You are on page 1of 42

Psychological Bulletin Cop\right 1992 h> t h e American Psychological Association, I I

1992, Vol. 112. No. 1.64-105

Risk and Protective Factors for Alcohol and Other Drug Problems
in Adolescence and Early Adulthood: Implications
for Substance Abuse Prevention
J. David Hawkins, Richard E Catalano, and Janet Y Miller
Social Development Research Group, School of Social Work
University of Washington

The authors suggest that the most promising route to effective strategies for the prevention of
adolescent alcohol and other drug problems is through a risk-focused approach. This approach
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

requires the identification of risk factors for drug abuse, identification of methods by which risk
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

factors have been effectively addressed, and application of these methods to appropriate high-risk
and general population samples in controlled studies. The authors review risk and protective factors
for drug abuse, assess a number of approaches for drug abuse prevention potential with high-risk
groups, and make recommendations for research and practice.

In spite of general decreases in the prevalence of the nonmed- life, destruction of families, and a weakening of the bonds that
ical use of most legal and illegal drugs in recent years, the abuse hold the society together.
of alcohol and other drugs during adolescence and early Given the serious consequences of drug and alcohol abuse,
adulthood remains a serious public health problem (Adams, considerable effort has been directed toward identifying effec-
Blanken, Ferguson, & Kopstein, 1990). The consequences of tive treatment. Until recently, applied research in the substance
drug abuse are acute on both a personal and a societal level. For abuse field has consisted primarily of experimental trials of
the developing young adult, drug and alcohol abuse under- various forms of treatment for alcohol and other drug abuse.
mines motivation, interferes with cognitive processes, contrib- The goal has been to identify ways to increase the effectiveness
utes to debilitating mood disorders, and increases risk of acci- of treatment and to prevent relapse following treatment. Strate-
dental injury or death. For the society at large, adolescent sub- gies ranging from self-help to aversive counterconditioning
stance abuse extracts a high cost in health care, educational have been advocated and assessed.
failure, mental health services, drug and alcohol treatment, and Many of these studies have demonstrated how abstinence
juvenile crime. can be achieved, but long-term maintenance of abstinence has
Added to the immediate personal and social costs of adoles- been more difficult. The reinforcing properties of alcohol and
cent drug abuse are the longer range implications for young- other drugs are themselves often reinforced by norms and be-
sters who continue to abuse alcohol and drugs into adult life. haviors of family members and others in the communities in
Drug abuse is involved in one third to one half of lung cancer which recovering people live. These combined reinforcements
and coronary heart disease cases in adults (R. Blum, 1987). often overcome short-term treatment gains. According to the
Alcohol and other drugs are major factors in acquired immuno- surgeon general: "For many drug-dependent persons, achieving
deficiency syndrome (AIDS), violent crimes, child abuse and
at least brief periods of drug abstinence is a readily achievable
neglect, and unemployment. The problems associated with al-
goal. Maintaining abstinence, or avoiding relapse, however,
cohol and other drug abuse carry costs in lost productivity, lost
poses a much greater overall challenge" (Surgeon general, 1988,
P. 311).
Added to disappointment with the staying power of drug
Preparation of this article was supported in part by Grant DA03721
from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and by Grant 87-JS-CX-
treatment is a growing recognition of the high cost of treatment
K084 from the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. and of the inability of existing treatment programs to keep up
Points of view or opinions expressed are those of the authors. with increasing demand. In recent years, these considerations
Our thanks to George S. Bridges, Elise Lake, Randy Gainey, Tim have stimulated interest in primary prevention of alcohol and
Murphy, John Campos, and Cheryl Yates for their assistance in the other drug abuse.
preparation of this article and to Patricia Huling for her management This article focuses on the prevention of alcohol and other
of the document's creation. Thanks also to Barry Brown, David drug abuse among adolescents. A number of views have been
Farrington, Michael Goodstadt, Rolf Loeber, Roger Weissberg, He- advanced about what constitutes substance abuse when con-
lene White, and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on
sidering adolescents (Hawkins, Lishner, & Catalano, 1985). In
drafts.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to J. this article, adolescent drug abuse is denned as the frequent use
David Hawkins, Social Development Research Group, School of So- of alcohol or other drugs during the teenage years or the use of
cial Work, University of Washington, 146 N. Canal Street, Suite 211, alcohol or other drugs in a manner that is associated with prob-
Seattle, Washington 98103. lems and dysfunctions. This conception of the problem is not
64
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 65

meant to condone the infrequent use of alcohol or other drugs drug abuse rather than generic contributors to adolescent prob-
by teenagers, which is a violation of the law. The present defini- lem behaviors. Current knowledge about the risk factors for
tion simply reflects a recognition that a relatively large propor- drug abuse does not provide a formula for prevention, but it
tion of teenagers try alcohol or other drugs without becoming does point to potential targets for preventive intervention. Im-
involved in the frequent use of these substances or developing plications for intervention are considered in this article after a
drug-related problems (Newcomb & Bentler, 1988; Shedler & review of known risk factors for drug abuse in adolescence and
Block, 1990). early adulthood.
Cloninger and his colleagues (Cloninger, Bohman, Sigvards- These risk factors can be roughly divided into two categories.
son, & von Knorring, 1985; Cloninger, Sigvardsson, & Boh- First are broad societal and cultural (i.e., contextual) factors,
man, 1988) have identified two types of alcoholism. One type is which provide the legal and normative expectations for behav-
associated with frequent impulsive-aggressive behavior and fol- ior. The second group includes factors that lie within individ-
lows an early onset of alcohol use and alcohol problems in ado- uals and their interpersonal environments. The principal inter-
lescence. This type of drug abuse is considered in this article. It personal environments in children's lives are families, school
is distinct from alcoholism that develops after age 25, which is classrooms, and peer groups. The risk factors have been de-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

not a focus of the current article. scribed elsewhere (Hawkins, Lishner, Catalano, & Howard,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Precursors of drug and alcohol problems have been de- 1986; Kandel, Simcha-Fagan, & Davies, 1986; Newcomb, Mad-
scribed as risk factors for drug abuse. Risk factors occur before dahian, & Bentler, 1986; Simcha-Fagan, Gersten, & Langner,
drug abuse and are associated statistically with an increased 1986) and are summarized here and in the left half of Table 1.
probability of drug abuse. A risk-focused approach seeks to This is not intended as a critical review of the methodologies of
prevent drug abuse by eliminating, reducing, or mitigating its the studies but rather as an overview of the evidence currently
precursors. This article suggests that a promising line for pre- available on risk factors for adolescent drug abuse.
vention research lies in testing interventions targeting multiple
early risk factors for drug abuse. Contextual Factors
A risk-focused approach in drug abuse prevention research
and policy is warranted given the apparent success of this ap- Individuals and groups exist within a social context: the val-
proach in reducing risk factors for problems as divergent as ues and structure of their society. For example, shifts in cultural
heart and lung disease (Bush et al., 1989; Vartiainen, Pallonen, norms, in the legal definitions of certain behaviors, and in eco-
McAlister, & Puska, 1990) and school failure (Berrueta-Cle- nomic factors have been shown to be associated with changes in
ment, Schweinhart, Barnett, Epstein, & Weikhart, 1984). The drug-using behaviors and in the prevalence of drug abuse. The
apparent failure of early prevention interventions, such as drug following risk factors (1 through 4 below) exist in the broad
information programs that did not address known risk factors social context:
for drug abuse (Stuart, 1974; Weaver & Tennant, 1973), also 1. Laws and norms favorable toward behavior. Recent re-
argues for this approach. search on the effects of laws on alcohol consumption has fo-
Many of the risk factors for adolescent drug abuse also pre- cused on three interventions by law: (a) taxation, (b) laws stating
dict other adolescent problem behaviors (Hawkins, Jenson, Ca- to whom alcohol may be sold, and (c) laws regarding how alco-
talano, & Lishner, 1988). There is evidence that adolescent drug hol is to be sold.
abuse is correlated with delinquency teenage pregnancy, and Alcohol consumption is affected by price, specifically the
school misbehavior and drop out (Elliott, Huizinga, & Menard, amount of tax placed on alcohol at purchase (Levy & Sheflin,
1989; Jessor & Jessor, 1977; Zabin, Hardy, Smith, & Hirsch, 1985). Cook and Tauchen (1982) found that increases in taxes
1986). Comprehensive risk-focused efforts probably can pre- on alcohol led to immediate and sharp decreases in liquor con-
vent other adolescent problem behaviors besides drug abuse. sumption and cirrhosis mortality.
If prevention of drug abuse (as denned above) is the goal, then Studies examining the relationship of minimum drinking
risk factors salient for drug abuse rather than for the occasional age and adolescent drinking and driving have generally shown
use of alcohol or other drugs should be targeted. A relatively that lowering the drinking age increases teen drinking and
small proportion of adolescent drinkers or users are frequent or driving and teen traffic fatalities and raising it decreases teen
problem users (Johnston, O'Malley, & Bachman, 1988; Shedler driving while intoxicated citations (DWIs) and deaths (Cook &
& Block, 1990). The following review focuses on factors that Tauchen, 1984; Joksch, 1988; Krieg, 1982; Saffer & Grossman,
have been shown to precede drug abuse. 1987).
Studies of restriction on how alcohol is sold have shown that
Risk Factors for Adolescent Drug Abuse allowing patrons to purchase distilled spirits by the drink in-
creased the consumption of distilled spirits and the frequency
Most studies to date have focused on small subsets of identi- of alcohol-related car accidents (Holder & Blose, 1987). How-
fiable risk factors for drug abuse. There is little evidence avail- ever, there was no increase in accidents involving males under
able regarding the relative importance and interactions of the legal drinking age of 21 (Blose & Holder, 1987).
various risk factors in the etiology of drug abuse, although Two general explanations of how laws affect the use of sub-
current studies are seeking to measure a broader range of iden- stances have been advanced. The first posits that laws reflect
tified risk factors. At this time, it is difficult to ascertain, for social norms and that use is largely a function of group norms
instance, which risk factors or combination of risk factors are (Watts & Rabow, 1983). Alcohol consumption rates vary among
most virulent, which are modifiable, and which are specific to (text continues on page 81)
66 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

rtumties
1~< IO
3 u

moking.
IH

8 -a *
•53
. - x
°-§ Is C T3 00 vf
r^
. ".
"r^
-~
O _
1. 1*
o 2P 3 e-8
B(S
a ^
O c 0
i s oo oort'ililp^ll
O 'w
« S > 3 o '*03 a0 uu t-H (4 ° 2 ^ en- o ^ c O <fi
O •"•
583 g> *P* ra
<g a a„
C ^ ^ r t - C i o W

S3
.CO *O
"*« e C <« 14
§ &3
•SS
'c &
i '^
2 BS
M 3(
U Sj -O
i- J2 u S^ S c-g
1 C
0
o*& -c'i-x •5 .1 'S oo a--e -S 7 1 S
o
If)
Srt •aO 8S Bo
o u S
«S)SSa"«>
... a ip ai ~ 0
o 0
1 M >>
>~tt"j B « c t a g g « " 3 ^ g - 5 e S
o B
O V
?u S-a T3^ co3|
'K J3 -G S
<" o o ~S
§| s § |.>' ^ S « g|'S P
w3
f 2g
B
zxx i
o co a <S
c 1i
o
1 J? U

c
0
•a doig r
t~ "3 ^
O
,., r-T 00
1 o\ "rl
zL C/3 r. QO Si"
J< OO
•*s
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

<=^^ .. C . OS 00 3 u (3
£ >, s^- r <N ta .* —. oo Q


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

c •o —MJ 00 C O O
3
55
o
x:
aoU
X a- -t G
,9 »<* a *e
o; g 0 a" -
« . SU ^*
<
oSj^"^
^* 0-.
E .-
s5?2 us^"
oo s2
oa * o~l
<a^" o 5S)i VIo S3 S'o ^ " 2 8 Sc<^ 00
>,SS S * J< 'C ° 2S 3 •* OO 00
o\ JK" if r o B
^2(2.5So |£«a2;H5 .e^
O J g « II
Q >-> CQ CU
^

W -a oo
c
rt .g
'K
(A § §^

i
B
C
0 O
VI
^|s° -^1 So
! 'S -a 3 -3 §*
-s|
OJ
S2-SS- !>•?
31
88
•I'a
a; £
aiililn II.
o2 uiHwSC 3X ^ S2^ -C^"3
«5
'S
1 tHISi
g .s 8 s g § T3
Q £
s
aa

c & fc 00
•^ "8 t S c •o S o rf § S3 M .S oo
•ao tf-e
£ a a-s
fc increase in tax on alcohol

o <*«o •£
ethnic and other group norm

alcohol among teens reportin


igher taxes associated with lo

Higher tax more salient tha


teen drinking and fatalities.
crease in alcohol tax led to sh
decrease in consumption and
•responding Prevention Fin<

[ore exposure to ads promot


ti g ou
.3 o c sociodemographic factors,
•§21-3 -sa
Evidence (findings)

§1:3 3§SB
«:is »is| S ° uU§42

higher drinking levels.


to 1/2% decrease in

° 2 ** S? r- t>
^0 S g= §ig
cirrhosis mortality,

S9a>-2 -315-S oo 3tsS^s «


,111-1 !|i?. to o g.,3
associated with

.S -o -a >• B «
consumption.

oj>,3 i.T>-p 5 u
acc3 S 3 S S ,2
fliil Hill ls!2-§8 3 J? 2 S H P
o £ ? la o >r

o C?" B a
lllll 111 11s-Ml'l
B 3
"B C T> 0 _0 5)
Z < 2
O
H
nee Abuse W

V~i
Grossman,

CIO
OS
liL- ^ .*
ical study

Tauchen,

3 00 > w.
Tf

2 c"
«j
8 jfig S
2 X! to
e5BS^
t^
- 85 00
T»- •* ~ °°
00 JJ 0

a So
1<*
Cfl
=3<N *r-
2.g[3j<
0
^j £ S°
"* i> -$* <N oo
o2
^8 S ^5>
IT- r- -0
•a
«
•2 wa OS
2| —tn —
«U- .sis
~
^ W Q1

_00

1 s ,2
r
IS
w OO
bS2
^
S ao g oo co
osOoso
q q « * ^ > —r w
^
<y oo 'S
2^5
o •—' ^
B
•S
.;5

§
2?° "SI e
^ Os

1"
oi>5
<
1
%
3 B
oo _ 00*0
fl-3

l13sa §i
^3 W1

^K. •« o
w jj
'S
^3 w
*
«,
Q
1-4

o
SB
•0 0
c •&
|« |S c1^3 -Sa
tS B a- 3
u 71
|j

2 M j^-,l|l
* *
2 .2
if
2 1J3 o | 3s a
•c 3 1,

1^ -S £. 2 s ~ S
£ a; ~"
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

t
a\ o\ o\
O\\O \O
OO OO OO
-2
I

<N
•a
q<

o
O
w

<
"o
"o

•o
•o
js

o
nforce drug and

i
to increases in d ig alcohol laws;

1|
prevalence, amo r alcohol resistance skills

|
consumed, heavj if alcohol. training

<;
*n
3
.variability affectec sf alcohol
and illegal drugs.

iQi

w
x
O

CO
C,
H

2
Jg
J3 C

«-8
O ^3
•g^

f .»-
8

•o
Hi
&
DM
i 0 0 CQ
•S 3 2
>.•«
O S S,

S3 .5 UTS o
8'i |g
tiiii

gf
O C

rtc - s c
O O
-81

Hi!!!
|.|

f- C S (3 5 C u

^•^
8J O J3 <u 5 'S »
J- O f 3
.
•g § 8 .1 1> «1

-I
.c :oo
w
xtreme poverty o

•32
S r~
factors increasin
alcohol and drug

g'S'Sa
•s-a g a
a-s-s'l
n! s

S«-8 3
who were antisoc
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS

st
c

1
67
68 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND i MILLER

0^
i—
I-* f

,cd *C
<~ c
•ss
B!
'C a»

0 jD

is
CO «

3
tc
W

B
O
S w-
u
& OJ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

T5 f*^
<U
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

S >, 3 V-,
"G
o o ,o
•o
,^ 'Eo >
bO
3 d>

1^ 3
UH
W5
c
o
"to
3 UJo •o
^
<U

C
to
VI «
a u
^o Q
c J .r t t > S G * - * * O
Q
e T 3 a C>c
S 'E.2 .•3 JGo 3 C e -^ a a
^
§S
"B.
B u .-a .a>u S^ u
rt ^
E? B
mobility, physical deterioration,
haracteristics of neighborhoods

low attachment, high crime are


related to juvenile crime and
such as population density,
childhood related to greater
ow socioeconomic status in
Evidence (findings)

drug use in adolescence.

C3~*--S *-— o o ^o J= « P. — uojra^co


lonoamine oxidase <
sation-seeking, earl;
/oidance predict earl

coholism linked to
sation seeking and

111 Illi II
111
drug trafficking.

coholism.

•rt S <u t*e!Tjnoi:>i;:5<L>J-wii'?fc*$L).e1-i'£o'2

j 0 o

u
,x >,
•o o
rn^
oo
Os
3
M •a oo<^
42
V~l
«*
p2
oc
<^ o a *s" *> °°


(5 3 Os c oo^ S.1 " 22
w ~^ >,O s e" oo g "
.s
_o
"e9
5
g =3
3 c
u> i 'He s-s — PH
-2
*5 •r;
aj
*~^ Cu oo
fc
u 00 P
S
X o\ s U- CS3
^
O
w s
«
<
O N «

c
1c
u.
1 c5
N
0 'S
o *e3
•Sa •-e
cd

..
o
,3, *n
^
03
X3 C3
J^ 2?
So
.s3 s-g5
2 "C M
o>
3
z •3
^f-'
(2
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 69
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Marsh &
umpfer,

-ilif!
u 8 .-3 C -7! S
00 e J3 « £ S5
JJ.S o 0.-S 3

a-ltff
•Ssl*
c a o <=
a s J3 o c•
— (•-. O -rt 4>
™ ° -a tJ
~ u
.S .-3-a
a

1-s.s-s

•<r
v~i - „ oo
id^c-o »
c
, 9

S ca •" .r
79;

w, u C ^ oo H
r~ _- i_ j3 "" r> o
2||5S2
I
a
5 •* — 9 Soji'S
•of- - ^ ^ o o t i ^
oo —
o—
O
5 r " ?—
tofflU o rO
S§o
O
u
>.
"5
S
S
•8
< «
70 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

i| f-s_ £ eg H

component produced positive

arent skills training improved

with lowered rates of tobacco,


arly childhood interventions

program package associated


outcomesforhigh-risk, low-

alcohol, and marijuana use.


family interaction, reduced
including a parenting skills

Parenting component not


Effects onriskfactor or
i^Il !iln illl §1

child problem behaviors.

homework assignments.
use/abuse (findings)

assessed separately, but


fill 111! h
fit

income children.
•o o B « 9 .j- g C — •ioS-?-o5£°° —
l-g.23 oo^S^o.2 £,§«T=S,8t;S
08 eg -S e •> !• S 8 .S « o 52, s St 8 3
S^^-S SK.S3.3S S'SJ^cS'o'e
X
g S3 5 <u
£ .2.-O S
K ta.3
a u .-aS .0
u ta; u
c ^- -SaoT ^3 u^ 2^ oi .-<M2
u cu U. EU (X. 0-

B
^0
_T oo r- ^S-o
c> 29 •« -a- 1 ^
S oo _
u (N~ rt i t~- ^,
H oo „ _ « — '"•> .<« 00 cK <^ 2^1J« ~ S
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2
ON ^ O ID ed
"-73 g 2: t~ —
ON "
, *s
C r« rii
_"
Cfl *~
£ - 5— -
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

u
B •§" ^ ^j *J H U ^i *-* —•' rtS^'O
^^
73 " '§.
~ MCOX a oo ? "(g'c
§ e S
m ^« e,» «£ ^c jei jl- aga l. H>• xs
u g-
^ 1«-K
I-. S oo OO aj
g T^ S fs" •§
oo h oo i-* ^g5 % 8 S 5«r- & I -<»>
SiS^^n
2J M-< *-* *-* *uJ
0 g<* 0
- S"-11" _g|3
u&<« Ili2*
»j a, u, d, — — g1 I?
g—
3 (X <C CU H O .

&
v>
C
*- c
~> >eS S.
§
O

1
"S.
S So
jrt GO *J
•2 f«
r-i C ^-"
7^ e^ -T-
"-1 «m "^g-^
H

_ *-
*^J u
ga S u
u c *; "3 1 2
"2>
•S
> M
S « S
'>_o«s
e 3
=g -a
'^5 s § 1c *o «-
9 <*• | 13 t, "" § oS
S Ǥ c
'a S 1? ° ^ m 00
0.73 2'*5 !i* 11 1 -sl sll 5 g t gi-ti
ithers' nondrug use, e:

(«« W (5 UK t-i Jj 4J oj CO 'C P ^ C U


discipline, low parent

permissiveness by the

'_S
stability enhanced effei

;rceived parent permii


Evidence (finding

educational aspiratior
toward drug, alcohol u

verinvolvement by on
adolescent drug, alcoh

children predict initia


important than actual

iLl !!sf!5 lit Hi!


accompanied by dista
ack of or inconsistent

associated with risk.

1c
nonuse of drugs.

i SSil
Iiilll§ilil5||.^i ll|i
drug use.

£ 0, J O
llilliililllhli
( S o Q
11 ai
o-

° =8
oo f2 t3-<y
,0 «>
4 >, o •*" <at OO ^
"A 00 .** OS o u C oo £* ^\
__r ed t~- C
1 CJ, _^ ifi rn o

pi
C — 13 u \o
i<iis oo .«
- -0 'g .S (N
2 oo 73
S^
O O
ON
1 J§
I
73 S t>^(«ft. „-
•-«!
9fc£^ i 1 Is ?2
u
1 * §-.
(-1 . - • *• 4>
-Sf ^
Q >j* I Sss
Q5S o
o O
S
4> r^ **^ c
to JS on oc C

S oo
o 3- —> « g ' g ^S^ •&23 2
S
-i
oi
°^
z
8
H
ffl S ^ rt — — « N
^

1 (-
o S
•S
•*^j •^J
U
S as
K (£2 >» Si 8
8
•^2- y; ."73 W "13

1 8 g§
<U H, a &
r-^
1
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.X
a
8
CTs

OO
1-
si
a

•8

>>

1
o -d f a
Ills

.-•g
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS

B
II
•§
71
72 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

G 3
O f,

jj' oo "O r
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

^ e _ C M

tsj . 2'88S

«^^«§ 3'i3 l'S-3


Slllllllll

w-»
oo
3
V*
CTJ
G "£
ca W
r^
i
^f) o
ii" -o i- S
.0 S
U
T3 3 iu &n j2 > <r>
4J 3 O J3 a £
0 'C j|
4~>
C
fll

T3
O
a •o
g if
§>
i/S ^^
>1 t3 3
U "3 's JS
2
CU
crj
U
>>
year-olds icontribute
•Difficult" t emperami
problems iin adultho

emotiona

CO

c 5 ^> (U ^
CD
3
CT

_C
C/5
1/5
"H<u ^ a
?•s'i
3
&s
X5
t«-<
O 15
O .> 0

OQ""
!# §>T3
drug probl
Aggressive m

problem a
predicts ft

maintaine
childhood,

delinquen

2 O
•f
o 1
E

1
O
"^ 3
u.
ett
to
z f>
>t
- >>
00
1 "3
/~\

CQ
Will
fi 5^ «•§ >
S
o
</i
S
^ 9 _*?•
<« <«. ^
-c "T-'/!"', °°
s f s » K t J \ o o r - i Jr;f^
o - S o o r - o o o o J>oo
r
£2 cS — "iS a N O v a s ( > 'S ! ''
a J3
t/5

£'
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 73

•Si
s
a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

« B

S? "O i_ t«
> j- u J3

ilil

s.1
!«1!
•fa u * .S 2

c ^
U ^ tr o
' E T> s "5 n. v« '" J2B
T3 to S 3
S'-a
u
^11.1111 In
X K

B
O
VI
•O o\
I
sj-o^jS • C
cd •a
"log., s
h U o f> <N
SJ o b oo oo
|SS22 lio I
.s
u
E
1
s

a
18
s «"
74 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

«" •a
c Sc~ c
co— J5 <_r -a
t. cu a
a> i/, u
0 ^_,

«c
~ 8 .*
S | •sg.§|7
sE^05^
use/abuse (findi
Effects on risk fac
S

3 a " « J8
3 £ 3 5 c .>
{ III!, ifl! fi ifll! ilill Lsfti
-.2So^s- aP' S"" s 0 - e o o S a o g g g ~~ > g-S S S .H, . - a C T 3 o S t i " g - „ c g
~ '3 '« § s -"• I ^P.H! -sjN §§. ^Si--g^o1|l-s-s^l§l-s-s
•s" •£•§>"'--
e «$ « gS-P1gso°|-gs fc8 |||-S« « ° J ^ § S i-2| 8 S ' g " i | 8 g | | « ' g § g S
^ 1 2 s "S § S •§ ° £ |
1 g « s a ^*I1-|*
=§ si; s^ >^«>os
111 1 1 -!s||^ll|fll-2!ll|1-vl ffl|liisi?lI|i|iS:iil
flstl
w
ilaill
o^ <%
lil
s
ililll
o
lllll
r
lll
H
illlll
o
llllll
o
llilll
a
.0 *-» ,x r -r
u
c «gS13 &
ist>M's;J_*j
.«*) ON g
D 03 - «r c oo *° - 5S
§ . SK§ < 5 * £ -^* "8 |.S oo lg.--2S S -r
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

l^ ^|§ «g a
V
Jj •* .0° — 2 S °° • •" -°
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

X
B T3
r i ^ r u -2
iSr^^^i •« ^ g*2
vi •_: »•
«» -3 S 2 ^
Ki ^ ~^
_r
-*^ cd
-g

Sg
,ir
«
•*-»
55^

^Ii r.*l|* t °°g .s~j<H S»." •*M S.. c,S3K- tsc 2tS 5* 2 ts
oo
2
|2 _ ~ -aSS"Jsg
r
f_ 2oOv-T"5° 'ON 1
*S
O.OV
•§£s.§ .goo-^-a^a -^s
u
S3" S
c ^ s u —. —. 1 3 _ u —. — o —. t
5" 1J^
-Q - g>
- c^«-
N^ n^«
]Q -3-" gB g«
02 03 CQ X Q <8 U 0 U.

•- ><
en E.S «
c <U -t^
^
^o "O
rt o o
C t4-H

ts
_o 8
««
"S. uw
X > .2
C
e
^ H !'•§ ^
O ea «
£

, i ISI 1|
s « ° «a
;e (findings)

s 8

H! Ij II
3g^ f| «glB
u
g l^all ^1
."2
'> clltlS |ll
w •"«4>'°ct>
ojj^^-^cd^a
'oJ=C
*^.—
w 3 o-'o .^U -o 13 c
3"rt^'J3^tH X-^r-
= 1«fc3-gD. g«.S
£ w o

x •S S3
•3s •a 'g •- - "
5 tn <^-g *
vfa-22 s
i
3
1 ^
o\ e£ («"
_
f=
^*
>^
73
iS
a IP
;-!•!
i* i r
N-»
2 is
UH i-In

f 1
3
g; o
ts **3
S
O <2 "o
•S ^4 o
VI

U
2 1
1
S
1
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 75

•is •§*
11
x
o
a
o. •Si

f 111 S-M^-O «

jim
JlfP
| |I o
8
?! 2
S Kg
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

I
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.


Er:<
ti*rftftfg-
|2
Ig'S^ll
!>25a|a
CO

03 Q X

111
III
CH

73
8K o >
2,-a«-
JS g ° S 3
~ -o 5 5 -o
2<s
c °a «"
§ "« «5
•8-3-Si's
|S§°^
•S2S.2I

|"S 8 8.8-B
a P

C
<L>

1
S "3
o o

3s
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

76

t2

4>
•S

£
3
1
1
j>

*^
a>
c

C
o
c

^
3
Effects onriskfactor or

*t3


55
B

o
c

"ft

«3
S

•>U
C
c
'•c3
use/abuse (findings)

"rt
O
T3
>,

to

UH
"o
¥

O
•ao
a

^
<S
3

O
>.

5
!
y
C
£i
°O

Q
O

r;
urriculum restructuring

<£x
ij O\
"Svg
increased positive self-

S ,

w -^ ^o
V-i' "^
n °°
c32
3I-*
concept, attachment to
school, belief in rules, ma
£«

scores; decreased school rat


of delinquency, drug use,
suspensions,

C7\
(W

C4
OO
oo

u
•at-
C
C
T3
<
ihool transition study (see

S
^>
'oj1
1
*
—• o

ffl <»
<u

U-c <«

BJ S
Si
« t-

o J2

.•31

oa

a. T3
'S S-

sl
O ou
fl S
.-S|S.2 143
'I & § § 81
* o.^. * -e
o

•a

! ~ <*>22

Ov
•o
00

CQ
flu
r- 05
D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

"> E tL, 0.^2 £ <2


•g a ~ <u o o ?
g ft
I « 8 -S -S =3
a=|s:l§'§ 1

0
«§•!
. c?
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.


at

00
eviews of social influence

ra
—' •

< T3
Sg
S
<u
CA

«"§
3 %
£

O 00
«C
resistance strate

vo
a K fa

o§I

.. «-e
g

|«l
3
.B « -

•§is
a a«
c

S o g>
found modest b ut signifies

« o" ^>,
1^1
«_»
reductions in the: onset and
o>

00 -tJ "^
CT\ •- ^ £
— o i-T
ia
prevalence of ci
c
00

smoking for groiups receivi

M cd *•
1) fll ^ '
ill
(-1

training in complarison will

-_;_;.-:
t/i

untrained contrc
&

,
jcial influence si
u t/5
c

2-3
I*

. r.
showed benefici

^i E
O —' —•
preventing or delaying the

- -
.^ O *0 —
•o
g

onset of alcohol
marijuana use.

« £>~

K
cd
I
o\

u
ON

o
"3

S
CO
o
S
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS

1
I
I
77
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

78

ea

F
I
""^
c:
c:
&c
C
0

£
W
s onriskfactor or

w
'>
••3
tC
C3

T3

w
.9

_o
8
^0
C

a
S
•o

3
o y

^
3

s
^ 3

~B.

•a
a
^
abuse (findings)

|
s

W
«
c
s*
•*
$

"o o^
S ~^
So
D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

•^

c
O

c/5
o
'>
3

J< —
oa oo
-°°

S "
llSllllllilillllllilljtiiHIIllllI
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

if,

2
^
Ci
"Q tf
lif-l
- 2 ON
« —• £>oo

2Sl3
oo 3 "~1
3 ON .^ ON
*.«
_-s
« sa
s
•a 62
C^
«S
ca ~
C ON
U —.
C *.

V^
^

<a
.jj
^
o. E
o E

11
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS

_u
2
o

•S'§s
.i g

2 S"«
u j- 3
O « 3

. 3 00
f K ?

Jin mi
79
80 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANQ AND J. MILLER

i~ ed
u >- f-j
10
o 2 ^H
§<2*
o r- -t-* °fi

ll
£•§
»2 3 _ 1 S c
3'o'c S
iS
vi
. C3 C

S g *§;'2
0> *3
_^ CC!

ll
0,0
_. « o c 2 ^ c —
-a — a <u
G « cO >
O t£
^ fc
.§3«s
W> H
ft>
Uls
& 2 ex -2

111 I•s "S £ »


tn -^
t2 4*
& % 2
№ ^? °- h
w 05
M O S ~ o. B
|2 g § 0> w 3K .S
S ea (-

c
o
•&
c
0
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

o
>,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Jj •a
3
Sri

& c
3 o
C
Ul OD "O S •« J2
O g g
> « §g
1
"a
.•s <«
•^
g-o
rt
(U
3 «
g
iǤ|
e 3£ a
- §."« 'C•- «.
^
CA

g Sii t> >•« g>


o> o X! _C
g ffl C 2: 2 t> -S S
X Q
>egin drinking

:s greater and

oc oo r~ 2
jarlier onset

g 2 g. <a .2 D
;e of more

2
8- S
-a
5
^g
«S
ug
32 i 1
1 Iff? 21 si
"E'SI'S -o s g1!
•s|?| ss is Z. •- ° 3
o e^ - a
11
3
^^
I
¥
c
o 82 1 ? g
°«&! ii si
"8 o^
j- U U fi
O S k «
" eS * •o
«'!/> 11
So.
c -« c w^ c J^ " S
2oi-i£ S ^ o oc' ' " ^ 'oS 3 g 3
•« 8 8 8 * o-S
1 2« a-o § ~s.s- Sg u« Sr 2?°- S
s £ ^e . °R-i -
•al§lSl&S:|g..s §••§•§§ rt
.i 2 o s
1 T3
i£ £ £ s w

^4
tfs
>*
, ^i ^r
S^
s ^ ^ -s« r s */%
s <«
gical stud

OO
(5 S
Sg° °c<i S 8 -S5 ON

gig— _ ^ W
OQ _:<$J.t;oo r2
s ^ g t " - •a «'l ^4
J**^ « »|la *j _- °°(2
_o ^r- vo "S 1
•8
feo 13 §<N 05
0 <1J C

3
w s^Bl la llalllli£
S, 5 US « US

OD
'— • 5
"^ •o o
i_t
•S o 2
SJi
sC flj
c t£3 •nS o 2
o 3 S .* 3
it- •« 3 fe .& oo
2 — > 0 S3 S
V <l 1 (U T3

1 vd t--'
F •~^
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 81

different ethnic groups—for example, in association with dif- with childhood behavior problems has it been shown to in-
ferences in the extent to which members find consumption crease risk for later alcoholism and drug problems.
socially acceptable (Flasher & Maisto, 1984; Vaillant, 1983). 4. Neighborhood disorganization. Neighborhoods with high
The second view of the law's effect focuses on supply and population density, lack of natural surveillance of public places
demand. As noted above, legal restrictions that influence the (C. A. Murray, 1983), high residential mobility, physical deterio-
availability or price of alcohol or other drugs, such as taxation ration, low levels of attachment to neighborhood (Herting &
or laws regarding sales, appear to limit consumption. Guest, 1985), and high rates of adult crime also have high rates
Legal restrictions on the purchase of alcohol and norms unfa- of juvenile crime (Wilson & Herrnstein, 1985) and illegal drug
vorable toward alcohol use clearly are associated with a lower trafficking (Fagan, 1988). Simcha-Fagan and Schwartz (1986)
prevalence of alcohol abuse. Conversely, laws and norms that assessed the contextual effects of neighborhood on delinquency
express greater tolerance for the use of alcohol are associated and found that community economic level and community dis-
with a greater prevalence of alcohol abuse. Johnston (1991) has order-criminal subculture were significantly related to offi-
suggested a similar relationship between norms regarding ille- cially recorded delinquency.
gal drugs and the prevalence of illegal drug abuse. When neighborhoods undergo rapid population changes,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2. Availability. The availability of drugs is dependent in victimization rates increase, even after accounting for race and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

part on the laws and norms of society. Nevertheless, availability age differences (Sampson, 1986; Sampson, Castellano, & Laub,
is a separable factor. Whether or not particular substances are 1981). Neighborhood disorganization has been hypothesized to
legal, their availability may vary and is associated with use. contribute to a deterioration in the ability of families to trans-
Research has shown that when alcohol is more available, the mit prosocial values to children (W McCord & McCord, 1959;
prevalence of drinking, the amount of alcohol consumed, and Reiss, 1986; Shaw & McKay, 1969). Although few studies of
the heavy use of alcohol all increase (Gorsuch & Butler, 1976). neighborhood disorganization have explicitly examined its re-
With regard to illegal drugs, Maddahian, Newcomb, and lationship with drug abuse, a deterioration in parental sociali-
Bentler (1988) found in an adolescent sample that two measures zation and supervision associated with neighborhood disor-
of drug availability were significantly related to the use of ciga- ganization could also be expected to produce high rates of drug
rettes, alcohol, marijuana, and other illegal drugs, even after involvement. More research is required to determine the effects
controlling for the amount of money available to the subjects. of neighborhood disorganization on adolescent drug abuse.
Dembo, Farrow, Schmeidler, and Burgos (1979) reported that
the availability of drugs affected substance use indirectly
among junior high school youths. G. D. Gottfredson (1988) Individual and Interpersonal Factors
found that drug availability varied in different schools and that
drug availability influenced the use of drugs beyond the influ- Certain characteristics of individuals and of their personal
ence of individual characteristics of subjects. environments are associated with a greater risk of adolescent
3. Extreme economic deprivation. Indicators of socioeco- drug abuse. These characteristics are summarized below as
nomic disadvantage, such as poverty, overcrowding, and poor Risk Factors 5 through 17.
housing, have been shown to be associated with an increased 5. Physiological factors. Sensation seeking and low harm
risk of childhood conduct problems and delinquency (Bursik & avoidance predict early-onset alcoholism (Cloninger et al.,
Webb, 1982; Farrington et al., 1990). However, research on so- 1988). Poor impulse control in childhood predicts frequent
cial class and drug use has not always confirmed popular stereo- marijuana use at age 18 (Shedler & Block, 1990). Zuckerman
types. A slight positive correlation between parental education (1987) has suggested that sensation seeking is linked biochemi-
and high school seniors' marijuana use has been reported cally to platelet monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity, which has
(Bachman, Lloyd, & O'Malley, 1981). R. A. Zucker and Harford also been found to be associated with early-onset alcoholism
(1983) found that parental occupational prestige and education (Tabakoff& Hoffman, 1988; von Knorring, Oreland, & von
were positively related to teenage drinking. D. M. Murray, Knorring, 1987).
Richards, Luepker, and Johnson (1987) found that mother's The enzyme aldehyde dehydrogenase (ALDH), important in
occupation was positively correlated with monthly alcohol use, the decomposition of ethanol in the body (Li, 1977), has also
heavy alcohol use, and marijuana use among seventh-grade stu- been linked to alcoholism. Asians without one ALDH enzyme
dents. The 1988 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse drink less and have lower rates of alcoholism than controls
revealed significantly higher lifetime prevalence rates for mari- (Harada, Agarwal, Goedde, & Ishikawa, 1983; Schuckit, 1987;
juana use among those with some college education as com- Suwaki & Ohara, 1985).
pared with those who had less than a high school education Researchers have also studied differences in genetically me-
(Adams et al., 1990). In contrast, Robins and Ratcliff (1979) diated biological responses to alcohol among children of alco-
found that extreme poverty, though not lower-class status per holics and nonalcoholics. Pollock, Volavka, and Goodwin
se, was one of three factors that increased the risk of adult (1983) reported more slow-wave activity on the electroencepha-
antisocial behavior, including alcoholism and illegal drug use, logram (EEG) for children of alcoholics compared with chil-
among children who were highly antisocial in childhood. dren of nonalcoholics. Schuckit, Parker, and Rossman (1983)
In summary, whereas there appears to be a negative relation- found differences in children of alcoholics and children of non-
ship between socioeconomic status and delinquency, a similar alcoholics in serum prolactin response to administration of al-
relationship has not been found for the use of drugs by adoles- cohol. Schuckit (1980) reported greater muscle relaxation in
cents. Only when poverty is extreme and occurs in conjunction response to ethanol, and Schuckit and Rayes (1979) found in-
82 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

creased levels of acetaldehyde after administration of alcohol in dren. Parental drug use is associated with initiation of use by
sons of alcoholics when compared with sons of nonalcoholics. adolescents (G. M. Johnson et al., 1984; Kandel, Kessler, &
Researchers have sought to assess the independent contribu- Margulies, 1978; McDermott, 1984) and with frequency of
tion of genetic factors to the development of alcoholism marijuana use (Brook et al., 1990). Similar findings have been
through twin and adoption studies. Kaij (1960) and Hrubec reported for adolescent drinking habits (Rachal et al., 1982;
and Omenn (1981) found that among males, monozygotic R. A. Zucker, 1979). G. M. Johnson et al. (1984) found that
twins were more than twice as likely as dizygotic twins to be parental use of marijuana was associated with adolescents' use
concordant for alcoholism, although in a study of both females of other illegal drugs, including cocaine and barbiturates.
and males, Curling, Clifford, and Murray (1981) reported con- Ahmed, Bush, Davidson, and lannotti (1984) examined the
cordance rates for alcoholism of 21% in monozygotic and 25% effects of parental modeling of drug use on children's expecta-
in dizygotic twins. tions to use drugs and on their drug use. In a study of 420
Adoption studies in Denmark, Sweden, and the United children in grades kindergarten (K) to 6, they found "salience,"
States have provided more consistent evidence for genetic a measure of the number of household users of a drug and the
transmission of alcoholism in males, reporting rates of alcohol- degree of children's involvement in parental drug-taking behav-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ism ranging from 18% to 27% for the adopted sons of alcoholics ior, to be the best predictor of both expectations to use and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

compared with only 5% to 6% for adopted males without a actual use of alcohol. Salience was also a predictor of children's
biological alcoholic parent (Bohman, 1978; Cadoret, Cain, & cigarette and marijuana use. The importance of number of
Grove, 1980; Cadoret & Gath, 1978; Goodwin et al., 1974; household users varied across substance. As the number of fam-
Goodwin, Schulsinger, Moller, Mednick, & Guze, 1977). No ily members who used alcohol or marijuana increased, so did
consistent evidence for genetic transmission of alcoholism in the probability that the child used or expected to use these
females has been reported (R. M. Murray & Stabenau, 1982). substances. For cigarette smoking, having one household
Note that the adoption studies suggesting a genetic factor in member who smoked cigarettes almost doubled the probability
male alcoholism also reveal that less than 30% of the sons of that a child smoked or expected to smoke, but additional
alcoholics themselves become alcoholic. Furthermore, about smokers in the home did not increase this probability further.
half of hospitalized alcoholics do not have a family history of Note that Hansen et al.'s (1987) structural equation modeling
alcoholism (Goodwin, 1985), suggesting that factors other than analyses using cross-sectional data indicated indirect, but not
genetic predisposition also contribute to alcoholism. direct, effects of parental modeling of drug use on children's
It is beyond the scope of this article to review thoroughly the drug use in early adolescence. Parental modeling was directly
recent developments in this area of alcoholism studies. Re- related to friends' use of drugs, which, in turn, was related to
search continues to point toward differences in physiological subjects' drug use. This finding is consistent with the findings
responses to ethanol among sons of alcoholics (Schuckit, 1987) of Brook et al. (1990), whose combined longitudinal and cross-
and to other possible genetic and biochemical "markers" of risk sectional studies revealed that nondrug use and emotional sta-
for alcoholism (K. Blum et al., 1990; TabakofF & Hoffman, bility in fathers enhanced the effect of peer nonuse of drugs and
1988). Early-onset alcoholism that is associated with impulsi- that psychological stability in mothers offset the effects of peer
vity and aggression apparently has a partial foundation in indi- drug use.
vidual physiological characteristics. Brook, Whiteman, Gordon, and Brook (1988) examined the
Little research has been conducted on genetic predisposition role of older brothers in younger brothers' drug use and found
and the abuse of drugs other than alcohol in humans, although that older brothers' advocacy of drugs and modeling of drug use
there is evidence from animal studies of a heritability in predis- were both associated with younger brothers' use. They also ob-
position to barbiturate and morphine abuse (Marley, Miner, served an interaction pattern in which some of the negative
Wehner, & Collins, 1986). effects of parental drug use were offset by the older brothers'
6. Family alcohol and drug behavior and attitudes. Families nonuse. Older brothers' and peers' drug modeling both were
affect children's drug use behaviors in a number of ways. more strongly associated with younger brothers' use than was
Beyond the genetic transmission of a propensity to alcoholism parental modeling of drug use.
in males, family modeling of drug using behavior and parental McDermott's (1984) research indicated that although paren-
attitudes toward children's drug use are family influences re- tal drug use and adolescent drug use are related, suggesting the
lated specifically to the risk of alcohol and other drug abuse. modeling effect discussed above, permissive parental attitudes
Poor parenting practices, high levels of conflict in the family, toward drug use as perceived by youths may be of equal or
and a low degree of bonding between children and parents greater importance than actual parental drug use in determin-
appear to increase risk for adolescent problem behaviors gener- ing the adolescent's use of drugs. This finding is consistent with
ally, including the abuse of alcohol and other drugs (Brook, Hansen et al.'s (1987) results. Similarly, Barnes and Welte (1986)
Brook, Gordon, Whiteman, & Cohen, 1990). In this section, found that parental approval of drinking was a significant pre-
the family risk factors specific to alcohol and other drug abuse dictor of the amount of alcohol consumed by teenage drinkers,
are reviewed. Family factors more generally predictive of adoles- and Brook, Gordon, Whiteman, and Cohen (1986) found that
cent problem behaviors are reviewed in subsequent sections. parental tolerance of drug use predicted adolescent drug use.
Parental and sibling alcoholism (Cloninger, Bohman, Sig- This relationship has been shown for Whites, Hispanics, Afri-
vardsson, & von Knorring, 1985; Cotton, 1979; Goodwin, can Americans, Native Americans and Asian Americans (Jes-
1985) and illegal drug use (G. M. Johnson, Schoutz, & Locke, sor, Donovan, & Windmer, 1980).
1984) increase the risk of alcoholism and drug abuse in chil- 7. Poor and inconsistent family management practices.
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 83

Kandel and Andrews (1987) found that lack of maternal involve- West, 1985; McCord, 1979; Rutter & Ciller, 1983). Rutter and
ment in activities with children; lack of, or inconsistent, paren- Giller have noted that parental conflict is associated with anti-
tal discipline (see also Baumrind, 1983; Penning & Barnes, social behavior in children even when the home is unbroken
1982); and low parental educational aspirations for their chil- (see also W McCord & McCord, 1959; Porter & O'Leary, 1980)
dren predict initiation of drug use. Stanton (1979), Kaufman and that even in samples in which all homes are broken, the
and Kaufman (1979), and Ziegler-Driscoll (1979) suggested extent of family conflict is associated with the likelihood of
that familial risk factors include a pattern of overinvolvement antisocial behavior in the children (see also Hetherington, Cox,
by one parent and distance or permissiveness by the other. & Cox, 1979; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980). Similarly, Simcha-Fa-
Differences between the effects of mothers' and fathers' dis- gan, Gersten, and Langner (1986) found that the use of heroin
ciplinary techniques were observed by Brook et al. (1990). Ma- and other illegal drugs was strongly associated with parental
ternal control techniques were more important than paternal marital discord. In summary, children raised in families high
techniques in explaining adolescent marijuana use. Specifi- in conflict appear at risk for both delinquency and illegal
cally, mothers' control patterns that included setting clear re- drug use.
quirements for responsible behavior led to less marijuana use, 9. Low bonding to family. Parent-child interactions charac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and mothers' use of guilt to control was correlated with greater terized by lack of closeness (Brook, Lukoff, & Whiteman, 1980;
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

drug use. Kandel et al., 1978) and lack of maternal involvement in activi-
Baumrind (1983) classified parenting styles as authoritative, ties with children (Braucht, Kirby, & Berry, 1978; Penning &
authoritarian, or permissive and found that children who were Barnes, 1982) appear to be related to initiation of drug use.
highly prosocial and assertive generally came from authorita- Conversely, positive family relationships—involvement and at-
tive families. She found that parental nondirectiveness or per- tachment—appear to discourage youths' initiation into drug
missiveness contributed to higher levels of drug use. Reilly use (Brook et al., 1986; Gorsuch & Butler, 1976; Jessor & Jessor,
(1979) found that common characteristics of families with ado- 1977; Kim, 1979; Norem-Hebeisen et al., 1984; Selnow, 1987).
lescent drug abusers included negative communication pat- Hundleby and Mercer (1987) found that adolescents' reports of
terns (criticism, blaming, lack of praise), inconsistent and un- parental trust, warmth, and involvement explained small por-
clear behavioral limits, and unrealistic parental expectations of tions of the variance in the extent of tobacco, alcohol, and mari-
children. juana use.
Shedler and Block (1990) found that the quality of mothers'
Bonding to family may inhibit drug involvement during ado-
interaction with their children at age 5 distinguished children
lescence in a manner similar to the way in which family bond-
who were frequent users of marijuana at age 18 from those who
ing inhibits delinquency (Hirschi, 1969). Brook et al. (1990)
had only experimented with marijuana use. Mothers of chil-
pointed to the salience of parent-child attachment in describ-
dren who became frequent users were relatively cold, underre-
sponsive, and underprotective with their children, giving their ing the pathways to marijuana use frequency in their combined
children little encouragement but pressuring them to perform longitudinal and cross-sectional studies. They reported a
in tasks. causal pathway in which parental internalization of traditional
Norem-Hebeisen, Johnson, Anderson, and Johnson (1984) values led to the development of strong parent-child attach-
also found that the quality of adolescents' relationships with ment; this mutual attachment led to the child's internalization
their parents was related to patterns of drug use. Generally, of traditional norms and behavior, which in turn led the young-
drug users perceived their fathers as more hostile, rejecting, and ster to associate with non-drug-using peers, which led to
adversarial than did nonusers. nonuse.
The evidence suggests an independent contribution of family 10. Early and persistent problem behaviors. The greater the
interactions to adolescent drug use, separate from the effects of variety, frequency, and seriousness of childhood antisocial be-
parental drug use. Tec (1974) found that parental drug use in a havior, the more likely antisocial behavior is to persist into
rewarding family structure only slightly promoted frequent adulthood (Robins, 1978).
marijuana use but that in a unrewarding context, there was a A longitudinal study of 5-year-olds followed into adulthood
clear association between levels of drug use by parents and their (Lerner & Vicary, 1984) found that a difficult temperament,
children. including frequent negative mood states and withdrawal, con-
In summary, the risk of drug abuse appears to be increased tributes to drug problems. Children characterized by with-
by family management practices characterized by unclear ex- drawal responses to new stimuli, biological irregularity, slow
pectations for behavior, poor monitoring of behavior, few and adaptability to change, frequent negative mood expressions,
inconsistent rewards for positive behavior, and excessively se- and high intensity of positive and negative expressions of affect
vere and inconsistent punishment for unwanted behavior. more often became regular users of alcohol, tobacco, and mari-
8. Family conflict. Although children from homes broken juana in adulthood than "easy" children, who evidenced
by marital discord are at higher risk of delinquency and drug greater adaptability and positive affect early in life. Similarly,
use (Baumrind, 1983; Penning & Barnes, 1982; Robins, 1980), Shedler and Block (1990) found that frequent marijuana users
there does not appear to be a direct independent contribution at age 18 were characterized in childhood by emotional dis-
of "broken homes" to delinquent behavior (Wilson & Herrn- tress. Lerner and Vicary (1984) suggested that the negative
stein, 1985). Conflict among family members appears more mood and withdrawal responses of the difficult child may be
important in the prediction of delinquency than does family analogous to the depression and social alienation frequently
structure per se (Farrington, Gallagher, Morley, Ledger, & reported for drug abusers (Knight, Sheposh, & Bryson, 1974;
84 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

Paton & Kandel, 1978; Paton, Kessler, & Kandel, 1977; Smith & and levels of use of illegal drugs (Smith & Fogg, 1978). Holm-
Fogg, 1978). berg (1985), in a longitudinal study of 15-year-olds, reported
Brook et al. (1990) found that children who were irritable, that truancy, placement in a special class, and early drop out
easily distractible, had temper tantrums, fought often with sib- from school were prognostic factors for drug abuse. 1 n contrast,
lings, and engaged in predelinquent behavior were more likely outstanding performance in school reduced the likelihood of
to use drugs in adolescence. frequent drug use among a ninth-grade sample studied by
Aggressive behavior in boys appears to signal another path Hundleby and Mercer (1987).
toward later antisocial behavior. Aggressiveness in boys as early What is not clear from the existing research is when, develop-
as ages 5-7 (Grades K-2) has been found to predict later antiso- mentally, poor school achievement becomes a stable predictor
cial behavior including frequent drug use in adolescence (Kel- of drug abuse. The available evidence suggests that social ad-
lam & Brown, 1982), drug problems in adulthood (Lewis, justment is more important than academic performance in the
Robins, & Rice, 1985; Nylander, 1979), and delinquency in ado- early elementary grades in predicting later drug abuse. Early
lescence (Loeber, 1988; Spivack, 1983). However, early aggres- antisocial behavior in school may predict both academic failure
siveness is not invariably followed by serious antisocial behav- in later grades (Feldhusen, Thurston, & Benning, 1973) and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ior. Approximately 30% to 40% of the boys engaged in maladap- later drug abuse. Academic failure in late elementary grades
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive, aggressive behaviors continue that behavior 4 to 9 years may exacerbate the effects of early antisocial behavior or con-
later (Loeber & Dishion, 1983). tribute independently to drug abuse.
Few youths develop highly physically aggressive behaviors in 12. Low degree of commitment to school. A low degree of
late childhood or adolescence if not engaged in such behaviors commitment to education also appears to be related to adoles-
in earlier childhood, and most boys grow out of early aggressive cent drug use. Annual surveys of high school seniors by John-
behaviors. However, if aggressive behavior continues into early ston, O'Malley, and Bachman (1985) show that the use of hallu-
adolescence (age 13), it is a relatively strong predictor of contin- cinogens, cocaine, heroin, stimulants, sedatives, or nonmedi-
ued aggressive behavior in late adolescence as well as of later cally prescribed tranquilizers is significantly lower among
alcoholism (Loeber, 1988; McCord, 1981). Furthermore, if anti- students who expect to attend college than among those who do
social behavior persists and becomes more varied in early ado- not plan to go on to college. G. D. Gottfredson (1988) found that
lescence to include fighting and school misbehavior, drug abuse truancy for both boys and girls was associated with drug involve-
is more likely (Barnes & Welte, 1986; Kandel, 1982). Barnes and ment, after accounting for effects of ethnicity, parental educa-
Welte found that school misconduct was one of the three most tion, and delinquency. Factors such as how much students like
important predictors of alcohol-related problems in a study of school (Kelly & Balch, 1971), time spent on homework, and
subjects from six ethnic groups in Grades 7-12. perception of the relevance of course work are also related to
Hyperactivity and attention-deficit disorders have been levels of drug use (Friedman, 1983), indicating a negative rela-
shown to increase risk for delinquency when combined with tionship between commitment to education and frequent drug
conduct problems including aggression (Loney, Kramer, & Mi- use among junior and senior high school students.
lich, 1979). Gittelman, Mannuzza, and Bonagura (1985) found 13. Peer rejection in elementary grades. Although it would
a higher prevalence of substance abuse disorders in late adoles- be premature to posit a direct link between peer rejection and
cence among subjects diagnosed as hyperactive in childhood. substance abuse, low acceptance by peers seems to put an ado-
As with delinquency, those at highest risk were those with both lescent at risk for school problems and criminality (Coie, 1990;
hyperactivity and conduct disorders. The Gittelman et al. find- Kupersmidt, Coie, & Dodge, 1990; Parker & Asher, 1987),
ing that hyperactivity, without accompanying conduct prob- which are also risk factors for substance abuse (Hawkins,
lems, predicts an increased risk of substance abuse has not been Lishner, Jenson, & Catalano, 1987).
replicated. However, it suggests further investigation into the Little research has been done on the direct link between peer
relationship between attention-deficit disorders, conduct prob- rejection and substance use, but traits of the child that have
lems, and substance abuse. been associated with peer rejection—aggressiveness, shyness,
11. Academic failure. Although there is an inverse relation- and withdrawal—have been examined for their relationship to
ship between intellectual ability and delinquency after control- drug use. For example, Kellam, Ensminger, and Simon (1980)
ling for socioeconomic status and race (G. D. Gottfredson, found that children who had been shy in first grade reported
1981), a similar relationship has not been reported for drug use, low levels of involvement in drug use, whereas those who had
in spite of the covariation in delinquent and drug-using behav- been aggressive or had shown a combination of aggressiveness
iors. In fact, in an African-American inner-city sample, higher with shyness in first grade had the highest levels of use. Brook et
scores on reading readiness and IQ tests in Grade 1 predicted al. (1986) found that childhood traits relevant to peer rejection
earlier and more frequent use of alcohol in adolescence (Flem- —social inhibition, isolation from peers, and aggression
ing, Kellam, & Brown, 1982). Similarly, in a national probabil- against peers—were not significantly associated with adoles-
ity sample, high intelligence, as assessed by the Armed Forces cent drug use stage. However, aggression against peers during
Qualifying Test, was associated with higher lifetime levels of adolescence was associated with stage of use, and teenagers
cocaine use among young adults age 19-26 (Kandel & Davies, who were less socially inhibited and less isolated from peers
1991). were likely to be at a more advanced stage of use.
Nevertheless, failure in school has been identified as a pre- These studies suggest that the link between peer rejection
dictor of adolescent drug abuse (Jessor, 1976; Robins, 1980). and subsequent drug use may not be a simple one. Shyness, by
Poor school performance has been found to predict frequency isolating a child from his or her peers, may protect the child
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 85

against drug use by eliminating one source of influence to use: predict lower drug involvement and a greater probability of
drug-using peers. Aggressiveness, on the other hand, though discontinuation of use (Kandel, Single, & Kessler, 1976).
resulting for some children in exclusion from groups of conven-
tional peers, may be associated with acceptance by other ag- Implications of Research on Risk
gressive and perhaps delinquent peers who could foster drug
use (Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest, & Gairepy, 1988). A risk-focused prevention approach requires identification
Hartup (1983) suggested that rejected children form friend- of those risk factors to be addressed. Unfortunately, all the in-
ships with other rejected children during the preadolescent formation needed to select the most promising risk factors for
years and that these friendship groups become delinquent dur- intervention is not yet at hand. Experimental research is
ing adolescence. However, this process is as yet unconfirmed needed to discover which risk factors are causal and which are
(Tremblay, 1988). spurious in the etiology of drug abuse. Only by addressing risk
14. Association with drug-using peers. Peer use of sub- factors in experimental trials and observing the effects on drug
stances has consistently been found to be among the strongest abuse can one determine whether a precursor of drug abuse is
predictors of substance use among youth (Barnes & Welte, causally related to drug abuse. Experimental prevention re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1986; Brook et al., 1990; Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985; search is therefore necessary both to understand the etiology of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Jessor et al., 1980; Kandel, 1978, 1986; Kandel & Andrews, drug abuse and to determine which risk factors should be tar-
1987). Studies among specific ethnic groups confirm this rela- geted in prevention policy and programs.
tionship. Newcomb and Bentler (1986) reported that the influ- Several general conclusions regarding risks for drug abuse
ence of peers on adolescent drug use was stronger than that of can be drawn, which have implications for prevention. First,
parents for Whites, African Americans, Asian Americans and the risk factors reviewed above have been shown to be stable
Hispanic Americans. Similar findings were reported by Byram over time in spite of changing norms. For example, despite
and Fly (1984). Harford (1985) found that African-American general changes in norms regarding the use of drugs such as
youths who did not drink alcohol reported fewer school friends marijuana over the past 20 years (Johnston, O'Malley, & Bach-
who drank than did those who drank, and Dembo et al. (1979) man, 1989), studies conducted in different times and places
have shown these factors to predict adolescent drug abuse rela-
found that friends' use of alcohol and marijuana was related to
tively consistently. This suggests the risk factors' stability as
a youth's own use for both African-American and Puerto Ri-
predictors and their viability as targets for preventive work.
can-American youths.
Second, risk factors from several domains predict drug
75. Alienation and rebelliousness. Alienation from the dom-
abuse. Some factors are characteristics of the individual; others
inant values of society (Jessor & Jessor, 1977; Kandel, 1982; are characteristics of families and their interactions, schools
Penning & Barnes, 1982), low religiosity (Jessor et al., 1980; and classroom experiences, peer groups, and broader commu-
Kandel, 1982; Robins, 1980), and rebelliousness (Bachman et nity, legal, economic, and cultural factors.
al., 1981; Kandel, 1982) have been shown to be positively related Third, different risk factors are salient at different periods of
to drug use and delinquent behavior. Shedler and Block (1990) development. For example, poor academic achievement in
found that interpersonal alienation measured at age 7 predicted Grades 1 and 2 does not appear to be a stable predictor of
frequent marijuana use at age 18. Similarly, high tolerance of teenage drug abuse (Kellam & Brown, 1982), though poor
deviance (Jessor & Jessor, 1977), a strong need for indepen- achievement in the later grades predicts drug abuse. Aggressive-
dence (Jessor, 1976), and normlessness (Paton & Kandel, 1978) ness at ages 5-7 predicts later drug abuse and, if it continues,
have all been linked with drug use. All these qualities would becomes more strongly predictive of drug abuse with increas-
appear to characterize youths who are not bonded to society. ing age.
16. Attitudes favorable to drug use. Research also has shown Fourth, there is evidence that the more risk factors present,
a relationship between drug use initiation and specific attitudes the greater the risk of drug abuse (Bry, McKeon, & Pandina,
and beliefs regarding drugs. Initiation into use of any substance 1982; Newcomb et al, 1986). Rutter (1980) found a multiplica-
is preceded by values favorable to its use (Kandel et al, 1978; tive effect of added risk factors on the likelihood of childhood
Krosnick & Judd, 1982; Smith & Fogg, 1978). psychopathology, and Newcomb, Maddahian, Skager, and
17. Early onset of drug use. Early onset of drug use predicts Bentler (1987) reported a similar contribution of combinations
subsequent misuse of drugs. Rachal et al. (1982) reported that of different risk factors to overall risk for adolescent drug use. It
misusers of alcohol appear to begin drinking at an earlier age is plausible that a greater length of exposure to environmental
than do users. The earlier the onset of any drug use, the greater risk factors exacerbates risk as well. Current research focuses on
the involvement in other drug use (Kandel, 1982) and the how risk factors interact in the etiology of drug abuse (Loeber &
greater the frequency of use (Fleming, Kellam, & Brown, 1982). Stouthamer-Loeber, 1986). Greater precision in estimating how
Earlier initiation into drug use also increases the probability of much various risk factors contribute to drug abuse will help to
extensive and persistent involvement in the use of more danger- focus prevention efforts on those risk factors that are most viru-
ous drugs (Kandel, 1982) and the probability of involvement in lent.
deviant activities such as crime and selling drugs (Brunswick & A risk-focused prevention approach does not require that risk
Boyle, 1979; O'Donnell & Clayton, 1979). Robins and Przybeck factors be manipulated directly. It may be impossible to reduce
(1985) found that the onset of drug use before the age of 15 was a or change certain risk factors directly through preventive inter-
consistent predictor of drug abuse in the samples they studied. vention. In these instances, the goal of prevention efforts will
Conversely, a later age of onset of drug use has been shown to be to mediate or moderate the effects of the identified but non-
86 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND 1 MILLER

manipulable risk factors. A family history of alcoholism, for that produce an enduring shield or resilience in the face of risk
example, may be difficult or impossible to change. Neverthe- for negative outcomes has direct relevance for risk-focused drug
less, it may be possible to moderate the effects of a family his- abuse prevention. It suggests that the goals of risk-focused pre-
tory of alcoholism by intervening with children who are at risk vention may be accomplished both through direct efforts at
because of their exposure to this environment. One task of risk reduction and through the enhancement of protective fac-
risk-focused prevention research is to determine which risk fac- tors that moderate or mediate the effects of exposure to risk.
tors can be manipulated, which risk factors cannot be changed Preventive work that seeks to address risk factors for drug abuse
but can be mediated or moderated, and which risk factors can- must clearly hypothesize how a particular intervention is ex-
not be affected at all. pected to address risk: by directly eliminating or reducing a risk
factor or by mediating or moderating its effects through the
Protective Factors Against Drug Abuse enhancement of protective factors or processes.
Little research has focused specifically on protection against
Because some risk factors for drug abuse may be resistant or adolescent drug abuse defined in this way. However, recently
impossible to change, the results of research on protective fac- Brook et al. (1990) identified two mechanisms by which protec-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tors are important for prevention policy. Protective factors medi- tive factors reduce risk for adolescent drug use. The first is a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ate or moderate the effects of exposure to risk (Cowen & Work, "risk/protective" mechanism through which exposure to risk
1988; Garmezy, 1985; Rutter, 1985; Werner, 1989). To the extent factors is moderated by the presence of protective factors. They
that protective factors are identified that inhibit drug abuse reported that the risk posed by drug-using peers was moderated
among those at risk, strategies can seek to address risk by en- by a strong attachment or bond between parent and adolescent
hancing these protective factors. Research with populations ex- and by parent conventionality. The second is a "protective/pro-
posed to multiple risks has identified substantial subgroups of tective" mechanism through which one protective factor poten-
individuals who are able to negotiate risk exposure successfully, tiates another protective factor, strengthening its effect. They
escaping relatively unscathed (Werner, 1989). These observa- reported that a strong bond of attachment between adolescent
tions have led to interest in the etiological importance of factors and father enhanced the effects of other protective factors such
that may protect against health problems including drug abuse. as adolescent conventionality, positive maternal characteristics,
Concepts of vulnerability and resiliency have been advanced and marital harmony in preventing drug use.
to identify the extent of individual susceptibility to risk (Rutter, In related research areas, Garmezy (1985) has identified pro-
1985). Vulnerability denotes intensified susceptibility to risk; tective factors among children exposed to extreme stress be-
resiliency is the ability to withstand or surmount risk. From cause of highly disturbed family circumstances. These include
this perspective, protection involves enhancing resilient re- a child's own positive temperament or disposition, a supportive
sponses to risk exposure. The hypothesis is that certain charac- family milieu, and an external support system that encourages
teristics or conditions mediate or moderate the effects of expo- and reinforces the child's coping efforts and strengthens them
sure to risk, thereby reducing the vulnerability and enhancing by inculcating positive values. Rutter (1985) has suggested that
the resiliency of those at risk and protecting them from unde- resilient children display a repertoire of social problem solving
sirable outcomes. To illustrate, Werner and Smith (1982) found skills and belief in their own self-efficacy.
that in rural Kauai, Hawaii, being raised in a small family with In designing interventions to reduce the negative effects of
low conflict, having high intelligence, and being a firstborn identified risk factors, it is important to focus attention on the
child buffered the effects of extreme poverty and other risk potential positive effects of such protective factors. The avail-
factors for poor educational, economic, and health outcomes. able evidence suggests that to be viable, a prevention strategy
For the concept of protective factors—as distinct from risk requires attention to risk and protective factors related to indi-
factors—to be useful, it must apply to differences in outcomes vidual vulnerability, poor child rearing, school achievement,
among individuals exposed to the same risks. Though some social influences, social skills, and broad social norms, all of
have viewed protective factors simply as the opposite of those which are implicated in the development of adolescent drug
variables identified as risk factors (Labouvie & McGee, 1986), abuse. Because risks are present in several social domains and
this conception does not appear particularly useful. Designat- cumulate in predicting drug abuse, multicomponent preven-
ing two distinct constructs (e.g., risk and protective factors) to tion strategies focused on reducing multiple risks and enhanc-
distinguish extreme levels of a single variable bearing a linear ing multiple protective factors hold promise. Such strategies
relationship to drug abuse adds little. It is not necessary to would be designed to build up protection while reducing risk.
postulate protective factors if better outcomes are observed in Each risk factor targeted should be addressed during the de-
those not exposed to risk. On the other hand, if protective fac- velopmental period at which it begins to stabilize as a predictor
tors are viewed as sources of differences in response to a given of subsequent drug abuse. Interventions must also target popu-
amount of exposure to risk, the construct stimulates attention lations at greatest risk—groups and individuals who are ex-
to nonlinear and interactive relationships among risk and pro- posed to a large number of risk factors—if the prevalence of
tective factors. drug abuse, as defined here, is to be reduced through preven-
In urging a focus on protective mechanisms, Rutter (1985) tion efforts. Although intervention with people who are not
described interactive processes to identify multiplicative inter- exposed to multiple risk factors may delay or prevent the onset
actions or synergistic effects, in which one variable potentiates of drug use in the general population, a desirable goal in its own
the effect of another. The idea of identifying protective pro- right, it may fail to reduce significantly the prevalence of drug
cesses or specifying particular interactions among variables abuse.
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 87

The evidence suggests a developmentally adjusted, multiple- site extremes of variables already identified as risk factors for
component risk-reduction strategy that cuts across traditional drug abuse (e.g., low commitment to schooling and alienation
health, education, and human service delivery systems. The and rebelliousness) or whether social bonding represents a dis-
strategy must reach those at highest risk by virtue of exposure tinct protective factor capable of buffering the effects of other
to multiple risk factors. It must address the most significant risk risk factors such as a family history of alcoholism or extreme
factors faced by those groups. Finally, the strategy may explic- poverty. Further research on this question is needed.
itly seek to increase protective factors as mediators or modera- The social development model specifies hypotheses regard-
tors against risks that cannot be changed by intervention. ing the processes that produce bonding to a social unit. Interac-
tions among (a) opportunities for involvement offered in each
Using Theory to Guide Prevention social unit, (b) the skills used by individuals in these social
units, and (c) the reinforcements offered in these units are hy-
Research and Practice pothesized to produce social bonds of attachment, commit-
To design a multicomponent intervention strategy that seeks ment, and belief in the values of the social units in which young
to reduce multiple risk factors and simultaneously enhance pro- people develop (see Catalano & Hawkins, 1986).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tective factors among those exposed to risk, it is useful to be Guided by this social development perspective, our own risk-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

guided by a theory of causation and prevention. Theory sup- focused prevention work has two purposes: (a) to understand
plies the explanatory framework for the observed evidence re- better the processes by which risk and protective factors contrib-
garding risk and protective factors for drug abuse by hypothe- ute to the etiology of drug abuse in adolescence and (b) to test
sizing causal relationships among these variables that lead to- promising approaches to prevent adolescent drug abuse. We
ward or away from drug abuse. Theory is also useful in guiding hypothesize that children who develop strong bonds to social
the design of complementary prevention interventions in dif- units holding norms antithetical to drug abuse will be less likely
ferent social units when multiple interventions are desired. To to abuse drugs.
guide prevention interventions, theory should (a) identify the To enhance social bonding, we manipulate social settings
factors that predict drug abuse, (b) explain the mechanisms and individual capacities using the principles of social learning
through which they operate, (c) identify the factors that influ- theory in developmentally appropriate ways. For example, in
ence these mechanisms, (d) predict points to interrupt the the school setting, we train teachers in methods of proactive
course leading to drug abuse, and (e) specify the interventions to classroom management, interactive teaching, and cooperative
prevent onset of drug abuse (Kazdin, 1990). learning, including students in peer teaching. The explicit,
It is not our goal to review theories of drug abuse (see Lettieri, theory-driven objectives of these intervention elements are (a)
Sayers, & Pearson, 1980, for a review). Nevertheless, an example to make available opportunities for children to be involved in
illustrates how theory can provide clear direction for preventive prosocial activities, (b) to provide skills needed to undertake
interventions of the type described here. these activities successfully, and (c) to provide positive reinforce-
As noted by Kazdin (1990), our delinquency and drug abuse ment for successful involvement. All of these objectives serve
prevention efforts have been grounded in the social develop- the broader goal of strengthening bonding to the social unit, in
ment model (Farrington & Hawkins, 1991; Hawkins & Lam, this case the school. From a social development perspective, the
1987; Hawkins & Weis, 1985). An integration of control theory same three objectives guide interventions with parents, day-
(Hirschi, 1969) and social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), the care providers, youth ministers, recreation workers, and others
social development model emphasizes the role of bonding to participating in the socialization of children. The framework of
prosocial family, school, and peers as a protection against the the social development model thus fosters a multicomponent
development of conduct problems, school misbehavior, prevention approach, grounded in knowledge of risk and pro-
truancy, and drug abuse. This concept of bonding is closely tective factors and consistent in goals, across a variety of social
related to the concept of attachment as denned by Bowlby settings.
(1969,1973) and as observed by Brook et al. (1990) to inhibit
adolescent drug abuse. It is also consistent with Garmezy's
(1985) identification of familial and external support and value
Current Risk-Focused Drug Prevention
systems as protective factors against exposure to stress in child- During the 1960s and 1970s there was little explicit attention
hood. to risk or protective factors for drug abuse in the design and
Four elements of social bonding have been shown to be in- development of preventive interventions. More recent research
versely related to drug use. These are strong attachment to par- on drug abuse prevention has focused on risk reduction, but has
ents (Brook, Brook, et al., 1990; Brook, Gordon, et al., 1986; not included attention to multiple risk or protective factors and,
Hundleby & Mercer, 1987; Jessor & lessor, 1977; Norem-Hebei- for the most part, has not addressed risk factors that appear
sen et al., 1984); commitment to schooling (Friedman, 1983; developmentally before the age of likely drug use initiation.
Johnston, Bachman, & O'Malley, 1981; Kim, 1979; Krohn & Most recent prevention research has targeted only two risk fac-
Massey, 1980); regular involvement in church activities (Schle- tors for drug abuse, both of which are most salient just at the
gel & Sanborn, 1979; Wechsler & McFadden, 1979); and belief point of drug use initiation: (a) laws and norms favorable to drug
in the generalized expectations, norms, and values of society use and (b) social influences to use drugs. Approaches targeting
(Akers, Krohn, Lanza-Kaduce, & Radosevich, 1979; Krohn & these risk factors are designed for a relatively quick "return," in
Massey, 1980). Research has not yet determined whether these that if they are effective, they should reduce or curtail drug use
elements of social bonding are best viewed simply as the oppo- immediately. Prevention approaches that target these risk fac-
D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND 1 MILLER

tors are summarized below; the right half of Table 1 summa- should not be expected, by themselves, to eliminate illegal drug
rizes the effects of these approaches on the risk factors ad- supplies, to significantly raise the price of illegal drugs, or to
dressed. eliminate drug abuse. Those who are at greatest risk of drug
abuse by virtue of low social bonding to society may view the
Supply Manipulation, Interdiction, and Enforcement relative benefits of drug dealing and drug use as worth the risks
Strategies of apprehension. The prevention of alcohol and other drug
abuse among those at greatest risk requires attention to the
Attention to the laws and norms of society related to the use factors that distinguish these people. The risk factors encoun-
of alcohol and other drugs is clearly warranted, given the link tered by these persons at highest risk must be addressed to
between these factors and rates of alcoholism and drug abuse. If reduce the demand for illegal drugs.
reduction of the prevalence of abuse of drugs is the goal, the
evidence does not support those who advocate the legalization
Changing Social Norms
of currently illegal drugs such as marijuana and cocaine (Clay-
ton, in press). Rather, the evidence supports efforts to limit A second approach currently emphasized is changing social
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior that is inconsistent with existing legal sanctions. This norms about drug- and alcohol-influenced behaviors. The ap-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

has been attempted through efforts to control the supplies of proach includes "Just Say 'NO!'" activities, community coali-
both legal and illegal drugs. tions against drugs, media campaigns, and certain policy
Since the repeal of prohibition, the supply of alcohol has changes.
been manipulated in several ways, including taxation, age re- C. A. Johnson and Solis (1983) and Perry, Klepp, and Shultz
strictions on consumption, restrictions on hours of purchase, (1988) reviewed a number of community health promotion pro-
and restrictions on liquor-by-the-drink sales. As noted earlier, grams aimed at reducing cardiovascular disease by changing
restricting availability and increasing the price of alcohol by smoking and other risk-related behaviors. These programs in-
increasing taxes on the purchase price can reduce rates of alco- cluded involvement of the mass media, risk-factor screening
hol abuse as indicated in rates of cirrhosis of the liver and alco- programs, and education programs for adults and youths. They
hol-related traffic fatalities. Although increasing age restric- have been associated with lower smoking onset rates among
tions on alcohol purchases and restrictions on liquor-by-the- youths (Perry, Klepp, & Shultz, 1988) and cessation or reduc-
drink sales appears less effective than taxation in limiting tion of smoking (C. A. Johnson & Solis, 1983).
alcohol abuse, these strategies also have shown desirable effects Of particular interest in this area is the influence of advertis-
in reducing alcohol-related traffic fatalities (Blose & Holder, ing on drug use. There is some indication that higher exposure
1987; Decker, Graitcer, & Schafmer, 1988; Krieg, 1982). to "life-style" ads promoting alcohol consumption is found
This evidence might appear to imply that supply manipula- among adolescents who report higher levels of drinking (Atkin,
tion strategies such as drug interdiction and arrests of drug Hocking, & Block, 1984).
dealers would have a similar desirable effect on the abuse of The media and advertising industries have cooperated in a
illegal drugs by raising the price of these drugs to the user. national project to encourage negative attitudes toward the use
However, existing evidence does not support this contention. of illegal drugs through the use of antidrug advertising. Results
Analysis by the Rand Corporation resulted in the conclusion of mall intercept surveys indicate that saturation advertising in
that neither a doubling of interdiction nor increased arrests of 10 markets was accompanied by significant normative changes
drug dealers would affect retail prices or the availability of ille- over a 1-year period (Black, 1989). College students and chil-
gal drugs (Polich, Ellickson, Reuter, & Kahan, 1984). Data dren were more negative in their attitudes toward drugs, viewed
from the Drug Enforcement Administration confirm this con- drug users less positively, and perceived less drug use among
clusion. In spite of an increase in federal spending on interdic- their friends in 1988 compared with 1987. Moreover, in areas
tion and law enforcement from $1.807 billion in 1986 to $3.770 that received saturation advertising, 9% to 15% more children
billion in 1989, the average street price of cocaine fell from $100 reported conversations about drugs with parents, teachers, and
to $75 dollars per gram during the same period. Although at siblings in 1988 than did in 1987. In the balance of the United
some level well beyond current spending, interdiction and en- States, there were no increases in such communications. Teen-
forcement might reduce drug supplies and drive up prices, the agers age 13 through 17 showed the fewest changes in attitudes
fiscal costs, effects on U.S. international trade, and constraints in association with saturation advertising, though they became
on individual rights required would be excessive. Moreover, in more positive in their views toward nonusers and perceived
this scenario, if demand for illegal drugs were not reduced, it is greater risks from marijuana and cocaine use (Black, 1989). Of
plausible that domestic producers of synthetic drugs would step course, these differences could have been produced by other
in to fill demand as interdiction began to reduce drug supplies, factors operating in communities sufficiently concerned about
thus continuing to hold down prices to users. drug abuse that their broadcast media would run a saturation-
In our view, the most powerful effect of interdiction and en- advertising campaign against drugs.
forcement activities is to communicate general social norms of Social norms regarding the use of specific drugs and their
disapproval for the distribution and use of illegal drugs. Social attendant risks and benefits can change over a relatively short
norms antithetical to use appear associated with reductions in period of time. From 1978 to 1983, the proportion of the na-
the prevalence of the frequent use of marijuana (Robins, 1984) tion's high school seniors who perceived there to be a great
and other illegal drugs (Johnston, 1991). Supply-reduction strat- health risk associated with the regular use of marijuana rose
egies communicate an important message to citizens but from 38% to 63%. Over the same period, the proportion of the
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 89

nation's seniors who used marijuana daily dropped from 10.7% quences of drug use; the provision of evidence that drug use is
to 5.5% (Johnston, 1985). not as widespread among peers as children may think; encour-
An important question for study concerns the role of broadly agement for children to make public commitments to remain
focused norm-change efforts, such as media campaigns, in pro- drug free; and, in some instances, the use of peer leaders to
ducing such changes in norms and the frequent use of drugs. teach the curriculum (Botvin, 1986; Klepp, Halper, & Perry,
Studies are needed that examine how these efforts affect chil- 1986).
dren at greatest risk for drug abuse. It is not known how chil- Social influence resistance approaches also have been com-
dren who come from poorly managed families, who have failed bined with training in problem-solving and decision-making
in school, who are aggressive, or who have lost commitment to skills, skills to increase self-control and self-efficacy, adaptive
school respond to "Just Say 'NO!'" or other antidrug messages coping strategies for relieving stress and anxiety, interpersonal
in the media or in their personal social environments. skills, and general assertive skills (Botvin, 1986; Flay, 1985). In
Changes in social norms have also been codified in school this regard, Botvin's skills training program has combined ele-
policies regarding drug-using behavior (Moskowitz & Jones, ments of both social influence resistance training and social
1988). Recent studies of school policies regulating smoking competence skills training discussed later (Botvin & Wills,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

have shown that more comprehensive policies, which empha- 1985). Recent projects have also combined classroom-based so-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

size prevention of use and restrictions on opportunities for use cial influence resistance curricula with mass-media program-
in or near school grounds, appear to reduce the amount of ming and parent involvement strategies in comprehensive inter-
smoking by students (Pentz, Brannon, et al., 1989), although ventions seeking to change norms toward drug use and increase
effects on smoking prevalence are less consistent. These poli- resistance to drug-prone influences among adolescents (Pentz,
cies appear to affect smoking behavior primarily through the Dwyer, etal., 1989).
clear specification of norms regarding smoking rather than Most published studies of social influence resistance strate-
through the enactment of punitive consequences for policy vio- gies have found modest but significant reductions, in compari-
lations, which have not shown effects in reducing smoking son with controls, in the onset and prevalence of cigarette smok-
(Pentz, Brannon, et al., 1989). Additional research is needed on ing after training (see Botvin, 1986; Bukoski, 1986; Flay, 1985;
the effectiveness of school policies in preventing or reducing Moskowitz, 1989; D. M. Murray, Davis-Hearn, Goldman, Pirie,
the use of drugs other than tobacco and on the effects of such & Luepker, 1988; Tobler, 1986, for reviews). A few studies have
policies on those at highest risk for drug abuse. reported beneficial effects of the strategy in preventing or de-
laying the onset of alcohol or marijuana use (Botvin, 1986; El-
Social Influence Resistance Strategies lickson & Bell, 1990; Hansen et al., 1988; McAlister, Perry, Kil-
len, Slinkard, & Maccoby, 1980; Pentz, Dwyer, et al., 1989).
As noted earlier, among the strongest correlates of teenage Student- or peer-led social influence resistance training in-
drug-using behavior is association with others who use drugs. If terventions have achieved greater reductions in drug use, com-
the relationship between association with drug-using peers and pared with interventions led by teachers (Botvin, Baker, Filaz-
drug-using behavior is actually causal, the manipulation of a zola, & Botvin, 1990; Klepp et al., 1986; McAlister, 1983; D. M.
factor that accounts for a great deal of variance in drug use Murray, Johnson, Luepker, & Mittelmark, 1984). This differ-
would hold promise for producing significant reductions in ado- ence may reflect greater fidelity in implementation of the cur-
lescent drug use. riculum by peer leaders (resulting in greater skill acquisition by
Prevention strategies focused on social influences to use students; Botvin et al., 1990), or the finding may reflect peer
drugs also are appealing from a cost-effectiveness perspective. leaders' stimulation of classroom norms antithetical to
Because peer influence to use drugs is salient developmentally drug use.
at the point of onset of drug use, long delays are not required A number of research issues remain to be addressed regard-
before effects of such interventions can be observed. ing the effects of social influence focused intervention. One
The most heavily researched strategy for addressing social question is whether smoking prevention programs, without
influences to use drugs is classroom-based skills training for content specific to other drugs such as alcohol or marijuana,
adolescents in Grades 5 through 10, most often Grades 6 and 7. have effects on the use of these drugs. Some studies suggest
The training teaches students through instruction, modeling, there may be a generalized effect of these prevention programs
and role play to identify and resist influences to use drugs and, on alcohol and marijuana use by subjects (G. M. Johnson et al.,
in some cases, to prepare for associated difficulties and stresses 1984; McAlister et al., 1980). A related question is raised by
anticipated in the process of resisting such influences (Botvin, Ellickson and Bell (1990), who sought to extend the social influ-
1986). Grounded in social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), ence model of smoking prevention to alcohol and marijuana.
social influence resistance strategies view drug use as a socially Results were mixed. Modest reductions in drinking for students
acquired behavior, initiated and reinforced by drug-using at three risk levels were observed immediately after the teen-led
others (Bukoski, 1986). version of the program but disappeared at 1-year follow-up.
Whereas all programs of this type offer skills in resisting Exposure to the curriculum was associated with reductions in
social influences to use drugs, many also seek to promote smoking among baseline experimenters but increases in smok-
norms negative toward drug use (Hansen, Johnson, Flay, Gra- ing among baseline smokers. Curriculum exposure was also
ham, & Sobel, 1988; Perry, 1986). These normative-change com- associated with reductions in both initiation and current use of
ponents have included efforts to depict drug use as socially marijuana. The investigators speculate that the apparent effec-
unacceptable; identification of short-term negative conse- tiveness of social influence approaches for tobacco and mari-
90 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

juana may reflect the generalized social norms against those grams involves the durability of effects. Evidence from a num-
two substances but for alcohol social influence training is less ber of recent studies points to deterioration of initially positive
effective because society has not developed a consensus against program effects as early as 2 to 3 years and as long as 8 years
its use. postinter vention (Botvin et al., 1990; Flay et al., 1989; Hansen et
Biglan, Glasgow, et al. (1987), on the other hand, found no al, 1988; D. M. Murray, Pirie, Luepker, & Pallonen, 1989).
generalization of effects to alcohol or marijuana use of a smok- Long-term results from the North Karelia Youth Smoking Pre-
ing refusal skills training program. Others have found that cog- vention Project (Vartiainen, Pallonen, McAlister, Koskela, &
nitive and interpersonal skills training interventions reduced Puska, 1983, 1986; Vartiainen et al., 1990) offer a somewhat
tobacco use but had no effects on alcohol, marijuana, or other more promising picture, with 4-year results favoring the schools
drug use (Gersick, Grady, & Snow, 1988). More research is given two versions of the social influence intervention over
needed to determine whether preventing the onset of an early matched comparison schools. At 8 years postintervention, only
behavior in a sequence, such as smoking in the progression of baseline nonsmokers showed significant program effects
drug use initiation, has effects on later behaviors in the se- (Vartiainen et al., 1990).
quence. That program participants may have equal or higher rates of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Another question is whether classroom-based social influ- substance use than program controls by 2 years after interven-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ence resistance interventions have significant effects on adoles- tion raises the question of whether social influence focused
cents at greatest risk for drug abuse. In most social influence interventions will have effects on drug abuse as defined here. To
resistance studies, risk groups have been denned by different date, virtually no prevention studies targeting drug abuse have
levels of baseline use, usually in smoking behavior (i.e., regular followed subjects long enough to assess effects in late adoles-
tobacco users, occasional tobacco users, and nonusers; Ellick- cence or early adulthood.
son, Bell, Thomas, Robyn, & Zellman, 1988). Although this Even if they are successful in reducing the prevalence of teen-
approach can reduce smoking among students with parents age drug use, social influence resistance strategies may have no
and friends who smoke (Botvin & Wills, 1985), few assessments significant effect on the prevalence of teenage drug abuse as we
are available of effects on those at greatest risk for drug abuse by have defined it. The lifetime prevalence of alcohol use among
virtue of exposure to multiple risk factors earlier in develop- U.S. high school seniors in 1987 was 92.2%, but only 4.8% used
ment. alcohol daily (Johnston et al, 1988). Social influence resistance
Some social influence focused studies have looked at the programs could show positive effects in reducing the preva-
effects of preventive interventions on groups with special demo- lence of alcohol use in the general population without affecting
graphic characteristics that may be related to higher risk. Bot- this 5% at greatest risk for alcohol-related problems.
vin et al. (1989) addressed a special population of urban Afri- Although social influence focused interventions have been
can-American youngsters with a smoking prevention program most widely tested, many of these same questions apply to
that was based on life skills training using cognitive behavioral interventions targeting other risk factors. To address these ques-
techniques. Of several smoking outcomes examined, the only tions, research methodologists must grapple with the complex-
significant effect observed was a smaller proportion of smokers ities of the multiple risk factors and causal pathways implicated
at posttest in the treatment than in the control group on the in substance abuse etiology.
basis of adjusted means for smoking status in the past month.
Some intervention effects were also observed for cognitive and Methodological Challenges for Risk-Based Intervention
attitude variables such as knowledge of smoking consequences Research
and normative expectations.
In another study designed to examine effects on a specific Although it is not our purpose to offer a methodological
population, Schinke, Botvin, et al. (1988) tested a social compe- evaluation of specific prevention studies, a number of research
tence/skills building intervention designed with cultural rele- issues that pertain to risk-focused investigations should be con-
vance for Native American adolescents. They found at posttest sidered when weighing the evidence, individually and cumula-
and 6-month follow-up that subjects who received the interven- tively, from the studies available (Moskowitz, 1989). Risk-fo-
tion improved more than control subjects on measures of sub- cused prevention studies require research designs that address
stance use knowledge, attitudes and interactive skills, and self- threats posed by mixed units of analysis, differential attrition,
reported rates of tobacco, alcohol and drug use. and differential implementation as well as the interpretive chal-
The same group of investigators examined still another spe- lenge presented by heterogeneous effects across risk groups and
cial population, children of blue-collar families, using a school- along the developmental life course. Careful theoretical specifi-
based social skills smoking prevention program (Schinke, Be- cation and multiple and varied statistical analysis techniques
bel, Orlandi, & Botvin, 1988). Lower use rates were observed can be used to meet these challenges.
and validated among pupils who received the skills-based inter- Mixed units of analysis. In many published studies of social
vention (as compared with discussion-based groups and control influence resistance programs, the basic premise of experimen-
groups) at 6, 12, 18, and 24 months follow-up. Although they tal design—that the randomized experimental unit is the unit
have isolated particular populations and baseline users, none of of analysis—is violated. Schools or classrooms are generally the
the social influence intervention studies have examined unit of random assignment to experimental or control condi-
whether program effects vary for groups characterized by multi- tion, but the unit of analysis often used is individual students.
ple risk factors predictive of heavy drug use. School or classroom differences are thus confounded with pro-
Another question regarding social influence resistance pro- gram effects on individuals (Biglan & Ary, 1985). Some studies
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 91

have addressed this problem by assigning multiple schools or gated, studies have consistently shown that subjects with a
classrooms to each condition, then analyzing at the classroom higher mean rate of tobacco smoking and marijuana use are
level (Biglan, Severson, et al., 1987; Botvin, Baker, Renick, Fi- most likely to be lost at follow-up (Biglan, Severson, et al., 1987;
lazzola, & Botvin, 1984; Hansen et al., 1988; Pentz, MacKin- Hansen et al., 1988), raising questions as to the generalizability
non, etal., 1989). of reported results to those at greatest risk. Several solutions to
When scarce resources impose limits on the number of units this problem have been proposed. McKinlay et al. (1989) recom-
that can be randomly assigned, some alternative solutions have mend the "intention-to-treat" approach, in which all subjects in
been suggested. Randomized block and factorial designs can the original cohort are retained for the analysis to avoid the bias
be used to stratify schools by factors known to affect key out- of differential attrition and preserve the integrity of the ran-
comes (McKinlay, Stone, & Zucker, 1989). Alternatively, to ac- domization. Alternatively, direct observation of the effects of
count for variability attributable to the school, multiple investi- missing data because of attrition may be obtained by including
gators conducting similar studies with different populations in a dummy-coded variable for subjects lost to the study in the
comparable or contrasting school settings could build a collec- analysis (Raymond, 1987).
tive case for the general effectiveness of a given approach. Clear Intervention implementation and intensity. Studies should
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

specification of the relevant features of the school settings and also address questions of differential intervention implementa-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

careful attention to implementation integrity would be critical tion and intensity (McKinlay et al., 1989). By randomly and
elements of this approach. Dwyer et al. (1989) have proposed independently selecting samples of classrooms or schools at
the use of linear regression models fit to aggregated data to each point in time, variable doses of the intervention may be
assess bias in standard errors when individual data are analyzed examined (e.g., length of exposure, level of teacher training,
but schools are assigned to conditions. D. M. Zucker (in press) variety of media used). All intervention studies demand sys-
has argued that use of the individual as the unit of analysis when tematic attention to implementation integrity. We have pro-
classrooms or schools are the units of assignment will always posed and used three steps in examining implementation: (a)
lead to positively biased tests, compromising the internal valid- collection of data to assess degree of implementation, (b) re-
ity of analyses. D. M. Murray, Hannan, and Zucker (1989) and porting of data on implementation for each dimension of the
D. M. Murray and Hannan (1990) have concluded from this interventions, and (c) inclusion of implementation data in the
that the most prudent course remains ensuring that the unit of tests of efficacy (Hawkins, Abbott, Catalano, & Gillmore,
analysis and unit of assignment are the same. 1991; Hawkins & Lam, 1987).
Classroom and school effects should be carefully examined Measuring developmental change and intervention effects.
when analysis at the level of assignment to conditions is pre- Designs nesting cross-sectional intervention studies within
cluded by small samples. The relative importance of classroom longitudinal panel studies have special relevance and appeal
context variables and individual-level variables, along with for risk-focused prevention work. Such designs are well suited
their potential interactions, can be addressed directly using con- to explore questions of group differences as well as change over
textual analysis. Such an approach requires a clear theory- time, thus providing for tests of intervention effectiveness and
driven specification of the nature of the predicted contextual for estimation of developmental sequences. Cross-sequential
effects (Bursik, in press). This requirement, however, poses a designs, conceived particularly to study developmental prob-
problem for current school-based research, in which the contri- lems (Schaie, 1965; Tonry, Ohlin, & Farrington, 1991), allow
bution of school and classroom variables, both to risk and to estimation of age, cohort, and period effects, thereby produc-
intervention effectiveness, are not well understood or coher- ing data on both the development of and changes in drug risk
ently organized in theory. and use patterns over time and on the effects of interventions
Homogeneity of effect across levels of risk. When sample size across cohorts of adolescents (Hawkins, Abbott, et al., 1991).
is sufficiently large, researchers can investigate directly the dif- A major challenge for social influence resistance studies at
ferential effects of intervention on groups at different levels of this time is to overcome methodological weaknesses. Much
risk. When subgroups are not large enough for such analysis, work is proceeding along the lines described above. Mean-
Dwyer et al. (1989) have proposed statistical methods, using while, the failure of social influence strategies to establish dura-
conditional proportional odds models with interaction be- bility of effects or to show consistent results with substances
tween intervention dummy variable and baseline behavioral other than tobacco, as well as the questions remaining about
level. Although this solution is proposed for assessing differen- the strategies' effects on drug abuse as opposed to initiation or
tial effects across baseline drug use levels, it may be applicable occasional use, suggests that a second major line of prevention
to other quantifiable risk factors as well. research should be pursued.
Systematic attrition. Problems of attrition are acute in Social influence resistance approaches address risk factors
school-based studies that are designed to follow longitudinal salient developmentally just before or simultaneous with initia-
cohorts of individual students but use the school or classroom tion of drug use. It is not known whether these approaches can
as the unit of random assignment. The external validity of re- protect children made most vulnerable by previous exposure to
sults from social influence resistance evaluations has been other risk factors. For example, a social influence resistance
compromised in many studies by systematic attrition of those program that demonstrated significant reductions in tobacco
at highest risk for drug abuse. Many published studies of this use for baseline experimenters and nonsmokers was actually
type have not addressed attrition, reporting results only for sub- associated with increases in tobacco use for baseline smokers
jects remaining in experimental and comparison classrooms (Ellickson & Bell, 1990). These youngsters, having already de-
(Biglan, Severson, et al., 1987). Where attrition has been investi- fined themselves as part of a smoking subculture with attach-
92 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

ments to tobacco-using peers, may have rejected drug resis- The Perry Preschool Project focused on enhancing the intel-
tance skills as antithetical to their social group identity. Learn- lectual and social development of 3- and 4-year-old African-
ing skills to resist prodrug social influences may be a necessary American children from backgrounds of extreme poverty. The
but not sufficient element of prevention for children who have experimental intervention reduced academic failure, adoles-
been "set up" for drug involvement by exposure to earlier indi- cent pregnancy rates, and criminal behavior when randomly
vidual, family, or community risk factors (Block, Block, & assigned experimental and control subjects were followed up
Keyes, 1988; Shedler & Block, 1990). This possibility suggests a and compared at age 19 (Berrueta-Clement et al., 1984). The
search for intervention strategies that have been effective in experimental program consisted of daily participation in a pre-
reducing other factors, developmentally earlier than social in- school over a l-to-2-year period and weekly home visits by
fluences to use drugs, that predict drug abuse. Such strategies trained teachers to teach mothers skills in child management.
should be investigated for their long-term effects in preventing By age 19, experimental preschool participants had lower arrest
drug abuse. rates and fewer arrests as well as lower rates of self-reported
fighting. They had higher rates of secondary school comple-
Prevention Approaches Targeting Early Risk Factors tion, lower rates of placement in special education classes, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

higher grade point averages than their randomly assigned con-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The following strategies merit attention from drug abuse pre- trol counterparts. Other studies of early childhood education
vention researchers, both because they address risk factors seen programs have shown similar positive effects on children's intel-
to occur before drug initiation and because they use interven- lectual development (Gotts, 1989; Lazar, Darlington, Murray,
tion methods that have demonstrated positive effects. The ap- Royce, & Snipper, 1982; Ramey, Bryant, Campbell, Sparling, &
peal of many of these interventions is strengthened by the fact Wasik, 1988).
that they target risk factors implicated in a range of disorders, These findings suggest that early childhood and parent sup-
including drug abuse as well as antisocial behavior, delin- port programs can buffer the effects of extreme poverty and
quency, and later adult criminality. These interventions have neighborhood disorganization by reducing three risk factors for
shown positive effects on targeted risk factors in controlled in- adolescent substance abuse: childhood behavior problems, fam-
tervention trials using experimental or quasi-experimental de- ily management problems, and academic failure. Research is
signs. As noted below, in some of the studies the interventions needed to evaluate the long-term effectiveness of early child-
appear to have increased protective factors against drug abuse hood and family support programs for high-risk subgroups in
in populations at high risk. preventing adolescent drug abuse.
/. Early childhood and family support programs. Several 2. Programs for parents of children and adolescents. Con-
interventions focusing on the prenatal and early infancy pe- trolled studies have shown that family management problems
riods with a variety of components ranging from health care; and child behavior problems can be reduced through parenting
nutrition; child care; social support for mothers; educational, skills training and functional family therapy. Parenting skills
career, and family planning services; and home visits from training has produced short-term improvements in family in-
health or social service workers have produced significant dif- teraction and reductions in children's problem behaviors
ferences between high-risk-program and control- or compari- (Baum & Forehand, 1981; Patterson, Chamberlain, & Reid,
son-group families. Positive intervention effects have been re- 1982). Parenting skills training combined with social skills
ported on child abuse and neglect (Olds, Henderson, Chamber- training for disruptive kindergarten boys reduced school ad-
lin, & Tatelbaum, 1986; Swift, 1988), early academic justment problems and delayed the onset of delinquent behav-
performance (Bronson, Pierson, & Tivnan, 1984), maternal ior (Tremblay et al., 1990). Functional family therapy has re-
employment and smaller family size, child's higher rate of duced delinquency among juvenile offenders (Alexander &
school attendance and lower rate of school special services, as Parsons, 1973) and prevented it among their siblings (Klein,
well as lower mother-rated antisocial behavior and lower Alexander, & Parsons, 1977).
teacher-rated aggression (Pierson et al., 1983; Seitz, Rosen- Most systematic evaluations of parent training have involved
baum, & Apfel, 1985). children with conduct problems. Parenting skills training fo-
Early childhood education has produced reductions in risk cused on teaching parents to monitor their children's behavior,
factors for drug abuse. Horacek, Ramey, Campbell, Hoffmann, to use moderate contingent discipline for undesired behavior,
and Fletcher (1987) randomly assigned socially and economi- and to consistently reward prosocial behavior (Patterson &
cally deprived children, at infancy and again at kindergarten, to Fleischman, 1979) has resulted in increases in parent-child at-
intervention or control groups, allowing evaluation of the ef- tachment, decreases in children's skill deficits, and decreases in
fects of different amounts of intervention. They successfully the children's targeted behavior problems (Fleischman, 1981;
identified at birth children at high risk for school failure. Rates Patterson & Reid, 1973; Peed, Roberts, & Forehand, 1977).
of retention in grade for high-risk children in the control group Randomized experimental tests of parenting skills training
were almost four times higher than for an average-risk group of have shown significant reductions in preadolescents' problem
peers. The intervention significantly reduced grade retention behaviors when compared with controls (Karoly & Rosenthal,
and improved test scores in math and reading, showing a 1977; Martin, 1977; Patterson et al., 1982; Walters & Gilmore,
greater impact on children who had participated in both inter- 1973). These results suggest that parenting skills training can
vention phases. The high-risk children who received the most buffer the risk of childhood behavior problems for adolescent
intervention achieved a rate of grade advancement nearly equal drug abuse by reducing family management problems and in-
to that of the average-risk group. creasing family bonding.
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 93

To date, little experimental research on the effectiveness of families could be expected to reduce children's behavior prob-
parent training for drug abuse prevention has been conducted. lems in preschool and elementary school years, to increase chil-
In one study, parenting skills training was tested with parents dren's academic performance in elementary and middle school,
who were narcotic and polydrug abusers participating in treat- and to empower children to deal effectively with social influ-
ment programs. A preliminary evaluation reported that par- ences to use drugs encountered in late elementary and middle
ents were successfully trained to develop more effective disci- school grades.
pline methods, that their children had fewer behavior problems Problems of nonparticipation, attrition, and implementation
after treatment, and that the children reported decreased inten- in parenting skills training programs have been well docu-
tions to smoke and use alcohol (DeMarsh & Kumpfer, 1986), mented (Bry, 1983; Fraser, Hawkins, & Howard, 1988; Grady,
although the effects of the intervention on the children's actual Kelin, & Boratynski, 1985; Hawkins, Catalano, Jones, & Fine,
drug use was not reported. These preliminary results suggest 1987; Perry, Crockett, & Pirie, 1987; Perry, Luepker, et al.,
that parents whose children are at high risk by virtue of paren- 1988). Parenting training interventions for parents of pre-
tal addiction can be successfully taught parenting skills. An- school, elementary, and middle school children that seek to
other study with a sample of youths at risk for substance abuse overcome these difficulties should be tested in experimental
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

has reported preliminary positive trends for parent-training drug abuse prevention trials.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

treatment groups on parent-child interaction, levels of tobacco 3. Social competence skills training. The evidence that ag-
use, and reduction of depression (Dishion, Kavanagh, & Reid, gression and other behavior problems in the early elementary
1989). grades is associated with an increased risk of later drug abuse
Parent involvement has been shown to be beneficial in im- has stimulated suggestions that educational strategies seeking
proving academic effort, grades, and attendance of students to enhance the social competencies of youngsters during child-
evidencing low commitment to school (Bien & Bry, 1980; hood could reduce the risk of later drug abuse (Hawkins, Jen-
Blechman, Taylor, & Schrader, 1981). Bry (1982) reported a son, Catalano, & Lishner, 1988). It has been argued that chil-
reduction in juvenile justice system involvement of experimen- dren who are aggressive, disruptive, and rejected by peers in
tal subjects receiving an intervention package consisting of (a) a elementary grades are deficient in basic interpersonal skills
parenting program involving regular contacts with the family that can be taught (Spivack & Shure, 1974).
emphasizing training and encouragement for parents to reward Advocates of skills training for interpersonal competence
school progress, (b) teacher goal setting for students, and (c) a cite evidence that socially competent children engage in less
schedule of rewards for students' goal attainment. These find- school misbehavior and have better cognitive skills in such
ings suggest that a promising method for increasing parental areas as basic problem solving essential for academic achieve-
involvement is through training and reinforcement for parents ment (Asher & Renshaw, 1981). They assert that learning cer-
to promote the classroom performance of their children. tain basic moral values like concern for the rights and needs of
Biglan, Glasgow, et al. (1987) found no effects on children's others is essential to the development of prosocial behavior
smoking of four parent messages mailed to the homes of stu- (Battistich, Solomon, Watson, & Schaps, n.d.) and that children
dents, designed to reinforce social influence resistance skills from families in which family management practices are poor
and commitment to nonsmoking taught in a classroom curricu- and family conflict is great do not learn basic interpersonal
lum. However, no implementation assessment was conducted, competencies at home.
and it is not clear that parents used the mailed messages. The Social competence promotion approaches have used a vari-
chosen intervention method may not have been sufficiently ety of methods. For example, socially rejected youths have been
potent to enlist parental participation. taught and coached in social interaction skills to increase the
Pentz, Dwyer, et al. (1989) involved parents in the experimen- frequency of their social interactions (Ladd & Asher, 1985), and
tal drug abuse prevention package tested in the Midwestern classroom instruction has been used to teach cognitive pro-
Prevention Project. The classroom prevention curriculum in- cesses and behavioral skills to handle interpersonal problems
cluded 10 homework assignments in which students were ex- (Weissberg & Allen, 1985). Such methods have been imple-
pected to involve their parents using active interviews and role mented in programs of widely varying duration, with samples
plays. Using interview data from teachers, the authors esti- ranging from all students at a particular grade level to identi-
mated that 80% of the experimental families participated in the fied students with behavior problems. They have been tested
homework assignments. The experimental program was asso- with inner-city, low-income samples (Shure & Spivack, 1982) as
ciated with lowered prevalence rates of tobacco, alcohol, and well as with White middle-class samples (Rotheram, 1982a). To
marijuana use, although the independent contribution of the date, most of these tests have focused on proximal outcomes
parenting component was not assessed. such as school adjustment rather than on drug use behavior.
The existing evidence suggests the promise of parenting Some studies have found positive effects immediately after
skills training and involvement approaches for preventing ado- training for both suburban and inner-city samples (Weissberg et
lescent drug abuse. Training adjusted to the developmental al., 1981).
stage of the child should help parents develop skills to (a) set Social competence promotion approaches have yielded vary-
clear expectations for behavior, (b) monitor and supervise their ing results. Some investigators have reported positive effects on
children, (c) consistently reinforce prosocial behavior, (d) create interpersonal behavior (Battistich et al., n.d.; Bierman & Fur-
opportunities for family involvement, and (e) promote the devel- man, 1984; Bierman, Miller, & Stabb, 1987;Gesten etal, 1982;
opment of their children's academic, social, and refusal skills. Ladd, 1981; Ladd & Asher, 1985; Rotheram, 1982b; Rotheram,
Acquisition and use of these skills by parents in managing their Armstrong, & Booraem, 1986; Shure & Spivack, 1982; Weiss-
94 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

berg & Caplan, 1989). Others have found no effects on adjust- shown positive effects on the risk factors of academic achieve-
ment (Allen, Chinsky, Larcen, Lochman, &Selinger, 1976). ment and problem behaviors in school and thus hold promise
There is promising evidence for social competence promo- for preventing drug abuse. The strategies include early child-
tion as a drug abuse prevention strategy. Social competence hood education, as previously discussed (Berrueta-Clement et
promotion has shown a significant impact both on students' al., 1984), alterations in classroom teachers' instructional prac-
willingness to try nondrug or nonalcohol options when con- tices in elementary and middle schools (Hawkins, Doueck, &
fronted with problem situations and on the certainty of their Lishner, 1988; Hawkins & Lam, 1987), and academic tutoring
intention not to use drugs or alcohol, when compared with a of low achievers (Coie & Krehbiel, 1984). The latter two are
matched control group (Ketchel & Bieger, 1989). Kim, discussed here:
McLeod, and Palmgren (1989) evaluated the effects of an inter- a. Alterations in classroom instructional practices. The use
vention in 4th grade on students in Grades 5 through 12. The of certain methods of instruction in classrooms has been
program consisted of one session a week for 9 weeks focusing shown in experimental and quasi-experimental studies to im-
on social skills. In the last session, students applied the skills prove achievement and social bonding to school and to reduce
learned to drug use choices. Across all grade levels, students student misbehavior. A number of studies have linked achieve-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

showed significantly lower use of alcohol, cigarettes, and mari- ment gains to the amount of active instruction and direct super-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

juana compared with students who had not participated in the vision of learning efforts that teachers provide to students
program. The largest impact was found in Grades 5 through 7; (Brophy & Good, 1986). Classroom teachers' use of a package
the intervention's positive effects declined rapidly at 9th grade. of instructional methods consisting of interactive teaching,
Lochman (1988) provided an anger management program proactive classroom management, and cooperative learning re-
during school hours for boys identified as aggressive by their sulted in significantly greater achievement gains in math and in
teachers. The program included role playing, goal setting, vid- levels of commitment to school and in significantly lower rates
eotaped modeling to develop self-statements, social problem of suspensions and expulsions from school among urban sev-
solving skills to cope with anger arousal, and group-produced enth-grade students in experimental classrooms when com-
videotapes illustrating alternative ways of coping with an pared with control classrooms (Hawkins & Lam, 1987).
anger-arousing situation. When compared with a matched Cooperative learning methods have been included in some
(though not randomly assigned) comparison group of untreated classroom interventions seeking to enhance achievement and
aggressive boys, the treated subjects were found to have signifi- commitment to school. An intervention targeting children
cantly lower rates of alcohol and marijuana use as well as fewer starting preschool attempted to bring all children in the study
negative consequences of alcohol use at age 14,3 years after the up to grade level by third grade (Slavin, Madden, Karweit,
intervention. Liverman, & Dolan, 1990). The program focused on language
This is an important area for additional prevention research. development, academic readiness, and improved self-concept
If school-based social competence promotion strategies reliably with preschool and kindergartners. In Grades 1 through 3, the
reduce aggressive and other problem behaviors as well as fre- intervention replaced pull-out programs and special education
quent and problem drug use during adolescence, they represent classes with in-class tutors and alternative classroom strategies,
a viable prevention strategy. including grouping of students across grades by ability. The
A question for future research is the age of children for whom program's effects were evaluated by comparison with a
such interventions are most effective. To illustrate, from about matched school and with individually matched students within
age 5, aggressiveness in boys predicts later deviance including the control school. The results showed significantly higher test
drug abuse (Kellam & Brown, 1982). Few boys appear to be- scores for intervention students at all grade levels.
come seriously aggressive if they did not manifest aggressive Freiberg, Brady, Swank, and Taylor (1989) found positive re-
behavior in childhood. However, aggression declines in preva- sults on academic achievement from a program combining co-
lence with age. Some boys desist from aggressive behavior be- operative learning strategies with improved classroom manage-
tween the ages of 5 and 14. Social competence promotion strate- ment, student and teacher motivation, parent contacts, interac-
gies that seek to reduce aggressiveness among young boys run tive instruction, and discipline prevention. Students in five
the risk of false-positive error, in that they may target for inter- target schools were compared with the students of five matched
vention some youngsters who will not show later deviant out- controls. At the end of the M-year intervention, students in the
comes (Loeber & Dishion, 1983). Conversely, programs that test schools surpassed the control students on standardized test
wait to intervene until aggression has crystallized as a pattern scores in every subject.
of behavior may minimize false-positive errors in identifying In the most effective cooperative-learning approaches, stu-
those at high risk for later drug abuse, but these interventions dents of differing abilities and backgrounds work together in
may have less chance of successfully eliminating the aggressive small groups of 4 to 6 children to master the curriculum mate-
behavior and other problems of adjustment and achievement rial, and they receive recognition as a team for the performance
produced by aggressive behavior during the elementary school of all members of the group. Cooperative-learning strategies
grades. Lochman's (1988) research, which showed lower rates of have been designed to encourage students to help and support
drug involvement at 3-year follow-up among aggressive boys peers of diverse ability, ethnicity, and background toward the
who received problem-solving-skills training at age 11, did not achievement of academic success.
show similar positive effects on aggressive behavior or general Controlled studies have shown positive effects of cooperative
behavioral deviance at 3-year follow-up. learning on achievement and attitudes toward school and peers
4. Academic achievement promotion. Three strategies have (DeVries & Slavin, 1978; Dolan, Kellam, & Brown, 1989; Mad-
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 95

den & Slavin, 1983; Ziegler, 1981) and, in combination with ing, reading, and test-taking programs and career exploration,
other instructional methods, have produced reductions in rates and (d) direct services to targeted high-risk students to increase
of suspension and expulsion from school among low achievers academic involvement and achievement, including individual
(Hawkins, Doueck, & Lishner, 1988). However, the use of the treatment plans, behavioral objectives, and monthly monitor-
"Jigsaw" cooperative learning method did not prevent drug use ing by specialists.
in the Napa Project (Schaps, Moskowitz, Malvin, & Schaeffer, High-risk students in participating schools were randomly
1986). assigned to treatment and control groups. At the end of 3 years
Cooperative learning methods hold promise for changing of intervention, students in experimental schools reported
peer influence patterns in schools, for reducing academic fail- lower rates of drug use, delinquent behavior, and alienation and
ure, and for increasing commitment to school and attachment higher rates of attachment to school, educational expectations,
to prosocial others. Given these effects on risk factors for drug and belief in school rules when compared with students in com-
abuse, cooperative learning should be investigated further for parison schools. However, the direct services for targeted high-
drug abuse prevention effects in spite of the apparent lack of risk students did not produce significant effects on risk factors
effect of the "Jigsaw" method in the Napa Project. or behavior (D. C. Gottfredson, 1986).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

b. Tutoring. Individual tutoring for low achievers with behav- Comer (1988) demonstrated positive gains in student aca-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ior problems has been used in conjunction with, as well as demic achievement over a 12-year period after the creation and
separately from, social competence skills training approaches support of school governance and management teams in two
discussed earlier (Coie & Dodge, 1988). Coie and Krehbiel New Haven schools serving predominantly low-income Afri-
(1984) found that even without social skills training, tutoring of can-American populations. The teams consisted of principal,
socially rejected, low-achieving fourth graders reduced peer re- parents, teachers, and a mental health worker and developed
jection and disruptive behavior in the classroom and produced and implemented comprehensive school plans for academics,
significant improvements in reading and math achievement. social activities, and special programs. Consistent schoolwide
The existing evidence suggests that both improvements in gains were achieved in scores on standardized reading and
methods of instruction used by teachers in mainstream math tests in comparison with national norms, though control
classrooms and individualized tutoring programs hold promise or comparison schools were not used.
for reducing academic failure and problem behaviors of chil- Felner and his colleagues (Felner, Adan, & Evans, 1987;
dren. Both types of intervention with children in elementary Felner, Ginter, & Primavera, 1982; Felner, Weissberg, & Adan,
and junior high school grades should be studied for effects in 1987) have altered the school environment for students making
preventing adolescent drug abuse. normal transitions from elementary to middle or junior high
5. Organizational changes in schools. Schools with the high- schools or from these to high schools. The intervention kept
est rates of student misbehavior and drug abuse are typically groups of transitioning students together in homeroom and
"demoralized" organizations (G. D. Gottfredson, 1988). Such core courses in circumscribed schools-within-a-school, in
schools are likely to have great difficulty implementing strate- which all homeroom and core teachers' classrooms were within
gies such as improved classroom teaching, tutoring programs, close proximity to one another. In addition, the homeroom
or parental involvement in promoting students' classroom per- teacher served as an advocate-counselor for all students in his
formance. Because school organizational characteristics ap- or her homeroom, contacting each homeroom student's family
pear to be related to student behavior, achievement, and bond- before the school year began and serving as a counseling link
ing to school and because they influence the ability of schools for homeroom students, their parents, and the rest of the school
to implement changes that might prevent drug abuse, they are (Felner & Adan, 1988). The intervention produced positive ef-
factors that should be considered potential targets for drug fects on academic performance, absenteeism, and school drop
abuse prevention efforts. out when participating students were compared with nonpartic-
There is evidence that school organizational factors can be ipating students in the same schools.
changed to reduce drug abuse risk factors (Comer, 1988; D. C. These results indicate that altering the organizational char-
Gottfredson, 1986,1988). In one school (D. C. Gottfredson & acteristics of schools can reduce risk factors for drug abuse as
Cook, 1986a, 1986b) a program of curriculum restructuring, well as drug use itself. Organizational change in school manage-
increased opportunities for student involvement, greater ment and the school environment should be further investi-
school-faculty-community integration, and changes in school gated for drug abuse prevention effects.
discipline procedures resulted in significant changes. Despite 6. Youth involvement in alternative activities. Activities in the
implementation problems resulting from district staff cuts, school setting that provide opportunities for youths to partici-
comparison with a control school showed increases in positive pate in contributing—roles such as involvement in school gov-
self-concept, attachment to school, and belief in rules as well as ernment, experience-based career education programs in
higher standardized test scores in math. Significant decreases which students are provided hands-on opportunities to learn
were found in the school's rates of delinquency, drug use, and about the world of work, and tutoring programs in which stu-
suspensions. dents are enlisted to help other students—have been hypothe-
In another study, D. C. Gottfredson (1986) evaluated an inter- sized to increase commitment to school and reduce alienation
vention that included (a) the establishment of an organizational and rebelliousness. When such activities have been provided
structure to facilitate shared decision making and management for adolescents, the emphasis has been on active involvement
in schools, (b) the use of curriculum and student concerns spe- (i.e., shifting the student's role from consumer of information to
cialists, (c) academic innovations including cooperative learn- producer of some benefit). From a risk-focused perspective, this
96 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

strategy might be expected to increase commitment to school duced significantly lower rates of school suspension and expul-
and to reduce the likelihood of violation of school standards of sion among seventh-grade experimental subjects (Hawkins,
behavior, including proscriptions against drug use. Doueck, & Lishner, 1988), significantly lower prevalence of
Similarly, it has been suggested that physically challenging early aggression among second-grade subjects (Hawkins, Von
risk-taking activities, such as those offered by Outward Bound Cleve, & Catalano, 1991), and significantly lower prevalence of
programs, might provide effective drug-free alternatives to self-reported delinquency (-6.7%) and alcohol use (-6.6%) by
those at risk for drug use by virtue of the personal characteris- Grade 5 among children exposed to the comprehensive pro-
tics of high novelty seeking and low harm avoidance. gram in Grades 1-4 in comparison with controls (Hawkins et
Evidence for the effectiveness of such approaches in prevent- al., in press). The intervention consisted of (a) teacher training
ing drug abuse is mixed. Neither cross-age tutoring nor operat- in methods of classroom management and instruction consis-
ing a school store prevented drug use among predominantly tent with the principles of the social development model, (b)
White middle-class students in eighth and ninth grades in the training for parents in developmentally adjusted curricula fo-
Napa Project (Schaps et al., 1986). On the other hand, there is cused on development of family management skills consistent
some evidence that when delivered at high intensity, alternative with the social development principles, and (c) involvement of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

programs that empower high-risk subjects to master new skills student subjects in classroom-based peer teaching and skill de-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

are associated with improved behavior and achievement velopment. The results suggest that by promoting consistent
(Tobler, 1986). Programs such as Outward Bound, which pro- opportunities and expectations for prosocial behavior at home
vide risk-taking challenges as opportunities to learn skills, and school, by enhancing skill development, using peer involve-
could be enhanced by continuation interventions designed to ment and teaching and parent monitoring, and by stressing
build mastery over the environments in which youths routinely positive reinforcements for prosocial involvement from family,
function. These latter approaches should be investigated fur- school, and peers, the incidence of early initiation of drug use
ther for drug abuse prevention effects. and delinquency can be reduced. Analysis indicates that these
7. Comprehensive risk-focused programs. Because drug outcomes are accompanied by effects on family- and school-
abuse is a phenomenon influenced by multiple risk factors, its bonding variables that are hypothesized in the theory on which
prevention may be most effectively accomplished by a combina- the comprehensive program is based (Hawkins et al., in press).
tion of interventions promoting consistent prevention princi-
ples across units of socialization. Comprehensive prevention
Summary
programs that combine multiple interventions focused on dif-
ferent sources of social influence have shown beneficial effects A risk-focused approach to drug abuse prevention holds
on smoking (Puska et al., 1982). promise for identifying effective prevention strategies. Imple-
Pentz and her colleagues (Pentz, Dwyer, et al., 1989) have menting and testing approaches that seek to reduce or buffer
tested a multicomponent communitywide program involving a the effects of known antecedents of adolescent drug abuse will
curriculum of social influence resistance skills training for stu- increase our knowledge of which are causally related to drug
dents in Grades 6 or 7 that includes (a) homework assignments abuse and what prevention strategies reliably address these risk
to be conducted with parents, (b) booster sessions in the year factors.
after initial intervention, (c) organizational and training oppor- Research has identified these antecedents of adolescent drug
tunities for parents in positive parent-child communication abuse: laws and norms favorable toward drug use; availability
skills and in reviewing school policies, (d) training of commu- of drugs; extreme economic deprivation; neighborhood disor-
nity leaders to organize drug abuse prevention task forces, and ganization; certain physiological characteristics; early and per-
(e) news coverage. The multicomponent program produced sistent behavior problems including aggressive behavior in
lower prevalence rates of weekly cigarette (-8%), alcohol (—4%), boys, other conduct problems, and hyperactivity in childhood
and marijuana (-3%) use after the 2nd-year intervention (Pentz, and adolescence; a family history of alcoholism and parental
Dwyer, et al., 1989) and significantly lower prevalence of use of illegal drugs; poor family management practices; family
monthly cigarette (-6%) and marijuana (—3%) use 3 years after conflict; low bonding to family; academic failure; lack of com-
the initial school intervention, though the prevalence of alcohol mitment to school; early peer rejection; social influences to use
use was not significantly reduced at this measurement point drugs; alienation and rebelliousness; attitudes favorable to drug
(C. A. Johnson et al., 1989). The comprehensive intervention use; and the early initiation of drug use. There is some evidence
appears to have been equally effective in lowering tobacco and that certain factors including personal attributes and a social
marijuana use prevalence among those at risk because of expo- bond to conventional society may protect against drug abuse,
sure to parental drug use, drug-using peers, and early initiation though more research is needed to determine the relationships
of use (C. A. Johnson et al., 1989). Although the unique contri- between risk and protective factors as related to adolescent drug
bution of each individual component in this comprehensive abuse.
program has not been determined, the results indicate that a Evidence from studies of the etiology of adolescent drug
multiple-component strategy focused on reducing risks is more abuse suggests that a viable prevention model would include
effective in reducing drug use prevalence than is mass-media simultaneous attention to a number of risk factors in different
coverage alone. social domains to be addressed during the developmental pe-
Similarly, research on a comprehensive teacher-, parent-, and riod when each begins to stabilize as a predictor of subsequent
peer-focused prevention program grounded in the social devel- drug abuse. The evidence further suggests that prevention ef-
opment model has shown that the comprehensive program pro- forts target populations at greatest risk of drug abuse because of
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 97

their exposure to a large number of risk factors during develop- vention with delinquent families: Impact on family process and re-
ment. A theory of adolescent drug abuse that accounts for the cidivism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 18, 219-225.
existing empirical evidence regarding risk and protective fac- Allen, G. J., Chinsky, J. M., Larcen, S. W, Lochman, J. E, & Selinger, J.
tors for adolescent substance abuse should be used to organize V (1976). Community psychology and the schools: A behaviorally ori-
and integrate the complex work of developing and testing pre- ented multilevel preventive approach Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
vention interventions. Asher, S. R., & Renshaw, P. D. (1981). Children without friends: Social
Most drug abuse prevention efforts have addressed two risk knowledge and social-skill training. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman
(Eds.), The development of children's friendships (pp. 273-296). New
factors for adolescent drug abuse: laws and norms favorable to
\brk: Cambridge University Press.
drug use and social influences to use drugs. These efforts in-
Atkin, C, Hocking, J., & Block, M. (1984). Teenage drinking: Does
clude supply manipulation, interdiction and enforcement strate-
advertising make a difference? Journal of Communication, 34, 157-
gies, efforts to change social norms regarding drug use, and 167.
social influence resistance skills training. Of these approaches, Bachman, J. G, Lloyd, D. J., & O'Malley, P. M. (1981). Smoking, drink-
social influence resistance strategies have been evaluated most ing, and drug use among American high school students: Correlates
extensively for preventive effects in controlled studies. and trends, 1975-1979. American Journal of Public Health, 71, 59-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Several available studies of social influence resistance skills 69.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

training for drug abuse prevention have produced short-term Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav-
effects on rates of drug initiation, including reductions in smok- ioral change. Psychological Review, 84,191-215.
ing and, in a few cases, in alcohol and marijuana use. Although Barnes, G. M., & Welte, J. W (1986). Patterns and predictors of alcohol
such results are promising, the limits of these programs should use among 7-12th grade students in New 'York State. Journal of Stud-
be considered. Peer influence resistance skills training meth- ies on Alcohol, 47, 53-62.
ods do not change the basic developmental conditions experi- Battistich, V, Solomon, D, Watson, M., & Schaps, E. (n.d.) Effects of an
enced by children. Although these methods have shown short- elementary school program to enhance prosocial behavior on chil-
term effects on the incidence of drug initiation in the general dren's cognitive social problem-solving skills and strategies. Unpub-
lished manuscript, San Ramon, Developmental Studies Center.
population, they may have little effect on drug abuse among
Baum, C. G, & Forehand, R. (1981). Long-term follow-up assessment
higher risk groups. Children who are at highest risk for adoles-
of parent training by use of multiple outcome measures. Behavior
cent drug abuse by virtue of poor family management, early Therapy, 12, 643-652.
and persistent behavior problems, low bonding to family, aca- Baumrind, D. (1983, October). Why adolescents take chances—Andwhy
demic failure, and low commitment to school may be unmoti- they don't. Paper presented at the National Institute for Child Health
vated to refuse or avoid drug use by late childhood. and Human Development, Bethesda, MD.
If the goal is to reduce drug abuse and its accompanying Berrueta-Clement, J. R., Schweinhart, L. J., Barnett, W S., Epstein,
social and health problems among children at high risk, it is A. S., & Weikhart, D. P. (1984). Changed lives: The effects of'the Perry
important to test preventive approaches that have successfully Preschool Program on youths through age 19. Ypsilanti, MI: High/
addressed risk factors present earlier in child development. Scope Press.
Promising risk-focused approaches that should be investigated Bien, N. Z., & Bry, B. H. (1980). An experimentally designed compari-
for drug abuse prevention effects are early childhood education son of four intensities of school-based prevention programs for ado-
and early family support, parent training, school-based social lescents with adjustment problems. Journal of Community Psychol-
competence promotion, school-based academic competence ogy, 8, 110-116.
promotion, and school organizational change strategies. Re- Bierman, K. L., & Furman, W (1984). The effects of social skills train-
cent studies indicate that coherent multiple-component or ing and peer involvement in the social adjustment of preadolescents.
Child Development, 55, 151-162.
comprehensive strategies, including but not limited to social
Bierman, K. L., Miller, C. L., & Stabb, S. D. (1987). Improving the
influence resistance, hold significant promise for preventing
social and peer acceptance of rejected boys: Effects of social skill
drug abuse and its attendant costs. These approaches should be training with instructions and prohibitions. Journal of Consulting
implemented in varying combinations and settings, and their and Clinical Psychology, 55,194-200.
effects on the initiation, use, and abuse of drugs should be stud- Biglan, A., & Ary, D. V (1985). Current methodological issues in re-
ied further in controlled field experiments. search on smoking prevention. In C. Bell & R. J. Battjes (Eds.), Pre-
vention research: Deterring drug abuse among children and adoles-
cents (pp. 170-195) (NIDA Research Monograph No. 63). Washing-
References ton, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Biglan, A., Glasgow, R., Ary, D., Thompson, R., Severson, H., Lichen-
Adams, E. H., Blanken, A. J., Ferguson, L. EX, & Kopstein, A. (1990). stein, E., Weissman, W, Faller, C, &Callison, C. (1987). How general-
Overview of selected drug trends. Rockville, MD: National Institute izable are the effects of smoking prevention programs? Refusal skills
on Drug Abuse. training and parent messages in a teacher administered program.
Ahmed, S. W, Bush, P. J., Davidson, E R., & lannotti, R. J. (1984, Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 613-627.
November). Predicting children's use and intentions to use abusable Biglan, A., Severson, H., Ary, D, Faller, C., Gallison, C, Thompson, R.,
substances. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Glasgow, R., & Lichtenstein, E. (1987). Do smoking prevention pro-
Public Health Association, Anaheim, CA. grams real ly work? Attrition and the internal and external validity of
Akers, R. L., Krohn, M. D., Lanza-Kaduce, L., & Radosevich, M. an evaluation of a refusal skills training program. Journal of Behav-
(1979). Social learning and deviant behavior: A specific test of a ioral Medicine, 10, 159-171.
general theory. American Sociological Review, 44, 636-655. Black, G. S. (1989). The attitudinal basis of 'drug use—1987 and chang-
Alexander, J. E, & Parsons, B. V (1973). Short-term behavioral inter- ing attitudes toward drug use—1988: Reports from the Media Adver-
98 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

Using Partnership for a Drug-Free America, Inc. Rochester, NY: Gor- ment. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching(3r'd
don S. Black. ed., pp. 328-375). New York: MacMillan.
Blechman, E. A., Taylor, C J.,&Schrader, S. M.(I981). Family problem Brunswick, A. F, & Boyle, J. M. (1979). Patterns of drug involvement:
solving versus home notes as early intervention with high-risk chil- Developmental and secular influences on age at initiation. Youth and
dren. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 919-926. Society, 11, 139-162.
Block, 1, Block, J. H., & Keyes, S. (1988). Longitudinally foretelling Bry, B. H. (1982). Reducing the incidence of adolescent problems
drug usage in adolescence: Early childhood personality and environ- through preventive intervention: One- and five-year followup. Amer-
mental precursors. Child Development, 59, 336-355. ican Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 265-276.
Blose, J. O., & Holder, H. D. (1987). Liquor-by-the-drink and alcohol- Bry, B. H. (1983). Empirical foundations of family-based approaches to
related traffic crashes: A natural experiment using time-series analy- adolescent substance abuse. In T. J. Glynn, C. G. Leukefeld, & J. P.
sis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 52-60. Ludford (Eds.), Preventing adolescent drug abuse: Intervention strate-
Blum, K., Noble, E. P., Sheridan, P. J., Montgomery, A., Ritchie, X, gies (pp. 154-171; NIDA Research Monograph No. 47). Washing-
Jagadeeswonan, P., Nogami, H., Briggs, A. H., &Cohen, J. B. (1990). ton, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Allelic association of human dopamine D2 receptor gene in alcohol- Bry, B. H., McKeon, P., & Pandina, R. J. (1982). Extent of drug use as a
ism. Journal of the American Medical Association, 263, 2094-2095. function of number of risk factors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Blum, R. (1987). Contemporary threats to adolescent health in the 91, 273-279.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 257, Bukoski, W J. (1986). School-based substance abuse prevention: A re-
3390-3395. view of program research. In S. Griswold-Ezekoye, K. L. Kumpfer, &
Bohman, M. (1978). Some genetic aspects of alcoholism and criminal- W J. Bukoski (Eds.), Childhood and chemical abuse (pp. 95-115).
ity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 269-276. New York: Haworth.
Botvin, G. J. (1986). Substance abuse prevention research: Recent de- Bursik, R. J., Jr. (in press). The use of contextual analysis in models of
velopments and future directions. Journal of School Health, 56, 369- criminal behavior. In J. D. Hawkins (Ed.), Some current theories of
374. delinquency and crime. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Botvin, G. J., Baker, E., Filazzola, A. D., & Botvin, E. M. (1990). A Bursik, R. J., Jr., & Webb, J. (1982). Community change and patterns of
cognitive-behavioral approach to substance abuse prevention: One- delinquency. American Journal of Sociology, 88, 24-42.
year follow-up. Addictive Behaviors, 15, 47-63.
Bush, P. 1, Zuckerman, A. E., Theiss, P. K., Taggart, V S., Horowitz, C,
Botvin, G. J., Baker, E., Renick, N. L., Filazzola, A. D., & Botvin, E. M. Sheridan, M. J., & Walter, H. J. (1989). Cardiovascular risk factor
(1984). A cognitive-behavioral approach to substance abuse preven- prevention in Black schoolchildren: Two-year results of the "Know
tion. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 137-147. Your Body" program. American Journal of Epidemiology, 129, 466-
Botvin, G. J., Batson, H. W, Witts-Vitale, S., Bess, V, Baker, E., & Du- 482.
senbury, L. (1989). A psychosocial approach to smoking prevention
Byram, O. W, & Fly, J. W (1984). Family structure, race and adoles-
for urban Black youth. Public Health Reports, 104, 573-582. cent's alcohol use: A research note. American Journal of Drug and
Botvin, G. J., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Personal and social skills training: Alcohol Abuse, 10, 467-478.
Cognitive-behavioral approaches to substance abuse prevention. In
Cadoret, R. J., Cain, C. A., & Grove, W M. (1980). Development of
C. Bell & R. J. Battjes (Eds.), Prevention research: Deterring drug
alcoholism in adoptees raised apart from alcoholic biologic rela-
abuse among children and adolescents (pp. 8-49) (NIDA Research
tives. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 561-563.
Monograph No. 63). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
Cadoret, R. J., & Gath, A. (1978). Inheritance of alcoholism in adop-
Office.
tees. British Journal of Addiction, 132, 252-258.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Basic Books. Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D, Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. D, & Gairepy,
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation-anxiety and J-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer support or
anger. New York: Basic Books. peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24, 815-823.
Braucht, G. N, Kirby, M. W, & Berry, G. J. (1978). Psychosocial corre- Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (1986). The social development model:
lates of empirical types of multiple drug abusers. Journal of Consult- A theory of antisocial behavior. Paper presented at the Safeco Lecture-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 46, 1463-1475. ship on Crime and Delinquency, University of Washington, Seattle.
Bronson, M. B., Pierson, D. E., & Tivnan, T. (1984). The effects of early Clayton, R. R. (in press). Legalization: An idea whose time has not
education on children's competence in elementary school. Evalua- come. American Behavioral Scientist.
tion Review, 8, 615-629. Cloninger, C. R., Bohman, M., Sigvardsson, S., & von Knorring, A. L.
Brook, J. S., Brook, D. W, Gordon, A. S,, Whiteman, M., & Cohen, P. (1985). Psychopathology in adopted-out children of alcoholics: The
(1990). The psychosocial etiology of adolescent drug use: A family Stockholm Adoption Study. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 3,
interactional approach. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology 37-51.
Monographs, 116 (Whole No. 2). Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., & Bohman, M. (1988). Childhood
Brook, J. S., Gordon, A. S., Whiteman, M., & Cohen, P. (1986). Some personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcoholism, 12,
models and mechanisms for explaining the impact of maternal and 494-503.
adolescent characteristics on adolescent stage of drug use. Develop- Coie, J. D. (1990). Towards a theory of peer rejection. In S. R. Asher&
mental Psychology, 22, 460-467. J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood(pp. 365-398). New York:
Brook, J. S., Lukoff, I. F., & Whiteman, M. (1980). Initiation into adoles- Cambridge University Press.
cent marijuana use. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137,133-142. Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1988). Multiple sources of data on social
Brook, J. S., Whiteman, M., Gordon, A. S., & Brook, D. W (1988). The behavior and social status in the school: A cross-age comparison.
role of older brothers in younger brothers' drug use viewed in the Child Development, 59, 815-829.
context of parent and peer influences. Journal of Genetic Psychology, Coie, J. D, & Krehbiel, G. (1984). Effects of academic tutoring on the
151, 59-75. social statusof low-achieving, socially rejected children. Child Devel-
Brophy, J., & Good, T. L. (1986). Teacher behavior and student achieve- opment, 55, 1465-1478.
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 99

Comer, J. P. (1988). Educating poor minority children. Scientific Ameri- crime. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child
can, 259, 42-48. psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 283-342). New York: Plenum Press.
Cook, P. J., & Tauchen, G. (1982). The effect of liquor taxes on heavy Feldhusen, J. F, Thurston, J. R., & Benning, J. J. (1973). A longitudinal
drinking. Bell Journal of Economics, 13, 379-390. study of delinquency and other aspects of children's behaviour. Inter-
Cook, P. J., & Tauchen, G. (1984). The effect of minimum drinking age national Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1, 341-351.
legislation on youthful auto fatalities, 1970-1977. Journal of Legal Felner, R. D, & Adan, A. M. (1988). The school transitional environ-
Studies, 13,169-190. ment project: An ecological intervention and evaluation. In R. H.
Cotton, N. S. (1979). The familial incidence of alcoholism: A review. Price, E. L. Cowen, R. P. Lorion, & J. Ramos-McKay, (Eds.), 14
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 40, 89-116. Ounces of Prevention: A Casebook for Practitioners (pp. 111-122).
Cowen, E., & Work, W (1988). Resilient children, psychological well- Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
ness, and primary prevention. American Journal of Community Psy- Felner, R. D., Adan, A. A., & Evans, E. (1987). Evaluation of school-
chology, 16, 591-607. based primary prevention programs. Unpublished manuscript.
Decker, M. D., Graitcer, P. L., & Schaffner, W (1988). Reduction in Felner, R. D, Ginter, M. A., & Primavera, J. (1982). Primary preven-
motor vehicle fatalities associated with an increase in the minimum tion during school transitions: Social support and environmental
drinking age. Journal of the American Medical Association, 260, structure. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10,227-240.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

3604-3610. Felner, R. D, Weissberg, R. P., & Adan, A. M. (1987). Long-term follow-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

DeMarsh, J., & Kumpfer, K. L. (1986). Family-oriented interventions up of a school transition program. Unpublished manuscript.
for the prevention of chemical dependency in children and adoles- Flasher, L. Y, & Maisto, S. A. (1984). A review of theory and research
cents. In S. Griswold-Ezekoye, K. L. Kumpfer, & W J. Bukoski (Eds.), on drinking patterns among Jews. The Journal of Nervous and Mental
Childhood and chemical abuse: Prevention and intervention (pp. 117- Disease, 772,596-603.
151). New York: Haworth. Flay, B. R. (1985). Psychosocial approaches to smoking prevention: A
Dembo, R., Farrow, D., Schmeidler, J., & Burgos, W (1979). Testing a review of findings. Health Psychology, 5, 449-488.
causal model of environmental influences on early drug involve- Flay, B. R., Koepke, D, Thomson, S. J., Santi, S., Best, J. A., & Brown,
ment of inner city junior high school youths. American Journal of K. S. (1989). Six-year follow-up of the first Waterloo school smoking
Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 6, 313-336. prevention trial. American Journal of Public Health, 79,1371-1376.
DeVries, D. L., & Slavin, R. E. (1978). Teams-games-tournament Fleischman, M. J. (1981). A replication of Patterson's "Intervention for
(TGT): Review often classroom experiments. Journal of Research Boys with Conduct Problems." Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and Development in Education, 12, 28-38. Psychology, 49, 342-351.
Fleming, J. P., Kellam, S. G., & Brown, C. H. (1982). Early predictors of
Dishion, T. 1, Kavanagh, K, & Reid, J. B. (1989, November). Child-
rearing vs. peer interventions in the reduction of risk for adolescent age at first use of alcohol, marijuana and cigarettes. Drug and Alco-
hol Dependence, 9, 285-303.
substanceuse and adjustment problems: A secondary prevention strat-
egy. Paper presented at the Conference for the Advancement of Ap- Fraser, M. W, Hawkins, J. D., & Howard, M. O. (1988). Parent training
plied Behavior Therapy, Washington, DC. for delinquency prevention. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Freiberg, H. J., Brady, M. P, Swank, P. R., & Taylor, R. D. (1989). Mid-
Dolan, L., Kellam, S., & Brown, C. H. (1989). Short term impact of a
mastery learning preventive intervention on early risk behaviors. Balti- dle school interaction study of mainstreamed students. Journal of
Classroom Interaction, 24, 31-42.
more: Johns Hopkins University.
Friedman, A. S. (1983, July). High school drug abuse clients. In Clinical
Dwyer, J. H., MacKinnon, D. V, Pentz, M. A., Flay, B. R., Hansen,
research notes. Rockville, MD: Division of Clinical Research, Na-
W B., Wang, E. Y. I., & Johnson, C. A. (1989). Estimating interven-
tional Institute on Drug Abuse.
tion effects in longitudinal studies. American Journal of Epidemiol-
Garmezy, N. (1985). Stress-resistant children: The search for protective
ogy, 7JO, 781-795.
factors. In J. E. Stevenson (Ed.), Recent research in developmental
Ellickson, P. L., & Bell, R. M. (1990). Drug prevention in juniorhigh: A
psychopathology(pp. 113-233). Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
multi-site longitudinal test. Science, 247,1299-1305.
chiatry, 4 (Book suppl.).
Ellickson, P., Bell, R. M., Thomas, M. A., Robyn, A. E., & Zellman, Gersick, K. E., Grady, K., & Snow, D. L. (1988). Social-cognitive skill
G. L. (1988). Designing and implementing Project ALERT. Santa development with sixth graders and its initial impact on substance
Monica, CA: The Rand Corporation. use. Journal of Drug Education, 18, 55-70.
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D, & Ageton, S. S. (1985). Explaining delin- Gesten, E. L., Rains, M., Rapkin, B. D, Weissberg, R. P., Flores de
quency and drug use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Apodaca, R., Cowen, E. L., & Bowen, R. (1982). Training children in
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D, & Menard, S. (1989). Multiple problem social problem-solving skills: A competence building approach,
youth: Delinquency, substance use and mental health problems. New first and second look. American Journal of Community Psychology,
York: Springer-Verlag. 70,95-115.
Fagan, J. (1988). The social organization of drug use and drug dealing Gittelman, R. S., Mannuzza, R. S., & Bonagura, N. (1985). Hyperactive
among urban gangs. New York: John Jay College of Criminal Justice. boys almost grown up: I. Psychiatric status. Archives of General Psy-
Farrington, D. P., Gallagher, B., Morley, L., Ledger, R. J., & West, D. J. chiatry, 42, 937-947.
(1985). Cambridge study in delinquent development: Long term fol- Goodwin, D. W (1985). Alcoholism and genetics: The sins of the fa-
low-up (First annual report to the home office, August 31, 1985). thers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42,171-174.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University. Goodwin, D. W, Schulsinger, F., Moller, N., Hermansen, L., Winokur,
Farrington, D. P., & Hawkins, J. D. (1991). Predicting participation, G, & Guze, S. B. (1974). Drinking problems in adopted and non-
early onset, and later persistence in officially recorded offending. adopted sons of alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 164-
Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 1,1-33. 169.
Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., Elliott, D. S., Hawkins, J. D., Kandel, Goodwin, D. W, Schulsinger, F, Moller, N., Mednick, S., & Guze, S.
D. B., Klein, M. W, McCord, J., Rowe, D. C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1977). Psychopathology in adopted and nonadopted daughters of
(1990). Advancing knowledge about the onset of delinquency and alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 1005-1007.
100 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

Gorsuch, R. L., & Butler, M. C. (1976). Initial drug abuse: A review of to citizens: Families, schools, and delinquency prevention (Vol. 3, pp.
predisposing social psychological factors. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 186-204). New York: Springer-Verlag.
120-137. Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. E, Morrison, D. M., O'Donnell, J., Abbott,
Gottfredson, D. C. (1986). An empirical test of school-based environ- R. D, & Day, L. E. (in press). The Seattle Social Development Proj-
mental and individual interventions to reduce the risk of delinquent ect: Effects of the first four years on protective factors and problems
behavior. Criminology, 24, 705-731. behaviors. In J. McCord & R. Tremblay (Eds.), The prevention of
Gottfredson, D. C. (1988). An evaluation of an organization develop- antisocial behavior in children. New York: Guilford Press.
ment approach to reducing school disorder. Evaluation Review, 11, Hawkins, J. D, Doueck, H. J., & Lishner, D. M. (1988). Changing teach-
739-763. ing practices in mainstream classrooms to improve bonding and
Gottfredson, D. C., &Cook, M. S. (1986a). Increasing school relevance behavior of low achievers. American Educational Research Journal,
and student dedsionmaking: Effective strategies for reducing delin- 25,31-50.
quency? Unpublished manuscript, Johns Hopkins University, Center Hawkins, J. D, Jenson, J. M., Catalano, R. E, & Lishner, D. M. (1988).
for the Social Organization of Schools, Baltimore. Delinquency and drug abuse: Implications for social services. Social
Gottfredson, D. C., & Cook, M. S. (1986b). A test of a school-based Service Review, 62, 258-284.
program to reduce delinquency by increasing social integration: The Hawkins, J. D, & Lam, T. (1987). Teacher practices, social develop-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Milwood Alternative Education Project. Unpublished manuscript, ment, and delinquency. In J. D. Burchard & S. N. Burchard (Eds.),
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Johns Hopkins University, Center for the Social Organization of The prevention of delinquent behavior (pp. 241-274). Newbury Park,
Schools, Baltimore. CA: Sage.
Gottfredson, G. D. (1981). Schooling and delinquency. In S. E. Martin, Hawkins, J. D, Lishner, D, & Catalano, R. F. (1985). Childhood predic-
L. B. Sechrest, & R. Redner (Eds.), New Directions in the rehabilita- tors and the prevention of adolescent substance abuse. In C. L. Jones
tion of criminal offenders. Washington, DC: National Academy & R. J. Battjes(Eds-), Etiology of drug abuse: Implications for preven-
Press. tion (NIDA Research Monograph No. 56, DHHS Publication ADM
Gottfredson, G. D. (1988). Issues in adolescent drug use. Unpublished 85-1335, pp. 75-126). Washington, DC: US. Government Printing
final report to the U.S. Department of Justice, Johns Hopkins Uni- Office.
versity, Center for Research on Elementary and Middle Schools, Bal- Hawkins, J. D, Lishner, D. M., Catalano, R. E, & Howard, M. O. (1986).
timore. Childhood predictors of adolescent substance abuse: Toward an em-
Gotts, E. E. (1989). HOPE revisited: Preschool to graduation, reflections pirically grounded theory. Journal of Children in Contemporary Soci-
on parenting and school-family relations. Charleston, WV: Appala- ety, 8, 11-48.
chia Educational Lab. Hawkins, J. D, Lishner, D. M., Jenson, J. M., & Catalano, R. F. (1987).
Grady, K.., Kelin, E. G., & Boratynski, M. (1985). Preparing parents for Delinquents and drugs: What the evidence suggests about preven-
teenagers: A step in the prevention of adolescent substance abuse. tion and treatment programming. In B. S. Brown & A. R. Mills
Family Relations, 34, 541-549. (Eds.), Youth at high risk for substance abuse (DHHS Publication No.
Curling, H. M. D., Clifford, L. A., & Murray, R. M. (1981). Genetic ADM 87-1537, pp. 81-133). Washington, DC: U.S. Government
contribution to alcohol dependence and its effects in brain function. Printing Office.
In L. Gedder, P. Pinsi, & W A. Nance (Eds.), Twin research (pp. Hawkins, J. D, Von Cleve, E., & Catalano, R. F. (1991). Reducing early
77-87). New York: Alan R. Liss. childhood aggression: Results of a primary prevention program.
Hansen, W B., Graham, J. W, Sobel, J. L., Shelton, D. R., Flay, B. R., & Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Johnson, C. A. (1987). The consistency of peer and parent influences 30, 208-217.
on tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana use among young adolescents. Hawkins, J. D., & Weis, J. G. (1985). The social development model: An
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, JO, 559-579. integrated approach to delinquency prevention. Journal of Primary
Hansen, W B., Johnson, C. A., Flay, B. R., Graham, J. W, & Sobel, J. Prevention, 6, 73-97.
(1988). Affective and social influences approaches to the prevention Herting, J. R., & Guest, A. M. (1985). Components of satisfaction with
of multiple substance abuse among seventh grade students: Results local areas in the metropolis. The Sociological Quarterly, 26,99-115.
from Project SMART. Preventive Medicine, 17, 135-154. Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. (1979). Play and social inter-
Harada, S., Agarwal, D. P., Goedde, H. W, & Ishikawa, B. (1983). Alde- action in children following divorce. Journal of Social Issues, 35,
hyde, dehydrogenase isozyme variation and alcoholism in Japan. 26-49.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 18,151-153. Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes a/delinquency. Berkeley: University of Cali-
Harford, T. C. (1985). Drinking patterns among Black and non-Black fornia Press.
adolescents: Results of a national survey. In R. Wright & T. D. Watts Holder, H. D., & Blose, J. O. (1987). Impact of changes in distilled
(Eds.), Prevention of Black alcoholism: Issues and strategies. Spring- spirits availability on apparent consumption: A time series analysis
field, IL: Charles C Thomas. of liquor-by-the-drink. British Journal of Addiction, 82, 623-631.
Hartup, W W (1983). Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook Holmberg, M. B. (1985). Longitudinal studies of drug abuse in a fif-
of child psychology: Vol. IV. Socialization, personality, and social de- teen-year-old population: 1. Drug career. Ada Psychiatrica Scandin-
velopment. New York: Wiley. avica, 71, 67-79.
Hawkins, J. D., Abbott, R., Catalano, R. F., & Gillmore, M. R. (1991). Horacek, H. J., Ramey, C. T, Campbell, F. A., Hoffmann, K. P., &
Assessing effectiveness of drug abuse prevention: Long-term effects Fletcher, R. H. (1987). Predicting school failure and assessing early
and replication. In C. Leukefeld & W Bukoski (Eds.), Drug abuse intervention with high-risk children. Journal of the American Acad-
prevention research: Methodological issues (NIDA Research Mono- emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 758-763.
graph Series No. 107, DHHS Publication No. ADM 91-1761, pp. Hrubec, Z., & Omenn, G. S. (1981). Evidence of genetic predisposition
195-212). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. to alcoholic cirrhosis and psychosis: Twin concordances for alcohol-
Hawkins, J. D, Catalano, R. F, Jones, G., & Fine, D. N. (1987). Delin- ism and its biological end points by zygosity among male veterans.
quency prevention through parent training: Results and issues from Alcoholism, 5, 207-215.
work in progress. In J. G. Wi Ison, & G. C. Loury (Eds.), From children Hundleby, J. D., & Mercer, G. W. (1987). Family and friends as social
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 101

environments and their relationship to young adolescents' use of tion by parents and peers. International Journal of the Addictions, 22,
alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 319-342.
125-134. Kandel, D. B., & Davies, M. (1991). Cocaine use in a national sample of
Jessor, R. (1976). Predicting time of onset of marijuana use: A develop- US. youth (NLSY): Epidemiology, predictors, and ethnic patterns.
mental study of high school youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical In Theepidemiologyofcocaineuseandabuse(NlDA. research mono-
Psychology, 44,125-134. graph, DHHS Publication No. ADM 91-1787, pp. 151-188). Wash-
Jessor, R., Donovan, J. E., & Windmer, K. (1980). Psychosocial factors ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
in adolescent alcohol and drug use: Thel 980 National Sample Study, Kandel, D. B., Kessler, R. C, & Margulies, R. S. (1978). Antecedents of
and the 1974-78 Panel Study. Unpublished final report, University adolescent initiation into stages of drug use: A developmental analy-
of Colorado, Institute of Behavioral Science, Boulder. sis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7,13-40.
Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and psychosocial Kandel, D, Simcha-Fagan, O, & Davies, M. (1986). Risk factors for
development: A longitudinal study of youth. San Diego, CA: Aca- delinquency and illicit drug use from adolescence to young adult-
demic Press. hood. The Journal of Drug Issues, 16, 67-90.
Johnson, C. A., Pentz, M. A., Weber, M. D, Dwyer, J. H., MacKinnon, Kandel, D. B., Single, E., & Kessler, R. (1976). The epidemiology of
D. P., Flay, B. R., Baer, N. A., & Hansen, W B. (1989). The relative drug use among New York State High School students: Distribution,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

effectiveness of comprehensive community programming for drug trends, and change in rates of use. American Journal of Public Health,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

abuse with risk and low risk adolescents, Unpublished manuscript, 66, 43-53.
University of Southern California, Department for Prevention Re- Karoly, P., & Rosenthal, M. (1977). Training parents in behavior modi-
search, Pasadena. fication: Effects on perceptions of family interaction and deviant
Johnson, C. A., & Solis, J. (1983). Comprehensive community pro- child behavior. Behavior Therapy, 8, 406-410.
grams for drug abuse prevention: Implications of the community Kaufman, E., & Kaufman, P. N. (1979). Family therapy of drug and
heart disease prevention programs for future research. National Insti- alcohol abuse. New \brk: Gardner Press.
tute on Drug Abuse Monograph Series, 47, 76-114. Kazdin, A. E. (1990, June). Prevention of conduct disorder. Paper pre-
Johnson, G. M., Schoutz, F. C, & Locke, T. P. (1984). Relationships sented to the National Conference on Prevention Research, Na-
between adolescent drug use and parental drug behaviors. Adoles- tional Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland.
cence, 19, 295-299. Kellam, S. G., & Brown, H. (1982). Social adaptalional and psychologi-
Johnston, L. D. (1985). The etiology and prevention of substance use: cal antecedents of adolescent psychopathology ten years later. Balti-
What can we learn from recent historical changes? In C. L. Jones, & more: Johns Hopkins University.
R. J. Battjes (Eds.), Etiology of drug abuse: Implications for preven- Kellam, S. G., Ensminger, M. E., & Simon, M. B. (1980). Mental health
tion (NIDA Research Monograph No. 56, DHHS Publication No. in first grade and teenage drug, alcohol, and cigarette use. Drug and
ADM 85-1335, pp. 155-177). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Alcohol Dependence, 5, 273-304.
Printing Office. Kelly, D. H., & Balch, R. W (1971). Social origins and school failure: A
Johnston, L. D. (1991). Toward a theory of drug epidemics. In L. Dono- reexamination of Cohen's theory of working-class delinquency. Pa-
hew, H. E. Sypher, & W J. Bukoski (Eds.), Persuasive communication cific Social Review, 14, 413-430.
and drug abuse prevention (pp. 93-131). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ketchel, J. A., & Bieger, G. (1989, April). The efficacy of a psychoso-
Johnston, L. D., Bachman, J. G, & O'Malley, P. M. (1981). Student drug cially based drug prevention program for young adolescents. Paper
use in America 1975-1981 (DHHS Publication No. ADM 82-1208). presented at the New England Educational Research Corporation
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Annual Meeting, Portsmouth, NH.
Johnston, L. D, O'Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. (1985). Use of licit Kim, S. (1979). An evaluation of ombudsman primary prevention pro-
and illicit drugs by America's high school students. 1975-1984. Rock- gram on student drug abuse. Charlotte, NC: Charlotte Drug Educa-
ville, MD: National Institute of Drug Abuse. tion Center.
Johnston, L. D, O'Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. (1988). National Kim, S., McLeod, J., & Palmgren, C. L. (1989). The impact of the "I'm
trends in drug use and related factors among American high school Special" program on student substance abuse and other related stu-
students and young adults. Rockville, MD: National Institute on dent problem behavior. Journal of Drug Education, 19, 83-95.
Drug Abuse. Klein, N. C., Alexander, J. F., & Parsons, B. V (1977). Impact of family
Johnston, L. D, O'Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. (1989). Drug use, systems intervention on recidivism and sibling delinquency: A
drinking, and smoking: National survey results from high school, col- model of primary prevention and program evaluation. Journal of
lege, and young adult populations (DHHS Publication No. ADM Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 469-474.
89-1638). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Klepp, K., Halper, A., & Perry, C. L. (1986). The efficacy of peer
Joksch, H. C. (1988). The impact of severe penalties on drinking and leaders in drug abuse prevention. Journal of School Health, 56,407-
driving. Washington, DC: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. 411.
Kaij, L. (1960). Alcoholism in twins: Studies on the etiology and sequelae Knight, R. C, Sheposh, J. P., & Bryson, J. B. (1974). College student
of abuse of alcohol. Stockholm, Sweden: Alonguist & Winkell. marijuana use and societal alienation. Journal of Health and Social
Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection and socialization in adoles- Behavior, 15, 28-35.
cent friendships. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427-436. Krieg, T. L. (1982, December). Is raising the legal drinking age
Kandel, D. B. (1982). Epidemiological and psychosocial perspectives warranted? The Police Chief, pp. 32-34.
on adolescent drug use. Journal of American Academic Clinical Psy- Krohn, M. D., & Massey, J. L. (1980). Social control and delinquent
chiatry, 21, 328-347. behavior: An examination of the elements of the social bond. Socio-
Kandel, D. B. (1986). Processes of peer influence in adolescence. In R. logical Quarterly, 21, 529-543.
Silberstein (Ed.), Development as action in context: Problem behavior Krosnick, J. A., & Judd, C. M. (1982). Transitions in social influence at
and normal youth development (pp. 203-228). New York: Springer- adolescence: Who induces cigarette smoking? Developmental Psy-
Verlag. chology, 18, 359-368.
Kandel, D. B., & Andrews, K. (1987). Processes of adolescent socializa- Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1990). The role of poor
102 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

peer relationships in the development of disorder. In S. A. Asher & McAlister, A. L. (1983). Social-psychological approaches. In T. J.
J. D. Coie, Peer rejection in childhood (pp. 274-305). Cambridge, En- Glynn, C. G. Leukefeld, & J. P. Ludford (Eds.). Preventing adolescent
gland: Cambridge University Press. drug abuse: Intervention strategies (NIDA Research Monograph No.
Labouvie, E. W, & McGee, C. R. (1986). Relation of personality to 47, pp. 36-50). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
alcohol and drug use in adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clini- McAlister, A. L., Perry, C. L, Killen, J., Slinkard, L. A., & Maccoby, N.
cal Psychology, 54, 289-293. (1980). Pilot study of smoking, alcohol, and drug abuse prevention.
Ladd, G. W (1981). Effectiveness of a social learning method for en- American Journal of Public Health, 70, 719-721.
hancing children's social interaction and peer acceptance. Child De- McCord, J. (1979). Some child-rearing antecedents of criminal behav-
velopment, 52, 171-178. ior in adult men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
Ladd, G. W, & Asher, S. R. (1985). Social skill training and children's 1477-1486.
peer relations: Current issues in research and practice. In L. L. Abate McCord, J. (1981). Alcoholism and criminality. Journal of Studies on
& M. A. Milan (Eds.), Handbook of social skills training and research Alcohol, 42, 739-748.
(pp. 219-244). New York: Wiley. McCord, W, & McCord, J. (1959). Origins of crime. Montclair. NJ:
Lazar, I., Darlington, R., Murray, H., Royce, J., & Snipper, A. (1982). Columbia University Press.
Lasting effects of early education: A report from the consortium for McDermott, D. (1984). The relationship of parental drug use and par-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

longitudinal studies. Monographs of the Society for Research inChild ent's attitude concerning adolescent drug use to adolescent drug use.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Development. 47, (2-3, Serial No. 195). Adolescence, 19, 89-97.


Lerner, J. V, & Vicary, J. R. (1984). Difficult temperament and drug McKinlay, S. M., Stone, E. J., & Zucker, D. M. (1989). Research design
use: Analyses from the New \fork longitudinal study. Journal of Drug and analysis issues. Health Education Quarterly, 16, 307-313.
Education, 14,1-8. Moskowitz, J. M. (1989). The primary prevention of alcohol problems:
Lettieri, D. J., Sayers, M., & Pearson, H. W (1980). Theories on drug A critical review of the research literature. Journal of Studies on
abuse: Selected contemporary perspectives (NIDA Research Mono- Alcohol, 50, 54-88.
graph No. 30, DHHS). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
Moskowitz, J. M., & Jones, R. (1988). Alcohol and drug problems in the
Office.
schools: Results of a national survey of school administrators. Jour-
Levy, D., & Sheflin, N. (1985). The demand for alcoholic beverages: An nal of Studies on Alcohol, 49, 299-305.
aggregate time-series analysis. Journal of Public Policy and Market-
Murray, C. A. (1983). The physical environment and community con-
ing, 4, 47-54.
trol of crime. In J. Q. Wilson (Ed.), Crime and Public Policy (pp.
Lewis, C. E., Robins, L. N., & Rice, J. (1985). Association of alcoholism 107-122). San Francisco: Institute for Contemporary Studies.
with antisocial personality in urban men. Journal of Nervous and
Murray, D. M., Davis-Hearn, M., Goldman, A. L, Pirie, P., & Luepker,
Mental Disease, 173, 166-174.
R. V (1988). Four and five-year follow-up results from four seventh-
Li, T. K. (1977). Enzymology of human alcohol metabolism. In A.
grade smoking prevention strategies. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
Meister (Ed.), Enzymology of human alcohol metabolism. New York:
I I , 395-405.
Wiley.
Murray, D. M., & Hannan, P. J. (1990). Planning for the appropriate
Lochman, J. L. (1988, August). Cognitive-behavioral intervention with
analysis in school-based drug-use prevention studies. Journal of Con-
aggressive boys: Three year follow-up effects. Paper presented at the
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 58,1-11.
96th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Associa-
tion, Atlanta, GA. Murray, D. M., Hannan, R J., & Zucker, D. M. (1989). Analysis issues in
school-based health promotion studies. Health Education Quarterly,
Loeber, R. (1988). Natural histories of conduct problems, delinquency,
16, 315-320.
and associated substance use: Evidence for developmental progres-
sions. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child Murray, D. M., Johnson, C. A., Luepker, R. V, & Mittelmark, M. B.
psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 73-124). New York: Plenum Press. (1984). The prevention of cigarette smoking in children: A compari-
Loeber, R. T., & Dishion, T. (1983). Early predictors of male delin- son of four strategies. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 274-
quency: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 68-99. 288.
Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. S. (1986). Family factors as corre- Murray, D. M., Pirie, P., Luepker, R. V, & Pallonen, U. (1989). Five and
lates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. six-year follow-up results from four seventh-grade smoking preven-
In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime and justice (Vol. 7, pp. 29- tion strategies. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 207-218.
149). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Murray, D. M., Richards, P. S., Luepker, R. V, & Johnson, C. A. (1987).
Loney, J., Kramer, J., & Milich, R. (1979). The hyperkinetic child grows The prevention of cigarette smoking in children: Two and three-year
up: Predictors of symptoms, delinquency, and achievement at follow- follow-up comparisons of four prevention strategies. Journal of Be-
up. Paper presented to the American Association for the Advance- havioral Medicine, 10, 595-611.
ment of Science, Houston, TX. Murray, R. M., & Stabenau, J. R. (1982). Genetic factors in alcholism
Maddahian, E., Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1988). Adolescent predisposition. In Encyclopedic handbook of alcoholism (pp. 135-
drug use and intention to use drugs: Concurrent and longitudinal 144). New York: Gardner Press.
analyses of four ethnic groups. Addictive Behaviors, 13,191-195. Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1986). Substance use and ethnicity:
Madden, N. A., &Slavin, R. E. (1983). Cooperative learning and social Differential impact of peer and adult models. Journal of Psychology,
acceptance of mainstreamed academically handicapped students. 120, 83-95.
Journal of Special Education, 17, 171-182. Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1988). Consequences of adolescent
Marley, R. J., Miner, L. L., Wehner, J. M., & Collins, A. C. (1986). drug use: Impact on the lives of young adults. Newbury Park, CA:
Differential effects of central nervous system depressants in long- Sage.
sleep and short-sleep mice. Journal of Pharmacology Experimental Newcomb, M. D., Maddahian, E., & Bentler, P. M. (1986). Risk factors
Therapeutics, 238, 1028-1033. for drug use among adolescents: Concurrent and longitudinal analy-
Martin, B. (1977). Brief family therapy intervention: Effectiveness and ses. American Journal of Public Health, 76, 525-530.
the importance of including the father. Journal of Consulting and Newcomb, M. D, Maddahian, E., Skager, R., & Bentler, P. M. (1987).
Clinical Psychology, 45, 1001-1010. Substance abuse and psychosocial risk factors among teenagers: As-
RISK. AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 103

sociations with sex, age, ethnicity, and type of school. American Walker, D. K. (1983). The impact of early education as measured by
Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 13, 413-433. classroom observations and teacher ratings of children in kindergar-
Norem-Hebeisen, A., Johnson, D. W, Anderson, D., & Johnson, R. ten. Evaluation Review, 7,191-216.
(1984). Predictors and concomitants of changes in drug use patterns Polich, J. M., Ellickson, P. L., Reuter, P., & Kahan, J. P. (1984). Strate-
among teenagers. The Journal of Social Psychology, 124, 43-50. gies for controlling adolescent drug use. Santa Monica, CA: Rand
Nylander, I. (1979). The development of antisocial behaviour in chil- Corporation.
dren. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 47, 71-80. Pollock, V E., Volavka, J., & Goodwin, D. W (1983). The EEG after
O'Donnell, J. A., & Clayton, R. R. (1979). Determinants of early mari- alcohol administration in men at risk for alcoholism. Archives of
juana use. In G. M. Beschner & A. S. Friedman (Eds.), Youth drug General Psychiatry, 40, 857-861.
abuse: Problems, issues, andtreatment (pp. 63-110). Lexington, MA: Porter, B., & O'Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord and childhood
Lexington Books. problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 287-295.
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Chamberlin, R., & Tatelbaum, R. (1986). Puska, P., Vartiainen, E., Pallonen, U., Salonen, J. X, Poyhia, P., Kos-
Preventing child abuse and neglect: A randomized trial of nurse kela, K., & McAlister, A. (1982). The North Karelia Youth Project:
home visitation. Pediatrics, 78, 65-78. Evaluation of two yearsof intervention on health behavior and CVD
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal risk factors among 13- to 15-year-old children. Preventive Medicine,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulle- 11, 550-570.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tin, 102, 357-389. Rachal, J. V, Guess, L. L., Hubbard, R. L., Maisto, S. A., Cavanaugh,
Paton, S. M., & Kandel, D. B. (1978). Psychological factors and adoles- E. R., Waddel, R., & Benrud, C. H. (1982). Facts for planning No. 4:
cent illicit drug use: Ethnicity and sex difference. Adolescence, 13, Alcohol misuse by adolescents. Alcohol Health and Research World,
187-200. 6(3), 61-68.
Paton, S., Kessler, R., & Kandel, D. (1977). Depressive mood and ado- Ramey, C. X, Bryant, D. M., Campbell, F. A., Sparling, J. J., & Wasik,
lescent drug use: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Genetic Psychol- B. H. (1988). Early intervention for high-risk children: The Carolina
ogy, 131, 267-289. Early Intervention Program. In R. H. Price, E. L. Cowen, R. P. Lo-
Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1982). A comparative rion, & J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.), 14 ounces of prevention: A casebook
evaluation of a parent training program. Behavior Therapy, 13,638- for practitioners (pp. 32-43). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
650. ical Association.
Patterson, G. R., & Fleischman, M. J. (1979). Maintenance of treat- Raymond, M. R. (1987). Missing data in evaluation research. Evalua-
ment effects: Some considerations concerning family systems and tion and the Health Professions, 9, 395-420.
follow-up data. Behavior Therapy, 10,168-185.
Reilly, D. M. (1979). Family factors in the etiology and treatment of
Patterson, G. R., & Reid, J. B. (1973). Intervention for families of ag- youthful drug abuse. Family Therapy, 11,149-171.
gressive boys: A replication study. Behavior Research Therapy, 11,
Reiss, A. J. (1986). Why are communities important in understanding
383-394.
crime? In A. J. Reiss & M. Tonry (Eds.), Communities and crime.
Peed, S., Roberts, M., & Forehand, R. (1977). Evaluation of the effec-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
tiveness of a standardized parent training program in altering the
Robins, L. N. (1978). Sturdy childhood predictors of adult anti-social
interaction of mothers and their noncompliant children. Behavior
behavior: Replicationsfrom longitudinalstudies. Psychological Med-
Modification, 1, 323-350.
icine, 5,611-622.
Penning, M., & Barnes, G. E. (1982). Adolescent marijuana use: A
review. International Journal of Addictions, 17, 749-791. Robins, L. N. (1980). The natural history of drug abuse. Acta Psychiat-
rica Scandinavia, <52(Suppl. 284), 7-20.
Pentz, M. A., Brannon, B. R., Charlin, V L., Barrett, E. J., MacKinnon,
D. P., & Flay, B. R. (1989). The power of policy: Relationship of Robins, L. N. (1984). The natural history of adolescent drug use. Ameri-
smoking policy to adolescent smoking. American Journal of Public can Journal of Public Health, 74, 656-657.
Health, 79, 857-862. Robins, L. N., & Przybeck, T. R. (1985). Age of onset of drug use as a
Pentz, M. A., Dwyer, J. H., MacKinnon, D. P., Flay, B. R., Hansen, factor in drug and other disorders. In C. L. Jones & R. J. Battjes
W B., Wang, E. Y. L, & Johnson, C. A. (1989). A multi-community (Eds.), Etiology of Drug Abuse: Implications for Prevention (NIDA
trial for primary prevention of adolescent drug abuse: Effects on Research Monograph No. 56, DHHS Publication No. ADM 85-
drug use prevalence. Journal of the American Medical Association, 1335, pp. 178-192). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Of-
261, 3259-3266. fice.
Pentz, M. A., MacKinnon, D. P., Flay, B. R., Hansen, W B., Johnson, Robins, L. N., & Ratcliff, K. S. (1979). Continuation of antisocial behav-
C. A., & Dwyer, J. H. (1989). Primary prevention of chronic diseases ior into adulthood. International Journal of Mental Health, 7, 96-
in adolescence: Effects of the Midwestern Prevention Project on 116.
tobacco use. American Journal of Epidemiology, 130, 713-724. Rotheram, M. J. (1982a). Assertiveness training in fourth and fifth-
Perry, C. L. (1986). Community-wide health promotion and drug abuse grade children. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10,
prevention. Journal of School Health, 56, 359-363. 567-582.
Perry, C. L., Crockett, S. J., & Pirie, P. (1987). Influencing parental Rotheram, M. J. (1982b). Social skills training with underachievers,
health behavior. Implications of community assessments. Health disruptive, and exceptional children. Psychology in the Schools, 19,
Education, 18(5), 68-77. 532-539.
Perry, C. L., Klepp, K. I, & Shultz, J. M. (1988). Primary prevention of Rotheram, M., Armstrong, M., & Booraem, C. (1986). Assertiveness
cardiovascular disease: Communitywide strategies for youth. Jour- training with elementary school children. American Journal of Com-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 358-364. munity Psychology, 10, 567-582.
Perry, C. L., Luepker, R. V, Murray, D. M., Kurth, C., Mullis, R., Crock- Rutter, M. (1980). Changing youth in a changing society. Cambridge,
ett, S., & Jacobs, D. R. (1988). Parent involvement with children's MA: Harvard University Press.
health promotion: The Minnesota Home Team. American Journal Rutter, M. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity: Protective factors
of Public Health, 78, 1156-1160. and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Pierson, D. E., Bronson, M. B., Dromey, E., Swartz, J. P., Tivnan, T, & 747,598-611.
104 D. HAWKINS, R. CATALANO, AND J. MILLER

Rutter, M., & Giller, H. (1983). Juvenile delinquency: Trends and per- findings and methodological issues (pp. 101-113). Washington, DC:
spectives. New York: Penguin Books. Hemisphere-Wiley.
Saner, H., & Grossman, M. (1987). Beer taxes, the legal drinking age, Spivack, G. (1983). High risk early behaviors indicating vulnerability to
and youth motor vehicle fatalities. Journal of Legal Studies, 16, 351- delinquency in the community and school. National Institute of Juve-
374. nile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Juvenile Justice
Sampson, R. J. (1986). Crime in cities: The effects of formal and infor- and Delinquency Prevention, Law Enforcement Assistance Admin-
mal social control. In A. J. Reiss & M. Tonry (Eds.), Crime and istration. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
justice: An annual review of research: Vol. 8. Communities and crime Spivack, G, &Shure, M. B. (1974). Social adjustment of young children.
(pp. 271-311). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Sampson, R. J., Castellano, T. C, & Laub, J. H. (1981). Juvenile criminal Stanton, M. D. (1979). The client as family member: Aspects of continu-
behavior and its relation to neighborhood characteristics.Washington, ing treatment. In B. S. Brown (Ed.), Addicts and aftercare: Commu-
DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. nity integration of the former drug user (pp. 81-102). Beverly Hills,
Schaie, K. W (1965). A general model for the study of developmental CA: Sage.
problems. Psychological Bulletin, 64, 92-107. Stuart, R. B. (1974). Teaching facts about drugs: Pushing or prevent-
Schaps, E. Moskowitz, J. M., Malvin, J. H., & Schaeffer, G. H. (1986). ing? Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 198-201.
Surgeon general. (1988). The health consequences of smoking: Nicotine
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Evaluation of seven school-based prevention programs: A final re-


addiction. A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, MD: U.S. De-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

port on the Napa Project. International Journal of the Addictions, 21,


1081-1112. partment of Health and Human Services.
Suwaki, H., & Ohara, H. (1985). Alcohol induced facial flushing and
Schinke, S. P., Bebel, M. Y, Orlandi, M. A., & Botvin, G. J. (1988).
drinking behavior in Japanese men. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
Prevention strategies for vulnerable pupils: School social work prac-
46, 196-198.
tices to prevent substance abuse. Urban Education, 22, 510-519.
Swift, C. F. (1988). Stopping the violence: Prevention strategies for
Schinke, S. P., Botvin, G. J., Trimble, J. E., Orlandi, M. A., Gilchrist, families. In L. A. Bond & B. M. Wagner(Eds.), Families in transition,
L. D., & Locklear, V S. (1988). Preventing substance abuse among primary prevention programs that work: Primary prevention ofpsy-
American-Indian adolescents: A bicultural competence skills ap- chopaihology (Vol. 11, pp. 252-285). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
proach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 87-90. Tabakoff, B., & Hoffman, P. L. (1988). Genetics and biological markers
Schlegel, R., & Sanborn, M. (1979). Religious affiliation and adoles- of risk for alcoholism. Public Health Reports, 103, 690-698.
cent drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 40, 693-703. Tec, N. (1974). Parent-child drug abuse: Generational continuity or
Schuckit, M. A. (1980). Self-rating alcohol intoxication by young men adolescent deviancy? Adolescence, 9, 350-364.
with and without family histories of alcoholism. Journal of Studies Tobler, N. S. (1986). Meta-analysis of 143 adolescent drug prevention
on Alcohol, 41, 242-249. programs: Quantitative outcome results of program participants
Schuckit, M. A. (1987). Biological vulnerability to alcoholism. Journal compared to a control or comparison group. Journal of Drug Issues,
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 301-309. 16, 537-567.
Schuckit, M. A., Parker, D. C, & Rossman, L. R. (1983). Ethanol-re- Tonry, M., Ohlin, L. E., & Farrington, D. P. (1991). Human development
latedprolactin responses and risk for alcoholism. Biological Psychia- and criminal behavior. New \brk: Springer-Verlag.
try, 18, 1153-1159. Tremblay, R. (1988, September). Peers and the onset of delinquency.
Schuckit, M. A., & Rayes, V(1979). Ethanol ingestion: Differences in Paper prepared for the Onset Working Group Program on Human
blood acetaldehyde concentrations in relatives of alcoholics and Development and Criminal Behavior, Castine, ME.
controls. Science, 203, 54-55. Tremblay, R. E., McCord, J., Boileau, H., LeBlanc, M., Gagnon, C,
Seitz, V, Rosenbaum, L. K., & Apfel, N. H. (1985). Effects of family Charlebois, P., & Larivee, S. (1990, November). The Montreal preven-
support intervention: A ten-year follow-up. Child Development, 56, tion experiment: School adjustment and self-reported delinquency
376-391. after three years of follow-up. Paper presented at the 42nd Annual
Selnow, G. W (1987). Parent-child relationships and single and two Meeting of the American Society of Criminology, Baltimore.
parent families: Implications for substance usage. Journal of Drug Vaillant, G. (1983). The natural history of alcoholism. Cambridge, MA:
Education, 77,315-326. Harvard University Press.
Vartiainen, E., Pallonen, U, McAlister, A., Koskela, K., & Puska, P.
Shaw, C. R., & McKay, H. D. (1969). Juvenile delinquency and urban
(1983). Effect of two years of educational intervention on adolescent
areas (Rev. ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
smoking (The North Karelia Youth Project). Bulletin of the World
Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug use and psychological
Health Organization, 61, 529-532.
health: A longitudinal inquiry. American Psychologist, 45, 612-630.
Vartiainen, E., Pallonen, U, McAlister, A., Koskela, K., & Puska, P.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1982). Interpersonal problem-solving in (1986). Four-year follow-up results of the smoking prevention pro-
young children: A cognitive approach to prevention. American Jour- gram in the North Karelia Youth Project. Preventive Medicine, 15,
nal of Community Psychology, 10, 341-356. 692-698.
Simcha-Fagan, O., Gersten, J. C., & Langner, T. (1986). Early precursors Vartiainen, E., Pallonen, U, McAlister, A., & Puska, P. (1990). Eight-
and concurrent correlates of illicit drug use in adolescents. Journal of year follow-up results of an adolescent smoking prevention pro-
Drug Issues, 16, 7-28. gram: The North Karelia Youth Project. American Journal of Public
Simcha-Fagan, Q, & Schwartz, J. E. (1986). Neighborhood and delin- Health, 80, 78-79.
quency: An assessment of contextual effects. Criminology, 24, 667- von Knorring, L., Oreland, L., & von Knorring, A. L. (1987). Personal-
703. ity traits and platelet MAO activity in alcohol and drug abusing teen-
Slavin, R. E., Madden, N. A., Karweit, N. L, Liverman, B. J., & Dolan, age boys. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 75, 307-314.
L. (1990). Success for all: First year outcomes of a comprehensive Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. (1980). Effects of divorce on the visiting
plan for reforming urban education. American Educational Re- father-child relationship. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137,1534-
search Journal, 27, 255-278. 1539.
Smith, G. M., & Fogg, C. P. (1978). Psychological predictors of early Walters, H. L, & Gilmore, S. K. (1973). Placebo versus social learning
use, Jate use, and non-use of marijuana among teenage students. In effects on parental training procedures designed to alter the behav-
D. B. Kandel (Ed.), Longitudinal research on drug use: Empirical ior of aggressive boys. Behavior Therapy, 4, 311-377.
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR DRUG PROBLEMS 105

Watts, R. K., & Rabow, J. (1983). Alcohol availability and alcohol-re- Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human nature. New
lated problems in 213 Californian cities. Alcoholism: Clinical and York: Simon & Schuster.
Experimental Research, 7, 47-58. Zabin, L. S., Hardy, J. B., Smith, E. A., & Hirsch, M. B. (1986). Sub-
Weaver, S. C, & Tennant, F. S. (1973). Effectiveness of drug education stance use and its relation to sexual activity among inner-city adoles-
programs for secondary school students. American Journal of Psychi- cents. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 7, 320-331.
atry, 730,812-814. Ziegler, S. (1981). The effectiveness of cooperative learning teams for
Wechsler, H., & McFadden, T. (1979). Patterns of alcohol consumption increasing cross-ethnic friendship, additional evidence. Human Or-
among the young: High school, college and general population stud- ganization, 40, 264-268.
ies. In H. J. Blane & M. E. Chafez (Eds.), Youth, alcohol, and social Ziegler-Driscoll, G. (1979). The similarities in families of drug depen-
policy (pp. 39-58). New \brk: Plenum Press. dents and alcoholics. In E. Kaufman & P. Kaufman, Family therapy
Weissberg, R. P., & Allen, J. P. (1985). Promoting children's social skills of drug and alcohol abuse (pp. 19-39). New York: Gardner Press.
and adaptive interpersonal behavior. In B. A. Edelstein & L. Michel- Zucker, D. M. (in press). An analysis of variance pitfall: The fixed
son (Eds.), Handbook of prevention (pp. 153-175). New \brk: Ple- effects analysis in a nested design. American Educational Research
num Press. Journal.
Weissberg, R. P., & Caplan, M. (1989, April). A follow-up study of a Zucker, R. A. (1979). Developmental aspects of drinking through the
school-based competitive promotion program for young adolescents.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

young adult years. In H. T. Blane & M. E. Chafetz (Eds.), Youth,


Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

alcohol and social policy (pp. 91-146). New \brk: Plenum Press.
in Child Development, Kansas City, MO. Zucker, R. A., & Harford, T. C. (1983). National study of the demogra-
Weissberg, R. P., Gesten, E. L., Carnrike, C. L., Toro, P. A., Rapkin, phy of adolescent drinking practices in 1980. Journal of Studies on
B. D., Davidson, E., & Cowen, E. L. (1981). Social problem solving Alcohol, 44, 974-985.
skills training: A competence-building intervention with second- to Zuckerman, M. (1987). Biological connection between sensation seek-
forth-grade children. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9, ing and drug abuse. In J. Engel & L. Oreland (Eds.), Brain reward
411-423.
systems and abuse (pp. 165-176). New Mark: Raven Press.
Werner, E. E. (1989). High-risk children in young adulthood: A longi-
tudinal study from birth to 32 years. American Journal of Orthopsy-
chialry, 59, 72-81. Received November 29,1989
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible. New Revision received August 7,1991
York: McGraw-Hill. Accepted August 11,1991 •

You might also like