You are on page 1of 159

INFORM ATION TO USERS

This was produced from a copy of a document sent to us for microfilming. While the
most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document
have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material
submitted.

The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand


markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction.

1-The sign or "target” for pages apparently lacking from the document
photographed is “ Missing Page(s)”. If it was possible to obtain the missing
page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages.
This may have necessitated cutting through an image and duplicating
adjacent pages to assure you o f complete continuity.

2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a round black mark it is an


indication that the film inspector noticed either blurred copy because of
movement during exposure, or duplicate copy. Unless we meant to delete
copyrighted materials that should not have been filmed, you will find a good
image of the page in the adjacent frame. If copyrighted materials were
deleted you will find a target note listing the pages in the adjacent frame.

3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., is part of the material being photo­
graphed the photographer has followed a definite method in "sectioning”
the material. It is customary to begin filming at the upper left hand corner of
a large sheet and to continue from left to right in equal sections with small
overlaps. If necessary, sectioning is continued again—beginning below the
first row and continuing on until complete.

4. For any illustrations that cannot be reproduced satisfactorily by xerography,


photographic prints can be purchased at additional cost and tipped into your
xerographic copy. Requests can be made to our Dissertations Customer
Services Department.

5. Some pages in any document may have indistinct print. In all cases we have
filmed the best available copy.

University
Microfilms
International
300 N. Z E E B RD., A N N A R B O R , M l 4 8 1 0 6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8207006

Price, Harry Edward, II

THE EFFECT OF CONDUCTOR ACADEMIC TASK PRESENTATION,


CONDUCTOR REINFORCEMENT, AND ENSEMBLE PRACTICE ON
PERFORMERS’ MUSICAL ACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTIVENESS, AND
ATTITUDE

Syracuse University Ed.D. 1981

University
Microfilms
International 300 N. Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48106

Copyright 1981

by
Price, Harry Edward, II
All Rights Reserved

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PLEASE NOTE:

In all cases this material has been filmed in the best possible way from the available copy.
Problems encountered with this document have been identified here with a check mark V .

1. Glossy photographs or pages______

2. Colored illustrations, paper or print_____

3. Photographs with dark background______

4. Illustrations are poor copy ;

5. Pages with black marks, not original copy______

6. Print shows through as there is text on both sides of page_____

7. Indistinct, broken or small print on several pages.

8. Print exceeds margin requirements______

9. Tightly bound copy with print lost in spine______

10. Computer printout pages with indistinct print_____

11. Page(s)___________ lacking when material received, and not available from school or
author.

12. Page(s)___________ seem to be missing in numbering only as text follows.

13. Two pages numbered___________ . Text follows.

14. Curling and wrinkled pages______

15. Other_____________________________________________________________________

University
Microfilms
International

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
THE EFFECT OF CONDUCTOR ACADEMIC TASK PRESENTATION, CONDUCTOR
REINFORCEMENT, AND ENSEMBLE PRACTICE ON PERFORMERS'
MUSICAL ACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTIVENESS, AND ATTITUDE

by

HARRY EDUARD PRICE

B. M.E., Florida State University, 197*1


M-M.E-, Florida State University, 1975

DISSERTATION

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements


for the degree of Doctor of Education in Music in
the School of Education of Syracuse University
July 1981

Approved

Date

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Copyright 1981

HARR? EDWARD PRICE

- i i -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CONTENTS

page
LIST OF TABLES ....................................... iv

LIST OF F I G U R E S ........................ v

CHAPTER I. ............................................... 1
INT E O D O C T I O N ............... 1
Statement of Problem. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Need for Study. .......... 3

CHAPTER I I . ................................................. 7
REVIEW OF LITERATURE ..................................... 7
Formal Versus Open Structure Classrooms . . . . . . 1 8
Reinforcement in Non-music Settings . . . . . . . . 2 1
Reinforcement in Music Settings . . . . . . . . . . 2 5
Music as Reinforcement. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 8
Active Versus Passive Activities. . . . . . . . . . 3 1
Research in Music Ensemble Settings . . . . . . . . 3 3
Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 8

CHAPTER III.................................................. 40
EXPERIMENTAL METHOD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . ......... 40
Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 0
Selection of Music. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 0
Experimental Environment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 1
Experimental Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 2
Treatment . . . . ................ . . . . . . . . . 4 3
Experimenters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 5
Equipment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 5
Measurement Procedures. . . . . . ......... . . . . 4 6
Frequency of Off-task. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 6
Frequency of Teacher Eye Contact . . . . . . . . 4 7
Rehearsal Time . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 8
Musical Performance Ratings. . . . . . . . . . . 4 8
Attitude Assessment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 8
Teaching Units . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 9
Reliability. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 3

CHAPTER I V ............. 57
RESULTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . ........................ 57
Treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 8
Reinforcement Frequency. . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 1
Direction Units. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62
Academic Presentation Units. . . . . . . . . . . 6 3
Student Attentiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 5
Musical Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 1
Student Attitude. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 4
Music. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 5
Rehearsal. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 8
"Turned On". . . . . . . . . . ......... . . . . 8 1
Conductor. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 5
Teacher. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 1
- i ii -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CONTENTS— Continued

CHAPTER V ..................... 96
DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS .......................... 96
Student Attentiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 8
Music Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
Student Attitude. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
Design. . . ......... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
Implications foe Future Besearch. . . . . . . . . 110

SUMMARY..................................................... 114

A P P E N D I C E S ................................................. 117
A. List of Possible Music. . . . . . . . . . . . 118
B. Pretest Music Ranking Form. . . . . . . . . . 119
C. List of Music Used. . < ? . . . . . . . . . . . 120
D. Design Model Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
E. Eguipment List. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122
F. Performance Bating Form . . . . . . ........ 123
G. Attitude Survey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
H. Seconds Spent in Treatment Behaviors. . . . . 128
I. Frequency of Units and Reinforcements . . . . 129
J. Percentage of Student Off-task. . . . . . . . 130
K. Mean Music Achievement Scores . . . . . . . . 131
L. Attitude Survey: Average for
Music Preference. = 132
M. Attitude Survey: Average for
Rehearsal Enjoyment . . . . . . . . . . . . 133
N. Attitude Survey: Average for
"Turned On" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134
0. Attitude Survey: Average for
Conductor Preference. . . . . . . . . . . . 135
P. Attitude Survey: Average for
Average Teacher Bating. . . . . . . . . . . 136

REFERENCES ...................... . . . . . . . . . . . 137

BIOGRAPHICAL DATA.......................................... 149

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- v -

TABLES

Table page

1. Teaching Unit System of Observation:


Average Beliability . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 5

2. Kendall Coefficient of Concordance Summary . . . . 56

3. Mean Time Spent in Seconds for Treatment


Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 0

4. Mean Frequency of Units and Beinforcements . . . . 60

5. Mean Percentage of Students Off-task:


for Treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 5

6. Mean Percentage of Off-task Behavior:


for Activity. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 7

7. Pre- Posttest Gain Scores for Musical


Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 2

8. Music Bating: Multiple Begression . . . . . . . . 7 5

9. Behearsal Enjoyment Bating: Multiple


Begression. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 9

10. "Turned On" Bating: Multiple Begression ......... 82

11. Liking the Conductor Bating: Multiple


Begression. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 6

12. Teacher Bating: Multiple Begression . . . . . . . 9 2

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
FIGUBES

Figure page

1. Teacher Presentation of task . . . . . . . . . . . 5 0

2. Teacher Beinforcement of Student Besponse. . . . . 52

3. Off-task Behavior, . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 6

4. Off-task During Non-performance. . . . . . . . . . 7 0

5. Bean Music Performance Batings . . . . . . . . . . 7 3

6. Music Bating . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 7

7. Behearsal Bating . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80

8. "Turned On" Bating . . . ......... . . . . . . . . 8 4

9. Liking the Conductor Bating by Treatment . . . . . 88

10. Liking the Conductor Bating by Conductor . . . . . 90

11. Conductor Bating as a Good Teacher . . . . . . . . 9 4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Statement of Problem

The art of teaching and rehearsing involves many di­

verse and complex behaviors, which may include thousands of

variables in operation when describing an effective teacher*

As a result there may be an infinite number of possible

combinations of interactions which occur in any multivariate

teaching/learning environment* Some variables involve tea­

cher behaviors, including expressed values and attitudes,

verbal presentation of tasks, nonverbal cues and gestures,

and verbal and nonverbal reinforcements of student behavi­

ors. Other variables involve student behaviors, including

expressed values and attitudes, quality of performance,

quantity of performance, attentiveness, and abilities to

reinforce teacher behaviors. In addition to these variables,

a specific situation may include many different values,

performance levels, student backgrounds, and others which

make the issue of identifying the gualities which enhance

teacher effectiveness more complex.

While there are many ingredients which comprise the

totality of a music ensemble rehearsal, the behavioral com­

ponents of such a rehearsal may be described as follows:

A. Teacher behaviors.

1. Verbal {singing, directions, academic and social

- 1 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
task presentations, approval and disapproval

comments).

2. Non-verbal (eye contact, conducting, facial ex­

pressions) .

3. Temporal (duration of behaviors, rehearsal pace).

B. Student behaviors.

1. Verbal (singing, talking).

2. Non-verbal (gestures, actions, eye contact, facial

expressions, posture, performance).

3. Attitudinal (written expression of likes and dis­

likes) .

4. Temporal (duration of behaviors, speed of response

to c o n d u c t o r s instructions).

C. Teaching Units.

1. Contextual (type of teacher/student behavior).

2. Seguential (order of types of teacher/student be­

haviors) .

3. Temporal (duration of units, pace of units).

The teacher and student behaviors above have been studied in

music ensemble rehearsal situations both descriptively and

experimentally, while teaching units in a music ensemble

situation have only been studied descriptively. It would

seem that the effect of all of the above could be studied in

an experimental situation, that is, the effect of teaching

units as well as individual teacher and student behaviors

could be studied by systematically varying the seguential

- 2 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
and temporal content of the teaching units because all are

defined as overt controllable behaviors.

Need for Study

In secondary and post-secondary schools, the student

musical ensemble is a basic part of the music curriculum.

Given its place in schools, it would be useful for the

teacher/conductor to develop efficient techniques with which

to achieve the maximum level of performance for a given

amount of time. Indeed, how to best use rehearsal time is a

primary problem for a teacher/conductor. Green (1969) ad­

dressed the need for efficient use of rehearsal time:

Early in his career, the student will learn there


is never enough rehearsal time. It is a most pre­
cious commodity and must not be wasted...No con­
ductor can disassociate himself completely from the
teaching facet of his trade. Knowing how to suggest
changes, without prejudicing the members of the
ensemble, is a valuable asset, (p. 2)

Rehearsal efficiency is addressed by Budolf (1950) al­

most to the exclusion of the many other skills required of a

conductor:

But all his musicianship and thorough study of


scores will help him little unless he knows how to
talk to people, work with them, and get resiits in
a guick and direct manner. Knowledge of a few sim­
ple principles of group psychology is a great as­
sistance in rehearsing efficiently and in stimula­
ting the players to a good performance, (p. 1)

The previous statements are directed towards the ef­

ficient use of rehearsal time with a focus on the skilled

use of verbalization; however, other authorities caution

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- h -

against the overuse of verbalizations. Bloomquist (1973)

sees rehearsal inefficiency, in the form of excess verbiage,

as a strong contributor to discipline problems in the se­

condary school ensemble;

My best advice is - don*t talk - do! Don’t attempt


to accomplish with your mouth vhat your baton can
do much better. An overly talkative director is
easy to ignore, (p. 79)

While there is no research on the subject of the effective­

ness of baton technigue in influencing performance, there

are data which indicate that non-performance portions of

rehearsals result in a decrease in attentiveness (Spradling,

1980; Yarbrough and Price, in press).

A noted composer and conductor, W. Frances McBeth, ad­

dressed the issue of playing time in rehearsals in an in­

terview reported by Davis (1978):

I continue playing as far as possible so as not to


make the rehearsal boring. After a stop or some i-
solated work with one group, I choose a starting
place generally farther back than necessary. This
method keeps a higher percentage of kids playing
during these work periods, (p. h0)

In an article on the efficient use of rehearsal time,

Begian (1968) seems to have addressed the concerns mentioned

to this point and summarizes as follows:

An efficient conductor knows what he is after,


tries to explain it with a minimum verbiage, and
develops his technique to a point where he does not
have to stop for every little correction. But when
technigue cannot accurately convey what he is
seeking musically, he must stop to explain, (p. 36)

Thus, the efficient rehearsal is clearly a concern of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 5 -

conductors and conducting teachers cited. The focus of these

authorities seems to be on the guantity of performance time

and the quality of verbalizations which occur during the

non-performance time.

Becker, Engelmann, and Thomas (1971) have suggested a

teaching model which is clearly applicable to both the

guantity of performance time and quality of verbalizations

which occur during the non-performance time in a rehearsal

situation. In this model, teaching is defined as a three-

step process: (1) teacher presents preceding stimulus; (2)

student responds; (3) teacher presents following stimulus

(S-R-S). Another way of stating this is: (1) the teacher

presents a task (direct or implied); (2) the student re­

sponds; and (3) the teacher gives the student feedback or

goes on to another task. When this is placed specifically in

the context of a music rehearsal situation, the three steps

might be: (1) the conductor conveys that which is souqht

musically; (2) the student attempts to perform in the manner

which the conductor has conveyed;and (3) the conductor re­

inforces the performance, or goes on to another presenta­

tion.

It would seem that the Becker, Engelmann, and Thomas

(1971) teaching model could be applied to a music ensemble

rehearsal setting in an effort to develop efficient rehear­

sal techniques and consequently to maximize the level of

performance for a given amount of rehearsal time. That fceinq

the case, skills and techniques designed to implement this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 6 -

model should be studied systematically in relationship to

performance outcomes. One observational study demonstrated

that this three-step process does occur in music ensemble

situations (Yarbrough 8 Price, in press) and that it is

applicable as a means of describing and analyzing the re­

hearsal process. Since the teaching model could be opera­

tionally defined and observed in a music ensemble rehearsal

situation, it appeared that it could be examined in an ex­

perimental setting. This study was designed as an initial

effort to apply this adaptation (Yarbrough and Price, in

press) of the Becker, Engelmann and Thomas (1971) teaching

model to an experimental setting in an effort to study the

effect on student responses.

Specifically, the purpose of this study was to inves­

tigate the effects of teaching units on attentiveness, per­

formance, and attitude of students in a university symphonic

band. Student attentiveness, performance, and attitude were

measured in three experimental treatments: (A) teacher di­

rections followed by student directions; (B) teacher aca­

demic task presentations, teacher directions followed by

student performance; and (C) teacher academic task presen­

tations, teacher directions, student performance, followed

by teacher reinforcement.

Student performance was measured using judges* ratings

of audiotaped performances; attentiveness was measured using

behavioral observations of videotaped rehearsals; and atti­

tude was measured using verbal reports of students.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

The successful isolation of some variables which link

teacher behavior to student behavior is a recent phenomenon.

The only effective teacher behavior which has been substan­

tiated by research findings that date as far back as the

early 1960's, is the effect of teacher reinforcement on

student behaviors (Altman 6 Linton, 1971; Hanley, 1970;

Kennedy 6 Willcutt, 1964; Lipe 8 Jung, 1971). It was not

until the 1970's that educational research was able to link

other teacher behaviors to student behaviors (Brophy, 1979;

Dunkin 8 Biddle, 1974; Rosenshine 6 Furst, 1973). Since then

efforts have continued to identify and investigate the

teacher/student behaviors which contribute to teaching ef­

fectiveness as indicated by student achievement, attentive­

ness, and attitude.

It has been suggested that "more attention needs to be

paid to classroom seguences and pupil behavior" (Dunkin 8

Biddle, 1974, p. 132) rather than to a myriad of variables

ranging from highly specified behaviors to nebulous and

ill-defined terms. In this review, the focus will first be

on teaching methods and their components defined as follows:

Teaching methods are recurrent instructional


processes, applicable to various subject matters,
and usable by more than one teacher. They are re­
current in that the activities are repeated over
intervals measured in minutes or weeks. They are
instructional processes, such as patterned behavior
(for example, lecturing, discussion, and recita-
- 7 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 8 -

tion); delivery systems for curriculum (for exam­


ple, printed subject matter, film, programmed in­
struction, and computer assisted instruction); and
organizational structures for promoting learning
(for example, tutoring and independent study),
. . . . The ways in which the teaching of
specific subject matter goes on we call "teaching
techniques," which are studied in courses on cur­
riculum and instruction in reading, science, social
studies, mathematics, English, and the like. The
requirement that a teaching method be usable by
more than one teacher means that it should not de­
pend upon the talents, traits, or resources unique
to an individual teacher. Bather, the use of a
teaching method should, in principle, be accessible
to any trained teacher. (Berliner S Gage, 1976,
P- 5)

Secondly, the focus of this review will be sequencing

of presentation. In The Study of Teaching, the authors sta­

ted that "it appears to us that any meaningful analysis of

teaching must involve sequential elements. . . . effective

teaching must consist of seguences of presentations that are

planned carefully and conducted sensitively" (Dunkin 6

Biddle, 1974, p. 353). Besults of recent research support

the hypothesis that "it is pattern of practice rather than

single teaching practices or teaching skills which account

for [teacher] effectiveness" (Powell, 1978, p. 28). It was

also reported that pattern of teaching practice is more

likely to be related to learning than a single teaching

practice (McDonald, 1976a).

A teaching model has been proposed which defines the

teaching/learning environment as basically a three-step

process; (1) teacher presents a "preceding stimulus", (2)

student responds, and (3) teacher presents a "following

stimulus" by either giving the student feedback or going on

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 9 -

to the next preceding stimulus (S-B-S) (Becker, Engelmann, 6

Thomas, 1971). The effectiveness of this teaching model

seems to be substantiated by the findings to date in non-

music settings. An analysis of data assessing the effec­

tiveness of the Engelmann-Becker Direct Instruction Hodel

(E-B) indicates that this model was effective. It mas im­

plemented by use of a curriculum for reading, arithmetic,

and language designed by Engelmann which was published under

the trade name DISTAB. In this curriculum, students learned

discriminations and operations in a rapid-paced fashion,

their attention was focused on tasks and discriminations,

they were cued to respond, and the responses were rein­

forced. It was effective in developing basic academic skills

in disadvantaged primary-grade children (Becker & Engelmann,

1976) .

Stallings (1975) "found that a variable which describes

a stimuli/response/feedback (S/B/F) sequence of interaction"

(p. 13) was significantly related to math and English

achievement scores of first and third grade students. This

was a result of a comprehensive observation study of class­

room processes and instructional practices in Follow Through

Planned Variation projects (Stallings & Kaskovitz, 197U).

The Follow Through Program was established by con­


gress in 1967 under the office of Economic Oppor­
tunity when it became apparent that a program was
needed in the early grades of public school to re­
inforce and extend the academic gains made by eco­
nomically disadvantaged children enrolled in Head
Start or similar pre-school proqrams. Project Fol­
low Through was and is a "planned variation" re­
search design; that is, the goal is to examine the
differential effectiveness of programs based on
divergent educational and developmental theories.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 10 -

Of 22 Follow Through educational programs,


seven were selected for observational study. Those
chosen represent a wide spectrum of innovative
educational theories. The range includes two models
based upon positive reinforcement theory (from the
University of Kansas and the University of Oregon),
a model based primarily upon the cognitive deve­
lopmental theory of Jean Piaget (High/Scope Foun­
dation) , an open classroom model based upon the
English Infant School theory (Educational Develop­
ment Center), and three other models drawn from
Piaget, John Dewey, and the English Infant Schools
(Far West Laboratory, University of Arizona, and
Bank street College).
The first part of the study examined how well
the educational models had been implemented. The
findings suggest that most teachers were implemen­
ting the models according to sponsor specifica­
tions. The second part of the study . . . examined
the relationships between classroom instructional
practices and child outcomes (i.e., test scores,
days absent, observed child behaviors). (Stallings,
1975, p. 1)

The findings of the second part of the study indicated that

higher reading and math scores, in first and third grade,

were obtained in classrooms using systematic in­


structional patterns where the teacher provides
information and asks a guestion about the informa­
tion- The child responds and the teacher immedi­
ately lets the child know whether the response is
right or wrong. If he is wrong, the child is guided
to the correct answer. If he is correct, he re­
ceives praise, a token, or some form of acknowl­
edgment (Stallings, 1975, p. 6).

This pattern reflected the procedures used in the University

of Kansas and University of Oregon (positive reinforcement

models) classrooms.

References to this and similar patterns of student/

teacher interaction as effective teaching models were found

with considerable freguency in the recent research litera-

tu?;e (Becker, 1978; Berliner S Eosenshine, 1976; Brophy,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 1 1 -

1979a, 1979b; Brophy 6 Evertson, 1974; Carnine, 1979;

Engelmann, 1971; Evertson, Anderson, Brophy, S Anderson,

1978; Good, 1979; McDonald, 1976a, 1976b; Medley, 1977;

Powell, 1978; Bosenshine, 1976a, 1976b, 1978, 1979; Soar,

1973; Stallings, 1975; Stallings S Kaskowitz, 1974). Brophy

(1979a) suggested an optimal pattern of instruction which

has many similarities to the pattern stated by Stallings

(1975):

The instruction that seems most efficient in­


volves the teacher working with the whole class (or
with small groups in the early grades), presenting
information in lectures/demonstrations and then
following up with recitations or practice exercises
in which the students get opportunities to make
responses and get corrective feedback. (Brophy,
1979a, p. 4)

In an earlier study, several factors which were consistent

with these patterns emerged. While some inconsistencies

existed in the results, focus, drill, convergence, struc­

tured activity with the teacher and time spent on reading,

and lesser amounts of pupil freedom were associated with

pupil gain in skill (Soar, 1973).

Rosenshine (1976a) reported that in the recent inves­

tigations which covered "the same context— primary grade

reading and mathematics for children from low SES back-

rounds . . . . there was convergence on an optimal pattern

for this instruction, which might be labeled as direct in­

struction" (p. 62-63). He identified the characteristics

composing direct instruction as a great deal of time spent

on academic activities, predominance of seatwork using

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 12 -

structured materials, teachers or materials providing imme-

diate feedback using praise and students working in teacher

supervised groups with little free time, resulting in less

off-task student behavior and a gain in achievement. Powell

(1978) suggested that patterns could be expected to be im­

portant. She described the pattern of direct instruction in

which students show a greater achievement in basic skills:

Students spend a good portion of the time allocated


to instruction actually engaged in instructional
tasks, and the teacher monitors and encourages task
engagement on the part of the students, feedback or
praise is given relatively guickly and is related
to the academic work. (Powell, 1978, p. 29)

As can be seen, direct instruction applies to consistencies

of results ’’which do not translate easily into rules or best

methods” (Powell, 1978, p. 29). Good (1979) echoed this ob­

servation when he cited Powell (1978) :

Direct instruction should not be viewed as a set of


prescriptive rules. It should be seen as a concep­
tual orientation that values active teaching, ex­
pository learning, focused learning, focused
learning and accountability. (Good, 1979, p. 55)

A large experiment was conducted in the context of

Follow Through, comparing direct instruction and eight other

major approaches to early childhood education. The approa­

ches that were compared were the Open Classroom Hodel (Edu­

cational Development Center), Cognitively-Oriented Curricu­

lum Model (High/Scope Educational Besearch Foundation), Bank

Street Early Childhood Educational Model (Bank Street Col­

lege of Education), Besponse Education Model (Far West La­

boratory for Educational Research), Tucson Early Education

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 13 -

Model (University of Arizona), Parent Education Model (Uni­

versity of Plorida), Language Development (Bilingual) Model

(Southwest Educational Development laboratory), Behavior A-

nalysis Model (University of Kansas), Direct Instruction

Model (University of Oregon). The effectiveness of these

approaches was studied in terms of basic skill, cognitive,

and affective measures, with Direct Instruction scoring the

highest in each of the three measures. (The Parent Education

Model was also found to be effective, but its major goal is

the training of parents to enhance student achievement, and

not student/teacher interactions, and as such will not be

addressed in this review.) Gordon (1971) found the use of

the Direct Instruction Model to be an effective approach

with both "high risk" students and those working at or above

grade level.

In a review and summarization of the Abt Associates*

Final Report of the Follow-Through Planned Variation

Experiment, Becker (1978) reported that the data show that

direct instruction clearly outperformed other approaches on

nearly all measures. Berliner and Rosenshine (1976) sugges­

ted similar findings in a report of the Beginning Teacher

Evaluation Study, conducted by the California Commission on

Teacher Preparation and Licensing. They reported that the

teachers who were viewed as effective in helping students to

acquire knowledge were engaged in direct instruction. A re­

view of recent process-product studies attempted to identify

effective teaching on the basis of achievement. Three major

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 14 -

conclusions Here presented:

(a) elementary school teachers do exert differen­


tial effects upon student achievement; (b) class­
room management skills are exceedingly important
[necessary but not sufficient]; and (c) a pattern
of teaching behavior called direct instruction
seems to be a useful heuristic for describing ef­
fective elementary classroom teacher. (Good, 1979,
P- 53)

The studies mentioned thus far focused on achievement

in the primary grades. A recent study of the teaching of

basic reading skills attempted to transfer these findings

for the primary grades to the secondary school environment.

This research identified teaching processes that mere ef­

ficient in helping secondary school students learn to read

(Stallings, 1978). The results were similar to those found

for primary grades. The findings suggested that teachers

should:

Have most interaction about reading


Give instructions to the total group
Ask guestions of specific students (no volunteers)
Provide positive feedback
Control negative behavior
Encourage positive behavior
Use guides and probing questions when students do not
know the answer, (p. 18)

Brophy (1979b) reported that recent findings indicated that

direct instruction in small groups, coupled with formal

classroom organization and management are as effective in

producing satisfactory learning results at the secondary

level as they are in teaching basic skills at the primary

levels Although the research is minimal, it does appear that

the findings at the primary grade level may be generalizable

to the secondary level.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 15 -

The literature related to direct instruction has basi­

cally dealt vith English and mathematics skills. It is only

recently that one £inds research in music which relates to

this model. There are two studies, and they are both de­

scriptive in nature. Moore (1981) made a relevant observa­

tion in a study comparing the use of teaching time by Ame­

rican and British elementary music specialists. "The segu­

ential order of events in the observed £ American and En­

glish ] lessons generally proceeded as follows: instruction -

preparation - musical activity - discussion - further in­

struction - musical activity - etc." (p. 66). This is very

similar to the S-R-S model proposed by Becker, Engelmann,

and Thomas (1971). Although it was not Moore’s (1981) in­

tention to do so, this is verification of the existence of

the pattern inherent in the Direct Instruction Model in

elementary school music settings.

In an attempt to examine whether the concepts of the

Direct Instruction Model could be found in high school en­

semble rehearsals, Yarbrough and Price (in press) conducted

a two-phase research study. In the first phase, an observa­

tion instrument was developed and validated. The analysis

included: (a) percentage of off-task behavior; (b) number of

seconds during which performance (entire group and section­

al) and non-performance occurred; (c) freguency of stops;

(d) freguency of academic and social approvals and disap­

provals; (e) freguency of seguence, approval, disapproval,

and conflicting cue errors; (f) frequency of eye contact to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 16 -

the ensemble and individuals; and (g) freguency of complete

and incomplete teaching units. The above variables seemed to

address most, if not all, of the issues brought to light by

previous research.

All observation procedures mere based on technigues


developed in previous behavioral and descriptive
research (Madsen 8 Madsen, 1974; Madsen 8
Yarbrough, 1980; Thurman, 1976), with the exception
of the observation procedure for complete and in­
complete teaching units vhich was developed for
this study and is based on the teaching/learning
model presented by Becker, Engelmann and Thomas
(1971). Observer reliability was computed by di­
viding total agreements by agreements plus disa­
greements. Average reliability was .91. (Yarbrough
S Price, in press)

The procedure divided the "teaching unit" into three parts:

(1) teacher presentation; (2) student response; and, (3)

teacher feedback/reinforcement. These three parts were then

delineated into categories to allow for careful analysis of

all teacher as well as student behaviors. Teacher presenta­

tions were subdivided into four categories; academic, soci­

al, conducting, and off-task. The teacher behavior was also

analyzed for the frequency of eye contact. Student responses

were subdivided into the four categories of performance,

sectional, verbal response, and nonverbal response. Student

attentiveness was also assessed and described as either on-

or off-task. The third part,, teacher feedback/reinforcement,

was analyzed for facial expressions and verbal content, and

then categorized as facial and/or verbal approval and/or

disapproval. These were also analyzed for feedback errors as

defined by the investigators. When this was completed, the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 17 -

parts were analyzed for sequencing (1-2-3) and completion as

completed “teaching units". They found that this model could

be used to analyze and describe most of the behaviors which

occurred in the ensemble rehearsals observed.

Both Moore (1981) and Yarbrough and Price (in press)

used descriptive research methods to examine teaching. In an

earlier experimental research study, Moore (1976) investi­

gated the effect of five differential teaching techniques on

125 college students. Contingency-managed instruction (CMI),

independent study, CMI-lecture discussion, contact control,

and delay control techniques were examined for their effects

on the achievement, attitude, and teaching skills of stu­

dents enrolled in elementary music education classes. All

the groups received the same materials, but the methods of

teaching were different for each group. The CMI and CMI-

lecture discussion groups had higher overall achievement

scores, the CMI groups generally demonstrated higher atti­

tude scores, and CMI-lecture demonstration resulted in sig­

nificantly higher scores for teaching skills. Contingency-

managed instruction includes several concepts which are si­

milar to direct instruction; information or materials are

broken down into small modules, frequent evaluations, and

immediate feedback. Moore (1976) suggested that higher

teaching skills scores for the CMI-lecture demonstration

groups may have been the function of the inclussion of tea­

cher modeling by way of class lecture.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 18 -

Formal Versus Open Structure Classrooms

Inherent In the above review is a comparison of formal

with open structured classrooms. Procedures used in the

University of Kansas and University of Oregon (positive re­

inforcement models) classrooms, which are formal, contribu­

ted more to achievement than five other models (Far Best

Laboratory, Bank street. University of Arizona, High Scope,

Educational Development Center) which were open structure

models (Stallings, 1976). The formal models have consis­

tently been rated superior to open and flexible instruction

on the basis of achievement (Becker, 1978; Berliner &

Bosenshine, 1976; Brophy, 1979a, 1979b; Carnine, 1979;

Powell, 1978; Bosenshine, 1976a, 1977; Soar, 1973; Stal­

lings, 1975, 1976; Stallings S Kaskowitz, 1974).

If one assesses these models on a basis other than

achievement in basic skills, it may be that formal structure

is not superior. Stallings (1976) reported:

Lower absence rates, higher scores on a nonverbal


problem solving test of reasoning can be attributed
in part to more open and flexible instructional
approaches in which children are provided a wide
variety of activities and selected their own groups
part of the time. (p. 16)

However, another major study found that while students in

formal classrooms were better at punctuation, they were no

worse at creative or imaginative writing than pupils in in­

formal classes (Bennett, Jordan, Long, 6 Bade, 1976). In

discussing the results of Bennett et. al. (1976), Bruner

(1976) summarized them and made some critical comments:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 19 -

The main findings can be summed up quickly.


The more formal the teaching, the more time pupils
spend working on the subject matter at hand. And in
general, though with some important exceptions, the
more time pupils spend working on a subject, the
more they improve at it - no huge surprise, but one
that grows in importance as one looks at the re­
sults. For though it may come as no revelation that
students in the more formal classrooms improved
considerably more in reading and in mathematical
skills than the less formally taught, it is more
revealing that pupils in informal settings did not
do any better on their creative writing than their
formally instructed fellows, (p. ix)

In a recent music study, the effects of structured re­

corder (a type of whistle flute) instruction presented with

contingent positive reinforcement was compared with a dis­

covery approach (Dorow 8 Greer, 1977). The structured in­

struction was found to be an effective means for the deve­

lopment of basic performance skills in elementary school

students.

The results [also] shoved that the discovery


method of instruction was not effective for teach­
ing the no-instruction group performance skills on
the recorder. Although students in the no-instruc­
tion group had self-instructional materials avail­
able to them . . . . It is clear that beginning
instrumentalists, students in elementary school
general music programs, or clients in music therapy
instrumental programs need structured instructional
programs. (Dorow 8 Greer, 1977, p. 15)

Three basic factors have been identified in research on

teacher effectiveness: (1) group instruction with the tea­

cher maintaining an academic focus; (2) guantity of time

students are academically engaged; and (3) teacher feedback.

The positive relationship of these factors amounts to a re­

commendation for a formal classroom (Brophy, 1979a;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 20 -

McDonald, 1976a; Bosenshine, 1979; Stallings, 1978).

There are no lists of essential behaviors for


teachers.
. . . Besearch to date also suggests that the
following instructional variables are usually as­
sociated with content covered, academically engaged
minutes, and academic achievement gain: teachers
maintaining a strong academic focus with encou­
ragement and concern for the academic progress of
each student; teacher, rather than student, selec­
tion of activities; grouping of students into small
and large groups for instruction; and using factual
guestions and controlled practice in teacher-led
groups. (Bosenshine, 1979, p. 52)

A consensus of support exists for the use of direct in­

struction in primary grades for basic skills achievement in

mathematic and English. Some research also supports the use

of this model for teaching reading at the secondary level,

but a great deal more research is needed to support its use

above the primary grade level and in other subject areas

(Good, 1979; Heath 8 Nielson, 1974; McDonald, 1976a; Bosen­

shine, 1976b).

It seems unlikely that one set of teaching behavi­


ors is most effective for teaching everything. If
there is an important interaction between type of
content and teaching behavior (given cognitive
achievement as criteria), then the conclusion about
which teaching behavior is effective may be deter­
mined as much by content as by teacher behavior.
(Heath S Nielson, 1974, p. 476)

It [also] seems unlikely that one set of teacher


behaviors is most effective for teaching everything
to everybody. (Heath 6 Nielson, 1974, p. 477)

Neither is there clear evidence regarding the non-cognitive

aspects of learning at any level. In short, "the extent to

which these generalizations need to be modified as one moves

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 21 -

to other grade levels and contexts remains to be determined"

(Bosenshine, 1976b, p. 364).

Reinforcecent in Non-music Settings

The theory of operant conditioning begins with the ba­

sic proposition that an organism will shov an increase in

the strength of any response that is reinforced (Reynolds,

1975; Skinner, 1953). This theory, as applied to a classroom

setting, suggests that students* behaviors are a result of

how they have been reinforced in the past, and that their

future behaviors will be a result of how they are currently

reinforced.

In the Follow Through Planned Variations, the Behavio­

ral Analysis Model and the Direct Instruction Model were

both found to be more effective than six other models

(Carnine, 1979). The Behavioral Analysis Model presented

objectives and focused on the extensive use of reinforcement

to attain these objectives. However, the Behavioral Analysis

Model was not as effective as the Direct Instruction Model

in teaching cognitive or basic skills.

The influence of teacher behavior on the behavior of

students in normal classroom situations has been studied

extensively, mostly by systematically varying ratios of ap­

proval/disapproval classes of teacher behavior. Student be­

havior has been subsequently analyzed for attentiveness and

achievement as well as attitudinal effects. Several reviews

of research in this area have been published (Altman 8

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 22 -

Linton, 1971; Hanly, 1970; Kennedy 6 Willicutt, 1964; Lipe 6

Jang, 1971; Bosenshine, 1971; Schultz S Sherman, 1976), and

they indicate a consensus regarding the effectiveness of the

use of reinforcement by the teacher. All forms of rein­

forcement are effective; however, the most powerful form may

be the use of approval for appropriate behavior in con-junc-

tion with ignoring inappropriate behaviors wherever pos­

sible, while using disapproval as a last resort (Altman £

Linton, 1971; Becker, Hadsen, Arnold £ Thomas, 1967; Hall,

Lund £ Jackson, 1968; Hadsen, Becker 6 Thomas, 1968; Hadsen,

Becker, Thomas, Koser K Plager, 1968; Hadsen £ Hadsen, 1974;

Nolen, Kunzelman, £ Haring, 1967; O eleary £ Becker, 1968;

Stallings £ Kaskowitz, 1974; Thomas, Becker, £ Armstrong,

1968; Tyler £ Brown, 1968). This research has primarily

dealt with the effect of reinforcement on social and aca­

demic behaviors, as well as self-report of student atti­

tudes.

A series of studies was carried out, which systemati­

cally varied teacher approval, disapproval, and ignoring, to

examine their effect on students* social behaviors. Becker,

Hadsen, Arnold, and Thomas (1967) instructed teachers to

provide rules of good classroom conduct, point out these

rules, and to ignore inappropriate behavior. This was found

to be ineffective in reducing inappropriate behaviors. The

teachers were then told that in addition to establishing

rules and ignoring inappropriate behaviors, Vhey should

dispense reinforcement in the form of smiles and praises

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 23 -

only for behaviors which were compatible with learning and

teaching. When this contingent teacher approval was insta­

ted, the rate of appropriate behavior increased signifi­

cantly, while the deviant behavior decreased from an average

of 62.13% to 29.19%. Another study found that teacher ap­

proval, which was contingent upon appropriate behavior, de­

creased inappropriate behavior and no approval increased it

(Thomas, Becker, 6 Armstrong, 1968). In another study, when

students who were out of their seats were told to "sit

down", the incidence of students getting out of their seats

increased markedly, but when being out of seats was ignored

and students were contingently praised for sitting in their

seats, the out of seat behavior was almost extinguished

(Madsen, Becker, Thomas, Koser, & Plager, 1968). These stu­

dies seem to indicate that teacher attention functions to

increase that behavior which is being attended to, regard­

less of whether the behavior is appropriate or not.

It is critical to note that effective reinforcement is

reinforcement which is contingent on a specific behavior or

a set of behaviors.

Sheer incidence of praise does not appear to have


consistent relationships with pupil achievement,
whereas praise used for the positive reinforcement
of pupil response does. (Ounkin & Biddle, 1974,
p. 414)

A study which was designed to increase academic responses

using positive reinforcement changed from contingent to

noncontingent tokens (Nolen, Kunzelman, S Haring, 1967). The

result indicated that a decrease in accuracy when tokens

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 24 -

were noncontingent, and an increase in correct responses

vhen tokens were contingent. It has also been reported that

there was an improvement in correct responses when tokens

were contingent on correct answers, and a decrease in cor­

rect responses when tokens were presented on a noncontingent

basis (Tyler S Brown, 1968). Feedback was also foond to be

superior to no feedback in assisting students in the at­

tainment of self-set goals for performance (Erez, 1976).

Praise and criticism have also been categorized as

either academic or nonacademic in focus (e.g., praise for

reading versus praise for working well in groups) (Stallings

& Kaskowitz, 1974). The relationship of both praise and

criticism to achievement was generally positive when they

were focused on academic activities, while the relationship

of nonacademic feedback to achievement was either mixed or

negative.

It does appear that the theory of operant conditioning,

when applied to a classroom setting, can be functional in

teacher/student interactions. The reinforcement must be

contingent, and in general the optimal application is the

use of teacher approval for appropriate student behaviors,

ignoring of inappropriate behaviors, and disapproval of

inappropriate behaviors as a last resort. These conclusions

are fairly consistent in non-music settings for both social

and academic student behaviors. It remains to be seen if the

application of these operant conditioning concepts functions

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 25 -

as profoundly in music settings such as the music classroom

and the music ensemble rehearsal.

Beinforcement in Kusic Settings

One study used music as the subject matter and had re­

sults which were similar to those of the non-music studies.

Hadsen and Madsen <1972) found a significantly greater

amount of intonational improvement when children were given

instruction and contingent reinforcement, versus no in­

struction and noncontingent reinforcement for improved per­

formance. This study was an exception, in that it did result

in a differential effect of reinforcement on music achieve­

ment. However, the systematic study of teacher behavior's

influence on the behavior of students in music classrooms as

well as musical ensembles has yielded results which indicate

that the use of teacher reinforcement may not function as

dramatically as it does in other classroom situations. Tea­

cher reinforcement does appear to have an effect on student

attitude and attentiveness, but the relationship of rein­

forcement to musical achievement (performance) is not clear.

In one of the earliest studies examining the effects of

approval and disapproval on children's behavior in music

classrooms, Bennett and Adams (1967) found that classes re­

ceiving positive reinforcement were more efficient. They

compared positive, negative, and no reinforcement classes;

and found that positive classes took the least amount of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 26 -

time to achieve desired responses, while the negative clas­

ses took the most amount of time.

Kuhn (1975) examined the effect of reinforcement and

instruction on attentiveness, musical achievement, and at­

titude of fifth-grade students. He employed a four-group

design in which two groups received instruction and rein­

forcement. In one of these two groups, the teacher was 80%

approving and 20% disapproving, while in the other group the

teacher was 20% approving and 80% disapproving. A third

group received instruction, but was not given feedback for

responses, and a fourth group functioned as a contact con­

trol group. There was a significant difference between the

high approval group and all other groups on attentiveness,

but no differences were found between the groups on

achievement or expressed attitude of the students. Forsythe

(1975) compared social behavior in music classrooms to so­

cial behavior in other academic classrooms. He found that

music teachers trained in high approval techniques had more

attentive students than teachers of other subjects. These

results have been replicated while also finding that the

children also spent longer periods listening to music taught

under approval conditions than to music taught under disap­

proval conditions (Dorow, 1977). Dorow (1977) also found

that treatment effects generalized from music selection be­

havior to concert attentiveness. In a classroom setting,

students who received music listening lessons under condi­

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 27 -

tions of adult high approval selected more of that music

than subjects taught under adult low approval (Greer, Dorow,

Wachhaus S White, 1972).

Kuhn (1975) made two recommendations which are germane

to musical research: (1) "Other studies should investigate

the effect of teacher interaction in large-group performance

ensembles" (p. 40); and (2) "perhaps differences [in musical

achievement] would be generated over a longer instructional

period" (p. 47). His study used six 15-minute instructional

lessons for a total of 90 minutes of treatment, but the

measurement instrument may not have been sensitive enough to

detect a musical achievement gain in this study. Hadsen,

Wolfe and Hadsen (1969) paired scale direction with monetary

reinforcement (pennies). The study resulted in no differen­

ces in improved scale singing regardless of reinforcement or

direction. Madsen and Madsen (1972) suggest that one reason

for the inability of standard teacher reinforcement tech­

niques to have the effect on music achievement they appa­

rently have on achievement in non-music settings might be a

function of the subject matter:

When the initial study (Madsen, Wolfe 6 Hadsen,


1969) which used pennies as a contingent reward did
not show significant differences between the group
that received pennies and the group that did not,
it was conjectured that perhaps music listening, as
well as participant music activities, constitute a
learning activity which is "intrinsically reward­
ing". Indeed, if music provides its own reward,
then perhaps the music activity represents the
contingency which is, in and of itself, the "posi­
tive" attraction for the participant. (Madsen 5
Madsen, 1972, p. 196)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 28 -

Music as Reinforcement

In 1955, Jeffrey suggested that music itself might be a

powerful motivator and reinforcer, but it was not until the

mid-sixties that this idea was investigated systematically.

The initial research on the use of music as a motivator and

reinforcer was in the field of music therapy. Music was in­

vestigated for its effect on people (Bonny, Cistrunk,

Makuch, Stevens, 8 Sally, 1965; Podvin, 1967) as well as for

its use as a reinforcer (Barrett, 196 5; Morgan S Lindsley,

1966). Many other studies followed closely (Beisman, 1967;

Bellamy & Sontag, 1973; Bough S Bough, 1965; Cook & Freethy,

1973; Dorow, 1975; Hanser, 1974; Hauck S Martin, 1970;

Jorgenson, 1974; Kramer, 1978; Madsen & Madsen, 1968; Mc­

Carty, McElfresh, Rice, 6 Wilson, 1978; Reid, Hill, Rawers,

5 Montegar, 1975; Sajway 6 Risley, 1970; Steele, 1968;

Talkington 6 Hall, 1970; Underhill 6 Harris, 1974; Wilson,

1976; Wolfe, 1980; and Wolpow, 1976), all of which focused

mainly on the use of music as a reinforcer and have sub­

stantiated it as a powerful reinforcer for learning disabled

and physically or mentally handicapped people.

In a study which used normal classroom students, the

use of music listening as reinforcement for correct mathe­

matical responses, versus the use of music as reinforcement

for attentiveness was investigated (Madsen, Moore, Wagner, 5

Yarbrough, 1975). The students who were reinforced for at­

tentiveness increased attentive behaviors, but the number of

correct math responses did not increase. The students who

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 29 -

were reinforced for correct math responses increased both

attentive behaviors and correct math responses. This is a

case which clearly supports the argument that music, speci­

fically music listening in this case, is a powerful and

useful reinforcer in the classroom. Music listening rein­

forced the specific behavior upon which it was contingent;

if it was contingent on attentiveness, the students were

more attentive but did not increase achievement, whereas it

was effective in increasing both achievement and attentive­

ness when it was contingent on achievement. Prom the results

of this and other studies, it appears that attentiveness "is

a necessary but not a sufficient condition for learning"

(McDonald, 1976b, p. 1361).

The results of this study have been replicated by other

researchers (Madsen 6 Forsythe, 1973; Miller, Dorow, 6

Greer, 1974). Yarbrough, Charboneau, and Wapnick (1977) also

investigated the use of music listening as reinforcement for

correct math and attending in low, middle, and high math a-

bility children. Their results indicated that music listen­

ing was only effective as a reinforcer for the low ability

group when compared with a contact control group. It should

be noted that the higher ability groups did improve with or

without music as a reinforcer, thus reinforcement of the

subject matter may have been internalized, and consequently

external reinforcement was unnecessary and ineffective.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 30 -

Achievement and attentiveness in a musical situation

were also investigated for their relationship to the use of

music listening and pennies as reinforcement by Greer, Ran­

dall, and Timberlake (1971). They used one pre- posttest

only group and four treatment groups, three of which re­

ceived different types of reinforcement (two types of music

listening and pennies), and one which received no rein­

forcement, in an attempt to improve vocal pitch acuity. The

students who were reinforced were specifically reinforced

for attending behaviors, and not for improved vocal pitch

acuity. They found that the reinforced groups were more at­

tentive than the non-reinforced groups, but there were no

differences in vocal intonation gain scores between the

groups. The results here are not surprising in light of

findings by Madsen, et al (1975), and other researchers.

This is another instance in which increased attentiveness

did not result in increased achievement. However the rein­

forcement used by Greer et al. (1971) was contingent on at­

tentiveness and not on achievement. One might also find that

if this study were replicated with the reinforcement con­

tingent on vocal pitch acuity, that it still would not af­

fect achievement due to the reinforcement value of the sub­

ject matter.

While studies have used music as a reinforcer to en­

hance music skills with some success (Madsen & Madsen,

1972) , most recent studies have used music lessons as a re­

inforcer for other academic subjects (Dorow, 1976; Gordon,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 31 -

1980; Hadsen, 1979; Hadsen, Dorow, Moore, S Womble, 1976;

Hadsen S Geringer, 1976). With the exception of Gordon

(1980), all of these studies used contingent televised music

lessons as a reinforcer for achievement in other academic

subjects. The results are consistent across all cf these

studies. The control groups did not gain in music or other

academic areas. The groups which received televised music

lessons as reinforcement gained in music as well as in other

academic areas. In all cases, the students were also recei­

ving both their normal music and other academic instruction.

It is clear from the data presented that music and music

instruction can function as reinforcement for attentiveness

and achievement.

Active Versus Passive Activities

The data indicate that students are more attentive in

music classroom and ensemble rehearsal situations, than in

non-music classrooms. Results of analyses of student atten­

tiveness under high approval music teachers compared to hiqh

approval classroom teachers indicated that students were

still more attentive in music classes (Forsythe, 1977). Some

studies seem to indicate that this may be a consequence of

active versus passive student participation (Forsythe, 1977;

Madsen 5 Madsen, 1972; Madsen & Alley, 1979; Hadsen, Wolfe 6

Madsen, 1969; Spradling, 1980; Yarbrough & Price, in press).

No difference was found in scale singing regardless of di­

rection (ascending or descending scales) or reinforcement;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 32 -

however a significantly greater amount of on-task behavior

was observed when children were singing when compared to

listening to directions or music (Hadsen, Wolfe, £ Hadsen,

1969). In another instance, attentiveness was examined in

various music classroom activities. Students appeared to be

more on-task during classroom activities requiring active

musical responding such as singing and playing instruments,

and more off-task during passive activities such as in­

struction and verbal student/teacher interactions (Forsythe,

1977). Hadsen and Alley (1979) found music rehearsals

unique, in that students are least off-task in choral and

instrumental music rehearsals, when compared to general

music and other academic classes, despite low approval ra­

tios of conductors. This suggests that music classroom ac­

tivities require more active participation resulting in an

increase in student attentiveness.

Wagner and Struhl (1979) compared pre-intern, intern,

and experienced elementary music specialists* (teachers*)

use of teaching time, reinforcement, and the student atti­

tudes toward them. Rate of reinforcement and student atti­

tudes did not differ between these groups. With regard to

teaching time, a significant difference was found in only

one teaching activity. Experienced teachers gave directions

in approximately half the time it took pre-interns and in­

terns. Several researchers (Madsen S Hadsen, 1972; Hadsen,

Wolfe, 6 Madsen, 1969; Forsythe, 1977) have reported that

directions resulted in the highest off-task of all the ac-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 33 -

tivities analyzed in their studies, which suggests that in

the music classroom context a passive activity results in

increased off-task. It may be that the experienced teachers

had vicariously learned this, and consequently reduced di­

rection time to increase student attentiveness.

Some studies in non-music settings have found a rela­

tionship of active engagement on academic tasks to achieve­

ment gains. Brophy and Evertson (1974) observed experienced

second- and third-grade teachers in non-music classrooms,

and found that maintained active engagement of students in

appropriate learning tasks was positively related to

achievement gains, although there were differential effects

in high- versus low-socioeconomic status (SES) schools. The

implications of these findings are germane to any research

which involves musical ensemble situations, due to the fact

that the focus of ensemble rehearsals is on the active par­

ticipation of the ensemble members though practice and per­

formance.

Research in Music Ensemble Settings

Besearch in music ensemble settings is relatively

sparse. Student behaviors have been investigated in high

approval and high disapproval rehearsals (Murray, 1975). No

significant differences were found in off-task behavior or

musical achievement of mixed choruses among three experi­

mental conditions: (1) regular conductor; (2) experimental

conductor, high approval; (3) experimental conductor, high

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 34 -

disapproval. He did find that students liked the mssic as

veil as the rehearsal of that music more under an 80% ap­

proval condition than under an 80* disapproval condition.

The lack of differences in attentiveness nay have been due

to the lack of control of active (singing) versus passive

(instruction and directions) time.

A study by Spradling (1969), used a director rehearsing

tvo junior high school bands in vhich each time the director

stopped the music to correct a mistake, he also approved an

appropriate behavior. He reported that this resulted in more

favorable attitudes and more efficient rehearsals. Wachhaus

and Wachhaus (1976) found that a majority of ensemble mem­

bers felt better and more motivated on the days the con­

ductor made more positive than negative comments.

In J975, Yarbrough used a unigue approach in maintain­

ing equivalent reinforcement ratios and frequencies across

two rehearsal conditions, but varied the magnitude to a-

chieve two conditions: (1) high magnitude; and (2) low mag­

nitude. No significant differences were found in attentive­

ness between the two conditions. No significant differences

were found in musical achievement across the regular, high

magnitude, and low magnitude directors. There was a signi­

ficant difference in mean attitude ratings, indicating a

preference for the high magnitude conductor over the low

magnitude conductor. This result is striking due to the fact

that the high magnitude and low magnitude conductors were

the same person. The study involved one university and three

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 35 -

high school mixed choruses, consequently the small "n" re­

duced the probability of achieving statistically significant

differences. With this point in mind, it should be noted

that the low magnitude condition resulted in the highest

off-task and lowest performance (achievement) ratings. This

was the first time that a trend in attentiveness and per­

formance had been indicated as a result of any conductor

behaviors. In the review of literature, Yarbrough (1975)

suggested that;

Behavioral studies in music that have manipulated


the behavior of the conductor or music teacher in
order to modify the behavior of the student can be
classified into three general areas: social beha­
vior, academic behavior, and music preference/at­
titude. These studies indicate that in the case of
performing groups, the teacher is not the source of
reinforcement that maintains appropriate behavior.
Instead it is suggested that music itself functions
as the reinforcement which maintains attending be­
havior. That is, regardless of the nature of tea­
cher reinforcement, approval or disapproval, stu­
dents appear to be on-task in music performance
groups, (p. 135)

Two descriptive studies have examined the activities

involved in musical ensemble rehearsals in tbs secondary

schools (Thurman, 1976; Yarbrough C Price, in press). Thur­

man (1976) analyzed rehearsal of five choruses, and found

that the conductors devoted approximately 40% of their re­

hearsal time to verbal communications, with a range of 58%

to 29%. Yarbrough and Price (in press) analyzed rehearsals

of three bands, two choruses, and one orchestra for percen­

tage of performance and non-performance time. They found

that the conductors devoted approximately 43% of their re-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 36 -

hearsal tine to non-performance, with a range of 62* to 31X,

both extremes vere band rehearsals. In the first attempt to

experimentally control and investigate some of these factors

in a musical ensemble rehearsal situation, Spradling (1980)

investigated the effect of frequency and duration of stops

(non-performance or passive) on attentiveness and attitude.

For this study, only instruction occurred during the stops,

and performance was perceived as reinforcement. Besults in­

dicated a significantly higher off-task rate during timeout

(passive) periods than during performance (active) periods.

Also some relationship was indicated between timeout periods

and expressed music preference; the duration and freguency

of stops had an inverse relationship to a positive music

preference.

Yarbrough and Price (in press) examined the relation­

ship of teacher/student activity to student attentiveness.

The analysis included percentage of off-task behavior; num­

ber of seconds during which performance (entire group and

sectional) and non-performance occurred; freguency of stops;

frequency of academic and social approvals and disapprovals,

freguency of seguence, approval, disapproval, and conflic­

ting cue errors; frequency of eye contact to the ensemble

and individuals; and frequency of complete and incomplete

teaching units. When all the variables were analyzed for a

relationship to attentiveness and the error factor of the

individual contribution of the conductors/teachers was ac­

counted for, two critical relationships emerged; (1) acti­

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 37 -

vity (performance versus non-performance), i.e. students

were more on-task while performing; and (2) teacher eye

contact, i.e. more teacher eye contact correlated positively

with on-task.

Results of research in music ensemble settings are even

less clear than those results for the music classroom set­

tings. There are several explanations for the lack of cohe­

sive findings in this research, the major one being the li­

mited number of research studies in this area. Of these, few

were experimental in nature, thus making it difficult to

define cause and effect relationships. The studies which did

not use active versus passive activities as an independent

variable (Hurray, 1975; Yarbrough, 1975), also did not con­

trol for it, and thus the results cannot be discriminated as

a conseguence of activity or treatment. Another difficulty

in the examination of teacher effect in an ensemble rehear­

sal is the fact that music functions as a reinforcer. It

also should be noted that a sensitive, well-defined, and

consistent assessment of achievement in an ensemble is dif­

ficult at best. In addition to all these problems, all pre­

vious ensemble research has dealt with small sample sizes,

which markedly decreased the probability of finding statis­

tically significant relationships.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 38 -

Summary

'The foregoing literature emphasized the role of the

teacher in eliciting student responses. In both music and

non-music classrooms, teachers have been able to affect

student attentiveness, achievement, and attitude. Hhile

these studies evidence that the teacher may be the control­

ling (reinforcing) factor, the results for music activities

and music ensemble rehearsals do suggest other critical

factors.

Educational research has recognized the importance of

reinforcement, direct instruction, and student activity. It

has investigated each of these factors and found them to

have some effect on student behaviors. In reviewing both the

non-music and music literature, it has become apparent that

the combination of instruction, activity, and reinforcement

need to be analyzed for their effect on attentiveness, at­

titude and achievement in the music ensemble rehearsal set­

tings. This has been made possible by the theoretical model

advanced by Becker, Engelmann, and Thomas (19711, and the

analysis instrument developed by Yarbrough and Price (in

press). With the use of this instrument, the act of teaching

can be analyzed as a sequential whole or parts, composed of

teacher presentation of tasks, student responses, and tea­

cher reinforcement/feedback.

The research on musical ensemble rehearsals is minimal,

and the only clear conductor/teacher effect is on student

attitudes, although some other trends have been indicated.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 39 -

It seems that tvo factors other than the teacher have

emerged as being critical; the power of music as reinforce­

ment, and active versus passive student tasks. The guestion

arises as to how to use rehearsal time most efficiently;

reinforcement versus no reinforcement, and performing versus

instruction. Therefore the purpose of this study was to de-

termine the effects of three treatments; (fi) directions on­

ly, followed by student performance; (B) academic task pre­

sentations, followed by directions, and then student per­

formance; and (C) academic task presentations, directions,

student performance, followed by contingent teacher rein­

forcement of student performance. The dependent measures

used in this study were attentiveness, performance, and at­

titude.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER III

EXPERIMENTAL METHOD

Subjects

Subjects were the members of the Syracuse University

Symphonic Band at Syracuse University in Syracuse, New York.

The band was a noa-auditioned group composed primarily of

non-music majors, with a total of 48 participants. The num­

ber attending each rehearsal varied from as few as 30 stu­

dents present to the maximum participation of all students.

Selection of Music

Prior to the experimental phase of the study, lists of

graded band compositions compiled by the New York State

School Music Association, and the Florida Bandmasters Asso­

ciation, were reviewed by the Director of Bands and the ex­

perimenter. From this list, music at an appropriate level of

difficulty was identified and 13 comparable compositions

were selected (see Appendix A). Only nine of the selections

were sightread by the Symphonic Band and ranked by each

member of the ensemble, due to an inadeguate amount of time

to read all thirteen compositions on the pretest session.

The ensemble members were given a form listing all thirteen

compositions listed, and were instructed to number the

(nine) compositions which they had sightread from one (most

preferred) to nine (least preferred), after they had sight-

- 40 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 41 -

read all of the compositions {see Appendix B). The four se­

lections ranked in the middle (four through seven) Here used

in the experiment. These selections were not rated signifi­

cantly different in preference as tested by the Kruskal-

Wallis one way analysis of variance (Siegel, 1956; p104-

193). Since the addition of any other selection which had

been sightread would have resulted iu using music which was

rated as significantly different, one of the four selec­

tions, a multi-movement work, was divided into three sec­

tions which were comparable in length and difficulty. Thus,

six musical selections were chosen for six experimental

conditions (see Appendix C).

Experimental Environment

The experiment was conducted during the regular re­

hearsal time and in the normal rehearsal room. Two experi­

mental teachers, one observer, two media technicians, and

audio-visual equipment surrounded the group during the

experiment. Two rehearsals, each 2 1/2 hours in length, were

used for acclimation purposes. During the second rehearsal

the students sightread and ranked the musical compositions

which were used in this study. Throughout the duration of

the experiment regular rehearsals continued within the ex­

perimental environment.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 42 -

Experimental Design

The investigation included a pretest session, five

treatment sessions, and a posttest session. Each of the five

treatment sessions included six musical compositions dis­

tributed across three treatments, by two conductors, in a

pre- posttest design to test the effect of conductor beha­

vior (see Appendix D). The three treatments were: (1) di­

rections followed by ensemble performance; (2) academic task

presentations followed by directions and ensemble perfor­

mance; and (3) academic task presentations, directions, en­

semble performance, followed by conductor reinforcement.

Each of the six musical compositions was paired with one of

the three treatments throughout, such that two conditions

received the same treatment with different musical composi­

tions being used for each of the two conductors. Two expe­

rimental conductors administered each of the three treat­

ments, and thus each rehearsed three compositions every

session. The conductors alternated the three compositions

which they rehearsed and the six compositions were presented

in a different random order each session, to control for

experimenter and order effect.

The sightreading of the compositions served as the

pretest for musical performance. Following each treatment,

the musical composition was played in its entirety to assess

any change in musical performance ratings.. After the last

treatment session, one additional session was scheduled in

order to play all the musical compositions without any

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 43 -

treatments preceding them. This was used as a posttest

assessment for musical performance ratings.

Treatment

Each treatment was 12 minutes in length. Conductors

were given the following instructions for all treatments;

a) Maintain eye contact with group and/or indivi­


duals throughout treatment sessions;

b) Constantly vary volume of voice. Use a wide


range of volume as well as speaking pitch. Voice
should reflect "enthusiasm and vitality;

c) Use arms and hands to aid in musical phrasing.


Use a great variety of movements. Vary size of
conducting patterns to indicate phrases, dyna­
mics, etc.

d) No verbalizations should occur during ensemble


performance.

Conductors were given the following instructions for Treat­

ment _A:

a) Verbalizations are only to consist of directions


which tell the ensemble at what point to begin
in the music. These are not to consist of any
feedback, or academic task presentations;

b) Allow the ensemble to perform as much as pos­


sible ;

c) The face should be aneutral mask at all times.

The instructions for Treatment Bwere:

a) In addition to directions, verbalizations are to


include academic task presentations. The verba­
lizations are not to consist of any feedback or
reinforcement, and must comprise 50% of the
treatment time.

b) The ensemble must perform for 50% of the treat­


ment time;

c) The face should be a neutral mask at all times.

The instructions for Treatment Cwere:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
a) In addition to directions and academic task
presentations, verbalizations are to include
appropriate reinforcement to the ensemble for
the performance immediately preceding. The re­
inforcement should consist of 80% academic ap­
provals and 20% academic disapprovals. Rein­
forcement should be directly related not only to
student performance, but also to the task pre­
sented. Academic task presentations and rein­
forcement should each comprise 25% percent of
the treatment time.

b) The ensemble must perform for 50% of the treat­


ment time;

c) The face should should appropriately reflect the


verbal reinforcement with sharp contrasts be­
tween approval and disapproval. Facial approval
is to be expressed by nodding the head verti­
cally, grinning, laughing aloud, smiling, rais­
ing eyebrows, and widening eyes. Facial disap­
proval is expressed by shaking the head hori­
zontally, frowning, knitting the eyebrows,
pursing the lips and narrowing the eyes.

The conductors* rehearsal time was monitored by a

trained observer listening to a pre-recorded signal tape.

The observer cued the conductors by pressing a switch con­

nected to signal lights mounted on the conductors* music

stand, out of the view of the members of the ensemble

(Spradling, 1980). A blue light cued the conductors to give

academic task presentations and/or directions, a white light

cued the conductor to have the ensemble play, and a green

light cued the conductor to give reinforcement.

Control of the conductors* approval/disapproval ratio

of verbal responses was achieved by providing a feedback

chart indicating the frequency of each type of response

(Murray, 1975 and Yarbrough, 1975). Two flip charts were

mounted on a music stand in view of the conductors, but

outside the view of the ensemble members. One chart had

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 45 -

green numbers representing approvals; the other, red numbers

representing disapprovals. Following each approval or dis­

approval given by the experimenter, an observer flipped the

corresponding green or red card.

Experimenters

The experimental conductors were people with both high

school and college-level conducting and teaching experience,

trained in behavioral classroom management technigues. Both

conductors also were experienced in research on conducting

and ensemble rehearsals.

The experimental conductors monitored each others* use

of treatment time and cued each other by means of signal

lights. Another observer monitored approvals and disappro­

vals using the flip cards previously described.

One media technician operated videotape eguipment in­

cluding microphone, audiocassette player, the television

camera focused on the conductor, videotape recorder, special

effects generator, and microphone mixer. Another technician

operated the camera focused on the ensemble members, and a

third technician operated all audiotape eguipment including

microphone and reel-to-reel tape deck.

Equipment

Audiotape and videotape recordings of the conductors

and students were made using an audiotape and a videotape

recorder, as well as two cameras and a special effects gen-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 46 -

erator to provide a split-screen effect for ex post facto

analyses. Videotapes were used for assessment of attentive­

ness and documentation of treatments. A prerecorded cassette

tape providing observation cues was used to synchronize ob­

servations of videotapes by mixing it into the sound on the

videotape. Audiotape recordings of musical performances were

used for subsequent judgement of musical achievement (for a

complete list of eguipment see Appendix E).

Measurement Procedures

Frequency of Off-task

After the experiment two trained observers recorded

overt student off-task behavior by viewing the videotapes.

Observations were time-sampled, consisting of a 10-second

“observe” interval followed by a 5-second “record" interval.

Observe cues were dubbed onto the videotapes. The camera

focused on a maximum of six students during each "observe”

interval and was rotated on the "record” cue throughout each

rehearsal so that all ensemble members were observed several

times. Student on/off-task was defined as follows:

On-task.: Active — When student is supposed to be


singing/playing he must be sing­
ing/playing and looking at the
music or teacher.
Passive - When student is not supposed to be
singing/playing he must be quiet
and looking at either music ,
teacher, or chorus/band/orchestra
members who are singing/playing.
Other --- Student must follow instructions
given by teacher.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 47 -

Off-Task: Observably not on-task (Madsen and Yarbrough,


1980, p~56) .

Using portions of The Instrumental Rehearsal Observation

Form (Madsen 6 Yarbrough, 1980, p. 55-60), observers counted

the total number of students in view and the number obser­

vably off-task for each 10-second interval. A percentage of

off-task was then computed for each condition by dividing

the number of students observed off-task by the total number

observed in that 12-minute segment. Thus, one measure of

off-task was obtained for each condition observed each day.

Frequency of Teacher Eye Contact

Using a portion of The Music Conductor Observation Form

(Madsen & Yarbrough, 1980, p. 61-63), teacher eye contact

was observed and recorded according to the definitions lis­

ted below:

G - Conductor is looking at entire group or section


for at least three continuous seconds.

I - Conductor is looking at individual in group or


accompanist for at least three continuous sec­
onds.

M - Conductor is looking at music for at least


three continuous seconds.

0 - Conductor is looking at something other than


group, individuals , or music, e.g., ceiling,
floor. Mark ”0" only if it occurs for the en­
tire interval (Madsen and Yarbrough, 1980, p.
63).

Total eye contact was considered the sum of the frequency of

group and individual eye contact.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 48 -

Behears al Time

Time spent in performance and non-performance vas mea­

sured in seconds through observation of the videotape re­

cordings of the treatments. A continuous time log was re­

corded using a stop-watch accurate to 1/10 of a second.

Performance and non-performance were defined as follows:

Performance: Playing or singing by the entire en­


semble or by smaller units (sections)
of the entire ensemble.

Non-performance: Teacher direction, instruction or re­


inforcement. Anything not involving
students* musical performance
(Yarbrough and Price, in press).

Musical Performance Batinqs

A panel of three expert judges independently heard au­

diotape recordings of all pre- and posttest performances,

and used an adaptation of the Performance Bating Form

(Madsen and Yarbrough, 1980, p. 141) to rate each perfor­

mance (see Appendix F).

Attitude Assessment

Attitude was assessed by means of an attitude scale

which had been adapted from the Attitude Survey for Perfor­

mance Group (Madsen and Yarbrough, 1980, p. 143). This five

question scale was administered to the members of the musi­

cal ensemble following each condition of the days* session

(see Appendix G). This procedure was used on the first,

third, fourth, and fifth session, in an effort to assess

trends in attitudes, as well as daily attitudes.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 49 -

Teaching Units

Videotape recordings were also used to analyze and

count complete teaching units. A completed teaching unit was

defined as consisting of three sequential parts: (1) teacher

presentation of a task; (2) student responses; and (3) tea­

cher reinforcement for student responses (Yarbrough and

Price, in press).

Each of the three basic parts of the teaching unit was

delineated into categories, coded, and the codes were re­

corded onto a summary sheet. The sheet was divided into

lines which coincided with the "observe" that was dubbed on

the videotape recordings.

The teacher presentation of a task was number coded 1

(one). This part of a teaching unit was subdivided into five

categories each of which was coded according to their defi­

nition (see Figure 1).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. - 50 -

Figure l. Teacher Presentation of Task: Codes, Definitions, Examples.

Codes Definitions Examples

Academic task presentation; telling 1. VP lay open G, Larry."


students how to play a passage or 2. "Space before the dotted quarter
where to begin playing. and then hold i t ."
3. "Look forward to the top note and
then breathe after it."

Social task presentation; telling 1. "Nod at m e . . Show me you're awake."


students how to behave or making 2. "There are still people who owe
announcements about future activi­ dinner money."
ties . 3. "Lesson sign-ups will be next week.
Be prepared to fill out the forms."

Conducting task presentation; a 1. A crouch and a warning glance with


nonverbal modeling of the music or finger placed over lips (meaning
a nonverbal conducting signal for "softer").
dynamic changes, beginning to play, 2. Right hand maintains beat pattern
stopping, style, phrasing, tempo, while left hand index finger points
and so forth. upward (meaning pitch is flat, ad­
just it) .
3 . Right hand maintains beat pattern
while left hand, palm upward, fin­
gers fluttering to and fro indicates
more expressive style of playing.

Off-task statements not related to 1. "If I can keep my score in place."


social or academic tasks. 2. "Once more. I'm trying to memorize
my c u e s ."
3. "Oh, she's absent. What a day she
picked to be absent."
- 51 -

The second part of the teaching unit, student response, vas

number coded 2 (two) and subdivided into four categories

each of which vas coded according to the definitions as

follow:

2p = Performance/rehearsal by the entire ensemble;

2s = Sectional performance/rehearsal;

2n = Non-performance/rehearsal by any or all


sections of the ensemble;

2v = Appropriate verbal (talking) response;

2nv= Non-verbal response; for example, marking a


part, or nodding a head;

Finally, the third part of a complete teaching unit, teach­

ing feedback reinforcement, was coded 3 (three) and subdi­

vided into six categories each of which was coded according

to their definition (see Figure 2)•

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 52 -

Figure 2» Teacher reinforcement of student responses: codes, definitions, examples

Codes Definitions Examples

Verbal academic or social ap­ 1. "That's what it's supposed to


3v a
proval; positive statement sound like." (Academic)
about student behavior. 2. "That's good, clarinets. Good
attack and release." (Academic)
3. "Thanks for lending me your music
(Social)
4. "You've worked very hard and now
you're excited about the music."
V
(Social)

Verbal academic or social 1. "Altos, you're not breathing cor­


3v d
disapproval; negative state­ rectly." (Academic)
ment about student behavior. 2. "It's not in tune." (Academic)
3. "Don't help him, Nancy." (Social)
4. "Shhh." (Social)

Facial approval; a positive Smiling, laughing, nodding head up


3f a facial gesture. and down.

Facial disapproval; a nega­ Frowning, grimacing, nodding head


3f d
tive facial gesture. from side to side.

Error; approving of an in­ 1. "i know it's hard" (approving of


or appropriate behavior, ap­ complaining).
proval mistake. 2. Smiling or laughing at wrong notes..

Error; disapproving of an 1. "Too much effort." (disapproving


appropriate behavior; dis­ of working hard).
or approval mistake. 2. Frowning, shaking head even when
performance has improved..

(^vd/fa) Error; conflicting feedback


involving verbal expression
1. "You blew it!" (while smiling).
2. "That was wonderful!" (while
or which is different from frowning— sarcasm).
facial expression.
(^VA/FE>) Teacher tells students to play in
Type 1 Teaching unit sequence mis­ tune (task presention), students play
Sequence take. Reinforcement not re­ in tune (student response), teacher
Error lated to task presented. dissapproves of poor balance.
(Teacher reinforcement unrelated to
task presented).

Type 2 Teaching unit sequence mis­ Teacher asks students to play softly
Sequence take; presentation of a new (task presentation), students play
Error task followed by reinforce­ softly (student response) , teacher
ment of a previous task as asks students to play staccato (task
an afterthought. presentation), teacher approves of
students' playing softly. A correct
sequence would have teacher approval
of students' playing softly immedi­
ately following the students' response.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 53 -

Teaching units vere then analyzed for completion and

content. Videotapes were also observed in order to carefully

note and accurately describe facial expressions, subtle ap­

provals and disapprovals, and seguence errors. All units

which followed a 1-2-3 seguence, without error, were consi­

dered complete units. Also, a 1-2 seguence in which the

student response was performance or sectional was considered

complete, recognizing the possibility that music performance

may serve not only as student response but also as rein­

forcement.

Finally, academic and social approvals, disapprovals,

and errors (see Figure 2) were identified and counted. A

facial or verbal reinforcement was counted as one approval

or disapproval. If a verbal
r
>
reinforcement was paired with an

appropriate (not conflicting) facial reinforcement, it was

scored as two approvals or two disapprovals in an attempt to

assess the magnitude of the reinforcement. This yielded an

approval/disapproval ratio and the reinforcement frequency

for each condition. The freguency and duration of teacher

presentations as well as student responses were also ex­

tracted for further documentation and analyses.

Reliability

All observations, except musical rating and attitude,

were synchronous, ie. data were gathered every 15 seconds,

so that it was possible to compare all variables within any

15-second interval of time. Observer reliability, for all

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 54 -

synchronous observations, was computed by comparing each

observational symbol during every interval, and using the

following formula:

__________Humber of agreements________ __
Number of agreements plus disagreements

Forty percent of the videotapes were independently

viewed by both observers for the purpose of determining

inter-observer reliability. Two observers were used for the

Instrumental Rehearsal Observation Form to observe student

activity and off-task. Reliability on 40* of the cases was

computed for the two observers. The average reliability was

.96, with a range of .9 3 to 1.00. Two observers recorded

observations for the category of eye contact on the Music

Conductor Observation Form for a sample of 40* of the vide­

otapes. Average reliability was computed for all observation

in the sample which resulted in an average reliability of

.91, with a range of .73 to .96.

Two observers recorded observations with the Teaching

Unit System of Observation. Reliability on 40* of the cases

was computed for the two observers. The average reliability

was .95. When the reliability was computed by treatment, the

treatment with the smallest variety of behaviors (Treatment

A) resulted in the highest and the one with the largest va­

riety of behaviors (Treatment C) resulted in the lowest

average reliability, although they were all above .90 (see

Table 1).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 55 -

Table 1

TEACHING UNITS SYSTEM OF OBSEBVATION

AVERAGE RELIABILITY BY TREATMENT SUMMARY

Treatment Reliability

A) .98
n%
ijjf .94

C) .93

Interjudge reliability (agreement) on music performance

scores was tested for each treatment using a Kendall coef­

ficient of concordance (Siegel, 1956, pp. 229-238). Table 2

shows the results of the analyses of the three treatments by

collapsing the seven ratings for each composition across

each treatment, thus yielding 14 scores for each treatment

by each judge. A significant (alpha level = .05) concor­

dance, among judges* ratings, was found to exist for per­

formance ratings by treatments.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 56 -

Table 2

KENDALL COEFFICIENT OF CONCOBDANCE SOMHABY

FOB HOSIC PERFORMANCE SCORES

Treatment s H df Chi Sguare

A) 1601.5 .783 13 30.56**

B) 1192.0 .585 13 22.82*

C) 1543.0 .757 13 29.54**

*p<.05 **p<.01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER Iv

RESULTS

The purpose of this study was to determine the effects

of teacher academic task presentations, student performance,

and teacher reinforcement of student performances on per­

formance, attentiveness, and attitude of students in a uni­

versity symphonic band. At the completion of this study five

hours of videotape recordings had been made of the band un­

der three treatments: (1) directions only, followed by stu­

dent performance; (2) academic task presentations, followed

by directions and student performance; and (3) academic task

presentations, directions, student performance, followed by

contingent teacher reinforcement. The three treatments were

replicated by a different conductor.

Videotapes of all treatments were first analyzed to

document whether treatments were implemented as defined a

priori. Ex post facto observation of the videotapes were

completed by behavioral observers using the Instrumental

Rehearsal Observation Form (Madsen and Yarbrough, 1980,

p. 58), the Music Conductor Observation Form (Madsen and

Yarbrough, 1980, p. 61) and the Teaching Onit System of Ob­

servation developed by Yarbrough and Price (in press). The

two forms used recorded frequencies of (1) student activity,

(2) students off-task, (3) conductor approval/disapproval

verbal reinforcement, (4) eye contact, and (5) approval/

- 57 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 58 -

disapproval facial expressions. Using the technique deve­

loped by Yarbrough and Price, the treatments were further

analyzed by identifying academic task presentations, direc­

tions, off-task comments, ensemble performance, student

verbal response, student non-verbal response, and verbal and

facial reinforcement. These behaviors were then analyzed for

their duration and order, so that the amount of time spent

in each category of activity, and the sequencing of these

behaviors could be assessed, a panel of expert judges inde­

pendently rated the audiotaped pre- and posttest perfor­

mances using a Performance Bating Form (Madsen and

Yarbrough, 1930, p. 141) (see appendix F). Ensemble attitude

was assessed by administering a modification of an attitude

Survey for Performance Groups (Madsen and Yarbrough, 1980,

p. 143) (see appendix G).

Treatment

Before analyzing the effects of treatments on atten­

tiveness, attitude, and achievement, it was necessary to

determine whether the treatments were implemented as

planned. Data gathered through the use of the Teaching Unit

Observation System (Yarbrough and Price, in press) were a-

nalyzed to indicate the relationship of the experimental

conductors* actual implementation of the treatments to the a

priori operational definition of those treatments. Data

consisted of the amount of time spent on directions, aca­

demic task presentations, student performance, and verbal

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 59 -

approvals/disapprovals. The treatments were further analyzed

for frequency of verbal approvals/disapprovals, facial ap­

provals/disapprovals, and errors of reinforcement, as well

as completed direction and academic teaching units. Table 1

provides a summary of the mean amount of time, in seconds,

spent on directions, academic task presentations, student

performance, and reinforcement for each treatment. In the

results. Treatment A refers to the treatment in which only

directions were provided by the conductors followed by stu­

dent performance; Treatment B to the one in which directions

and academic tasks were presented followed by student per­

formance, but no reinforcement was provided; and Treatment C

refers to the treatment in which reinforcement was provided

after the sequence of academic task presentations, direc­

tions followed by student performance.

It is clear that the three treatments were signifi­

cantly different from each other based on comparisons of the

amount of time spent giving directions, academic task pre­

sentations, and reinforcements, as well as comparisons of

the amount of time students spent performing (see Table 3

and Appendix H).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 60 -

Table 3

MEAN TIME SPENT IN SECONDS FOB TBEATMENT BEHAVIOfiS

Treatment Directions Academic Performance Beinforcement


Tasks

A) 123 7 561 2

B) 61 287 336 12

C) 58 186 326 130

Table 4 provides a summary of the mean frequencies of

completed direction units, completed academic units, and

reinforcement which includes both verbal and facial rein­

forcement.

Table 4

MEAN FREQUENCY OF UNITS AND REINFORCEMENT

Treatment Directions Academic Reinforcement


Presentations

A) 9.9 .8 1.3

B) .4 8.1 3.7

C) .6 7.8 26.1

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 61 -

In summary. Treatment A contained more time in direc­

tions than either Treatment B or C, less time in academic

task presentations than either Treatment B or C, more time

in student performance than either Treatment B or C, and

less reinforcement time than Treatment C. Treatment B con­

tained less direction time than Treatment A, more academic

task presentation time than either Treatment A or C, less

student performance time than Treatment A, and less rein­

forcement time than Treatment C- Treatment C contained less

time in directions than Treatment A, less time in academic

task presentations than Treatment B, but more than Treatment

A, less time in student performance than Treatment A, and

more reinforcement time than either Treatment A or B. There

mere no differences between conductors or among days for

directions, academic task presentations, student performance

or reinforcement. Data thus demonstrate that the treatments

were implemented according to the a priori definitions ac­

curately by both conductors, independent of days {see Ap­

pendices H and I ) .

Beinforcement Frequency

The frequency of reinforcement during Treatments A and

B was similar (mean frequency of reinforcement for Treatment

A = 1.3 reinforcements; mean frequency of reinforcement for

Treatment B = 3.7 reinforcements). However, results indicate

that Treatment C contained considerably more reinforcements

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 62 -

than either Treatment A or B (mean frequency of reinforce­

ment for Treatment C = 26.1 reinforcements).

Freguency of reinforcement was further analyzed for

differences between conductors and among days. The total

frequency of reinforcement by both conductors was similar

(mean frequency of reinforcement for Conductor 2 = 10.1

reinforcements; mean frequency of reinforcement for Con­

ductor 1 = 10.6 reinforcements). Frequency of reinforcement

each day also appeared to be similar (mean frequency of re­

inforcement for Day 1 = 12.7 reinforcements; Day 2 =11.0

reinforcements; Day 3 = 7.8 reinforcements; Day 4 = 9.0

reinforcements; Day 5 = 11.3 reinforcements). As might be

expected* data indicate that the only treatment which by a

priori definition was supposed to include reinforcement

(Treatment C ) , did contain a much higher frequency of rein­

forcement (see Appendix I).

Direction Units

The frequency of direction units (units in which stu­

dent performance is preceded by directions with no academic

presentations) was similar for Treatments B and C (mean

frequency of direction units for Treatment B = .4 units;

mean frequency of direction units for Treatment C = .6 u-

nits). Treatment A contained considerably more direction u-

nits than either Treatment B or C (mean frequency of direc­

tion units for Treatment A = 9.9 units).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 63 -

Freguency of direction units was further analyzed for

differences between conductors and among days. The freguency

of direction units administered by both conductors was very

close (mean freguency of direction units for Conductor 2 =

7.8 units; mean freguency of direction units for Conductor 1

= 7 . 4 units). Freguency of direction units for each day also

appeared to be similar (mean freguency of direction units

for Day 1 = 4.2 units; Day 2 = 5.0 units; Day 3 = 2.8 units;

Day 4 = 2.8 units; Day 5 = 3.3 units).

Treatment A had more direction units than Treatments 5

or C, while Treatments B and C had comparable amounts of

direction units. According to the a priori definition,

Treatment A was to include directions and exclude academic

task presentations, thus the student performance could only

have been preceded by directions and not academic task pre­

sentations. These results indicate that Treatment A was im­

plemented in accordance with the a priori definition (see

Appendix I).

Academic Presentation Dnits

The freguency of academic presentation units (units in

which student performance is preceded by academic presenta­

tions and then directions) was similar for Treatments B and

C (mean freguency of academic units for Treatment B = 8.1

units; mean freguency of academic units for Treatment C =

7.8 units). Treatment A contained considerably fewer aca­

demic units than either Treatment B or C (mean freguency of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 64 -

academic units for Treatment A = ,8 units).

Freguency of academic units mas further analyzed for

differences among conductors and days. The freguency of

academic units administered by both conductors was very

close (mean freguency of academic units for Conductor 2 =

5.4 units; mean freguency of academic units for Conductor 1

= 5 . 7 units). Freguency of academic units for each day also

appeared to be similar (mean freguency of academic units for

Day 1 = 5.5 units; Day 2 = 5.2 units; Day 3 = 5.8 units; Day

4 = 5.8 units; Day 5 = 5.5 units).

Treatment A had fever academic units while Treatments B

and C had comparable amounts of academic units. According to

the a priori definitions for Treatments B and C, student

performance was to be preceded by both academic task pre­

sentations and directions. The definition of a complete

academic unit was; an academic task presentation followed by

directions, and then student performance. The presence of

reinforcement was not necessary for an academic unit to be

counted as complete. These results indicate that Treatments

B and C were implemented in accordance with the a priori

definitions, since Treatment A had significantly fewer aca­

demic units than either Treatment B or C, and Treatments B

and C had a comparable number (see Appendix I).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 65 -

Student attentiveness

The measure of student attentiveness was computed by

observing the videotapes of the treatments. Observers coun­

ted the number of performers in view and those observably

off-task for each 15-second interval. A percentage of off-

task behavior was then computed for each 12-minute treatment

session by dividing the number of students off-task by the

total number observed for that treatment session (see Ap­

pendix J). Table 5 is a summary of the percentage of off-

task by treatment and day.

Table 5

MEAN PERCENTAGE OF STUDENTS OFF-TASK: TREATMENT BY DAY

i -1" - 1™ ™

satment Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5

A) 6.73 7.31 5.78 9. 15 5.26

B5 15.38 18.04 16.36 14.05 11.22

C) 10.80 17.86 13.64 11.79 11.38

The following figure (see Figure 3) is a graphical

display of the data in Table 5. It demonstrates that the

students (ensemble members) were least off-task during

Treatment A. There also appears to be a trend towards less

off-task for Treatment C than for Treatment B.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 66 -

Figure 3

OFF-TASK BEHAVIOR

o
o
C\J

o
o
m
CO

Cl)
o
o

PERCENTAGE STUDENT OFF-TASK

in _L (2) (2)

(2)
a>
Cl)
Cl) (2)
O
o
JB
CO
/C3>\

O ......
O C3) C3)
CO ***-#C3>
<3>
O
o
CO

o
o +
1. 0 2.0 3.0 II. 0 5.0
SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 67 -

Less off-task occurred during the performance part of

each of the treatments than during the non-performance part

(see Table 6).

Table 6

MEAN PEBCENTAGE OF OFF-TASK BEBAVIOB: ACTIVITY BY TBEATMENT

Activity Treatment A Treatment B Treatment c

Non-
Performance 20.66 25.98 22.97

Performance 2.84 2.89 2.18

Upon re-examination of data in Figure 3, it was found that

off-task bore some relationship to performance time (see

Appendix J ) • The number by each point in the figure repre­

sents an ordering of the guantity of performance time, with

(1) being the least amount of performance time and (3) being

the most. Treatment A had much more performance time and

therefore consistently less off-task than either Treatment B

or C. The results for Treatments B and C are not quite as

apparent and indicate the possibility of a relationship be­

tween off-task and both treatment and performance time.

However, when performance time is compared to off-task data,

it may make a stronger case for a treatment effect. For ex­

ample, even when there were 32 seconds more performance time

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 68 -

in Treatment B (Session 4.0 in Figure 3), it still resulted

in a higher percentage of students off-task, and the one

instance in which Treatment C had a higher off-task, it

contained 27 seconds less performance time (Session 5.0 in

Figure 3). The most performance time (Treatment A) consis­

tently resulted in the lowest student off-task. However, the

least performance time did not always result in the highest

off-task. In the latter instance, it may be that attentive­

ness was not only a function of performance time, but was

also affected by the treatments.

Since the amount of performance time may have been re­

lated to the quantity of off-task during a treatment, the

portion of the rehearsal which did not involve performance

was analyzed for percentage of student off-task. Hon-per-

formance off-task percentage also appears to be a function,

in part, of performance time. Figure 4 is a plot of off-task

percentage during non-performance time. The number by each

point on the plot represents the quantity of non-performance

time, with (1) being the least amount of performance time,

and (3) being the most. In three instances, the least per­

formance time corresponds to the highest percentage of off-

task, and in three instances the most performance time cor­

responds to the lowest off-task. Either the more the stu­

dents performed, the more attentive they were when not per­

forming, or it may be that they had less opportunity to be

inattentive. However, again Treatment C resulted in less

off-task than Treatment B, regardless of performance time,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 69 -

except on the fifth day. During session 3, Treatment c con­

tained less off-task than Treatment A which had signifi­

cantly more performance time.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 70 -

Figure 4

OFF-TASK BEHAVIOR DURING KON-PERFOHMANCE

o
o

CO
CO
CD
o
PERCENTAGE STUDENT OFF-TASK DURING NON-PERFORMANCE

o
m
o rC2)
CO
C3>
CD
CD
m
(2)
cu

o
CD /C2)
m
=t*
C\J (3> (2)
o
o

/N
CM
(3) CD
CD
O
CO
CD C2)

CD
CD
LO J- C3)
C3)

o
o
CM

1. 0 2.0 3.0 U.O 5.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 71 -

u U S x C u l. n C h i. c 7 £ S t d lt

A panel of three expert judges, all of whom had expe­

rience in conducting clinics, public school and university

ensembles, and adjudicating, heard seven audiotaped pre­

postperformances of each composition in random order. Into­

nation, blend, balance, tempo, dynamics, tone guality, rhy­

thm, phrasing, ensemble, articulation, style, and over-all

artistic effect (musicality) were rated from 1 (poor) to 4

(excellent) .

Hean musical achievement scores for the treatments are

summarized in Table 7 (see Appendix K). The mean gain for

Treatment C was highest (9.53) although that for Treatment A

was close (9.08). Treatment B resulted in the smallest gain

(4.85) which is almost one-half of that for Treatment C.

While the mean treatment scores on the pretest had a narrow

range (1.67), the range on the posttest was more than three

times larger (5.53), indicating a differential effect among

treatments as was also evidenced by the gain scores.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 72 -

Table 7

MEAN PBE- POSTTEST GAIN SCORES FOR MUSICAL ACHIEVEMENT

TREATMENT SUMMARY

Treatment Pretest Posttest Gain

- "

A) 12.50 21.58 9.08

B) 13.32 18.17 4.85

C) 14. 17 23.70 9.53

RANGE: 1.67 5.53 4.68

Figure 5 is a plot of mean achievement scores for each

treatment by day (see Appendix K). One might note that for

three of the five posttreatment performances the mean rat­

ings for Treatment A mere lowest, and for the other two,

mean ratings for Treatment B were lowest. Mean achievement

scores for Treatment C were consistently higher. While these

data clearly are not conclusive, it is possible that there

may have been a treatment effect as measured by performance

ratings.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 73 -

Figure 5

KEAN MUSIC PERFORMANCE RATINGS BY TREATMENT AND DAY

o
CD
m
CU

O
O
OJ
CU

o s.
o
m
Q
CM
MUSIC PERFORMANCE SCORE

O
O
co J-

o
o
(6 _L

o
o

o /
o
cu J.

o
o
m
o + 1—
PRE 1.0 2.0 3.0 u.O 5.0 POST
SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 74 -

Student Attitude

An attitude survey, in the form of a paper and pencil

test, was completed by each student following each treat­

ment, on the first, third, fourth, and fifth days. This

survey consisted of five questions in which they rated how

much they liked the music, enjoyed the rehearsal, liked the

conductor, and were "turned on" during the rehearsal, as

well as whether they thought the conductor was a good tea­

cher. These items were scaled from 1 (strongly disagree) to

10 {strongly agree) , thus the higher the score the more po­

sitive the rating.

The data for each of the five guestions were analyzed

by use of a multiple regression technique (Statistical Ana­

lysis System, General linear Models Procedure, release 79.3A

of SAS at Syracuse University), with student responses to

each question as the dependent measures. Carrier variables

(Hosteller and Tukey, 1977; Neter and Wasserman, 1974) for

these regression analyses were treatments, compositions

nested within treatments, conductors, interaction of treat­

ments with conductors, and interaction of compositions with

conductors. These carrier variables were coded and entered

respectively using a hierarchical model in which the re­

sponses to each of the five items were used as the dependent

measure. Variability due to individual subject differences

was accounted for by using a repeated measures design. The

ques tiOuS were also analyzed by the use of graphical dis­

plays for treatment responses.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 75 -

Music

The responses to the survey item in which the students

rated the statement "I like this music11. were analyzed by

the use of multiple regression and graphical analysis tech­

niques. The factors tkat were found to have a significant

relationship (alpha level = .05} were subjects, composi­

tions, and conductors (see Table 8). Treatments were not a

significant factor in the responses to this survey item.

Table 8

DEPENDENT VARIABLE: MUSIC RATING

MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSIS SUMMARY

Source df SS MS F

MODEL 58 1184.32 20.42 7.87***

ERROR 752 1950.13 2. 59 ---

CORRECTED TOTAL 810 3134.45 --- ---

SUBJECT 47 887.11 16. 88 7.28***

TREATMENT 2 11.79 5.90 2.27

COMPOSITION(TREATMENT) 3 246.16 82.05 31.64***

CONDUCTOR 1 17.62 17.62 6.79**

TREATMENT *CONDUCTOR 2 1.64 -82 .32

COMPOSITION*CONDUCTOR 3 20.00 6.66 2.57

**p<.01 ***p<. 0001

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 76 -

Figure 6 is a plot of the mean music preference ratings

of the music for each composition by day (see Appendix L).

As indicated by the regression analysis in Table 8, one can

see that a clear relationship exists between the liking of

the music and the compositions themselves. Three of the

compositions were consistently rated lower than the other

three. These ratings are comparable to the pretest rankings

from which the compositions were selected. Each of the three

treatments is represented in both the sets of lower and

higher rated compositions.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 77 -

figure 6

PLOT OF MUSIC EATING BY MUSIC AND DAY


MUStC LIKING RATING

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 78 -

During the first and third days (data points 1 and 2 of

Figures 5 through 11; the attitude survey was not adminis­

tered on the second day) of this study, the conductors con­

ducted the same compositions; Conductor 1 conducted the

higher rated compositions, and Conductor 2 conducted the

lover rated compositions. On the fourth and fifth days (data

points 3 and 4 of Figures 5 through 11), the conductors

switched the compositions, thus accounting for the change in

attitudes toward the compositions displayed in Figure 6, and

the experimenter effect indicated in Table 8.

Rehearsal

The responses to the survey item in which the students

rated the statement "I enjoyed rehearsing this music", were

also analyzed by the use of multiple regression and graphi­

cal analysis technigues. The factors which were found to

have a significant relationship to rehearsal enjoyment rat­

ings (alpha level = .05) were subjects, treatments, compo­

sitions, conductors, and the interaction of compositions

with conductors (see Table 9). It appears that all the fac­

tors in the model, with the exception of the interaction of

treatments with conductors, had an effect on the way the

enjoyment of the rehearsal was rated by the subjects.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 79 -

Table 9

DEPENDENT VARIABLE: REHEARSAL ENJOYMENT RATING

MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSIS SUMMARY

Source df SS MS F

MODEL 58 1391.82 24.00 9.45***

ERROR 752 1908.60 2.54 ---

CORRECTED TOTAL 810 3300.42 ——_

SUBJECT 47 995.04 21. 17 8. 34***

TREATMENT 2 30.43 15.21 5. 99**

COMPOSITION(TREATMENT) 3 270.11 90.04 35. 48***

CONDUCTOR 1 22.76 22.76 8. 97**

TREATMENT*C0NDUCT0R 2 .02 .01 • 00

C0MP0SITI0N*C0NDUCT0R 3 73.46 24.49 9. 65***

**p<.01 ***p<. 0001

Figure 7 is a plot of the mean ratings of rehearsal

enjoyment by treatment and day (see Appendix M). Treatment B

was rated the lowest in rehearsal enjoyment on all days,

while Treatment C was rated the highest on three of the four

days, and nearly tied with Treatment A on the fourth. It may

be that treatment had an effect on the enjoyment of the re­

hearsal, or that music had an effect on the enjoyment of the

rehearsal, or both.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 80 -

Figure 7

PLOT OF REHEARSAL R A TI N G BY T R E A T M E N T AND DAY

to
o

o
to
to

o
to
REHEARSAL' ENJOYMENT RATING

to

o
= 1*
to

o
<u
to

o
o
to

o
to
in

o
to
in
2.0 3.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 81 -

"Turned O n ”

The responses to the survey item in which the students

rated the statement “during this rehearsal I was (turned on,

to turned off)” , were also analyzed by use of multiple re­

gression and graphical analysis techniques. The factors

which were found to have a significant relationship (alpha

level = .05) were subjects, treatments, compositions, con­

ductors, and the interaction of compositions with the con­

ductors (see Table 10). Evidently all the factors in the

model had an effect on the students* affective state.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 82

Table 10

DEPENDENT VARIABLE: "TURNED ON " RATING

MULTIPLE DEGRESSION ANALYSIS SUMMARY

Source df SS MS F

MODEL 58 1040.96 17.95 9.39***

ERROR 752 1436.96 1.91 ---

CORRECTED TOTAL 810 2477. 93 ---

SUBJECT 47 773.08 16.45 8.61***

TREATMENT 2 34.61 17.30 9. 06***

COMPOSITION(TREATMENT) 3 147. 19 49.06 25.68***

CONDUCTOR 1 42.69 42.69 22.34***

TREATMENT*CONDUCTOR 2 . 14 .07 .04

COMPOSITION*CONDUCTOR 3 43.27 14.42 7.55***

***p<. 0001

Figure 8 is a plot of the mean rating of how "turned

on" the students were during each treatment by day (see Ap-

pendix N ) . Here a pattern similar to that found in Figure 7

can be seen. The students rated Treatment C highest in every

instance. Treatment A next highest in every instance, and

Treatment B lowest in every instance. It appears that rein­

forcement (Treatment C) had a more positive function than

did p e r f o r m a n c e time (Treatment A ) . Also performance time

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 83 -

had a more positive function than academic task presenta­

tions with no reinforcement (Treatment B).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 84 -

Figure 8

PLOT OF "TOBNED ON 11 BATING FOB TBEATHENTS BY DAY

in
o

o
CO
to

CO
TURNED ON" RATING

o
CO
CO

to

o
CO

CO

o
o
CO
2.0 3.0 U.O 5.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 85 -

Conductor

The responses to the survey item in which the students

rated the statement MI like this conductor", were also ana­

lyzed by the use of multiple regression and graphical ana­

lysis techniques. The factors which were found to have a

significant relationship (alpha level = .05) were subjects,

compositions, conductors, and the interaction of composi­

tions with conductors (see Table 11). It should be noted

that the treatments had no apparent effect on how much the

students "like" the conductors, but there appeared to be

some discrimination for compositions and conductors.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 86 -

Table 11

DEPENDENT VARIABLE: LIKING THE CONDUCTOR BATING

MULTIPLE BEGBESSION ANALYSIS SUMMARY

Source df SS MS F

MODEL 58 1544.02 26.62 22.28***

ERROR 744 888.96 1.20 ---

CORRECTED TOTAL 802 2432.98

SUBJECT 47 764. 17 16.26 13.61***

TREATMENT 2 .35 . 18 . 15

COMPOSITION(TREATMENT) 3 56.76 18.92 15.83***

CONDUCTOR 1 693.38 693.38 580.31***

TR EATMENT*CONDUCTOR 2 1.20 .60 .50

COMPOSITION*CONDUCTOR 3 28. 16 9.38 7.85***

***p<.0001

Figure 9 is a plot of the mean rating of how much the

students liked the conductor by treatment and day {see Ap­

pendix 0). Here a pattern emerges similar to that found in

Figure 6, which is a plot of the responses to the item re­

garding enjoyment of music, with a more dramatic interac­

tion. Again the conductors traded the compositions which

they rehearsed on the fourth day so that the top group of

three scores was consistently with the same conductor. While

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 87 -

there may be some consistency in the ratings for the musical

compositions across days, clearly the conductors had a pro­

found effect on the responses to this item as is indicated

by the regression analysis (see Table 11)-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PLOT OF "LIKING” THE CONDUCTOR BY TREATMENT AND DAY

o
CO

o
CO
LIKING OF CONDUCTOR RATING

o
to
r-

o
CM

o
o

2.0 3.0 5.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 89 -

In light of this, student preference ratings for "liking”

the conductor were analyzed for comparing the experimental

conductors (see Appendix 0). It was found that the students

consistently expressed a greater "liking" for Conductor 1

than for Conductor 2 (see Figure 10).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 90 -

Figure 10

PLOT OF " L I K I N G " T H E C O N D U C T O B BY C O N D U C T O B AND DAY

o
o
CO

o
in
co

o
LIKING OF CONDUCTOR RATING

o
co

o
10

o
o

o
10
to

o
o
to H — -I— H— H
2.0 3.0 1.0 5.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 91 -

Teacher

The responses to the survey item in which the student

rated the statement "this conductor is a good teacher", were

also analyzed by the use of multiple regression and graphi­

cal analysis techniques. The factors which were found to

have a significant relationship (alpha level = .05) were

subjects, treatments, compositions, conductors, and the in­

teraction of compositions with conductors (see Table 12).

Once again, it appears as though all the factors in the mo­

del, with the exception of the interaction of treatments

with conductors, had an effect on the manner in which the

students rated the conductor as a teacher.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 92 -

Table 12

DEPENDENT VARIABLE: TEACHER RATING

MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSIS SUMMARY

Source df SS MS

MODEL 58 1362.83 23.50 22.86***

ERROR 737 757.44 1.03 ---

CORRECTED TOTAL 795 2120.26 --- ---

SUBJECT 47 917.94 19.53 19.00***

TREATMENT 2 14.42 7.21 7.01**

COMPOSITION(TREATMENT) 3 27. 18 9.06 8.82***

CONDUCTOR 1 382.71 382.71 372.38***

TREATMENT*CONDUCTOR 2 1.93 .96 .94

COMPOSITION*CONDUCTOfi 3 18.65 6.22 6.05**

**p<.001 ***p<. 0001

Figure 11 is a plot of the rating of the conductor as a

teacher, with the higher rating being a better rating (see

Appendix P). This is the first instance in which this unigue

pattern emerges. In Treatment C, the teacher was rated the

highest at every data point, this was the teacher that gave

them some academic task, direction, had the students per­

form, and gave them reinforcement. This high rating of

Treatment C was not unusual, however in this instance,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 93 -

Treatment B received a higher overall rating by day than did

A, for the first and only time. Apparently, while the stu­

dents may have rated the music, rehearsal, and their affec­

tive state higher for Treatment A than B, they still per­

ceived the treatment with academic task presentations as

being more educational in nature.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 94 -

Figure 11

PL OT OF C O N D U C T O R AS A " G O O D TEACHES*' BY T R E A T M E N T AN D DAY

o
CO

o
CO
GOCJ TEACHER RATING

o
n*
/ /

o
CM

C3
o

o
CO
CO -I------- + — I
1.0 2.0 3.0 «i.O 5.0

SESSION

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 95 -

In summary, for all survey items analyzed in Tables 8

through 12, the effects of compositions, conductors, and

their interactions were significant. Both the music which

was used and the conductor had an effect on the manner in

which students responded to all the survey items. In Tables

9, 10 and 12, a treatment effect was found. The treatments

did have an effect on the students* ratings of rehearsal

enjoyment, affective state ("turned o n " ) , and the conductor

as a good teacher; however, no treatment effect was indica­

ted in rating the liking of the music or the conductor. It

is also interesting to note that even though the students

enjoyed the rehearsal more, and were more "turned on" for

Treatment A than B (see Figures 7 £ 8), they rated the con­

ductor in Treatment B as a better teacher than the one in

Treatment A (see Figure 11). In the instances where a

treatment effect was found, Treatment C was consistently

rated highest overall.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Results from this study indicate that the different

rehearsal approaches or treatments had some effects on stu­

dent attentiveness, achievement, and attitudes. Before one

can try to interpret the meaning of these results, differ­

ences and similarities among the treatments need to be enu­

merated.

There were three treatments. Treatment A consisted of

directions followed by student performance. Treatment B

consisted of academic task presentations, and directions

followed by performance. Treatment C consisted of academic

task presentations, directions, and performance followed by

reinforcement. The treatments were all approximately 12 mi­

nutes {+ or - 10 seconds) in length, and were followed by a

posttest which consisted of performing the composition just

rehearsed in the treatment. The compositions used consisted

of six segments of compositions approximately 4 minutes in

duration, and were of comparable level of difficulty.

Treatments B and C consisted of eight 90-second units

with 45 seconds each spent in teacher verbalization and

student performance. Live observers monitored, cued, and

gave immediate feedback to the conductors who were control­

ling the treatments. The frequency and duration of units in

Treatment A were not defined a priori in an effort to ac-

- 96 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 97 -

complish as much performance time as possible, and therefore

they were not monitored. However, without the advantages of

this cuing, monitoring, and feedback, the conductors tended

to stop more freguently, and consequently this resulted in

more teaching units than either Treatment B or C for the

first two days of this study. After the first treatment day,

the conductors were informed of the disparity, nevertheless

attempts at self-correction proved fruitless. Thus it was

necessary to implement a cuing, monitoring, and feedback

system comparable to that used in the other two treatments.

From the third day to the conclusion of the study, the fre­

quency of stops and teaching units appeared comparable for

all treatments. Treatment A still had slightly more stops

and teaching units after the second day, since the increased

performance time resulted in reaching the end of the compo­

sition being rehearsed more freguently, and starting again,

thus incurring more stops and teaching units.

The mean time spent in the teaching units and their

verbal and performance components were comparable for

Treatments B and C. Treatment A was considerably different

due in part to the a priori definition of as much perfor­

mance time as possible, and in part to the initial problem

of the conductors stopping more freguently, resulting in

more teaching units.

This study attempted to maintain the duration of the

entire teaching unit constant while varying the quality and

duration of its contents. The experimental conductors were

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 98 -

able to accomplish this with the cuing, monitoring, and

feedback techniques used in this study. Varying the duration

of the teaching unit as a whole was not examined in this

study, but clearly bears examination since there are tasks

which may require more or less time to present, perform,

and/or reinforce, and these variations may have a measurable

effect on student responses. This study also did not deal

with social task presentations, verbal and nonverbal student

responses, or social reinforcement. The social content of a

teaching unit may have a strong relationship to attentive­

ness and possibly, due to increased attentiveness, to

achievement, especially in the public schools. It may be

that a wider variety of student responses, both verbal and

nonverbal, may affect cognitive development, and possibly

increase music achievement. This variety may also result in

increased attentiveness and/or improved attitudes^

Student Attentiveness

If one looked at the data for student attentiveness in

a cursory manner, the assumption of treatment effect would

be quite evident. Indeed the percentage of students off-task

was a function of treatment. However, another critical fac­

tor in controlling attentiveness was the quantity of per­

formance time within the treatment. In every instance the

treatment with the largest quantity of performance time had

the lowest off-task; Treatment A had the most performance

time every day by definition and implementation, and this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 99 -

resulted in the students being the least off-task during the

treatment. This replicates the results of past music re­

search on the relationship of attentiveness to active (per­

formance) and passive (non-performance) activities

(Forsythe, 1977; Madsen and Alley, 1979; Madsen, Wolfe and

Madsen, 1969; Spradling, 1980; Yarbrough and Price, in

press).

The results for Treatments B and C are not readily ex­

plained by the quantity of performance time. On four of five

days Treatment C resulted in lower off-task, even when it

contained as much as 32 seconds less performance time than

Treatment B. In the only instance in which Treatment C re­

sulted in higher off-task than Treatment E, it had 27 sec­

onds less performance time. These two treatments were in­

tended to have comparable amounts of performance time and

they indeed were not very different. However, Treatment B

did have a slightly higher mean for performance time, which

should have resulted in slightly lower off-task if this were

strictly a function of performance time. It did not. Conse­

quently, it does appear that there were differences in off-

task due to treatments and performance time.

Although performance time was clearly a powerful fac­

tor, within certain limitations, reinforcement (Treatment C)

may also have had an effect above and beyond the effect of

performance time in reducing the percentage of student off-

task. The effect of reinforcement (Treatment C) might have

been more pronounced had the reinforcement been contingent

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 100 -

on social instead of academic behaviors. However, for the

purposes of this study all the tasks and reinforcements

dealt only with academic behaviors. Furthermore, it may be

that students were more attentive when being reinforced for

their performances than when tasks were being presented.

Attention to the social aspects of a rehearsal is war­

ranted in future research. It may be that increased atten­

tiveness does not result in increased achievement as was

found by Madsen, Moore, Wagner, and Yarbrough (1975). At no

point in the current study did total off-task (i.e. off-task

across both performance plus non-performance portions of

rehearsal sessions) exceed 19% for any treatment. However,

off-task for the non-performance portions of the treatment

sessions consistently exceeded 20%, while the performance

portions never exceeded 3%. This study was done with a uni­

versity group and these figures for off-task are not ex­

ceedingly high when compared to those for the public

schools. Forsythe (1975) reported a mean of 17% off-task for

non-music classes and a mean of 8% off-task for music clas­

ses. In a later study he reported a range of 1% off-task for

active activities to 32% for passive activities in the music

classroom (Forsythe, 1977). Madsen and Alley (1979) found

pupil off-task ranged from 13% to 56%, with the lowest off-

task occuring in ensembles. It is possible that the results

for the treatments might have been more disparate had the

study been extended to the public school with its younger

and less mature students. It may be worthwhile to study

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 101 -

music rehearsals at several age and ability levels with

careful attention to reducing off-task during non-perfor­

mance and thus hopefully, but not necessarily, increasing

musical achievement.

Music Achievement

Although research on the teaching of music performance

in a music ensemble situation is minimal, based upon these

data it appears that musical achievement is the most elusive

of the three dependent measures used in this study. Here a

gain in musical achievement was found for all three treat­

ments, and while the pretest scores were similar. Treatments

A and C were found to have received higher posttest scores

than Treatment B, and also showed a considerably larger

gain. Treatment C had a larger gain and posttest score than

Treatment B, although the difference between the two treat­

ments vas small.

These results support the notion that given eguivalent

amounts of performance time and verbal time, the addition of

academic reinforcement (Treatment C) might produce higher

performance achievement. This supports results of earlier

research on the effects of feedback versus no feedback on

academic achievement (Erez, 1976; Nolen, Kunzelman, &

Haring, 1967; Tyler 6 Brown, 1968). Treatment C was the only

treatment which did not show a decrease from the last

treatment session to the posttest which was two days later.

It may be that reinforcement assisted the students in the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 102 -

retention and transfer of that vhich they had learned to the

posttest performance. It should be remembered that this was

the only treatment in which the students were reinforced,

and that the only purpose of this reinforcement was to im­

prove performance. However, since improved performance is an

understood objective for all performing ensembles, it is

understandable that there was a positive gain for all

treatments.

There was also no attempt to measure cognitive gains in

this study. Had this been attempted, the treatments with

academic content might have shown a greater gain than the

one without. To date, there has been little research vhich

has shown a relationship between improved musical and cog­

nitive gains.

All treatments exhibited gains in musical achievement,

which may be attributed to the factors of maturation, prac­

tice, and/or experience. These are clearly factors in

Treatment A where academic task presentations and rein­

forcements were almost non-existent= This study vas done

with a university group and one might assume that they had

more experience and knowledge than many secondary and ele­

mentary school ensembles. The added experience may have

helped this ensemble to improve with little benefit from the

teacher, and the treatment effects may be of a higher mag­

nitude in direct relationship to the maturity and experience

of the ensemble; the younger the ensemble, the more it may

need the guidance of a teacher. It should also be noted that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 103 -

this ensemble had met only once before the study began. The

recency of the ensemble's inception may have resulted in an

exaggeration of the maturation factor for ensemble perfor­

mance, vhich might not have been as large a contributor had

the study been executed later in the term, thus allowing the

ensemble time to stabilize.

Student Attitude

Responses to the five-question attitude survey indicate

that the students discriminated according to the item and

the treatment. These responses can be classified into two

categories: those which do and those vhich do not reflect a

treatment effect. Two of the item responses, one in which

the students rated how much they liked the music, and the

other in which they rated how much the liked the conductor,

did not appear to have a strong relationship to the treat­

ments, but did relate to compositions and conductors. The

responses to the other three items in which rehearsal en­

joyment, affective state (’’turned on” versus ’’turned off”) ,

and the conductor as a teacher did show a significant rela­

tionship to the treatments in addition to the compositions

and conductors. The responses to all five questions indicate

that a music and conductor effect on attitudes was in evi­

dence for all treatments.

The music effect is fairly straightforward and clear.

The ensemble consistently rated three of the compositions

superior to the other three. This preference reflected the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 104 -

pre- and posttest rankings which were done independently of

the treatments. On the pretest ranking? the largest group of

compositions which were not ranked significantly different

from each other contained four compositions. The fourth

composition, a lengthy multi-movement work was divided into

three sections comparable in length, difficulty, and style.

This composition was the lowest rated of the four and the

three sections of this composition were consistently rated

lover than the other three compositions throughout the

study, thus a music effect existed.

Although there are no treatment data to explain the

effects of the different conductors, there are a number of

feasible standard explanations. The data for the independent

variables demonstrated that the conductors did administer

the treatments as defined a priori, and did so with a com­

parable content of these independent variables within each

category. It is clear that these variables were controlled

and manipulated as planned, so what vas responsible for the

differences in the student responses to the conductors?

There are two highly probable explanations for the

differences in responses to the conductors. First, it is

possible that the disparity of the responses was the func­

tion of an authority figure bias on the part of the stu­

dents. One conductor was the Director of Bands at the uni­

versity, while the other conductor was a doctoral candidate

and full-time student. This may have resulted in the stu­

dents feeling that the Director of Bands was a better tea­

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 105 -

cher in spite o£ treatment. The second possible explanation

is that although this ensemble had never met before this

study began, many of the students had been in another en­

semble under the Director of Bands the previous semester.

Very few of the students knew the doctoral candidate^ This

difference in subject/conductor interaction histories might

have resulted in an a priori bias in favor of the Director

of Bands. Another, less scientific explanation might be that

the Director of Bands was a better teacher and a more lika­

ble person.

In spite of the fact that the effects of the composi­

tions and conductors permeated the responses to all the

guestions in the attitude survey, treatment effects were

found in the responses to three of the five items. Two of

the three item response patterns are similar. In the re­

sponses for rehearsal enjoyment and "turned on", for the

eight data points of the two items combined. Treatment B

received the lowest rating in every instance and Treatment C

received the highest rating for seven of the eight data

points. It is clear that the students enjoyed the overall

rehearsal more when they were reinforced than when they were

not, given comparable amounts of performance time. There

were more favorable responses under Treatment C than Treat­

ment A which may indicate that reinforcement is more moti­

vating than being able to play more.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 106 -

The third item responses vhich indicated a treatment

effect were the ones in vhich the students rated the con­

ductor as a teacher. This item revealed a unigue response

pattern relative to the other four items on the survey. For

this item the conductor for Treatment C received the highest

rating as a teacher for all four days, for Treatment B re­

ceived the next highest rating for three of the four days,

and for Treatment A received the lowest rating on three of

the four days, with the conductors for A and B virtually

tied on one day. The fact that the conductor for Treatment C

received the highest rating is not unusual for any of the

survey items, but the fact that Treatment B was rated higher

than Treatment A is unigue. On every other item Treatment B

was rated the lowest on every occasion. This was the only

item in which the students were asked to rate any kind of

competence such as the teaching skills of the conductor.

They evidently felt that the conductor under the treatment

which involved academic task presentations, student perfor­

mance, and reinforcement vas better than the one under the

treatment which involved only task presentations and student

performance without reinforcement. However, the treatment

without reinforcement was still rated higher than the situ­

ation in which the primary content was student performance.

Evidently while the students preferred the situation in

which they played more (Treatment A) than the one in which

academic tasks were presented without reinforcement (Treat­

ment B ) , they perceived the latter as better teaching. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 107 -

character of this ensemble may help to explain this apparent

dichotomy. It may be that this ensemble did not value

learning as highly as playing; it vas a non-audition group

mostly composed of non-music majors who were taking the

course as an elective or for no credit. It is possible that

the students just wanted to play in a group and were not

interested in developing any further. Learning and enjoyment

are not necessarily synonymous. Bith all this taken into

account, it should be remembered that Treatment C still r e ­

ceived the highest rating for all the items, so that it was

perceived as the most enjoyable, and the conductor was also

rated as the best teacher.

The results from this survey seem to indicate that al­

though the compositions and conductors were perceived dif­

ferently, there appeared to be a treatment effect. The data

point to the power of both the use of positive teacher re­

inforcement and music for reinforcement as teaching tools in

a rehearsal situation. In this situation, students rated

both the treatment where there was more performance time but

no teaching, and the treatment in which instruction oc­

curred, and student performance was followed by positive

reinforcement positively, although they rated the conductor

as a poorer teacher in the first instance. It does appear

that the students enjoyed the learning situation in vhich

there was positive reinforcement the most (Treatment C) .

However, when there was no teacher reinforcement (Treatments

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 108 -

A and B), they preferred to just perform and not receive any

academic instruction (Treatment A).

Design

A summary of design advantages and disadvantages is

important both in assessing the results and the implications

of its future use (see Appendix C).

Conductors were rotated to control for experimenter

effect. Compositions were paired with the same treatment

throughout, but they were presented in a different random

order each day. Thus there was a different random order for

treatments, conductors, and compositions for each day. The

purpose of the randomization was to control for an order

effect.

The pairing of compositions with treatments instead of

randomly distributing them across different treatments each

day created a confounding which prevented the isolation of

treatment effects from composition effects. In an effort to

compensate for this confounding, compositions which were not

ranked significantly different on a pretest survey of the

ensemble members were used. The top three rated were ran­

domly assigned to the three treatments, as were the bottom

three, so that each treatment was paired with two different

compositions. The pairing of two different compositions with

the same treatment was done in an effort to strengthen a

case for treatment effect. Besults could be analyzed for

compositions within treatments as well as across treatments.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 109 -

This allowed for the partialling out of the music and

treatment effects. This a priori decision was a tradeoff in

an effort to look for musical gains over more than one day.

Since musical improvement can be a slow process, it was

hoped that by continuing the treatments over a period of

time longer than one rehearsal from pre- to posttest that

some musical achievement would be in evidence.

The original design called for 10 treatment sessions

extending over six weeks, but after five rehearsals in which

students had been exposed to each 4-minute composition for a

total of 90 minutes, students appeared to be less respon­

sive, and performance level appeared to have peaked, so the

study was terminated. Even with this reduction in the length

of the study, gains and an implication of some treatment

effects on those gains were found.

It is important to note that there are clear limita­

tions in the interpretation of the results and the genera-

lizability of this study. It was done with only one ensemble

over a period of three weeks. This ensemble was a university

level group which was composed mostly of non-music majors.

These results may not generalize to other school level en­

sembles such as high school, middle school, elementary

school, or even a high performance level university ensemble

of music majors. This ensemble may even have been atypical

when compared to others with similar characteristics.

Therefore, this can only be considered an initial step, and

the questions regarding the limitations will only be clari­

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 110 -

fied by future research which may or may not replicate the

findings of this study in other ensemble situations.

Implications for Future Besearch

The results in this study imply that this may be a

fruitful course of research to continue. The teaching model

and research design used here could be applied to different

age and ability levels in various music settings to examine

their feasibility and usefulness. The effects of this model

could be studied in elementary, junior and senior high

school, as well as in other university ensemble rehearsal

situations, to examine its effect at various maturity le­

vels. It may be that the lower student levels would benefit

more from this direct instruction approach, thus yielding a

greater disparity between treatments at elementary level

than at the university level. It may be that the elementary

students* gains in performance without the aid of a teacher

would not be nearly as great as those for more advanced

students. One might hypothesize that more advanced students

are able to continually improve without the guidance of a

ceacher to a point of total independence of a teacher*s

academic task presentations or reinforcement. On the other

hand, the less experienced the ensemble, the sooner the

teacher may need to intervene and assist the students in

improving their performance.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 111 -

It also nay be profitable to apply this research design

and methodology to elementary and secondary general music

classes. This setting involves more diverse teacher presen­

tations and student activities. Therefore, its applicability

and use may be more difficult and restricted in this set­

ting.

It may be feasible to modify this design to extend a

study over a longer period of time than three veeks without

the concomitant occurrence of student apathy and peak in

performance levels which was observed by the conductors in

this study. One method of extension might be to rehearse the

music involved at alternate rehearsals, rather than at every

rehearsal, thus realizing the same guantity of treatment

time over a longer period. In retrospect, had the two pieces

used for each treatment of this study each been rehearsed at

alternate rehearsals, thus only taking one hour of the re­

hearsals and rehearsing each composition once a week, the

problems of satiation and performance peaking might have

been avoided.

Another area which needs to be examined is the effect

of the teaching model and its components on cognitive deve­

lopment in music settings. The general music setting clearly

deals with the knowledge of music, and the public school

ensemble setting is intended as a learning situation for

knowledge about music as well as the necessary performance

skills. Cognitive learning was not specifically assessed in

this study.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 112 -

The social and academic content of the teaching units

could also be varied in order to examine the differential

effects these variations may have on attentiveness,

achievement, and attitude. It may be necessary to include

social content (social task presentations and reinforce­

ments) in the teaching unit to increase attentiveness. In

addition, it may also be necessary to include academic con­

tent (academic task presentations and reinforcements) to

improve achievement. It is possible that increased atten­

tiveness does not necessarily lead to improved achievement,

and improved achievement may not be a result of increased

attentiveness. When these variations are examined, their

effect on attitude may also be better understood. Another

factor which could be modified in all these settings is the

duration and freguency of teaching patterns used in this

study. The Direct Instruction Model uses rapid-paced in­

struction in non-music settings. In certain situations, this

may be examined by varying the duration of this cycle. A

short duration cycle may be difficult to apply in a rehear­

sal setting, but even varying of the length of these cycles

within a treatment, which was not examined in this study,

may prove useful in creating some variety in this setting.

As the body of research grows in this area, it may be

that the results will support the usefulness of this teach­

ing model. If this occurs, then the research could be moved

to the area of teacher training and preparation. The first

step would be to train teachers to develop these technigues

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 113 -

and skills. After a learning situation is designed vhich

accomplishes this in teacher preparation, these teachers

could be folloved into the field to see if they transfer

these skills to the classroom. Assuming this occurs, these

newly trained teachers could be compared to other teachers

who were not trained in these technigues. These comparisons

can be done on the basis of student attentiveness, achieve­

ment, and attitude, as well as teacher behaviors.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
SUMMARY

The purpose of this experiment was to investigate the

effect of conductor academic task presentation, direction,

reinforcement, and student performance on attentiveness,

achievement, and attitude of students in a university sym­

phonic band. The band was rehearsed under three treatments:

(A) directions and performance; (B) academic tasks, direc­

tions, and performance; and (C) academic tasks, directions,

performance, and contingent academic reinforcements. Two

conductors, three treatments and six musical compositions

were used and presented randomly to control for order ef­

fect. Conductors were rotated to control for experimenter

effect.

Student attentiveness was defined as the percentage of

students overtly off-task. Musical achievement was indepen­

dently rated by a panel of expert judges on the basis of

audiotape recordings. Student attitude was assessed by a

survey which was adapted for this study from Madsen and

Yarbrough (1980).

Results indicated that there was a treatment effect on

the percentage of students off-task. Two variables appeared

to affect off-task, performance time and treatment. Treat­

ment A resulted in the lowest percentage of student off-

task, and it included considerably more performance time

than either of the other treatments. Treatment C, the

treatment which included reinforcement, resulted in less

- 114 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 115 -

off-task than Treatment B, although their performance time

was comparable.

All the treatments resulted in gains in musical

achievement. Treatment B, which had less performance time

than Treatment A, and did not include reinforcement, resul­

ted in the lowest gain and posttest scores. Treatments A and

C both gained considerably more and scored higher on the

posttest than Treatment B. Treatment C, which included aca­

demic task presentations, directions, performance, and re­

inforcement, scored the largest gain and posttest scores.

There was a significant relationship for both conduct­

ors and compositions to all the item responses on the atti­

tude survey. However, the treatments had a significant re­

lationship to the way the students rated the rehearsal and

the conductor as a teacher. Students consistently rated the

rehearsals in which positive reinforcement occurred (Treat­

ment C) highest in terms of enjoyment and general affect.

The treatment which included the most performance (Treatment

A) was consistently rated higher than Treatment B, which was

comparable to Treatment C with the exclusion of reinforce­

ment, on the basis of enjoyment and general affect. Thus,

Treatment B was consistently rated lowest on these bases.

When the students rated the conductors as teachers, they

consistently rated the conductors in Treatment C highest.

However, Treatment B was rated consistently higher than

Treatment A, which only included directions and performance

and no academic task presentations or feedback, on the basis

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 116 -

of the quality of teaching.

The complete teaching model, which included academic

task presentations, directions, student performance, and

feedback in Treatment C, was effective when assessed on the

basis of attentiveness, achievement, and attitude. It was

clearly superior to the comparable treatment which did not

include feedback (Treatment B ) , on the basis of attentive­

ness, achievement, and attitude. Treatment A was comparable

to Treatment C on the basis of achievement and resulted in

lowest off-task. In the only instance where Treatment B was

rated superior to either of the other two treatments, it was

consistently rated superior to Treatment A on the basis of

the students* rating of good teaching.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDICES

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX A

LIST OF POSSIBLE MOSIC

Alleluia! Laudamus Te. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .fieed


Belwin Hills Publishing Corp.

American Variations. . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Alfred Publishing Co., Inc.

♦Colonial Song. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Grainger


Carl Fischer, Inc.

♦Divergents. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Southern Music Company

♦Folk Song Suite. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .


Boosey 6 Hawkes Ltd.

♦Giles Farnaby Suite^ . . . . . . . . . . . . .


Boosey 6 Hawkes, Inc.

♦Jubilance. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Giovannini/Robinson
Sam Fox Publishing Company, Inc.

♦Pageant. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .Persichetti
Carl Fischer, Inc.

Boyce Hall Suite. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .


Associated Music Publishers, Inc.

Scenes from ,sThe Louvre”. . . . . . . . . . . . .Dello Joio


Edward B. Marks Music Corp.

♦Third Suite. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Volwein Bros., Inc.

♦Toccata............. .. Frescobaldi/Slocum
Mills Music, Inc.

♦William Byrd Suite. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .


Boosey e Hawkes, Inc.

♦— the nine compositions which were sightread

- 118 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 119 -

APPENDIX B

ON THE LEFT SIDE, PLEASE NUMBER THE COMPOSITIONS FROM 1 TO 13, MOST
PREFERRED TO LEAST PREFERRED. PLEASE WAIT UNTIL YOU HAVE READ ALL THE
COMPOSITIONS.

ON THE RIGHT SIDE, PLEASE CHECK YES IF YOU HAVE PLAYED THE
COMPOSITION BEFORE, OF NO IF YOU HAVE NOT. THANK YOU FOR YOUR
COOPERATION.

ORDER YES NO

Pageant.............................. . ..... ....

Folk Song Suite....... ..... .....

Jubilance........ .......................... ..... .....

Hilliam Byrd Suite............................ _____ _____

Giles Farnaby Suite. .... ................ ..........

Royce Hall Suite.............................. __ _____

_____ Third Suite................................... ________ ___ _

Colonial Song................................. _____ _____

_____ Scenes from "The Louvre"......................________ __________

_____ Toccata....................................... _____ _____

Divergents.................................... _____ _____

American Variations........................... _____ _____

I.au Daumus Te................................._____ __________

Instrument___________________ # Birthdate_________ Sex_____

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 120 -

APPENDIX C

LIST OF MUSIC USED

Selection Treatment Music

A Third Suite (1st Mov. (no re­


peats) & 3rd Mov. Meas. 23)

2 B Pageant (3rd beat of Meas. 66)

3 C Divergents (No. 2 (Meas. 12)


& No. 4)

Wm. Byrd Suite (No. 1 (Meas. 25)


S No. 3)

5 B Wm. Byrd Suite (No. 4)

6 C Wm. Byrd Suite (No. 5 (no re­


peats) & No. 6)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 121 -

APPENDIX D
DESIG £ flODEL SUMMARY

Pre­ Post
>osition test Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 test

1) 0 X (A1) 0 X(A2)0 X (A1) 0 X(A2)0 X (A2) 0 0

2) 0 X(B1)0 X(B2)0 X ( B 1)0 X(B2)0 X (B2) 0 0

3) 0 X <C1)0 X (C2) 0 X (C1) 0 X(C2)0 X (C2) 0 0

4) 0 X(A2)0 X(A 1)0 X (A2) 0 X(A1)0 X(A1)0 0

5) 0 X(B2)0 X(B1)0 X (B2) 0 X(B1) 0 X(B1)0 0

6) 0 X(C2)0 X (C1) 0 X (C2) 0 X(C1)0 X(C1)0 0

X(*#): * - Treatment:
(A) directions and performance
(B) academic tasks, directions, and
performance
(C) academic tasks, directions, performance,
and reinforcement
# = Conductor
(1) Director of Bands
(2) Doctoral Candidate
0: Musical achievement rating and attitude inventory.
* During X the students and conductors were videotaped to assess
student attentiveness and the implementation of treatment.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 122 -

APPENDIX E

EQUIPMENT LIST

1 Sony A? 3600 Videocorder


2 Sony AVC 3250 Cameras
2 Sony VCL-1206 Television Zoom Lenses
1 Sony CVN 115 9" Transistor Video Monitor
1 Sony SEG-1 Special Effects Generator
1 Realistic 33-1057 Two-channel Stereo Microphone Mixer
1 Realistic 33-1056 Condenser Microphone
2 Wollensak 2851AV Cassettes Systems
3 AVID LT-730 Monaural Headphones

1 Revox A77 Pull Track Monophonic Recorder


1 Altec 195A Microphone Base
1 Altec 29B Condenser Microphone Head (cardioid directivity
pattern)
1 30* Sound Recording Boom
1 Sony TC K61 Stereo Cassette Deck
1 Koss PRC 4-A Stereo Headphone

1 3-Socket Light System With Rotary Switch

Assorted Cables, Plugs, and Adapters

5 Scotch 461 Video Tape (2400 ft.)


6 Ampex 406 Audio Tape (1200 ft.)
2 Ampex 406 Audio Tape (2400 ft.)
18 Scotch AVM 90 Studio Master Cassette Tape

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 123 -

APPENDIX F

PERFORMANCE RATING FORM

NO. Selection

Circle your rating: Poor Fair Good Excellent

Intonation............ 2 3 4

Blend................. 2 3 4

Balance............... 2 3 4

Tempo..,..»............ 2 3 4

Dynamics......... ..... 2 3 4
-9
Tone Quality... 2 4

Rhythms........... 2 3 4

Phrasing................. . 2 3 4

Ensemble.................. 2 3 4

Articulation.............. 2 3 4

Style........ . 2 3 4

Overall Artistic Effect... 2 3 4

(Musicality)

Total:

Judge*s Name

Rating

Rank
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 124 -

APPENDIX G

ATTITUDE SURVEY

Instrument_____________ Birthdate Sex F M

Circle the number that best expresses your agree­


ment or disagreement with each of the statements belon:

Selection 1-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

B. I enjoyed rc»hearsinq this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

C. I like this conductor.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I was

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out of Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

E. This conductor is a good teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Selection 2-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 125 -

B. I enioyed rehearsina this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

C. I like this conductor.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I was

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out of Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

E. This conductor is a crood teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Selection 3-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree aqree

B. I enioved rehearsina this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

C. I like this conductor.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I was

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out of Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 126 -

E. This conductor is a qood teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Selection 4-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

B. I enioyed rehearsinq •this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree aqree

C, I like this conductor.•

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I was

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out O f Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

E. This conductor is a qood teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Selection 5-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Stronqly
disagree agree

B. I enioyed rehearsinq this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 127 -

C. I like this conductor,

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 ____ 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I was

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out of Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

E. This conductor is a qood teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Stronqly
disagree agree

Selection 6-

A. I like this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Aqree Stronqly
disagree agree

B. I enioved rehearsinq this music.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

C. I like this conductor.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

D. During this rehearsal I vas

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Turned Out of Partici­ With it Turned
off it pating on

E. This conductor is a qood teacher.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Strongly Disagree Uncertain Agree Strongly
disagree agree

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 128 -

APPENDIX fl
SECONDS SPENT IN TEEATHENT BEHAVIORS

Directions Academic Performances Reinforcements


Tasks

Conductor: 1 2 1 2 1 2 1 2
------- -----------------
DAY 1)
Treatment A: 148 120 12 0 5 47 586 1 0
Treatment B: 66 69 251 281 354 349 27 0
Treatment c: 61 59 207 176 343 334 113 151
DAY 2)
Treatment A: 159 87 0 20 571 552 0 14
Treatment B: 51 68 327 309 339 343 12 0
Treatment c: 65 54 193 176 364 343 85 152

DAY 3)
Treatment A: 149 89 4 3 566 595 0 0
Treatment B: 64 73 314 244 350 358 0 16
Treatment c: 39 47 216 184 349 376 135 114

DAY *0
Treatment A: 132 101 7 2 581 613 0 0
Treatment B: 43 79 287 279 367 343 18 0
Treatment c: 63 52 177 194 300 346 129 118

DAY 5)
Treatment A: 138 109 19 0 549 589 6 0
Treatment B: 50 49 277 300 360 362 44 5
Treatment c: 86 62 152 188 336 330 156 143

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 129 -

APPENDIX I

FREQUENCY OF UNITS AND REINFORCEMENTS

Direction Academic Reinforcements


Units Units

Conductor: 1 2 1 2 1 2
DAY 1)
Treatment A: 12 12 0 0 3 2
Treatment B: 0 0 8 8 12 1
Treatment c: 1 0 8 9 27 31
DAY 2)
Treatment A: 16 10 0 1 3 2
Treatment B: 0 1 8 8 8 0
Treatment c: 1 2 8 6 25 28
DAY 3)
Treatment A: 8 8 2 0 0 0
Treatment B: 0 1 9 8 0 0
Treatment C: 0 0 8 8 0 3
DAY 4)
Treatment A: 6 9 2 1 0 1
Treatment B: 0 2 8 8 2 0
Treatment c: 0 0 8 8 22 29
DAY 4)
Treatment A: 8 10 2 0 1 1
Treatment B: 0 0 8 8 10 1
Treatment c; 2 0 7 8 25 30

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 130 -

APPENDIX J

PERCENTAGE OF STUDENT OFF-TASK

Performance Non- Total


Performance

Conductor: 1 2 1 2 1

DAY 1)
Treatment A: 3.4 2.3 22.0 19.4 8.20 5.26
Treatment B: 0.1 5.6 26.0 28.7 12.87 17.90
Treatment C: 2.2 0.0 22.0 13.5 13.59 8.06

DAY 2)
Treatment A: 2.1 2.8 14.0 32.2 10.08 H.55
Treatment B: 3.9 1.9 29.9 33.0 18.20 17.83
Treatment C: 7.0 2.9 36.6 30.0 18.97 16.74

DAY 3)
Treatment A: 3.9 2. 1 13.7 16.7 5. 49 6.06
Treatment B: 3.6 3.3 30.6 25.0 18.37 14.34
Treatment C: 5.6 0.8 27.1 18.9 17. 16 10 12.
DAY 4)
Treatment A: 5.2 3.6 35.6 22.0 10.92 7.38
Treatment B: 1.0 1.6 25.0 25.6 14.66 13.44
Treatment C: 2.5 0.0 26.3 16.3 14.23 9.36

DAY 5)
Treatment A: 1.0 2.0 19.8 11.2 6.32 4.20
Treatment B: 3.9 4.0 19.5 16.5 12.59 9.86
Treatment C: 0.8 0.0 23.8 18.2 13.41 9.34

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 131 -

APPENDIX K
MEAN MOSIC A C HIE VEM ENT SCOBES

Performance Treatment A Treatment B Treatment c

Pretest 12.50 13.32 14. 17

Session 1 15.47 18.73 16.57

Session 2 18.32 18.45 20. 17

Session 3 18.17 20.83 19.83

Session 4 21.97 21.27 22-83

Session 5 24.60 19.67 23.00

Posttest 21.58 18.17 23.70

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 132 -

APPENDIX L

ATTITUDE SURVEY: AVERAGE POR MUSIC PREFERENCE

Treatment A Treatment B Treatment c

Conductor: 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean

Session 1 7.66 6.17 6.97 7.10 6.45 6.77 7.61 6.39 7.00

'Session 2

Session 3 7.34 6.03 6.69 7.65 5.46 6.55 7.67 6.29 6.98

Session 4 6.70 7.54 7.12 6.66 7.09 6.88 7.06 7.27 7.16

Session 5 6.53 7.84 7. 19 6. 52 7.38 6.96 6.56 7.56 7.06

Mean 7.06 6.90 6.99 6.98 6.60 6.79 7.22 6.88 7.05

*— no attitude survey was administered in Session 2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 133 -

APPENDIX H

ATTITUDE SURVEY: AVERAGE FOR REHEARSAL ENJOYMENT

Treatment A Treatment B Treatment C

Conductor: 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean

Session 1 7.31 5.37 6.34 6.58 5.84 6.21 7.35 5.68 6.52

♦Session 2

Session 3 7. 03 5.53 6.27 6.95 4.89 5.92 6.95 6.00 6.47

Session 4 6.29 7.29 6.77 6.20 6.92 6.56 6.83 7.06 6.94

Session 5 6,36 7 S50 6.93 6.03 6.74 6.38 6.47 7.33 6.90

Mean 6.75 6.42 6.58 6.44 6.10 6.27 £.90 6.52 6.71

*— no attitude survey was administered in Session 2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 134

APPENDIX N

ATTITUDE SURVEY: AVERAGE FOR "TURNED ON”

Treatment A Treatment B Treatment C

Conductor: 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean 1 2 Mean

Session 1 7.52 5.80 6.66 6.77 5.97 6.37 7.61 6.36 6.98

♦Session 2

Session 3 6.96 5.84 6.40 7.00 5.14 6.07 7.33 6.29 6.81

Session 4 6.67 7.09 6.38 6.53 7.09 6.81 6.91 7.09 7.00

Session 5 6.68 7.38 7.03 6.56 6.78 6.67 7.02 7.14 7.08

Mean 6.96 6.53 6.74 6.72 6.24 6.48 7.22 6.72 6.97

♦— no attitude survey was administered in Session 2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 135 -

APPENDIX 0

ATTITUDE SURVEY: AVERAGE FOR CONDUCTOR PREFERENCE

Treatment A Treatment B Treatment c

Conductor: 1 2 He an 1 2 Hean 1 2 Hean

Session 1 8.72 6. 13 7.43 8.74 6.60 7.67 8.71 5.81 7. 26

♦Session 2

Session 3 8.55 6.32 7.43 8.40 6.08 7.24 8.66 6.60 7.36

Session 4 8.44 7.18 7.81 8.50 7.16 7.83 8.46 7.06 7.76

Session 5 8.25 7.03 7.64 8.48 7.06 7.77 8.56 6.88 7.72

He an 8.49 6.66 7.58 8.53 6.72 7.63 8.60 6.59 7.59

*— no attitude survey was administered in Session 2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 136 -

APPENDIX P

ATTITUDE SURVEY: AVERAGE FOR TEACHER RATING

Treatment A Treatment B Treatment C

Conductor: 1 2 Bean 1 1 2 Hean 1 2 Hean

Session 1 8-25 6.38 7.32 8.20 6.83 7.51 8.53 6.73 7.63

♦Session 2

Session 3 8.08 6.13 7.10 7.95 6.29 7.10 8.28 6.58 7.43

Session 4 7.84 6.59 7.22 7.93 7.23 7.58 8.20 7.09 7.64

Session 5 8.00 7.05 7.52 8.23 7.06 7.64 8.29 7. 16 7.72

Hean 8.04 6.54 7.29 8.08 6.84 7.46 8.32 6.89 7.60

♦— no attitude survey was administered in Session 2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
DEFERENCES

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
BEFEBEKCES

Altman, K. I. & Linton, T. E. Operant conditioning in the


classroom setting: A review of the research. Journal
of Educational Besearch, 1971, 64, 277-86.

Barrett, B. H. Seduction in rate of multiple ties by free


operant conditioning. L. P. Altman 8 L. Krasner
(Eds.), Case Studies in Behavior Modification. Mew
York: Holt, Binehart & Winston, 1965.

Becker, W. C. Symposium on direct instruction: The Follow


Through data show that some programs work better than
others. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Educational Besearch Association, Toronto, March, 1978.

Becker, W. C., Enqelmann, S. Analysis of achievement data


on six cohorts of low-income children from 20 school
districts in the Oniversity of Oregon direct instruc­
tion follow through model. Eugene, Oregon: Oniversity
of Oregon, 1976. (EBIC Document Beproduction Service
Mo. ED 145 922)

Becker, W- C., Engelmann, S., S Thomas, D. B. Teaching: A


course in applied psychology. Chicago: Science Re­
search Associates, Inc., 1971.

Becker, W. C., Madsen, C. H., Jr.- Arnold, C. B . , S Thomas,


D. R. The contingent use of teacher attention and
praise in reviewing classrooa behavior problems. Jour­
nal of Special Education. 1967, J, 287-307.

Begian, H. Emulate the professional. Instrumentalist.


1968, 23(4) , 35-37.

Beisman, G. B. Effect of rhythmic accompaniment upon


learning of fundamental motor skills. Besearch Quar­
terly. 1967, 38, 172-176.

Bellamy, T. 6 Sontag (Eds.). Use of group contingent music


to increase assembly line production rates of retarted
students in a simulated sheltered workshop. Journal of
Music Therapy. 1973, JO, 125-136.

Bennett, L., & Adams, J. A comparative study of the effects


of positive and negative reinforcement on the effici­
ency of musical learning. Council for Besearch in Mu­
sic Education. 1967, _10, 39-46.

- 138 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Bennett, N., Jordan, J., Long, G., £ Hade, B. Teaching
styles and pupil progress. Cambridge, Massachusetts:
Harvard Oniversity Press, 1976.

Berliner, D. C . , £ Gage, N. L. The psychology of teachinq


methods. In N. L. Gage (Ed.), The psychology of teach­
inq methods. Chicago, Illinois: The National Society
for the Study of Education, 1976.

Berliner, D. C. £ Bosenshine, B. The acquisition of know­


ledge in the classroom. Beginning Teacher Evaluation
Study. Technical report I V - 1. San Francisco, Cali­
fornia: Far West Laboratory for Educational Besearch
and Development, 1976. (EBIC Document Reproduction
Service No. 2D 146 158)

Bloomguist, K. G. Discipline. Instrumentalist. 1973, 27-


(8) , 78-79.

Bonny, H. B., Cistrunk, H . , Hakuch, B., Stevens, E., £


Sally, J. Some effects of music on verbal interaction
in groups. Journal of Music Therapy. 1965, 2, 61-62.

Bough, J. C . , £ Bough, J. R. The effects of four types of


music on the learning of nonsense syllables. Journal
of Music Therapy. 1965, 2, 69-74.

Brophy, J. E. Advantages in teacher effectiveness re­


search. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Association of Colleges for Teacher Education, Chicago,
March 1979a.

Brophy, J. E. Advances in teacher effectiveness research.


occasional paper no. 18. East Lansing, Michigan: In­
stitute for Besearch on Teaching, 1979b. (EBIC Docu­
ment Reproduction Service No. ED 173 340)

Brophy, J. E. £ Evertson, C. M. Process-product correla­


tions in the texas teacher effectiveness studv: Final
report. Austin, Texas: Besearch and Development Center
for Teacher Education, ?974. (EBIC Document Reproduc­
tion Service No. ED 091 394)

Bruner, Forevard. In N. Bennett, J. Jordan, G. Long, £ B.


Hade (Eds.), Teachinq styles and pupil progress.
Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press,
1976.

Carnine, D.W. Direct Instruction: A successful system for


educationally high-risk children. Journal of Curricu­
lum Studies. 1979, , 29-45.

- 139 -

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 140 -

Cook, M. & Freethy, M. The use of music as a positive re­


inforcer to eliminate complaining behavior. Journal of
Music Therapy. 1973, JO, 213-216.

Darch, B. & Thorpe, H.W. An intervention strategy for


teachers of the mildly handicapped (Note 1. Engelmann,
S. Personal communication, March, 1976.). Education
and Training of the Mentally Retarded. 1978, J3, 9-15,.

Davis, D. An interview with W. Frances McBeth. Instrumen­


talist. 1978, 32, 39-41.

Dorow, L.G. Conditioning music and approval as new rein­


forcers for imitative behavior with the severely re­
tarded. Journal of Music Therapy. 1975, J2, 30-39.

Dorow, L.G. Televised music lessons as educational rein­


forcement for correct mathematical responses with the
educable mentally retarded. Journal of Music Therapy.
1976, J3, 77-86.

Dorow, L.G. The effect of teacher approval/disapproval ra­


tios on student music selection and concert attentive­
ness. Journal of Research in Music Education. 1977,
25, 32-40.

Dorow, L.G. & Greer, R.D. The reinforcement value of a


music instrument for beginning instrumentalists and the
influence of discovery versus teacher approval on
achievement. Journal of Music Therapy. 1977, J4, 2-16.

Dunkin, M . S Biddle, B. The study of teaching. New York:


Holt, Rinehart, 8 Winston, 1974.

Engelmann, S. The effectiveness of direct verbal instruc­


tion on I.Q. performance and achievement in reading and
arithmetic. In B.C. Becker (Ed.), An Empirical Basis
for Change in Education. Chicago: Science Besearch
Associates, 1971, 461-483.

Erez, M. Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal set­


ting- performance relationship. Research report No.
10. Washington, D.C.: Office of Naval Research,
1976. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service So. ED 127
359)

Evertson, C. M., Anderson, L. M., Brophy, J. E. , 5 Anderson,


c» Texas junior high school study: Final report of
process-outcome relationships. Volume I, Research de­
velopment report no. 4061. Austin, Texas: Research and
Development Center for Teacher Education, 1978. (ERIC
Document Reproduction Service No. ED 173 744)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 141 -

Forsythe, J.8. The effect of teacher approval, disapproval,


and errors on student attentiveness: music versus
classroom teachers. In C.K. Madsen, R.D. Greer, & C.H.
Madsen (Eds.), Besearch in Music Behavior. New York:
Teachers College Press, 1975, 49-55.

Forsythe, J.R. Elementary student attending behavior as a


function of classroom activities. Journal of Besearch
in Music Education. 1977, 25, 228-239.

Good, T. L. Teacher effectiveness in the elementary


school. Journal of Teacher Education. 1979, 30, 52-64.

Gordon, M.B. (Ed.) Distar instructional system: Summaries


of case studies on the effectiveness of the Distar In­
structional System. Science Besearch Associates, Inc.,
1971.

Gordon, M.V.W. The use of music instruction as a contingen­


cy for increased reading behavior. Paper presented at
the National Symposium for Besearch in Music Behavior,
New York, October, 1980.

Green, E.A.H. The modern conductor. New Jersey: Pren-


tice-Hall, Inc., 1969.

Greer, B. D., Dorow, L.G., Hachhaus, G., 6 White, E.B. Adult


approval and student*s music selection behavior. Un­
published paper, Teachers College Columbia University,
1972.

Greer, B.D., Randall, A., S Timberlake, C. The discrimina­


tive use of music listening as a contingency for im­
provement in vocal pitch acuity and attending beha­
vior. Council for Besearch in Music Education. 1971,
26, 10-18.

Hall, R.V., Lund, D., S Jackson, D. Effects of teacher at­


tention on study behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis. 1968, J, 1-12.

Hanley, E.M. Review of research involving applied behavior


analysis in the classroom. Review of Educational Be­
search. 1970, 40, 597-625.

Hanser, S.B. Group-contingent music listening with emotio­


nally disturbed boys. Journal of Music Therapy. 1974,
H , 220-225.

Hauck, I.P. & Martin, P.L. Music ao a reinforcer in patient


controlled duration of time-out. Journal of Music The-
raj2Z, 1970, 7, 43-53.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 142 -

Heath, R. W. & Nielson, M. A. The research basis for per­


formance-based teacher education. Review of Educatio­
nal Research. 1974, 44, 463-489.

Jeffrey, H. E. New technique for monitoring and reinforcing


children. Science. 1955, 2JI, 371.

Jorgenson, H« The use of a contingent music activity to


modify behaviors which interfere with learning. Jour­
nal of Music Therapy. 1974, J_1, 41-46.

Kennedy, W.A. & Willicutt, H.C. Praise and blame as incen­


tives. Psychological Bulletin. 1964, 62, 323-332.

Kramer, S.A. The effects of music as a cue in maintaining


handwashing in preschool children. Journal of Mnsic
Therapy. 1978, 15, 136-144.

Kuhn, T.I. The effect of teacher approval and disapproval


on attentiveness, musical achievement, and attitude of
fifth grade students. In C.K. Madsen, R.D. Greer, &
C.H. Madsen, Jr. (Eds.), Besearch in Music Behavior.
New York: Teachers College Press, 1975, 40-48.

Lipe, D. & Jung, S. H. Manipulating incentives to enhance


school learning. Review of Educational Besearch. 1971,
<n, 249-280.

Madsen, C. H., Jr., Becker, W. C . , G Thomas, D. P. Rules,


praise and ignoring: Elements of elementary classroom
control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1968,
1, 139-150.

Madsen, C. H., Jr., Becker, W. C., Thomas, D. R . , Koser, L . ,


G Plager, E. An analysis of the reinforcing function
of "sit down" commands. In R. K. Parker (Ed.), Read­
ings in educational psychology. Boston: Allyn G
Bacon, 1968.

Madsen, C. K. The effect of music subject matter as rein­


forcement for correct mathematics. Council for Re­
search in Music Education. 1979, 59, 54-58.

Madsen, C. K. & Allay, J« M. The effect of reinforcement on


attentivenass: A comparison of behaviorally trained
therapists and other professionals with implications
for competency-based academic preparation. Journal of
Music Therapy. 1979, 1jS, 70-82.

Madsen, C. K . , Dorow, L. G., Moore, R. S., G Worable, J. V.


Effect of music via television as reinforcement for
correct mathematics. Journal of Research in Music Edu­
cation. 1975, 24, 51-59.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 143 -

Madsen, C. K., 6 Forsythe, J. 1. Effect of contingent music


listening on increases of mathematical responses.
Journal of Besearch in Music Education. 1973, 2J., 176-
181.

Madsen, C. K. 8 Geringer, J. M. Choice of televised music


lessons versus free play in relationship to academic
improvement. Journal of Music Therapy. 1976, .13, 154-
62.

Madsen, C- K.. 8 Madsen, c. H. Music as behavior modifica­


tion technique with a juvenile delinquent. Journal of
Music Therapy. 1968, 5, 72-76.

Madsen, c. K. 8 Madsen, c. H. Selection of music listeninq


or candy as a function of contingent versus noncontin­
gent reinforcement and scale singing. Journal of Music
Therapy. 1972, 9, 190-198.

Madsen, C. K. 8 Madsen, C. H., Jr., Teaching discipline; A


positive approach for educational development. Second
edition. Boston: Allyn 8 Bacon, 1974.

Madsen, C. K., Moore, R. S., Hagner, M. J., 8 Yarbrough, C.


A comparison of music as reinforcement for correct ma­
thematical responses versus music as reinforcement for
attentiveness. Journal of Music Therapy. 1975, 12.
84-95.

Madsen, C. K., Wolfe D. E., 8 Madsen, C. H. The effect of


reinforcement and directional scalar methodology on
intonational improvement. Council for Besearch in Mu­
sic Education. 1969, J8, 22-34.

Madsen, C. K. 8 Yarbrough, C. Competency-based approach to


music education. New Jersey; Prentice-Hall, Inc.,
1980.

McCarty, B. C . , McElfresh, C. T . , Bice, S. Y. 8 Wilson, S.


J. The effect of contingent backround music on inap­
propriate best behavior. Journal cf Music Therapy.
1978. Ji5, 150-156.

McDonald, F. J. Report on phase II of the Beginning Teacher


Evaluation Study. Journal of Teacher Education, 1976a,
12, 39-42.

McDonald, F. J. Beginning Teacher Evaluation Study; Phase


I I . 1973-74. summary report. Sacramento, California:
California State Commission for Teacher Preparation and
Licensing, 1976b. (EBIC Document Reproduction Service
No. ED 127 375)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 144 -

Medley, D. M. Teacher competence and teacher effectiveness.


A review of process-product research. Washington, D.
C.: American Association of Colleges for Teacher Edu­
cation, 1977. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED 143 629)

Killer, D. M . , Dorow, L. G . , 5 Greer, B. D. Journal of Mu­


sic Therapy. 1974, JJ, 57-64.

Moore, R. S. Effect of differential teaching techniques on


achievement, attitude and teaching skills. Journal of
Besearch in Music Education. 1976, 24, 129-141.

Moore, R. S. Comparative use of teaching time by American


and British elementary music specialists. Council for
Research in Music Education. 1981, 66-77. 62-68.

Morgan, B- J. & Lindsley, A. R. Operant preference for


stereophonic over monophonic music. Journal of Music
Therapy. 1966, 3, 135-143.

Mosteller, P. 6 Tukey, J. W. Data analysis and regression.


Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.,
1977.

Murray, K. The effect of teacher approval/disapproval on


the performance level, attentiveness, and attitude of
high school choruses. In C. K. Madsen, R. D. Greer, 8
C. H. Madsen (Eds.), Besearch in Music Behavior. New
York: Teachers College Press, 1975.

Neter, J. 6 Wasserman, W« Applied linear statistical mo­


dels: regression. analysis of variance, and experimen­
tal designs. Homeward, Illinois: Bichard D. Irwin,
Inc., 1974.

Nolen, P., Kunzelman, H . , & Haring, M. Behavior modifica­


tion in a junior high learning disabilities classroom.
Exceptional Children. 1967, 37, 163-168.

O ’Leary, K. D., 6 Becker, W. C. The effects of the inten­


sity of teacher’s reprimands on children’s behavior.
Journal of School Psychology. 1968-1969, 7, 8-11.

Podvin, M. G. The influence of music on the performance of


a work task. Journal of Music Therapy. 1967, 4, 52-56.

Powell, M. Besearch on Teaching. The Educational Forum,


1978, 43, 27-37.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 145 -

Reid, D. H. , Hill, B. K.s Ravers, R. J., & Montegar, C. A.


The use of contingent music in teaching social skills
to a nonverbal hyperactive boy. Journal of Husic The­
rapy. 1975, J2, 2 - IS. ~

Reynolds, G. S. A primer of operant conditioning (revised


edition). New York: Glennviev, Illinois: Scott,
Foresman 6 Company, 1975.

Rosenskine, B. Teaching behaviours and student achieve­


ment. London, England: National Foundation for Educa­
tional Research in England and Hales, 1971.

Rosenshine, B. Recent research on teaching behaviors and


student achievement. Journal of Teacher Education.
1976a, 27, 61-64.

Rosenshine, B. Classroom instruction. In N. L. Gage (Ed.),


The psychology of teaching methods. Chicago, Illinois:
The National Society for the Study of Education, 1976b.

Rosenshine, B. Primary grades instruction and student


achievement gain. 1977. (ERIC Document Reproduction
Service tlo. ED 142 308)

Rosenshine, B. Instructional principles in direct instruc­


tion. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Toronto, Harch, 1978.

Rosenshine, B. V. Content, time, and direct instruction.


In P. L. Peterson S H. J. Walberg, Research on teach­
ing. California: McCutchan Publishing Corp., 1979,
28-56.

Rosenshine, B. 6 Furst, N. The use of direct observation to


study teaching. In R. Travers (Ed.), Second handbook
of research on teaching. Chicago, Illinois: Rand
HcNally, 1973.

Rudolf, M. The grammar of conducting. Nev York: Schirmer


Books, 1950.

Sajvay, R. S Risley, T. Development and generalization of


writing skills in a retarded girl using manipulation of
task variables. Proceedings of the annual convention
of the American Psychological Association. 1970, 5(Pt.
2), 749-750.

Schultz, C. B. S Sherman, R. H. Social class development


and differences in reinforcer effectiveness. Review of
Educational Besearch. 1976, 46, 25-59.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 146 -

Siegel, S- Nonparametric statistics foe the behavioral sci­


ences. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1956.

Skinner, B. F. Science and hnman behavior. New York:


Macmillan, 1953.

Soar, 8. S. Follow Through classroom process measurement


and pupil growth (1970-71). Final report. Gainesville,
Florida: Institute for Development of Human Besources,
1973. (EHIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 106
297)

Spradling, R. L. The use of positive and negative rein­


forcement in a rehearsal situation. Unpublished paper,
Florida State University, 1969.

Spradling, R. L. The effect of timeout from performance on


attentiveness and attitude of university band students.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Florida State Uni­
versity, 1980.

Stallings, J. Relationships between classroom instructional


practices and child development. Paper presented at
the meeting of the American Educational Research Asso­
ciation, Washington, D. C . , March 1975.

Stallings, J. A. How instructional processes relate to


child outcomes in a national study of Follow Through.
Journal of Teacher Education. 1976, 27, 43-47.

Stallings, J. A. Teaching basic reading skills in secondary


schools. Paper presented at the meeting of the Ameri­
can Educational Research Association, Toronto, March
1978.

Stallings, J. A. S Kaskowitz, D. H. Follow Through class­


room observation evaluation 1972-1973. Menlo Park,
California: Stanford Research Institute, 1974. (ERIC
Document Reproduction Service No. ED 104 969)

Stallings, J. A. S Kaskowitz, D. H. A study of Follow


Through implementation. Paper presented at the meetinq
of the American Educational Research Association, Menlo
Park, April 1975.

Steele, A. L. Programmed use of music to alter uncoopera­


tive problem behavior. Journal of Music Therapy.
1968 (December), 5, 103-107.

Talkington, L. W. S Hall, S. M. A music application of


premack's hypothesis to low verbal retardates. Journal
of Music Therapy. 1970, 7, 85-99.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 147 -

Thomas, D. R., Becker, 8. C., 6- Armstrong, M. Production


and elimination of disruptive classroom behavior by
systematically varying teacher's behavior. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis. 1968, 1, 35-45.

Thurman, V. L. A frequency and time description of selected


rehearsal behaviors used by five choral conductors.
Paper presented at the Music Educators National Con­
ference National Convention, Atlantic City, March,
1976.

Tyler, V. S Brown, G. Token reinforcement of academic per­


formance with institutionalized delinquent boys. Jour­
nal of Educational Psychology* 1968, 59, 164-168.

Underhill, K. K- S Harris, L. M. The effect of contingent


music on establishing imitation in behaviorally dis­
turbed retarded children. Journal of Music Therapy.
1974, 11, 156-186.

Vachhaus, G. B. & Wachhaus, G. E. The effect of a music en­


semble conductor*s verbal behavior on student musicians
attitude. Paper presented at the Music Educators Na­
tional Conference National Convention, Atlantic City,
March, 1976.

Wagner, M. J. & Struhl, E. P. Comparisons of beginning


versus experienced elementary music educators in the
use of teaching time. Journal of Besearch in Music
Education. 1979, 27, 113-125.

Wilson, C. V. The use of rock music as a reward in behavior


therapy with children. Journal of Music Therapy. 1976,
13, 39-48.

Wolfe, D. E. Music motor behavior and the neuroloqicallv


impaired child. Paper presented at the National Sym­
posium for Besearch in Music Behavior, New York, Octo­
ber, 1980.

Wolpow, R. T. The independent effects of contingent social


and academic approval upon the musical on-task perfor­
mance behaviors of profoundly retarded adults. Journal
of Music Therapy . 1976, 13 29-38.

Yarbrough, C. Effect of magnitude of conductor behavior on


students in selected mixed choruses. Journal of Re­
search in Music Education. 1975, 23, 134-146.

Yarbrough, C., Charboneau, M. S Wapnick, J. Music as rein­


forcement for correct math and attending in ability
assigned math classes. Journal of Research in Music
Education. 1977, !_4, 77-88.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 148 -

Yarbrough, C. 6 Price, H. E. Prediction of performer at­


tentiveness based upon rehearsal activity and teacher
behavior. Journal of Research in Music Education, in
press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
- 149 -

BIOGHAPHICAL DATA

Name: Harry Edward Price

Date and Place of Birth: January 11, 1953


Havana, Cuba

Elementary School: Ojus Elementary School


Ojus, Florida
Graduated, 1964

Junior High School: John F. Kennedy Junior High School


North Miami Beach, Florida
Graduated, 1967

High School: Miami Norland Senior High School


Miami, Florida
Graduated, 1970

University: Florida State University


Tallahassee, Florida
B. M. E. 1974

Graduate Work: Florida State University


Tallahassee, Florida
Band Graduate Assistant
M. M- E, 1975

Syracuse University
Syracuse, New York
University Fellow
Ed. D. 1981

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like