You are on page 1of 9

NELTA

Mediation in Peer Interaction


Saraswati Dawadi

Abstract

This article examines the strategies used by the high-level English as a foreign language learners
to mediate understanding in peer interactions. The data was generated from peer interactions
of post graduate level students in their regular classroom. It was found that the students were
able to understand communicative intent of their peers and provide support to each other;
they worked collaboratively and co-constructed knowledge. They used different strategies to
mediate understanding. The major strategies included: repetition, elaboration, definition,
contextual cues, scaffolding, paralinguistic cues and real life examples.

Key words: mediation, zone of proximal development (ZPD), peer interaction, strategies, and
scaffolding.

Introduction worlds in which our tools consist of


symbols, e.g., numbers, graphs, models,
Mediation broadly means the use of some drawings, and linguistic symbols (Lantolf
auxiliary objects or tools to perform an 2006; Karpov &Hayward, 1998). When
activity. It is the creation and use of interacting with other human beings, we
artiûcial auxiliary means of acting mostly use linguistic symbols, and language
physically, socially and mentally (Lantolf, is one of the most important symbolic
2009). Vygotsky (1978) argues that humans systems for the mediation of human mental
do not act directly on the physical world; activity (Lantolf, 2009).
instead, rely on physical and symbolic tools
For mediation to be useful, it must be
(the auxiliaries) created by human
sensitive to the individual’s or even group’s
culture(s) over time and are made available
zone of proximal development (ZPD)
to succeeding generations. In the physical
(Lantolf, 2009). Vygotsky (1978) defines
world, such auxiliaries include: shovels,
ZPD as “ the distance between the actual
hammers, bulldozers, dynamite, etc., which
developmental level as determined by
greatly enhance our capacity to perform a
independent problem solving and the level
task with less effort. For example, when we
of potential development as determined
need to dig a hole in the ground to plant a
through problem solving under adult
tree, we use a shovel (the auxiliary means)
guidance, or in collaboration with more
to make our tasks much easier than digging
capable peers” (p.86). Vygotsky viewed
it with our own hands. The shovel functions
peer interaction as an effective way of
as a mediator to perform our tasks
developing skills in learners, and believed
efficiently. Similarly, we use such auxiliary
that when a learner is in the ZPD for a
means even in the social and psychological
particular task, providing the appropriate

Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014 47


NELTA

assistance will give the learner enough SCT views language learners not as
‘boost’ to do the task successfully. Thus, processers of input or producers of output
ZPD is concerned with the features of but as speakers/ hearers involved in
language learners; the concrete activities developmental processes which are realized
they participate in, and as a result of which in interaction (Ohta, 2000). Higher mental
what is unachievable alone becomes activity in a human arises in the interactions
achievable (Lantolf, 2009). This indicates when we enter into with other members in
that a person, who mediates, needs to know our society (Lantolf, 2009; Donato &
what an individual has already achieved McCormick, 1994). Watanbe (2008) agrees
and what s/he can achieve with assistance with Vygotsky (1978) that learning
or additional mediation. Additional originates in social mediation; concepts
mediation may come from someone else emerge through dynamic interaction,
from the integration of artefacts. According shaping and transforming each other in an
to Olmedo (2003) participation in a interconnected system. Therefore, language
mediation role in a peer interaction development is intrinsically a social process
includes the following steps: (Apple and Lantolf, 1994).

a. The learner (language mediator) Another view of SCT is that language


must understand the communicative development occurs in the context of joint
intent of another person’s speech in activity (Vygotsky, 1978). Walqui (2006)
whatever way or whatever language believes learning to be deeply embedded in
it is expressed. a socio cultural milieu as he thinks,
b. The person must monitor the “learning is a matter not only of cognitive
behaviour of a peer or peers to be sure development but also of shared social
that they have not understood his practices” (p.159). Similarly, van Lier
message. (1996) appears to believe “in order to learn,
a person must be active” (p.171).
c. The learner should select proper
Development does not proceed as the
strategy that can ensure proper
unfolding of inborn capacities, but as the
understanding of the message
transformation of innate capacities once
conveyed by him.
they intertwine with socio culturally
d. The learner must address the peers constructed meditational means (Lantolf &
using that strategy. Pavlenko, 1995).
Theoretical Framework
Peer interaction
The construct of mediation is central to the
The nature of peer interaction involving L2
socio cultural theory (SCT) of Vygotsky
learners and its significance to learning has
(1986). The foundational principle of SCT
received some attention in the second
is that all forms of human mental
language acquisition (SLA) literature. It is
(cognitive and emotional) activity that
generally assumed that peer interaction
includes learning a language are mediated
promotes learning. L2 learners are expected
by psychological tools such as language,
to be sensitive to the language skills of their
signs and symbols (Karpov & Hayward,
peers (Donato, 1994) and capable of
1998). Swain, Lapkin, Knouzi, Suzuki and
providing support to their peers for
Brooks (2009) and van Lier (2000) view that
learning. They try to create a context of
when a human interacts with other human
shared understanding in which the
beings and artefacts; his/her behaviour is
negotiation of language form and meaning
mediated by sign/ symbol system.
co-occur (Donato, 1994).

48 Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014


NELTA

They can also provide scaffolding to their knowledgeable learner(s). Thus, the two
peers quite efficiently using interactive interaction patterns: collaborative and
strategies that appear sensitive to their expert- novice, have collaborative
ZPD; which helps to maximize orientation and the other two (dominant-
communication and comprehension dominant and dominant- passive) have
(Lantolf, 2009).Wood, Bruner and Rose non-collaborative orientation. Learning is
(1976) define scaffolding as”Those elements generally expected to be more effective in
of the task that are initially beyond the the pairs or groups with collaborative
learner’s capacity, thus permitting him to orientation than in the pairs or groups with
concentrate upon and complete only those non-collaborative orientation (Stroch,
elements that are within his range of 2002).
competence”(p.90). Successful scaffolding
requires peers or group members to respect Related Research
one another’s perspectives and trust each
other’s opinions (Stone, 1993).It is assumed There has been a growing interest
that L2 learners can exploit the affordances investigating the ways L2 learners mediate
(van Lier, 2000) or occasions for learning while interacting with peers. Donato
(Swain & Lapkin, 1998) which are made (1994) collected protocols from third
available by their peers in their interaction. semester students of French at an American
Highlighting the learning that takes place University who participated in a
in peer interaction, Olmendo (2003) states collaborative planning. The study showed
that group members, as a mediator, that the university level students are able
canunderstand the communicative intent to construct collectively a scaffold for each
of their peers’ speech, judge their other’s performance in a collaborative
understanding and facilitate their interaction. Similarly, Olmedo (2003)
comprehension. generated data from elementary school
children in Chicago. His study found that
However, it should be noted that there can even the small children (the beginner-level
be different patterns of interaction and L2 learners of English) use different types
certain patterns of interaction are claimed of strategies to mediate understanding such
to be more conducive to learning than those as: translation, paraphrasing, code
of others (e.g Nelson & Murphy, 1993; switching, scaffolding by providing verbal
Stroch, 2001, 2002). According to Storch cues, paralinguistic cues, modelling the
(2002), there are four different patterns of behaviour, providing situational cues and
interaction: collaborative, dominant- interpreting contextual and situational
dominant, dominant- passive and expert- cues. Additionally, in Watanbe’s (2008)
novice. While in collaborative interaction, study both the higher and the lower
two or more learners work together proficiency peers could provide
throughout the task completion, in opportunities for learning when they
dominant-dominant interaction learners worked collaboratively.
show unwillingness to engage with each
other’s contribution. Similarly, in While Storch (2002) claims learning to be
dominant- passive interaction, a dominant more effective in pairs that interact in an
learner takes control of the task and the expert-novice relationship, Lantolf (2009)
passive learner or learners maintain(s) a believes that all language development does
subservant role, but in expert- novice not emerge from expert– novice mediation;
interaction a learner who is more learning can emerge even in the absence of
knowledgeable supports less expert mediation. Furthermore, Swain (1995)

Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014 49


NELTA

highlights that dialogues among learners can participated in interactions in which they had
be as effective as the instructional to talk to each other and understand some
conversations between teachers and learners concepts related to second language
and Lantolf (2009) further claims, “dialogic acquisition.
mediation among peers is likely to be more
effective than the monologic mediation Method
displayed by teachers” (p.84).
This study is solely based on the primary
However, Gibbons (2003) investigated on source of data. The data was generated from
how the teachers, through interaction with a real class devoted to developing the
students, mediate between the students’ learners’ understanding about different
current linguistic level in English and the social approaches to language learning. The
understanding of the content of the subject. students in their class were divided into
The study indicated that teachers mediate different groups and two of the groups were
language learning in several ways. The randomly selected to record their voices.
major strategies used by them include mode The recording was continued for about an
shifting through recasting, indicating the hour during which both groups
need for reformulation, signalling to participated in three interactions. Then, one
learners how to formulate and of the interactions from each group was
recontextualizing personal knowledge. purposively selected and transcribed to
investigate the strategies used by the
Although a good number of studies have students to mediate understanding. While
focused on peer interaction, very little transcribing the data, pseudonyms were
research has documented how L2 learners used.
mediate understanding in peer interaction.
Furthermore, to the best of my knowledge, Data Analysis
no attempt has been made to explore the
strategies used by high-level EFL learners In this section the interaction of the
to mediate understanding when engaged in participants is presented and the way they
peer interaction. To this end, this study has mediate understanding is analyzed. The
been designed to investigate on the same. interaction patterns and the tasks they were
involved in are as follows.
Subjects
Task 1
This study included six post graduate
students studying at Lancaster University, In this task, the students were asked to
UK. Four of them were studying their MA in discuss the question ‘Can language be used
TESOL (Teaching English to Speakers of without consideration of context?’
Other Languages) and the other two were
Sabu: Ok… what do you think?
doing a MA in TEFL (Teaching English as a
Roj: No…
Foreign Language). They were from two
different countries (China and Japan). All the Sabu: You mean…?
participants had learnt English as a foreign Roj: um… no language without
language in their countries, and none had context…uh because we always
the experience of living and studying in a use language to communicate.
foreign country prior to their visit to the UK. ..Ok we not communicate and
Their ages ranged between 25 to 35 years. use language (Laughs) <6>. It
During the data collection period, they cannot be separate.

50 Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014


NELTA

Sabu: Yes...(Laughs)… They cannot information, may be, to highlight the idea
separate. What do you think? presented by her friend or to show her
[May be indicating to Saru] agreement. After this, it can be seen that
Saru: …Uh… Communication should Sabu is unable to understand the
be based on context. pronunciation when Saru pronounces the
Sabu: Contact? word ‘context’. Saru modifies her
Saru: Context (modified pronunciation but it also does not work.
pronunciation) Then, Roj appears to mediate between them
with another pronunciation.
Sabu: Con-?
Roj: Context Next, following Saru, Sabu tries to add
Sabu: Oh, context. more information to make her idea more
Saru: Yeah, Ok… I cannot think any clear. This time, she tries to make the
example without consideration concept of ‘context’ clear, using two
of context. examples: linguistics and social but Saru
Sabu: Yeah fails to understand the second term (social)
Roj: Yeah and asks for clarification. Then, to mediate
Sabu: Without consi - I think it’s understanding, Sabu gives a definition of
meaningful... because <6> in the the term; as a result, Saru is able to
absence of context no language understand.
takes place… we need at least a
context …for example … It can Task 2
be linguistics …
Roj: Yeah… In this task, the students are asked to find
the meaning of ‘Ellipsis’ and find examples
Sabu: Or …social
from their language.
Saru: Social?
Sabu: It means uhh… the real situation Excerpt 2
in the world or the society in
which the language is used. Rosy: I am not very understand
Saru: Oh, yeah. Thank you. ellipsis… What is ellipsis?
Suravi: Umm… No idea…
(Note: The transcription is based on the
convention used by Brooks, Swain, Lapkin Sony: Uh...This is omission… deletion of
and Knouzi 2010; see the conventions in some items.
Appendix A).
Sony: Mm …deletion?
In the excerpt above, it can be observed that Sony: Yeah, deleting some parts from an
the participants work collaboratively to utterance.
build knowledge. At the beginning, they
Rosy: Umm… How?
might not have clear concept about the
content, but they initiate interaction and Sony: … For example <6> If somebody
use different techniques to mediate says ‘What do you like? Tea or
understanding. The interaction is initiated coffee?’ What would be your
by Sabu who might not have an idea about answer?
the question. Roj replies to her using a single
Rosy: Tea.
word and Sabu fails to understand and
makes a clarification request. Then, Roj Suravi: Tea.
elaborates the idea and Sabu recasts the

Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014 51


NELTA

Sony: Yeah…Simply Tea. Then what is repeats the information. But the repetition
the meaning here?... Means, I like does not help and Rosy asks for
tea. (slowing the pace and using clarification. Then, Sony changes her
low tone ) strategy and provides an example which,
most probably, happens in real life situation
Suravi: Yeah
of her peers ‘What do you like tea or
Rosy: Yeah coffee?’With this example, she is trying to
Sony: …This is an example of ellipsis… contextualize personal knowledge
Got the idea? (Gibbons, 2003). Then, again when she says
the sentence, ‘I like tea’ she slows down her
Suravi: Oh, Yeah. pace and uses a high and low intonation
Rosy: Can you give example from your pattern. This seems to be her another
language? strategy to mediate understanding. She also
makes comprehension check by saying ‘Got
Suravi: Umm… It’s very similar in my the idea?’ to be sure that they had
language too… understood. At the end of the conversation,
the sentence, ‘It is very similar to my
(Note: The transcription is based on the language’ produced by Suravi gives an
convention used by Brooks et al. 2010; see indication that Suravi has now clearly
the conventions in Appendix A). understood the concept.

This excerpt shows ‘expert- novice’ pattern Discussion and Conclusion


of interaction (See, Storch, 2002). At the
beginning of the interaction, Rosy and The results can be explained by reference
Suravi seem to be unfamiliar with the term to Vygotsky’s (1978) theory of cognitive
ellipsis but Sony seems to have understood development. Languaging, using language
the term. The interaction indicates that as a cognitive tool to make and shape
more knowledgeable person or expert meaning (Swain, 2006), has helped the
(Sony) supports less knowledgeable persons participants to mediate their
(Rosy and Suravi) to understand the understanding and knowledge is
concept; as a result, Rosy and Suravi are constructed actively by the learners
able to understand the meaning of the term. (Donato, 1994).
Thus, it can be argued that their peer (Sony)
is able to provide them with an opportunity It is quite interesting to observe that the
for learning (Wantabe, 2008; Stroch, 2002). two interactions selected for the study
Sony uses different strategies to mediate represent two different types of interaction
understanding (Olmendo, 2003). patterns: collaborative and expert- novice;
learning took place effectively in both. The
It can be seen that the very first utterance, first interaction indicates that the peers are
‘I am not very understand ellipsis’ working collaboratively. At the beginning,
produced by Rosy is not grammatically all of them seem to be somehow unfamiliar
correct but it still carries a meaning so her with the content, but as a result of the
peers are able to understand her. To mediate interaction, they are able to co-construct
understanding, Sony starts with giving the their knowledge. The results are consistent
meaning of the word ellipsis. She uses the with the previous studies (e.g. Lantolf,
word ‘omission’ then she substitutes it with 2009; Watanbe, 2008; Donato, 1940) that
another word ‘deletion’. Rosy is not able to collaborative work among the participants
understand the meaning again but Sony provides them an opportunity to support

52 Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014


NELTA

each other and co-construct knowledge. This communicative intent of their peers and
might also provide evidence to the claims capable of using different strategies to
made by Vygotsky (1978) that learning is a mediate understanding; learning takes
social process and by Lantolf (2009) that place in both types of interaction:
learning takes place even in the absence of collaborative and expert- novice. More
expert mediation. However, the second importantly, perhaps from a pedagogical
interaction (expert - novice) indicates that perspective, the finding of this study
more knowledgeable learner can create a suggests that, when students get an
supportive environment, in which less opportunity to work in group, they are able
knowledgeable learners can participate and to produce comprehensible input to their
acquire knowledge (Greenfield, 1984). Hence, peers(Pica, 1989),as a result learning is
this finding is in line with the finding by likely to occur. Thus, the implication of this
Storch (2002) that learning becomes more study is that peer interaction in an EFL
effective in pairs that interact in an expert- classroom may provide learners with an
novice relationship. When learners are opportunity to learn from their peers.
engaged in a peer interaction, a novice or less
knowledgeable learner gets an opportunity About the author
to learn from more knowledgeable person
or expert ( see Vygosky 1978). Saraswati Dawadi is a lecturer at the
Department of English Education, Tribhuvan
Very similar to the previous studies University, Kirtipur. Currently, she is a PhD
(Donato 1994; Olmando, 2003; Watanbe scholar at The Open University, Milton
2008), this study indicates that L2 learners Keynes, England. Her research interests
use different strategies like repetition, include language assessment, second language
elaboration, contextual cues, paralinguistic acquisition and task based language teaching.
cues (modified pronunciation, intonation
pattern, pace of speech etc.) and real life References
examples to mediate understanding. They
also appear to be able to provide scaffolded Appel, G., & Lantolf, J. (1994). Speaking as
help to their peers during the interactions mediation: A study of Li and L2 recall
(Donato, 1994 and Webb, 1989) and tasks. The Modern Language Journal, 78,
facilitate comprehension of their peers 437-452.
(Olmendo, 2003; Webb, 1989). This further
indicates that L2 learners work
Brooks, L., Swain, M., Lapkin, S., & Knouzi,
collaboratively and all the group members
I. (2010). Mediating between scientific
equally contribute to the task completion
and spontaneous concepts through
when all of them are somehow equally
languaging. Language Awareness,19
knowledgeable about the task. As a result
(2), 89-110.
of the collaborative effort, they can co-
construct knowledge. But, when a group
consists of the members who are not equally Donato, R. (1994). Collective scaffolding in
knowledgeable, all the members may not be second language learning.Vygotskian
able to contribute equally to task approaches to second language
completion. However, learning takes place research, 33456.
in such interactions too.
Donato, R., & McCormick, D. (1994). A
Thus, it can be concluded that the high-level sociocultural perspective on language
EFL learners are sensitive to the learning strategies: The role of

Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014 53


NELTA

mediation. The Modern Language Storch, N. (2002). Patterns of interaction in


Journal, 78(4), 453-464. ESL pair work. Language
learning,52(1), 119-158.
Gibbons, P. (2003). Mediating language
learning: Teacher interactions with Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output
ESL students in a content based in second language learning. In G.
classroom. Tesol Quarterly, 37(2), 247- Cook & B. Seidlhofer (eds.), Principle
273. & practice in applied linguistics. Studies
in Honour of H. G.Widdowson, Oxford:
Karpov, Y. V., & Haywood, H. C. (1998). Oxford University Press, 125-44.
Two ways to elaborate Vygotsky’s
concept of mediation. American Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis
Psychologist, 53(1), 27. and beyond: Mediating acquisition
through collaborative
Lantolf, J. P. (2006). Sociocultural theory dialogue. Sociocultural theory and
and L2: State of the art. Studies in Second second language learning, 97, 114.
Language Acquisition, 28(01), 67-109.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (1998). Interaction
Lantolf, J. P. (2009). Second language learning and second language learning: Two
as a mediated process. Language adolescent French immersion
Teaching, 33(2), 79-96. students working together. The
Modern Language Journal, 82(3), 320-
Lantolf, J.P. (2011). The sociocultural 337.
approach to second language
acquisition. In Dwight, A. (Ed) Swain, M., Lapkin, S., Knouzi, I., Suzuki, W.,
Alternative approaches to second & Brooks, L. (2009). Languaging:
language acquisition. London, New University students learn the
York: Routledge. grammatical concept of voice in
French. The Modern Language
Lantolf, J. P., & Pavlenko, A. (1995). Journal, 93(1), 5-29.
Sociocultural theory and second
language acquisition. Annual Review Van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the
of Applied Linguistics, 15(1), 108-124. language curriculum: Awareness,
autonomy and authenticity. London:
Ohta, A.S. (2000). Rethinking interaction in Longman.
SLA: Developmentally appropriate
assistance in the zone of proximal Van Lier, L. (2000). From input to
development and the acquisition of affordance: Social-interactive
L2 grammar. In J.P. Lantolf (Ed.), learning from an ecological
Sociocultural theory and second perspective. Sociocultural theory and
language learning (pp. 51–78). Oxford: second language learning, 245- 259.
Oxford University Press.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society.
Olmedo, I.M. (2003). Language mediation Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
among emergent bilingual children. Press.
Linguistics and education, 14(2), 143-162

54 Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014


NELTA

Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language. Wood, D., Bruner, J., and Ross, G. (1976).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. The role of tutoring in problem
solving. Journal of Child Psychology
Walqui, A. (2006). Scaffolding instruction for and Child Psychiatry, 17, 89"100.
English language learners: A
conceptual framework. International Appendix 1
Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism, 9(2), 159-180. Transcription Convention

1. ... Pause of less than 5 seconds


Watanabe, Y. (2008). Peer–peer interaction
between L2 learners of different 2. [ ] Transcriber’s commentary
proficiency levels: Their interactions
and reflections. Canadian Modern 3. <> pause of more than 5 seconds
Language Review/La Revue canadienne
des langues vivantes, 64(4), 605-635. 4. - incomplete utterance

Webb, N. M. (1989). Peer interaction and 5. Bold emphasis


learning in small groups. International
Journal of Educational Research, 13(1), 21- 6. Underlining Overlapped speech
39.

Journal of NELTA, Vol 19 No. 1-2, December 2014 55

You might also like