You are on page 1of 9

Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychology of Sport and Exercise


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport

The role of emotion in bridging the intention–behaviour gap: The case


of sports participation
Changiz Mohiyeddini a, *, Regina Pauli a, Stephanie Bauer b
a
Whitelands College, Roehampton University, Holybourne Avenue, London SW15 4JD, London, UK
b
Center for Psychotherapy Research, University Hospital Heidelberg, Bergheimer Street 54, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Problem: Psychological models of sports participation frequently draw on the Theory of Planned
Received 30 August 2007
Behaviour (TPB) in order to predict health-behaviour-related intentions. While these models commonly
Received in revised form 19 August 2008
show high predictive power with respect to intention, they often fall short in the prediction of behaviour
Accepted 22 August 2008
Available online 13 September 2008 (the ‘intention–behaviour gap’). The present study contends that integrating emotional processes into
TPB can substantially improve the model’s predictive power over and above traditional cognitive
predictors.
Keywords:
Intention–behaviour gap Method: In a longitudinal study structural equation modelling was employed to test a traditional model
Emotions (based on TPB) and an extended model including emotion associated with the intention to exercise as
Theory of planned behaviour a mediator variable. A community sample (N ¼ 237) completed a questionnaire measuring the compo-
Physical exercise nents of a traditional TPB model. Prior to each of 16 expected exercise sessions (over 8 weeks) intention
Health behaviour to exercise and emotion associated with the intention were measured.
Maintenance of physical activity
Results: Results confirmed a substantial increase of 17% in explained variance of exercise frequency and
20% in exercise duration for the extended model. Emotional appraisal of the intention to exercise thus
appears to mediate the traditional intention–behaviour relationship, suggesting that emotionally based
interventions aimed at increasing sports participation may be helpful.
Conclusion: It is concluded that emotion variables should be added to traditional TPB models in order to
predict health behaviour more fully.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Regular physical activity is an important component of a healthy which relate to perceived expectations of others, and attitudes
lifestyle (Pate, Dowda, O’Neill, & Ward, 2007). Empirical results which can be positive or negative towards the specific intended
show that participation in physical activity is low in significant behaviour. PBC, which is defined as the perception of subjective
proportions of the population (Dishman & Buckworth, 2001; Mar- control at the point of translation of goal states into actual behav-
tinez-Gonzalez et al., 2001) and that interventions designed to iour, predicts behaviour both directly and via intentions (Hagger,
increase sports participation do not always have the desired effect Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002).
on behaviour despite increasing intention in participants to become Applications of TPB to the relationship between intention to
physically more active (Milne, Orbell, & Sheeran, 2002). exercise and subsequent behaviour have revealed that 45% of the
A number of social and health behaviours have been studied in variance in intention can be predicted from PBC, subjective norms
relation to the theory of planned behaviour (TPB, Ajzen, 1991), and attitudes but only 27% of the variance in exercise behaviour can
which contends that intention is a primary predictor of subsequent be predicted on the basis of intention (based on Hagger et al.’s
behaviour (Armitage & Conner, 2000; Wallston & Armstrong, (2002) meta-analysis of 72 studies). Sutton’s (1998) review also
2002). TPB postulates that behaviour is determined by behavioural indicated that 19–38% of the variance in behaviour can be explained
intentions and perceived behavioural control (PBC). Intentions are by intention and PBC. Similarly, in a recent longitudinal study 49%
defined as explicit, behaviourally orientated decisions to act, i.e. of the variance in intention could be explained by PBC, subjective
formulations of goal states which are achievable through behav- norms and attitudes, whereas only 22% of the variance in actual
iour. They are independently influenced by subjective norms, exercise behaviour could be explained (Armitage, 2005). In
a related vein, an experimental study conducted by Chatzisarantis
and Hagger (2005) demonstrated that attitudes can predict inten-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ44 (0) 20 8392 3616; fax: þ44 (0) 20 8392 3527. tions sufficiently whereas intentions do not always lead to physical
E-mail address: c.mohiyeddini@roehampton.ac.uk (C. Mohiyeddini). activity.

1469-0292/$ – see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.08.005
C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234 227

The implication is that intention formation is well understood as sadness. In contrast, positive emotional responses such as excite-
it can be well predicted, but the processes involving the translation ment or happiness will appear in situations in which no serious
of intentions into behaviour are not fully accounted for by current barriers counteract the implementation of a person’s intention.
models. This ‘‘intention–behaviour gap’’ (Sheeran, 2002) is also Such positive responses can enhance a person’s motivation to act
evident in a lack of temporal stability of the intention–behaviour (Chen & Bargh, 1999) e.g. by stimulating approach or avoidance
relationship. Recent research has focused on cognitive variables as tendencies (see Carver, Sutton, & Scheier, 2000). Partly based on
mediators of the intention–behaviour relationship, e.g. motiva- Carver et al.’s (2000) work, we assume that the emotional states
tional and volitional variables. Milne et al. (2002) showed that that are activated by the formulation of intentions may support
interventions directed at volitional (aimed at implementation initiating (and maintaining) exercise behaviour because one feels it
intention) as well as motivational variables significantly increased is a positive thing to do (approach tendency) or because of a need to
subsequent likelihood of exercise behaviour compared with inter- avoid the negative consequences of not doing so (avoidance
ventions to increase motivation alone. Moreover, empirical tendency). In line with this, Bagozzi et al. (1998) assume that the
evidence has emerged, showing that self-regulation processes attainment of a sub goal leads to positive emotions and to the
(Abraham, Sheeran, & Johnston, 1998) and perceived self-efficacy continuation of a person’s current plan, while the failure to achieve
(Coumeya & McAuley, 1994; Estabrooks & Carron, 1998) can explain a sub goal is associated with negative emotions. Consequently,
additional variance in behaviour. positive emotions may facilitate the translation of intentions to
exercise into action as well as increase the frequency and duration
Emotions and the intention–behaviour gap of physical activity. In contrast, negative emotions may lead to
a decrease of the likelihood of commencing an exercise regime, as
It is generally agreed in psychology that behaviour can be well as to a decrease of the frequency of exercise sessions and the
generated through multiple processes. Hence it is our contention duration of each exercise event.
that emotions act as an important missing link in the intention– There is some support for the assumption that emotions affect
behaviour gap. Conner and Armitage (1998) have already pointed intention and action. Zhu and Thagard (2002) have argued that
out, that an important shortcoming of TPB is its exclusion of emotions significantly affect action generation (intention to act) as
affective processes, which are crucial for the intentional process well as action execution and control. This means that emotions are
(Van der Pligt, Zeelenberg, van Dijk, de Vries, & Richard, 1998). Even considered to be positive or negative responses towards an inten-
though attitudes as a component of TPB are usually described to ded action relevant in mediating the association between intention
have an affective component (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993), this and behaviour. Additional support stems from qualitative studies
component seems to be so closely correlated to cognitive and concluding that emotional mediation of behaviour may play a role
behavioural components of attitudes that its (unique) impact on in the maintenance of exercise behaviour. Specifically, positive
behaviour is hardly measurable (e.g. Dillon & Kumar, 1985). Hence emotional outcomes were shown to be related to intention, moti-
we suggest adding emotions as a separate component to the TPB. vation and behaviour in participants engaging in outdoor aerobic
In line with Perugini and Bagozzi (2001), the nature and role of exercise (Marttila & Nupponen, 2000).
emotions as suggested here differ from attitudes mainly in two In sum, a variety of theoretical perspectives provide the basis for
respects: 1) while our specification of emotions is focused on the assuming that the inclusion of emotion as a mediator between
intention to exercise, attitudes focus directly on the actual behav- intention and behaviour can lead to a significant increase in the
iour, 2) emotions are considered as short-term and dynamic variance accounted for in exercise behaviour. Mohiyeddini and
processes. In contrast, attitudes are assumed to be learnt and Bauer (2007) have shown that including a single assessment of the
disposition-like reactions to specific objects or actions. A further emotional states that are activated by the formulation of the
difference between emotions and attitudes in the present study intention to exercise yielded a significant increase in the variance
pertains to the measurement of these constructs. In accordance explained compared with traditional TBP models based only on
with Perugini and Bagozzi (2001), we suggest using uniploar items cognitive predictors. This was the case despite a considerable time
to assess emotions (for example, the experience of e.g. ‘alertness’ lag (4 months) between assessment of predictors of intention,
along a continuum from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’) and bipolar items intention and associated emotion on the one hand and measure-
to assess attitudes. ment of subsequent exercise duration on the other, indicating that
The questions on how cognition (e.g. intention) and emotion this may be a promising approach in addressing the intention–
interrelate and on the function of emotion in relation to action are behaviour gap with respect to exercise behaviour. However,
currently unresolved and controversial questions in theories of Mohiyeddini and Bauer (2007) excluded non-intenders and those
emotion (Leventhal & Scherer, 1987; Scherer, Schorr, & Johnstone, who failed to translate their intentions to exercise into actual
2001; Solomon, 2000). However, the way in which intentions may behaviour thus limiting the generalisability of their findings. They
cause an emotional reaction which in turn affects subsequent suggest that a longitudinal study in which cognitive and emotional
behaviour is addressed in Bagozzi’s theory of self-regulation components of the model are repeatedly assessed may serve to
(Bagozzi, 1992; Bagozzi, Baumgartner, & Pierters, 1998; Bagozzi, resolve the issue of whether emotion is an important additional
Moore, & Leone, 2004). This theory suggests that behaviour can be variable in explaining variance in exercise frequency and duration.
conceived as a goal-directed activity and can be studied from the Based on these considerations, we conducted a longitudinal study
perspective of trying to act, where people set goals and make plans to examine the predictive validity of the traditional TPB model as
to implement their decisions. According to this theory, the inten- well as the extended TPB model in which emotions were included
tion–emotion link may depend on the success (or failure) of the as a mediator variable.
implementation of these decisions, i.e. ultimately on the success (or
failure) of goal attainment. This success requires planning and
continuous commitment despite potential (internal and external) The present research
barriers as well as the self-managed ability to resist upcoming
temptations (Bagozzi et al., 2004). In the present study the frequency and the duration of physical
Consequently, various impediments (e.g. lack of personal activity, the intention to exercise as well as the emotional appraisal
competencies, situational barriers, contradicting intentions) can of the intention were measured on 16 occasions (twice weekly)
lead to a negative emotional response such as frustration or over a two month period. In line with the TPB it was expected that
228 C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234

intention to become physically active is influenced by attitudes, (Fs(5, 231) < 1.96, n.s.), educational attainment groups (Fs(2,
subjective norms and PBC. 234) < 2.69, n.s.), and income groups (Fs(5, 231) < 2.31, n.s.) with
Furthermore we assumed that the manifest exercise behaviour respect to the variables measured in this study and they were not
measured in terms of exercise frequency (as indicator for mainte- considered in any further analysis.
nance of physical activity) and duration can be predicted from
intention to become physically active and PBC. It was further Previous experience with exercise
hypothesised that emotions associated with the intention to Participants were asked to evaluate their experience with
become physically active, serve as mediators (see Baron & Kenny, exercise on a four point scale (inexperienced, some experience,
1986) in the intention–behaviour relationship and their inclusion experienced, highly experienced) due to findings that previous
leads to a substantial increase in the explained variance of exercise behaviour significantly influences intentions, attitudes, subjective
frequency and duration. norms, PBC, and behaviour (Bagozzi, 1981; Hagger, Chatzisarantis,
Biddle, & Orbell, 2001; Hagger et al., 2002). 36.3% of participants
Method described themselves as inexperienced, 40.1% as having some
experience, 16.5% as experienced and 7.2% as highly experienced.
Participants The four groups did not differ in respect of any of the variables
measured in this study (Fs(3, 233) < 1.39, n.s.). Therefore previous
A German speaking Zurich community sample of 285 people experience with exercise was not analyzed any further.
initially agreed to participate in this study in response to infor-
mation distributed via mailing lists. However 29 of these did not Attitudes, subjective norms and PBC
respond to the initial researcher correspondence (see procedure). A Attitudes, subjective norm and PBC were measured with
further 19 participants completed an initial questionnaire pack but a slightly modified German version of Armitage’s items (see
either failed to confirm their password electronically (n ¼ 14) or Armitage, 2005, p. 237): Attitudes were measured using a 6 item
made no further contact subsequently (n ¼ 5). No data is available semantic differential scale. Participants rated adjective pairs in the
from these participants and they were therefore excluded from the context of the following item anchor; ‘‘Exercising twice a week is
analysis. The remaining 237 participants were on average 26.94 ..... for me.’’. Adjective pairs included dull–interesting (item
years old (SD ¼ 9.9; range ¼ 17–69). Male participants (n ¼ 108) A1), unpleasant–pleasant (item A2), boring–stimulating (item A3),
yielded a mean of 26.25 years (SD ¼ 9.29; range ¼ 18–64) and the unhealthy–healthy (item A4), bad–good (item A5), and useless–
mean age of the female participants (n ¼ 129) was 27.51 years useful (item A6). These were rated on a seven point scale from 3 to
(SD ¼ 10.55; range ¼ 17–69). There were no age differences þ3. Cronbach’s alpha for the attitude scale was 0.74.
between male and female participants (t(235) ¼ 0.97, n.s.). Subjective norms were measured with three items. These were
‘‘People close to me think I should participate in regular physical
Variables activity’’ (disagree/agree) (item SN1); ‘‘People who are important to
me would.’’ (disapprove of my participating in regular physical
Age and gender activity/approve of my participating in regular physical activity) (item
Previous findings indicate that sample characteristics such as SN2); and ‘‘People close to me think I.’’ (should not participate in
age and gender may influence the relative contributions of the TPB regular physical activity /should participate in regular physical activity)
cognitions on explanation of intention and behaviour (Ajzen & (item SN3). Subjective norm items were rated on a unipolar scale (þ1
Fishbein, 1980; Henderson, Bialeschki, Shaw, & Freysinger, 1999; to þ7). Cronbach’s alpha for the subjective norms scale was 0.84.
Sheeran & Orbell, 1998). Additionally, it was found that engagement Four items were used to measure PBC. These were ‘‘To what
in physical activity declines with age (Armstrong & McManus, extent do you see yourself as being capable of participating in
1994). In this study gender differences were explored by ANCOVA regular physical activity?’’ (incapable–capable) (item PBC1); ‘‘How
with age as a covariate. The covariate was not significant (Fs(1, confident are you that you will be able to participate in regular
234) < 3.53, n.s.). Furthermore, males and females did not differ in physical activity?’’ (not very confident–very confident) (item PBC2);
attitudes, subjective norms, PBC, intention, emotions, training ‘‘I believe I have the ability to participate in regular physical
duration or frequency (Fs(1, 234) < 3.18, n.s.). Therefore, gender and activity’’ (definitely do not–definitely do) (item PBC3); and ‘‘How
age were excluded from further analysis. much personal control do you feel you have over participating in
regular physical activity?’’ (no control–complete control) (item
Socioeconomic status (SES) PBC4). All items were measured on unipolar (þ1 to þ7) scales.
Previous studies have shown that physical activity is related to Cronbach’s alpha for the PBC scale was 0.83.
SES. Low SES, i.e. low education level, income and/or blue-collar
occupation, was found to decrease the probability of physical Intention
activity (Brown & Siahpush, 2006). Therefore we measured the level Intention to engage in exercise was repeatedly assessed at 16
of education, income and occupation. In the present study, 46.4% of time points, prior to each expected exercise event. Based on Blan-
participants were employed or self-employed, 21.5% were students, chard, Rodgers, Courneya, Daub, and Knapik (2002), intention was
21.5% described themselves as home makers, 8% were unemployed measured with two items: ‘‘My goal is to exercise tomorrow for at
and 2.5% were retired. In terms of educational attainment, 33.3% of least 30 minutes’’ and ‘‘I intend to exercise tomorrow for at least 30
participants held a university degree, 31.6% were educated to minutes’’. Participants rated both items on a seven point bipolar
tertiary level (Matura) and 35% had completed high school (a scale ranging from 3 (not at all true for me) to þ3 (completely true
minimum of 9 years of full time schooling). for me).
For the assessment of income, we used a 6 point income scale.
The categories were chosen by the following portions of partici- Emotional appraisal
pants: up to 25000CHF (approx. USD 20000) per year: 1.7%, up to Emotion associated with the intention to exercise was assessed
40000CHF (approx. USD 33000): 17.3%, up to 55000CHF (approx. immediately following each measurement of intention. Partici-
USD 45000): 30.4%, up to 70000CHF (approx. USD 58000): 35.4%, pants were asked to rate their feelings when thinking about their
up to 85000CHF (approx. USD 70000): 12.7% and above 85000CHF: intention to exercise the following day. They rated four emotional
2.5%. There were no differences between the occupational status adjectives (that were selected from 24 adjectives of the Profile of
C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234 229

Mood States-Adolescents; Lane & Terry, 1998) on a 6 point scale Model specification
ranging from 1 (not at all) to 6 (extremely). Initially, three experts In accordance with TPB, four latent variables were specified:
rated the 24 adjectives with respect to their usefulness to assess attitude (6 items), subjective norms (3 items) and behavioural
potential emotions associated with the intention to exercise. The control (4 items). The intention variable was calculated as the mean
eight adjectives with the highest ratings (active, alert, energetic, of the two items (mean correlation r ¼ 0.55; p < 0.01, SD ¼ 0.16)
lively, downhearted, unhappy, uncertain, worried) were then pre- relating to intention at each point of measurement. The average
sented to 30 undergraduate psychology students. They were asked correlation of the 16 intention measures was r ¼ 0.45 (p < 0.01,
to imagine that they intended to exercise regularly. The eight SD ¼ 0.13). The intention measures were then pooled across two
adjectives were then rated in terms of their usefulness in capturing week periods resulting in four indicators for measurement of the
the emotional responses associated with the intention to exercise latent variable ‘intention’ (intention indicators I1–I4). Cronbach’s
and the four items with the highest ranking were selected (alert, alpha for these four indicators was 0.91. The average frequency and
energetic, unhappy, and downhearted). duration of physical activity were included as dependent variables.
Taking into account the recommendations of Dunlap and
Physical activity: frequency and duration Kemery (1987) for the control of multicollinearity, the latent vari-
Following every exercise session, participants completed an able ‘emotion’ was specified as follows for the extended model.
Internet-based electronic exercise diary. Participants were asked Firstly, negative emotion items were recoded. At each time point,
about the type of exercise, location and duration. Participants were the four emotion indicators correlated highly (mean correlation
required to answer all the questions, as blank answers were not r ¼ 0.64; range ¼ 0.52–0.81, SD ¼ 0.09). In a first step the four
permitted in any of the questions. Exercise frequency is considered emotion indicators were pooled for each assessment point result-
to be an indicator of maintenance of physical activity. ing in 16 time specific measures. The average correlation of these
was r ¼ 0.62 (p < 0.01, SD ¼ 0.06). In a second step, analogous to the
Procedure calculation of the latent intention variable, emotion measures were
pooled across two week periods in order to obtain four indicators of
The study was conducted over the course of nine weeks. emotion (emotion indicators E1–E4). These four indicators yielded
Following their response to a recruitment email, participants a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.96.
received a postal questionnaire pack (including prepaid return
envelope). The participants were requested to mark the question- Method of estimation and fit indices
naires with a code word to enable data integration with intention, SEM was performed using the method of maximum likelihood
emotion and exercise behaviour measurement at a later point. The (FML) estimation (see, e.g. Bollen, 1989). FML is the most widely used
questionnaires included requests for demographic and socioeco- fitting function for general structural equation models. Usually FML
nomic information as well as the attitude, subjective norm and PBC is scale invariant and scale free (Swaminathan & Algina, 1978).
scales described above. One week later, participants were emailed Lisrel provides a range of goodness-of-fit indices to estimate the
a username and password to enable them to access their personal adequacy of the proposed model. The most common test for the
exercise diaries online. In accordance with the recommendations of assessment of model fit is the chi square goodness-of-fit test (c2).
the U.S. Surgeon General, the American College of Sports Medicine Since the c2 statistic is highly sensitive to sample size and often
and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, participants inflates Type 1 error (Bollen, 1989), it is now recommended to
were asked to exercise twice a week for 30–60 min (USDHHS, 1996). employ a combination of fit indices in conjunction with the c2
At the beginning of the first of eight weeks designated for exercising, statistic to determine the adequacy of model fit (Bentler, 1990; Hu &
participants each nominated two days on which they were able to Bentler, 1999; Jöreskog, 1993; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993; Schu-
exercise for 30 min. On the evening prior to their first nominated macker & Lomax, 1996): the standardized root-mean-square
day, they were sent a reminder email with a request to log on to their residual (SRMR), the comparative fit index (CFI) and the root-mean
personal exercise diary in order to complete intention and emotion square error of approximation (RMSEA) are used as indicators of
ratings with respect to their first planned exercise session. The model misspecification (Hu & Bentler, 1998). Hu and Bentler (1998)
following day, participants were required to log on to fill out the suggest that cut-off values of SRMR  0.08, RMSEA  0.08 and
electronic post-exercise questionnaire, to indicate whether they CFI  0.95 indicate a relatively good fit between the hypothesised
had completed their planned training and, if yes, how long their model and the observed data. Another minimum sample discrep-
exercise session had lasted. This process was repeated for the ancy function, the c2/df ratio, is considered to be a useful criterion.
remaining 15 exercise sessions (2 sessions per week over 8 weeks). Bollen and Long (1993) suggest a c2/df ratio not larger than 2–5
Participants were requested to continue completing the electronic times the degrees of freedom.
intention and emotion scales for subsequent exercise sessions even
if they had missed one or more sessions. Participants were offered Results
individual feedback as an incentive for taking part in this study.
Training duration and frequency
Data reduction and analysis
In total, participants completed 2670 exercise sessions
In order to investigate the effects of attitudes, subjective norms, PBC (M ¼ 11.27, SD ¼ 4.23) which equals 70% of the foreseen sessions. The
and intention on training duration and training frequency (model 1, see average training duration was 32.11 min (SD ¼ 7.83), thus indicating
Fig. 1) structural equation modelling (SEM) using LISREL 8.8 was per- that, on average, participants achieved the minimum prescribed
formed (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1999). Using the same statistical proce- exercise duration of 30–60 min per session. Table 1 shows the
dure, emotion was then included in the model 2 (Fig. 2) to examine the correlations, means and the standard deviation of all variables.
effects of the predictor variables on duration and frequency of training
and also the mediating effect of emotion on duration and frequency of Model 1: the prediction of training frequency and duration:
training. Prior to analysis, all variables were normalised using the traditional TPB model
PRELIS 2.50 algorithm (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1999), as the assumption of
multivariate normality was violated (skewness and kurtosis in the Table 2 shows the factor loadings and internal consistency
univariate analysis contradicted normality). coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) for all latent variable indicators.
230 C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234

Attitude

.18* (.19*)
.16

Frequency
Subjective .20** (.19*) Intention .70** (.65**)
.22** (duration)
Norm 2
R = .17 (.17)
R2= .49 (.42)

.18*
.22** (.22**) .02 (.03)

Perceived
Behavioural
Control

Note. *p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.

Fig. 1. Model 1 with standardized regression coefficients.

For frequency of training, the traditional hypothesised predic- and explain 17% of the variance in intention. There was no direct
tion model (Fig. 1) yielded the following fit indices: c2(128, effect of PBC on frequency of training (B ¼ 0.02, t(235) ¼ 0.42, n.s.).
N ¼ 237) ¼ 196.60, p ¼ 0.000, RMSEA ¼ 0.048 (90% CI ¼ 0.034; Therefore the link between PBC and frequency of training was
0.060), p(RMSEA < 0.05) ¼ 0.60, CFI ¼ 0.97, NNFI ¼ 0.96, suppressed in model 2.
SRMR ¼ 0.063 and c2/df ¼ 1.53. RMSEA, CFI and NNFI are above the The initial traditional prediction model (Fig. 1) showed the
acceptable cut-off (0.95) and the c2/df ratio is very small. Hence the following fit indices for the duration of training: c2(128,
model can be accepted. Intention has a substantial impact on N ¼ 237) ¼ 186.57, p ¼ 0.000, RMSEA ¼ 0.044 (90% CI ¼ 0.029;
frequency of training (B ¼ 0.70, t(235) ¼ 12.21, p < 0.001) and can 0.057), p(RMSEA < 0.05) ¼ 0.76, CFI ¼ 0.97, NNFI ¼ 0.97,
explain 49% of variance in exercise frequency: stronger intention SRMR ¼ 0.063 and c2/df ¼ 1.45. CFI and NNFI are clearly above the
predicts higher frequency of the training session indicating better acceptable cut-off (0.95). Furthermore the very small size of the c2/
maintenance. Attitudes (B ¼ 0.18, t(235) ¼ 2.36, p < 0.05), subjec- df ratio indicates also that the model has a good fit. RMSEA, and
tive norms (B ¼ 0.20, t(235) ¼ 2.80, p < 0.01) and PBC (B ¼ 0.22, SRMR are small which suggested that discrepancies between the
t(235) ¼ 2.94, p < 0.01) all have a significant impact on intention present model and the true model were relatively small. Globally,

Emotion
R2= .27 (.27)
Attitude

.16 .19*(.19*)
.52** (.52**) .45** (.53**)

Frequency
Subjective Intention
.22** .48** (.37**) (duration)
Norm .20** (.19*)
R2= .17 (.17) R2= .66 (.62)

.18*
.21** (22**)
Perceived
Behavioural
Control

Note. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.

Fig. 2. Model 2 with standardized regression coefficients.


C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234 231

Table 1
Correlations, means and standard deviations of all variables

A1 A2 A3 A4 A5 A6 SN1 SN2 SN3 PBC1 PBC2 PBC3 PBC4 I1 I2 I3 I4 E1 E2 E3 E4 D F


A1 –
A2 0.21** –
A3 0.38** 0.26** –
A4 0.38** 0.28** 0.28** –
A5 0.29** 0.30** 0.42** 0.18** –
A6 0.34** 0.40** 0.43** 0.34** 0.29** –
SN1 0.19** 0.01 0.10 0.12 0.01 0.13* –
SN2 0.11 0.06 0.04 0.10 0.00 0.05 0.75** –
SN3 0.15* 0.01 0.10 0.21** 0.09 0.13 0.57** 0.58** –
PBC1 0.26** 0.03 0.15* 0.12 0.06 0.05 0.14* 0.14* 0.17** –
PBC2 0.26** 0.04 0.13* 0.09 0.06 0.05 0.14* 0.11 0.14* 0.58** –
PBC3 0.22** 0.04 0.16* 0.14* 0.05 0.06 0.09 0.05 0.08 0.59** 0.57** –
PBC4 0.21** 0.01 0.03 0.14* 0.03 0.10 0.12 0.09 0.06 0.50** 0.55** 0.53** –
I1 0.20** 0.04 0.18** 0.03 0.01 0.16* 0.19** 0.16* 0.16* 0.18** 0.20** 0.26** 0.09 –
I2 0.27** 0.16* 0.24** 0.03 0.05 0.17** 0.22** 0.18** 0.16* 0.20** 0.23** 0.26** 0.09 0.80** –
I3 0.10 0.08 0.16* 0.03 0.04 0.10 0.12 0.07 0.04 0.06 0.13* 0.17* 0.02 0.69** 0.73** –
I4 0.23** 0.08 0.19** 0.03 0.00 0.19** 0.30** 0.28** 0.23** 0.23** 0.27** 0.33** 0.09 0.74** 0.73** 0.68** –
E1 0.19** 0.03 0.14* 0.05 0.09 0.02 0.13* 0.08 0.08 0.14* 0.16* 0.13 0.12 0.39** 0.41** 0.32** 0.32** –
E2 0.18** 0.05 0.11 0.07 0.09 0.02 0.15* 0.11 0.08 0.05 0.10 0.07 0.03 0.35** 0.37** 0.28** 0.30** 0.89** –
E3 0.24** 0.10 0.20** 0.10 0.06 0.05 0.12 0.07 0.08 0.17* 0.19** 0.20** 0.12 0.52** 0.52** 0.42** 0.45** 0.87** 0.86** –
E4 0.27** 0.07 0.16* 0.11 0.08 0.03 0.19** 0.13* 0.15* 0.13* 0.16* 0.14* 0.06 0.47** 0.50** 0.39** 0.43** 0.86** 0.88** 0.88** –
D 0.19** 0.08 0.12 0.06 0.09 0.06 0.19** 0.13* 0.12 0.10 0.14* 0.16* 0.07 0.58** 0.59** 0.52** 0.50** 0.67** 0.65** 0.71** 0.68** –
F 0.20** 0.08 0.12 0.00 0.09 0.07 0.26** 0.15* 0.12 0.15* 0.20** 0.22** 0.11 0.61** 0.63** 0.61** 0.59** 0.64** 0.64** 0.68** 0.66** 0.78** –
M 4.59 4.43 4.43 4.41 4.37 4.47 4.58 4.70 4.19 4.37 4.31 4.31 4.32 3.87 4.05 2.86 2.79 3.44 3.25 3.26 3.36 32.11 11.27
SD 1.37 1.31 1.31 1.32 1.39 1.36 1.17 1.16 1.22 1.37 1.41 1.44 1.42 0.76 0.91 0.81 0.70 1.03 1.10 1.02 0.97 7.83 4.23

Note. N ¼ 237. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. A1–A6 ¼ attitude 1 to attitude 6, SN1–SN3 ¼ subjective norm 1 to subjective norm 3, PBC1–PBC4 ¼ perceived behavioural control 1 to
perceived behavioural control 4, I1–I4 ¼ Intention indicators 1–4, E1–E4 ¼ Emotion indicators 1–4. D ¼ Average duration of exercise. F ¼ Average frequency of exercise.

these fit indices suggest that the model is acceptable. Attitudes Model 2: are the predictor effects on duration and frequency of
(B ¼ 0.19, t(235) ¼ 2.41, p < 0.05), subjective norms (B ¼ 0.19, training mediated by emotions?
t(235) ¼ 2.73, p < 0.01) and PBC (B ¼ 0.22, t(235) ¼ 2.55, p < 0.01)
all have a significant impact on intention and explain 17% of the According to Baron and Kenny (1986) four conditions must be
variance in intention. More positive attitudes and subjective norms met when a variable functions as a mediator variable: (1) the link
and more PBC predict stronger intentions. Intention has a strongly between the independent variable (intention) and the dependent
significant impact on exercise duration (B ¼ 0.65, t(235) ¼ 11.23, variable (frequency and duration of training) must be significant,
p < 0.001) and explains 42% of the variance of exercise duration – (2) the link between the independent variable and the mediator
participants with stronger intentions exercise longer. PBC has no variable (emotion) must be significant, (3) the link between the
significant direct impact on exercise duration (B ¼ 0.03, t mediator variable and the dependent variable must be significant,
(235) ¼ 0.56, n.s.). Therefore the link between PBC and exercise and (4) testing if the mediator variable reduces or eliminates the
duration was excluded in model 2. link between the independent variable and the dependent variable.

Table 2
Factor loadings for constructs

Frequency of training Duration of training

Factor loadings within Factor loadings within Factor loadings within Factor loadings within
constructs for model 1 constructs for model 2 constructs for model 1 constructs for model 2
A1 0.59 0.59 0.59 0.60
A2 0.52 0.52 0.52 0.51
A3 0.65 0.65 0.65 0.65
A4 0.52 0.52 0.52 0.52
A5 0.50 0.49 0.49 0.50
A6 0.65 0.65 0.65 0.65
SN1 0.87 0.87 0.87 0.87
SN2 0.86 0.86 0.86 0.86
SN3 0.66 0.66 0.67 0.67
PBC1 0.76 0.76 0.76 0.76
PBC2 0.77 0.77 0.77 0.77
PBC3 0.77 0.77 0.77 0.77
PBC4 0.68 0.68 0.68 0.68
I1 0.88 0.88 0.88 0.88
I2 0.90 0.90 0.90 0.90
I3 0.80 0.80 0.80 0.80
I4 0.83 0.83 0.83 0.83
E1 – 0.93 – 0.93
E2 – 0.94 – 0.94
E3 – 0.93 – 0.93
E4 – 0.94 – 0.94

Note. A1–A6 ¼ attitude 1 to attitude 6, SN1–SN3 ¼ subjective norm 1 to subjective norm 3, PBC1–PBC4 ¼ perceived behavioural control 1 to perceived behavioural control 4,
I1–I4 ¼ Intention indicators 1–4, E1–E4 ¼ Emotion indicators 1–4.
232 C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234

While condition 1 was tested in model 1, conditions 2, 3, and 4 are lead to an increase of the explained variance of the TPB model.
investigated in model 2. Moreover, in contrast with Sheeran, Conner, and Norman (2001)
The hypothesised mediation model 2 (Fig. 2) shows the but in line with Armitage (2005), the present study supports the
following fit indices for the training frequency: c2(201, idea that the TPB can account for the maintenance of health
N ¼ 237) ¼ 343.85, p ¼ 0.00, RMSEA ¼ 0.055 (90% CI ¼ 0.044; behaviour. Consistent with prior retrospective studies, the present
0.065), p(RMSEA < 0.05) ¼ 0.20, CFI ¼ 0.97, NNFI ¼ 0.97, research demonstrated that intention was the dominant predictor
SRMR ¼ 0.061 and c2/df ¼ 1.71. The fit indices indicate that the of exercise maintenance (measured as frequency of exercise
mediation model can be accepted. Intention (B ¼ 0.48, behaviour). This finding differs from the results of Armitage (2005),
t(235) ¼ 9.26, p < 0.001) and emotions (B ¼ 0.45, t(235) ¼ 9.14, which indicated that PBC was the dominant predictor of exercise
p < 0.001) have significant impact on the frequency of training and maintenance. A possible explanation for this is that Armitage’s
can explain 66% of variance in exercise frequency – stronger sample reported a high initial level of PBC (M ¼ 6.02, SD ¼ 1.05) and
intention and more positive emotions lead to higher frequency of relatively low intention scores (M ¼ 2.49, SD ¼ 0.80). In contrast,
the training sessions. In addition, intention has a substantial impact the sample in the present study displayed substantially lower PBC
on (positive) emotions (B ¼ 0.52, t(235) ¼ 8.09, p < 0.001), stronger scores (M ¼ 4.32, SD ¼ 1.14) and higher intention (M ¼ 3.04,
intention predicts a higher level of positive emotions. SD ¼ 0.96). Another reason for this discrepancy may lie in the fact
With respect to duration of training the hypothesised mediation that attitudes, subjective norms and PBC were measured only at
model 2 (Fig. 2) generated the following fit indices: c2(201, baseline in the present study to minimise the burden on partici-
N ¼ 237) ¼ 345.90, p ¼ 0.00, RMSEA ¼ 0.055 (90% CI ¼ 0.045; pants. This lack of temporal correspondence may provide an invalid
0.065), p(RMSEA < 0.05) ¼ 0.19, CFI ¼ 0.97, NNFI ¼ 0.97, test of the mediation effect of emotions. Furthermore, the attitude
SRMR ¼ 0.064 and c2/df ¼ 1.72. According to these fit indices the items did not correspond with the intention items (twice a week of
mediation model can be accepted. Intention (B ¼ 0.37, t(235) ¼ 7.06, exercise was specified only in the attitude items) which resulted in
p < 0.001) and emotions (B ¼ 0.53, t(235) ¼ 10.26, p < 0.001) have less variance being explained in intentions as compared to behav-
a substantial impact on the duration of training and can explain 62% iour. Concurrently, the close temporal proximity of intention and
of variance in exercise duration – stronger intention and more emotion ratings and the observed exercise behaviour might have
positive emotions predict longer duration of training sessions. inflated the correspondence between intention, emotion and
Furthermore intention has a significant impact on (positive) behaviour. This limitation of the current study should be addressed
emotions (B ¼ 0.52, t(235) ¼ 8.16, p < 0.001) – stronger intention in future research. However, it seems unlikely that the significant
predicts a higher level of positive emotions. predictive validity of emotions (adding around 17% to the predic-
tion of behaviour) would become non-significant if PBC and
Further exploratory analysis behaviour corresponded in the element of time.
The hypothesis that emotions associated with the intention to
In two further analyses (for duration and frequency) we tested exercise mediate the intention–behaviour relationship was sup-
the question whether an extension of model 2 in which regression ported both with respect to exercise frequency and duration. Baron
paths of attitudes, subjective norms, and PBC on emotions are free, and Kenny’s conditions for a mediation model are, thus, fulfilled:
lead to a better model fit and an increase of the explained variance (1) intention predicts frequency and duration of the training
of emotions. The results show that none of the regression paths are sessions, (2) intention and emotions are highly related, (3) emotion
significant (Bs < 0.05, ts(235) < 0.74, n.s.). has a substantial impact on both exercise duration and frequency,
and (4) the mediator variable ‘emotion’ reduces the link between
Discussion intention and the frequency and duration of training significantly
(partial mediation). Inclusion of the emotion variable resulted in
The aim of this research was to examine predictors of duration a 17% increase in variance explained in exercise frequency and
and maintenance of physical activity in the context of the TPB. The a 20% increase in predicting exercise duration. The observed
central assumption was that inclusion of an emotion variable as increase in variance accounted for in behaviour is accompanied by
a mediator between intention and behaviour may serve to diminish a reduction in strength of the direct link between intention and
the ‘intention–behaviour gap’ which has been commonly observed behaviour (22% decrease for exercise frequency and 28% for exer-
in studies based on TPB (Hagger et al., 2002). cise duration). This partial mediation effect of emotion emphasises
The initial model tested in this research was a traditional TPB on the one hand the role of intention as a main predictor of
based model. The results demonstrated that the intention compo- behaviour and clarifies on the other hand that in a ‘‘theory deep-
nent of the TPB could account for the duration and maintenance of ening’’ procedure (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001, p. 80) emotion as
exercise behaviour. Furthermore findings indicate that satisfactory a mediator variable can improve the predictive validity of the
prediction of intention to exercise can be achieved from attitudes, traditional TPB model.
subjective norms and perceived behavioural control (PBC). Whilst These findings fully replicate those of Mohiyeddini and Bauer
the predictor variables account only for 17% of the variance in (2007), but the observed effects are considerably stronger in the
intention to exercise, a low to moderate amount of explained current study. Thus, the longitudinal, prospective design incorpo-
variance in comparison to the studies cited in Hagger et al.’s (2002) rating repeated measurement of both intentions and emotions
meta-analysis, all three predictors contributed significantly to the associated with intentions has resulted in explanation of a large
prediction of intention to exercise as expected. Furthermore, 49% of proportion of the variance in the intention–behaviour relationship.
the variance in training frequency and 42% in training duration The increase in explained variance in the mediator model raises
were accounted for by the measure of intention to exercise. These the question of the theoretical mechanisms by which emotion is
represent relatively high proportions of variance in accounting for influenced by intention on the one hand and affects behaviour on
the relationship between intention and behaviour compared to the other. Differences may arise, for example, when intentions are
Sutton (1998). One reason for this may be that the design of the motivated by approach (e.g. positive aspect of physical activity) or
study required participants to make specific plans by rating their avoidance (e.g. the negative consequence of not being active)
intention to exercise and emotions associated with the intention tendencies respectively (Carver et al., 2000). In the present study
prior to each training episode. This supports Sniehotta, Scholz, and emotion was measured purely as positive or negative affect asso-
Schwarzer’s (2005) finding that variables such as planning may ciated with the intention to exercise. A more differentiated
C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234 233

assessment of emotional appraisal of reported intentions may yield thoroughly examine the role of emotions in bridging the intention–
a differential account of the motivational factors underlying such behaviour gap by using objective measures of exercise behaviour in
reported emotions. This may be further influenced by self-regula- future research.
tion and emotion regulation variables (Mohiyeddini, 2005). Thus it In sum, the results emphasise the relevance of TPB in the context
might be expected that hedonistic emotion regulation (the ability of exercise behaviour. In addition, our study points to an important
to maintain and intensify positive emotions and to terminate theoretical possibility of how to increase the predictive power of
negative emotions) strengthens positive emotions and weakens the TPB. As Armitage (2005) pointed out, research into mainte-
negative emotions associated with intention. On the other hand nance of exercise behaviour as a process is still in its infancy. One
distress-augmenting emotion regulation (maintenance and inten- potential application of our results for research into maintenance
sification of negative emotion) should have the opposite effect. would be to describe not only the content of an intervention and its
With respect to the link between emotion and behaviour, it is duration but also the emotional processes and their changes
not clear how or when emotions affect a behavioural sequence. occurring during the maintenance of health behaviour in general,
Initiation, maintenance, completion or re-initiation of behaviour and exercise behaviour specifically. We suggest considering both
(Lewis & Haviland, 1993) may be differentially implicated. Similarly, emotional processes as well as emotion regulation processes
Zhu and Thagard (2002) have hypothesised that emotions may (Gross, 2007) in future research and in the design and imple-
impact action generation, action execution and/or action control. mentation of intervention programs. Specifically an intervention
However, further research needs to clarify the behavioural and focusing on hedonistic emotion regulation (Mohiyeddini, 2005;
emotional processes underlying the predictive power of positive Wirtz et al., 2006) may facilitate the implementation of desired
emotions. Furthermore, future research should consider that health behaviours and contribute to their maintenance over time.
emotions influence availability of mental capacity producing
‘‘ironic effects’’ (Wegner, 1994). As Wegner pointed out under
References
conditions of stress (e.g. produced by positive or negative
emotions) people tend to generate the opposite of what they intend Abraham, C., Sheeran, P., & Johnston, M. (1998). From health beliefs to self-regu-
to generate so that ‘‘desired happiness becomes sadness, desired lation: theoretical advances in the psychology of action control. Psychology and
relaxation becomes anxiety, desired interest becomes boredom, Health, 13, 569–591.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organisational Behavior and Human
desired love becomes hate, and so on’’ (p. 39; see also Wegner, Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Erber, & Zanakos, 1993). In addition, possible moderator effects, i.e. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social
an interaction effect of intention and emotion on exercise behav- behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2000). Social cognition models and health behavior:
iour should be investigated.
a structured review. Psychology and Health, 15, 173–189.
Given that we used a non-random sampling method and Armitage, J. (2005). Can the theory of planned behavior predict the maintenance of
participation required IT availability, the external validity of the physical activity? Health Psychology, 3, 235–245.
present study is under question. However, since both the SES Armstrong, N., & McManus, A. (1994). Children’s fitness and physical activity:
a challenge for physical education. British Journal of Physical Education, 25,
variables and previous experience with training did not show any 20–26.
effects, the results of the study may apply to the general population Bagozzi, P. B., Moore, D. J., & Leone, L. (2004). Self-control and the self-regulation of
of Switzerland. dieting decisions: the role of prefactual attitudes, subjective norms, and resis-
tance to temptation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26, 199–213.
Several limitations of this study should be addressed in future Bagozzi, R. P. (1981). Attitudes, intentions and behavior: a test of some key
research. First, exclusively self-report measures were used. Self- hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 607–627.
reports require the ability to accurately reflect on and report one’s Bagozzi, R. P. (1992). The self-regulation of attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 55, 178–204.
behaviour. They also rely on recall, are susceptible to memory and Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., & Pieters, R. (1998). Goal-directed emotions.
may reflect a unique pattern of reporting biases (e.g. primacy and Cognition and Emotion, 12, 1–16.
recency effects; Murdock, 1962) and social desirability (e.g. report Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical consider-
of incorrect duration or frequency of training). Also, the Likert type ations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
items employed in this study for the purpose of measuring the Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological
valence and intensity of emotions associated with the intention to Bulletin, 107, 238–246.
Blanchard, C., Rodgers, W., Courneya, K., Daub, B., & Knapik, G. (2002). Does barrier
exercise may be regarded as a cognitively mediated emotion
efficacy mediate the gender/exercise adherence relationship during phase II
measurement (see Robinson & Clore, 2002). Thus ratings of such cardiac rehabilitation? Rehabilitation Psychology, 47, 106–120.
items represent cognitive appraisal of the underlying emotion. A Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
more direct assessment of emotion might be obtained by using, for Bollen, K., & Long, J. (Eds.). (1993). Testing structural equation models. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
example physiological measures of arousal during contemplation of Brown, A., & Siahpush, M. (2006). Socioeconomic predictors of a sedentary lifestyle:
the intention. However, whilst physiological measures may be results from the 2001 National Health Survey. Journal of Physical Activity and
more informative in terms of the intensity of arousal, they are less Health, 3, 90–101.
Carver, C., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality:
clear with respect to the valence of the associated emotions. emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
Furthermore, in model 2 positive and negative emotions were 741–751.
represented as a single latent variable to avoid multicollinearity Chatzisarantis, N., & Hagger, M. (2005). Effects of a brief intervention based on the
theory of planned behaviour on leisure time physical activity participation.
between a latent variable ‘‘positive emotions’’ and a latent variable Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 470–487.
‘‘negative emotions’’. Although this ensures parsimony, the main Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate
issue with this strategy is, as indicated by previous research, that behavioral predispositions to approach and avoid the stimulus. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.
positive emotions do not necessarily reflect a lack of negative Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior:
emotions (Watson & Clark, 1984). However, further research is a review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
needed to explore the differential effects of positive and negative 28, 1429–1464.
Coumeya, K. S., & McAuley, E. (1994). Are there different determinants of the
emotions as mediators of the intention–behaviour link. A further
frequency, intensity, and duration of physical activity? Behavioral Medicine, 20,
limitation is a lack of consideration of the intensity of completed 84–90.
exercise sessions. Including measures of training intensity would Dillon, W. R., & Kumar, A. (1985). Attitude organization and the attitude–behavior
facilitate a more detailed interpretation of frequency and duration relation: a critique of Bagozzi and Burnkrant’s reanalysis of Fishbein and Ajzen.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 33–46.
of exercise behaviour. Given the potential pitfalls of self-reporting Dishman, R. K., & Buckworth, J. (2001). Exercise psychology. Champaign, IL: Human
and the crucial role of exercise behaviour, it is essential that we Kinetics.
234 C. Mohiyeddini et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10 (2009) 226–234

Dunlap, W. P., & Kemery, E. R. (1987). Failure to detect moderating effects: is Pate, R. R., Dowda, M., O’Neill, J. R., & Ward, D. S. (2007). Change in physical activity
multicollinearity the problem? Psychology Bulletin, 102, 418–420. participation among adolescent girls from 8th to 12th grade. Journal of Physical
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Belmont, CA: Activity, & Health, 4, 3–16.
Wadsworth. Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and anticipated emotions in
Estabrooks, P., & Carron, A. V. (1998). The conceptualization and effect of control goal-directed behavior: broadening and deepening the theory of planned
beliefs on exercise attendance in the elderly. Journal of Aging and Health, 10, behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98.
441–457. Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessi-
Gross, J. J. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. bility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., Biddle, S., & Orbell, S. (2001). Antecedents of Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal processes in
children’s physical activity intentions and behavior: predictive validity and emotion: Theory, method, research. New York: Oxford University Press.
longitudinal effects. Psychology and Health, 16, 391–407. Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (1996). A beginner’s guide to structural equation
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2002). A meta-analytic modeling. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–behavior-relations: a conceptual and empirical
activity: predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal review. In M. Hewstone, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), European review of social
of Sport, & Exercise Psychology, 24, 3–32. psychology, Vol. 12 (pp. 1–36). New York: Wiley.
Henderson, K. A., Bialeschki, M. D., Shaw, S. M., & Freysinger, V. J. (1999). Both gaps Sheeran, P., Conner, M., & Norman, P. (2001). Can the theory of planned behavior
and gains: Feminist perspectives on women’s leisure. State College, PA: Venture explain patterns of health behavior change? Health Psychology, 20, 12–19.
Publishing. Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1998). Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis
Hu, L., & Bentler, M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity and examination of six moderator variables. British Journal of Social Psychology,
to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 37, 231–250.
424–453. Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Bridging the intention–behavior
Hu, L., & Bentler, M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure gap: planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance
analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation of physical exercise. Psychology, & Health, 20, 143–160.
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. Solomon, R. C. (2000). The philosophy of emotions. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-
Jöreskog, K. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed.). (pp. 17–29) New York: Guilford.
(Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 307–314). Newbury Park: Sage Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and explaining intentions and behavior: how well are
Publications. we doing? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1317–1338.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS Swaminathan, H., & Algina, J. (1978). Scale freeness in factor analysis. Psychometrika,
command language. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software. 43, 581–583.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1999). LISREL 8.51. Chicago: Scientific Software. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity and health:
Lane, A. M., & Terry, P. C. (1998). Development of normative data for profile of mood A report to the surgeon general. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and
states-C among adult and young athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 16, 94–95. Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center
Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.
a functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3–28. Van der Pligt, J., Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., de Vries, N. K., & Richard, R. (1998).
Lewis, M., & Haviland, J. M. (1993). Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press. Affect, attitudes and decisions: let’s be more specific. In W. Stroebe, &
Martinez-Gonzalez, M. A., Varo, J. J., Santos, J. L., De Irala, J., Gibney, M., Kearney, J., et al. M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (pp. 33–66). Chi-
(2001). Prevalence of physical activity during leisure time in the European Union. chester: Wiley.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 33, 1142–1146. Wallston, K., & Armstrong, C. (2002). Theoretically-based strategies for health
Marttila, J., & Nupponen, R. (2000). Health enhancing physical activity as perceived behavior change. In M. P. O’Donnell (Ed.), Health promotion in the workplace (3rd
in interviews based on the theory of planned behaviour. Psychology and Health, ed.). (pp. 182–201) Albany, NY: Delmar.
15, 593–608. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience
Milne, S., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining motivational and volitional aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465–490.
interventions to promote exercise participation: protection motivation theory Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101,
and implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 163–184. 34–52.
Mohiyeddini, C. (2005). Persönlichkeit, Emotionsregulation und Psychopathologie. Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control
[Personality, emotion regulation and psychopathology]. Habilitationsschrift. of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Universität Tübingen. 65, 1093–1104.
Mohiyeddini, C., & Bauer, S. (2007). Intentions-Verhaltens-Lücke bei sportlichen Wirtz, P. H., Von Känel, R., Mohiyeddini, C., Emini, L., Ruedisueli, K., Groessbauer, S.,
Aktivitäten: Die Bedeutung von Emotionen. [Intention–behavior gap in physical et al. (2006). Low social support and poor emotional regulation are associated
exercise: the role of emotions]. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 14, 3–13. with increased stress hormone reactivity to mental stress in systemic hyper-
Murdock, B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental tension. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 91, 3857–3865.
Psychology, 64, 482–488. Zhu, J., & Thagard, P. (2002). Emotion and action. Philosophical Psychology, 15, 19–36.

You might also like