrant behavior in Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
an outpatient setting: A summary of 79 cases. Journal Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis
of Applied Behavior analysis, 25, 713–721. of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Dicesare, A., McAdam, D. B., Toner, A., & Varrell, J. 27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention
(2005). The effects of methylphenidate on a functional in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982).
analysis of disruptive behavior: A replication and Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
extension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An analy-
125–128. sis of procedural form and function. Journal of Applied
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional commu- Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144.
nication training to reduce challenging behavior: Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act
Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal of 2004, Pub. L. No. 108-446 (2004).
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 251–264. Kendall, S. B. (1974). Preference for intermittent rein-
Edrisinha, C., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., & forcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Choi, H. Y. (2010). Influence of motivating operations Behavior, 21, 463–473.
and discriminative stimuli on challenging behavior Kennedy, C. H., & Souza, G. (1995). Functional analysis
maintained by positive reinforcement. Research in and treatment of eye poking. Journal of Applied
Developmental Disabilities, 32, 836–845. Behavior Analysis, 28, 27–37.
Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., Stricker, J., Galensky, Kern, L., & Clemens, N. H. (2007). Antecedent strategies
T. L., & Garlinghouse, M. (2000). Functional assess- to promote appropriate classroom behavior. Psychology
ment and intervention for challenging behaviors in the in the Schools, 44, 65–75.
classroom by general classroom teachers. Journal of Kern, L., Childs, K. E., Dunlap, G., Clarke, S., & Falk, G.
Positive Behavior Interventions, 2, 85–97. D. (1994). Using assessment-based curricular inter-
Ervin, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., Kern, L., & Friman, P. C. vention to improve the classroom behavior of a student
(1998). Classroom-based functional and adjunctive with emotional and behavioral challenges. Journal of
assessments: Proactive approaches to intervention Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 7–19.
selection for adolescents with attention deficit hyper- Kodak, T., Northup, J., & Kelly, M. E. (2007). An evalua-
activity disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, tion of the types of attention that maintain problem
31, 65–78. behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40,
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., 167–171.
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- Kuhn, D. E., DeLeon, I. G., Fisher, W. W., & Wilke, A. E.
munication training with and without extinction and (1999). Clarifying an ambiguous functional analysis
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, with matched and mismatched extinction procedures.
26, 23–36. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 99–102.
210 D.A. Napolitano et al.

Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F., and Developmental Disabilities Research and Reviews,
O’Connor, J. T., Paclawsky, T. R., et al. (2003). 5, 322–344.
Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious Newcomer, L. L., & Lewis, T. J. (2004). Functional
behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases. behavioral assessment: An investigation of assessment
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219. reliability and effectiveness of function-based inter-
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1995). Prevalence of the ventions. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
extinction burst and its attenuation during treatment. Disorders, 12, 168–181.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 93–94. Northup, J., Fussilier, I., Swanson, V., & Borrero, J.
Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999). (1997). An evaluation of methylphenidate as a poten-
Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting and tial establishing operation for some common class-
aggression during the treatment of self-injurious room reinforcers. Journal of Applied Behavior
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, Analysis, 30, 615–625.
1–8. O’Neill, R. E., Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Sprague, J. R.,
Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the status of Storey, K., & Newton, J. S. (1997). Functional assess-
knowledge for using punishment: Implications for ment and program development for problem behavior:
treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied A practical handbook (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove: Brooks/
Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464. Cole.
Lieving, G. A., Hagopian, L. P., Long, E. S., & O’Connor, O’Reilly, M. F., Lacey, C., & Lancioni, G. E. (2000).
J. (2004). Response-class hierarchies and resurgence Assessment of the influence of background noise on
of severe problem behavior. The Psychological Record, escape-maintained problem behavior and pain behav-
54, 621–634. ior in a child with Williams syndrome. Journal of
Mace, F. C., & Belfiore, P. (1990). Behavioral momentum Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 511–514.
in the treatment of escape-motivated stereotypy. Patel, D. R., Pratt, H. D., & Patel, N. D. (2008). Team
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 507–514. processes and team care for children with develop-
Magee, S. K., & Ellis, J. (2000). Extinction effects during mental disabilities. Pediatric Clinics of North America,
the assessment of multiple problem behaviors. Journal 55, 1375–1390.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 313–316. Pelios, L., Morren, J., Tesch, D., & Axelrod, S. (1999).
Matson, J. L., Mayville, S. B., & Laud, R. B. (2003). A The impact of functional analysis methodology on
system of assessment for adaptive behavior, social treatment choice for self-injurious and aggressive
skills, behavioral function, medication side-effects, behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,
and psychiatric disorders. Research in Developmental 185–195.
Disabilities, 24, 75–81. Peterson, F. M., & Martens, B. K. (1995). A comparison
McCord, B. E., Thompson, R. J., & Iwata, B. A. (2001). of behavioral interventions reported in treatment stud-
Functional analysis and treatment of self-injury asso- ies and programs for adults with developmental dis-
ciated with transitions. Journal of Applied Behavior abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 16,
Analysis, 34, 195–210. 27–41.
McLaren, E. M., & Nelson, C. M. (2009). Using func- Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H., &
tional behavior assessment to develop behavior inter- Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of
ventions for students in Head Start. Journal of Positive matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic
Behavior Interventions, 11, 3–21. reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Miltenberger, R. G. (2008). Behavior modification: 33, 13–27.
Principles and procedures (4th ed.). Belmont: Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996).
Thompson/Wadsworth. Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior and
Miltenberger, R. G., Flessner, C., Gatheridge, B. J., demand fading in the treatment of escape-maintained
Satterlund, M., & Egemo, K. (2004). Evaluation of destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
behavioral skills training to prevent gun play in chil- Analysis, 29, 569–572.
dren. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, Sarakoff, R. A., & Sturmey, P. (2004). The effects of
513–516. behavioral skills training on staff implementation of
Moore, J. W., & Edwards, R. P. (2003). An analysis of discrete-trial teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior
aversive stimuli in classroom demand contexts. Analysis, 37, 535–538.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 339–348. Scott, T. M., McIntyre, J., Liaupsin, C., Nelson, M.,
Nahgahgwon, K. N., Umbreit, C., Liaupsin, C. J., & Conroy, M., & Payne, L. D. (2005). An examination of
Turton, A. M. (2010). Function-based planning for the relationship between functional behavior assess-
young children at risk for emotional and behavioral ment and selected intervention. Journal of Positive
disorders. Education and Treatment of Children, 33, Behavior Interventions, 7, 205–215.
537–559. Stenhoff, D. M., Davey, B. J., & Linugaris/Kraft, B.
Napolitano, D. A., Jack, S. L., Sheldon, J. B., Williams, D. (2008). The effects of choice on assignment comple-
C., McAdam, D. B., & Schroeder, S. R. (1999). Drug- tion and percent correct by a high school student with
behavior interactions in persons with mental retarda- a learning disability. Education and treatment of
tion and developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation Children, 31, 203–211.
12 Treatment Planning 211

Sugai, G., Lewis-Palmer, T., & Hagan, S. (1998). Using Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G.,
functional assessments to develop behavior support & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in the
plans. Preventing School Failure, 43, 6–13. treatment of attention-maintained self-injurious behav-
Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2007). A comparison of ior: Noncontingent reinforcement and differential
outcomes from descriptive and functional analysis of reinforcement of other behavior. Journal of Applied
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21.
Analysis, 40, 33–338. Wood, A. L., Luiselli, J. K., & Harchik, A. E. (2007).
Umbreit, J., Ferro, J., Liaupsin, C., & Lane, K. (2007). Training instructional skills with paraprofessional ser-
Functional behavioral assessment and function-based vice providers at a community-based habilitation set-
intervention: An effective, practical approach. Upper ting. Behavior Modification, 31, 847–855.
Saddle River: Prentice-Hall. Wood, B. K., Ferro, J., Umbreit, J., & Liaupsin, C. J.
Underwood, M. A., Umbreit, J., & Liaupsin, C. (2009). (2011). Addressing the challenging behavior of young
Efficacy of a systematic process for designing func- children through systematic function-based interven-
tion-based interventions for adults in a community set- tion. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 30,
ting. Education and Training in Developmental 221–232.
Disabilities, 44, 25–38. Worsdell, A. S., Iwata, B. A., Hanley, G. P., Thompson, R.
Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J. S., Favell, J. E., H., & Kahng, S. W. (2000). Effects of continuous and
Foxx, R. N., Iwata, B. A., et al. (1988). The right to intermittent reinforcement for problem behavior dur-
effective behavioral treatment. The Behavior Analyst, ing functional communication training. Journal of
11, 111–114. Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 167–179.
Ethical Issues and Considerations
13
Alan Poling, Jennifer L. Austin,
Stephanie M. Peterson, Amanda Mahoney,
and Marc Weeden

Functional behavioral assessment (FBA) refers their functions, that is, the reinforcers that main-
to a range of methods designed to identify the tain those behaviors. FBA is one of several ways
environmental variables that control problem- of collecting information about clients, and pro-
atic behaviors. Methods for revealing these fessional organizations such as the American
variables include indirect measures, such as Psychological Association (APA) and the
interviews and questionnaires, or direct meth- Behavior Analyst Certification Board (BACB)
ods, such as narrative recording of the anteced- have established general ethical guidelines
ents that precede responses of interest and the regarding how assessments should be conducted
consequences that follow them. Many behavior and interpreted. For example, Standard 9 of the
analysts believe that the “gold standard” of FBA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of
is experimental functional analysis (FA) (Iwata, Conduct promulgated by the APA (2010) is
Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richmond, devoted entirely to assessment. The same is true
1982/1994), which systematically arranges con- of Standard 3.0 of the Behavior Analyst
sequences for problem behaviors to identify Certification Board Guidelines for Responsible
Conduct (BACB Guidelines, BACB, 2011). That
standard is presented in Table 13.1. Any practi-
A. Poling (*) • S.M. Peterson • A. Mahoney tioner who abides with the standards established
Department of Psychology, Western Michigan there and elsewhere in the Guidelines is there-
University, Kalamazoo, MI 49008-5439, USA
e-mail: alan.poling@wmich.edu;
fore behaving ethically, regardless of whether
stephanie.peterson@wmich.edu; he or she is involved in functional assessment or
mahoney.am@gmail.com another professional activity.
J.L. Austin Because FBA can be an integral part of effec-
School of Psychology, University of Glamorgan, tive treatment, as other chapters in this book
Pontypridd, CF37 1DL, UK clearly illustrate, including it in treatment plan-
e-mail: jlaustin@glam.ac.uk
ning is ethical conduct. This perspective is evi-
M. Weeden dent in standard 3.02 of the current BACB
Department of Psychology, Western Michigan
University, Kalamazoo, MI 49008-5439, USA
Guidelines (BACB, 2011), which states: “The
behavior analyst conducts a functional assess-
Department of Special Education and Literacy Studies,
Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo,
ment, as defined below, to provide the necessary
MI 49008, USA data to develop an effective behavior change
e-mail: marc.a.weeden@wmich.edu program.”

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 213


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_13,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
214 A. Poling et al.

Table 13.1 Guidelines for responsible conduct with respect to assessing behaviora
3.0 Assessing behavior
Behavior analysts who use behavioral assessment techniques do so for purposes that are appropriate in light of
research.
(a) Behavior analysts’ assessments, recommendations, reports, and evaluative statements are based on information
and techniques sufficient to provide appropriate substantiation for their findings.
(b) Behavior analysts refrain from misuse of assessment techniques, interventions, results, and interpretations and
take reasonable steps to prevent others from misusing the information these techniques provide.
(c) Behavior analysts recognize limits to the certainty with which judgments or predictions can be made about
individuals.
(d) Behavior analysts do not promote the use of behavioral assessment techniques by unqualified persons, i.e.,
those who are unsupervised by experienced professionals and have not demonstrated valid and reliable
assessment skills.
3.01 Behavioral assessment approval
The behavior analyst must obtain the client’s or client-surrogate’s approval in writing of the behavior assessment
procedures before implementing them. As used here, client-surrogate refers to someone legally empowered to make
decisions for the person(s) whose behavior the program is intended to change; examples of client-surrogates include
parents of minors, guardians, and legally designated representatives
3.02 Functional assessment
(a) The behavior analyst conducts a functional assessment, as defined below, to provide the necessary data to
develop an effective behavior change program.
(b) Functional assessment includes a variety of systematic information-gathering activities regarding factors
influencing the occurrence of a behavior (e.g., antecedents, consequences, setting events, or motivating
operations) including interview, direct observation, and experimental analysis.
3.03 Explaining assessment results
Unless the nature of the relationship is clearly explained to the person being assessed in advance and precludes
provision of an explanation of results (such as in some organizational consultation, some screenings, and forensic
evaluations), behavior analysts ensure that an explanation of the results is provided using language that is reason-
ably understandable to the person assessed or to another legally authorized person on behalf of the client.
Regardless of whether the interpretation is done by the behavior analyst, by assistants, or others, behavior analysts
take reasonable steps to ensure that appropriate explanations of results are given.
a
Published by the Behavior Analysis Certification Board (2011) in the Behavior Analyst Certification Board Guidelines
for Responsible Conduct and reproduced by permission

It is, however, true that one can easily envi- dictate what is the right or wrong course in any
sion situations in which specific applications of one of the many decisions that must be made
FBA raise interesting ethical questions, as in the throughout the FBA process. Instead, we aim to
hypothetical case 3.02B presented by Bailey identify applications of the analysis procedures
and Burch (2011) in a book devoted entirely to that can give rise to important questions that
ethics for behavior analysts. In this case, the behavior analysts should consider carefully. We
issue is whether an FA is necessary in situations begin our discussion with considerations regard-
where the person engages in self-injury, particu- ing what might be considered “traditional” uses
larly when informal assessment methods have of FA; in other words, using FA to identify main-
not led to an effective intervention. On the face taining variables for self-injurious behavior
of it, it appears that further discussion of the eth- (SIB) of individuals with developmental disabil-
ics of FBA, in general, and of FA, in particular, ities. We then discuss additional ethical consid-
is merited. The purpose of this chapter is to initi- erations when expanding FBA to a broader range
ate such discussion and to provide some general of populations and settings, using school-based
guidelines for ethical FBA. Our goal is not to FBA as an exemplar.
13 Ethical Issues 215

entitled “Human Subjects Protection” that com-


Special Considerations prises 56 lines. In it, Iwata and his colleagues
Regarding FA indicated that procedures were approved by a
human subjects committee (i.e., an Institutional
Since the technique was first described by Iwata Review Board, IRB), individuals who were at
et al. (1982/1994), FA has been widely used to risk of severe physical harm were excluded from
isolate controlling variables for self-injury and participation, and all potential participants
other challenging behaviors exhibited by people received a complete medical exam, with neuro-
with intellectual and other developmental disabil- logical, audiological, and visual evaluations as
ities (e.g., Hanley, Iwata, McCord, 2003;; Hastings appropriate “to assess current physical status and
& Noone, 2005). Essentially, the procedure to rule out organic factors that might be associ-
involves the systematic delivery of stimuli after ated with or exacerbated by self-injury” (p. 199).
the occurrence of problem behavior, whereby one Criteria for terminating sessions were established
of more of those stimuli is assumed to function as through consultation with a physician. The phy-
reinforcers. Decisions about functions of behavior sician or a nurse observed sessions intermittently
are made by comparing rates of responding across to assess whether termination criteria needed to
different conditions. Those conditions that result be adjusted. If termination criteria were met,
in the highest rates of behavior are assumed to participants were immediately removed from the
reveal the reinforcers for those behaviors. therapy room and evaluated by a physician or
However, because it requires targeted behaviors to nurse, who determined whether the sessions
occur undercontrolled circumstances, FA in par- would continue. After every fourth session, each
ticular poses special ethical considerations. participant was examined by a nurse. Finally,
each case was reviewed at least weekly both in
departmental case conferences and in interdisci-
FA and the Primum non Nocere plinary rounds. Using safeguards such as those
Principle arranged by Iwata et al. and limiting the number
and length of sessions to the minimum required
The so-called Hippocratic injunction to first do to provide useful information minimizes harm to
no harm [in Latin, primum non nocere] has long participants during FA.
been an axiom central to the education of medi- Despite the possibility that harmful behavior
cal and graduate students in the helping profes- will be temporarily reinforced (and thus increased)
sions (Smith, 2005). Likewise, behavior analysts during FA sessions, it is important to point out
have a fundamental responsibility to not harm that a properly conducted FA does not increase
their clients or to allow harm to occur under their the risk of harm to participants relative to that
watch (Bailey & Burch, 2011). Iwata and his col- they encounter in their everyday environment, a
leagues (1982/1994) were careful to uphold this point made by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) in their
principle in their seminal description of FA of seminal article. If it is ethically acceptable for a
SIB. In brief, Iwata et al. arranged five test con- target behavior, such as SIB, to occur outside FA
ditions in an analogue setting to determine the sessions, then the same should be true within
conditions under which SIB regularly occurred, such sessions, although safeguards to prevent
hence the variables that appeared to control such serious harm might be required. Interestingly,
responding. They pointed out that the possibility published studies rarely mention such safeguards.
of participants seriously injuring themselves dur- Of 116 articles describing the FA of SIB recently
ing the assessment of controlling variables was a reviewed by Weeden, Mahoney, and Poling
real concern. They were very careful to arrange (2010), 9 (7.7%) described session termination
protections to prevent this from occurring and criteria and 23 (19.8%) described other proce-
they described those protections clearly and in dural safeguards for reducing risk to participants.
detail. In fact, their article contains a section As Weeden et al. pointed out, it is possible, even
216 A. Poling et al.

probable, that appropriate safeguards to prevent they are—describing them precisely and concisely
harm to participants were in place in the other would be easy. If safeguards are not in place,
some explanation may be appropriate. We encour-
studies but were not described. Nevertheless, it is age authors of future articles to ensure, first and
important for those implementing functional foremost, that the protections they arrange to pre-
analysis procedures to consider the potential vent serious harm to participants are in fact ade-
importance of having in place structured termina- quate and to ensure as well that readers of those
articles have sufficient information to evaluate
tion criteria and safeguards in place to protect and, if desired, to replicate those safeguards. We
individuals engaged in FAs. also encourage editors of relevant journals to
require them to do so before their work is pub-
lished. FA is an invaluable tool and these actions
Institutional Review Boards are suggested not to criticize what has been done
and Informed Consent in the past, but rather to improve that which is
done in the future. (p. 302)
Having research approved by a Human Subjects These recommendations appear prudent and
Institutional Review Board is one way to ensure we endorse them. From a methodological stand-
that procedures are ethically sound (Bailey & point, however, it is important to note that the use
Burch, 2011), but only six of the articles (5%) of protective equipment could potentially alter
reviewed by Weeden et al. (2010) indicated that the results of an FA. For example, Le and Smith
such approval was obtained. Another safeguard is (2002) found that FA of the SIB of three partici-
securing written informed consent. Section 3.01 in pants yielded different results when they did and
the BACB Guidelines (BACB, 2011) states that: did not wear protective equipment. When protec-
The behavior analyst must obtain the client’s or tive equipment was worn, very little SIB occurred
client-surrogate’s approval in writing of the behav- and no clear functions were revealed. In the
ior assessment procedures before implementing absence of protective equipment, however, SIB
them. As used here, client-surrogate refers to
someone legally empowered to make decisions for appeared to be maintained by negative reinforce-
the person(s) whose behavior the program is ment (escape from demands in one participant
intended to change; examples of client-surrogates and escape from a wheel chair in a second par-
include parents of minors, guardians, and legally ticipant) in two participants and by nonsocial
designated representatives
(i.e., automatic) reinforcement in the third.
Although this datum was not reported by Although other studies using FA have revealed
Weeden et al. (2010), only one of the 116 studies clear functions in the presence of protective
they evaluated (0.8%) specified that clients (or equipment (e.g., Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, &
their surrogates) had provided written informed Miltenberger, 1994; Mace & Knight, 1986), the
consent. Based on the information provided, one findings of Le and Smith call attention to the need
must presume that no harm was done in most of to consider whether inconclusive FA results
these studies. In fact, Weeden et al. assumed that might be the result of protective equipment or
protections generally were adequate and were other participant safeguards, such as very conser-
careful not to accuse researchers of unethical vative session termination criteria that might pre-
conduct. They wrote: vent the collection of adequate data, and, if so,
The present findings in no way suggest that FA whether those safeguards could be safely and
procedures as arranged in studies of SIB are ethically withdrawn. Such decisions should be
ethically or otherwise questionable. It is for that made by an informed team of individuals that
reason that we do not cite specific studies when includes a legal representative of, and an advo-
making the case that high levels of SIB were
sometimes present across many sessions with no cate for, the participant.
safeguards reported. These findings do, however, This example is an illustration of why there
clearly suggest that important information about are not clear answers to questions as to the “right”
safeguards arranged to protect participants is not and “wrong” course of action during an FA.
included in many articles. If safeguards, such as
criteria for terminating sessions and excluding While it may be considered “right” to include
participants, are in place—and we assume that protective equipment in the analysis to protect
13 Ethical Issues 217

the clients from harm during the analysis, if doing empirical support (e.g., Bloom, Iwata, Fritz,
so calls into question the validity of the obtained Roscoe, & Carreau, 2011; LaRue et al., 2010;
results, then the inclusion of protective equip- Sigafoos & Saggers, 1995; Wallace & Knights,
ment may be less “right.” The questions of if, 2003; Wilder, Chen, Atwell, Pritchard, &
when, and how protective equipment should be Weinstein, 2006). Trial-based FAs involve com-
included in an FA is one the behavior analyst paring brief test conditions [motivational opera-
should be carefully consider before commencing tion (MO) present] with control conditions (MO
an FA. absent) of the same length. Trial-based FAs have
the potential to yield the same results as extended
FAs, thus reducing the time spent engaging in
Brief FA and Harm Reduction harmful behavior during the assessment. For
example, Wallace and Knights (2003) compared
Because of the potential to strengthen harmful the results of trial-based FA with extended FA
behavior temporarily during an FA, minimizing and found that results were the same for two of
occurrences of the target behavior to the lowest the three participants. Further, they reported that
number (and intensity) adequate to reveal the brief evaluations took an average of 36 min
controlling variables is an ethically sound goal. to complete, whereas the extended procedures
Brief FA (Northup et al., 1991) is one way to took an average of 310 min (an 88.4% difference
accomplish this goal. Brief FA is a modification in session time). More recently, LaRue et al.
to traditional FA procedures in which clients par- (2010) found exact correspondence across trial-
ticipate in fewer, truncated sessions and fewer based and traditional FA models for the problem
types of sessions rather than the traditional five behaviors (e.g., aggression, self-injury, disrup-
(alone, escape, control, tangible, and attention) tion, and inappropriate vocalization) of four of
described by Iwata et al. (1982/1994). Studies five participants, with partial correspondence
have shown that brief FAs are robust and can pro- obtained for the fifth participant. The authors also
vide meaningful information about the variables reported that traditional procedures took an aver-
that control target responses. For example, Kahng age of 208 session minutes to complete, whereas
and Iwata (1999) compared data from 50 tradi- the trial-based analysis took an average of only
tional FAs (35 with clear response patterns and 31.6 (an 84.8% difference). Bloom et al. (2011)
15 undifferentiated) with data from brief FAs conducted their analyses in a more naturalistic
constructed by isolating the first session of each classroom setting and found correspondence in
condition from the rest of the complete analyses. six of the ten analyses they compared (with par-
They concluded that brief FA and within-session tial correspondence on a 7th case). However,
analysis (the examination of response rates within their results revealed more modest savings in
the isolated sessions to uncover within-session assessment time (271 min for traditional FA and
trends which may be obscured by overall session 233 min for trial-based FA; a 14% difference).
average) yielded results comparable to those of Another potentially viable brief assessment
more lengthy evaluations in 66 and 68% of cases, technique involves measuring latency to the first
respectively. Further analysis of the data revealed response. In this arrangement, the participant is
correspondence between brief and traditional presented with conditions that resemble a typical
methods in 27 of the 35 data sets (77%) where a FA, but the session ends following the first
function was clearly identified. However, it is instance of problem behavior, and the latency to
important to note that when full FA outcomes the problem behavior across conditions is mea-
were undifferentiated, correspondence for the sured. Call, Pabico, and Lomas (2009) compared
within-session analysis was substantially higher results of a demand condition only latency FA
(80% vs. 40%) than for the brief procedures. and a standard FA with two participants who
More recently, another type of brief FA, exhibited SIB and disruptive behavior. The
termed trial-based FA, has gained considerable latency FA yielded a hierarchy of demand
218 A. Poling et al.

aversiveness based on the latency to the first forms of FA to identify behavioral function and
problem behavior. During subsequent functional to only proceed to the more traditional model
analyses, the shorter latency demands produced when behavioral function cannot be identified by
more differentiated outcomes. Thomason-Sassi, these methods (Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, &
Iwata, Neidert, and Roscoe (2011) conducted ret- Roane, 1995).
rospective analyses of 38 FA data sets, in which
data were graphed first as response rates (or, if
appropriate, percentage of intervals) across ses- FA and Right to Effective Treatment
sions and secondly as latency to respond to the
first target response within a session. Eighty six Behavior analysts strongly agree that their clients
percent of the cases showed a high degree of cor- have a right to effective treatment (Van Houten
respondence between the two types of response et al., 1988). Inherent in this principle is the right
measurement. Further, ten newly conducted FAs to a treatment that is both appropriate and timely.
in which both traditional and latency analyses A potential ethical issue with any form of FBA,
were performed showed correspondence in nine but one especially likely to emerge when tradi-
out of ten cases. These results suggest that latency tional FA is used, is that treatment is not designed
may be a viable measure of responding in situa- and implemented until assessment is finished,
tions where repeated occurrences of behavior are which can require many hours or even days
dangerous or when response opportunities are (Vollmer & Smith, 1996). Behavior analytic
limited. Despite these promising results, how- practitioners, as well as researchers, should con-
ever, research on latency FA is currently some- sider whether time is better spent designing and
what limited. More research is needed to draw evaluating an intervention based on other forms
firm conclusions about the utility of this method. of FBA data (e.g., brief FA or descriptive analy-
Regardless of the particular method, brief, sis) or on collecting extensive FA data in the hope
trial-based, and latency-to-first response func- of eventually developing a superior intervention.
tional analyses necessarily expose participants to In reality, it is likely that many practitioners do
fewer sessions or session types (e.g., Barretto, not have the time or resources needed to conduct
Wacker, Harding, Lee, & Berg, 2006; LaRue an extensive experimental analysis of the vari-
et al., 2010; Northup et al., 1991) and/or sessions ables that control a target behavior; they will of
of shorter duration (e.g., Barretto et al.; Kahng & necessity prioritize assessing treatment effects,
Iwata, 1999; LaRue et al.; Northup et al.; Wallace not accessing the functions of the target behavior.
& Iwata, 1999) than conventional FA. As a result, In our view, this strategy is defensible from both
these forms of FA may limit opportunities to ethical and practical perspectives. In truth, behav-
engage in harmful responses, reduce the likeli- ior analysts use a relatively small number of
hood that new topographies of harmful responses behavior-change strategies and interventions for-
will occur, and make it unlikely that delivering mulated with and without FA data are often com-
putative reinforcers (e.g., attention and tangible parable (Schill, Kratochwill, & Elliott, 1998).
items) will significantly strengthen target FA, like FBA in general, is a useful tool but it can
responses. Moreover, these forms of FA offer the easily be overused. Moreover, FA that does not
possibility of quickly ascertaining the variables lead to an effective intervention does not benefit
that control a targeted response and using this participants. More than a few published studies
information to develop an effective intervention. involving the FA of SIB do not even describe an
All of these considerations are positive and a intervention, but instead focus on delineating
strong case can be made that from an ethical per- controlling variables per se. Such work is at best
spective these forms of FA are preferred to tradi- incomplete. In our view, the best (and most ethi-
tional functional assessment, whenever possible. cal) FA research delineates controlling variables,
It may be advisable to begin with more brief designs an intervention that takes those variables
13 Ethical Issues 219

into account, and demonstrates that the interven- ioral intervention plan for such child.” Moreover,
tion produces clinically significant changes in “a child with a disability who is removed from
target behavior(s) in the participant’s everyday the child’s current [educational] placement
environment, not just in short experimental [because of a code violation] shall receive, as
sessions. appropriate, a functional behavior assessment,
behavioral intervention services, and
modifications, that are designed to address the
Issues in Expanding the Use of FBA behavior so that it does not recur.”
Across Settings and Populations When FBA is applied in school settings, it is
often applied across a wide range of problem
FBA and Best Practice behaviors and across a wide range of popula-
tions—reaching far beyond the population for
Since its inception, FBA has been used to iden- which the methodology was originally pioneered
tify controlling variables for a range of problem (i.e., individuals with developmental disabilities
behaviors in various populations and settings in a highly controlled settings). The mandate set
(Hanley et al., 2003). In 1991, a National Institutes forth in IDEA required that FBAs be applied to
of Health (NIH) consensus panel identified FA a wide range of topographies of problem behav-
as a “best practice” for designing behavioral ior other than self-injury, including aggression,
interventions for individuals with developmental noncompliance, off-task behavior, bullying, and
disabilities. The value assigned to FBA in more bringing weapons to school, as these behaviors
recent years is evident in federal legislation deal- may all result in a placement change for an indi-
ing with the education of students with disabili- vidual with disabilities.
ties. When the Individuals with Disabilities Moreover, in addition to its being required by
Education Act (IDEA, P.L. 105–117) was reau- federal law in some instances, there is a growing
thorized in 1997, FBA was specifically mandated consensus that FBA is in general “best practice”
for certain students. IDEA states: in developing behavioral interventions in school
The [Individualized Education Program, IEP] team settings (e.g., Gresham, Watson, & Skinner,
must address through a behavioral intervention 2001; Steege & Watson, 2008), as well as a num-
plan any need for positive behavioral strategies and ber of other settings, such as community mental
supports. In response to disciplinary actions by health. For purposes of this discussion, we will
school personnel, the IEP team must within 10
days meet to develop a functional behavioral focus on the application of FBA to school set-
assessment plan [italics added] to collect informa- tings. One reason for the “best practice” view is
tion. This information should be used for develop- that several authors have suggested conducting
ing or reviewing and revising an existing behavior FBAs prior to selecting school-based interven-
intervention plan to address such behaviors.
In addition, states are required to address the in- tion selection will produce better treatment out-
service needs of personnel (including professionals comes than selecting interventions with no FBA
and paraprofessionals who provide special educa- data (e.g., Asmus, Vollmer, & Borrero, 2002;
tion, general education, related services, or early Crone & Horner, 2000; Vollmer & Northup,
intervention services) as they relate to developing
and implementing positive intervention strategies. 1996). Given that “best” practices are (or should
be) evidence-based, one would expect there are
This mandate was retained in a second reau- compelling data clearly showing that interven-
thorization, the Individuals with Disabilities tions based on FBAs are significantly superior to
Education Improvement Act of 2004 (P.L. 108– alternative interventions across a range of behav-
446), which states that whenever a child’s educa- iors and educational settings. Reviewing the
tional placement is to be changed because of a existing literature for school-based FBA, how-
violation of a code of student conduct, the IEP ever, does not necessarily support such a simple
team must “conduct an FBA and develop a behav- and strong conclusion.
220 A. Poling et al.

FBA in School Settings: Best Practice? interventions will be effective only if the target
behaviors are maintained by specific kinds of
Despite the wealth of studies that employ FBA reinforcers.
prior to designing treatments (for reviews, see In the studies just described, the researchers
Ervin et al., 2001; Hanley et al., 2003), few implemented one or two general interventions
school-based studies directly compare function- across participants whose target behaviors were
based interventions to those selected without the maintained by different kinds of events. Another
benefit of FBA data. Several studies appear to strategy for illustrating the importance of FBA in
support the use of FBA prior to intervention in developing effective interventions is to compare
school settings. However, some of the studies the effects of interventions based specifically on
have produced conflicting results. known functions with those of similar interven-
Much of the data confirming the effectiveness tions without those specific components. This
of function-based interventions have come from tack was taken by Ingram, Lewis-Palmer, and
studies that evaluated the relative effectiveness Sugai (2005) in a study in which relevant ante-
of a given intervention when applied to behaviors cedents and consequences affecting the behavior
maintained by different kinds of consequences of two boys in middle school initially were deter-
(i.e., operant responses with different functions). mined via informant and descriptive assessments.
For example, Taylor and Miller (1997) compared Following the FBA, two behavior intervention
the effectiveness of time-out interventions with plans were designed for each child. One plan was
children whose problem behaviors were main- designed to address specific variables identified
tained by attention and with children whose prob- in the FBA (e.g., task difficulty, escape from
lem behaviors were maintained by escape. demands), whereas a second, similar plan omit-
Time-out generally was effective for attention- ted key elements related to function. Interventions
maintained behaviors but not for escape-main- were then rated by two experts not associated
tained behaviors. In a similar but more with the study for technical adequacy (i.e., level
complicated study, Meyer (1999) evaluated the of research support for the intervention compo-
effects of two interventions, one that allowed nents) and match to hypothesis (i.e., how well the
children to access assistance with tasks and a sec- intervention addressed variables identified in the
ond that allowed children to access praise for FBA). Technical adequacy was deemed to be high
working. Those children identified in an initial for both types of interventions. Match to hypoth-
FA phase as exhibiting higher levels of problem- esis was rated higher for both function-based
atic behavior in the presence of difficult tasks interventions as compared to their non-function-
(regardless of the frequency of praise) responded based counterparts. When the interventions were
more positively to the treatment that taught them implemented, results clearly showed that prob-
how to recruit help appropriately. In contrast, lematic behaviors were less frequent under
those children whose behaviors were maintained function-based interventions as compared to
by adult attention (regardless of task difficulty) those that did not address relevant environmental
exhibited fewer problem behaviors when taught events.
to recruit praise. In a third study, Romaniuk et al. In a related study, Newcomer and Lewis
(2002) demonstrated that children whose behav- (2004) compared the effects of function-based and
iors were maintained by attention were less likely non-function-based treatments on the behaviors
to benefit from choice-making interventions than of three elementary school students. Hypotheses
those whose behaviors were maintained by about maintaining variables for the target behav-
escape. For the latter group of children, reduc- iors were constructed using descriptive and
tions in target behaviors were not observed until experimental analyses. Following completion
the implementation of differential reinforcement of the FBA, each child was exposed to a non-
for on-task behavior. These studies and others function-based intervention followed by a func-
(e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985) suggest that certain tion-based intervention in a multiple baseline
13 Ethical Issues 221

design. For all students, problematic behaviors tion can potentially make problems worse. If the
were as high as or higher than baseline when strategies compared to FBA-informed treatments
non-function-based interventions were used. reinforce responses targeted for reduction or
When the function-based treatment was intro- increase their probabilities in other ways, it is
duced, problematic behaviors decreased immedi- not surprising that function-based strategies
ately for two of the children and more gradually would prove more effective. Granted, it is pos-
for the third. Ellingson, Miltenberger, Stricker, sible that these authors intentionally used inter-
Galensky, and Garlinghouse (2000) also com- ventions contraindicated by the FBAs in an
pared interventions based on hypothesized func- attempt to approximate the relatively common
tions to those targeting a different function than error among school personnel of using interven-
that revealed by informant and descriptive assess- tions that are based on form, not function
ments. For one of the three participants, results (Vollmer & Northup, 1996). It is unclear, how-
revealed that the function-based intervention was ever, whether the outcomes would have been the
superior to the non-function-based alternative in same if the comparison interventions had not
reducing problematic behavior. Results were less reinforced problem behavior.
compelling for the remaining participants, but Most investigations within this limited litera-
suggested that the function-based interventions ture suggest that function-based interventions
were more effective. produce better treatment outcomes, but the
Although these direct comparison studies findings are not universally positive. For instance,
appear to suggest that function-based interven- Schill et al. (1998) compared treatments based on
tions produce more favorable outcomes that non- FBAs to standard treatment packages (i.e., those
function-based treatments in school settings, a developed without a preceding assessment of rel-
few methodological cautions are warranted. In evant antecedents and consequences of behav-
each of the three studies just described (Ellingson ior). Nineteen children in Head Start who
et al., 2000; Ingram et al., 2005; Newcomer & displayed persistent problem behaviors were ran-
Lewis, 2004), the non-function-based treatments domly assigned to one of two groups. Teachers of
for some of the participants included compo- children in Group 1 met with trained consultants
nents that were contraindicated by the FBAs. to functionally assess problem behaviors and
Specifically, baseline and nonfunctional treat- develop interventions based on hypothesized
ment conditions in Ellingson et al.’s study rein- functions (functional approach). Teachers of chil-
forced problem behavior using stimuli that the dren in Group 2 met with trained consultants to
FBA suggested maintained those behaviors (i.e., describe the topography of problem behaviors
teacher attention). Likewise, one of the children (technological approach). Behaviors were
in Newcomer and Lewis’s study engaged in classified as externalizing (e.g., aggression, non-
behaviors that appeared to be maintained by compliance) or internalizing (e.g., social with-
escape from peer interactions. During the non- drawal), and then Group 2 teachers were given a
function-based treatment, the child was exposed self-help manual that described strategies for
to a dependent group contingency that effectively intervening with both categories of behaviors.
put him in closer contact with his peers. It is, Analysis of effect sizes between groups revealed
therefore, not surprising that his behavior wors- no significant differences between function- and
ened during the implementation of the interven- non-function-based treatments; both types of
tion, as it occasioned more opportunities for interventions were equally effective. It is impor-
escape. Similarly, Ingram and her colleagues tant to note, however, that one potential reason
used teacher ignoring as part of the non-function- there were no significant differences between
based treatment package for a behavior main- treatments is that the interventions used in the
tained by escape from demands. two conditions were often identical. For example,
As suggested by Iwata et al. (1994), extinc- differential reinforcement, goal-setting, and
tion interventions based on form instead of func- praise featured prominently as intervention
222 A. Poling et al.

components in both the functional and techno- based interventions to alternative interventions
logical approaches. Failure to observe significant commonly used in school settings and favored by
differences in treatment outcomes potentially teachers (e.g., token economies) and often imple-
could be accounted for by the inadvertent use of mented without the benefit of a FBA would be of
function-based treatments in the technological particular practical value. In all comparisons, it is
condition. Because function was not assessed for essential that a legitimate attempt is made to
the children in the technological group, it is develop maximally effective interventions and to
impossible to discern whether the treatments ensure that those interventions are implemented
selected for those children did or did not address with sufficient integrity. Until further research is
functions of behaviors. conducted, in our view there are not sufficient
Given the extant literature, drawing strong data to conclude with confidence that interven-
conclusions regarding the utility of conducting tions tied to FBA are always, or even typically,
FBAs prior to designing school-based interven- more effective than alternative interventions for
tions for problem behavior is somewhat difficult. reducing undesired target behaviors in school
One reason for this difficulty is that the database settings.
is relatively sparse and based primarily on small To say this is not to disparage FBA or to deny
n research designs. This is not to suggest that its usefulness, but it is to suggest that if taken lit-
single-case designs cannot reveal phenomena erally to imply that behavior analysts working in
that hold widely, but only to emphasize that to do school settings must always conduct FBA before
so requires sufficient replications of their results. developing an intervention, then Standard 3.02 of
As of yet, the data are simply too limited to draw the current BACB Guidelines (BACB, 2011) is
firm conclusions. Further, and importantly, inconsistent with Standard 1.01, which reads:
Gresham et al.’s (2004) review of 150 school- “Behavior analysts rely on scientifically and pro-
based intervention studies published in the fessionally derived knowledge when making
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis over a scientific or professional judgments in human
9-year period (1991–1999) revealed that treat- service provision, or when engaging in scholarly
ments preceded by FBAs were no more effective or professional endeavors.” In fact, there may be
than those in which FBAs were absent (or at least several instances where an FBA is simply not
not reported). Blakeslee, Sugai, and Gruba (1994) warranted for effective intervention, and in these
found a similar pattern across intervention stud- cases, ethical conduct might involve behavioral
ies reported over a wider range of settings in interventions that are not preceded by an FBA.
journals considered to be primarily or exclusively
behavior analytic in nature.
Limitations within the existing literature lead Effective Intervention
to the conclusion that a good deal more research in the Absence of FBA
is needed to provide a firm empirical base for the
use of FBAs prior to school-based treatment Research data and our professional experience
planning. This is not to suggest that FBAs should certainly indicate that FBA can play an invalu-
not be used in school settings. It is, however, a able role in developing effective treatments for
call to researchers to conduct additional studies reducing undesired behavior in school settings.
in the utility of school-based FBA to broaden our But they also indicate that FBA is not always
literature base and the evidence upon which best needed. Consider, for example, a situation in
practices can be made. Specifically, investiga- which a behavior analyst is called in to help a
tions that directly compare interventions indi- special education teacher develop an interven-
cated, contraindicated, and unrelated to behavioral tion that the teacher can use to reduce the disrup-
function should be conducted to assess the tive behavior of students to acceptable levels.
relative effectiveness and efficiency of different The consultant’s first visit to the classroom
intervention approaches. Comparisons of function- reveals that the teacher lacks basic behavior
13 Ethical Issues 223

management skills. Clear rules for appropriate current BACB Guidelines, “the behavior analyst
student conduct are lacking as are meaningful conducts a functional assessment … to provide
consequences for inappropriate or appropriate the necessary data to develop an effective behav-
behavior. Activities are poorly organized and the ior change program,” it is important to acknowl-
overall impression is one of chaos. In such a sit- edge that “the necessary data” sometimes means
uation, FBA is not a pressing priority. Regardless limited if any FBA data. FBA is a useful tool, not
of the variables that control the undesired behav- a panacea, for improving the behavior of school
iors of the students, establishing effective strate- children. The same is true with respect to other
gies for general classroom management is the populations, where studies similar to those con-
obvious first step and a prerequisite to reducing ducted in schools suggest that treatments tied to
disruptive behavior. FBA data generally are more successful than
The same can occur when consulting with alternative treatments (Carr et al., 1999; 2009;
individuals with developmental disabilities. For Kurtz et al., 2003), although it is beyond our pur-
example, consider another example we recently pose to review the relevant data. Given the extant
experienced. One of the authors was asked to pro- literature, in our opinion the widespread use of
vide an FA for a 26-year-old young man who was FBA is easily justified on both ethical and practi-
reported to engage in elopement from his home cal grounds, but it is inappropriate to elevate its
and aggression toward his mother. Upon the use to an ethical imperative.
behavior analyst’s first visit to the home, the
behavior analyst learned that the young man was
only allowed out of his house for therapy 6 h per The Competent Use of FBA
week. During these times, he displayed appropri-
ate behavior in the community and never eloped. Although FBA is not always required to develop
The rest of the week he was required to stay in his an effective behavior-change intervention, it is
house, because no services were available, and his often of real and significant value. For that value
mother did not feel she could handle him in the to be realized, however, FBA data must be col-
community. She also did not allow him in the yard, lected and interpreted appropriately and interven-
because he often eloped from the yard. Observations tions skillfully crafted in view of those data.
within the home revealed a rather sterile environ- Standard 1.02 (a) of the BACB Guidelines
ment, For example, all of the cupboards were (BACB, 2011) dictates that, “behavior analysts
locked to keep him from getting into them. Rather provide services, teach, and conduct research
than conducting an FA, the behavior analyst only within the boundaries of their competence,
focused on identifying ways to increase the cli- based on their education, training, supervised
ent’s access to community activities, as it was experience, or appropriate professional experi-
hypothesized that this would decrease the motiva- ence,” and this convention obviously applies to
tion for elopement and addressed an underlying the use of FBA. It is essential than any behavior
problem of limited services that resulted in the cli- analyst who uses FBA ensures that he or she is
ent’s restricted access to functional activities. In competent with respect to FBA in general and
addition, the behavioral intervention focused on with respect to the specific information-gather
teaching him skills he could use to be even more strategies that she or he uses. Given the recog-
successful in the community. nized importance of FBA in behavior analysis,
Good interventions are those which produce graduate training programs in the area typically
desired and lasting effects, and ethical profes- provide appropriate instruction and useful infor-
sional conduct comprises actions that lead to such mation about the topic can be obtained at work-
interventions, regardless of how the interventions shops, such as those held at the Association for
are selected or their modality (Poling, 1994; Behavior Analysis International conference, and
Poling, Ehrhardt, Wood, & Bowerman, 2010). in written works such as this book. Given these
In our view, in interpreting standard 3.02 of the considerations, it appears that most legitimate
224 A. Poling et al.

applied behavior analysts currently possess, or assessments and classroom-based FAs, as well as
could easily acquire, expertise in FBA. simultaneously collect data on behavior. Training
The same is not true, however, for school per- consisted of providing a written description of
sonnel. Although the majority of educators are the FBA procedures combined with approxi-
not trained in behavior analysis, legislative man- mately 2 h of instruction and practice for each
dates may require that they conduct FBAs, despite procedure. Data from teacher-conducted assess-
their reservations regarding skills for doing so. ments and analyses were compared to data
Pindiprolu, Peterson, and Bergloff (2007) sur- yielded by a “conventional” FA conducted by
veyed special education teachers, administrators, Sasso. Results revealed a high degree of similar-
support staff, and general educators and found ity in teacher- and experimenter-collected data,
that the vast majority of them reported that devel- suggesting teachers could accurately identify
oping interventions for problem behavior and controlling variables and descriptive assess-
conducting FBAs were among the areas in which ments produced the same results as FAs. One
they most desired professional development. In potential limitation of this investigation was that
addition, when specifically asked about their skill the procedures for training teachers were not
level in conducting FBAs, special education described in sufficient detail to allow for replica-
teachers stated they felt especially weak in (1) tion. Fortunately, later investigations have sup-
testing hypotheses regarding the purpose of prob- plied more clearly specified protocols for teaching
lem behaviors, (2) interviewing caregivers regarding FA and other FBA skills to people with limited or
problem behaviors, (3) devising procedures for no training in behavior analysis.
measuring problem behaviors, and (4) developing The most notable among these is Iwata et al.
intervention plans to decrease problem behaviors (2000), who provided a detailed account of pro-
or increase desired behaviors. cedures used to train undergraduate students to
If schools are to use FBAs effectively to conduct attention, demand, and play conditions
inform treatment selection, then ensuring these of an FA (Iwata et al., 1982/1994) using a combi-
assessments are done with integrity is a critical nation of written instructions, video modeling,
issue. Further, if school personnel are to conduct and feedback. Consistent with the results of Sasso
FBAs, then it may be up to behavior analysts to et al. (1992), Iwata et al. noted that training pro-
train them how to assess and analyze behavioral cedures could be completed in about 2 h (assum-
functions appropriately. It is incumbent upon ing that the written materials had been read prior
these behavior analysts not only to teach school to the start of face-to-face training). Interestingly,
personnel to use best practices in FBA and inter- Iwata et al. (2000) observed that their participants
vention selection, but also to use best practices were fairly accurate in implementing conditions
in the training procedures used to teach these after simply reading the written descriptions and
skills. instructions. Although these results could imply
that learning to conduct an FA is a relatively sim-
ple process, several factors caution against this
Ethics and FBA Training conclusion. First, the participants in the study
were upper-level undergraduate psychology
Given the relative scarcity of behavior analysts in majors who had completed a course in behavior
schools, teaching others to conduct FBAs is often analysis. The ease of training observed by Iwata
necessary to attenuate resource deficits. Therefore, et al. probably was at least partially the result of
several researchers have endeavored to develop participants’ prior knowledge of behavior ana-
effective training strategies for school personnel lytic principles, which seemingly exceeded the
and to evaluate the effects of those procedures. In knowledge teachers would have garnered from
an early study, Sasso et al. (1992) showed that their training programs. Remarkably, many teach-
with minimal training two special education ers fail to receive even the most basic informa-
teachers could be taught to conduct descriptive tion on managing problematic behaviors, much
13 Ethical Issues 225

less on identifying how classroom variables rying out school-based FBAs. The FBA process
affect student responding (Latham, 2002). requires a much broader repertoire, including
Second, data on accuracy of performance were selecting the appropriate assessment/analysis
collected during role play situations with a gradu- strategies to match available resources and com-
ate student assuming the role of a student/client. petence, correctly carrying out selected strate-
Accurate implementation in more naturalistic gies, appropriately scoring and graphing data,
settings might have proved more challenging, accurately analyzing data, and effectively using
and thus might have required additional training. data to inform intervention selection. Therefore,
In an attempt to extend the findings of Iwata additional research has been undertaken to
et al. (2000), Moore et al. (2002) showed that address some of these issues.
similar procedures could be used to train three One example is Pindiprolu, Peterson, Rule,
general education teachers to implement atten- and Lignugaris/Kraft (2003), who provided web-
tion and demand FA conditions. Consistent with based, experiential cases as a training tool for
the procedures of Iwata et al., the initial phase of preservice special education teachers, and then
the study required teachers to read materials per- used pre- and posttests to evaluate the effects of
taining to FA and answer questions with the the case study instruction on students’ knowledge
researchers. Unlike Iwata and colleagues’ par- and application of FBAs. Participants were
ticipants, however, teachers’ accuracy during taught to conduct FBA interviews, and design
this phase was relatively low (thereby support- FAs based on their interviews. Different methods
ing the hypothesis that prior exposure to behavior of teaching were used: reading materials only
analysis might bolster the effectiveness of written that summarized client information, reading the
training materials). With the addition of individu- results of an FBA interview, and being able to
alized feedback, however, performance of all conduct their own interview. Students in all three
three participants increased substantially and groups improved significantly from pre- to post-
maintained during classroom probes. test, but no differences in effectiveness of the dif-
Other studies also have shown that teachers ferent teaching tactics among groups were
could be quickly trained to conduct FA sessions. observed. Further, differences in pre- and post
For example, Wallace, Doney, Mintz-Resudek, scores for all groups revealed that mean scores
and Tarbox (2004) demonstrated that teachers for groups did not exceed 67% for declarative
could accurately arrange conditions after a 3-h knowledge or 59% for application of skills.
workshop that included opportunities to role play Therefore, although the improvements were sta-
each condition and receive feedback on perfor- tistically significant, the scores suggest that the
mance. Similarly, Moore and Fisher (2007) students still failed to master much of the basic
showed that staff at a center for treatment of information pertaining to FBAs and the skills
severe behavior disorders could be trained to required to conduct them. This study suggests
conduct attention, demand, and play conditions that teaching the analytic skills involved in
via written materials, lecture, and video model- designing effective FBAs (as opposed to con-
ing. Although exact times spent in training were ducting experimental session) may be more chal-
not reported, Moore and Fisher speculated that lenging than initially meets the eye.
successful staff training could potentially be Unlike Pindiprolu et al. (2003), who focused
accomplished with video models in as little as on teaching the assessment portion of FBA, Scott
15 min, assuming the videos showed sufficient et al. (2005) examined the effects of FBA training
exemplars. on school staff’s abilities to identify effective
Although these studies have demonstrated interventions for problem behavior. The research-
effective strategies for training people who are ers provided FBA training to five staff members
not behavior analysts to conduct the experimen- from four elementary schools. Training
tal conditions of an FA, they have not addressed lasted 6 h, and included descriptions of proce-
many of the other skills that are required for car- dures for both conducting FBAs and developing
226 A. Poling et al.

function-based interventions. Participants also provide a description of interventions strategies


practiced skills using three video case studies, that would likely result in “effective (i.e., rapid
both with the trainer and in small groups, and and semi-permanent) control of [the student’s]
were provided feedback on their performance. problem behavior” (p. 167) in an open-ended
Each participant subsequently was assigned the question format. In addition, the assessment
role of facilitator in their school’s intervention required participants to answer five multiple
team, ensuring that at least one member of each choice questions about FBA strategies and pur-
team had been trained in conducting FBAs and poses. Identical assessments also were sent to
linking interventions to FBA outcomes. The teachers who had not completed the training.
authors then reviewed the teams’ behavior plans Although trained participants answered more
for 31 students and compared the suggested strat- questions about function correctly, they were no
egies with those of experts who were asked to more likely than untrained participants to suggest
develop interventions based on each student’s interventions that matched behavioral function. It
case and the teams’ FBAs. is interesting that this study employed a longer
Both experts and teams selected a range of period of training than other studies (i.e., three
intervention strategies from a district-generated 7-h days of training), yet participants still did not
list (e.g., antecedent manipulation, instructional achieve one of the primary goals of the in-ser-
techniques, consequences for positive behavior vice. Although it is difficult to discern what might
and misbehavior), but that teams were much have accounted for these negative results (e.g.,
more likely than experts to select punitive and quality of training, treatment integrity, effects of
exclusionary intervention components, regardless 6 week delay), they nonetheless raise concerns
of the identified function. Although intervention about the outcomes produced by the training
plans prior to FBA training were not evaluated, strategies commonly employed by behavioral
these results suggest that FBA training did not researchers and practitioners alike.
necessarily produce a bias toward reinforcement- In addition to the often discouraging results of
based interventions. Scott et al. (2005) did not studies aimed at training broad FBA skill reper-
assess whether the hypotheses generated by the toires, another important issue concerns measure-
teams were reasonable given the data or whether ment of learning outcomes. Specifically, it is
the strategies selected matched the hypothesized unclear whether identifying functions from writ-
functions of the behaviors, it is impossible to ten scenarios and designing corresponding inter-
assess the effectiveness of their training strategy ventions is analogous to engaging in these
in teaching these two very important skills. behaviors in more authentic contexts. Van Acker,
Dukes, Rosenberg, and Brady (2007) also Boreson, Gable, and Potterton (2005) presented a
evaluated the effects of FBA training on special compelling and disconcerting portrait of FBAs
educators’ knowledge of behavioral function and and behavior intervention plans (BIPs) in
subsequent intervention selection. Teachers were Wisconsin schools, finding that 70% of the FBAs/
trained over 3 full days, with the second and third BIPs failed to identify or define the target behav-
training days separated by 6 weeks. Teachers, ior, 25% failed to identify a function for the
trained in groups of 45–100, were exposed verbal behavior, and 46% proposed the use of aversive
instruction, a written manual, case studies, and strategies as the sole means of changing behav-
role plays. Training was specifically designed to ior. Further, the results showed that school per-
teach teachers to identify functions of behaviors sonnel with substantial training in the FBA
and then link functions to intervention selections. process were no more likely to define target
Several weeks after the completion of training, behaviors clearly or to design interventions to
participants were given an assessment comprising modify the physical or social context than those
five scenarios. Participants were asked to identify with no training. These findings clearly show the
the likely function of the behaviors described in potential for disconnect between training and
each scenario from a list of functions, and then to practice. On a more positive note, the authors
13 Ethical Issues 227

found that FBA/BIP teams with at least one to know whether a complex range of skills can
trained member were more likely to verify the be effectively taught and maintained, and if so,
hypothesized function through some sort of test- how much and what type of training is required
ing, to incorporate behavioral function into the to do so.
design of the behavior intervention plan, to use Given the relative paucity of information
reinforcement based strategies, and to plan for regarding the strategies needed to teach the full
treatment monitoring. These latter findings bode complement of FBA skills puts practitioners in
well for the potential to train school personnel to somewhat of an ethical conundrum: Schools want
identify functions and develop corresponding effective training in FBA and intervention design,
interventions, but there is still much left to do if but our own literature makes it difficult to know
we are to effectively and consistently train exactly how meet these needs. Further, we want
sufficient repertoires of FBA and intervention our science and technology to be accessible to
skills across a broad population of learners. others, but we want procedures to be imple-
As noted, the results of some studies might mented with integrity by those who are fully
suggest that teaching others, including teachers, competent. Standard 3.0(d) of the BACB
to conduct FBAs is a relatively easy endeavor Guidelines for Responsible Conduct (BACB,
that takes minimal time and resources (e.g., Iwata 2011) states that “(b)ehavior analysts do not pro-
et al., 2000; Moore et al., 2002; Moore & Fisher, mote the use of behavioral assessment techniques
2007; Wallace et al., 2004). Perhaps this finding by unqualified persons, i.e., those who are unsu-
explains the propensity of some behavior analysts pervised by experienced professionals and have
to agree to teach functional assessment and not demonstrated valid and reliable assessment
analysis to school personnel during relatively skills.” Behavior analysts who provide training
short in-services or workshops. Before making in FBA for teachers or other care providers should
such agreements, they should recognize that these recognize that their efforts will not necessarily
studies were designed to assess training methods provide trainees with adequate, including valid
for a very limited scope of FBA skills (i.e., and reliable, assessment skills. Even though a
arranging FA sessions), not for establishing broad conservative interpretation of standard 3.0(d)
FBA competencies. Clearly, training this rela- might provide a basis for doing so, in our view it
tively limited skill set does not address the skills is pointless and inappropriate to accuse behavior
required to collect and interpret FBA data. analysts who provide such training of unethical
Moreover, outside the realm of research, FA conduct. However, it is appropriate to call atten-
(and in particular, analogue analyses) are not tion to the need to develop and use empirically-
likely to be recommended as viable FBA strate- validated training procedures that maximize the
gies in schools (Bambara & Kern, 2005; Chandler likelihood that trainees acquire the repertoire of
& Dahlquist, 2010). It is much more likely that complex and inter-related skills needed to use
FBAs will be conducted via informant and FBA successfully.
descriptive assessments (Van Acker et al., 2005),
and much less is known about how to teach
school personnel to collect these types of data in Potentially Effective Models
a valid and reliable manner (see Neef & Peterson, for Collaboration to Increase
2007) than is known about teaching FA. Although FBA Competence
some researchers have attempted to provide eval-
uations of a broader scope of training (e.g., Dukes Given the current status of our training literature,
et al., 2007; Pindiprolu et al., 2003; Sasso et al., practitioners should perhaps focus on training
1992; Scott et al., 2005), their contributions have school personnel (or any other relevant stakehold-
produced mixed results that make it impossible ers) to be good collaborators in the FBA process,
to establish clear training guidelines. Current as opposed to attempting to train a very complex
gaps in the existing literature also make it difficult skill repertoire with little evidence about which
228 A. Poling et al.

training methods are most effective. Peck Peterson, of FBA in school settings is intended to illustrate
Derby, Berg, and Horner (2002) suggested a col- that there is substantial room for improvement
laborative model for conducting home-based with respect to how FBA is used in school set-
FBAs with family members who may have little tings and to offer strategies for increasing the
background in behavior analysis. This model likelihood that FBA eventually will be used to the
involves family members and behavior analysts maximum benefit of teachers and students,
assuming different roles during each stage of the thereby approaching the excellence that all con-
functional behavior assessment process (i.e., prob- cerned individuals value.
lem identification and hypothesis development,
hypothesis testing, design of intervention, evalua-
tion and adjustment, and efficiency redesign). Concluding Comments
The overall role of the behavior analyst in this
process is to “improve the technology, expand “Doing FBA” is not ethical conduct. “Doing
the science, and make more effective the design FBA” in a manner that produces maximum
of environments that reduce problem behavior benefit and minimal harm for the people whose
and increase prosocial behavior” (Peck Peterson behaviors are of concern is ethical conduct and
et al., 2002, p. 19). Complementing the behavior should be the goal of behavior analysts. It is, for
analyst’s role is that of the family, which provides example, not enough for members of an IEP team
“the context for the most efficient FA and ongoing to conduct a poor FBA and design a weak inter-
intervention” (Peck Peterson et al., p. 19). Perhaps vention for a student with a developmental dis-
this model could be adapted to describe the ability who is facing disciplinary action, although
appropriate roles of school personnel and behav- doing so might meet the requirements of IDEA.
ior analysts in conducting functional behavior In a 1994 discussion of the ethics of using psycho-
assessments in school settings. This adapted tropic drugs to manage behavior in people with
model is outlined in Table 13.1 and may provide developmental disabilities, Poling wrote:
the cooperative, on-site training model preferred
It is critical that decisions concerning [medication]
by school personnel (Pindiprolu et al., 2007), as use are individualized and data-based to the fullest
well as the collaboration between school districts, extent possible. Because we can never know a priori
state departments of education, and institutes of how a given person will respond to medication, we
higher education recommended by Shellady and must always determine what the medication is
intended to do and whether this goal is accom-
Stichter (1999). Table 13.2 also provides infor- plished. Moreover, we must take care to ensure that
mation about collaboration between school per- observed benefits are evaluated relative to real and
sonnel and behavior analysts for functional possible costs to the patient, and that all decisions
analyses and behavioral interventions. are made in her or his best interests. If this is done,
treatment is rational and ethical as well. (p. 171)
The preceding discussion has focused on the
use of FBA in school settings, but similar consid- To capture the essence of the ethical use of FBA,
erations apply in all circumstances where people “FBA-based intervention” can simply be substi-
are trained to use FBA and put that knowledge to tuted for “medication” in the foregoing passage.
use in an attempt to improve behavior. “Benefitting No reasonable person argues that it is funda-
others” is one of the core ethical principles that mentally wrong, hence unethical, for applied
guide the practice of psychology (Koocher & behavior analysts or others in the helping profes-
Keith-Spiegel, 1998) and of applied behavior sions to try to determine why their clients emit
analysis (Bailey & Burch, 2011). For example, inappropriate behaviors or fail to emit appropri-
Standard 2.0 of the BACB Guidelines (BACB, ate behaviors, and then use this knowledge to
2011) reads: “The behavior analyst has a respon- help the clients. From a conceptual perspective
sibility to operate in the best interest of clients.” FBA is perfectly acceptable as a general approach
“Pursuit of excellence” is another core ethical for designing behavior-change interventions and
principle (Bailey & Burch, 2011). Our discussion from an empirical perspective it is a general
13 Ethical Issues 229

Table 13.2 Collaboration between school personnel and behavior analysts for functional analyses and behavioral
interventions (adapted from Peck Peterson et al., 2002)
Stage of functional
behavior assessment Role of school personnel Role of behavior analyst
Problem identification 1. Broadly define the problem and the 1. Guide organization of information
and hypothesis goals for intervention 2. Clarify patterns
development (interviews 2. Describe antecedents and conse- 3. Determine what conditions will be used
and naturalistic quences associated with problem in experimental manipulations
observations) behavior
Hypothesis testing 1. Implement experimental conditions 1. Coach personnel as they conduct
(experimental with guidance experimental conditions
manipulations) 2. Take data; perhaps check interobserver 2. Assist personnel in determining what
agreement stimuli will be used in experimental
conditions
3. Check fidelity of implementation;
perhaps conduct interobserver agreement
4. Assist personnel in interpreting results of
experimental manipulations
Design of intervention 1. Identify intervention methods that fit 1. Identify intervention methods that are
within classroom routines and/or consistent with best practice and research
modify classroom routines so in behavior analysis
intervention is a good fit 2. Predict expected patterns and rates of
2. Identify external supports needed behavior change
(if any)
Evaluation 1. Collect data on problem and/or 1. Assist in designing practical data
and adjustment replacement behaviors collection procedures
2. Identify aspects of the intervention 2. Provide checks to ensure data are being
that are problematic for implementation collected
and participate in intervention redesign 3. Assist personnel in interpreting results of
(if necessary) intervention implementation
4. Monitor intervention fidelity and provide
feedback to school personnel on fidelity
5. Ensure that intervention redesign
(if necessary) is consistent with best
practice and research in behavior analysis
Maintenance 1. Implement prompt fading and 1. Assist school personnel with recommen-
and generalization reinforcement schedule thinning dations for prompt fading and
procedures reinforcement schedule thinning
2. Continue collecting data on problem 2. Continue monitoring intervention fidelity
and/or replacement behaviors and providing feedback to school
3. Identify stimuli (i.e., settings, people, personnel on fidelity as intervention
materials, cues) for which generaliza- changes over time
tion programming is necessary 3. Continue providing checks to ensure data
4. Identify maintenance and generalization are being collected
problems and participate in intervention 4. Assist personnel in interpreting results of
redesign (if necessary) intervention implementation

approach of demonstrated value. As we point no compelling conceptual or empirical basis for


out, however, successful interventions can be claiming that ethical or effective behavior analy-
designed in the absence of FBA data and collect- sis always begins with FA or another form of
ing such data does not ensure that a treatment FBA. To date, FBA has been used primarily in
will be effective. Moreover, support for the the context of developing interventions to decel-
contention that interventions based on FBA are erate inappropriate behaviors in people with
generally more effective than alternative inter- developmental disabilities. FBA has rarely been
ventions is less than overwhelming and further used to delineate the variables responsible for
research is certainly needed. At present, there is the non-occurrence of desired responses or to
230 A. Poling et al.

ascertain why low-rate, high-intensity behaviors hope was not to cause offense, but to call atten-
occur (Irwin et al., 2001). Moreover, the utility tion to the kinds of variables that behavior ana-
of FBA for understanding rule-governed behav- lysts and laypeople consider in determining
ior is unclear (Irwin et al., 2001). None of these whether or not a professional’s actions relevant
considerations should be taken as criticisms of to FBA are or are not “ethical.” As behavior ana-
FBA, but they should serve as cautions against lysts, we see this as a matter of stimulus control,
overenthusiastic and naïve endorsements. As not morality. In other words, there often may not
Irwin, Ehrhardt, and Poling (2001) pointed out, be a “right” or “wrong” thing to do at certain
“The logic and methods of functional assess- points in time. Rather, specific stimulus condi-
ment are evident in Skinner’s writings, and many tions (e.g., type of curriculum being used with an
early researchers and practitioners influenced by individual, other behavioral supports and rules in
his ideas employed functional assessment place, type and severity of problem behavior dis-
[although it was not labeled as such] in design- played) frequently interact to create a variety of
ing interventions in school and other settings” interesting dilemmas for the behavior analyst.
(p. 173). Contemporary behavior analysts— The behavior analyst must constantly evaluate
including us—continue to use FBA to the great these stimulus conditions in order to determine
benefit of those they serve. the best course of action for completing an FBA
Certain applications of FBA, however, nota- in order to make decisions that comply with both
bly those involving FA of seriously harmful the letter and spirit of the ethical codes of con-
responses, raise interesting ethical issues and we duct guiding our field.
attempted to illustrate some of these issues.
Although some general guidelines were sug-
gested, it is important to recognize that ethical
treatment of clients is inevitably individualized References
treatment. As Johnston and Sherman (1993)
American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical
emphasize in a discussion of the Least Restrictive
principles of psychologists and code of conduct: 2010
Alternative (LRA) principle, a cornerstone for Amendments. Washington, DC: Author.
protecting people with disabilities, “to be an Asmus, J. M., Vollmer, T. R., & Borrero, J. C. (2002).
effective constitutional safeguard, the LRA must Functional behavioral assessment: A school based
model. Education and Treatment of Children, 25,
be a subjective and dynamic principle tailored to
67–90.
individual needs (Parry, 1985). Likewise, in Bailey, J. S., & Burch, M. R. (2011). Ethics for behavior
determining the needs of [people with develop- analysts (2nd ed.). Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum.
mental disabilities], treatment decisions cannot Bambara, L. M., & Kern, L. (2005). Individualized sup-
ports for students with problem behaviors: Designing
be made in isolation from the individual’s per-
positive behavior plans. New York: Guilford.
sonal preferences, values, and circumstances” Barretto, A., Wacker, D. P., Harding, J., Lee, J., & Berg,
(p. 112). This statement holds true regardless of W. K. (2006). Using telemedicine to conduct behav-
whether FBA is or is not being consider as part of ioral assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 39, 333–340.
or is being used in the treatment.
Behavior Analysis Certification Board. (2011). Guidelines
In closing, we should acknowledge that fram- for responsible conduct for behavior analysts.
ing a discussion in terms of ethical issues may Retrieved from http://www.bacb.com/index.php?
render emotion-laden what would otherwise be page=57.
Blakeslee, T., Sugai, G., & Gruba, J. (1994). A review of
innocuous points. It is, for example, one thing to
functional assessment use in data-based intervention
say that it is better practice to arrange a few short studies. Journal of Behavioral Education, 4, 397–413.
FA sessions than to arrange many long ones, quite Bloom, S. E., Iwata, B. A., Fritz, J. N., Roscoe, E. M., &
another to claim that a person who does the latter Carreau, A. B. (2011). Classroom application of a
trial-based functional analysis. Journal of Applied
is unethical. We have attempted to avoid making
Behavior Analysis, 44, 19–31.
ethical judgments and apologize in advance if Call, N. A., Pabico, R. S., & Lomas, J. E. (2009). Use of
our suggestions strike a reader as accusatory. Our latency to problem behavior to evaluate demands for
13 Ethical Issues 231

inclusion in functional analyses. Journal of Applied Irwin, R. A., Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001).
Behavior Analysis, 42, 723–728. Functional assessment: Old wine in new bottles.
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior School Psychology Review, 30, 173–179.
problems through functional communication training. Irwin, R. A., Radford, P. M., Bertsch, K., Piper, A. L.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001). A descriptive
Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Turnbill, A. P., Marquis, J. G., analysis and critique of the empirical literature on
McLaughlin, D. D., McAtee, M. L., et al. (1999). school-based functional assessment. School
Positive behavior support for people with develop- Psychology Review, 30, 193–210.
mental disabilities: A research synthesis. Washington, Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
DC: American Association for Mental Retardation. Richman, G. S. (1982/1994). Toward a functional
Carr, J. E., Severtson, J. M., & Lepper, T. L. (2009). analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
Noncontingent reinforcement is an empirically sup- Analysis, 27, 197–209. Reprinted from Analysis and
ported treatment for problem behavior exhibited by Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20
individuals with developmental disabilities. Research (1982).
in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 44–57. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
Chandler, L. K., & Dahlquist, C. M. (2010). Functional R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An analy-
assessment: Strategies to prevent and remediate chal- sis of procedural form and function. Journal of Applied
lenging behavior in school settings (3rd ed.). Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144.
Columbus: Prentice Hall. Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S. W., Lindberg, J.
Crone, D. A., & Horner, R. H. (2000). Contextual, con- S., Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill
ceptual, and empirical foundations of functional acquisition in the implementation of functional analy-
behavioral assessment in schools. Exceptionality, 8, sis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior
161–172. Analysis, 33, 181–194.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act, Johnston, J. M., & Sherman, R. A. (1993). Applying the
20 U.S.C. § 1400 (2004). least restrictive alternative principle to treatment deci-
Dukes, C., Rosenberg, H., & Brady, M. (2007). Effects of sions: A legal and behavioral analysis. The Behavior
training in functional behavior assessment. International Analyst, 16, 103–115.
Journal of Special Education, 22, 163–173. Kahng, S., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Correspondence
Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., Stricker, J., Galensky, between outcomes of a brief and extended FA. Journal
T. L., & Garlinghouse, M. (2000). Functional assess- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 149–160.
ment and intervention for challenging behaviors in the Koocher, G. P., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (1998). Ethics in
classroom by general classroom teachers. Journal of psychology: Professional standards and cases. New
Positive Behavior Interventions, 2, 85–97. York: Oxford University Press.
Ervin, R. A., Radford, P. M., Bertsch, K., Piper, A. L., Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F.,
Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001). A descriptive O’Connor, J. T., Paclawskyj, T. R., et al. (2003).
analysis and critique of the empirical literature on Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious
school-based functional assessment. School behavior in young children a summary of 30 cases.
Psychology Review, 30, 193–210. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219.
Gresham, F. M., McIntyre, L. L., Olson-Tinker, H., LaRue, R. H., Lenard, K., Weiss, M. J., Bamond, M.,
Dolstra, L., McLaughlin, V., & Van, M. (2004). Palmieri, M., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). Comparison of
Relevance of functional behavioral assessment traditional and trial-based methodologies for conduct-
research for school-based interventions and positive ing functional analyses. Research in Developmental
behavioral support. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 480–487.
Disabilities, 25, 19–37. Latham, G. I. (2002). Behind the schoolhouse door:
Gresham, F., Watson, T. S., & Skinner, C. H. (2001). Managing chaos with science, skills, and strategies.
Functional behavioral assessment: Principles, proce- North Logan: P & T Ink.
dures, and future directions. School Psychology Le, D. D., & Smith, R. G. (2002). FA of self-injury with and
Review, 30, 156–172. without protective equipment. Journal of Developmental
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). and Physical Disabilities, 14, 277–290.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Mace, F. C., & Knight, D. (1986). FA and treatment of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. severe pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19,
Hastings, R. P., & Noone, S. J. (2005). SIB and FA: Ethics 411–416.
and evidence. Education and Training in Developmental Meyer, K. A. (1999). Functional analysis and treatment of
Disabilities, 40, 335–342. problem behavior exhibited by school children.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 229–232.
of 1997, 20 U.S.C. (1997) Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E.,
Ingram, K., Lewis-Palmer, T., & Sugai, G. (2005). Riley, J., Dubard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002). Teacher
Function-based intervention planning: Comparing the acquisition of functional analysis methodology.
effectiveness of FBA function-based and non-func- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 72–77.
tion-based intervention plans. Journal of Positive Moore, J. W., & Fisher, W. W. (2007). The effects of
Behavior Interventions, 7, 224–236. videotape modeling on staff acquisition of functional
232 A. Poling et al.

analysis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior properties of aberrant behavior in school settings.
Analysis, 40, 197–202. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 809–821.
National Institutes of Health. (1991). Treatment of destruc- Schill, M. T., Kratochwill, T. R., & Elliott, S. N. (1998).
tive behaviors in persons with developmental disabili- Functional assessment in behavioral consultation:
ties. Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Health and A treatment utility study. School Psychology Quarterly,
Human Services. 13, 116–140.
Neef, N. A., & Peterson, S. M. (2007). Functional behav- Scott, T. M., McIntyre, J., Liaupsin, C., Nelson, C. M.,
ior assessment. In J. O. Cooper, T. E. Heron, & W. Conroy, M., & Payne, L. D. (2005). An examination of
Heward (Eds.), Applied behavior analysis (pp. 500– the relation between functional behavior assessment
524). Columbus: Merrill-Prentice Hall. and selected intervention strategies with school-based
Newcomer, L. L., & Lewis, T. J. (2004). Functional teams. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7,
behavioral assessment: An investigation of assessment 205–215.
reliability and effectiveness of function-based inter- Shellady, S., & Stichter, J. P. (1999). Training preservice
ventions. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral and inservice educators to conduct functional assess-
Disorders, 12, 168–181. ments: Initial issues and implications. Preventing
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, School Failure, 43, 154–159.
K., Cook, J., et al. (1991). A brief FA of aggression Sigafoos, J., & Saggers, E. (1995). A discrete-trial approach
and alternative behavior in an outclinic setting. Journal to the functional analysis of aggressive behaviour in
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 509–522. two boys with autism. Australia & New Zealand
Parry, L. (1985). Least restrictive alternative: An over- Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 20, 287–297.
view of the concept. Mental and Physical Disabilities Smith, C. M. (2005). Origin and uses of primum non noc-
Law Reporter, 9, 963–972. ere—above all, do no harm! The Journal of Clinical
Peck Peterson, S. M., Derby, K. M., Berg, W. K., & Pharmacology, 45, 371–377.
Horner, R. H. (2002). Collaboration with families in Steege, M. W., & Watson, T. S. (2008). Best practices in
the functional behavior assessment and intervention functional behavioral assessment. In A. Thomas & J.
for severe behavior problems. Education and Treatment Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology V
of Children, 25, 5–25. (pp. 347–248). Washington, DC: National Association
Pindiprolu, S. S., Peterson, S. M., & Bergloff, H. (2007). of School Psychologists.
School personnel’s professional development needs Taylor, J., & Miller, M. (1997). When timeout works some
and skill level with functional behavior assessments in of the time: The importance of treatment integrity and
ten midwestern states in the United States: Analysis functional assessment. School Psychology Quarterly,
and issues. Journal of the International Association of 12, 4–22.
Special Education, 8, 31–42. Thomason-Sassi, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Neidert, P. L., &
Pindiprolu, S. S., Peterson, S. M. P., Rule, S., & Lignugaris/ Roscoe, E. M. (2011). Latency as an index of response
Kraft, B. (2003). Using web-mediated experiential strength during functional analysis of problem behav-
case-based instruction to teach functional behavioral ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 51–67.
assessment skills. Teacher Education and Special Van Acker, R., Boreson, L., Gable, R. A., & Potterton, T.
Education, 26, 1–16. (2005). Are we on the right course? Lessons learned
Poling, A. (1994). Pharmacological treatment of behav- about current FBA/BIP practices in schools. Journal
ioral problems in people with mental retardation: of Behavioral Education, 14, 35–56.
Some ethical considerations. In L. J. Hayes, G. J. Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J. S., Foxx, R. M.,
Hayes, S. C. Moore, & P. M. Ghezzi (Eds.), Ethical Iwata, B. A., & Lovaas, O. I. (1988). The right to
issues in developmental disabilities (pp. 149–177). effective behavioral treatment. Journal of Applied
Reno: Context. Behavior Analysis, 28, 381–384.
Poling, A., Ehrhardt, K., Wood, A., & Bowerman, R. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
(2010). Psychopharmacology and behavior analysis in H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
autism treatment. In J. A. Matson & E. A. Mayville extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
(Eds.), Behavioral foundations of effective autism aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
treatment. New York: Sloan. Analysis, 28, 561–576.
Romaniuk, C., Miltenberger, R., Conyers, C., Jenner, N., Vollmer, T. R., & Northup, J. (1996). Some implications
Jurgens, M., & Ringenberg, C. (2002). The influence of functional analysis for school psychology. School
of activity choice on problem behaviors maintained by Psychology Quarterly, 11, 76–92.
escape versus attention. Journal of Applied Behavior Vollmer, T. R., & Smith, R. G. (1996). Some current
Analysis, 35, 349–362. themes in functional analysis research. Research in
Sasso, G. M., Reimers, T. M., Cooper, L. J., Wacker, D., Developmental Disabilities, 17, 229–249.
Berg, W., Steege, M., et al. (1992). Use of descriptive Wallace, M. D., Doney, J. K., Mintz-Resudek, C. M., &
and experimental analyses to identify the functional Tarbox, R. S. F. (2004). Training educators to implement
13 Ethical Issues 233

functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Weeden, M., Mahoney, A., & Poling, A. (2010). SIB and
Analysis, 37, 89–92. FA: Where are the descriptions of participant protec-
Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session tions? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31,
duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of 299–303.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175–183. Wilder, D. A., Chen, L., Atwell, J., Pritchard, J., &
Wallace, M. D., & Knights, D. J. (2003). An evaluation of Weinstein, P. (2006). Brief functional analysis and
a brief functional analysis format within a vocational treatment of tantrums associated with transitions in
setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior
125–128. Analysis, 39, 103–107.
About the Editor

Johnny L. Matson is Professor and Distinguished Research Master, and


Director of Clinical Training in the Department of Psychology at Louisiana
State University in Baton Rouge, LA. Prior to his 25-year tenure at LSU,
Dr. Matson was a professor of Psychiatry at the University of Pittsburgh
School of Medicine. He is the author of over 600 publications including
34 books and is editor-in-chief of two journals, Research in Developmental
Disorders and Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 235


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
Index

A experimental designs, 18
ABA. See Applied behavior analysis (ABA) experimentation with humans, 5–6
A–B–C data. See Antecedent–behavior–consequence functional analysis
(A–B–C) data antecedent-only, 19
Adaptive skills, 93 data interpretation, 19
Aggression duration, 18–19
adverse consequences, 36 training, 19–20
definition, 33 historical roots, 4–5
forms and classification, 33–34 idiosyncratic variables, 17–18
populations and problems, 73–74 nonexperimental methods, 20
prevalence, 34–35 Attention alone/in combination behaviors
risk factors, 35–36 for bizarre speech, 51
Alone condition, 128 inappropriate sexual behavior, 50
American Psychological Association (APA), 213 intellectual disability, 51–52
Antecedent–behavior–consequence (A–B–C) data Prader–Willi syndrome, 52
analysis, 101 task analysis, 50–51
charts, 160–161 Attention condition, 127
contingency event recording, 109–111 Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), 25–26.
description, 65 See also Challenging behavior
recording form, 101 Automatic reinforcement
Antecedent intervention, 201–202 ABA, challenging behaviors, 10–11
APA. See American Psychological Association (APA) challenging behaviors
Applied behavior analysis (ABA) auditory masking, 58
after 1982, 13 with autism patient, 58–59
behavior observation, 91–92 dangerous behavior, 57
birth of, 6–7 intervention program, 56–57
challenging behaviors oral stimulation, 59
automatic reinforcement, 10–11 positive effects, 57
function of, 7–8 sensory extinction, 57
negative reinforcement, 9–10 description, 187
positive reinforcement, 8–9 differential reinforcement, 189–190
ecological validity, 20–21 extinction, 187–188
expansion across behaviors response-independent delivery of alternate
access to stereotypy, 17 sources, 188–189
across settings, 15–16
control, 17
low-rate behaviors, 15 B
multiple topographies, 14–15 BACB Guidelines. See Behavior Analyst Certification
precursor behaviors, 15 Board Guidelines for Responsible Conduct
tangible function, 16–17 (BACB Guidelines)
expansion across populations, 14 Behavioral skills training, 207–208

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 237


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
238 Index

Behavior analysis function of, 7–8


ABA expansion idiosyncratic events access, 59–60
access to stereotypy, 17 negative reinforcement, 9–10
across settings, 15–16 pica, vomiting and rumination
control, 17 adverse consequences, 39–40
low-rate behaviors, 15 definition, 38–39
multiple topographies, 14–15 prevalence, 39
precursor behaviors, 15 risk factors, 40
tangible function, 16–17 positive reinforcement, 8–9
measurement, treatment planning, 207 SIB
treatment methods, 179 adverse consequences, 31
Behavior Analyst Certification Board definition, 26–27
Guidelines for Responsible Conduct forms and classification, 27–28
(BACB Guidelines), 213 prevalence, 28–30
risk factors
risk factors: adaptive behavior
C functioning, 31
CAQ. See Contingency analysis questionnaire (CAQ) risk factors: age, 31
Caregiver training, 208 risk factors: ASD diagnosis, 30
Challenging behavior risk factors: autistic symptoms, 30
aggressive behavior risk factors: comorbid conditions, 30
adverse consequences, 36 risk factors: gender, 31
definition, 33 risk factors: genetic syndromes, 30–31
forms and classification, 33–34 risk factors: intellectual disability (ID), 30
prevalence, 34–35 risk factors: placement, 30
risk factors, 35–36 stereotypic behavior
ASD, 25–26 adverse consequences, 32–33
assessment strategies definition, 31–32
descriptive methods, 46–47 forms and classification, 32
functional analysis, 47–48 prevalence, 32
rating scales, 46 risk factors, 33
attention alone/in combination tangible items access, 53–54
for bizarre speech, 51 Consequence interventions, 203
inappropriate sexual behavior, 50 Contingency analysis questionnaire (CAQ), 172
intellectual disability, 51–52 Contingency event recording, 109–111
Prader–Willi syndrome, 52 Continuous event recording
task analysis, 50–51 direct observation, 111
automatic reinforcement, 10–11 psychometric properties, 114
auditory masking, 58 Control condition, 128
with autism patient, 58–59
dangerous behavior, 57
intervention program, 56–57 D
oral stimulation, 59 Dangerous behaviors, 92–93
positive effects, 57 Data
sensory extinction, 57 collection (see also Real-time
demand escape, 54–56 data issues)
description, 25–26, 45–46 by electronic equipment, 148
feeding disorders event recording, 144–146
adverse consequences, 37–38 interval recording, 99
definition, 36–37 problems in, 148–149
forms and classification, 37 time sampling, 146–148
prevalence, 37 interpretation and use, 155–156
risk factors, 38 reliability
functional analysis calculating method, 150
different assessment condition, 49 coding system, 150–151
educational/rehabilitation context, 49–50 description, 149–150
function identification, 50 observer characteristics, 153–154
and possible adaptations, 48–50 observer drift, 153
range extension, 48 reactivity, 152
short version design, 48–49 training, 151–152
Index 239

validation Event recording


description, 154 data sheet, 97
observer effects and reactivity, 154–155 intensity, 98
Demand rate, 98
condition, 127–128 sample of, 98
escape, 54–56 Experimental functional analysis (EFA)
Differential negative reinforcement of alternative alone condition, 128
behavior (DNRA) attention condition, 127
differential negative reinforcement control condition, 128
of other behavior, 186 demand, 127–128
differential reinforcement of compliance, 186 description, 125–126
functional communication training, 185–186 functional behavioral assessment, 106
response-independent delivery of negative historical overview of, 126–127
reinforcement, 187 limitations
Differential reinforcement covert behavior, 137–138
treatment methods, 183–184 difficult treatment implementation, 136–137
treatment planning, 204–205 high-intensity behavior, 138
Differential reinforcement of alternative inconclusive outcomes, 136
behavior (DRA), 204 low frequency behavior, 138
Direct observation methods time and resource constraints, 137
contingency event recording, 109–111 preassessment considerations
continuous event recording, 111 experimental design, 135–136
description, 109 informed consent, 133–134
scatter plot analysis, 111–112 medical monitoring and protection, 134
Direct scaling methods, 160. See also Scaling methods response topography, 134–135
Disruptive behavior, 130–131 session duration, 134
DNRA. See Differential negative reinforcement SIB
of alternative behavior (DNRA) disruptive behavior, 130–131
DRA. See Differential reinforcement of alternative hand banging, 130
behavior (DRA) negative reinforcement, 128–129
Duration coding positive reinforcement, 128–129
description, 100 tangible items delivery, 130–131
latency, 101 tangible condition, 128
rate, 100–101 utility of
clinical assessment, 131–132
diverse response topography, 132
E participant screening, 133
EFA. See Experimental functional analysis (EFA) research method, 132
Ethical issues translational research, 132–133
APA, 213 Extinction, 203–204
BACB guidelines, 213
FA
effective treatment, 218–219 F
and harm reduction, 217–218 FA. See Functional analysis (FA)
institutional review boards and informed FACT. See Functional assessment for multiple
consent, 216–217 causaliTy (FACT)
and primum non nocere principle, 215–216 FACTS. See Functional Assessment Checklist
FBA, 213 Teachers and Staff (FACTS)
absence of, effective intervention, 222–223 FAIP. See Functional Assessment and Intervention
best practice, 219 Program (FAIP)
competent use of, 223–224 FAST. See Functional analysis screening tool (FAST)
effective model for collaboration, 227–228 FBA. See Functional behavioral assessments (FBA).
in school setting, 220–222 Feeding disorders
training, 224–227 adverse consequences, 37–38
functional analyses vs. behavioral interventions, 229 definition, 36–37
guidelines, assessing behavior, 214 forms and classification, 37
SIB, 214 populations and problems, 77–78
systematic delivery, stimuli, 215 prevalence, 37
Event intervention, 199–201 risk factors, 38
Index 241

O observation methods
Observation methods continuous event recording, 114
description, 95–96 vs. multiple assessment methods, 115–117
direct scatter plot analysis, 114–115
contingency event recording, 109–111
continuous event recording, 111
description, 109 Q
scatter plot analysis, 111–112 QABF. See Questions about behavioral function (QABF)
duration coding, 100–101 QABF-MI. See Questions about behavior
FBA, 120–121 function–mental illness (QABF-MI)
interval recording, 98–99 QABF-SF. See Questions about behavior function–short
location, 96 form (QABF-SF)
measurement system selection Questions about behavioral function (QABF)
characteristics, 97 challenging behavior profile, 169
coding systems, 97 description, 46, 166
data collection system, 96 interview methods, 106
event recording, 97–98 multiple functions, 169–170
interval recording, 98–99 reliability
momentary time sampling, 99–100 factor structure, 168–169
psychometric properties internal consistency, 168
continuous event recording, 114 inter-rater, 166–167
vs. multiple assessment methods, 115–117 test-retest, 167–168
scatter plot analysis, 114–115 validity, 168
Operant mechanisms, 180 Questions about behavior function–mental illness
(QABF-MI), 170–171
Questions about behavior function–short form
P (QABF-SF), 170
Partial-interval recording, 99
PBQ. See Problem behavior questionnaire (PBQ)
Pica R
adverse consequences, 39–40 Real-time data issues
definition, 38–39 behavior, defining, 144
populations and problems, 75–77 data collection
prevalence, 39 by electronic equipment, 148
risk factors, 40 event recording, 144–146
Populations and problems problems in, 148–149
ABA, expansions, 14 time sampling, 146–148
aggression, 73–74 description, 143–144
feeding problem, 77–78 interpretation and use, 155–156
noncompliance behavior, 78–80 reliability of, data
pica, 75–77 calculating method, 150
property destruction, 74 coding system, 150–151
school refusal, 81 description, 149–150
SIB, 69–72 observer characteristics, 153–154
stereotyped behaviors, 72–73 observer drift, 153
tantrums, 74–75 reactivity, 152
transition, 80–81 training, 151–152
Positive reinforcement validity of, data
ABA, challenging behaviors, 8–9 description, 154
challenging behavior, 8–9 observer effects and reactivity, 154–155
experimental functional analysis, Reinforcement contingencies, 180
SIB, 128–129 advantages, 180
Problem behavior questionnaire (PBQ), 171 components, 181
Property destruction, 74 definition, 180
Punishment, 205–206 discriminative stimulus, 180
Psychometric properties operation motivation, 180
interview methods social-positive, 180
FACTS, 113 Replacement/teaching strategy, 202–203
FAIP, 113 Response-independent delivery, 184–185
student-guided functional assessment Restrictive behaviors, 93
interview, 113–114 Rumination. See Pica
242 Index

S Stereotypic behavior
Scaling methods adverse consequences, 32–33
ABC charts, 160–161 definition, 31–32
analogue methods, 161 forms and classification, 32
CAQ, 172 populations and problems, 72–73
comparison of, 172–174 prevalence, 32
direct methods, 160 risk factors, 33
FACT, 171–172 Student-assisted functional assessment interview, 108
FAST, 172 Student-guided functional assessment interview
functional assessment psychometric properties, 113–114
interviews, 162 teacher/caregiver, interview methods, 108–109
methods, 159–160
indirect methods, 161–162
informant-based scales, 162 T
MARS, 172 Tangible items
MAS access, challenging behaviors, 53–54
description, 162 experimental functional analysis, SIB, 130–131
reliability, 162–166 Tantrums, 74–75
PBQ, 171 Target behavior
QABF A–B–C analysis, 101
challenging behavior profile, 169 adaptive skills, 93
description, 166 dangerous behaviors, 92–93
multiple functions, 169–170 identification, 92
reliability, 166–169 interfering behaviors, 93
QABF-MI, 170–171 observation methods
QABF-SF, 170 description, 95–96
scatter plots, 160 duration coding, 100–101
Scatter plot analysis interval recording, 98–99
direct observation method, 111–112 location, 96
psychometric properties, 114–115 measurement system selection, 96–98
scaling methods, 160 momentary time sampling, 99–100
School refusal, 81 operational definition, 94–95
Self-injurious behavior (SIB) restrictive behaviors, 93
adverse consequences, 31 social significance, 92
definition, 26–27 Teacher/caregiver interview methods
experimental functional analysis FACTS, 107
disruptive behavior, 130–131 FAIF, 107
hand banging, 130 FAIP, 108
negative reinforcement, 128–129 student-assisted functional assessment interview, 108
positive reinforcement, 128–129 student-guided functional assessment interview,
tangible items delivery, 130–131 108–109
forms and classification, 27–28 student interviews, 108
populations and problems, 69–72 Transition, 80–81
prevalence, 28–30 Treatment methods
risk factors automatic reinforcement
adaptive behavior functioning, 31 description, 187
age, 31 differential reinforcement, 189–190
ASD diagnosis, 30 extinction, 187–188
autistic symptoms, 30 response-independent delivery of alternate
comorbid conditions, 30 sources, 188–189
gender, 31 behavior analysis, 179
genetic syndromes, 30–31 differential reinforcement, 183–184
intellectual disability (ID), 30 DNRA
placement, 30 differential negative reinforcement of other
SIB. See Self-injurious behavior (SIB) behavior, 186
Social-negative reinforcement differential reinforcement of compliance, 186
description, 185 functional communication training, 185–186
extinction, 185 response-independent delivery of negative
Social-positive reinforcement, 180–183 reinforcement, 187
Index 243

environmental variables, 179 definition and behavior measurement, 207


functional communication training, 182–18 differential reinforcement, 204–205
operant mechanisms, 180 event intervention, 199–201
reinforcement contingencies extinction, 203–204
advantages, 180 and functional assessment, 195–196
components, 181 function-based intervention, 196–197
definition, 180 function-based treatment development, 206
discriminative stimulus, 180 function clarification and procedure, 206–207
operation motivation, 180 implementation, 207
social-positive, 180 interprofessional team planning, 197–198
response-independent delivery, 184–185 modifications, 208
social-negative reinforcement punishment, 205–206
description, 185 replacement/teaching strategy, 202–203
extinction, 185
social-positive reinforcement, 180–183
Treatment planning V
additional caregiver training, 208 Vomiting. See Pica
antecedent intervention, 201–202
behavioral skills training, 207–208
competing behavior mode, 199 W
consequence interventions, 203 Whole-interval recording, 99

You might also like