You are on page 1of 15

J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

DOI 10.1007/s10826-016-0459-9

ORIGINAL PAPER

Failing to Meet the Good Parent Ideal: Self-Stigma in Parents


of Children with Mental Health Disorders
Kim Eaton1 • Jeneva L. Ohan1,3 • Werner G. K. Stritzke1 • Patrick W. Corrigan2,3

Published online: 4 June 2016


 Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract Self-stigma detracts from the wellbeing, self- Keywords Self-stigma  Stigma internalisation  Parent 
esteem, and social connectedness of adults with mental Child  Mental illness  Blame  Shame
health disorders. Although emerging research has indicated
that self-stigma may have similar consequences for parents
of children with mental health disorders, currently we lack Introduction
a comprehensive description of how parents experience
self-stigma. To address this, we investigated parents’ lived Stigma has been defined as an ‘‘attribute that is deeply
experiences of self-stigma using a descriptive qualitative discrediting,’’ one that reduces the bearer of the attribute
approach. Directed by interview questions informed by a ‘‘from a whole and usual person to a tainted, discounted
parent-based participatory action research group (n = 4), one’’ (Goffman 1963, p. 3). Across many cultures, people
we conducted individual semi-structured interviews with with mental illness are amongst the most highly stigma-
12 parents of children (aged 5–13) diagnosed with emo- tised members of society (Bos et al. 2013). This stigma
tional and/or behavioural disorders. Data obtained from extends even to young children with mental health disor-
interviews with 11 mothers was coded and thematically ders, who are similarly stereotyped as dangerous and
analysed. Five themes were found: (1) the ‘good parent’ incompetent, and are often treated in negative and punitive
ideal, (2) awareness of external stigma, (3) outcomes of ways as a result (Pescosolido et al. 2007; Walker et al.
external stigma (social avoidance and self-doubt), (4) self- 2008). Their parents, by virtue of their close relationship
stigma (believing self-doubt and external stigma), and (5) with and responsibility for their child, experience an
refuting self-stigma. Our findings show that parents of extension of this stigma (Corrigan and Miller 2004;
children with mental health disorders experience self- Mukolo et al. 2010). In addition to this external stigma
stigma. However, because it leads to a diminished sense of from others, individuals may also experience ‘self-stigma,’
being a good parent, the self-stigma is of a different type to which occurs when a stigmatised individual accepts and
that which has been described for adults with mental ill- endorses stigmas as valid or true of the self (Corrigan and
ness. This has important implications for the conceptuali- Watson 2002). For adults with a range of types of mental
sation and assessment of parent self-stigma as research in illness, self-stigma is particularly harmful, resulting in
this area moves forward. diminished self-esteem, self-efficacy, and social connect-
edness (Corrigan and Rao 2012; Corrigan et al. 2006;
Perlick et al. 2001; Watson et al. 2007). Despite the ple-
& Kim Eaton
kim.eaton@research.uwa.edu.au
thora of research on the causes and consequences of self-
stigma for adults with mental illness, we have only a few
1
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, emerging reports on how parents of children with mental
M304, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia health disorders experience self-stigma.
2
Illinois Institute of Technology, Chicago, IL, USA For adults with mental illness, self-stigma is thought to
3
National Consortium on Stigma and Empowerment, Chicago, occur through a process of becoming aware of external
IL, USA stigma, agreeing with the stigma, and ultimately applying

123
3110 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

the stigma to the self (Corrigan and Watson 2002). This increased caregiving stress, and decreased caregiver well-
process of awareness, agreement, and application of stigma being and satisfaction with the caring role.
to the self is known as the ‘3A model’ of self-stigma Although informative, these studies may not have fully
(Corrigan and Watson 2002). The ‘3A model’ has more encapsulated self-stigma in parents of children with mental
recently been extended to explain that ultimately self- health disorders. In other studies in which the stigma
stigma will lead to self-devaluation and a compromised experiences of these parents have been investigated, par-
drive to attain life goals, known as the ‘why try effect’ ents have reported stigma that extended beyond the cour-
(Corrigan et al. 2009, 2016). Despite explaining self-stigma tesy and affiliate (self-shame) stigma they experienced
and its effects in adults with mental illness, the 3A model because of their association with their child (Corrigan and
of self-stigma and the ‘why try effect’ may not extend well Miller 2004; Corrigan et al. 2012; Fernández and Arcia
to the parent context because parents experience a unique 2004; Francis 2012). Rather, these parents also experienced
form of stigma that distinguishes their experience from that stigma in the form of blame, judgement, and criticism from
of adults with mental illness (Hinshaw 2004; Moses 2014; others regarding the quality of their parenting and their role
Mukolo et al. 2010). Specifically, parents of children with in the causation, exacerbation or continuation of their
mental health problems experience a form of stigma known child’s mental health problem (Corrigan and Miller 2004;
as courtesy stigma—alternatively referred to as ‘associa- Fernández and Arcia 2004; Moses 2010). These stigmas
tive stigma’ (Mehta and Farina 1988), ‘stigma by associ- are different to courtesy stigma because the focus of the
ation’ (Neuberg et al. 1994), and ‘family stigma’ (Corrigan stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination is the parent, not
and Miller 2004; Phelan et al. 1998). Courtesy stigma is the child (Francis 2012). This direct form of stigma
defined as stigma that is transferred to the parent from their towards the parent has been labelled ‘parent-blaming’ or
stigmatised child because of the familial relationship ‘bad-parent’ stigma in the literature (Francis 2012; Moses
between the two—not because of any particular attribute 2010). Several studies document that parents are keenly
possessed by the parent (as would be the case for adults aware of both parent-blaming and bad-parent stigma (e.g.,
with mental illness; Francis 2012; Goffman 1963; Moses Blum 2007; Corrigan and Miller 2004; Fernández and
2014). Given their close relationship with their child, it is Arcia 2004; Moses 2010; Singh 2004). Experiencing such
not surprising that parents of children with mental health stigma creates emotional distress for parents because it
disorders frequently report that they experience courtesy induces a sense of inadequacy and failure in the highly-
stigma (Corrigan and Miller 2004; Moses 2010). valued parenting role (Koro-Ljungberg and Bussing 2009;
Mak and Cheung (2008, 2012) proposed that courtesy Moses 2010, 2014; Singh 2004).
stigma is particularly harmful as it contributes to the The experience of parent-blaming and bad-parent
development of self-stigma for parents of offspring with stigma is thought to be particularly damaging to parents
intellectual disabilities and/or mental illness. Called ‘affil- as it often results in self-blame (Moses 2010; Todd and
iate stigma,’ this form of self-stigma is thought to be Jones 2003). Parents blame themselves for not ‘doing
characterised primarily by self-shame, and to occur enough’ to prevent, reduce or eliminate their child’s
because the disabilities or mental illnesses are socially problem (Fernández and Arcia 2004; Moses 2010)
deemed as contagious, a punishment for wrongdoing, a because of an intrinsic belief of being responsible in some
sign of weakness, or dishonourable (Mak and Cheung, way for the onset of the problem and/or for its continu-
2008). In this conceptualisation, parents and caregivers are ation (e.g., as a result of inadequate or incompetent par-
embarrassed by the disgrace and status loss of being related enting; Harden 2005). For example, Koro-Ljungberg and
to a person stigmatised in such a way and the associated Bussing (2009) interviewed 30 parents of adolescents
courtesy stigma they experience, leading the parent or with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and
caregiver to withdraw from society as a shamed individual found that participants felt a sense of responsibility for
(Mak and Cheung 2008, 2012). Others have similarly the onset of their child’s disorder. This self-blame was
found that parents experience self-stigma in the form of compounded by social stigma experiences that confirmed
shame (e.g., Hasson-Ohayon et al. 2011; Mak and Kwok to the parent that he/she was at fault. Similarly, Moses
2010; Zisman-Ilani et al. 2013). As such, this suggests that (2010) found that of the 68 parents of adolescents diag-
an important aspect of parents’ self-stigma is the internal- nosed with mental illness in her study, 60.3 % blamed
isation of courtesy stigma as self-shame, which then has themselves to some extent for their child’s problem; of
consequences for parents’ wellbeing. Consistent with this these, 33.8 % reported that they did so because of an
framework, Mak and Cheung (2008) found that in samples intrinsic sense of failing to be a good parent. Both Ferriter
of parents of offspring (ranging in age from child to adult) et al. (2003) and Milliken (2001) have argued that parents
with intellectual disabilities or mental illness, affiliate self-blame even in the absence of blame from others,
stigma (that is, parents’ self-shame) was correlated with suggesting that there may be other factors contributing to

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3111

parent self-blame (and thus parent self-stigma) than solely emotional and/or behavioural disorder (EBD; e.g., ADHD,
stigma experiences (Moses 2014). an anxiety disorder, a depressive disorder, oppositional
In sum, parents’ self-blame, especially for being a bad defiant disorder) participated in this study. Of this sample,
parent, appears to play a key role in parents’ self-stigma. 4 mothers participated in the participatory action research
However, because the focus of these studies has been on group, 2 of whom decided to also participate in individual
parents of adolescent (or adult) children, the degree to which interviews. We interviewed only one father, and despite
these experiences represent those of parents of younger (i.e., best efforts were unable to engage other fathers. Despite
pre-adolescent) children is not known. During adolescence, the similarities between the experiences of this one father
behavioural control shifts away from parents and towards in comparison to the mothers of this study, there are lim-
adolescents, and as such there may be a parallel shift in the itations in drawing conclusions based on a sample size of
focus of stigma away from the parent and towards the ado- one (Guest et al. 2006). As such, mothers’ experiences will
lescent (Mukolo et al. 2010). Accordingly, it may be that be focussed on throughout the remainder of the analysis
parents of younger children harbour a developing and per- and we encourage further exploration of fathers’ experi-
haps deeper, more pervasive form of self-blame than that ences. A total of 11 individual interviews with mothers
found in parents of adolescent (or adult) offspring. Thus, past were conducted. Diagnosis and other demographic infor-
studies on these older samples may have underestimated the mation was obtained via parent self-report, a summary of
extent of self-blame and its potential importance to under- which is provided in Table 1.
standing self-stigma in parents.
Despite this, and evidence showing that parents expe- Procedure
rience stigma and its associated negative effects, there is a
paucity of research in which parents’ self-stigma has been Ethics approval was granted by our university’s human
the primary investigative topic. Given the important role research ethics office. Given that we have taken an
that parents play in the upbringing and development of exploratory approach to understanding parents’ self-
their children (Moses 2010, 2014), it is surprising that self- stigma, involving parents in the development of the study
stigma in parents of children with mental health disorders was essential (Chevalier and Buckles 2013; Snell et al.
has not been considered more comprehensively. As 2009). To assist us with conducting a sensitive and
Mukolo et al. (2010) have proposed, this is an area that is respectful study that was suitable to address our research
grossly under-researched, and by focussing on parent self- question, we created a participatory action research group
stigma we stand to gain further insight into how self-stigma (PARG) consisting of four parents of children with diag-
effects the parent, their parenting of their child, and ulti- nosed EBDs. Participatory action research provides an
mately, the child. As Crotty (1998) stated, to understand a opportunity for individuals to take an active role in
phenomenon we need to get ‘‘back to the things them- addressing issues that affect themselves and their families
selves’’ (p. 78) by seeking the meaning of the phenomenon (Ditrano and Silverstein-Bordeaux 2006). Thus, we made a
from those who have a ‘lived experienced’ of it; that is, to commitment to work with parents whose voices are not
those who have first-hand knowledge of, and experience often heard regarding self-stigma and considered these
with, the phenomenon under study (Lindseth and Norberg parents as the experts on the topic. The PARG collabora-
2004). Thus, to understand parents’ self-stigma we need to tively guided our research by helping to develop the
ask parents who have a child with a mental health disorder questions constituting the interview protocol. Not only did
about their experiences. With this in mind, in this study we the group advise the research team as to the questions that
aimed to explore parents’ lived experience of self-stigma to would most likely access the experience of parent self-
provide a rich and comprehensive description of the phe- stigma, they also ensured that the questions were designed
nomenon. To achieve this aim, a descriptive qualitative to be respectful and sensitive. By involving parents in
approach was used to investigate the following core decisions directing the research, we aimed to provide a
question: How do parents of children (i.e., under 13 years) means by which parents could articulate and assert their
with mental health disorders experience self-stigma? interests and lead the research team (and research com-
munity more broadly) to new insights (Ditrano and Sil-
verstein-Bordeaux 2006).
Method We recruited PARG members and individual interview
participants by listing recruitment notices on parenting web
Participants pages, and placing notices in psychologists’ offices, public
facilities (e.g., libraries, shopping centres), and parent
A convenience sample of 14 Australian biological parents support agencies. The PARG was recruited within 3 weeks.
(13 mothers, 1 father) of children diagnosed with an We continued to recruit individual interview participants

123
3112 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

Table 1 Participant
PARG (n = 4 mothers) Interviewed mothers (n = 11)
demographics for PARG and
individual parent interviews Parent age (years) 39–56 (M = 46.12) 30–56 (M = 40.91)
Residence
Urban/semi-urban 4 8
Rural/semi-rural 3
Parent employment status
Employed part-time/casual 3 6
Employed full-time 1 1
Student 1
Home-maker 3
Average family income (AUD) 25,000–100,000 (M = 98,000) 25,000–175,000 (M = 82,727)
Parent education
High school (years 11–12) 2 3
Apprenticeship/trade 1 5
Undergraduate degree 1 3
Parent marital status
Single 2 3
a
Married/defacto 2 6
Separated/divorced 2
Child Sons (n = 3); Daughters (n = 1) Sons (n = 12); Daughters (n = 3)

Child age (years) 4–12 (M = 9.5) 5–13 (M = 8.73)


Child primary diagnosis
ADHD 2 8
An anxiety disorder 1 4
A depressive disorder 2
ODD 1 1
Time since diagnosis (years) 1–8 (M = 6) .41–8 (M = 2.64)
Diagnosing professional
GP 1 1
Psychologist 2 9
Paediatrician 1 5
Treatment type(s)
Medication 2 9
Psychotherapy 3 7
Home-based beh strategiesb 2 6
Duration of treatment (years) 1–8 (M = 3.35) .25–8 (M = 2.17)
ADHD attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ODD oppositional defiant disorder
a
Defacto is defined as living together but not married
b
Home-based behavioural strategies include: time-out, bed and meal-time routines, reward systems. Two
parents from the PARG self-nominated to go on to complete individual interviews

until thematic saturation was reached (a total of 4 months). We then conducted 12 semi-structured individual
Parents who expressed interest in participating in the interviews (with 12 parents), directed by the set of inter-
PARG and/or interviews (by email or telephone call to the view questions that the PARG assisted in creating. These
first author) were forwarded participant information and questions were primarily designed to investigate: (1)
consent forms and a meeting/interview date was set. Par- stigma experiences (e.g., what kinds of stigmatising
ents gave written and verbal consent for the study at the experiences have you had or felt as a parent?), and (2) the
commencement of the PARG and interviews. internalisation of stigma (e.g., at times, do you feel any of

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3113

these stigmas about yourself as a parent?). Probing ques- discrepancies, which were resolved by negotiated agree-
tions were used to explore responses (e.g., in what way? ment (Campbell et al. 2013; Garrison et al. 2006). Fol-
Are there any examples that would highlight your experi- lowing training, to minimise potential bias in the notes
ence?). Participants were free to provide any information being documented during interviews, scribes recorded the
they felt was most salient to them regarding their stigma interviewer’s questions to contextualise participant
experiences. Two interviews were conducted over two time responses. All interviews were video (n = 8) or audio
points, as both were interrupted so that the parents involved (n = 4) recorded, with the recordings providing supple-
could attend to the needs of their children. A brief hiatus mentary information during analysis of the scribed nota-
resulted (i.e., *3 h per interview). Upon recommence- tions (Halcomb and Davidson 2006). Reflexive journaling
ment, the interviewer returned to the last topic discussed occurred after each interview and during analysis to iden-
prior to the break. When tallying the first and second parts tify and ameliorate potential bias (Shaw 2010). Finally, the
of these interviews, the total time for each interview was first author reviewed each set of scribed notations to ensure
64.81 (17 plus 47.81 min) and 63.26 (29 plus 34.26 min) that the interview protocol was adhered to and that notes
respectively. Overall, interviews ranged from had been recorded for all questions as a check of protocol
29.49–63.6 min in duration (M = 42.43) and were held and note-taking fidelity.
either in person (i.e., face-to-face; n = 7; duration of
interviews M = 41.8 min), by telephone (n = 4; duration Data Analyses
of interviews M = 51.51 min) or via video-conferencing
(n = 1; duration of interview 45 min). The multi-mode We used Giorgi’s (2009) descriptive qualitative method of
interviewing technique was essential given the remote exploring lived experiences. This method allows the
geographical location of four parents which made it diffi- researcher to give ‘voice’ to the perspectives of participants
cult to conduct interviews in person (and for three parents, without abstracting their ‘story’ through analysis. This
no available internet connection to facilitate video-con- method is grounded in social interactionism and focusses
ferencing). These parents were eager to share their stories on describing the participant’s experience and how that
and to exclude them on the basis of distance would itself experience is then understood by the researcher, thus cre-
have been limiting to the diversity of our sample. Coding ating a shared meaning (Giorgi 2009). The role the
revealed that the content of these interviews were largely researcher has played in the co-construction of this ‘story’
consistent with that of the face-to-face interviews, with the is acknowledged (Crotty 1998; Giorgi 2009). Variants on
central themes being found across all interviews. the descriptive qualitative approach have been found to be
To avoid the difficulties that can be experienced during conducive for the purposes of exploring and describing
qualitative analysis due to the copious and often unwieldy parents’ lived experiences of stigma (e.g., Arcia et al. 2005;
data produced through verbatim transcription (Halcomb Saltzburg 2009).
and Davidson 2006; McNall and Foster-Fishman 2007), a The analysis methods were modelled on those of Giorgi
leaner transcript was scribed during each interview and (2009), supplemented by the theoretical sampling and
subsequently analysed (e.g., Corrigan et al. 2015). To do focussed coding strategies of Charmaz (2006). We provide
so, a scribe (a third person within the interview, who acted a descriptive account (as per Giorgi 2009) of parent stigma
independently to the interviewer and did not participate in and self-stigma experiences as opposed to a theoretical
the interview) documented salient quotes, repetitive themes account, permitting a rich description of the phenomenon,
and contextual, emotional, non-verbal and any other salient without forcing theoretical meaning on the findings. Con-
detail likely to influence the interpretation of meaning. In sistent with Giorgi, the coding team (consisting of the two
addition to the first author, two postgraduate students (all scribes and the first author) began with pre-reading of the
enrolled in a combined Masters and Ph.D. program in scribed data following each interview for familiarity and to
clinical psychology with clinical and interviewing experi- obtain a general sense of the messages conveyed. This was
ence) served as interviewers (4 interviews each) and followed by open-coding, whereby scribed data was seg-
scribes (4 interviews each). Interviewers and scribes were mented into units of meaning and coded (Giorgi 2009). The
supervised by the second author, who is a registered psy- first author alone then conducted a phase of focussed
chologist. One interviewer and one scribe were present coding; a recursive and iterative process of theme refine-
during interviews. Prior to conducting the interviews, ment, moving from individual codes to themes with an aim
scribes were trained by the first author using qualitative of identifying core themes (Charmaz 2006). Repetitions
data separate to the present study (specifically, 3 interview within and between themes were examined and themes
segments, 60 min in total), for which the thematic analysis collapsed, removed, amended, or new themes created
had already occurred. Scribed notes were reviewed against where appropriate. Exceptions and deviant cases were
transcripts of the training interviews to identify searched for to enhance reliability and validity of the

123
3114 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

analysis. Examples of such cases included the absence of stigma, and (5) refuting self-stigma. Each theme (and the
stigma or self-stigma and reactions to stigma that acted to associated subthemes) are described in turn and can be
prevent or limit self-stigma. In keeping with Charmaz found in Table 2. To contextualise the exemplar quotes, a
(2006), coding was iterative, with ongoing recruitment and brief summary of participant characteristics are provided in
interviewing, concurrent with analysis until thematic sat- Table 3.
uration was reached (i.e., theoretical sampling, but in our
case thematic sampling). Finally, the first author conducted The ‘Good Parent’ Ideal
post-structural analysis of the themes and the data consti-
tuting each, reflecting on the material as a whole and the Regardless of their child’s age or diagnosis, all mothers
extent to which it synthesised and described the partici- placed a strong emphasis on being and/or being recognised
pants’ lived experiences without forcing meaning (Giorgi as good parents. All mothers described that a ‘good parent’
2009). In accordance with Malterud (2012), thematic sat- was one who protected, nurtured, and provided for the
uration was achieved when subsequent interviews revealed child. The mothers felt this was their unwavering right and
no new themes. An audit trail including memos, field notes, responsibility, and the ultimate goal of parenting; for
interviewer and scribe reflexive notes, and a record of the example, mothers made comments that, ‘‘as a parent, all
theme evolution was maintained and referred back to. The you want to do is protect and be there for your child’’
original scribed notations and audio/video recorded data (Dee), ‘‘you have to do what’s right for your children’’
from each interview also provided a source of reference. (Kate), and ‘‘they’re my kids, I’ll do what’s right for them’’
Multiple methods of triangulation were used to confirm (Eve). Conversely, a bad parent did not achieve these aims:
the reliability and validity of the emergent themes. First,
For me, if you give them food, love and shelter, that’s
we reconvened the PARG who discussed the themes in
being a good parent. A bad parent is someone who
terms of their ability to comprehensively and truthfully
doesn’t provide those things, doesn’t talk to their
conceptualise parents’ lived experiences of self-stigma.
children, doesn’t nurture their growth, pays no
One change to the overall thematic structure was recom-
attention to them, is absent… I think over time we
mended by the PARG, this being that the identity of a
[parents of children with EBD] are good parents
parent as a good parent was a primary concern and should
because we provide those things (Eve).
receive greater emphasis. We compiled all data evidencing
the use of terms reflecting ‘good’, ‘ideal’, or ‘bad’ parent, Mothers described that the ‘good parent’ ideal had
and analysed this sub-set of data on three domains: (1) developed over many years as a result of their own
frequency of reference, (2) relative importance, and 3) upbringing and interpretation of social messages regarding
implications. We found that being a good parent held great the ‘ideal parent’ and ‘perfect child.’ The following three
importance, with words to this effect frequently used. mothers spoke of the development of the ‘good parent’
Perceived failure at being a good parent was reported as ideal: ‘‘that’s the ideal isn’t it, that’s what’s been rammed
quite distressing. Thus, we were able to confirm that the down our throats since we were little, that idea of being a
data supported the PARG’s emphasis on this theme. Sec- good person, and a good parent’’ (Jane), ‘‘like watching the
ond, to ensure richness, depth, and breadth of the emergent Brady Bunch, those sorts of concepts of a good parent’’
themes, each theme constituted multiple quotes that had (Eve), ‘‘it was expected that is what my life would be, I
been provided by more than one participant (Tobin and would have a job, then I’d get married, and be a good
Begley 2004). Finally, the final thematic construction was mother’’ (Ava). Thus, being a good parent was both
reviewed for adequacy, accuracy and comprehensiveness socially expected and personally desired. However, the
by the first, second and third authors of this paper, adding mothers revealed various ways in which their sense of
to the methodological and interpretive rigour of the study attaining the ‘good parent’ ideal became compromised
(Koch 2006). To maintain the anonymity of our partici- during the development of their self-stigma (as described in
pants, we refer to each by a pseudonym chosen by the first the themes below).
author (Kaiser 2009).
Awareness of External Stigma

Results Mothers were aware of stigma originating from many


domains in their lives. Mothers reported stigma that was
Our analyses revealed five key themes that characterised both directly experienced (enacted) and perceived (felt)
mothers’ self-stigma: (1) the ‘good parent’ ideal, (2) towards them and their child and resulted from interactions
awareness of external stigma, (3) outcomes of external with family, friends, co-workers, strangers, systems, and
stigma, (4) internalising stigma and self-doubt as self- services. Many feelings were associated with these

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3115

Table 2 Themes table: Parent


Theme Subtheme
self-stigma
The ‘good parent’ ideal NA
Awareness of external stigma Blame and bad-parent stigma
Courtesy stigma
Vicarious stigma
Outcomes of external stigma Avoidance
Doubting the self
Internalising stigma and self-doubt as self-stigma NA
Refuting self-stigma: The good enough parent NA

Table 3 Participant
Pseudonym Description
pseudonyms and brief
description Jane Mother of a son with ADHD, depression, and anxiety who has been a parent for 17 years
Ava Mother of a daughter with depression and anger related issues
Jen Mother of young son with anxiety
Eve Mother of a son with ADHD, ODD, and autism like behaviours
Sue Mother of a son with depression and ADHD, and a daughter with anxiety and ADHD
Dee Mother of a son and daughter with ADHD and anxiety
Lisa Mother of a son with ODD, ADHD and anxiety living regionally
Diane Mother of a son with ODD and ADHD
Ella Mother of a son with ADHD living in a remote area
Kate Mother of a son with ADHD
Bea Mother of a son with ADHD and depression
ADHD attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ODD oppositional defiant disorder

experiences, such as, ‘‘it’s difficult, stressful, upsetting, Blame and Bad-Parent Stigma
hurtful; you feel sad and angry’’ (Ava). External stigma
could be experienced as either an impediment to, and/or an All mothers reported that they had, at some time, felt and/
accusation of, not fulfilling the responsibilities of the good or were blamed for creating, exacerbating, or not doing
parent role, thus implying that the mother was not pro- enough to remedy their child’s EBD (i.e., parent-blaming
viding adequately for her child, or caring and loving suf- stigma). One mother was accused of causing her son’s
ficiently: for example, ‘‘it makes you feel like a failure’’ anxiety, and described times when ‘‘close family were
(Ava). Mothers reported that they inferred from these saying to me that I created the problem, that it was
insinuations that others believed they did not have the best something that I did, it must be something I did… they said
interest of the child foremost in mind; for example, ‘‘It’s ‘you’re so anxious yourself, that’s why he’s like that’’’
not really the things that are done or the things that are said, (Jen). Mothers also reported feeling stigmatised regarding
it’s the implication that you haven’t brought your child up the adequacy and competency of their parenting skills (i.e.,
right, you haven’t disciplined them, things along those bad-parent stigma); for example, ‘‘I always feel like I’m
lines, the lack of parenting and things like that’’ (Dee). getting judged as a parent, that I’m a bad parent, that I’m
Mothers stated that this was particularly hurtful because not doing the right thing, or raising my child the right way’’
being a good parent was of upmost importance to them. As (Ella). Mothers recounted times when they were called
one mother described, ‘‘my kids come first in my life’’ ‘‘shit parents’’ (Lisa), ‘‘unfit parents’’ (Eve), and were
(Ava). Although each mother recounted many and varied asked, ‘‘well, what kind of a parent are you?’’ (Kate),
stigmatising events, we found that the stigma content of which they felt suggested both parent blame and judgment
each of these events could be categorised into three broad as a ‘bad parent.’
subthemes: (1) blame and bad-parent stigma, (2) stigma For all mothers in this study, parent blame and bad-
due to the child’s behaviour and/or symptoms (courtesy parent stigma were underpinned by the notion that child-
stigma), and (3) feeling alongside the child when the child hood mental illness did not exist—rather, that the child’s
experienced stigma (vicarious stigma). We discuss each in problems were a result of failing to be a good parent.
turn. Examples included that the child was ‘‘naughty and in need

123
3116 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

of a good smack’’ (Sue), and that as a parent ‘‘you haven’t on the basis of their child’s behaviour. This form of
disciplined him [child] right, you haven’t raised him up external stigma often resulted in the mother feeling
properly’’ (Dee). As one mother explained, ‘‘my partner’s embarrassed; as one mother described, ‘‘I’m so embar-
mum doesn’t believe in mental illness or anything like that, rassed that people know he’s my son, I know I shouldn’t be
she thinks it’s a load of bullshit and I’m over-reacting’’ embarrassed, but I do, that my son is always the one that’s
(Jane). Mothers found such opinions were particularly in trouble, or has been suspended from school, or has hurt
hurtful, frustrating, and disrespectful because they were another child’’ (Ella). Given the frequent occurrence of
based on ignorance of the mother’s situation, ‘‘because such experiences, mothers reported being ‘‘a bit paranoid’’
they don’t have a child like mine, they don’t have that (Ava), ‘‘sensitive’’ (Sue), and ‘‘hypervigilant’’ (Kate) in
understanding’’ (Ella), ‘‘I am constantly surprised by other their anticipation of courtesy stigma, which they described
people’s behaviours, the arrogance that they can have, how feeling as though it was inevitable.
dare they’’ (Jane).
Such stigma intimated to mothers that because others Vicarious Stigma
believed they were failing to be good parents, their opin-
ions were of less value, and their community participation In addition to drawing stigma and having the potential for
less welcome; as one mother emphatically stated, ‘‘heck embarrassment, mothers also described that the child’s
yes! People blamed me and avoided me’’ (Eve). One non-normative behaviour and/or symptoms also resulted in
mother described feeling so unwelcome at the parents’ the child being directly stigmatised. Mothers experienced
committee at her son’s school that she withdrew, ‘‘you their child’s stigma vicariously; that is, they became sad-
know, I have opinions and I want to make a contribution on dened and worried when their child was harmed by stigma.
things… like by being in the P and C, but I can’t … I feel For example, two mothers explained, ‘‘that hurt me as a
like I can’t join them because I have a child who is diffi- parent when other parents were saying to their child ‘don’t
cult’’ (Bea). play with him,’ … it was alienating him’’ (Eve) and, ‘‘you
Conversely, mothers described that the absence of get kids at school who bully him about his medication and
blame or bad-parent stigma in situations where it was things, so the kids pick on him and that upsets you as a
expected was a welcome relief. This was because in these parent when you know your child is being bullied or crit-
situations, mothers felt that their needs for inclusion, sup- icised’’ (Dee). This form of stigma intimated to mothers
port, and empathy were met. Although these experiences that they were failing to protect their child from harm,
were few, and surprising when they did occur, the mothers thereby failing to be a good parent. For example, ‘‘I just
described feeling validated as a person and as a parent. One want to protect him from that [stigma], but I can’t’’ (Jane).
mother described an experience of speaking with the Mothers felt frustrated, helpless, and guilty because they
school principal after attending family court; she had felt at were unable to entirely eliminate their child’s experiences
risk of blame for her child’s difficulties as a result of the of stigma.
court issues but instead received support:
Outcomes of External Stigma: Social Avoidance
Every time I have been in family court I’ve always
and Self-Doubt
had to give them [child’s school] a court order, so
everyone’s on the same page. So they’ve been really
Regardless of the type of stigma a parent experienced,
supportive. Best state school I have ever come across
ongoing encounters resulted in mothers trying to avoid
in my life!… The principal said ‘you’re really strong,
social situations in which they (or their child) might
you go through family court and that must be hard for
experience prejudice, criticism and discrimination. Should
you, but you’re doing a good job’ (Ava).
this fail, external stigma had the propensity to create self-
doubt. Social avoidance and self-doubt are explored in the
Courtesy Stigma subthemes below.

The judgement and criticism regarding the quality of one’s Social Avoidance
parenting was often magnified in social settings where the
child’s misbehaviour and/or symptoms (e.g., defiance, Mothers reported perceiving stigma to be so insuperable
social anxiety, or hyperactivity) were particularly obvious; that it was beyond the average parent’s strength and ability
for example, ‘‘you have your child yelling, swearing at you, to challenge it; for example, ‘‘that’s just the way it is, you
and being rude and then you have other people looking at just have to put up with it’’ (Jen). As such, stigma was seen
you and saying ‘oh you have no control over that child’’’ as burdensome; it ‘‘made things harder, made everything
(Ava). Thus, mothers reported that they were stigmatised harder’’ (Dee). This resulted in some mothers reacting with

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3117

avoidance. Two mothers explained their social avoidance you can be having a fantastic run and everything is just
in direct terms, ‘‘I do get asked some places by friends, but working well and all it takes is just one person to put in a
I try to avoid it, only because I feel I have that stigma that little snide remark somewhere and that just makes me
they think I am a bad mum’’ (Ella), and ‘‘I just avoid the question myself all over again’’ (Eve). Thus, stigma
neighbours… I don’t speak to them, don’t say hello to instigated or exacerbated self-doubt in mothers, which
them, just avoid them, like they’re really not there’’ (Eve). could lead mothers to adopt the stigma about themselves.
Other mothers engaged in secrecy, refusing to disclose
their child’s problem, ‘‘I feel the blame come back on me Internalising External Stigma and Self-Doubt
so I don’t try to explain it to others’’ (Jen). Ultimately, as Self-Stigma
mothers who reported concealing their child’s diagnosis
did so with the expressed aim of limiting exposure to According to mothers, being aware of external stigma and
anticipated stigma towards themselves and their child. doubting one’s competency as a good parent was neces-
sary, but not sufficient, for the development of self-stigma.
Self-Doubt Mothers self-stigmatised because they came to believe that
the content of their self-doubt and the external stigma they
If a mother was unable to avoid exposure to situations in experienced was accurate. One way that mothers described
which external stigma manifested, the ongoing exposure self-stigma was in terms of feeling like a ‘bad parent.’ The
served to create or exacerbate existing self-doubt regarding following excerpt explains how a mother, who was already
one’s ability to achieve the goal of being a good parent. concerned about the quality of her parenting, and who was
Like external stigma, self-doubt was pervasive and was also experiencing repeated bad-parent stigma from others,
experienced by all mothers interviewed. Self-doubt was was more inclined to believe that stigma was true of herself
expressed in terms of falling short of the ‘good parent’ and her parenting, ‘‘of course, how could I not believe that
ideal. Mothers asked themselves, ‘‘am I a good enough [bad-parent stigma]? My child wasn’t acting the same as
parent?’’(Jane), ‘‘did I do the wrong thing?’’ (Diane), ‘‘am I normal children, he wasn’t hitting those milestones that
doing the right thing?’’ (Dee), ‘‘am I enough for him other children were…but of course I believed it, how could
[son]?’’ (Jane). Mothers frequently questioned their ability I not?’’ (Lisa). Although such thoughts may not have been
to be the good parent that they and others expected them to constantly present, many mothers reported believing that at
be; in their own words, ‘‘in myself there’s always that some point in time they had been an inadequate parent. For
‘maybe I didn’t do this enough’, or maybe ‘I didn’t do that example, one mother reported feeling as though she ‘‘had
enough’… you’re always second-guessing yourself’’ failed as a mother’’ (Diane) and another stated, ‘‘maybe
(Kate). I’m not cut out to have a child with these issues, you know,
Mothers also questioned their role in the causation, maybe I’m not enough for him?’’ (Jane).
exacerbation or continuation of their child’s problem. Self-stigma was also described in terms of self-blame.
Taking prescribed medication during pregnancy, compli- Once again reflecting the content of their own self-doubt
cations during childbirth, making choices about returning and the stigma they experienced, mothers blamed them-
to work, not devoting enough time to the child in order to selves for the onset, continuation, and/or exacerbation of
prevent or ‘solve’ the problem, being a single parent, and their child’s problem. Every mother made statements
being in an unhappy marital relationship, were some of the regarding this, such as, ‘‘I felt like that because he had a
many ways mothers felt they might have contributed to the traumatic birth, that I had caused this… I asked myself,
onset of the child’s problems. well did I imprint this exhaustion and anxiety on him, is
Several mothers described self-doubt as acting like a this where it comes from?’’ (Jen) and ‘‘you get to the stage
seed that, once planted and fed by external stigma expe- where you say, should I have done things differently, why
riences, was difficult to disregard: is my son like this, what could I have done to prevent it?’’
(Dee) Although self-blame fluctuated in terms of its sal-
It stems from, well, we are the carrier of the chil-
ience and influence, it remained an unresolved issue for all
dren… and so in the core of you, you’re just like – I
the mothers we interviewed. As one mother explained,
gave birth to this child and this child is like this. So
‘‘yeah I suppose you always [long pause], I always have
there is that little guilt seed right from the start and
thoughts of ‘‘is it something I did, did something happen
from there things happen, people say stuff, and I
when I was pregnant, or during the younger years?’’ (Jane).
think the seed just grows and grows and grows (Eve).
When self-stigma in the form of self-blame, self-de-
Stigma could act as a jolting reminder of self-doubt even valuation as a bad parent, and self-shame was present it
when a mother felt she was achieving the ‘good parent’ created many negative emotions, with mothers reporting
ideal: ‘‘I think you will always question yourself. I mean feeling ‘‘quite down’’ (Jen), ‘‘so sad’’ (Dee), ‘‘so guilty’’

123
3118 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

(Bea), and ‘‘pissed off’’ (Jane). Such feelings created a Reframing the good parent ideal was also influenced by
desire in the mother for a more ideal life and sadness the belief that one had always done what was ‘right’ and
regarding the current situation; for example, ‘‘it’s sad, it’s ‘best’ for the child and the family, irrespective of others’
like you want a better life for yourself in a lot of ways’’ opinions. Doing what was ‘right’ involved a resolute need
(Eve). The effects of self-stigma and the situations that to protect the child from harm and this meant putting the
induced it could be perceived to be unrelenting; as one child’s needs before one’s own. As two mothers described,
mother described, ‘‘I just think that when the situation is at ‘‘You have to put these things aside. If I feel this bad, well
its worst it’s almost unbearable, and very hard. It doesn’t think about what this is like for my son. So I put the ego
feel like it’s ever going to get any better… In there, in the aside, my son is more important’’ (Eve) and, ‘‘my son picks
thick of it, it’s devastating’’ (Diane). Whilst wrestling with up on these things that other people say and do… so I have
these emotions, mothers felt that external stigma made to take him out of the situation to stop that, to protect him
these feelings worse: from that’’ (Jen). Mothers utilised a number of strategies to
achieve this aim: enlisting teachers, psychologists, and
When that child is really acting up… there is a part of
occupational therapists to support the child at school,
you that does not like that child, cannot stand being
standing in the playground to ensure the child was not
with that child. And you’ve got the shame of feeling
teased or bullied, correcting others who made inappropriate
like that and dealing with that thought and then
comments, starting sporting teams so their child could be
someone approaches you and you’re like ‘whoa don’t
included and play sport with other children, and restricting
you dare, I feel crap enough’ (Eve).
their own social worlds. Often, protecting the child meant a
The overall effect of self-stigma was not only to elicit such mother had to ‘‘educate those people’’ (Jen), ‘‘correct them
negative feelings, but also to decrease self-esteem and and their ignorance’’ (Eve), and ‘‘tell it to them straight’’
personal wellbeing, which one mother described as, ‘‘when (Bea). These selfless acts confirmed to mothers that they
you’ve had major stigma, or lots of stigma towards you that were good parents because they were acting in the best
really has some negative impacts on you and your self- interests of the child.
esteem, and your happiness, and that affects your life, and Mothers also looked for evidence to refute self-stigma
to this day I still feel that creeping in’’ (Ava). Thus, self- beliefs. Receiving a diagnosis that provided a non-blaming
stigma was characterised by a belief that the content of reason for the child’s problem, becoming more informed
external stigma and one’s self-doubt was true and accurate; about the child’s problem and how best to support them,
this being that one was an incompetent, inadequate parent identifying genetic origins of the child’s problem that
and/or was to blame for the onset, continuation or vindicated the mother, parenting other offspring without an
exacerbation of the child’s problem. EBD, being supported and ‘‘given permission’’ (Eve) by
others to stop self-blaming, and having others ‘‘stick up and
defend’’ (Ella) them were some amongst the many forms of
Refuting Self-Stigma: The Good Enough Parent refutable evidence mothers reported. Evidence could also
be that the child was showing improvement; that the child
Although all the mothers we interviewed reported that they ‘‘got better’’ (Sue), because ‘‘you can see the changes’’
had self-stigmatised, the negative influence this had on (Dee). As one mother described:
them appeared to wane in response to mothers believing
We’ve made the right decisions, it helps when you
that despite repeated experiences of stigma and lingering
see him [son] doing well, because it puts your mind at
self-doubt that they were meeting their own standards of
ease, that you have done ok as a parent and that you
the ‘good parent’ ideal. Mothers explained that their con-
have made the right decision by putting him on
cept of the ‘good parent’ became increasingly defined by
medication. So that makes you feel better, when you
the mother’s own expectations for themselves as opposed
see those improvements and differences (Dee).
to idealistic societal expectations. These expectations
changed because mothers came to an understanding that This evidence served to incrementally restore faith in the
their parenting journey was unique and could not be self as a good parent and to diminish the value and truth of
defined by the standards of others. As such, the good parent the stigma mothers experienced, so that mothers began to
ideal was reframed from ‘‘doing what everyone else feel that they ‘‘must be doing something right’’ (Lisa). As
expects’’ (Jane) to ‘‘doing what’s best for my kids’’ (Kate) such, a number of factors contributed to the changing
and ‘‘doing the best I can’’ (Bea) given the circumstances. standards of the good parent ideal. As the ideal was
As one mother stated, ‘‘In truth, I know my journey, I know reframed as the child aged, the belief that one was indeed a
what I’ve been through and I know I’m doing pretty well… good parent, or at least not a bad parent, became more
I know I’m doing what’s right by my kids’’ (Ava). assured with every piece of evidence the mother gathered

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3119

that confirmed her good parent status or absolved her from literature (Blum 2007; Corrigan and Miller 2004; Fernán-
blame: dez and Arcia 2004; Mak and Cheung 2008; Moses 2010;
Todd and Jones 2003), the predominant focus of parent
How come it’s different [seeing self as a good par-
stigma research has been on courtesy and parent-blaming
ent]? Partly time apart from that situation [others’
stigma (Mukolo et al. 2010). Generally, these two forms
stigma]. Also because [son] has grown and devel-
are considered to independently contribute to the inter-
oped. We had some shaky times but he’s on a much
nalisation of stigma (e.g., Mak and Cheung 2008; Moses
better pathway at the moment. More positive things
2010). We concur with Mak and Cheung (2008, 2012) that
are going on. The negative instances are few and far
courtesy stigma results in shame and thus contributes to
between (Diane).
self-stigma, but hypothesise that courtesy stigma (and, if
Despite this, mothers harboured lingering self-doubt internalised, self-shame) are but one part of a larger issue.
regarding their role in the onset of the child’s problem. Mothers in our study described that all forms of stigma
In this way, mothers indicated that self-blame may be quite they experience have the propensity to contribute to self-
enduring and difficult to relinquish. As one parent stigma because each form sends the same underlying
described, ‘‘…but I know I will always blame myself message: that the parent is not a good parent. Given these
about that [child’s disorder], I’ll always question myself’’ findings, we agree with Corrigan et al. (2012) and Corrigan
(Dee). and Miller (2004) in that stigma, and hence self-stigma, is
best considered as a multifaceted construct.
Considering stigma in such a way may address an
unanswered question from the stigma literature surround-
Discussion ing parents’ tendency to self-blame. Specifically, Ferriter
et al. (2003) and Milliken (2001) have argued that parents
Our main aim was to describe how parents of children with self-blame even in the absence of blame from others,
EBDs experience self-stigma using a qualitative investi- although to date the reasons for this remain unclear (Moses
gation of those with lived experience. To this end, we 2014). One possibility is that mothers self-blame because,
interviewed 12 parents (1 father), and focussed on the in their own eyes, they do not measure up to their own
experiences and perspectives of the 11 mothers. Five ‘good parent’ ideal. An additional possibility is that
themes were found: (1) the ‘good parent’ ideal, (2) mothers might self-blame because they experience multiple
awareness of external stigma, (3) outcomes of external forms of stigma that could equally induce the feeling of
stigma, (4) internalising stigma and self-doubt as self- being an inadequate parent (such as by being at fault,
stigma, and (5) refuting self-stigma. The first theme – that inadequate, or incompetent). Indeed, all forms of stigma
of the ‘good parent’ ideal – was crucial to understand as it have been shown to compromise parents’ sense of moral
served as the basis for understanding all of the themes. worth and competence (Mak and Cheung 2008; Moses
Specifically, mothers described the stigma that they 2010; Todd and Jones 2003).
received questioned their abilities to be a ‘good parent,’ A key finding of this study is that mothers’ self-stigma
such as through implicating their role in the causation, (characterised by self-blame, self-shame, and/or bad-parent
exacerbation or continuation of their child’s problem self-view) develops as a result of mothers becoming aware
(theme 2). On the basis of these messages, mothers of or experiencing external stigma, which creates or
reported that they doubted their ‘good parenting,’ and if exacerbates self-doubt regarding one’s ability to be a good
they were unable to find evidence to refute this self-doubt parent, and (should mothers be unable to refute external
(theme 3), they experienced self-stigma (theme 4). Thus, stigma and their own self-doubt), culminates in self-stigma.
understanding mothers’ desires to be and to be recognised Although we are tentative in drawing conclusions regard-
as a ‘good parent’ was central to understanding why stigma ing a temporal progression of self-stigma (due to the con-
caused such negative emotional effects, why its content current nature of our data), our findings appear to be
mattered to the parent, and why it could not be easily broadly consistent with Corrigan and Watson’s (2002) 3A
dismissed. Understanding the pervasive importance of the model of self-stigma. Specifically, our findings indicate
‘good parent’ ideal also provides context for mothers’ some overlap with the central aspects of the 3A model,
internal motivation and drive towards refuting self-stigma especially the first (‘awareness of stigma’) step. However,
(theme 5). there are some differences; namely, our findings lead us to
We found that mothers experienced stigma from three propose that social avoidance and the development of self-
key domains: bad-parent/parent-blaming stigma, courtesy doubt as a good parent are the second step, not ‘agree’ as in
stigma, and vicarious stigma. Although each of these the 3A model. Further research aimed at testing the pos-
stigma types have been mentioned in the parent stigma sible pathways to self-stigma is key, as understanding the

123
3120 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

process by which self-stigma develops is needed to address Although this study is the first investigation to consider
the problem (Corrigan et al. 2011). mothers’ self-stigma as the primary research focus, it is not
Self-stigma was important to the mothers in our study, as without its limitations. Qualitative inquiry has the benefits
they described self-stigma as damaging their self-esteem, of providing insight into others’ lived experiences; how-
self-efficacy, and personal wellbeing. These effects are sim- ever, caution should be applied in generalising findings.
ilar to those noted by adults with mental illness who experi- Although our data analysis did reach saturation in our
ence self-stigma (e.g., Corrigan et al. 2006; Corrigan and Rao themes, our small sample size is recognised and we
2012). Corrigan et al. (2009) argue that such negative effects encourage future researchers to consider a larger sample,
can lead to self-devaluation and a sense of futility, and ulti- particularly one that includes fathers and non-biological
mately a withdrawal from society. Similarly, mothers in the parents. The sample self-selected for inclusion, and as
current study described withdrawing from certain social sit- such, the experiences of parents not opting to participate
uations. This loss of social capital and increasing social iso- may differ. Because participants were required to have a
lation is concerning as it is likely to negatively influence their child diagnosed with an EBD, our findings may not extend
child’s development as well as further compromise their own to parents who have not engaged in the diagnostic and
sense of maternal self-efficacy (Arditti et al. 2013; Coleman treatment process. The experiences of these parents may
and Hilderbrandt-Karraker 2003; Moses 2014). Low mater- differ conceptually, because stigma may be impeding their
nal self-efficacy is correlated with outcomes such as high treatment seeking altogether. We did not establish parent
levels of maternal stress and distress, behavioural problems in mental health status; it is possible that parents’ own
children and passive parental coping with these behaviours experiences of stigma and self-stigma may be influential to
(Coleman and Hilderbrandt-Karraker 2003; Ohan et al. that experienced as a result of their child’s mental health
2000), suggesting that social isolation may only serve to disorder.
worsen outcomes for mothers and children alike. Our study constituted only Australian mothers, of pre-
Still, these possible negative outcomes may be counter- dominantly children with ADHD (i.e., 52 % of the sample).
balanced by actions that mothers took to protect their This raises questions as to the generalizability to other
children, as mothers commonly reported that they disre- countries, and to parents of children with disorders other
garded threats to their own wellbeing in favour of focuss- than ADHD. Prevalence rates of this disorder (as well as
ing all their efforts and attention on their child. By other related disorders, such as anxiety and depression) in
advocating for and protecting their child, mothers felt that Australian children are largely consistent with that of other
they were meeting not only sociocultural expectations of nations (e.g., Asia, Europe and North America; Polanczyk
being a ‘good parent’, but also their own expectations. In et al. 2007, 2015). As in the United States, ADHD in
doing so, mothers instil in themselves a greater sense of Australia is predominantly treated with stimulant medica-
parental competency, leading to self-efficacy and resilience tion, and rates of medication treatment are showing the
(Coleman and Hilderbrandt-Karraker 2003; Masten 2014; same increasing trend in Australia as they are world-wide
Singh 2004). Thus, for many of the mothers in our study, (e.g., Hinshaw et al. 2011). Although it may be believed
self-stigma had been fleeting, usually present in the early that ADHD is less stigmatising than other disorders, cur-
stages of the child’s mental health disorder before the rent evidence suggests that this is not the case. For
parent had built a stronger belief in their parenting com- example, stigma towards children with ADHD is similar to
petency. This belief grew as mothers perceived that they that towards children with depression (Ohan et al. 2013).
were acting in the best interests of the child and/or Furthermore, Mak and Cheung (2008) report that parent
encountered further evidence to refute not only the stigma self-shame is consistent regardless of the child’s diagnosis
they were experiencing, but also their own self-doubt. The (e.g., schizophrenia, anxiety or mood disorders). In sum,
‘good parent’ ideal became increasingly attainable because this suggests that there are likely to be similarities in the
it was reframed to match this changing view from that of experiences of parents regardless of nationality or disorder
self as bad parent, to self as good enough parent, to self as type.
the best parent possible given the circumstances. This is To conclude, our findings indicate that mothers’ self-
consistent with Milliken and Northcott (2003), who argue stigma is characterised by a view of self as a failed parent.
that parents redefine their parenting identity over time to However, those mothers who react to and/or refute the
meet the demands of the caregiving role and in response to stigma and self-doubt through seeking evidence appear to
a progressive resolution of caregiver grief and self-blame. minimise self-stigma. Reflecting this, to support mothers
To this, we also add the resolution of guilt, shame, self- who self-stigmatise, assistance may be most effective when
depreciation and self-doubt, with this resolution enhanced concentrated on reducing self-doubt by enhancing sense of
by a belief that one is acting in the best interests of the parenting competency and beliefs of being a good parent
child and the identification of other refutable evidence. (and thus parents’ resilience). Increasing self-esteem and

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3121

empowerment have been shown to be effective in reducing Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all indi-
self-stigma in adults with mental illness (Brohan et al. vidual participants included in the study.
2010; Ilic et al. 2011; Mittal et al. 2012). A focus on
decreasing self-blame might also be helpful and could be
References
achieved by providing explanations for the child’s beha-
viours that are not parent-blaming as it appears that Arcia, E., Fernández, M. C., & Jáquez, M. (2005). Latina mothers’
unhelpful cognitions maintain these beliefs. Helping characterizations of their young children with disruptive behav-
mothers to identify ways to discount the stigma they iors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 14(1), 111–125.
experience as holding no truth or value might also be doi:10.1007/s10826-005-1126-8.
Arditti, J. A., Gryzwacz, J. G., & Gallimore, S. W. (2013). A
beneficial. Cognitive behavioural strategies have been demedicalized view of maternal distress: Conceptualisation and
found to be effective in addressing stigma-related negative instrument development. Psychological Services, 10(4), 386–394.
self-beliefs (Mittal et al. 2012), and may be applicable in doi:10.1017/a0029954.
the parent context. Parents report that peer led family Blum, L. M. (2007). Mother-blame in the Prozac nation: Raising kids
with invisible disabilities. Gender and Society, 21(2), 202–226.
support groups (e.g., ‘In Our Own Voice—Family Com- doi:10.1177/0891243206298178.
panion; Perlick et al. 2011) are helpful in normalising their Bos, A. R., Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., & Stutterheim, S. E. (2013).
situation, provide a means for sharing experiences with Stigma: Advances in theory and research. Basic and Applied
similar others and create positive social comparisons Social Psychology, 35(1), 1–9. doi:10.1080/01973533.2012.
746147.
(Frigerio and Montali 2015). However, we offer a rather Brohan, E., Elgie, R., Sartorius, N., & Thornicroft, G. (2010). Self-
disparate range of possible supports. To date, there is a lack stigma, empowerment and perceived discrimination among
of empirically validated programs that may be used to people with schizophrenia in 14 European countries: The
assist parents with the alleviation of their self-stigma. We GAMIAN-Europe study. Schizophrenia Research, 122(1–3),
232–238. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.1065.
encourage the development of such a program, ideally with Campbell, J. L., Quincy, C., Osserman, J., & Pedersen, O. K. (2013).
parent participation. Coding in-depth semistructured interviews: Problems of uniti-
We have shown that parent self-stigma develops in a zation and intercoder reliability and agreement. Sociological
fashion that is somewhat consistent with existing expla- Methods and Research, 42(3), 294–320. doi:10.1177/
0049124113500475.
nations of self-stigma. However more research is needed to Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide
clarify whether adopting and applying theory and measure through qualitative analysis. London: Sage.
originally derived from research with adults with mental Chevalier, J. M., & Buckles, D. J. (2013). Participatory action research:
health problems to parents of children with mental health Theory and methods for engaged inquiry. UK: Routledge.
Coleman, P. K., & Hilderbrandt-Karraker, K. (2003). Maternal self-
disorders is appropriate so as not to lead to erroneous efficacy beliefs, competence in parenting, and toddlers’ beha-
conclusions regarding parent self-stigma. We propose that viour and developmental status. Infant Mental Health Journal,
because self-stigma was found to be multifaceted, a con- 24(2), 126–148. doi:10.1002/imhj.10048.
sideration of all facets would provide a more comprehen- Corrigan, P. W., & Miller, F. E. (2004). Shame, blame, and
contamination: A review of the impact of mental illness stigma
sive assessment of self-stigma in parents than considering on family members. Journal of Mental Health, 13(6), 537–548.
single facets in isolation as has been done in the past. With doi:10.1080/09638230400017004.
these issues in mind, it is important to prioritise self-stigma Corrigan, P. W., & Rao, D. (2012). On the self-stigma of mental illness:
when considering parents of children with mental health Stages, disclosure, and strategies for change. Canadian Journal of
Psychiatry, 57(8), 464–469. doi:10.1177/070674371205700804.
disorders, especially as research in this area moves Corrigan, P. W., & Watson, A. C. (2002). The paradox of self-stigma
forward. and mental illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
9(1), 35–53. doi:10.1093/clipsy.9.1.35.
Acknowledgments The authors would like to extend their sincerest Corrigan, P. W., Watson, A. C., & Barr, L. (2006). The self-stigma of
gratitude to the parents who contributed to this research. The insight mental illness: Implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy.
into your experiences is valued greatly. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25(8), 875–884.
doi:10.1521/jscp.2006.25.8.875.
Funding This research was funded by the University of Western Corrigan, P. W., Larson, J. E., & Rüsch, N. (2009). Self-stigma and
Australia. the ‘‘why try’’ effect: Impact on life goals and evidence-based
practices. World Psychiatry, 8(2), 75–81. doi:10.1002/j.2051-
Compliance with Ethical Standards 5545.2009.tb00218.x.
Corrigan, P. W., Rafacz, J., & Rüsch, N. (2011). Examining a
Conflict of interest The authors declare no potential conflicts of progressive model of self-stigma and its impact on people with
interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of serious mental illness. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 339–343.
this article. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.024.
Corrigan, P. W., Michaels, P. J., Vega, E., Gause, M., Watson, A. C.,
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving & Rüsch, N. (2012). Self-stigma of mental illness scale-short
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of form: Reliability and validity. Psychiatric Research, 199(1),
the University of Western Australia Human Research Ethics Office. 65–69. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.009.

123
3122 J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123

Corrigan, P. W., Pickett, S., Kraus, D., Burks, R., & Schmidt, A. of mental illness. International Journal of Social Psychiatry,
(2015). Community-based participatory research examining 58(3), 246–257. doi:10.1177/0020764010392058.
health care needs of African Americans who are homeless with Kaiser, K. (2009). Protecting respondent confidentiality in qualitative
mental illness. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and research. Qualitative Health Research, 19(11), 1632–1641.
Undeserved, 26(1), 119–133. doi:10.1353/hpu.2015.0018. doi:10.1177/1049732309350879.
Corrigan, P. W., Bink, A. B., Schmidt, A., Jones, N., & Rüsch, N. Koch, T. (2006). Establishing rigour in qualitative research: The
(2016). What is the impact of self-stigma? Loss of self-respect decision trail. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 53(1), 91–100.
and the ‘‘why try’’ effect. Journal of Mental Health, 25(1), doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.03681.x.
10–15. doi:10.3109/09638237.2015.1021902. Koro-Ljungberg, M., & Bussing, R. (2009). The management of
Crotty, M. (1998). The foundations of social research: Meaning and courtesy stigma in the lives of families with teenagers with
perspective in the research process. London: Sage Publications. ADHD. Journal of Family Issues, 30(9), 1175–1200. doi:10.
Ditrano, C. J., & Silverstein-Bordeaux, L. (2006). Listening to parents’ 1177/0192513X09333707.
voices: Participatory action research in the schools. Professional Lindseth, A., & Norberg, A. (2004). A phenomenological hermeneu-
Psychology: Research and Practice, 37(4), 359–366. doi:10. tical method for researching lived experience. Scandinavian
1037/0735-7028.37.4.359. Journal of Caring Sciences, 18(2), 145–153. doi:10.1111/j.1471-
Fernández, M. C., & Arcia, E. (2004). Disruptive behaviours and 6712.2004.00258.x.
maternal responsibility: A complex portrait of stigma, self- Mak, W. W. S., & Cheung, R. Y. M. (2008). Affiliate stigma among
blame, and other reactions. Hispanic Journal of Behavioural caregivers of people with intellectual disability of mental illness.
Science, 26(3), 356–372. doi:10.1177/0739986304267208. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 21(6),
Ferriter, M., Huband, N., & Ferriter, N. (2003). Experiences of 532–545. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2008.00426.x.
parents with a son or daughter suffering from schizophrenia. Mak, W. W. S., & Cheung, R. Y. M. (2012). Psychological distress
Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 10(5), and subjective burden of caregivers of people with mental
552–560. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2850.2003.00624.x. illness: The role of affiliate stigma and face concern. Community
Francis, A. (2012). Stigma in an era of medicalisation and anxious Mental Health Journal, 48(3), 270–274. doi:10.1007/s10597-
parenting: how proximity and culpability shape middle-class 011-9422-9.
parents’ experiences of disgrace. Sociology of Health & Illness, Mak, W. W. S., & Kwok, Y. T. Y. (2010). Internalisation of stigma
34(6), 927–942. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9566-2011.01445.x. for parents of children with autism spectrum disorder in Hong
Frigerio, A., & Montali, L. (2015). An ethnographic-discursive Kong. Social Science and Medicine, 70(12), 2045–2051. doi:10.
approach to parental self-help groups: The case of ADHD. 1016/j.socscimed.2010.02.023.
Qualitative Health Research, 26(7), 935–950. doi:10.1177/ Malterud, K. (2012). Systematic text condensation: A strategy for
1049732315586553. qualitative analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health,
Garrison, D. R., Cleveland-Innes, M., Koole, M., & Kappelman, J. 40(8), 795–805. doi:10.1177/1403494812465030.
(2006). Revisiting methodological issues in transcript analysis: Masten, A. S. (2014). Ordinary magic: Resilience in development.
Negotiated coding and reliability. Internet and Higher Educa- New York: Guilford Publications.
tion, 9(1), 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.jheduc.2005.11.001. McNall, M. A., & Foster-Fishman, P. G. (2007). Methods of rapid
Giorgi, A. (2009). The descriptive phenomenological method in evaluation, assessment, and appraisal. American Journal of
psychology. Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University Press. Evaluation, 28(2), 151–168. doi:10.1177/1098214007300895.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled Mehta, S. I., & Farina, A. (1988). Associative stigma: Perceptions of
identity. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster. the difficulties of college-aged children of stigmatized fathers.
Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). How many interviews are Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7(2), 192–202.
enough? An experiment with data saturation and variability. Field doi:10.1521/jscp.1988.7.2-3.192.
Methods, 18(1), 59–82. doi:10.1605/01.301-0006629869.2009. Milliken, J. P. (2001). Disenfranchised mothers: Caring for an adult
Halcomb, E., & Davidson, P. M. (2006). Is verbatim transcription of child with schizophrenia. Health Care for Women International,
interview data always necessary? Applied Nursing Research, 22(1–2), 149–166. doi:10.1080/073993301300003135.
19(1), 38–42. doi:10.1016/j.apnr.2005.06.001. Milliken, J. P., & Northcott, H. C. (2003). Redefining parental
Harden, J. (2005). Parenting a young person with mental health identity: Caregiving and schizophrenia. Qualitative Health
problems: Temporal disruption and reconstruction. Sociology of Research, 13(1), 100–113. doi:10.1177/1049732302239413.
Health & Illness, 27(3), 351–371. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9566. Mittal, D., Sullivan, G., Chekuri, L., Allee, E., & Corrigan, P. W.
2005.0046.x. (2012). Empirical studies of self-stigma reduction strategies: A
Hasson-Ohayon, I., Levy, I., Kravetz, S., Vollanski-Narkis, A., & critical review of the literature. Psychiatric Services, 63(10),
Roe, D. (2011). Insight into mental illness, self-stigma, and the 974–981. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201100459.
family burden of parents of persons with a severe mental illness. Moses, T. (2010). Exploring parents’ self-blame in relation to
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 52(1), 75–80. doi:10.1016/j.compp adolescent’s mental disorders. Family Relations: An Interdisci-
sych.2010.04.008. plinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 59(2), 103–120.
Hinshaw, S. P. (2004). Parental mental disorder and children’s doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00589.x.
functioning: Silence and communication, stigma and resilience. Moses, T. (2014). Stigma and family. In P. W. Corrigan (Ed.), The
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(2), stigma of disease and disability (pp. 247–268). Washington DC:
400–441. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_22. American Psychological Association.
Hinshaw, S. P., Scheffler, R. M., Fulton, B. D., Aase, H., Mukolo, A., Heflinger, C. A., & Wallston, K. A. (2010). The stigma
Banaschewski, T., Cheng, W., & Weiss, M. D. (2011). Interna- of childhood mental disorders: A conceptual framework. Journal
tional variation in treatment procedures for ADHD: Social of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
context and recent trends. Psychiatric Services, 62(5), 459–464. 49(2), 92–103. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2009.12.003.
doi:10.1176/appi.ps.62.5.459. Neuberg, S. L., Smith, D. M., Hoffman, J. C., & Russell, F. J. (1994).
Ilic, M., Reinecke, J., Bohner, G., Röttgers, H., Beblo, T., Driessen, When we observe stigmatised and ‘normal’ individuals inter-
M., & Corrigan, P. W. (2011). Protecting self-esteem from acting: Stigma by association. Personality and Social Psychol-
stigma: A test of different strategies for coping with the stigma ogy Bulletin, 20(2), 196–209. doi:10.1177/0146167294292007.

123
J Child Fam Stud (2016) 25:3109–3123 3123

Ohan, J. L., Leung, D. W., & Johnston, C. (2000). The parenting Saltzburg, S. (2009). Parents’ experience of feeling socially supported
sense of competence scale: Evidence of a stable factor structure as adolescents come out as lesbian and gay: A phenomenological
and validity. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 32(4), study. Journal of Family Social Work, 12(4), 340–358. doi:10.
251–261. doi:10.1037/h0087122. 1080/10522150903261932.
Ohan, J. L., Visser, T. A. W., Moss, R. G., & Allen, N. B. (2013). Shaw, R. (2010). Embedding reflexivity within experiential qualita-
Parents’ stigmatising attitudes toward psychiatric labels for tive psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 7(3),
ADHD and depression. Psychiatric Services, 64(12), 1270–1273. 233–243. doi:10.1080/14780880802699092.
doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201200578. Singh, I. (2004). Doing their jobs: Mothering with Ritalin in a culture
Perlick, D. A., Rosenheck, R., Clarkin, J., Sirey, J., Salahi, J., of mother-blame. Social Science and Medicine, 59(6),
Struening, E., & Link, B. (2001). Stigma as a barrier to recovery: 1193–1205. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.01.011.
Adverse effects of perceived stigma on social adaption of Snell, P., Miguel, N., & East, J. (2009). Changing directions:
persons diagnosed with bipolar affective disorder. Psychiatric Participatory action research as a parent involvement strategy.
Services, 52(12), 1627–1632. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.52.12.1627. Educational Action Research, 17(2), 239–258. doi:10.1080/
Perlick, D. A., Kalvin, C., Huntington, B., Holman, C. S., Nelson, A. 09650790902914225.
H., Mattias, K., & Corrigan, P. W. (2011). In our own voice- Tobin, G. A., & Begley, C. M. (2004). Methodological rigour within a
family companion: Reducing self-stigma of family members of qualitative framework. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48(4),
persons with serious mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 62(12), 388–396. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03207.x.
1456–1462. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.001222011. Todd, S., & Jones, S. (2003). ‘Mum’s the word!’: Maternal accounts
Pescosolido, B. A., Fettes, D. L., Martin, J. K., McLeod, J. D., & of dealings with the professional world. Journal of Applied
Monahan, J. (2007). Perceived dangerousness of children with Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 16(3), 229–244. doi:10.
mental health problems and support for coerced treatment. 1046/j.1468-3148.2003.00163.x.
Psychiatric Services, 58(5), 619–625. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.58.5. Walker, J. S., Coleman, D., Lee, J., Squire, P. N., & Friesen, B. J.
619. (2008). Children’s stigmatization of childhood depression and
Phelan, J. C., Bromet, E. J., & Link, B. G. (1998). Psychiatric illness ADHD: Magnitude and demographic variation in a national
and family stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24(1), 115–126. sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033304. Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 912–920. doi:10.1097/CHI.
Polanczyk, G. V., De Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & 0b013e318179961a.
Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A Watson, A. C., Corrigan, P., Larson, J. E., & Sells, M. (2007). Self-
systematic review and metaregression analysis. The American stigma in people with mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942–948. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp. 33(6), 1312–1318. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl076.
164.6.942. Zisman-Ilani, Y., Levy-Frank, I., Hasson-Ohayon, I., Kravetz, S.,
Polanczyk, G. V., Salum, G. A., Sugaya, L. S., Caye, A., & Rohde, L. Mashiach-Eizenberg, M., & Roe, D. (2013). Measuring the
A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the internalised stigma of parents of persons with a serious mental
worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and illness. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 201(3),
adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and 186–187. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182845d00.
Allied Disciplines, 56(3), 345–365. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12381.

123

You might also like