You are on page 1of 19

| |

Received: 13 April 2019    Revised: 8 April 2020    Accepted: 9 April 2020

DOI: 10.1111/desc.12981

PAPER

The processing of rhythmic structures in music and prosody


by children with developmental dyslexia and developmental
language disorder

Martina Caccia1,2 | Maria Luisa Lorusso1

1
Unit of Child Psychopathology –
Neurodevelopmental Disorders of Language Abstract
and Learning, Scientific Institute IRCCS E. Rhythm perception seems to be crucial to language development. Many studies have
Medea, Bosisio Parini, Italy
2 shown that children with developmental dyslexia and developmental language disor-
School of Advanced Studies IUSS Pavia –
Center of Neurocognition, Epistemology and der have difficulties in processing rhythmic structures. In this study, we investigated
Theoretical Syntax (NETS), Pavia, Italy
the relationships between prosody and musical processing in Italian children with
Correspondence typical and atypical development. The tasks aimed to reproduce linguistic prosodic
Maria Luisa Lorusso, Scientific Institute
structures through musical sequences, offering a direct comparison between the two
“IRCCS E. Medea”, via Don Luigi Monza 20,
23842 Bosisio Parini, Italy. domains without violating the specificities of each one.
Email: marialuisa.lorusso@lanostrafamiglia.it
About 16 Typically Developing children, 16 children with a diagnosis of
Funding information Developmental Dyslexia, and 16 with a diagnosis of developmental language disor-
Ministero della Salute, Grant/Award
der (age 10–13 years) participated in the experimental study. Three tasks were ad-
Number: RC 2018-2019
ministered: an association task between a sentence and its humming version, a stress
discrimination task (between couples of sounds reproducing the intonation of Italian
trisyllabic words), and an association task between trisyllabic nonwords with differ-
ent stress position and three-notes musical sequences with different musical stress.
Children with developmental language disorder perform significantly lower than
Typically Developing children on the humming test. By contrast, children with de-
velopmental dyslexia are significantly slower than TD in associating nonwords with
musical sequences. Accuracy and speed in the experimental tests correlate with
metaphonological, language, and word reading scores. Theoretical and clinical impli-
cations are discussed within a multidimensional model of neurodevelopmental disor-
ders including prosodic and rhythmic skills at word and sentence level.

KEYWORDS

developmental dyslexia, developmental language disorders, lexical stress, music, Prosody,


rhythmic structures

1 |  I NTRO D U C TI O N two disorders specify that the child has to have adequate hear-
ing and no major handicapping condition that might interfere with
Developmental language disorder (DLD) and developmental dyslexia learning (Bishop & Snowling, 2004). DLD manifests itself as a dif-
(DD) are both developmental disorders that affect a sizeable pro- ficulty in acquiring language despite otherwise normal intellectual
portion (~7%–10%) of the school-aged population (Snowling, 2000; functioning, normal hearing, and an adequate learning environment
Tomblin, Hannon, & Snyder, 1997). The diagnostic criteria of the (Leonard, 1998). Previous definitions of language disorders focused

Developmental Science. 2020;00:e12981. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/desc |


© 2020 John Wiley & Sons Ltd     1 of 19
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12981
|
2 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

on the syntactic difficulties of children with DLD, which was often


characterized in terms of deficits in grammar and, in particular, in
Research Highlights
morpho-syntax (e.g., Fonteneau & Van der Lely, 2008; Jakubowicz,
• Children with developmental language disorder have
Nash, Rigaut, & Gerard., 1998). Yet, evidence accumulating over the
difficulties in precisely perceiving and recognizing the
past decades has shown that isolated syntactic deficits are the ex-
prosodic structure of heard sentences
ception rather than the rule and that different types of impairments
• Children with developmental dyslexia have difficulties in
may be present, spanning over different linguistic domains—at the
perceiving the prosody of nonwords (pseudowords) and
same time or in different stages of development, as described by
associating them with musical sequences
Leonard (1998). Moreover, the fluid nature of DLD, whose macro-
• Children with developmental language disorder or de-
scopic characteristics and general trajectories are rather stable
velopmental dyslexia require longer time to process
across development but fine-grained manifestations constantly
rhythmic features in verbal and musical stimuli than
change over time (e.g., Conti-Ramsden & Botting, 1999; Rescorla &
typically developing children
Turner, 2015) has become clear.
• Accuracy in prosodic and musical processing correlates
Dyslexia is often characterized in terms of difficulties at the
with metaphonological, language and word reading
phonological level, in particular in phoneme manipulation (Bradley
scores, confirming that rhythmic skills are involved in
& Bryant, 1983; Catts, Adlof, Hogan, & Weismer, 2005; Joanisse,
language and reading acquisition
Manis, Keating, & Seidenberg, 2000), and poor verbal short-term
memory as measured by tasks such as digit span and nonword rep-
etition (Snowling, 2000; Szenkovits & Ramus, 2005). Moreover,
many children diagnosed with DLD are also dyslexic, and vice versa
(Bishop & Snowling, 2004; Catts et al., 2005; McArthur, Hogben, who did not. Among the few studies which applied this distinction,
Edwards, Heath, & Mengler, 2000). The relationship between DD some (e.g., Brizzolara et al., 2006; Catts & Kahmi, 2005) did show
and DLD is a highly debated issue, and several models have been differences in profiles between the two conditions, even if common
proposed to account for the frequent co-occurrence of the two impairments are obviously present, suggesting that DD and DLD
disorders. Indeed, very different rates of co-occurrence have been represent two distinct—though partially overlapping—disorders.
documented (reported rates ranging from below 20% to over 80%), Indeed, although several hypotheses exist for the origin of
depending on the population studied (clinical vs. general popula- dyslexia, many researchers now converge on the idea that several
tion, see Catts et al., 2005), on diagnostic criteria for the two disor- causes and factors concur to the emergence of reading disorders
ders, on orthographic transparency of the language (see Caravolas (Pennington, 2006). Among these factors, deficits in auditory pro-
et al., 2012) and on the kinds of measures that were used (espe- cessing of incoming stimuli deserve special attention because their
cially whether comprehension measures were included or not: in influence is exerted at very early stages of language development
fact, it has been suggested that children with DLD present, over the (e.g., Lorusso, Cantiani, & Molteni, 2014; Ramus & Szenkovits, 2008;
school-years, a decrease in their written text decoding deficit along Tallal, 1980, 2004). Furthermore, several studies (e.g.,Corriveau,
with more permanent impairments in reading comprehension—see Goswami, & Thomson, 2010; Peterson & Pennington, 2012;
Botting, Simkin, & Conti-Ramsden, 2006; Snowling, 2000; Buil- Thomson, Leong, & Goswami, 2013; Tierney & Kraus, 2013)
Legaz, Aguilar-Mediavilla, & Rodríguez-Ferreiro, 2015). Some au- have stressed the importance of rhythm perception for language
thors have suggested that DD may be a less severe form of language development.
disorder (Snowling, 2000) or that children with language disorders It is well known that music and language share many similarities
share phonological deficits with children with DD but have additional (e.g., Patel, 2003a) and following a rhythmic structure is one of the
non-phonological language impairments (Bishop & Snowling, 2004). commonalities. Yet, there are also notable differences. As suggested
Ramus, Marshall, Rosen, and Van Der Lely (2013) went a step fur- by Patel (2003b), if we focus on three components of linguistic
ther highlighting that children with DLD do not always have a pho- rhythm (i.e., grouping, durational patterning of syllables, and config-
nological deficit, and do not always have a reading impairment, thus urational patterning of stressed vs. unstressed syllables), we see that
proposing a multiple-component model of language abilities in DLD all components have similar features in language and music, with the
and in dyslexia, based on three dimensions: phonological, non-pho- exception of the second component, since regular periodicity is a
nological, and grammatical/lexical abilities. characteristic of music, but not of language.
In spite of the attention devoted to the relationship between Thus, speech and music make use of structured patterns of pitch,
DLD and DD, the large majority of the studies focusing on either duration, and intensity. These elements contribute to create the su-
disorder of failure to distinguish between pure and comorbid forms. prasegmental part of the speech, known as prosody. Prosody is nec-
While it is difficult to foresee which of the children with DLD, usually essary to convey a number of different things, for example, lexical
assessed at pre-school age, will also develop DD, it is definitely possi- stress, focus, some aspects of meaning, and emotion (Ladd, 2008;
ble—and possibly enlightening—to distinguish between children with Peppé, McCann, Gibbon, O’Hare, & Rutherford, 2006, among
DD (diagnosed at school-age) who also have or had a DLD and those others).
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      3 of 19

The structure and expression of musical phrases is sometimes Italian prosody is usually described as syllable-timed (as opposed
referred to as “musical prosody” (see, e.g., Palmer & Hutchins, 2006) to stress-timed languages such as English and German and to mo-
and shows many parallels with linguistic prosody. Actually, a clear ra-timed languages such as Japanese), with no vowel reduction at
association between prosody and music perception, especially in the phonological level and limited vowel reduction at the phonetic
the domain of rhythm perception (time and stress), was found by level (Farnetani & Busà, 1999). Italian also has a free word order and
Hausen, Torppa, Salmela, Vainio, and Särkämö (2013) using an on- assigns prominence to the elements which are in focus (Bocci, 2008)
line music perception test as well as a prosodic verbal task that but lacks differentiation in duration and pitch between stressed and
measures the perception of word stress. According to the authors, unstressed syllables (Giordano & D'Anna, 2010), while large vari-
since rhythm processing is associated to bilateral brain activation, ability is observed across different individuals and different regions.
whereas pitch processing seems to be more right-lateralized, it could Most three- and four-syllable words are stressed on the penultimate
be hypothesized that larger overlap between language and music ex- syllable, which is considered as the dominant (or “regular”) stress.
ists for rhythm than for pitch perception, due to left lateralization A smaller proportion of polysyllabic words are stressed on the an-
of language. It should further be considered that rhythm itself, as a tepenultimate syllable. Stress Position has been found to modulate
distinct entity different from melody or pitch, seems to be composed the effect of the parameters determining lexical accent perception
of separable subskills supported by different neural structures and in TD children, but marginally so in children with DD (see Caccia
circuits (see Tierney & Kraus, 2015). In particular, beat tapping dis- et al., 2019).
sociates from rhythm sequencing/memorizing; moreover, drumming Many studies have shown that children with DD and DLD
to a steady pacing (isochronous) beat and remembering rhythmic are impaired in processing slow (<10 Hz) temporal modulations
patterns seem to be independent skills in school-age children, al- in speech causing poor perception of speech rhythm and syllable
though both correlate with drumming to the beat of a music excerpt stress (Barry, Harbodt, Cantiani, Sabisch, & Zobay, 2012; Leong,
(Bonacina, Krizman, White-Schwoch, Nicol, & Kraus, 2019). Finally, Hämäläinen, Soltész, & Goswami, 2011), compared to both read-
it has been shown that beat perception is at least partly independent ing-level and age-matched controls (Goswami et al., 2013). The lon-
of beat production (Fujii & Schlaug, 2013). gitudinal predictors of sensitivity to syllable stress at 13 years were
As mentioned above, both linguistic and musical abilities de- auditory sensory processing and sub-lexical phonological aware-
velop quite early in life and show similar developmental trajectories ness (rhyme awareness) measured at 9 years. Prosodic sensitivity
(Brandt, Gebrian, & Slevc, 2012). Children as young as 6–9 months at 9 years, on the other hand, was a significant longitudinal predic-
prefer the prosody of their native language (Höhle, Bijeljac-Babic, tor of reading ability at 13, possibly through its close relationship
Herold, Weissenborn, & Nazzi, 2009; Juszcyk, Friederici, Wessels, with sub-lexical phonological sensitivity as shown in concurrent
Svenkerud, & Jusczyk, 1993), and can use prosodic information to measurements.
help them segment words and phrases and to infer some of the syn- Children with DLD (SLI in the original paper) were found by
tactic properties of their mother tongue (Christophe, Nespor, Guasti, Corriveau, Kathleen, Pasquini, Elizabeth, and Goswami (2007) to
& Van Ooyen, 2003; Morgan & Demuth, 1996; Soderstrom, Seidl, have remarkable impairments in auditory processing of Amplitude
Kemler Nelson, & Jusczyk, 2003). Similarly, toddlers and pre-school- Rise Time (ART) and sound duration, but not of sound intensity nor
ers show a preference for native over non-native musical structures in prosodic tasks (including lexical stress tasks). Indeed, Rise time
(Heffner & Slevc, 2015). is crucial in speech signal, as it reflects the patterns of amplitude
Linguistic prosody seems to play a crucial role in enhancing the modulation that facilitate syllable segmentation. Therefore, a poor
perception of single sounds in children's phonological representa- perception of amplitude envelope structure may lead to difficulties
tions during speech processing (Chiat, 1983; Pierrehumbert, 2003). at the phonological level (Goswami, 2011). Richards and Goswami
Consequently, implicit representations of prosodic patterns (such (2015) used two measures of prosodic sensitivity, the DeeDee task
as English stress and Mandarin tone/pitch) may be important to (Whalley & Hansen, 2006) requiring to match real words to series
detect segmentation cues in speech sound. Prosodic awareness of stressed (DEE) or unstressed (dee) syllables reproducing their
would then be crucial to later reading acquisition, since children prosodic structure, and a Lexical Stress task. The authors found
might use familiar, language-specific prosodic patterns (based on, that children with DLD were impaired in both prosodic tasks and
e.g., intensity variations for English speakers vs. pitch variations showed an insensitivity to both ART and duration, although not to
for Mandarin speakers) as segmentation cues to sound out words simple sound frequency or intensity. In a further study using the
(Chung, Jarmulowicz, & Bidelman, 2017). Italian newborns have DeeDee task and a stress misperception task, Cumming, Wilson,
been reported to discriminate different stress patterns in di- and and Goswami (2015) showed that children with only DLD had re-
trisyllabic pseudo-words (e.g.,/’takala/ vs. /ta'kala/), and in lists duced sensitivity to ART in speech stimuli, whereas children with
of pseudo-words with consonantal variation (/’daga ‘nata/ vs. / both language and reading impairment also showed reduced sen-
da'ga na'ta/) (Sansavini, Bertoncini, & Giovanelli, 1997). Similarly, sitivity to sound duration. Additionally, individual differences in
2-month-old English infants can discriminate the stress patterns of processing syllable stress were associated with auditory process-
disyllabic pseudo-words (/’bada'gada/ vs. /ba'da ga'da/) (Jusczyk & ing. In light of the above, Cumming, Wilson, and Goswami (2015)
Thompson, 1978). formulated the “prosodic phrasing” hypothesis, which suggests
|
4 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

TA B L E 1   Demographic characteristics
  DD (n = 16) DLD (n = 16) TD (n = 16)
and developmental milestones (as
Male: female, n:n 11:5 11:5 11:5 emerging from the Parents Questionnaire)
Age, mean (SD), in 12.4 (1.23) 12.4 (0.92) 12.4 (0.97) of the sample
years
Handedness (R:L) 15:1 [0] 13:2 [1] 14:1 [1]
[missing]
First words, mean 9.93 (3.81)*DLD 18.93 (12.86)*TD,DD 9.33 (3.46)*DLD
age in months (SD) [2] [2] [1]
[missing]
First sentences, mean 18.23 (9.3) 26.14 (9.77)* TD 12.85 (3.6)* DLD
age in months (SD) [3] [8] [3]
[missing]

Note: Significant differences at Tukey's post hoc tests are indicated by asterisks.
Abbreviations: DD, developmental dyslexia; DLD, developmental language disorder; TD, typical
development.

that grammatical difficulties in DLD may reflect perceptual diffi- thus be more easily compared without losing their specific charac-
culties with global prosodic structure related to auditory impair- teristics, providing new insights in the multi-faceted and still largely
ments in processing amplitude rise time and duration. The absence unknown relationships between musical, general language, and
of a group with DD only in this study, however, does not allow reading skills.
to clearly disentangle the relationship of the two types of impair- To this aim, three experimental tasks were created: a task
ments (language and reading) with prosodic sensitivity. requiring to associate sentences with an ambiguous syntactic
Several studies have tried to shed further light on the link structure to their humming version (Humming Task), an auditory
between rhythmic processing and language through neurophys- discrimination task (Sound Stress Discrimination Task), and a task
iological and psychophysiological investigations, which have re- requiring association between a nonword and its musical version
vealed a surprising correspondence between modulation energy (NonWord-Music Association Task). The Humming task is similar
along multiple timescales within the speech envelope and mod- to a DeeDee task but with less phonological content; indeed this
ulations in cortical activity during speech processing (Giraud technique allows to isolate F0 contour from the other linguistic
& Poeppel, 2012; Myers, Lense, & Gordon, 2019, among oth- components. In the auditory discrimination task, the Italian lexical
ers). This phenomenon, known as neural entrainment, has been prosody is musically reproduced, testing rhythmic discrimination
described also for music (Doelling & Poeppel, 2015; Tierney & independent of linguistic content. Finally, the association task
Kraus, 2014) and seems to build the neurological basis for the re- allows to test rhythmic skills in a supra-modal and cross-modal
lationship between music and language. Other studies, focusing task, where the link has to be found between musical and linguis-
on the differences found between musicians and nonmusicians, tic elements sharing the same rhythmic pattern. The tasks were
and on the presence and characteristics of dyslexic individuals in administered to a group of children diagnosed with developmen-
both populations, have further highlighted that many of the ad- tal dyslexia (DD) and to another group of children diagnosed with
vantages shown by musicians are found with nonverbal (rhyth- developmental language disorders (DLD). The children's perfor-
mic or musical) as well as with verbal stimuli (Magne, Schön, & mances were compared both with those of typically developing
Besson, 2006; Weiss, Granot, & Ahissar, 2014; Zuk et al., 2017). (TD) children of the same age and between the two clinical groups.
Several studies have demonstrated associations between musical The hypothesis was that children with DD and DLD would be im-
rhythm aptitude, speech perception, and literacy skills in children paired in processing musical sequences reproducing linguistic
(Myers et al., 2019). To our knowledge, however, most studies on prosody, compared to TD; moreover, that the two clinical groups
the relationships between language and music in children with DD would differ in the degree and typology of impairment in pro-
and DLD have focused on pure musical stimuli and, separately, on cessing different rhythmical structures. Since sentence prosody
linguistic stimuli (related to phonology, the lexicon etc.) looking for is characterized by modulations in frequency, duration, intensity,
correlations between the two; yet, only few did actually try to rec- and ART, and DLD children appear to be impaired in sound dura-
reate prosody of linguistic stimuli in the domain of music, showing tion and ART processing (but, as mentioned above, not intensity
important analogies but also differences in the neural and behav- and lexical stress), whereas children with DD seem to be espe-
ioral characteristics of processing in the two domains (Cumming, cially impaired at ART and rhythmic processing at sublexical level,
Wilson, Leong, Colling, & Goswami, 2015; Falk, Lanzilotti, & it could be hypothesized that lexical prosody would be problem-
Schön, 2017). atic for both groups, but it is not clear whether higher level pros-
The present work is an attempt to recreate the prosody of Italian ody (extending from word to sentence level) would pose identical
speech in music: the rhythmic structures of music and language can difficulties to both.
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      5 of 19

2 | M E TH O D so were children who had been previously treated with music-based
intervention programs. All children were familiar with at least one
2.1 | Participants musical instrument as part of the standard school curriculum.
All the children with DLD and DD had been evaluated by expert
Forty-eight children took part in the experiment. Sixteen participants clinicians who had excluded any sensory, emotional, or major psychi-
were typically developing (TD), 16 had a diagnosis of developmental atric disorders, as well as a history of irregular school attendance, as
dyslexia (DD), and 16 participants had a diagnosis of developmental part of the diagnostic process. Diagnosis of DLD had generally oc-
language disorder (DLD). Children belonging to the three groups were curred when the children were aged 4–6 years, whereas diagnosis of
extracted from a larger sample of 59 children so as to be matched DD had been formulated after age 7, according to standard national
one-to-one on chronological age (from 10 to 13 years) and gender (see diagnostic procedures (Lorusso, Vernice, et al., 2014). All the parents
Table 1). TD children were recruited from local primary and second- further filled a questionnaire addressing clinical history (age of first
ary schools, while children with DD and DLD were selected among words and first sentences, past, and present literacy difficulties, SLT
those diagnosed at the Institute according to standard ICD-10 criteria history, mother tongue, other languages spoken) (see Table 1).
(World Health Organization, 1992). Eight of the children diagnosed Diagnosis of DLD was based on the results of full language as-
with DLD had additional diagnoses of reading impairments: following sessment with standardized tests normed on the Italian popula-
the hypothesis that they may represent a different group, a careful tion including articulation, receptive and productive vocabulary,
analysis of all the diagnostic language test scores was performed. In receptive and productive phonology, receptive and productive
fact, the language scores obtained by the DLD children with addi- morphosyntax. Only children whose diagnosis indicated significant
tional reading disorders did not significantly differ from those of chil- weaknesses in phonological and/or lexical and/or morphosyntactic
dren diagnosed with DLD only (see Table 2), so it was decided to treat abilities were finally included, whereas pure sound speech disorders
them as a single group1 . (with isolated phonological weaknesses) were not.
All participants were monolingual, native Italian speakers and Children with DD were included if they had a score at least 2 SD
they were regularly attending school; children with additional diagno- below the mean in at least two reading tests (speed and/or accuracy
sis of Attention Deficit Disorders (ADD-ADHD) were excluded, and parameters) and IQ scores ≥85. Children with DD with previous and/or

TA B L E 2   Means and standard deviations of the scores obtained on reading and language tests by the three groups, and results of
pairwise comparisons (Tukey post hoc tests)

Mean Mean TD versus DD TD versus DLD DD versus DLD


Test Mean (TD) SD (TD) (DD) SD (DD) (DLD) SD (DLD) p p p

Raven CPM z 1.005 0.662 0.6456 0.881 0.5425 0.7383 0.387 0.213 0.923
MT Errors z / / −1.8675 1.2572 −0.8875 1.1365 / / 0.06
MT Speed z / / −1.7131 0.805 −0.9144 1.0993 / / 0.026
DDE Word Speed z 0.4014 0.7255 −2.7469 1.6954 −1.2744 2.0203 <0.001 0.018 0.034
DDE Word Error z 0.0343 0.9215 −2.8962 3.2772 −0.6869 1.6106 0.002 0.65 0.019
DDE NonWord 0.4500 0.7033 −2.8119 1.5498 −1.1338 1.9753 <0.001 0.019 0.009
Speed z
DDE NonWord 0.1371 0.5929 −2.1450 2.0219 0.0881 1.2153 <0.001 0.995 <0.001
Errors z
Spoonerims r 27.062 2.743 23.062 4.1387 22.000 7.4654 0.085 0.022 0.833
Rhyme Recognition r 14.50 1.264 14.250 1.1254 14.250 1.0645 0.814 0.814 1.000
Sentence Repetition 0.4687 0.5516 0.0669 1.1027 −1.2825 1.8563 0.653 0.001 0.013
z
Direct-Indirect 0.3263 1.1641 −0.3931 1.0511 −0.6458 0.87129 0.133 0.029 0.779
Speech z
Active–Passive 0.0813 1.0834 −0.4150 0.70383 −1.2519 0.74119 0.243 <0.001 <0.001
Voice z
Clitics z 0.1969 1.0137 −0.7181 1.0899 −1.4756 1.2536 0.066 <0.001 0.148
Short-Term Memory / / −0.77 0.65 −1.33 0.83 / / 0.108
z
Working Memory z / / −0.17 0.67 −0.3 0.58 / / 0.640

Scores converted into Z-Scores according to age-appropriate Italian norms are indicated with “z”, raw scores are indicated by “r”
Significant p-values in bold
|
6 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

F I G U R E 1   Linguistic (a) and reading (b) profiles of the three groups, expressed in Z-scores with respect to Italian age norms

concurrent language disorders as resulting from their clinical records (three “correct” and four “incorrect” judgments) in the clitics sub-
were excluded. Children with DLD had a score at least 1.5 SD below test. Thus, scores for each subtest ranged between 0 and 7.5.
the mean in at least one language test and IQ scores were above 85. 3. A test of sentence repetition (Ferrari, De Renzi, Faglioni, &
TD children, before being included in the control group, were Barbieri, 1981) requiring the participants to repeat a list of 14
administered a battery of tests to evaluate their general intellectual sentences of increasing length and complexity. One point was as-
and linguistic abilities. Children with scores 1.5 SD or more below signed for each sentence repeated correctly after the first read-
the mean in one or more tests were excluded. All parents signed ing, 0.5 points after the second reading.
informed consent. The study had been approved by the Ethics 4. Two tests of metaphonological abilities: spoonerism and rhyme
Committee of the University of Pavia according to standards of the recognition. In the spoonerism task (CMF, Marotta, Trasciani, &
Helsinki Declaration (1964). Vicari, 2008), children are asked to swap the initial phonemes of
two one-syllable auditorily presented nonwords (for example:
“des” and “mag” should become “mes” and “dag”). Fifteen pairs of
2.2 | Standardized language and cognitive tests word were presented and 1 point was assigned for each correct
word (for up to 30 points). In the rhyme recognition test (VAU-
The following tests, standardized on the Italian school-age popula- MeLF, Bertelli & Bilancia, 2006), a target word is orally presented
tion, were administered: followed by three other words (a semantically related word, a
phonologically related word, and a rhyming word) and then chil-
1. Coloured Progressive Matrices—CPM (Belacchi, Scalisi, Cannoni, dren are asked to decide which of the three words rhymes with
& Cornoldi, 2008; Raven, 1947) the target word. Fifteen items were presented and 1 point was
2. A test of morphosyntactic comprehension and production assigned for each correct answer (for up to 15 points).
(CoSiMo, described in Cantiani, Lorusso, Perego, Molteni, & 5. A test of word and nonword reading (DDE-2, Sartori, Job, &
Guasti, 2015). The test is still unpublished but it has been stand- Tressoldi, 2007). This test assesses speed and accuracy (expressed
ardized in a large, well-controlled normative sample from various in number of errors) in reading word lists (4 lists of 24 words) and
regions of Italy. Three subtests were administered: a direct to nonword lists (3 lists of 16 nonwords) and provides grade norms
indirect speech transformation task (“speech”), an active to pas- from the second to the last grade of junior high school.
sive voice transformation task (“voice”), and a task on free mor-
phology where the use of clitics has to be judged and corrected
when necessary (“clitics”). The battery relies on an implicit use of The scores of the following tests were additionally re-
morphosyntactic transformations and avoids any reference to trieved from the children’s clinical records:
explicit rules, by simply providing examples of transformations
as instructions. Each subtest included seven items and 1 point
was assigned for each correctly produced transformation, while 1. MT-2 text reading test (Cornoldi, Colpo, & M. T. Group,
0.5 points were additionally assigned for each correct judgment 2011); “Test of speed and accuracy in reading, developed
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      7 of 19

by the MT group”, a text-reading task meant to assess Type 2) Six sentences with a “total ambiguity” (TA). E.g.,:
reading abilities for meaningful material. It provides sep-
arate scores for speed and accuracy. Texts increase in (2) (a) [La giovane] [ fotografa la pianta]
complexity with grade level. Age norms are provided for     [“The young lady”] [“photographs the plant”]
each text.   (b) [La giovane fotografa] [la pianta]
2. TEMA Test (Italian version of the TOMAL Test Of Memory And
    [“The young photographer”] [“plants it”]
Learning, Reynolds & Bigler, 1994) to evaluate short-term mem-
ory and working memory; backward and forward digit span (the In this kind of Italian sentences, each phrase has a double syntac-
scores were available for 10 children in the DD group and 10 in tic function which is assigned by prosody. In example 2 “la giovane”
the DLD group). Forward digit span was considered as a measure (“the young”), in Italian, could be both a noun phrase (equivalent to
of Verbal short-term memory, Backword digit span as a measure “the young lady”) or an adjective phrase; “fotografa” could be both a
of working memory. Raw scores and age-appropriate z-scores noun phrase (“photographer”) or a verbal phrase (“photographs”), and
were recorded. “la pianta” could be a determiner + noun (“the plant”) or a clitic + verb
(equivalent to “plants it”).
The reading and language profiles of the two groups are rep- Type 3) Six sentences in which ambiguity results from the
resented in Figure 1. As confirmed by a one-way ANOVA and Prepositional Phrase (PP) attachment (in the example: “with the
post hoc tests (see Table 2), the children with DD were less im- hat”), which can be either “high” (referring to “greets”) or “low” (re-
paired than children with DLD on all language tests, but more ferring to “the girl”). E.g.,:
impaired on reading tests. Children with DLD, on the other hand,
were clearly impaired with respect to TD children on both lan- (3) (a) [Gianni saluta] [la ragazza con il cappello]
guage and reading tests, with the exception of word and espe-     [“John greets”] [“the girl with the hat”]
cially nonword reading accuracy, in which they performed similar   (b) [[Gianni saluta la ragazza] con il cappello]
to TD children.
    [[“John greets the girl”] “with the hat”]

3 | M ATE R I A L S For each sentence, the two possible disambiguated versions
with different prosodic patterns spoken by a female trained speaker
3.1 | Humming Task (HT) were recorded with Audacity software (http://www.audac​
ityte​
am.org/) and then background noise was filtered out through the
Sentences with an ambiguous structure were proposed in the orig- software.
inal version and in a humming modality (See Appendix A, Figures To create the humming effect the F0 of each sentence was ex-
A1–A2). tracted and analyzed through PRAAT software (Boersma, 2006).
Eighteen sentences with an ambiguous syntactic structure were Then a female trained speaker (the same who had recorded
created. The lexicon of the sentences was controlled for frequency the target sentences) was asked to reproduce the prosody of
and semantic complexity, using a corpus of primary school-related the sentences without words, in such a way that each syllable
Italian language (Marconi, Ott, Pesenti, Ratti, & Tavella, 1993). The corresponds to a low continuous vibrating sound like that of a
sentences belonged to three different types (for an in-depth dis- prolonged/m/.
cussion of the linguistic characteristics of the sentence types and The children had to listen to 18 ambiguous syntactic sentences,
how they are processed by children with DD and DLD, see Caccia & disambiguated through prosody, while a sound symbol and two mu-
Lorusso, 2019): sical notes appeared on the PC screen, corresponding to each of the
Type 1) six sentences with a temporary syntactic closure ambi- two humming sentences. After listening to the sentence, the two
guity (TSCA). E.g.,: humming sentences were played one after the other, with a pause of
1s in between to avoid overloading of auditory memory. Participants
(1) (a) [Quando Marta guida] [la macchina fuma] were asked to indicate which of the two humming sentences they
    [“When Mary drives”] [“the car smokes”] judged to be the target sentence by pressing “1” or “2” on the key-

  (b) [Quando Marta guida la macchina] [fuma] board, with the index finger of the dominant hand (See Appendix A,
Figure A3).
    [“When Mary drives the car”] [“(she) smokes”]
In each sequence, only one of the two possible prosodic versions
of each sentence was presented to avoid exclusion-based strategies.
In these sentences the F0 guides syntactic closure during pars- Nonetheless, it was ensured that the two alternative structures for
ing; in the above example, the verbal phrase “drives” could refer to each type were equally represented in each of the sequences. Half
the immediately preceding noun phrase (“car”) or make a high at- of the children received the first sequence; the other half received
tachment (“Mary”). the second sequence, balancing across and within groups.
|
8 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

3.2 | Sound Stress Discrimination Task (SSDT) ([ʹtatata], [taʹtata], [tataʹta]) were recorded by a female Italian trained
speaker. Each nonword corresponded to one of the three musical
Musical stimuli, made up of three notes, were created to reproduce sequences used for the SSDT, made up of three notes reproducing
the prosody of Italian trisyllabic words with different stress posi- the prosody of Italian trisyllabic words with different stress posi-
tions (on first, second, or third syllable). A professional musician tions (on first, second, or third syllable, therefore named A, B, and
analyzed the nonword spectrum sound with Audacity software C). Such stimuli were presented in combinations of one nonword and
(http://www.audac​ityte​am.org/), extracting the notes of the speech two possible musical sequences, one of which corresponded to the
through Logic software (https://www.apple.com/it/logic​
-pro/). nonword stress pattern, while the other did not. The pairs of musi-
Then the new musical sounds were played using the repetition of cal sequences could be: first position stress versus second position
the D3 note. A neutral (no accent) MIDI file was created with the stress (AB/BA); first versus third position stress (AC/CA) and second
MuseScore software (https://muses​core.org/it) and then it was im- position versus third position stress (BC/CB). The nonwords were
ported in GarageBand software (https://www.apple.com/it/mac/ recorded by a female Italian trained speaker.
garag​eband​/ ) to manipulate intensity and duration (pilot studies on The experimental procedure for this task was the following: each
adults and children had shown that simultaneous manipulation of of the 36 nonwords was played to the children, while the image of
both duration and intensity facilitated stress position perception). a sound symbol and two musical notes appeared on the PC screen.
The stressed note had an increase of 20% in intensity and of 50% in Then, two musical sequences were played with a pause of 1s in be-
duration, compared to non-stressed notes. Each note had its peak in tween. Two numbered musical notes appeared on the screen and
correspondence with the vowel in the syllable and there were seam- after listening to the nonword and to the sounds, participants were
less transitions between consecutive notes. Eventually, the three asked to choose the musical sequence that they judged to corre-
MIDI files (with the stress on the first, second, and third note) were spond to the nonword by pressing “1” or “2”, with the index finger
played with a sampled Steinway Grand Piano sound. A consecutive of the dominant hand, on the keyboard (See Appendix B, Figure B5).
letter was attributed to each of the possible stressed elements: A All the tasks had been previously administered to a group of 27
(first sound), B (second sound), and C (third sound). Each sequence adult participants for a pilot study: the stimuli were progressively
of three sounds was thus named after the letter corresponding to refined until accuracy rates reached at least 80% (so as not to be too
the sound that was stressed (A, B, or C). Then, the sequences were difficult for the children).
grouped into pairs, half of which were a repetition of the same se-
quence, while the other half consisted of two different sequences.
So, pairs of sounds could be constituted respectively by a musical 4 | PRO C E D U R E S
sequence with accent on the first note versus a musical sequence
with second position accent (AB/BA), second position versus third All the experiments were delivered through a Dell Inspiron 7,347
position accent (BC/CB), first position versus third position accent ×64 laptop, 13.3” screen, the algorithms were specially designed
(AC/CA), first position versus first position accent (AA), second ver- using Psychopy software (Peirce, 2007). All participants were indi-
sus second (BB), and third versus third position accent (CC). vidually tested in a quiet room, seated next to the experimenter, in
The children listened to 18 pairs of musical-prosody sequences. front of the PC screen. They listened to the stimuli through XB550AP
Two musical notes appeared on the PC screen and children had to Sony Extra Bass headphones. No feedback was given regarding re-
listen to two sounds, with 1s pause in between. Two stickers with sponse accuracy. Response and responding time were automatically
“=” and “≠” were stuck on the keys “A” and “L” of the PC keyboard. recorded by the program. No time limits were given for responding
Participants had to press “=” on the keyboard if they thought that but the recorded files could not play again. Children started with a
the two sounds were identical and “≠” if they thought that they were warm-up session. The three tasks were presented in fixed order: HT,
different musical sounds (See Appendix B, Figure B1). SSDT, NW-MAT.

3.3 | NonWord-Music Association Task (NW-MAT) 4.1 | Data analysis

Trisyllabic nonwords with different stress positions were created. The following systematic procedure was followed for data analysis:
Each nonword consisted of three subsequent repetitions of the (a) When no a priori hypotheses had been formulated concern-
syllable/ta/. It was chosen to repeat the same syllable in order to ing the results of a certain analysis and different, unrelated vari-
control for phonological variability. The syllable/ta/ was chosen for ables were included in multiple analyses, Bonferroni correction
two reasons: first of all, lexical stress, in Italian, can change vowel ar- was applied; (b) whenever clear a priori hypotheses had been for-
ticulation except for the vowel/a/ (Bertinetto, 1980), secondly, the/ mulated pointing to specific results to be expected, or (c) when-
ta/ syllable produced less co-articulation effects than using other ever the variables involved in a group of analyses were correlated
syllables (See Appendix B, Figures B2–B4). The three nonwords (r ≥ 0.300) no corrections were applied in order to avoid inflating
formed by the sequence “TATATA” with different stress-position the probability of type II errors.
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      9 of 19

In correlation analyses, due to the relatively small sample size,


first of all correlations in the whole sample were computed, based
on a priori hypotheses. Compound scores were computed from sin-
gle scores to express reading (average of word and nonword read-
ing, for both speed and accuracy z-scores) and language (average
of z-scores from repetition and grammatical tests) skills, so as to
reduce the number of variables and increase power and reliability.
Component scores have been analyzed as post hoc tests, only when
significant correlations had emerged for the compound scores. Only
correlations with r or ρ ≥ 0.350 were considered (based on conven-
tional classification of correlation strength in medical research, see
Akoglu, 2018, and on power analysis showing that ρ = 0.35 would
yield a power of 0.8 in a sample of 46 participants) among the sig-
nificant ones. Relevant correlations were then re-analyzed excluding
the TD group, in order to ensure that they were not simply spuri-
ous products of large differences between the clinical and control
group in reading and language performance due to inclusion criteria.
A final check of these correlations in the two groups separately was
foreseen, to better characterize the functional differences between F I G U R E 2   Mean % correct responses for the three groups
the populations. In this case, only correlations above 0.45 were in the three experimental tasks (HT, humming task; SSDT, sound
considered, based on power analysis showing a power of 0.8 for 16 stress discrimination task; NW-MAT, Nonword-Music Association
participants. Task). Significantly different pairs are marked by same-color
asterisks
A power analysis (G-Power) for pairwise nonparametric tests
based on means and SDs obtained in the pilot tests on the three
tasks (taking into consideration both accuracy and speed) showed
that a size of 16 participants per group allowed to obtain a power of
0.8 with alpha at 0.05 (indeed, 14 participants per group appeared
to be sufficient to highlight differences with the effect sizes ob-
served in the pilot study, which were very similar to the ones re-
ported in the manuscript).

5 | R E S U LT S

First of all, nonparametric correlations between performance scores


on the three tasks were computed, highlighting significant cor-
relations both within correct responses (Spearman ρ(48) between
0.322 and 0.365) and within Reaction Times (ρ(48) between 0.341
and 0.551) but no significant correlations across variable types (ac-
curacies and RTs). For this reason, corrections for multiple testing
were applied considering the two sets of variables (accuracy and
RTs) as unrelated. Alpha was thus set at 0.025 following Bonferroni
F I G U R E 3   Mean response times (sec) for the three groups in the
correction.
three experimental tasks (HT, humming task; SSDT, sound stress
Mean percent accuracy in the three tasks and mean RTs are re- discrimination task; NW-MAT, Nonword-Music Association Task).
ported in Figures 2 and 3, respectively. Significantly different pairs are marked by same-color asterisks
A preliminary comparison between the two subgroups within
the DLD group (with and without additional reading difficulties)
was performed on all experimental scores, that is, accuracy and RT 5.1 | Humming Task (HT)
values for the three experimental tasks. None of the comparisons
(nonparametric tests, Mann–Whitney) revealed any significant dif- Due to non-normal distributions of response categories, as expected,
ference (all ps > .4), confirming that the DLD group could be consid- data were analyzed with nonparametric tests (Kruskal–Wallis), using
ered sufficiently homogeneous with respect to the variables under SPSS software. Comparing correct response distributions (Target
investigation. choices were clearly above chance—50%—level for all groups) in
|
10 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

the three groups (TD, DD, DLD), the Kruskal–Wallis test revealed significant difference between the three groups. Combination of
2
that children of the three groups differed significantly (χ  = 10.363; stress positions in each pair of sounds was considered but no signif-
p = .006) (Figure 2). A post hoc analysis (Mann–Whitney) showed icant effects were found.
a significant difference between children with TD and with DLD Analyses of RTs (ANOVA) also failed to highlight any significant
(U = 41.000; two-tailed p = .001). group difference.
As written above, an exploratory comparison between pure A Spearman correlation analysis, controlling for age, was con-
and comorbid subgroups (Mann–Whitney) revealed that the DLD- ducted between accuracy and RTs in the SSD task on the one hand,
only (Mdn = 72.25) and the DLD with additional reading difficul- and compound scores from language, metaphonological, and read-
ties (DLD + RD, Mdn = 75.00) subgroups did not differ significantly ing tests on the other hand. No significant correlations emerged
(p > .6), and that each of the subgroups significantly differed from the in the whole sample. No correlations with Working Memory were
TD group (Mdn = 88.9; U = 24.00, p = .013, r = 0.507 and U = 17.00, found either.
p = .003, r = 0.596, respectively).
A General Linear Model was performed on RTs, but no differ-
ences emerged between groups (p > .4). 5.3 | NonWord-Music Association Task (NW-MAT)
Then, a nonparametric (Spearman) correlation analysis between
reading, metaphonological and language scores, and the Humming Correct responses were analyzed with respect to stress position in
Task scores was run on the whole sample. Significant correlations the target nonword stimulus (first, second, and third position stress).
emerged between accuracy (correct responses) in the experimental Since the scores in this test presented no gross deviation from nor-
task and metaphonology (ρ(48) = 0.348, p = .015) as well as language mal distribution, a Repeated Measures General Linear Model was
(ρ(48) = 0.487, p < .001) compound scores. A post hoc analysis re- performed with Stress Position as within-subject factor and Group
vealed that the correlations with language mainly depended on the as between-subject factor (TD vs. DD vs. DLD). A significant Stress
Sentence Repetition and Morphosyntactic Tests and the correlation Position effect (F(1,660; 74,721) = 10,690, p < .001, etap2 = 0.192)
with metaphonology mainly depended on Rhyme Recognition (see was found. No interaction emerged.
Table 3). The correlation with language was confirmed also when ex- A post hoc paired t-test analysis revealed a significant differ-
cluding the TD group (ρ(32) = 0.470, p = .007). Spearman correlations ence between Third Stress Position Target Responses and both
revealed no associations with WM scores (ρ(20) = 0.024 for accuracy First (t = −3,087, p = .003) and Second Stress Position (t = −4,732,
and = 0.020 for RT, ps > .919). p < .001) (Figure 4).
Then, a Repeated Measures General Linear Model was per-
formed with Stress Combination (in Musical Pairs, whereby AB = 1
5.2 | Sound Stress Discrimination Task (SSDT) vs. 2 stress position, BC = 2 vs. 3, AC = 1 vs. 3) as within-subject
factor and Group as between-subject factor (TD vs. DD vs. DLD).
With regard to sound stress discrimination, it was preliminarily en- A Combination main effect (F(1,922; 86,482) = 3,863, p = .026)
sured that Target choices were clearly above chance level (50%) for emerged, but the Group x Combination interaction was far from sig-
all groups. Rather, performance in the TD group was close to ceiling, nificance (p = .83). A post hoc paired t-test analysis showed a signif-
with 43% of the children responding 100% correct, 37% making 1 icant difference between AB (1 vs. 2 stress position) and AC (1 vs. 3
error (on 18 trials) and only 12.5% making 3 or more errors. In the stress position) (t = −2,616, p =.012) (Figure 5).
DLD group, however, only 25% of children had 100% accuracy, and A one-way ANOVA was performed to determine group
43% of the children made 3 or more errors. DD children's perfor- differences in Response Time (RT). A significant group differ-
mance was at an intermediate level. ence in RT was found (F(2, 45) = 4,475, p = .017, eta p2 = 0.166).
Then, d-prime indexes were computed for each participant, tak- Specifically, a Tukey post hoc analysis revealed a significant
ing into account the total number of hits and false alarms in the whole difference between TD and DD (p = .024) and TD and DLD
task. Since the distribution of d-prime scores was clearly non-normal (p = .049) (Figure 3).
and skewed, a nonparametric analysis was performed for the com- A Repeated Measures General Linear Model was performed on
parison of the three groups. Kruskal–Wallis test did not reveal any RTs for target responses with Stress Position as within-subject factor

TA B L E 3   Spearman Correlations
Rhyme
(N = 48) between number of correct
Recognition Raw Sentence repetition Voice (active- passive) Clitics
responses at the Humming Task and
  score Z-score Z-score Z-score
reading and language tests
Accuracy humming task
ρ 0.389 0.369 0.528 0.396
p .006 .01 <.001 .005

Only significant correlations with ρ ≥ 0.35 are reported


CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      11 of 19

(ps > .320). However, the comparison of the TD group (Mdn = 37.75)


with each of the DLD subgroups revealed a significant difference for
the DLD + RD subgroup (U = 18.00; p = .005, r = 0.575) not emerging
for the DLD-only subgroup (U = 48.00; p = .327).
Spearman correlations with compound variables for language,
reading, and metaphonology revealed significant associations of
speed in the NW-MAT task with reading speed (ρ(46)  =  −0.434,
p = .003), whereas accuracy in the task correlated with metaphonol-
ogy (ρ(48) = −0.460, p = .001). Only the correlation of accuracy with
metaphonology was still significant after exclusion of the TD group
(ρ(32) = −0.354, p = .047).
No correlations with Working Memory were found.
Further analyses in the single groups showed significant cor-
relations (≥0.45) of RT in the NW-MAT task with reading accuracy
only for the TD and the DLD group, but in opposite directions, as
reported in Table 4. (ρ(14) = 0.557 p = .039, for TD, ρ(16) = −0.597,
F I G U R E 4   Mean % correct responses with different target
p = .015 for DLD).
stimuli ([ʹtatata] = 1, [taʹtata] = 2, [tataʹta] = 3) in the NonWord-
Music Association Task. Significantly different pairs are marked by A final, interesting correlation emerged between both Humming
same-color asterisks and NW-MAT accuracy and language milestones reported in parents
questionnaires (see Table 5): the higher the scores on these tests, the
earlier the milestones had been acquired.

6 | D I S CU S S I O N

The study investigated the relationships between prosody and musi-


cal processing in children with developmental dyslexia (DD) or de-
velopmental language disorder (DLD) compared with TD children.
It was shown that the ability to process rhythm in music has
strong connections with the processing of linguistic prosody and,
additionally, with metaphonological and reading skills. Its role in
language development is further supported by the correlations be-
tween the scores obtained in the experimental tasks and time of
achievement of language milestones.
In the Humming Task (HT) the processing of suprasegmental
aspects of language, precisely, of the F0 component was investi-
F I G U R E 5   Mean % correct responses in different combinations
gated. Most studies on prosody processing in these populations
of sound stimuli (AB = Stress Position 1 vs. 2, BC = 2 vs. 3, AC = 1
have focused on pragmatic aspects conveyed by prosody or on pros-
vs. 3) in the NonWord-Music Association Task, in the whole sample.
Significantly different pairs are marked by same-color asterisks ody in phrasing (e.g., Marshall, Harcourt-Brown, Ramus, & van der
Lely, 2009). Our results have shown that children with DLD have
more difficulties, compared to their DD and TD peers, in associating
and Group as between-subject factor (TD vs. DD vs. DLD). A Group linguistic structure to the humming F0 contour. Correlations reveal
main effect emerged as before, but no significant Stress Position a link between the ability to process sentence F0 contour and both
main effect and no interaction with Group was found. Furthermore, metaphonological and morphosyntactic skills. It is plausible that
no effects of Stress Combination in Musical Sequences on RT good prosodic processing facilitates grammatical and lexical process-
emerged. ing, and this link may have been manifesting for the linguistic task
An exploratory comparison of subgroups (in spite of very small stimuli as well (sentences to be repeated or manipulated). In addi-
sample sizes, N = 8 each subgroup) was performed in order to check tion, the humming task requires the association of a spoken sentence
whether comorbidity had an influence on the above effects. As men- (which is heard first) with one of two humming sentences. Therefore,
tioned above, a nonparametric test comparing children with DLD- the syntactic structure of the sentence, and the degree to which it
only (Mdn = 58.05) and with DLD + RD (Mdn = 46.94) had shown had been parsed and understood by the child, is likely to have a di-
no significant differences (Mann–Whitney, p = .248). The same was rect influence on the processing of the humming sentence, in a sort
true for the comparison of DD with DLD-only and with DLD + RD of top–down process. Since the spoken sentence is a syntactically
|
12 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

TA B L E 4   Spearman correlations (ρ (p)) between accuracy (ACC) and speed (RT) at the/NonWord-Music Association Task (NW-MAT) and
reading/metaphonological tasks, in the TD and DLD groups

DDE-Non DDE-Non
MT-errors MT-SILL/SEC DDE-word errors word speed word errors Spoonerism Rhyme
Group (n) Z Z Z Z Z Raw Recognition Raw

TD (16)
ACC — — ns ns ns 0.534 (0.033) ns
RT — — 0.453 (0.104) 0.492 (0.074) 0.556 (0.039) ns ns
DLD (16)
ACC ns ns ns ns ns 0.480 (0.06) ns
RT −0.547 (0.028) −0.558 (0.025) ns ns ns ns −0.561 (0.024)

Only significant correlations with ρ ≥ 0.45 are reported (no correlations emerged in the DD group).

TA B L E 5   Spearman Correlations (N = 48) between accuracy association task might be a characteristic of the subgroup of DLD
at the Humming Task/Nonword-Music Association Task (NW- children who also have reading difficulties. This observation, to be
MAT) and language milestones as emerging from the Parents
confirmed with larger groups of children, would support the hy-
Questionnaires
pothesis that reading difficulties rather than language difficulties
Age of first words Age of first are associated with difficulties in processing the interface between
  (n = 43) sentences (n = 33) language and music at word level.
Accuracy humming task In general, target stimuli with last position stress and the com-
ρ −0.448 −0.618 parison of musical sequences on first note accent versus last note
p .003 <.001 accent produced more correct and faster responses. Indeed, last

Accuracy NW_MAT position stress generally appears to be more salient (also as a conse-
quence of its low frequency) and thus easier to discriminate, espe-
ρ   −0.480
cially when compared to the most different stimulus, with stress in
p   .005
first position. Facilitation due to stress position, though, affected all
Only significant correlations with ρ ≥ 0.35 are reported.
groups in a similar way.
Interestingly, prosodic–rhythmic processing has different char-
ambiguous sentence that can only be disambiguated through pros- acteristics in children with developmental dyslexia and those with
ody, the opposite process, with prosodic ability influencing syntac- developmental language disorder. On one hand, children with DD
tic comprehension, is also operating. Thus, a bidirectional influence seem to be more impaired in stress and rhythm perception at word
between the two components of the task, the prosodic one and the level. On the other hand, children with DLD show more difficulties
syntactic one, must be occurring during task performance (and a in processing prosody and rhythm at sentence level. Developmental
similar, bidirectional influence between the two is also likely to be language disorder is often characterized in terms of difficulties with
occurring during the development of both skills; see also Richards syntax and morphosyntax but we know very little about prosodic
& Goswami, 2019). This may be interpreted in the framework of the perception in this condition (Leonard, 1998). The present study has
“prosodic phrasing” hypothesis proposed by Cumming, Wilson, and shown that children with DLD have difficulties in processing sen-
Goswami (2015). A further discussion of this interaction at the pros- tence rhythm but we do not know whether such difficulties reflect
ody–syntax interface is found in Caccia and Lorusso (2019). impairment in accessing deeper linguistic structures (i.e., syntax) or
The aim of the Sound Stress Discrimination Task (SSDT) was whether rhythmic difficulties are among the causes of impairment in
to investigate if children belonging to the three groups showed language processing. In line with the “prosodic phrasing” hypothe-
differences in discriminating musical rhythmic sequences which sis (Cumming, Wilson, & Goswami, 2015), our data have shown that
reproduced the intonation of trisyllabic nonwords but had no pho- children with DLD have difficulties with global prosodic structure
nological content. The lack of significant differences between the but only when it relates to sentence structure. In other terms, chil-
three groups suggests that processing of rhythmic sequences per se dren with DLD were more impaired in processing sentence prosody,
does not constitute a real obstacle for children with DD and DLD, whereas children with DD showed a deficit in lexical stress percep-
unless an interface with linguistic stimuli is required. tion with both musical and linguistic stimuli. This may suggest that if
The NonWord-Music Association Task (NW-MAT) investigated a language deficit is present, it is characterized in terms of a severe
the ability to associate a lexical element, with different stress charac- and general impairment of prosodic/rhythmic abilities regardless of
teristics, to the corresponding musical sequence. The TD group was the presence of additional reading impairments. By contrast, the
faster in responding than both DD and DLD children. Exploratory prosodic/rhythmic deficits shown by children with only DD seem
analyses suggest that slowness in responding to the nonword-music to be more specific and restricted than those of children with DLD.
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      13 of 19

Of course, it cannot be excluded that the absence of significant ef- distinguishing phonological impairment at phoneme level from a
fects in the sound discrimination task depends on the presence of phonological impairment extending to the suprasegmental level
almost at-ceiling performance in the TD group, and that a more chal- (see Figure 6). In our model, we further highlight the possible link
lenging task or more complex stimuli would have highlighted more between the “definitional” characteristics of DD and DLD children,
clear-cut differences. Indeed, the presence of rhythmic structure namely difficulties with reading and with language acquisition, re-
processing difficulties with nonverbal stimuli is well documented spectively, and the processing of prosodic and rhythmic structures
in both DD and DLD populations (e.g., Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch, in music and of prosodic structures in language (i.e., the interface
Tallal, & Temple, 2007; Lorusso, Vernice, et al., 2014; Tallal, 1980). between language and music), at both word and sentence level. In
Nonetheless, it should be noted that the stimuli in the sound stress Figure 6, this link is represented by adding the dimensions of reading
discrimination task were the same used in the NonWord-Music and language processing (in green lines), which do not constitute the
Association Task; this implies that the rhythmic stimuli that the results of the present study but synthesize what is already known
children could easily process and discriminate were still difficult about different characteristics of the two populations as emerging
(especially for dyslexic children) to associate with linguistic stimuli from the literature outlined in the Introduction.
sharing the same stress pattern. Again, the interface with language These results are partially in contrast with previous studies
is the crucial element allowing to highlight the processing difficulties showing difficulties at both word and sentence level in DD (e.g.,
of children with reading disorders (as also suggested by Goswami Holliman, Wood, & Sheney, 2010; Holliman, Wood, & Sheney, 2012),
et al., 2002, 2013). Clearly, associating the musical with the linguistic possibly due to not distinguishing between DD with and without lan-
stimuli is a more complex task, requiring to abstract a similar pat- guage difficulties. Overall, the humming task can be considered as
tern from stimuli belonging to different domains and to keep them in providing at least initial information about the interface between a
memory in order to compare them. Cross-modal processing is indeed purely linguistic and a non-phonological version (Hope et al., 2014)
one of the specific requirements of reading more than of language reproducing sentence prosody, but it does not include proper music
acquisition, and may (at least partly) constitute the basis for the asso- stimuli.
ciation of this task with reading ability. The correlations found with On the whole, then, the results of the present study point to
both metaphonology and reading also suggest that the link between the need of taking into consideration comorbidity and the different
processing the language/music interface and reading may be medi- characteristics of the various subgroups (with/without comorbidity)
ated by metaphonological skills. Indeed, it has been shown by previ- in order to understand the patterns of weaknesses and strengths in
ous studies on Italian children with DD with/without additional DLD the various domains, from language to reading to music, and at dif-
that metaphonological difficulties are more typically found in the ferent levels, in the range from segmental to suprasegmental.
comorbid than in the DD-only population (Brizzolara et al., 2006). Regardless of group divisions, and in line with the previous lit-
The different direction of the correlation found between speed in erature (e.g., Goswami, 2011; Rosen, 2003, etc.), prosodic abilities
the NW-MAT and reading accuracy for children with DLD as com- and musical processing turn out to be linked to both metaphonolog-
pared to TD children may point to the fact that, within the range of ical-reading skills and morphosyntactic abilities. This confirms that
skilled reading, slower responses may indicate a reflexive approach rhythmic skills play an important role in typical as well as in atypical
which may be advantageous and lead to more accurate decoding. By language development, and later on in reading acquisition, possi-
contrast, very slow responses in children with language impairments bly constituting one of the pre-requisites for optimal development
may reflect difficulties that also influence decoding accuracy. (Corriveau et al., 2010; Gordon et al., 2015; Tierney & Kraus, 2013),
In light of these considerations, our proposal is to expand Ramus independent of the role of Working Memory. This is in line with the
et al.’s (2013) model on the relationship between developmental results of studies on musicians and nonmusicians, showing large ef-
dyslexia and developmental language disorder, introducing a further fects of musical training on verbal skills, ranging from phonology to
dimension concerning prosodic and rhythmic skills. In their model, morphosyntax.
Ramus and colleagues proposed that language abilities in children Some limitations of the present study should be acknowledged,
with DLD and with DD vary across three dimensions: phonological, first of all the restricted number of participants in each group, not al-
non-phonological, and grammar/lexicon abilities. Considering the lowing to fully distinguish between potentially interesting subgroups
phonological deficit, they suggested that children with developmen- such as DLD with/without DD. SES was not controlled. Working
tal language disorder and developmental dyslexia show partly dis- memory tests were available for only a part of the sample, while no
tinct profiles along these different dimensions. A similar reasoning data were available from TD children, suggesting that the absence of
could apply to the prosodic and rhythmic abilities; children with DD significant correlations of the experimental variables with WM should
and DLD show partially distinct profiles in the processing of rhyth- be confirmed with larger datasets. Moreover, the experimental de-
mic skills in terms of both suprasegmental elements and pure musical sign would have been more complete with the presence of purely
sequences; nonetheless both groups differ from typically developing musical sequences as equivalents of linguistic stimuli at the sentence
children. In Ramus’ model, phonology is essentially analyzed at pho- level, in addition to prosodic sequences. Indeed a pilot study, requir-
neme and syllable levels. Introducing the prosodic dimension allows ing a) to discriminate between musical phrases similar in length and
us to have further information on the children's linguistic profile, rhythmic structure to the verbal sentences employed in the study
|
14 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

F I G U R E 6   Processing abilities (on a


continuum from + = good to − = poor)
of DLD and DD in a multidimensional
model. Processing skills are analyzed
at both word level and sentence level,
for each of two domains: music and the
language/music interface. The same
analysis is represented for the reading
and language domains (drawn in green:
NB not representing original findings
from the present study but describing the
characteristics of the two populations as
emerging from the literature), to highlight
the analogy with the new findings and
suggest links between different domains.
DLD + RD, DLD with additional reading
disorder; DLD – RD, DLD without
additional reading disorder

and b) to associate such “verbal and musical sentences”, showed clear motivating tasks that can easily be delivered in the form of games
floor effects that discouraged the use of such tasks with children of (e.g., dee-dee—analogous tasks, prosody imitation, sentence–pic-
this age. This means that the conclusions on the relationship between ture matching tasks etc.). The use of both perceptual and motor
rhythmic structures in the linguistic and musical domains emerging exercises capitalizes on auditory and rhythmic entrainment at the
from the present study refer to the basic components of linguistic neural level (Thaut, McIntosh, & Hoemberg, 2015). Indeed, recent
rhythm (duration, intensity, and pitch) and musical rhythm (duration studies have shown that training in processing musical and rhyth-
and intensity), not extending to the potential contribution of pitch mic components can be effective for both DLD (Cumming, Wilson,
variations in musical sequences, that is, not extending to melodic Leong, et al., 2015; Kraus & Chandrasekaran, 2010; Schön &
processing (see Tillmann, 2014). Nonetheless, the task that was em- Tillmann, 2015) and DD (Bhide, Power, & Goswami, 2013; Bonacina,
ployed to test processing of language and prosody at sentence level Lanzi, Lorusso, & Antonietti, 2015; Flaugnacco et al., 2015; Habib
(the humming test) reflected the specific characteristics of rhythmic et al., 2016; Overy, 2003). Musicians show greater sensitivity to the
structures in the language domain, encompassing duration, inten- metric structure of words, as well as to pitch in both language and
sity, and pitch variations (which were all preserved in the prosodic, musical stimuli, as compared to nonmusicians (Magne et al., 2006;
non-linguistic sequence). Marie, Magne, & Besson, 2011). Also the studies on dyslexic musi-
The clinical implications of the present work are multi-faceted. cians and nonmusicians (in spite of the difficulty to control for biases
In an early diagnosis-oriented perspective, testing prosodic abili- due to participants’ pre-existing ability) suggest that, even if some of
ties considering both lexical and sentence patterns could provide the deficits characteristic of DD (e.g., verbal working memory and
useful markers to distinguish the linguistic profiles of children the discrimination of syllables varying in amplitude envelope cues)
with developmental dyslexia and developmental language dis- are not influenced by musical training, some others (psychoacoustic
order in future studies, it would be interesting to find equivalent skills, auditory sequencing, and sensory–motor accuracy) could be
(developmentally appropriate) tests that could be administered to improved by training (Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, Hannon, &
pre-school children, to use as potential distinctive markers of the Snyder, 2015; Weiss et al., 2014; Zuk et al., 2017).
two disorders also before the children learn to read. To this aim,
further research is needed to better understand the relationships AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S
and mutual influences between language and rhythm perception in The present study was supported by the Italian Ministry of Health
these populations, and to define the developmental trajectories of (RC 2018–2019). The authors thank Mauro Martini and Alessandro
such relationships. Moreover, further studies are needed to shed Milesi for their precious contribution in creating the musical stimuli,
light on the processing of the single acoustic parameters (duration, and Valentina Di Chiaro, Noemi Mapelli, Francesca Marelli, Iolanda
pitch, and intensity) in children with DD and DLD (see also Caccia Paladino, Laura Panizza, Elisa Rosa, Federica Sartori for help in data
et al., 2019). In an intervention-oriented perspective, prosodic collection and transcription.
tasks specifically designed to address segmental or suprasegmen-
tal analysis could be useful tools for the prevention and rehabili- C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T
tation of DLD in the clinical practice, since they are enjoyable and The authors report no conflict of interest for the present study.
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      15 of 19

DATA AVA I L A B I L I T Y S TAT E M E N T Brandt, A., Gebrian, M., & Slevc, L. R. (2012). Music and early language
acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 327. https://doi.org/10.3389/
The data that support the findings of this study are available on re-
fpsyg.2012.00327
quest from the corresponding author, Maria Luisa Lorusso. The data Brizzolara, D., Chilosi, A., Cipriani, P., Di Filippo, G., Gasperini, F.,
are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions. Mazzotti, S., … Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Do phonologic and rapid au-
tomatized naming deficits differentially affect dyslexic children with
and without a history of language delay? A study of Italian dyslexic
E N D N OT E
children. Cogitive and Behavioural Neurology, 19, 141–149. https://doi.
1 Children belonging to the DLD group were subdivided according to
org/10.1097/01.wnn.00002​13902.59827.19
an ad-hoc rule requiring that all reading z-scores were above −2 (pro-
Buil-Legaz, L., Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., & Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J. (2015).
ducing two subgroups of nine and seven participants, respectively
Reading skills in young adolescents with a history of specific lan-
satisfying and not-satisfying the requirement). The two subgroups
guage impairment: The role of early semantic capacity. Journal of
were compared with nonparametric tests (Mann–Whitney) on all lan-
Communication Disorders, 58, 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
guage scores: the absence of significant differences (all ps above 0.6
jcomd​is.2015.08.001
for grammatical tests, above 0.4 for memory tests and above 0.06 for
Caccia, M., & Lorusso, M. L. (2019). When prosody meets syntax: The
metaphonological tests) supported the decision to consider the group
processing of the syntax-prosody interface in children with devel-
as a single group.
opmental dyslexia and developmental language disorder. Lingua,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2019.03.008
Caccia, M., Presti, G., Toraldo, A., Radaelli, A., Ludovico, L. A., Ogliari,
REFERENCES A., & Lorusso, M. L. (2019). Pitch as the main determiner of Italian
Akoglu, H. (2018). User's guide to correlation coefficients. Turkish Journal lexical stress perception across the lifespan: Evidence from typical
of Emergency Medicine, 18(3), 91–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. development and dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1458. https://
tjem.2018.08.001 doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01458
Barry, J. G., Harbodt, S., Cantiani, C., Sabisch, B., & Zobay, O. (2012). Cantiani, C., Lorusso, M.L., Perego, P., Molteni, M., & Guasti, M.T. (2015).
Sensitivity to lexical stress in dyslexia: A case of cognitive not per- Developmental Dyslexia with and without language impairment:
ceptual stress. Dyslexia, 18(3), 139–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ERPs reveal qualitative differences in morphosyntactic process-
dys.1440 ing. Developmental neuropsychology, 40(5), 291–312. https://doi.
Belacchi, C., Scalisi, T. G., Cannoni, E., & Cornoldi, C. (2008). CPM-Coloured org/10.1080/87565​6 41.2015.1072536
Progressive Matrices. Standardizzazione italiana [CPM-Coloured Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-
Progressive Matrices. Italian Standardization]. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Quintanilla, E., … Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of predic-
Bertelli, B., & Bilancia, G. (2006). VAUMeLF Batterie per la valutazione tion of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies.
dell’attenzione uditiva e della memoria di lavoro fonologica nell’età evo- Psychological Science, 23, 678–686. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567​
lutiva [Batteries for the assessment of auditory attention and phono- 97611​434536
logical working memory in the developmental age]. Firenze: Giunti Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are spe-
O.S. cific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of
Bertinetto, P. M. (1980). The perception of stress by Italian speakers. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(6), 1378–1396. https://
Journal of Phonetics, 8(4), 385–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095​ doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/096)
-4470(19)31495​- 0 Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (Eds.) (2005). Speech perception in dyslexic
Bhide, A., Power, A., & Goswami, U. (2013). A rhythmic musical interven- children with and without language impairments. In The connections
tion for poor readers: A comparison of efficacy with a letter-based between language and reading disabilities (pp. 82–101). Mahwah, NJ:
intervention. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7(2), 113–123. https://doi. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
org/10.1111/mbe.12016 Chiat, S. (1983). Why Mikey's right and my key's wrong: The sig-
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dys- nificance of stress and word boundaries in a child's output sys-
lexia and developmental language disorder: Same or different? tem. Cognition, 14(3), 275–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-
Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 0277(83)90007​- 0
0033-2909.130.6.858 Christophe, A., Nespor, M., Guasti, M. T., & Van Ooyen, B. (2003).
Bocci, G. (2008). On the syntax-prosody interface: An analysis of the Prosodic structure and syntactic acquisition: The case of the head-di-
prosodic properties of postfocal material in Italian and its implica- rection parameter. Developmental Science, 6(2), 211–220. https://doi.
tions. Nanzan Linguistics, Special Issue, 5, 13–42. org/10.1111/1467-7687.00273
Boersma, P. (2006). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. http://www. Chung, W. L., Jarmulowicz, L., & Bidelman, G. M. (2017). Auditory pro-
praat.org/ cessing, linguistic prosody awareness, and word reading in Mandarin-
Bonacina, S., Cancer, A., Lanzi, P. L., Lorusso, M. L., & Antonietti, A. speaking children learning English. Reading and Writing, 30(7), 1407–
(2015). Improving reading skills in students with dyslexia: The effi- 1429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1114​5-017-9730-8
cacy of a sublexical training with rhythmic background. Frontiers in Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999). Classification of children with
Psychology, 6, 1510. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01510 SLI: Longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Bonacina, S., Krizman, J., White-Schwoch, T., Nicol, T., & Kraus, N. Disorders, 42, 1205–1219. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1195
(2019). How rhythmic skills relate and develop in school-age chil- Cornoldi, C., Colpo, G. & M. T. Group (2011). Prove di lettura MT-2 per la
dren. Global Pediatric Health, 6, 2333794X1985204. https://doi. Scuola Primaria [Reading Tests for Primary School]. Firenze: Giunti
org/10.1177/23337​94X19​852045 O.S.
Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Associated reading skills Corriveau, K. H., Goswami, U., & Thomson, J. (2010). Auditory process-
in children with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Reading ing and early literacy skills in a preschool and kindergarten popu-
and Writing, 19(1), 77–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1114​5-005-4322-4 lation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 369–382. https://doi.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning org/10.1177/00222​19410​369071
to read—a causal connection. Nature, 301(5899), 419. https://doi. Corriveau, K., Pasquini, E., & Goswami, U. (2007). Basic auditory pro-
org/10.1038/301419a0 cessing skills and developmental language disorder: A new look at
|
16 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

an old hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, improve reading and related disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 26.
50(June), 647–667. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/046) https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00026
Cumming, R., Wilson, A., & Goswami, U. (2015). Basic auditory pro- Hausen, M., Torppa, R., Salmela, V. R., Vainio, M., & Särkämö, T. (2013).
cessing and sensitivity to prosodic structure in children with devel- Music and speech prosody: A common rhythm. Frontiers in Psychology,
opmental language disorders: A new look at a perceptual hypoth- 4(SEP), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00566
esis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(July), 972. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Heffner, C. C., & Slevc, L. R. (2015). Prosodic structure as a parallel to
fpsyg.2015.00972 musical structure. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(DEC), 1–14. https://doi.
Cumming, R., Wilson, A., Leong, V., Colling, L. J., & Goswami, U. (2015). org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01962
Awareness of rhythm patterns in speech and music in children with Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T.
specific language impairments. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, (2009). Infant Behavior and Development Language specific pro-
672. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00672 sodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from
Doelling, K. B., & Poeppel, D. (2015). Cortical entrainment to music and German and French infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 32,
its modulation by expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy 262–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.03.004
of Sciences, 112(45), E6233–E6242. https://doi.org/10.1073/ Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2010). The contribution of sen-
pnas.15084​31112 sitivity to speech rhythm and non-speech rhythm to early reading
Falk, S., Lanzilotti, C., & Schön, D. (2017). Tuning neural phase entrain- development. Educational Psychology, 30, 247–267. https://doi.
ment to speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(8), 1378–1389. org/10.1080/01443​41090​3560922
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01136 Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2012). A cross-sec-
Farnetani, E., & Busà, M. G. (1999). Quantifying the range of vowel re- tional study of prosodic sensitivity and reading difficul-
duction in Italian. In Proceeding of the 14th International Congress of ties. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(1), 32–48. https://doi.
Phonetic science. org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01459.x
Ferrari, E., De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., & Barbieri, E. (1981). Standardizzazione Hope, T. M., Prejawa, S., Jones, P., Oberhuber, M., Seghier, M. L., Green,
di una batteria per la valutazione dei disturbi del linguaggio nell’età D. W., & Price, C. J. (2014). Dissecting the functional anatomy of
scolare [Standardization of a battery for language assessment in auditory word repetition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 246.
school-age]. Neuropsichiatria Infantile, 235, 148–158. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00246
Flaugnacco, E., Lopez, L., Terribili, C., Montico, M., Zoia, S., & Schön, Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., Rigaut, C., & Gerard, C. L. (1998). Determiners
D. (2015). Music training increases phonological awareness and and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language
reading skills in developmental dyslexia: A randomized control Acquisition, 7(2–4), 113–160. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532​ 7817l​
trial. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0138715. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​ a0702​- 4_3
al.pone.0138715 Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000).
Fonteneau, E., & van der Lely, H. K. (2008). Electrical brain responses Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonol-
in language-impaired children reveal grammar-specific deficits. PLoS ogy, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(1), 30–
ONE, 3(3), e1832. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0001832 60. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2553
Fujii, S., & Schlaug, G. (2013). The Harvard Beat Assessment Test (H- Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M., Svenkerud, V. Y., &
BAT): A battery for assessing beat perception and production and Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants′ sensitivity to the sound patterns of
their dissociation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(1), https://doi. native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(3), 402–
org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00771 420. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1022
Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., & Temple, E. (2007). Jusczyk, P. W., & Thompson, E. (1978). Perception of a phonetic con-
Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in chil- trast in multisyllabic utterances by 2-month-old infants. Perception
dren with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: An and Psychophysics, 23(2), 105–109. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF032​
fMRI study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25, 295–310. 08289
Giordano, R., & D'Anna, L. (2010). A comparison of rhythm metrics in Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the devel-
different speaking styles and in fifteen regional varieties of Italian. In opment of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), 599.
Speech Prosody 2010-Fifth International Conference. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2882
Giraud, A. L., & Poeppel, D. (2012). Cortical oscillations and speech pro- Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
cessing: Emerging computational principles and operations. Nature University Press.
Neuroscience, 15, 511–517. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3063 Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with developmental language disorder.
Gordon, R. L., Shivers, C. M., Wieland, E. A., Kotz, S. A., Yoder, P. J., & Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Devin Mcauley, J. (2015). Musical rhythm discrimination explains Leong, V., Hämäläinen, J., Soltész, F., & Goswami, U. (2011). Rise time per-
individual differences in grammar skills in children. Developmental ception and detection of syllable stress in adults with developmental
Science, 18(4), 635–644. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12230 dyslexia. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(1), 59–73. https://doi.
Goswami, U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmen- org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.09.003
tal dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 3–10. https://doi. Lorusso, M. L., Cantiani, C., & Molteni, M. (2014). Age, dyslexia subtype
org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001 and comorbidity modulate rapid auditory processing in developmen-
Goswami, U., Mead, N., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Barnes, L., & Leong, V. tal dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(May), 1–16. https://
(2013). Impaired perception of syllable stress in children with dys- doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00313
lexia: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(1), Lorusso, M. L., Vernice, M., Dieterich, M., Brizzolara, D., Mariani, E.,
1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.03.001 De Masi, S., … Mele, A. (2014). The process and criteria for diag-
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., nosing specific learning disorders: Indications from the Consensus
Rosen, S., & Scott, S. K. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and de- Conference promoted by the Italian National Institute of Health.
velopmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Annali Dell’istituto Superiore Di Sanità, 50(1), 77–89. https://doi.
Academy of Sciences, 99(16), 10911–10916. https://doi.org/10.1073/ org/10.4415/ANN_14_01_12
pnas.12236​8599 Magne, C., Schön, D., & Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect
Habib, M., Lardy, C., Desiles, T., Commeiras, C., Chobert, J., & Besson, pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician
M. (2016). Music and dyslexia: A new musical training method to children: Behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. Journal
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      17 of 19

of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(2), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1162/ Rescorla, L., & Turner, H. L. (2015). Morphology and syntax in late talk-
jocn.2006.18.2.199 ers at age 5. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(2),
Marconi, L., Ott, M., Pesenti, E., Ratti, D., & Tavella, M. (1993). Lessico 434–444. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR​-L-14-0042
elementare: Dati statistici sull’italianoletto e scritto dai bambini delle el- Reynolds, C. R., & Bigler, E. D. (1994). Test TEMA–Memoria e apprendi-
ementari [Elementary lexicon: Statistical data for Italian written and mento [Italian adaptation of TOMAL Test of Memory and Learning].
spoken by elementary school children]. Bologna: Zanichelli. Trento: Erickson.
Marie, C., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2011). Musicians and the metric Richards, S., & Goswami, U. (2015). Auditory processing in developmen-
structure of words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 294–305. tal language disorder (DLD): Relations with the perception of lexical
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21413 and phrasal stress. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Marotta, L., Trasciani, M., & Vicari, S. (2008). Test CMF. Valutazione delle 58(4), 1292–1305. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015
competenze metafonologiche. [Assessment of metaphonological abili- Richards, S., & Goswami, U. (2019). Impaired recognition of metrical and
ties]. Trento: Edizioni Erickson. syntactic boundaries in children with developmental language dis-
Marshall, C. R., Harcourt-Brown, S., Ramus, F., & van der Lely, H. K. J. orders. Brain Sciences, 9(2), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/brain​sci90​
(2009). The link between prosody and language skills in children 20033
with developmental language disorder (DLD) and/or dyslexia. Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and developmental
International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders/Royal language disorder: Is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it ex-
College of Speech and Language Therapists, 44(4), 466–488. https:// plain anything? Journal of Phonetics, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095​
doi.org/10.1080/13682​82080​2591643 -4470(03)00046​-9
McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, Sansavini, A., Bertoncini, J., & Giovanelli, G. (1997). Newborns discrim-
E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and inate the rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words. Developmental
specific language impairment. The Journal of Child Psychology Psychology, 33(1), 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.3
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(7), 869–874. https://doi. Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2007). DDE-2 Batteria per la valu-
org/10.1111/1469-7610.00674 tazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva [Battery for
Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (1996). Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from Developmental Dyslexia and Developemental Dysorthography].
speech to grammar in early acquisition. New Jersey: L. Erlbaum Associates. Firenze: Giunti O. S.
Myers, B. R., Lense, M. D., & Gordon, R. L. (2019). Pushing the enve- Schön, D., & Tillmann, B. (2015). Short-and long-term rhythmic inter-
lope: Developments in neural entrainment to speech and the bio- ventions: Perspectives for language rehabilitation. Annals of the New
logical underpinnings of prosody perception. Brain Sciences, 9(3), 70. York Academy of Sciences, 1337(1), 32–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/
https://doi.org/10.3390/brain​sci90​3 0070 nyas.12635
Overy, K. (2003). Dyslexia and music: From timing deficits to musical Snowling, M. J. (2000). Language and literacy skills: who is at risk and why.
intervention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 497– Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics,
505. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.060 Intervention and Outcome, 245–259.
Palmer, C., & Hutchins, S. (2006). What is musical prosody? Psychology of Soderstrom, M. J. (2000). The prosodic bootstrapping of phrases:
Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory, https://doi. Evidence from prelinguistic infants. Journal of Memory and Language,
org/10.1016/S0079​-7421(06)46007​-2 49(2), 249–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749​-596X(03)00024​-X
Patel, A. D. (2003a). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Szenkovits, G., & Ramus, F. (2005). Exploring dyslexics' phonological defi-
Neuroscience, 6(7), 674. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1082 cit I: Lexical vs sub-lexical and input vs output processes. Dyslexia,
Patel, A. D. (2003b). Rhythm in language and music: Parallels and differ- 11(4), 253–268. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.308
ences. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999(1), 140–143. Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading dis-
https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.015 abilities in children. Brain and Language, 9(2), 182–198. https://doi.
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. org/10.1016/0093-934X(80)90139​-X
Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13. https://doi. Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time.
org/10.1016/j.jneum​eth.2006.11.017 Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(9), 721–728. https://doi.org/10.1038/
Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of de- nrn1499
velopmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2), 385–413. https://doi. Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., & Hoemberg, V. (2015). Neurobiological
org/10.1016/j.cogni​tion.2006.04.008 foundations of neurologic music therapy: Rhythmic entrainment
Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2006). and the motor system. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1185. https://doi.
Assessing prosodic and pragmatic ability in children with high-func- org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01185
tioning autism. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1776–1791. https://doi. Thomson, J. M., Leong, V., & Goswami, U. (2013). Auditory processing
org/10.1016/j.pragma.2005.07.004 interventions and developmental dyslexia: A comparison of phone-
Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. mic and rhythmic approaches. Reading and Writing, 26(2), 139–161.
The Lancet, 379(9830), 1997–2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140​ https://doi.org/10.1007/s1114​5-012-9359-6
-6736(12)60198​-6 Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2013). Music training for the development of
Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and reading skills. In Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 207). Elsevier B.V.
acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 115–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63327​-9.00008​- 4
https://doi.org/10.1177/00238​3 0903​0 4600​20501 Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2014). Neural entrainment to the rhythmic
Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & Van Der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). structure of music. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 400–408.
Phonological deficits in developmental language disorder and devel- https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00704
opmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136(2), Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2015). Evidence for multiple rhythmic skills.
630–645. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain​/aws356 PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0136645. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? The al.pone.0136645
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 129–141. https:// Tillmann, B. (2014). Pitch processing in music and speech. Acoustics
doi.org/10.1080/17470​21070​1508822 Australia, 42(2), 124–130.
Raven, J. C. (1947). Coloured progressive matrices. London, UK: H. K. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E.,
Lewis. & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of developmental language
|
18 of 19       CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO

disorder in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and with dyslexia: Auditory sequencing and speech abilities. Journal of
Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. https://doi.org/10.1044/ Experimental Psychology: General, 146(4), 495. https://doi.org/10.1037/
jslhr.4006.1245 xge00​0 0281
Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, C. M., Hannon, E. E., & Snyder, J. S.
(2015). Finding the music of speech: Musical knowledge influences
pitch processing in speech. Cognition, 40(6), 135–140. https://doi. S U P P O R T I N G I N FO R M AT I O N
org/10.1016/j.cogni​tion.2015.06.015 Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Weiss, A. H., Granot, R. Y., & Ahissar, M. (2014). The enigma of dyslexic
Supporting Information section.
musicians. Neuropsychologia, 54, 28–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuro​psych​ologia.2013.12.009
Whalley, K., & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in chil-
dren’ s reading development. Literacy, 29(3), 288–303. https://doi. How to cite this article: Caccia M, Lorusso ML. The
org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00309.x processing of rhythmic structures in music and prosody by
World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and
children with developmental dyslexia and developmental
behavioral disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines, Vol.
1. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. language disorder. Dev Sci. 2020;00:e12981. https://doi.
Zuk, J., Bishop-Liebler, P., Ozernov-Palchik, O., Moore, E., Overy, K., org/10.1111/desc.12981
Welch, G., & Gaab, N. (2017). Revisiting the “enigma” of musicians

APPENDIX A
Humming task examples
1. [Inline Image Removed1][[Gianni saluta ] la ragazza
con il cappello].
  [[John greets ] the girl with the hat].

2. [Inline Image Removed1] [[Gianni saluta la ragazza]


con il cappello].
  [[John greets the girl] with the hat].

F I G U R E A 3   Main screen of the Humming task.

APPENDIX B
Sound Discrimination and Nonword-Music Association Task
examples
F I G U R E A 1   Sound Spectrum and Pitch Contour (F0) of the
Humming version of Sentence 1.

F I G U R E A 2   Sound Spectrum and Pitch Contour (F0) of the F I G U R E B 1   Main screen of the Sound Stress Discrimination
Humming version of Sentence 2. task.
CACCIA and LUISA LORUSSO |
      19 of 19

F I G U R E B 2   In the left box, sound Spectrum and Pitch Contour (F0) of the musical sequence with first position stress. In the right box,
sound Spectrum and Intensity of [ʹtatata].

F I G U R E B 3   In the left box, sound Spectrum and Pitch Contour (F0) of the musical sequence with second position stress. In the right box,
sound Spectrum and Intensity of [taʹtata].

F I G U R E B 4   In the left box, sound Spectrum and Pitch Contour (F0) of the musical sequence with third position stress. In the right box,
sound Spectrum and Intensity of [tataʹta].

F I G U R E B 5   Main screen of the NonWord-Music Association


task.

You might also like