You are on page 1of 53

Handbook of Visual Optics, Volume

One: Fundamentals and Eye Optics 1st


Edition Artal
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-visual-optics-volume-one-fundamentals-
and-eye-optics-1st-edition-artal/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Handbook of Visual Optics, Two-Volume Set 1st Edition


Pablo Artal (Editor)

https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-visual-optics-two-
volume-set-1st-edition-pablo-artal-editor/

Handbook of Visual Optics, Volume Two : Instrumentation


and Vision Correction 1st Edition Artal

https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-visual-optics-
volume-two-instrumentation-and-vision-correction-1st-edition-
artal/

Photonics rules of thumb : optics, electro-optics,


fiber optics, and lasers Third Edition Edward Friedman

https://textbookfull.com/product/photonics-rules-of-thumb-optics-
electro-optics-fiber-optics-and-lasers-third-edition-edward-
friedman/

Fundamentals of Nonlinear Optics, Second Edition Peter


E. Powers

https://textbookfull.com/product/fundamentals-of-nonlinear-
optics-second-edition-peter-e-powers/
Progress in Optics Volume 61 1st Edition Visser

https://textbookfull.com/product/progress-in-optics-
volume-61-1st-edition-visser/

Principles of Electron Optics Basic Geometrical Optics


2nd Edition Peter W. Hawkes

https://textbookfull.com/product/principles-of-electron-optics-
basic-geometrical-optics-2nd-edition-peter-w-hawkes/

Handbook of Optoelectronics, Second Edition: Applied


Optical Electronics (Volume Three) (Series in Optics
and Optoelectronics) (Volume 3) John P. Dakin

https://textbookfull.com/product/handbook-of-optoelectronics-
second-edition-applied-optical-electronics-volume-three-series-
in-optics-and-optoelectronics-volume-3-john-p-dakin/

Optics Eugene Hecht

https://textbookfull.com/product/optics-eugene-hecht/

Nonlinear optics Boyd

https://textbookfull.com/product/nonlinear-optics-boyd/
HANDBOOK OF VISUAL OPTICS
VOLUME I
Handbook of Visual Optics

Handbook of Visual Optics: Fundamentals and Eye Optics, Volume One


Handbook of Visual Optics: Instrumentation and Vision Correction, Volume Two
HANDBOOK OF VISUAL OPTICS
Fundamentals and Eye Optics
VOLUME I

edited by
Pablo Artal
MATL AB® is a trademark of The MathWorks, Inc. and is used with permission. The MathWorks does not warrant the accuracy of the text or exercises in
this book. This book’s use or discussion of MATLAB® software or related products does not constitute endorsement or sponsorship by The MathWorks of a
particular pedagogical approach or particular use of the MATLAB® software.

CRC Press
Taylor & Francis Group
6000 Broken Sound Parkway NW, Suite 300
Boca Raton, FL 33487-2742

© 2017 by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC


CRC Press is an imprint of Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

No claim to original U.S. Government works

Printed on acid-free paper


Version Date: 20161115

International Standard Book Number-13: 978-1-4822-3785-6 (Hardback)

This book contains information obtained from authentic and highly regarded sources. Reasonable efforts have been made to publish reliable data and
information, but the author and publisher cannot assume responsibility for the validity of all materials or the consequences of their use. The authors and
publishers have attempted to trace the copyright holders of all material reproduced in this publication and apologize to copyright holders if permission
to publish in this form has not been obtained. If any copyright material has not been acknowledged please write and let us know so we may rectify in
any future reprint.

Except as permitted under U.S. Copyright Law, no part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmitted, or utilized in any form by any electronic,
mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or in any information storage or
retrieval system, without written permission from the publishers.

For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.com (http://www.copyright.com/) or contact
the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (CCC), 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organization that provides
licenses and registration for a variety of users. For organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by the CCC, a separate system of payment
has been arranged.

Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without
intent to infringe.

Library of Congress Cataloging‑in‑Publication Data

Names: Artal, Pablo, editor.


Title: Handbook of visual optics / [edited by] Pablo Artal.
Description: Boca Raton : Taylor & Francis, [2017] | Includes bibliographical
references.
Identifiers: LCCN 2016030030| ISBN 9781482237856 (hbk : alk. paper) | ISBN
9781315373034 (ebk) | ISBN 9781315355726 (epub) | ISBN 9781315336664
(mobi/Kindle) | ISBN 9781482237863 (web PDF)
Subjects: | MESH: Vision, Ocular--physiology | Optical Phenomena | Vision
Tests--instrumentation | Eye Diseases--therapy
Classification: LCC QP475 | NLM WW 103 | DDC 612.8/4--dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2016030030

Visit the Taylor & Francis Web site at


http://www.taylorandfrancis.com

and the CRC Press Web site at


http://www.crcpress.com
Contents
Preface vii
Editor ix
Contributors xi

Part I INtrODUCtION 1

1. History of physiological optics in the twentieth century 3


Gerald Westheimer
2. Possibilities in physiological optics 11
David R. Williams and Sarah Walters

Part II FUNDaMENtaLS 25

3. Geometrical optics 27
Jim Schwiegerling
4. Wave optics 39
Daniel Malacara
5. aberrations in optical systems 75
José Sasián
6. Photometry 83
Yoshi Ohno
7. Characterization of visual stimuli using the standard display model 93
Joyce E. Farrell, Haomiao Jiang, and Brian A. Wandell
8. Basic ophthalmic instruments 103
Walter D. Furlan
9. Instrumentation for adaptive optics 121
Chris Dainty
10. anatomy and embryology of the eye: an overview 129
Vivian Choh and Jacob G. Sivak
11. the retina 141
Michael A. Freed
12. Visual system architecture 159
Jonathan Winawer and Hiroshi Horiguchi
13. Visual psychophysical methods 181
Denis G. Pelli and Joshua A. Solomon

Part III OPtICaL PrOPErtIES OF tHE EYE 187

14. the cornea 189


Michael Collins, Stephen Vincent, and Scott Read
15. the lens 211
Fabrice Manns, Arthur Ho, and Jean-Marie Parel
16. Schematic eyes 235
David A. Atchison
17. axes and angles of the eye 249
David A. Atchison
18. the retina and the Stiles– Crawford effects 257
Brian Vohnsen
vi Contents

19. refractive errors 277


David A. Wilson
20. Monochromatic aberrations 293
Susana Marcos, Pablo Pérez-Merino, and Carlos Dorronsoro
21. Peripheral aberrations 313
Linda Lundström and Robert Rosén
22. Customized eye models 337
Juan Tabernero
23. Scattering, straylight, and glare 349
Thomas J.T.P. van den Berg
24. accommodation mechanisms 363
Shrikant R. Bharadwaj
25. accommodation dynamics 377
Lyle S. Gray and Barry Winn
26. Eye Movements 387
Andrew J. Anderson
27. aging and the eye’s optics 405
W. Neil Charman
28. Polarization properties 413
Juan M. Bueno

Index 431
Preface
For many years, first as a student and later as a senior researcher instruments. Each chapter is self-contained but oriented to pro-
in the area of physiological optics, I have wanted a comprehensive vide the proper background for the rest of the handbook. Basic
resource for frequently arising questions. Although the situation optics is covered by Schwiegerling (geometrical optics), Malacara
in today’s Internet era is different than before, still I believe there (wave optics), and Sasián (aberrations). The concepts of photom-
is need for a reliable single source of encyclopedic knowledge. etry and colorimetry are summarized in Chapter 6 (Ohno). The
Finally, the dream of my youth—a handbook in visual optics—is basics and limits of the generation of visual stimuli are described
a reality and in your hands (or on your screen). I hope this will in Chapter 7 (Farrell et al.). Furlan provides a complete revision
help interested readers for a long time to come. on the main ophthalmic instruments, and Dainty an introduc-
At the beginning of this adventure of compiling the hand- tion on adaptive optics. While the first chapters of this part are
book, I wanted to accomplish a number of goals (probably, devoted to the more technical aspects, the three next chapters
too many!). Among others, I wanted to provide general useful have a different orientation to provide the physiological basis
information for beginners, or for those approaching the field from for the eye and the visual system. Choh and Sivak describe the
other disciplines, and the latest research presented from the most anatomy and embryology of the eye in Chapter 10. Freed reviews
recent experiments in laboratories. As with most activities in life, the retina, and Winawer the architecture of the visual system. In
success depends on the quality of individuals involved. In this the final chapter in this part, Pelli and Solomon describe psy-
regard, I was tremendously fortunate to have such an exceptional chophysical methods. Part II sets the foundation for the various
group of contributors. If we can apply the optical equivalence, principles that follow in the rest of the handbook.
this handbook is the result of a coherent superposition of excep- Part III covers the current state of the art on the understand-
tional expertise. ing of the optics of the eye and the retina. Collins et al. and
This handbook builds from the fundamentals to the current Manns describe, respectively, what we know today about the
state of the art of the field of visual optics. The eye as an opti- optical properties of the cornea and the lens. Atchison reviews in
cal instrument plays a limiting role in the quality of our vision. Chapters 16 and 17 the different schematics eyes and the defini-
A better understanding of the optics of the eye is required both tions and implications of the axes and angles in ocular optics.
for ophthalmic instrumentation and vision correction. The hand- The optics of the retina is detailed in Chapter 18 (Vohnsen).
book covers the physics and engineering of instruments together Once the different components are evaluated, the next chapters
with procedures to correct the ocular optics and its impact on concentrate on the impact of optical quality. Refractive errors
visual perception. The field of physiological, or visual, optics (Wilson) and monochromatic (Marcos et al.) aberrations are
is a classic area in science, an arena where many new practical described. Although traditionally most attention has been paid
technologies have been tested and perfected. Many of the most to optical characteristics of the eye in the fovea, the important
brilliant scientists in history were interested in the eye. Based in role of peripheral optics is described in Chapter 21 (Lundström
well-established physical and physiological principles, the area and Rosén). Tabernero describes personalized eye models in
was described as nearly completed in the second part of the twen- Chapter 22. Beyond refractive errors and aberrations, scatter-
tieth century. However, from the 1980s onward, a tremendous ing in the eye affects image quality. van den Berg exhaustively
new interest in this field appeared. This was driven in part by new reviews the state of the art of the impact and measurements of
technology, such as lasers and electronic cameras, which allowed this phenomenon (Chapter 23). The eye in young subjects has the
the introduction of new instrumentation. For example, the use ability to focus objects placed at different distances efficiently.
of wave-front sensors and adaptive optics concepts on the eye Bharadwaj provides a review of the accommodative mechanism
completely changed the field. In relatively few years, these ideas (Chapter 24), and Winn and Gray describe its dynamics (Chapter
expanded to the clinical areas of ophthalmology and optom- 25). The eyes are continually moving to place the fovea on the
etry. Today, research in new aspects of vision correction and area of interest. This dynamic behavior has important implica-
instruments is extremely active, with many groups working on tions described in Chapter 26 (Anderson). Although the human
it around the world. This area is a mixture of fundamentals and eye is very robust, serving us over many years, aging obviously
applications, and is at the crossroad of many disciplines: physics, affects its optics. In Chapter 27, Charman reviews how the eye
medicine, biology, psychology, and engineering. I tried to find an changes with age. Several species are able to detect the state of
equilibrium among the different approaches and sensibilities to polarization of light. Although our visual system is not capable of
serve all tastes. This book can be accessed sequentially, but also by something similar, polarization plays a role in optical properties
individual parts whenever a particular topic is required. as described in Chapter 28 (Bueno).
The handbook is organized in two volumes, with five total Volume Two focuses on the important topics of instrumenta-
parts. Volume One begins with an introductory part that gives tion and vision correction. Part I is dedicated to novel ophthalmic
an exceptional appetizer by two giants of the field: Gerald instrumentation for imaging, including the anterior segment and
Westheimer presents an historical account of the field, and David the retina, and for visual testing. An introductory chapter is dedi-
Williams explores the near past and the future. Part II covers cated to reviewing the concepts of light safety (Barat). Molebny
background and fundamental information on optical principles, presents a complete description of different wavefront sensors and
ocular anatomy and physiology, and the eye and ophthalmic aberrometers in Chapter 2. Hitzenberger reviews the principle
viii Preface

of low-coherence interferometry (Chapter 3). This was the basis tissues may open the door to new reversible future treatments.
for one of the most successful techniques in ophthalmology: Chapter 17 (van de Pol) presents the state of the art of using corneal
optical coherence tomography (OCT). Grulkowski concentrates onlays and inlays for vision correction.
on the current state of the art in OCT applied to the anterior Part III reviews the relationship between the ocular optics
segment (Chapter 4). Popovic (Chapter 5) and Doble (Chapter 6) and visual perception. Aspects related to optical visual met-
present how adaptive optics implemented in ophthalmoscopes rics (Chapter 18, Guirao) and the prediction of visual acuity
has changed the field in recent years. A different application of (Chapter 19, Navarro) are included. Adaptation is a key ele-
adaptive optics is its use for visual testing. Fernandez (Chapter 7) ment in vision and may have significant clinical implications.
shows the history, present, and future potential of this technol- Chapters 20 (Webster and Marcos) and 21 (Shaeffel) describe
ogy. Imaging of the ocular media using multiphoton microscopy adaptation to blur and contrast. Visual functions change with
is a recent scientific frontier. Jester (Chapter 8) and Hunter age. A description of these characteristics is a useful resource
(Chapter 9) cover, respectively, the applications of this emerging for those interested in any practical application. Chapter 22
technology for the cornea and the retina. (Wood) reviews age-related aspects of vision. Finally,
Part II describes the different devices and techniques for surgical Chapter 23 (Jimenez) explores the impact of the eye’s optics in
and nonsurgical visual correction procedures, from traditional to stereovision.
futuristic approaches. Ophthalmic lenses are still the most widely I thank the many people who contributed to this handbook:
used approach and clearly deserve to be well recognized. Malacara of course, all the authors for providing accurate and up-to-date
(Chapter 10) presents a complete overview of this topic. Contact chapters; Carmen Martinez for helping me with secretarial work,
lenses are described in depth in Chapter 11 (Cox). The specific and Luna Han from Taylor & Francis Group for her guidance
case of correcting highly aberrated eyes is addressed in Chapter 12 and patience. I am also indebted to the financial help received by
(Marsack and Applegate). A particularly relevant type of correct- my lab, which allowed dedication to this endeavor: the European
ing devices is intraocular lenses (IOLs), implanted to substitute Research Council, the Spanish Ministry of Science, and the
the crystalline lens after cataract surgery. Two emerging types of Fundacion Seneca, Murcia region, Spain.
IOLs, accommodating and adjustable, are reported in Chapters 13
(Findl and Himschall) and 14 (Sandstedt). Chapter 15 (Alio and Pablo Artal
El Bahrawy) presents a review of refractive surgical approaches for Universidad de Murcia
the cornea. The potential for nonlinear manipulation of the ocular Murcia, Spain
Editor
Pablo Artal was born in Zaragoza (Spain) in 1961. He stud- He has published more than 200 reviewed papers that received
ied Physics at the University of Zaragoza. In 1984, he moved more than 7600 citations with an H-index of 45 (12700 and 60
to Madrid with a predoctoral fellowship to work at the CSIC in Google scholar) and presented more than 200 invited talks
“Instituto de Optica.” He was a postdoctoral research fellow, first in international meetings and around 150 seminars in research
at Cambridge University (UK) and later at the Institut d’Optique institutions around the world. He was elected fellow member
in Orsay, France. After his return to Spain, he obtained a of the Optical Society of America (OSA) in 1999, fellow of the
permanent researcher position at the CSIC in Madrid. In 1994 Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology in 2009
he became the first full professor of optics at the University of and 2013 (gold class), and fellow of the European Optical Society
Murcia, Spain, where he founded his “Laboratorio de Optica.” in 2014.
Prof. Artal was secretary of the Spanish Optical Society from In 2013, he received the prestigious award “Edwin H. Land
1990 to 1994, associated dean of the University of Murcia Science Medal” for his scientific contributions to the advancement of
Faculty from 1994 to 2000, and director of the Physics Department diagnostic and correction alternatives in visual optics. This award
at Murcia University from 2001 to 2003. From 2004 to 2007 he was established by the OSA and the Society for Imaging Science
was in charge of the reviewing grants panel in physics at the Spanish and Technology to honor Edwin H. Land. This medal recog-
Ministry of Science in Madrid. Since 2006 he is the founding direc- nizes pioneering work empowered by scientific research to create
tor of the Center for Research in Optics and Nanophysics at Murcia inventions, technologies, and products. In 2014, he was awarded
University. He was president of the Academy of Science of the with a prestigious “Advanced Grant” of the European Research
Murcia Region from 2010 to 2015. From 2015 he is the president Council. In 2015, he received the “King Jaime I Award on New
of the “Fundación de Estudios Medicos,” an outreach organization Technologies” (applied research). This is one of the most presti-
dedicated to promote science. During his career he often spent peri- gious awards for researchers in all areas in Spain. It consists of a
ods doing collaborative research in laboratories in Europe, Australia, medal, mention, and 100000€ cash prize.
Latin America, and the United States. This included two sabbatical He is a coinventor of 22 international patents in the field of
years in Rochester (USA) and Sydney (Australia). optics and ophthalmology. Twelve of them extended to different
Dr. Artal’s research interests are centered on the optics of the countries and in some cases expanded to complete families of
eye and the retina and the development of optical and electronic patents covering the world. Several of his proposed solutions and
imaging techniques to be applied in vision, ophthalmology, and instruments are currently in use in the clinical practice. Dr. Artal
biomedicine. He has pioneered a number of highly innovative is the cofounder of three spin-off companies developing his
and significant advances in the methods for studying the optics concepts and ideas.
of the eye and has contributed substantially to our understand- He has been the mentor of many graduate and postdoctoral
ing of the factors that limit human visual resolution. In addition, students. His personal science blog is followed by readers, mostly
several of his results and ideas in the area of ophthalmic instru- graduate students and fellow researchers, from around the world.
mentation over the last years have been introduced in instruments He has been editor of the Journal of the Optical Society of America A
and devices currently in use in clinical ophthalmology. and the Journal of Vision.
Contributors
Andrew J. Anderson Michael A. Freed
Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences Department of Neuroscience
The University of Melbourne University of Pennsylvania
Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

David A. Atchison Walter D. Furlan


School of Optometry and Vision Science Diffractive Optics Group
Queensland University of Technology Universidad de Valencia
Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Valencia, Spain

Shrikant R. Bharadwaj Lyle S. Gray


Brien Holden Institute of Optometry and Vision Sciences Department of Life Sciences
Bausch and Lomb School of Optometry Glasgow Caledonian University
and Glasgow, United Kingdom
Hyderabad Eye Research Foundation
L V Prasad Eye Institute Arthur Ho
Hyderabad, India Brien Holden Vision Institute
Sydney, Australia
Juan M. Bueno
and
Laboratorio de Óptica
Universidad de Murcia Miller School of Medicine
Murcia, Spain University of Miami
Miami, Florida
W. Neil Charman
and
Division of Pharmacy and Optometry, Faculty of Biology,
Medicine and Health School of Optometry and Vision Science
University of Manchester University of New South Wales
Manchester, United Kingdom Sydney, Australia

Vivian Choh Hiroshi Horiguchi


School of Optometry and Vision Science Department of Ophthalmology
University of Waterloo Jikei University School of Medicine
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada Tokyo, Japan

Michael Collins Haomiao Jiang


School of Optometry and Vision Science Department of Psychology
Queensland University of Technology Stanford University
Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Stanford, California

Linda Lundström
Chris Dainty
Department of Applied Physics
Institute of Ophthalmology
KTH Royal Institute of Technology
University College London
Stockholm, Sweden
London, United Kingdom
Daniel Malacara
Carlos Dorronsoro Centro de Investigación en Optica
Instituto de Optica León, Mexico
Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas
Madrid, Spain Fabrice Manns
College of Engineering
Joyce E. Farrell and
Department of Electrical Engineering Bascom Palmer Eye Institute
Stanford University University of Miami
Stanford, California Coral Gables, Florida
xii Contributors

Susana Marcos Juan Tabernero


Instituto de Optica Laboratorio de Óptica
Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas Universidad de Murcia
Madrid, Spain Murcia, Spain

Yoshi Ohno Thomas J.T.P. van den Berg


Sensor Science Division Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience
National Institute of Standards and Technology Royal Academy
Gaithersburg, Maryland Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Jean-Marie Parel Stephen Vincent


Bascom Palmer Eye Institute School of Optometry and Vision Science
and Queensland University of Technology
College of Engineering Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
University of Miami
Miami, Florida Brian Vohnsen
School of Physics
Denis G. Pelli University College Dublin
Department of Psychology Dublin, Ireland
Center for Neural Science
New York University Sarah Walters
New York, New York Center for Visual Science
University of Rochester
Pablo Pérez-Merino Rochester, New York
Instituto de Optica
Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas Brian A. Wandell
Madrid, Spain Department of Electrical Engineering
and
Scott Read Psychology Department
School of Optometry and Vision Science Stanford University
Queensland University of Technology Stanford, California
Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Gerald Westheimer
Department of Molecular and Cell Biology
Robert Rosén
University of California, Berkeley
Applied Research
Berkeley, California
AMO Groningen BV
Groningen, the Netherlands
David R. Williams
Center for Visual Science
José Sasián
University of Rochester
College of Optical Sciences
Rochester, New York
University of Arizona
Tucson, Arizona
David A. Wilson
Brien Holden Vision Institute
Jim Schwiegerling and
College of Optical Sciences School of Optometry and Vision Science
University of Arizona University of New South Wales
Tucson, Arizona Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Jacob G. Sivak Jonathan Winawer


School of Optometry and Vision Science Department of Psychology
University of Waterloo Center for Neural Science
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada New York University
New York, New York
Joshua A. Solomon
Centre for Applied Vision Research Barry Winn
City University London Sohar University
London, United Kingdom Sohar, Sultanate of Oman
Part
I
Introduction
1 History of physiological optics
in the twentieth century
Gerald Westheimer

Contents
1.1 Status at the beginning of the century 3
1.2 The foundations 3
1.3 Structural optics of the eye 4
1.3.1 Eye dimension and axial length 4
1.3.2 Cornea 4
1.3.3 The crystalline lens 4
1.3.4 Transmission of the ocular media 5
1.3.5 Retinal optics 5
1.4 The retinal image 5
1.4.1 Aberrations of the eye 5
1.4.2 Quality of the retinal image 5
1.4.3 Optical transfer function 6
1.4.4 Strehl ratio 6
1.4.5 Stray light 6
1.5 Ophthalmic instrumentation 7
1.5.1 Ophthalmoscopy 7
1.5.2 Optometers and automatic objective refractometers 7
1.6 Spurt at the end of the twentieth century 7
References 8

1.1 STATUS AT THE BEGINNING The pioneering study by David Maurice (1957) on the cornea
was influential here.
OF THE CENTURY The final biological stage of light capture resides in the
Physiological Optics, as confirmed by its central manifestation, receptors. Starting with an observation by Ernst Brücke in the
Helmholtz’s three-volume handbook, was understood at the 1840s, there have been consistent attempts to assign to them
time to be synonymous with the current Vision Science. But special light-gathering properties. The directional sensitivity and
nomenclature has to go along with the explosive expansion wave guide nature of retinal receptors has been an active area
of scientific knowledge. Hence the more optical components now for the last 80 years. It may be noted parenthetically that the
are now subsumed under Visual Optics, and even here further optics of invertebrate eyes, left out of consideration in this review,
subdivision is needed. Optical imagery in the living eye is has deservedly been given much attention (Exner 1891, Snyder
continually conditioned on factors arising from being embedded and Menzel 1975, Land and Nilsson 2002).
in motor apparatuses, specifically those controlling the pupil
aperture and the ciliary muscle. Hence a division into structural
visual optics, relating to the image-forming properties of the
1.2 THE FOUNDATIONS
static normal eye, and functional visual optics, which would fold The eye’s image-forming properties were well understood in the
in accommodative and aperture size factors, seems indicated. middle of the eighteenth century as shown in the classic treatise
Though it is not recognized as a distinct discipline, one can by Robert Smith (1689–1768) (Smith 1738). Through the efforts
identify a branch of research as histological optics. Insofar as it of astronomers, for example, Bessel, Seidel, and Airy (1801–1892),
transmits light unimpeded, eye tissue, such as the cornea and optics as a discipline was thoroughly established in the nine-
the crystalline lens, needs to have unusual biological structure. teenth century. Maxwell’s (1831–1879) electromagnetic theory
This became more evident and constituted a challenge around took command of the subject in 1861 and has never needed
the middle of the century when electron microscopy began to superseding. The giants of physiological optics, Thomas Young
reveal the subcellular makeup of corneal and lenticular layers. (1773–1839), Jan Purkinje (1787–1869), Listing (1808–1882), and
4 History of physiological optics in the twentieth century

Helmholtz (1821–1894), laid and cemented the foundations, and so was the progress toward labs equipped with oscilloscopes,
ophthalmologists Donders (1818–1889), Landolt (1846–1926), photomultipliers, digital computers, transistor devices, in time
and Snellen (1834–1908) developed clinical applications. Thus, followed by lasers, LCD, and deformable mirrors. For decades,
a century ago, at the time of the beginning of the First World vision laboratories thrived on “war surplus” lenses, mirrors,
War, a student had available comprehensive compendia containing prisms, and filters. Light that was once generated by candles,
the available knowledge, specifically Helmholtz’s Physiological and had its intensity controlled by the inverse square law and
Optics in the new edition updated in particular by Gullstrand its duration by episcotister disks, was produced by lamps with
(1862–1930), the Graefe-Saemisch Handbuch der Augenheilkunde specific filaments and then by high-pressure mercury arcs with
in its many volumes and several editions, followed in the next wavelength range restricted by narrowband interference filters
couple of decades by important chapters in Vol XII of Bethe and intensity adjusted by neutral density wedges. To achieve
et al., Handbuch der normalen und pathologischen Physiologie high retinal illuminance, the filaments were imaged in the pupil
(1932) and in Abderhalden’s Handbuch (1920), and in Vol 1 in Maxwellian view well before its optical subtleties had been
of Duke-Elder’s Textbook of Ophthalmology (1932). When the realized (Westheimer 1966).
author entered optometry school in 1940, the assigned textbook,
the second edition of Emsley’s Visual Optics (Emsley 1939),
contained a treatment of the subject that would rival current
1.3 STRUCTURAL OPTICS OF THE EYE
accounts and in some respects exceed their scope. By that time,
1.3.1 EYE DIMENSION AND AXIAL LENGTH
too, quantum theory insofar as its characterization of the photon
was concerned had solid footing and quite soon thereafter gripped The ingenious method of measuring the eye’s axial length by
the vision community when used to underpin our understanding utilizing x-ray phosphenes (Goldmann and Hagen 1942) soon
of the absolute visual threshold (Hecht et al. 1942). The vision gave way to sonography that in the form of corneal pachometry
community has been well served by the authoritative treatment of (Molinari 1982) is in clinical use and now has become a reliable
the subject by Yves LeGrand (1908–1986) in various versions of means of evaluating the refractive needs associated with cataract
his textbook (LeGrand 1949), a model of clarity. extraction (Hoffer 1981).
Exhaustive literature surveys of vision science up to the
1.3.2 CORNEA
beginning of the twentieth century had been provided in the
encyclopedic scholarship displayed in the appendixes by A. Because it is the principal source of the eye’s refractive power
Koenig to the second edition of Helmholtz’s Physiological Optics and because, unlike the other refractive surfaces, it is immedi-
(almost 8000 references up to 1894) and F. Hofmann in his ately accessible, the cornea has always attracted much attention.
Graefe-Saemisch Handbuch chapters (almost 1500 references on Gullstrand in his appendix to the Third Edition of Helmholtz
spatial vision alone) and by A. Tschermak in the voluminous foot- went into much detail about the shape of the corneal surface and
notes of his chapters in Bethe et al. Handbuch (Tschermak 1931). the various means of measuring it. Keratoscopy and keratometry
As will be seen, developments in visual optics during the formed a strong chapter in Abderhalden’s Handbuch. As contact
second half of the twentieth century required an expanded view lenses became ubiquitous and their fitting needed good information
beyond geometrical optics and the simple application of diffrac- of the corneal surface on which they rested and whose optical
tion theory in Airy’s disk, yet the foundations for it were well properties they largely preempted, rapid and accurate measure-
in place. Abbe (1873) and Rayleigh (1896) in their treatment of ments of corneal curvature could be performed in the clinic
microscope resolution used a framework that contained, almost by cleverly designed electro-optical apparatuses, the subject of
explicitly, all the elements that were to become mainstream in the continued attention and technical innovation (Fowler and Dave
Fourier theory of optics that has since become dominant. Though 1994). Polarization effects, which can be made visible, have been
it was more of academic than practical interest at the time, the ascribed to the cornea (Stanworth and Naylor 1950).
theory applicable to coherent light (and what is more challeng-
1.3.3 THE CRYSTALLINE LENS
ing, partially coherent light) was put on the table by van Cittert
(1934), and so were the celebrated polynomials of Zernike (1934). The anterior and posterior surfaces of the crystalline lens are of
The upswing in the growth of optics, specifically as it plays a critical relevance in how the eye accommodates, that is, changes
role in vision, in the middle of the twentieth century thus had focus under neural control by contraction of the ciliary muscle.
their origin elsewhere. Most prominently, it was the harnessing E.F. Fincham (1937a), in part using optical means, made the
of scientific and industrial resources in the conduct of the Second major contribution to this topic in his monograph. It became
World War that ended in 1945. The scientific community virtually clear early in the optical modeling of the eye that the anterior
unanimously rallied behind the war effort, contributing insight and and posterior curvature of the lens could not fully account for its
inventiveness to a heady mixture that also included technological total refractive power for a biologically realistic refractive index,
innovation and industrial prowess. After the war, this continued and giving the eye a solid interior core was not supported by
Introduction

in university and some corporate laboratories, blossoming into a evidence. Hence the proposition that a remarkable proportion of
research enterprise of unprecedented magnitude and productivity. its total refractive power is provided by a refractive index gradient
The sequence into cybernetics (Wiener 1948), information had gained acceptance by the time Gullstrand wrote his 1911
theory (Shannon and Weaver 1949), and the linear systems appendix. More recent approaches show just how challenging a
approach (Trimmer 1950) and control theory was seamless and topic this is (Campbell 1984, Pierscionek and Chan 1989).
1.4 The retinal image 5

1.3.4 TRANSMISSION OF THE OCULAR MEDIA interest, therefore, is the attempt by Ingelstam (1956) to show
that the ultramicroscopic structure of receptors, which had just
For a variety of reasons, the transmissivity of the ocular media
been discovered, might allow wavelength-dependent differential
has been of interest, more recently because of concern for possible
energy concentration. Most of these conjectures were laid to
damage from exposure to intense sources. For decades the data
rest by Brindley and Rushton’s (1959) demonstration that to the
accepted as authoritative came from the study by Ludvigh and
human observer colors looked the same whether light entered the
McCarthy (1938). It formed the basis for the confirmation that
retina from the front or the back. The effect of the concentration
the energy exchange at absolute visual threshold involved only a
of photopigments on their absorption spectrum—called self-
handful of photons (Hecht et al. 1942). Wavelength dependency
screening—must, however, be considered in color vision theory
of light absorption in the media became an issue in the charac-
(Brindley 1960) and probably plays a role in the Stiles–Crawford
terization of retinal photopigments, one of the most important
effect of the second kind (Stiles 1939), color changes associated
research enterprises of vision science in the middle of the twentieth
with direction of incident light.
century (Rushton 1959, Wald 1964).

1.3.5 RETINAL OPTICS 1.4 THE RETINAL IMAGE


In the vertebrate, before it reaches the receptors, light has to
1.4.1 ABERRATIONS OF THE EYE
traverse several retinal layers that therefore have to be essentially
transparent, not necessarily a quality automatically associated with A theoretical approach to the monochromatic aberrations in
active neural tissue. Haidinger’s brushes, an entoptic phenomenon, an optical system requires adequate knowledge of the optical
have their origin in retinal optical structure (Naylor and Stanworth parameters, position and curvature of the surfaces, and refractive
1954). Myelination of the ganglion cell axons, helpful in enhancing index of the media. Because the precision needed to estimate
velocity of action potentials, does not start till they exit the eyeball image quality by ray tracing was lacking, this topic of visual
at the optic disk. The vascular tree of Purkinje, a prominent feature optics was largely unattended until it was, so to speak, turned
of the fundus, is somehow compensated for and made visible only upside down quite recently by nulling out the aberrations. There
by special tricks. The central region of the retina in the primate was a brief flurry of activity centered on spherical aberration,
is suffused by a pigment, selectively absorbing light of some wave- when it was fingered to account for night myopia (Koomen et al.
lengths. It seems to have a role in Haidinger’s brushes (Bone and 1951), of practical importance during the Second World War.
Landrun 1984, Mission 1993), but whether it is the origin of The enlarged pupil in the scotopic state allows light to enter into
the entoptic phenomenon known as Maxwell’s spot (Maxwell the eye through regions manifesting spherical aberration, but
1890/1965, p. 278) has been subject of an interesting debate the more likely explanation of accommodation activity in empty
(Polyak 1941, Walls and Mathews 1952). Of great significance fields (Otero and Aguilar 1951) won out. In a curious interlude,
are its possible protective properties (Snodderly 1995). a quite adequate experimental determination of the eye’s spherical
Optics becomes critical however, in the operation of receptor aberration (Ivanoff 1953) was marred by inclusion of a point
cells, whose diameter is of the order of the wavelength of light. derived from the wrong supposition that the eye was always
A start was made in the 1840s by Ernst Brücke, at the time focused precisely on the target plane. When this is corrected
Helmholtz’s fellow student in Johannes Müller’s Institute in (Westheimer 1955), outlines of spherical aberration across the
Berlin, who made the observation that rodlike retinal receptor pupil looked regular.
cells acted like light guides. He seems not to have published it; Because it needs to be factored into the stimulus situation
all we have is Helmholtz’s (1866) report and the comment that in color vision research, vision researchers throughout the
light once it had entered a receptor and impinged on the cylindri- twentieth century remained aware of the eye’s chromatic aberra-
cal boundary separating media of high from low refractive index tion (Hartridge 1918, Ames and Proctor 1921). Axial chromatic
would undergo total reflection and proceed further along the aberration (Wald and Griffin 1947) was mostly seen consonant
receptor and not leave it. with that of eye media with the dispersion of water. To obviate
Receptor optics became mainstream with the discovery of possible effects of chromatic aberration in color vision research,
the retinal directional sensitivity by Stiles and Crawford (1933) an “achromatizing lens” with the reverse of the eye’s chromatic
and the conjecture by Toraldo (1949) of retinal cones being wave aberration was designed (Bedford and Wyszecki 1957). During
guides. This set into motion extensive research activity, still ongoing. the same period, the role of lateral chromatic aberration of the
The state of the subject is well captured in the contributions to eye in engendering spurious stereoscopic disparity was given
Enoch and Tobey’s Vertebrate Receptor Optics (1981). due consideration (Vos 1960), but some inadequacies in the
Whereas rods and their rhodopsin photopigment had been explanation remain.
fully identified with scotopic vision, the same could not be said
1.4.2 QUALITY OF THE RETINAL IMAGE
about cones and the cone pigments till the 1960s. Before that,
Introduction

because there was no firsthand knowledge of the phototransduction Helmholtz was fully aware that the central issue and best descrip-
that underlies color vision, the possibility remained open that tor in the specification of the quality of the retinal image is the
there was only a single cone pigment and that wavelength analysis light distribution at a sharp target edge, though it took a little
came about through an intracellular filtering process, as indeed while for the realization that the point-spread function is even
is the case with oil drops in birds (Walls 1942). Of historical more basic. The most influential contributor at the beginning
6 History of physiological optics in the twentieth century

of the century was Hamilton Hartridge (1922), and it is hard to and therefore have their x-axes point in opposite directions: light
imagine anyone doing better at a time before the idea of direct spread over extensive regions tends toward infinite distances in
measurement took hold. Assuming that the shape of the point- the image plane but toward zero (the DC point) in the spatial-
spread function was Gaussian and making rather good guesses frequency spectrum. In principle, the diffraction image of a point
of the parameter, Fry and Cobb (1935) were able to achieve some source with a round pupil never stays at zero intensity, though
synthesis between image light spread and thresholds for simple its central lobe, the Airy disk, has a well-defined diameter. But
line targets. the spatial-frequency spectrum has a distinct cutoff point beyond
In retrospect, the direction of future development was clearly which there is no representation of grating targets. When this
foreshadowed by exceedingly insightful indirect approaches to became understood and increasingly popular from the middle of
retinal image quality by LeGrand (1935) using interference and the twentieth century on, the resolution limit of optical devices
Shlaer (1937) employing what is tantamount to Abbe’s theory was better appreciated and could be related to the electrical
of microscope resolution. But, at the start of my career in vision circuits to which they were increasingly being coupled. Yet the
science just after the Second World War, these were not adequately fundamental distinction always needs pointing out: on the one
understood or appreciated. hand, a firm cutoff spatial frequency in optical imagery and, on
In their place, the interest was in direct measurements, the other, the sloping transfer function, in principle never quite
first in an approachable if not particularly informative animal reaching zero transmission in electronics.
preparation, the excised steer eye, expeditiously brought from The eye’s actual optical transfer function exemplified by the
the abattoir to the lab, as Jay Enoch, one of the collaborators original one provided in the Campbell and Green study of 1965
explained to me (Boynton et al. 1954). Needless to say, light included the effect of aberrations and the pupil diameter, but still
spread was very extensive, making one the researchers wonder needed extension to include not only amplitude but also phase,
why, if the image is so bad, visual acuity is so good (DeMott discarded in the power spectrum. For many years, from the
1959). As the research during the remainder of the century, and seminal paper by Schade (1956) and the widely quoted data of
continuing to the present, has made clear, such a proposition was van Nes and Bouman (1967) on, the majority of interpretations of
ill-posed—the need instead was to pursue the question of how the modulation transfer function of the whole visual system failed
good, in the end, the retinal image might actually be, with all the to stress that it lacked phase information and hence did not allow
experimental prowess that can be marshaled. a unique description of light spread from the power spectrum. It
The most interesting and productive laboratory of the time was took almost a couple of decades from the first enunciation of the
at the Institut d’Optique in Paris where Arnulf and his students Fourier theory of vision till the explicit demonstration that phase
were in daily contact with the change in approach to the theory was more important than amplitude (Piotrovsky and Campbell
of optics that began with Duffieux’s (1946) paperback. To call the 1982). Yet it has been shown by Hopkins (1955) and Steel (1956)
turn to Fourier optics revolutionary would be an exaggeration, that defocus manifests itself prominently in the phase of the opti-
because it is implicit in the resolution formulations of Rayleigh cal transfer function.
and Abbe and Fourier’s Lehrsatz is explicitly used on p. 185 of
1.4.4 STREHL RATIO
Born (1933). In the single most significant paper in the subject
of retinal imagery of the twentieth century, Francoise Flamant Attempts at capturing image quality in a single number go back
(1955) used the principle of the ophthalmoscope to measure the to Strehl (1895), a high school teacher with an abiding interest
width of the reflected image of a narrow slit. Being familiar with in telescope design, who suggested the ratio of the height of the
the theorem that convolution becomes multiplication in the actual point-spread function at its center to that of the diffraction
Fourier domain, she undid the double convolution due to the image, generally the Airy disk, defined by the instrument’s
light traversing the eye media twice by taking the square root aperture and the wavelength of light. It was conservatively
of its Fourier transform. Needless to say, Flamant’s results were estimated at 0.2 (Gubisch 1967) in a good eye, but in practice
much closer to the human optics than those on the excised steer may be much less, because even a miniscule level of stray light
eye. In a sequence of more and more sophisticated experiments using (see below) at outlying image distances, covering as it does large
photomultiplier tubes in place of Flamant’s grainy photographic retinal areas, would be integrated in the constant volume of light
film (Westheimer and Campbell 1962, Campbell and Gubisch involved in computing the Strehl ratio. Areal summation of light
1966), objective data were accumulated on the optical image probably makes a low value of the Strehl ratio not as severe a
quality of the normal human eye that proved quite compatible visual handicap as it may appear.
with psychophysical ones employing the principle of interfer-
1.4.5 STRAY LIGHT
ence fringes (Westheimer 1960, Campbell and Green 1965).
Conjectures on hypothetical image sharpening mechanisms with Whereas the shape of the central lobe of the point-spread
their improbable information-theoretical basis could be discounted function is an important factor in visual acuity, its long tail plays
(Gubisch 1967). a role in a different visual phenomenon, glare. The veiling effect
Introduction

of bright sources in quite remote retinal areas can have deleterious


1.4.3 OPTICAL TRANSFER FUNCTION
influence on some visual tasks and early on in the twentieth
Nowhere is the complementarity of the traditional spatial and the century attempts were made to distinguish between optical and
modern spatial-frequency descriptions of imagery more evident physiological causes (Holladay 1927, Stiles and Crawford 1937).
than in counterposing the image light spread and the optical This required the estimation of the retinal illuminance distribution
transfer functions. They are Fourier transforms of each other caused by light scatter in the eye, which also, depending on the
1.6 Spurt at the end of the twentieth century 7

wavelength and the red reflectance of the fundus, could act as J.C. Maxwell, Lord Rayleigh, Airy, and others in the nineteenth
an integrating sphere. The visual system has been used as a null century and preserved in ophthalmological (Duke-Elder 1932)
detector to calibrate the threshold-raising effect of a uniform and optometric texts (Laurence 1926, Emsley 1939). Specifically
field of known luminance against that of distant outer zones of deserving of mention as upholding and furthering the tradition in
bright glare sources. This has yielded useful contributions to our the twentieth century are E.F. Fincham (1893–1963) and Arthur
knowledge of the quality of the retinal image (Fry and Alpern G. Bennett (1912–1994).
1953, Vos 1962). On the continent, where Kepler, Descartes, and Scheiner
had earlier clarified the image-forming properties of the eye, the
1.5 OPHTHALMIC INSTRUMENTATION work of the giants, Gauss and Listing (1808–1882), Purkinje,
and Helmholtz, soon percolated down to the clinic and then
1.5.1 OPHTHALMOSCOPY to productive collaboration with optical industry. Von Graefe
(1828–1870), Donders, Landolt, Snellen, and Gullstrand held on
The introduction of the ophthalmoscope by Helmholtz led and maintained the tradition, and so did, at least in the realm
to an unsurpassed blossoming in the diagnosis of eye diseases of scholarship, Moritz von Rohr (1868–1940) and Armin von
and, when the optical industry was ready at the turn of the Tschermak (1870–1952) in the next generation. Emblematic
twentieth century, to the development of high-performing of what followed is Max Born (1882–1970) and his magisterial
instruments. Successively versions were self-luminous, reflex-free, textbook Optik. Removed in 1933 from their native habitat, they
and stereoscopic. The Thorner design made by Busch was pitted reemerged in another, more welcoming language and environment
against the Gullstrand version made by Zeiss. and with immensely augmented success and influence. Principles
1.5.2 OPTOMETERS AND AUTOMATIC OBJECTIVE of Optics by Born and Wolf is now in its seventh edition.
REFRACTOMETERS Important laboratories in the middle of the twentieth
century were located in the Netherlands, sparked by Maarten
In order to clearly visualize the fundus in ophthalmoscopy, the Bouman (1919–2011), and at the Istituto Nazionale di Ottica
patient’s refractive error needs to be compensated. Schmidt- in Arcetri-Florence under the auspices of the Ronchi family.
Rimpler in 1877 used this phenomenon to obtain an objective In Paris at the Institut d’Optique, where Marechal and others
measure of the refractive error. Since then there have been many (Fleury et al. 1949) dug deeply into the fundamentals of image
versions of what are called optometers or refractometers or refrac- formation, diffraction, Fourier filtering, and apodization
tionometers. E.F. Fincham’s design of a Coincidence Optometer (Dossier 1954), Albert Arnulf (1898–1984) led a group
is perhaps the highlight of this trend (Fincham 1937b) early of investigators who in the 1950s were unmatched in the
in the century. They depended on an observer detecting either point of attack and skill in physiological optics experiments.
the sharpness of an image or, as in Fincham’s instrument, the Fergus Campbell once told me that whenever he started a
alignment of two lines each carried by a separate beam through a research project, he found that Arnulf had been there before.
different region of the eye’s pupil. Cambridge, England, had been the site of Thomas Young’s
The automatic recording infrared optometer of Campbell and major discovery. In the middle of the twentieth century, it
Robson (1959) put an end to an era when records of the eye’s saw an extraordinary blossoming of vision research and, as
accommodative changes were secured by cinematography of the host of innumerable students and visitors, predominantly
the Purkinje image from the anterior surface of the lens (Allen from the United States, had a lasting international impact.
1949) and the emphasis shifted to using light reflected from the Of the several centers in the United States, mention should
fundus. be made of the Dartmouth Eye Institute (Burian 1948) where
With the advent of modern optical and electronic components, collaboration with a research arm of the American Optical
automated objective refractometers became compact and Company resulted in the design of ophthalmic diagnostic
user-friendly and by the turn of the twenty-first century had and corrective devices. The work of scientists Paul Boeder and
established themselves firmly in the eye clinic. Documentation of Kenneth N. Ogle (1902–1968), later at the Mayo Clinic, helped
this development can be found elsewhere. give it an optical basis. In the same period, vision research
in general, and often physiological-optical in substance,
1.6 SPURT AT THE END OF THE was prominent at Columbia University, where Selig Hecht
(1882–1947) in Biophysics and C.H. Graham (1906–1971)
TWENTIETH CENTURY in Psychology operated well-supported laboratories and their
The narrative so far, covering developments in physiological optics many students spread a research culture characterized by up-
narrowly defined to include the optical properties of human to-date methodology and experimental rigor. The same applied
eye in the major portion of twentieth century, was informed by to Lorrin Riggs (1912–2008) at the Psychology Department of
the author’s personal experience: undergraduate training based Brown University and Glenn A. Fry (1908–1996) at the Ohio
Introduction

on the state of knowledge prior to the outbreak of the Second State University School of Optometry.
World War, his active involvement in the discipline over the rest The dramatic transformation that took place in the last third
of the century, and his personal acquaintance with all the major of the twentieth century had its origin less in any theoreti-
participants in the story. cal or conceptual changes than in the prodigious advances in
Much of the groundwork was laid in the British Isles by Smith the materials from which optical and electronic components
and Porterfield in the eighteenth century and by Thomas Young, are constructed: optical fibers, crystals, transistors, integrated
8 History of physiological optics in the twentieth century

circuits, CRTs, LEDs, LCDs, and the list goes on. Right from Campbell, F.W. and R.W. Gubisch. Optical quality of the human eye.
its inception a couple of centuries ago, the study of the eye’s J Physiol 186 (1966): 558–578.
image-forming properties has always been a prologue and Campbell, F.W. and J.G. Robson. High-speed infrared optometer. J Opt
necessary preliminary to vision as a perceptual process, it is Soc Am 49 (1959): 268–272.
Campbell, M.C. Measurement of refractive index in an intact crystalline
what would now be called the front end. Adherent of idealist or lens. Vision Res 24(1984): 409–415.
materialist philosophy alike, the student of physiological optics DeMott, D.W. Direct measures of the retinal image. J Opt Soc Am 49
was interested in the rules imposed by the laws of physics on (1959): 571–579.
what reaches the brain. Expressing electromagnetic disturbance Dossier, B. Recherches sur l’Apodisation des Images Optiques. Revue
distributions in Fourier—rather than position—space was not a d’Optique 33 (1954): 257–267.
revolutionary step, but generating such disturbance with lasers, Duffieux, P.M. L’ integrale de Fourier et ses applications a l’optique.
orders of magnitude higher in intensity and coherence than other Imprimeries Oberthur, Rennes, France, 1946.
light sources, was. Quantitative change in sufficient measure, Duke-Elder, W.S. Text-Book of Ophthalmology, Vol. I. London, U.K.:
Henry Kimpton, 1932.
so F. Engels interpreted G.F. Hegel, becomes a qualitative one.
Emsley, H.H. Visual Optics, 2nd edn. London, U.K.: Hatton Press, 1939.
When the intensity and coherence of light sources, the sensitivity Enoch, J.M. and F.L. Tobey. Vertebrate Photoreceptor Optics. Berlin,
of light detection, its temporal and spatial resolution, the storage Germany: Springer-Verlag, 1981.
capacity for the resultant signals, and the speed and power of Exner, S. Die Physiologie der Facettirten Augen. Leipzig, Germany:
analysis increase by a factor of 103, in some cases even 106, the Deuticke, 1891.
character of the whole enterprise changes. This has been the Fincham, E.F. The mechanism of accommodation. Br J Ophthalmol
case within a single generation for the entire armamentarium (1937a), Monograph Supplement VIII.
used by the vision scientists in his—now, of course, his or her— Fincham, E.F. The coincidence optometer. Proc Phys Soc 49 (1937b):
456–468.
laboratory. Unimagined ease of generating visual stimuli with
Flamant, F. Étude de la répartition de lumière dans l’image rétinienne
devices controlled by fast and powerful computers with virtually d’une fente. Revue d’Optique 34 (1955): 433–459.
unlimited memory has relegated to the historical dust heap the Fleury, P., A. Maréchal, and C. Anglade. (Eds.). La Théorie des Images
metal and wood—more recently even the electronic—shops of Optique, Colloques Internationaux du CNRS. Paris, France:
just 50 years ago. Flexible and versatile optical components made Éditions de la Revue d’Optique, 1949.
of novel materials are allowing experimental forays that Selig Fowler, C.W. and T.N. Dave. Review of past and present techniques of
Hecht, let alone Helmholtz or Lord Rayleigh, could not have measuring corneal topography. Ophthal Physiol Opt 14 (1994):
dreamt of. The evidence of this development is provided in the 49–58.
Fry, G.A. The image-forming mechanism of the eye. In Handbook of
following chapters, detailing stunning advances, yet built on the
Physiology, Vol. 1, J. Field (Ed.). Washington, DC: American
strong knowledge base erected over the course of many previous Physiological Society, 1959, pp. 647–670, XXVII.
generations and solidified by the twentieth-century scientific Fry, G.A. and M. Alpern. The effect of a peripheral glare source upon
work sketched here. the apparent brightness of an object. J Opt Soc Am A 43 (1953):
189–195.
REFERENCES Fry, G.A. and P.W. Cobb. A new method for determining the blurred-
ness of the retinal image. Trans Am Acad Ophthalmol Otolaryngol
Abbe, E. Beiträge zur Theorie des Mikroskops und der microskopischen (1935): 432–448.
Wahrnehmung. Arch Mikroskop Anatomie 9 (1873): 413–468. Goldmann, H. and R. Hagen. Zur direkten Messung der
Abderhalden, E. (Ed.). Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden. Totalbrechkraft des lebenden menschlichen Auges. Ophthalmol
Berlin, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 1920. Basel 104 (1942): 15–22.
Allen, M.J. An objective high speed photographic technique for simul- Goodman, J.W. Introduction to Fourier Optics. San Francisco, CA:
taneously recording changes in accommodation and convergence. McGraw-Hill, 1968.
Am J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom 26 (1949): 279–289. Gubisch, R. Optical performance of the human eye. J Opt Soc Am A 57
Ames, A. and C.A. Proctor. Dioptrics of the eye. J Opt Soc Am 5 (1921): (1967): 407–415.
22–84. Gullstrand, A. Appendix to Part I. In Handbuch der Physiologischen
Bedford, R.E. and G. Wyszecki. Axial chromatic aberration of the Optik, H.V. Helmholtz (Ed.). Hamburg, Germany: Voss, 1909.
human eye. J Opt Soc Am 47 (1957): 565. Hartridge, H. Chromatic aberration and the resolving power of the eye.
Brindley, G.S. Physiology of the Retina and Visual Pathway, 1st edn. J Physiol 52 (1918): 175–246.
London, U.K.: Edward Arnold, 1960. Hartridge, H. Visual acuity and the resolving power of the eye. J Physiol
Brindley, G.S. and W.A.H. Rushton. The colour of monochromatic light 57 (1922): 52–67.
when passed into the human retina from behind. J Physiol 147 Hecht, S., S. Shlaer, and M.H. Pirenne. Energy, quanta and vision.
(1959): 204–208. J Gen Physiol 25 (1942): 819–840.
Bone, R.A. and J.T. Landrum. Macular pigment in Henle fiber membranes: Helmholtz, H.V. Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik, 3rd edn.
A model for Haidinger’s brushes. Vision Res 24 (1984): 103–108. Hamburg, Germany: Voss, 1909.
Born, M. Optik. Berlin, Germany: J. Springer, 1933. Hoffer, K.J. Accuracy of ultrasound intraocular lens calculation. AMA
Introduction

Boynton, R.M., J.M. Enoch, and W.R. Bush. Physical measures of stray Arch Ophthalmol 99 (1981): 1819–1823.
light in excised eyes. J Opt Soc Am 44 (1954): 879–886. Hofmann, F.B. Die Lehre vom Raumsinn. In Handbuch der gesamten
Burian, H.M. The history of the Darmouth Eye Institute. AMA Arch Augenheilkunde, Vol. 1, A. Graefe and T. Saemisch (Eds.). Berlin,
Ophthalmol 40 (1948): 163–175. Germany: Julius Springer, 1920, pp. 1–213.
Campbell, F.W. and D.G. Green. Optical and retinal factors affecting Holladay, L.L. Action of a light source in the field of view in lowering
visual resolution. J Physiol 181 (1965): 576–593. visibility. J Opt Soc Am 14 (1927): 1–15.
References 9

Hopkins, H.H. The frequency response of a defocused optical system. Southall, J.P.C. (Ed.). Helmholtz’s Treatise on Physiological Optics.
Proc R Soc (Lond) A 231 (1955): 91–103. New York: Dover, 1924/1962.
Ingelstam, E. Possible interpretation of the ultrastructure of retinal Stanworth, A. and E.J. Naylor. The polarization optics of the isolated
receptors including an electromagnetic theory of the primary cornea. Br J Ophthalmol 34 (1950): 201–211.
mechanism of colour vision. In Problems in Contemporary Optics. Steel, W.H. The defocused image of sinusoidal gratings. Opt Acta 3
Proceedings of the Florence Meeting, September 10–15, 1954. (1956): 49–55.
Firenze, Italy: Istituto Nazionale di Ottica, 1956, pp. 640–667. Stiles, W.S. The directional sensitivity of the retina and the spectral
Ivanoff, A. Les Aberrations de L’Oeil. Paris, France: Revue d’Optique, 1953. sensitivities of the rods and cones. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 127
Koomen, M., R. Scolnik, and R. Tousey. A study of night myopia. J Opt (1939): 64–105.
Soc Am 41 (1951): 80–83. Stiles, W.S. and B.H. Crawford. The luminous efficiency of rays entering
Land, M.F. and D.E. Nilsson. Animal Eyes. New York: Oxford the eye pupil at different points. Proc R Soc (Lond) B 112 (1933):
University Press, 2002. 428–450.
Laurence, L. Visual Optics and Sight Testing, 3rd edn. London, U.K.: Stiles, W.S. and B.H. Crawford. The effect of light glaring source on
School of Optics, 1926. extrafoveal vision. Proc R Soc (Lond) B 122 (1937): 255–280.
Le Grand, Y. Sur la mesure de l’acuité visuelle au moyen de franges Strehl, K. Aplanatische und fehlerhaft Abbildung im Fernrohr.
d’interférence. Compte rendus de l’Académie des Sciences 200 Zeitschrift für Instrumentenkunde 15 (1895): 362–370.
(1935): 490–491. Toraldo di Francia, G. Retina cones as dielectric antennas. J Opt Soc Am
Le Grand, Y. Optique Physiologique. Paris, France: Editions de la revue 39 (1949): 324.
d’optique, 1949. Trimmer, J.D. The Response of Physical Systems. New York: John Wiley,
Ludvigh, E. and E.F. McCarthy. Absorption of visible light by the refractive 1950.
media of the human eye. AMA Arch Ophthalmol 20 (1938): 37–51. Tschermak, A. Optischer Raumsinn. In Handbuch der normalen und
Maurice, D.M. The structure and transparency of the cornea. J Physiol pathologischen Physiologie, A. Bethe et al. (Eds.). Berlin, Germany:
136 (1957): 263–286. Julius Springer, 1931.
Maxwell, J.C. The Scientific Papers of James Clerk Maxwell. New York: van Cittert, P.H. Die Wahrscheinliche Schwingungsverteilung in einer
Dover, 1890/1965, Vol. II. von einer Lichtquelle direkt oder mittels einer Linse beleuchteten
Misson, G.P. The form and behaviour of Haidinger’s brushes. Ophthal Ebene. Physica 1(1934): 201–210.
Physiol Opt 13 (1993): 392–396. van Nes, F.L. and M.A. Bouman. Spatial modulation transfer in the
Molinari, J.F. A review of pachometry. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 59 human eye. J Opt Soc Am 57 (1967): 401–406.
(1982): 912–917. Vos, J.J. Some aspects of color stereoscopy. J Opt Soc Am 50 (1960):
Naylor, E.J. and A. Stanworth. The measurement of the Haidinger 785–790.
effect. J Physiol 123 (1954): 30-1P. Vos, J.J. On Mechanisms of Glare. Utrecht, the Netherlands: University
Otero, J.M. and M. Aguilar. Accommodation and night myopia. J Opt of Utrecht, 1962.
Soc Am 41 (1951): 1061–1962. Wald, G. The receptors of human color vision. Science 145 (1964):
Pierscionek, B.K. and D.Y. Chan. Refractive index gradient of human 1007–1016.
lenses. Optom Vis Sci 66 (1989): 822–829. Wald, G. and D.R. Griffin. The change in refractive power of the human
Piotrowski, L.N. and F.W. Campbell. A demonstration of the visual eye in dim and bright light. J Opt Soc Am 37 (1947): 321–336.
importance and flexibility of spatial-frequency amplitude and Walls, G.L. The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Detroit, MI:
phase. Perception 11 (1982): 337–346. Cranbrook Institute, 1942.
Polyak, S.L. The Retina. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 1941. Walls, G.L. and R.W. Mathews. New means of studying color blindness
Purkinje, J. Beobachtungen und Versuche zur Physiologie der Sinne. and normal foveal color vision. Univ Calif Publ Psychol 7 (1952):
Prague, Czech Republic: Calve, 1823. 1–172.
Rayleigh, L. On the theory of optical images, with special reference to Westheimer, G. Spherical aberration of the eye. Opt Acta 2 (1955): 151.
the microscope. Philos Mag 42 (1896): 167–195. Westheimer, G. Modulation thresholds for sinusoidal light distributions
Rushton, W.A.H. Visual pigments in man and animals and their rela- on the retina. J Physiol Lond 152 (1960): 67–74.
tion to seeing. Prog Biophys Biophys Chem 9 (1959): 240–283. Westheimer, G. and F.W. Campbell. Light distribution in the image
Schade, O.H. Optical and photoelectric analog of the eye. J Opt Soc Am formed by the living human eye. J Opt Soc Am 52 (1962):
46 (1956): 721–739. 1040–1045.
Shannon, C.E. and W. Weaver. The Mathematical Theory of Westheimer, G. The Maxwellian view. Vision Res 6 (1966): 669–682.
Communication. Urbana, IL: U. Illinois Press, 1949. Westheimer, G. Optical properties of vertebrate eyes. In Handbook of
Shlaer, S. The relation between visual acuity and illumination. J Gen Sensory Physiology, Vol. VII/2, M.G.F. Fuortes (Eds.). Berlin,
Physiol 21 (1937): 167–188. Germany: Springer-Verlag, 1972, pp. 449–482.
Snodderly, D.M. Evidence for protection against age-related macular Wiener, N. Cybernetics: Or, Control and Communication in the Animal
degeneration by carotenoids and antioxidant vitamins. Am J Clin and the Machine. Cambridge, MA: Technology Press, 1948.
Nutr 62 (1995): 1448S–1461S. Zernike, F. Beugungstheorie des Schneidenverfahrens und Seiner
Snyder, A.W. and R. Menzel. (Eds.). Photoreceptor Optics. New York: Verbesserten Form, der Phasenkontrastmethode. Physica 1 (1934):
Springer-Verlag, 1975. 689–704.
Introduction
2 Possibilities in physiological optics

David R. Williams and Sarah Walters

Contents
2.1 Introduction 11
2.2 Disruptive technologies for refracting the eye 11
2.3 A transformational technology for vision correction 13
2.4 Can elucidating the fundamental mechanism of emmetropization help us prevent refractive error? 14
2.5 Virtual and augmented reality: A renaissance for applied visual psychophysics 14
2.6 How good can the ophthalmoscope get? 15
2.7 Outstanding issues about how the retina catches photons 16
2.8 Can optical technology help us disentangle the neural circuitry of the retina? 17
2.9 Can optical technology accelerate the next generation of cures for blindness? 18
References 19

2.1 INTRODUCTION the biggest impact. We feel that the chapter will have succeeded
if a single independent thinker concludes that our crystal ball
This chapter speculates about future directions of physiological is murky and is inspired to innovate in an entirely different and
optics, identifying a few of the grand challenges that we think productive direction.
might offer the richest rewards, though they may also rank
among the most difficult to achieve. The cliché that prediction 2.2 DISRUPTIVE TECHNOLOGIES FOR
is hard, especially when it is about the future, bears repeating
here. Nonetheless, we have decided to plunge ahead, driven by
REFRACTING THE EYE
the conviction that short-term planning for the next experiment Over the last 400 years, the evolution of technology to measure
always benefits from a longer-term vision for the larger scientific and correct the eye’s optical defects has sprung as much from
and technological goals our field could eventually realize. experts in other fields, especially optics and astronomy, as it
Optimized investment decisions in science and engineering has from within. Examples abound. In the early seventeenth
require a risk–benefit analysis, whether undertaken by those who century, Johannes Kepler (1604), the astronomer best known
provide resources or those who use them. Deciding what criteria for describing the laws of planetary motion, arguably provided
to deploy to define benefit is by itself controversial, ranging from the first clear articulation of the fact that the retinal image
the value of advancing basic science to improving eye care to is inverted. Shortly afterward, Christoph Scheiner (1619), an
realizing commercial success. We have tended to favor challenges astronomer well known for his bitter dispute with Galileo about
here where fundamental advances in vision science are most likely the discovery of sunspots, was the first to measure the eye’s plane
to result in eye care improvements. Risk is equally difficult to of focus. He used a simple subjective method, now referred to as
calibrate. At the time of this writing, the pendulum has swung in Scheiner’s disc, that consisted of two laterally displaced pinholes
the direction of risk aversion, at least in many Western countries, in an opaque plate held close to the pupil. Light passing through
with increasing resource competition in science and engineering the holes formed a double image on the retina for objects at
demanding increasingly compelling evidence that each new distances nearer or further than the focal plane. Only for objects
endeavor will succeed. One of the motivations for writing this lying at the focal plane did these double images merge into one,
chapter is to remind ourselves that there are many potentially uniquely identifying the focal plane. Scheiner’s disc was arguably
transformative benefits from investing our energy and resources the first wavefront sensor ever deployed in the human eye, the fact
in physiological optics, if only we are willing to accept the risks that it could characterize only the single, lowest-order Zernike
required to secure them. While the list of challenges we have mode notwithstanding. Examples from the nineteenth century
selected is idiosyncratic, we hope that it will be useful, especially of the dominant role played by astronomers in vision correc-
to young scientists who are pondering where they might have tion include Sir John Herschel’s concept for the first contact
12 Possibilities in physiological optics

lens to correct the optical defects of the cornea (Herschel 1845). refraction, the ubiquitous procedure in the optometry clinic.
Thomas Young (1801) first described the astigmatism of the A purely automatic refraction could greatly accelerate patient
eye, though he was much better known for his contributions to flow through the clinic and likely achieve a more reliable refrac-
the wave theory of light, among other things. The migration of tion than the slow traditional method that requires the patient’s
technology from optics and astronomy to vision continues today, response. Evidence for this comes from the fact that a wavefront
the best example being the application of the Hartmann–Shack sensor combined with a deformable mirror in an adaptive optics
wavefront sensor to measure the eye’s wave aberration, which was retinal imaging system can track the optical defects of the eye
borrowed from the optical metrology of astronomical telescopes automatically in real time with sufficient accuracy to resolve the
(Liang et al. 1994). There is no reason we can think of to suppose smallest rods and cones in the living human eye. The tolerances
that this historical trend will diminish, and it seems likely that required for this level of resolution are considerably tighter than
advances in optics, astronomy, and increasingly microscopy will those required to refract the eye to within a quarter or even an
continue to fuel future generations of ophthalmic technology. eighth of a diopter, essentially because we can now build cameras
The explosive development of computing power over the last with considerably higher resolution than a patient’s visual system.
50 years has already transformed ophthalmic instrumentation, Indeed, wavefront aberrometry has already demonstrated reliable
and we speculate that its impact is destined to accelerate. refractive measurement and has been used to greatly enhance
A telling example comes from Smirnov’s landmark paper on understanding of clinically challenging conditions, ultimately
the aberrations of the human eye in 1961, published before the leading to expanded treatment options (Applegate et al. 2014).
potential of computation was clear (Smirnov 1961). Smirnov A full adaptive optics phoropter (Jaeken et al. 2014), though
described a subjective method, similar to but much more expensive, could offer even more functionality, its closed-loop
sophisticated than Scheiner’s, to characterize not only the eye’s control could provide a more accurate correction than an open-
defocus but also astigmatism and higher-order aberrations. loop wavefront sensor, and it would offer the opportunity for the
Smirnov did not believe that his approach would ever find patient to view various corrections prior to delivery of spectacles,
practical application, stating the following: contact lenses, or some other modality for vision correction.
The most common argument against abandoning the subjective
The method applied in the present work of determining the refraction is that, unlike the objective autorefraction, it engages
wave aberration is quite laborious; although the measurements the patient’s nervous system in the final judgment of optimum
can be taken in 1–2 hours, the calculations take 10–12 hours… image quality. There is no question that the optical correction that
Therefore, it is unlikely that such detailed measurements will ever optimizes subjective image quality is not necessarily the same as
be adopted by practitioner-ophthalmologists. that which delivers the best objective optical quality (Chen et al.
2005), and that the neural visual system is adapted to the par-
Smirnov could not have foreseen the digital revolution that ticular pattern of aberrations with which it has prior experience
ultimately made possible automated computation of the wave aber- (Artal et al. 2004). This is the basis for the standard, time-honored
ration in a small fraction of a second (Hofer et al. 2001). The mar- practice of undercorrecting astigmatism. The patient present-
riage of optical metrology with modern computational methods ing with a large amount of astigmatism is likely to reject, at least
presaged the widespread use of Hartmann–Shack and related initially, a complete correction of astigmatism because the brain’s
wavefront sensing methods in the eye. These methods provided understanding of what the world should look like has been learned
a much more complete description of the eye’s monochromatic with that aberration present. On the other hand, the mystery sur-
aberrations than was possible before. Moreover, the wave aberra- rounding the brain’s contribution to the best refraction is rapidly
tion specifies how light passing through each and every point in falling away, to the point where its contribution could probably
the pupil must be advanced or delayed to achieve perfect imaging, be faithfully incorporated in an automatic refraction. There is
thereby providing a map in the pupil plane that indicates where mounting evidence that the brain is plastic in the face of changes
modifications are required to improve vision correction technology. in the wave aberration (Mon-Williams et al. 1998; Sabesan and
The introduction of accurate wavefront sensing methods raised the Yoon 2010), and some reason to suspect that providing the best
possibility of achieving supernormal vision through the correction optical correction will, provided adequate time is allowed for
of higher-order aberrations. Enthusiasm for this idea has waned, adaptation, eventually provide the best visual performance. It
at least as applied to the normal population who typically have seems likely, and would also be open to empirical validation, that
relatively minor loss in image quality due to aberrations beyond the the optimum subjective refraction lies on a line in aberration space
defocus and astigmatism. Moreover, technologies for vision correc- between the native wave aberration and the best correction in
tion that are commercially available are sufficiently imprecise that purely optical terms. In that case, a single scalar multiplier could
higher-order aberration correction is difficult to achieve in any case. be selected by the clinician to set the appropriate balance between
Nonetheless, for some patients, such as those with keratoconus or the two. Admittedly, there are additional, second-order factors
penetrating keratoplasty, the correction of higher-order aberrations that can influence the optimal refraction as well. For example,
Introduction

remains a very exciting challenge. Yoon et al. have demonstrated the eye’s Stiles–Crawford effect, which apodizes the generalized
the ability to do this with contact lenses, but the technology is not pupil function, has a minor influence on refraction (Atchison
yet available for the typical patient (Sabesan et al. 2007, 2013). et al. 1998), but this could also be incorporated into an automated
Despite the plethora of devices now available to autorefract the computation of the optimal refraction.
eye, the wavefront sensor being just one especially sophisticated It seems possible that the refraction could be achieved
example, none have replaced the subjective phoropter-based objectively with a very simple camera, perhaps even a smartphone
2.3 A transformational technology for vision correction 13

camera, equipped with the appropriate computational algorithm. They have shown that a femtosecond laser can produce local
Methods to refract the eye with a cell phone already exist, using changes in refractive index in living cornea at exposures that are
the subjective Hartmann–Shack approach (Pamplona et al. 2010). below those that produce scattering changes caused by tissue dam-
Others have obtained an objective refraction using the light age. This approach, known as laser-induced refractive index change
reflected from the retina in a manner consistent with eccentric (LIRIC), has the potential to be a much less invasive treatment for
photorefraction. However, the accuracy is poor to date, and it refractive errors, since there is no flap-cutting step in which focused
is typically used only as a screening tool (Arnold and Armitage femtosecond laser pulses are applied above the damage threshold
2014). In principle, it should be possible to compute the lower- in corneal tissue. Interestingly, the range of exposures that modify
order wave aberration (sphere, cylinder, and axis) simply from the the refractive index had never been discovered before, presumably
evolving intensity distribution of light returning from the retina, because changes in refractive index unaccompanied by changes in
without the need for a Hartmann–Shack wavefront sensor. transparency are not typically seen with casual inspection. LIRIC
The inertia of the optometry profession, which long ago follows conceptually from earlier efforts to adjust refractive state
established adequate albeit tedious methods to refract the eye, not by light modification of the refractive index of an intraocular lens
to mention the risk that optometrists might be disenfranchised by (Sandstedt et al. 2006), but with the added advantage of modifying
automation, may actually pose a larger hurdle than the technical the native collagen of the anterior optics of the eye.
challenges that remain. Indeed, a sufficiently sophisticated To date, the refractive index change produced with a single
autorefractor could provide a refraction without the need for exposure regimen in the living cat eye has remained stable for
human intervention at all, at kiosks in grocery stores and possibly upward of 12 months (Savage et al. 2014). It remains to be seen
even at home. We speculate that the time will come when how large a refractive correction can be produced with this method
completely automated refraction will be routine, and perhaps even relative to the range of refractive errors in the human population,
coupled to a 3-D printer that could fabricate personalized vision though it is estimated that ±3.00 D is feasible (Krueger et al. 2013).
correction technology on the spot. Although initial in situ studies were performed in cat cornea, histo-
logical studies have indicated LIRIC could be used for noninvasive
refractive correction in human corneal tissue. Should this method
2.3 A TRANSFORMATIONAL be translated to safe use in humans, it could transform how vision
TECHNOLOGY FOR VISION is corrected, obviating the need for tissue-removing refractive sur-
gery, contact lenses, or spectacles in many cases.
CORRECTION This approach does not directly address presbyopia, the loss of
The history of the major technologies for vision correction has accommodation with age. A variety of creative approaches have
marched inexorably toward approaches that are increasingly been developed to combat this problem, including multifocal
proximal to the eye itself. The first approach was a crude lens lenses (which could also be realized with LIRIC), variable focus
held before the eye, followed by spectacles fixed to the head, then spectacles, and corneal inlays to name a few (Charman 2014a),
followed by contact lenses resting on the surface of the cornea. and it seems certain that innovations in this space will continue.
Next came refractive surgery in which the cornea itself is modified. Although these approaches certainly make the inevitable loss
Here we will not review the myriad alternative vision correction of accommodation manageable, the true restoration of accom-
technologies that are under development today but instead briefly modative ability would be preferable. So far, however, this has
describe an approach that we think, though it is in an early proven resistant to successful implementation (Charman 2014b).
stage of development, shows unusual promise because it could Many of the currently available surgical approaches to correcting
correct vision but leave the patient in as close to the native state presbyopia require the lens to be removed, which is typically only
as possible. In refractive surgery, the patient’s refractive error is necessitated in the case of a cataractous lens. One such example
corrected in a one-time procedure that does not require the patient is that of capsular refilling, in which the dysfunctional crystal-
to wear an additional optical device. But despite the impressive line lens is removed and the capsular bag is filled with a material
improvements that have been made in refractive surgery, especially that returns deformability (Nishi et al. 2009; Hirnschall and
since the early days of radial keratotomy, even the best refractive Findl 2012). IOLs that accommodate are a propitious concept,
surgical techniques today carry some risk because the corneal though there is a history of overpromising functionality in this
epithelium must be debrided, as in PRK, or a corneal flap cut as area (Findl and Leydolt 2007). Nevertheless, advances are being
in LASIK. In either case, vision correction requires the removal of made to restore accommodative function to the native lens
corneal tissue with an excimer laser. The application of wavefront noninvasively. Myers and Krueger (1998) first proposed using a
sensing to refractive surgery made it possible to discover where in femtosecond laser to restore some deformability to the lens, and
the pupil plane refractive surgery had failed to correct the wavefront, this method is now under development (Lubatschowski et al.
providing the critical feedback for continuous improvement in 2010; Krueger et al. 2013). Regardless of the approach taken in
patient outcomes (Krueger et al. 2004). Perhaps the most impor- attempts to restore accommodation, none have achieved a full
Introduction

tant contribution of wavefront sensing in refractive surgery has restoration of accommodative ability to date, and this will likely
been not the correction of higher-order aberrations in the patient, require further innovation. One could imagine that regenera-
but correction of iatrogenic aberrations induced by the procedure, tion of the crystalline lens to a more youthful state may one
especially spherical aberration (Yoon et al. 2005). day be possible through the use of stem cells (Charman 2014b).
It may eventually be possible to correct the eye’s optics without Efforts to correct presbyopia would certainly benefit from a more
the need to debride, cut, or ablate the cornea (Savage et al. 2014). complete understanding of the mechanisms leading to loss of
14 Possibilities in physiological optics

accommodation, and in fact, may pave the way to realizing a pharmacological intervention, perhaps in the form of eye
therapy that prevents presbyopia altogether. drops, could someday mitigate the development of refractive
error. In fact, the idea of a pharmacological solution to myopia
2.4 CAN ELUCIDATING THE was proposed as early as the nineteenth century (Saw et al.
2002). Topically administered atropine appears to show some
FUNDAMENTAL MECHANISM success in inhibiting the progression of axial eye growth, but
OF EMMETROPIZATION HELP US the underlying cause of this effect remains elusive (Tong et al.
2014). The molecular pathways underlying emmetropization have
PREVENT REFRACTIVE ERROR? a genetic basis that could provide a route to identifying them
If we had the capacity to prevent refractive errors before they (Wojciechowski 2011; Zhang 2015). If and when an effective
occur, it could ultimately obviate the need for more cumbersome pharmacological agent is identified, we speculate that it is highly
and/or invasive corrective solutions such as those discussed earlier. likely that a fundamental understanding of the mechanisms of
Over the last three decades, a number of elegant experiments emmetropization, established with basic scientific research using
using animal models have greatly clarified the role of visual animal models, will have been of critical importance in guiding
experience on the development of refractive error (Wallman the search.
and Winawer 2004; Smith et al. 2014; Chakraborty and Pardue Perhaps the circuit that provides the error signal for eye
2015). It has been shown that viewing the world through positive growth, once discovered, could eventually be controlled through
or negative lenses slows or accelerates eye growth respectively in the genetic expression of a channelrhodopsin combined with
a number of species including the primate, firmly establishing a spectacles with a spectral transmittance designed to increase or
feedback loop that controls eye growth based on visual experience decrease the rate of eye growth as required. On the other hand,
(Hung et al.1995). We also know from experiments in which someone may eventually devise an effective environmental
the optic nerve is sectioned that a control loop resides in the eye solution. There has been some excitement about the discovery
that does not require input from extraretinal stages of the visual that myopia is reduced by outdoor activity and exposure to
system (Wildsoet and Wallman 1995; Wildsoet 2003). This sunlight during development (Rose et al. 2008; Wang et al.
control loop is even more localized since different regions of the 2015). But so far, despite the fact that nearly 2.5 billion humans
retina that experience different refractive states grow at different are myopic (Asbell 2016) and despite everything we have learned
rates (Wallman and Adams 1987; Wallman and Winawer 2004; about the factors that control emmetropization, a robust lifestyle
Smith et al. 2010). This machinery must contain a sensor that regimen for avoiding refractive error has proven frustratingly
generates an error signal indicating the sign of defocus in the difficult to identify. A solution based on prevention that is less
retinal image, and then this signal must somehow modulate local cumbersome than the methods currently available for correction
eye growth. could be transformative.
Neither of these components of the control loop has
been thoroughly characterized, though tantalizing clues are 2.5 VIRTUAL AND AUGMENTED
beginning to appear (Zhong et al. 2004; Ho et al. 2012). Given
the remarkable diversity of cell classes in the mammalian retina REALITY: A RENAISSANCE
(Dacey 1999; Masland 2012), it seems likely that there is a neural FOR APPLIED VISUAL
circuit specialized to sense the sign of defocus in the retinal
image. A conceivable way this could be achieved is through
PSYCHOPHYSICS
measurement of the contrast of the retinal image simultaneously The remarkable developments over the last 50 years in compu-
in two axially distinct focal planes. The difference between the tational power and digital connectivity are now transforming
signals from these two planes could provide an opponent error communication so rapidly that their impact seems destined to be
signal sufficient to guide eye growth. Whether evolution has profound. Just the ability to copy text more efficiently than could
actually generated such a circuit is anyone’s guess, but it may not a scribe, enabled by Gutenberg’s invention of moveable type in
be so far-fetched given that the retina has at least two different the mid-1400s, is often credited with fomenting the Renaissance
planes, axially displaced, for absorbing light, albeit for a differ- and the Scientific Revolution (Man 2002). Our new found ability
ent purpose. Intrinsically photosensitive ganglion cells (Berson to move enormous amounts of information around the planet
et al. 2002) in the inner retina respond directly to the incoming essentially instantly, not to mention the burgeoning capacity
light, controlling pupil diameter and the body’s circadian clock. with which computers can make intelligent decisions about that
Ho et al. (2012) reported a differential response in the human information, will surely have an impact at least as significant as
multifocal ERG to the sign of defocus, though this method moveable type. Whereas the telephone of just a few years ago
does not reveal the cellular basis for the response. Diedrich and delivered information only via voice, today’s smartphone now
Schaeffel (2009) failed to find a subset of cells sensitive to defocus makes accessible a substantial fraction of the cumulative wisdom
Introduction

recording from chick retinal ganglion cells with a multielectrode (and folly) of the human species with a remarkably small number
array, but the signal must exist somewhere in the retina, waiting of key strokes, delivering information through both auditory and
to be discovered. visual sensory modalities. We suggest that we are now at the same
This discovery would not only be a major scientific advance, point in delivering digital information to the eyes that we were
it might also point the way to new methods to prevent refractive a half a millennium ago in delivering focused images to them.
error from developing in childhood. It is conceivable that At that time, before the advent of spectacles, there was limited
2.6 How good can the ophthalmoscope get? 15

availability of handheld lenses to correct vision. The convenience for distinguishing motions of the retinal image generated by
of methods to fix these lenses in place with respect to the eyes, self-motion and motion in the external world. When head
either by pinching them onto the nose or with the hinged movements, for example, are sensed by a virtual reality system
extensions that wrap around the ears we now use, predated the and then used to displace the virtual field of view accordingly,
widespread adoption of spectacles (Rubin 1986). The smartphone, small errors in gain and timing can be very disruptive. The virtual
for all its technical sophistication, is the modern-day equivalent field of view can depart from a natural view in many other ways.
of the handheld lens, an awkward technology from an ergonomic For example, motion parallax and binocular disparity cues must
perspective that requires at least one and often both hands to use. be captured faithfully, as well as distance-dependent blur (Held
A recent survey by Verizon and KRC Research stated that smart- et al. 2010, 2012). Ideally, the virtual reality device would create
phone users drop their device, on average, upward of once per a light field that is physically indistinguishable from that arriving
week (Burrows 2016), and another survey determined that 26% at the eye from the full 3-D scene. Holograms have this prop-
of iPhone owners had damaged the screen (SquareTrade 2014). erty but can pose problems with speckle. Approaches with roots
Freeing the hands by attaching the technology to the head is the dating back to the integral photography methods of Lippmann
next logical step just as it was for spectacles. (1908) may be more promising (Okano et al. 1999). In the case of
The heads up displays developed by the military beginning augmented reality technology, the need to merge digital informa-
in the mid-twentieth century were among the first attempts to tion with a clear view of the world raises a number of issues that
augment reality with supplementary analog and then digital visual psychophysicists are especially well equipped to answer.
information. The Google Glass is a recent commercial innovation For example, issues of visual performance as a function of retinal
in this vein, and while its rollout has been controversial (Efrati eccentricity, visual attention, and visual crowding are all critical
2013; Rosenberger 2014), it seems inevitable to us that future to making the best engineering decisions about how to integrate
designs will become ubiquitous, especially as improvements in digital information without compromising performance in the
voice recognition also help to free up the hands for other tasks. real world. Of course, a premium will be placed on designs that
The growing commercial interest in augmented reality technolo- are unobtrusive, lightweight, and compact, while also providing
gies parallels that in virtual reality systems, where visual informa- high resolution over a large field of view. Conventional rotationally
tion about the local environment is completely replaced by that symmetric optics constrain such parameters unduly and the best
of a remote environment or one generated by computer. Although designs will likely abandon that constraint in favor of free-form
virtual reality systems have enormous commercial potential in optic designs (Bauer and Rolland 2014).
the gaming industry, applications for the technology span a wide Just as methods to correct vision have historically migrated
gamut of industries that is ever growing as commercial systems toward closer proximity to the eye, it is natural to ponder
improve. As one could imagine, virtual reality offers a unique whether contact lenses might someday deliver digital imagery
ability to simulate emergency situations for training purposes, to the retina. If such a contact lens or IOL could replicate the
whether that be for respondents to fires or natural disasters, or wavefront of the light that emanated from a visual scene, it would
medical personnel treating injuries in the emergency room or be possible to create a retinal image without the need for external
on the battlefield (Brownstone 2014). In the medical sphere, a projection optics to deliver the image to the retina. The technical
number of virtual reality simulations have been developed for challenges of achieving this are indeed formidable and a problem
practice of medical procedures (Ruthenbeck and Reynolds 2015). exacerbated by the fact that an eye tracker would be required
On the complete opposite end of the spectrum, corporations in to enable the observer to scan the scene through natural eye
the design and manufacturing space are making use of virtual movements.
reality to assist engineers with product development and testing
(Reis 2014). Other applications are as diverse as providing chil- 2.6 HOW GOOD CAN THE
dren with practice crossing the street safely (Schwebel et al. 2016)
and assisting those who are autistic with immersion in a variety of
OPHTHALMOSCOPE GET?
social situations (Lorenzo et al. 2016). The last quarter century has witnessed striking improvements in
While the study of human factors of vision has had a long our ability to visualize the interior of the living eye, especially
and valuable history of informing the design of everything from through increases in resolution. Perhaps the most successful of
television to road signs to aircraft cockpits, the commercial these innovations has been optical coherence tomography (OCT),
push for augmented and virtual reality is now reinvigorating which can image both the anterior and posterior segment of
the practical relevance of the sometimes arcane field of visual the eye with an axial resolution as small as 3 microns (Fercher
psychophysics. The goal of designing both virtual and augmented et al. 2003; Drexler and Fujimoto 2015; Huang et al. 2015).
reality systems is to deliver imagery to the eye as seamlessly as it Prior to OCT, we had essentially no axial imagery of the eye so
would appear in native viewing. Failure to achieve this can have, that its introduction provided access to an entirely new spatial
and historically often has had, significant negative consequences dimension in which diseases of the eye could be observed. The
Introduction

for the user. Visual fatigue and even nausea continue to pose use of adaptive optics to correct the eye’s aberrations, reviewed
formidable problems for the design of such technology. The in Williams 2011 and Roorda and Duncan 2015, provides a
visual system maintains an exquisite calibration between sources transverse resolution in two spatial dimensions of roughly 2 µm,
of information about its own eye and head movements, such as exceeding that of conventional ophthalmoscopy including OCT.
the vestibular system, and the consequences they produce for Since then, a host of additional imaging modalities have been
the retinal image. This calibration is critical in normal viewing combined successfully with adaptive optics such as fluorescence
16 Possibilities in physiological optics

imaging, first of the single-photon variety, and subsequently optics–based measures, largely focused on the photoreceptors to
two-photon imaging. Fluorescence imaging offers an exciting date, are already being deployed in clinical trials (Roorda and
opportunity to not only image structure but also track functional Duncan 2015). No doubt the number of papers in that literature
activity of retinal neurons. In particular, two-photon imaging will continue to grow that provide new adaptive optics–based
allows for monitoring of molecular species that are excited in the descriptions of the impact of specific retinal diseases, especially
ultraviolet, previously inaccessible with one-photon methods due with the incorporation of different imaging modalities and func-
to the transmission window of the ocular media. Recently, the tions such as tracking and microstimulation.
demonstration of split detection and other nonconfocal imaging
methodologies have shown considerable promise, enabling
visualization of structures and cell classes that have until now 2.7 OUTSTANDING ISSUES ABOUT
been elusive in confocal imaging because of the transparency of HOW THE RETINA CATCHES
the retina (Elsner et al. 2000; Chui et al. 2012; Scoles et al. 2014;
Sulai et al. 2014; Guevara-Torres et al. 2015).
PHOTONS
Since the fundamental work of Abbe (1873), the diffraction Understanding how the optics of photoreceptors guide photons
limit has long been presumed to pose a fundamental barrier to to the photopigment has been a revered problem among the most
the resolution of optical instruments. Overcoming the diffraction dedicated disciples of physiological optics ever since the discov-
limit could allow imaging features at even smaller spatial scales ery of the Stiles–Crawford effect (Stiles and Crawford 1933).
in retinal imaging. The field of microscopy has recently wit- For cones outside the foveal center, light entering near the pupil
nessed a remarkable transformation in which not one but several center can be absorbed with nearly an order of magnitude more
new methods have emerged to overcome the diffraction limit, efficiency than light entering through the pupil margin. This sub-
including structured illumination (Gustafsson 2000), stimulated jective phenomenon, along with its objective correlates observed
emission depletion (Hell and Wichmann 1994), stochastic optical when light exits the pupil after two transits through the receptors
reconstruction microscopy (Rust et al. 2006), and photoactivated (Marcos and Burns 1999), reflects the waveguide properties of
localization microscopy (Betzig et al. 2006). None of these receptors. This waveguiding improves the retinal image in at least
methods has yet been applied successfully to retinal imaging, two ways. First, by funneling light entering the photoreceptor
though efforts have begun (Shroff et al. 2009, 2010). aperture at the lip of the inner segment into the smaller diameter
Though the first closed-loop adaptive optics systems for outer segment, it increases the photon yield and reduces photon
imaging the eye were demonstrated nearly 20 years ago (Liang noise in the retinal image (Packer and Williams 2003). Without
et al. 1997), their widespread use has been slow to develop. To this funneling effect, receptors would need to support more
date, their cost, their complexity, and the time required for photopigment molecules to sustain the same photon capture rate
postprocessing the images they generate have tended to make they presently enjoy. Not only would there be metabolic costs
them better suited for the research laboratory than the clinic. of this, but also the more photopigment molecules the receptors
The quality of the images they produce has systematically and need to maintain, the higher the receptor noise due to random
significantly improved since their introduction and methods to thermal isomerization, which also presumably encourages optical
accelerate image processing have been developed (Yang et al. concentration of light onto a smaller reservoir of photopigment.
2014), but hurdles remain. For example, a natural clinical Second, waveguiding, along with other strategies such as absorb-
application of the adaptive optics scanning light ophthalmoscope ing uncaught photons via the rich concentration of melanin in
(AOSLO) would be to count the fraction of individual receptors the RPE, saves the precious photopigment molecules for the
missing as a quantitative measure of the severity of a retinal imaging-forming photons that arrive directly from the pupil
degeneration or the efficacy of therapy in rescuing receptors. But, while effectively rejecting photons scattered by the sclera or the
as pointed out by Roorda and Duncan (2015), the quantitative retina that would otherwise reduce image contrast.
interpretation of such images is complicated by fact that the Perhaps the most interesting mystery remaining in the realm
failure to image cells at any particular location does not guarantee of photoreceptor optics is how the receptors manage to orient
that they are absent. Other factors within either the system itself themselves toward the pupil, as several studies have shown they
or the eye can often obscure cells, confounding an accurate count. are equipped to do (Bonds and MacLeod 1978; Applegate and
Tracking of eye motion to produce a stabilized retinal image Bonds 1981; Smallman et al. 2001). Smallman et al. (2001)
shortens image acquisition time and improves quality, especially showed that a shift in the entry point of light in the pupil caused
in patients whose retinal degeneration precludes them from by cataract surgery was followed over the next several days by a
achieving good fixation (Ferguson et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2014; systematic shift in the Stiles–Crawford maximum toward the
Sheehy et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2015). Stabilization enables postsurgical pupil. Neither the error signal that captures the
testing the response of single photoreceptors to a visual stimulus initial misalignment nor the motor that corrects for the misalign-
(Harmening et al. 2014) and holds great promise for studying ment has been discovered. Speculation about both error signal
Introduction

retinal disease as well as spatially localized visual psychophysics and motor has largely focused on the receptors themselves (e.g.,
(Bruce et al. 2015). Regardless, in vivo adaptive optics ophthal- Laties and Burnside 1979). On the other hand, it is unclear how
moscopy has given us a new microscopic view of a host of retinal a photoreceptor would directly sense its own misalignment.
diseases (Carroll et al. 2013) that was only rarely accessible Variations in the distribution of intensity within the receptor
before from the postmortem histology of retinas fortuitously might be a cue, perhaps created by changes in the modes propa-
obtained but often compromised by an artifact. In fact, adaptive gating within the waveguide, and the motor might involve the
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
“Gosh!” laughed Russ, as he and the others started on a run for
the place whence the voices sounded. “I guess Vi would ask
questions if the house was on fire.”
“Sounds as if Margy and Mun Bun had fallen into the brook,” said
Rose.
And that’s just what had happened. The three older Bunkers came
upon Violet, Margy and Mun Bun a few seconds later. It was at a
place where a small plank was laid across the brook as a bridge.
Standing in the water on one side of the plank was Mun Bun. In
the water up to her knees on the other side of the plank, was Margy.
Both children were in the middle of the brook, and Violet was on one
shore.
“I guess Mun Bun’s feet are wet, and Margy’s, too!” chuckled
Russ. “What’s the matter, Vi?” he asked. “What happened?”
“Oh, these children started to cross the little bridge, and Margy
wanted to go first and Mun Bun wanted to go first, and they pushed
and shoved and———”
“Which one went into the water first?” asked Rose, with a laugh,
for, after all, the accident was not a bad one.
“I fell in first!” cried Mun Bun, as if this was something to be proud
of.
“No, I did!” declared Margy.
“Well, you’d both better come out,” advised Rose. “You’ll have to
go up to the house and get on dry shoes and stockings.”
“I’m going to ask mother if I can’t go barefoot,” said Mun Bun.
“So’m I,” declared Margy.
Their mother let them go barefoot after scolding them a little for
getting their shoes and stockings wet. She said they should have
been more polite and not have tried one to get ahead of the other in
crossing the plank.
“Well, I guess you’ll have to expect such things as wet feet and
muddy clothes if the children play about the farm,” said Farmer
Joel’s sister, who was getting ready to go off on her vacation.
“Oh, I don’t mind as long as the children aren’t hurt,” said their
mother, with a laugh. “They’ll get used to the place after a while and
know how to have fun without getting into too much trouble. Don’t go
far away now,” she added. “Supper will soon be ready.”
“I’ll stay and help set the table,” offered Rose. And as Miss Todd
would be busy with her own affairs and as Norah had the cooking to
look after, Mrs. Bunker was glad of Rose’s help.
Russ and Laddie went back to where they had been building the
water wheel when Rose was frightened by the dog, and Violet, Mun
Bun and Margy said they would go with Adam North, who started out
to the barn to gather the eggs.
“Where do the hens lay their eggs?” asked Violet, starting some of
her usual questions.
“Oh, in different places,” answered the hired man. “Sometimes
away under the barn, and I have to crawl under the beams to get
them out.”
“We could do that for you,” offered Violet. “We’re small and we
could easy fit under the barn.”
“Yes, I do have trouble there,” replied Adam. “Once I got stuck
under the barn floor.”
“Did you have to stay there a long time?” Violet asked.
“I did until they could take up some boards in the floor and let me
crawl out that way,” laughed Adam.
Violet watched him go about in different places in the barn to
gather the eggs. She saw Margy and Mun Bun climbing about in the
haymow, and then she forgot about her little brother and sister for a
few moments, as Adam found a nest with more than a dozen eggs in
it and called Violet to look at them.
When she returned to the middle of the barn she could not see
either Mun Bun or Margy.
“Where are you?” she called.
Back came the answer, but in queer, muffled voices.
“We’re in the hay,” roared Mun Bun.
“And we can’t get out and it’s dark!” wailed Margy.
“What has happened to them?” Violet asked Adam North.
CHAPTER IX
WHEN THE COWS CAME HOME

The hired man carefully set down the basket of eggs he had
gathered from different places in the barn. Then he looked up toward
the haymow. This mow was where the hay was piled in the barn to
be kept dry so it could be fed to the horses.
“Were Margy and Mun Bun up there?” asked Adam of Violet.
“Yes, they went up there to slide down. Hay’s slippery, you know,”
answered Violet. “Course it isn’t as slippery as snow or ice, but you
can slide down hill on a pile of hay.”
“I know,” chuckled Adam. “I often used to do it when I was a boy
on the farm. But I don’t see the children now.”
“You can hear them—listen!” advised Violet.
Again came the voices of Mun Bun and Margy.
“We’re in the dark! We’re in the dark!” wailed Margy, who did not
like dark places.
“An’ the hay tickles me, it does!” howled Mun Bun. “I don’t like the
hay to tickle me! Vi! Vi! Come and get me!”
Violet climbed up a little ladder that led from the floor of the barn to
the top of the haymow. The ladder went all the way to the roof of the
barn, for in winter the haymow was piled that full. But now there was
only a little hay in the mow. It rose a few feet over the head of Adam
as he stood on the barn floor, and Violet did not have to climb up
many rungs of the ladder to see over the top of the pile of hay.
“They aren’t here!” she called down to Adam. “I can’t see Margy or
Mun Bun anywhere, but I can hear them. And I hear a hen cackling.”
“I guess a hen has her nest up there,” said the hired man.
“Maybe the hen bit Margy and Mun Bun,” suggested Violet.
“I shouldn’t wonder but what she might peck at ’em if they tried to
move her off her nest,” chuckled the hired man. “But she couldn’t
hurt ’em much. Let me get up there, Violet. I think I can find Margy
and Mun Bun.”
Violet climbed up higher on the ladder until she could step off upon
the soft, springing pile of hay. Adam North followed her, and then,
going to one corner of the mow, the hired man called:
“Here they are! I’ve found ’em!”
“Where were they?” asked Vi. “Were they hiding?”
“Well, sort of,” answered Adam, with a smile, as he reached down
in the hay and lifted up first Margy and then Mun Bun. “But I guess
they didn’t hide on purpose. They slipped down into the feed chute.”
“What’s that?” asked Vi.
“It’s the place where we push hay down to the horses in their
stalls,” explained the hired man. “If you don’t know the feed chutes
are here it’s easy to slip in ’em and fall down to the stalls.”
“Oh, would you get killed?” asked Violet, with widely opened eyes.
“No,” answered Adam. “All that would happen would be that you’d
fall into the horse manger, and if the horse was there you might
scare it a bit. But there aren’t any horses in the barn just now.”
Mun Bun and Margy, both of whom had been crying, now stopped,
and Violet looked at the place where they had been lost in the hay.
At the rear of the mow were several long wooden places, like
chimneys, made of smooth boards. Down these “chimneys,” or
chutes, hay could be pushed, dropping into the mangers of the
horses stabled below.
Margy and Mun Bun had been running and sliding about on the
pile of hay and, without knowing it, had come too near the feed
chute. Into it they both slipped at the same time, carrying with them
some wads of the dried grass.
As both children slid into the upright chute at the same time, they
became wedged fast, together with some hay, and this stopped them
from sliding all the way down to the manger. And there they had
remained, caught fast, until Adam pulled them out.
“Are you hurt?” asked the hired man, as he helped the little ones
down the ladder.
“No,” answered Margy. “But it was awful dark!”
“And the hay tickled the back of my neck,” added Mun Bun. “I
sneezed.”
“And when he sneezed he made me bump my nose and I—now, I
cried,” confessed Margy.
“Well, you’re all right now,” said Violet consolingly. “And maybe
you can find some eggs.”
“Oh, I’d like to find eggs!” exclaimed Margy, quickly drying her
tears.
“So would I,” added her brother, rubbing his eyes with his fists.
“All right, come on!” said Adam North. “I haven’t gathered all the
eggs yet—not half, I guess.”
So the children had a good time looking for the nests in the
different places the hens had hidden them. A hen, you know, likes to
“steal her nest,” as the farmers call it. That is, she likes to sneak
away in some quiet place and lay her eggs. Each day, or every other
day, she will lay an egg in the same place. And, if the nest is not
found for a week or more, sometimes there may be a dozen eggs in
it, for often two or more hens may lay eggs in the same nest, taking
turns.
And, when there are a dozen, or perhaps thirteen, eggs in the
nest, some hen will begin to “set” on them—hovering over them for
three weeks until little chickens hatch out of the eggs. The warm
body and feathers on the mother hen bring the little chickens to life
inside the egg, and with their beaks they pick open the shell and
come out.
It is because a hen does not like to be disturbed when she is
hatching out her eggs that she steals away to make her nest in as
quiet and as dark a place as she can find. But farmers who raise
eggs to sell do not always want them hatched out into chickens, so
that is why it is needful to hunt for these hidden nests to take away
the eggs.
“There’s a nest away back in there,” said Adam, who had looked
under a low part of the barn. “I see some eggs, but I can’t reach
them.”
“Let me crawl in an’ get ’em!” begged Mun Bun.
“Yes, I guess you’ll have to. I’m too big to get under there,” said
the hired man.
“I want to get half the eggs,” said Margy.
But it was decided that it would be best for Mun Bun only to crawl
under the low place in the barn, and soon he was wiggling and
crawling his way there, toward the hen’s nest.
“If the old hen is on won’t she pick him?” asked Violet.
“There’s no hen on. If there had been I should have seen her,”
Adam North answered. “Mun Bun will be all right if he doesn’t get
stuck fast under the barn as I once was.”
But nothing like this happened, and Mun Bun brought out four
eggs, one at a time, from the hidden nest. He was a proud little boy
when he crawled out with the last egg, not having broken one.
“I like egg-hunting,” he said, with a laugh.
Back to Farmer Joel’s house went Margy, Mun Bun, and Violet
with Adam, who was carrying the eggs. Every one laughed when
they all heard how Margy and Mun Bun had been stuck in the feed
chute.
It was now almost supper time, and Mother Bunker told the
children to wash and get ready for the meal. Mr. Todd’s sister was
going to leave on her journey soon after supper.
The meal was a merry one, for Farmer Joel was jolly and made a
lot of jokes. He even started Laddie’s trick of asking riddles, and he
asked many funny ones—riddles to which there was no answer.
Then, after supper, Farmer Joel drove his sister over to the
railroad station, where she was to take a train to visit some relatives
in the West.
The six little Bunkers were so tired after their day of travel and
their afternoon of fun on the farm that they went to bed early. There
was plenty of room in Farmer Joel’s house.
Sleeping in strange beds did not keep the children awake, and
they were soon sound asleep. Mrs. Bunker lay awake, however,
making plans for the next day, and she was somewhat surprised
when, after she had been in bed an hour, she saw a ghostly white
little figure coming into her room.
“Who is it? What do you want?” she asked.
“I’ve got to find the eggs!” murmured the voice of Margy. “I’ve got
to crawl under and get the eggs!”
For a moment Mrs. Bunker did not know what to think as she saw
Margy get down on her hands and knees and begin to crawl under
the bed. Then, as Mrs. Bunker picked up her little daughter, she saw
that Margy’s eyes were staring in a strange fashion.
“She’s walking and talking in her sleep!” she exclaimed to Daddy
Bunker. “Wake up, Margy! Wake up!” she called, giving Margy a
gentle shake.
“What’s the matter? Is it morning?” asked Margy, in a sleepy voice,
and then she blinked her eyes and looked around in surprise. “Oh!”
she exclaimed. “What’s the matter?”
“You were thinking so hard about hunting eggs that you got up in
your sleep and began to search for some under my bed,” said Mrs.
Bunker gently, as she carried Margy back to her own room. “Go to
sleep now.”
Margy did. Nothing else happened that night, and the children
were up bright and early the next morning. The day was filled with
fun. Russ and Laddie finished their water wheel, about which I shall
tell you more later.
Rose, after helping her mother, went down to the brook to gather
watercress for her father, Farmer Joel having told her where to find
some, and Margy, Violet and Mun Bun had a little picnic by
themselves under the trees in the orchard.
It was toward the close of the afternoon that the barking of a dog
was heard in front of the farmhouse. The six little Bunkers were in
the back yard having some bread and jam that Norah had brought
out to them.
“Maybe that is Ralph come to take us after the cows!” cried Russ.
So it proved, but when all six little Bunkers wanted to go to the
distant pasture to help Ralph gather up his herd, Mrs. Bunker said:
“It’s too far for Margy and Mun Bun. But you four may go if you
wish.” She knew where the cow pasture was.
Mun Bun and Margy began to cry, as they wanted to go also, but
Farmer Joel said they could go egg-hunting with Adam, and this
pleased the smaller children so that smiles drove away their tears.
The path to the cow pasture lay through pleasant fields, and half
way to the place was a clear, sparkling spring of water at which the
children stopped for a drink.
Then they climbed a hill, went down in a little valley, and as they
reached a broad field, Ralph said:
“Here’s where we pasture our cows. But I don’t see all of them—
the two black ones are missing.” There were ten cows in the pasture
where there should have been twelve.
“Do you think anybody stole those two cows?” asked Russ.
“Oh, no,” answered Ralph. “I guess they just wandered away.
They do, sometimes.”
“What do you have to do?” Violet wanted to know.
“Have to hunt ’em,” Ralph answered. “Jimsie helps me. There are
lots of places where cows can hide in this pasture—lots of low
places, and bushes and trees. Sometimes it takes me an hour to find
the lost cows.”
“Why don’t you yell for ’em?” asked Laddie.
“I will,” said Ralph. “Co, boss! Co, boss! Co, boss!” he called
loudly, the hills echoing his voice.
Then the other children called:
“Co, boss! Co, boss! Co, boss!”
But the missing cows did not come out of the cool, shady places
where, doubtless, they had gone to keep out of the sun.
“We’ll have to scatter and look for ’em,” said Ralph. He and his
dog Jimsie went one way, Rose and Russ went another way, and
Laddie and Violet a third way. Soon the three searching parties were
some distance apart.
Then, suddenly, from a part of the pasture where there was a
dense clump of bushes, came shrieks from Violet.
“Oh, we’ve found the cows! We’ve found them, but they’re going to
hook us!” she yelled. “Russ! Russ! The cows are going to hook us!”
CHAPTER X
BUZZING BEES

Russ and Rose, who had been walking along the shores of a little
brook looking for the missing cows, heard Violet’s yells. A moment
later they heard shouts from Laddie. He was saying:
“Get back there, you old cows! Get back there! Don’t you dare
hook my sister!”
Then Violet’s voice sounded again:
“Oh, but Laddie, they are going to hook me! Oh! Oh!”
“Come on!” called Russ to his sister Rose, and together they
rushed up out of the little glen where the stream ran and hastened
toward the clump of trees and bushes whence came the voices of
Laddie and Violet. Ralph and his dog were not in sight.
“Do you suppose the cows are trying to hook Laddie and Violet
with their horns?” asked Rose.
“I don’t think cows would,” panted Russ as he ran on followed by
Rose, who could not go quite so fast. “Cows don’t hook you, I guess,
but bulls do, though I didn’t hear Ralph say there were any bulls in
this pasture.”
“Is a bull worse than a cow?” Rose asked.
“For hooking you a bull is the worst there is,” Russ answered. “But
I don’t suppose it’s a bull. Maybe the cows are only shaking their
heads at Violet and she thinks they’re trying to hook her.”
And this is just what had happened. Laddie and Violet had found
the lost cows. The two black animals were standing peacefully in a
shady place, chewing their cud. Perhaps they were day-dreaming, if
cows ever do such things. At any rate the cows paid no attention to
the “co, boss” called by the children.
Laddie had fairly stumbled upon the hiding place of the cows, and
as Russ and Rose reached the place they saw Laddie and his twin
sister standing with their backs against a big tree, as Rose had stood
when Jimsie barked at her.
In front of Laddie and Violet were the two cows, chewing their cud,
as I have said. But as Russ looked he could see no signs that the
cows were going to “hook” Violet, as she had shouted they were
about to do.
However, just as Russ and Rose reached the place one of the
cows shook her head violently and Vi screamed:
“There! Look! The old thing wants to hook me! Oh, Russ! Oh,
Rose! Laddie! Why don’t you do something!”
“Don’t be silly!” exclaimed Russ, who had little patience with Violet
sometimes. “She isn’t going to hook you!”
“But what makes her shake her head?” demanded Violet, half
crying.
“She’s doing it to shake off the flies that are biting her,” answered
Russ, for he observed that when the cow shook her head a cloud of
flies rose from behind her ears. “She’s only doing it to get rid of the
flies, Vi,” said Russ.
“That’s what I told her, but she wouldn’t believe me,” remarked
Laddie. “I said the cows wouldn’t hook her.”
“Well, they looked as if they were going to hook me, anyhow,” said
Violet, who was not frightened now that her older brother and sister
were there with her.
“I’m glad we found the cows, anyhow,” said Rose. “Now we can
drive ’em out with the others and we can call Ralph and his dog and
go home.”
The two black cows that had wandered away from the rest of the
herd seemed gentle enough when the children urged them out of the
shady bushes and into the open pasture. The other ten cows were
gathered down near the pasture bars, waiting for them to be opened.
Ralph and Jimsie came slowly up the hill from another part of the
pasture, where they had gone to search for the missing animals.
“Oh, you found them! That’s good!” cried the farm boy, as he saw
Rose and Laddie with Violet and Russ slowly driving the black cows.
“You were pretty lucky,” he added. “Sometimes I’ve hunted an hour
for lost cows.”
“I guess Vi thinks she’s lucky they didn’t hook her,” said Russ, with
a laugh.
“What do you mean—hook her?” inquired Ralph.
THE TWO COWS FRIGHTENED VIOLET AND LADDIE.
Six Little Bunkers at Farmer Joel’s. (Page 97)

And when they told him he laughed and said:


“Our cows never hook anybody—they’re very gentle. But we have
a bull in the barn that’d hook you if he could get out. And Mr. Todd’s
got a bull, too.”
“Why can’t he get out?” Violet wanted to know.
“Because he’s chained fast to a ring in his nose,” answered Ralph.
“He dassn’t pull too hard on the chain ’cause it hurts his nose. So he
has to be good. But if he got loose he’d hook you all right.”
“He couldn’t hook me! I’d throw stones at him,” boasted Laddie.
“You’d better not try it if he ever does get loose,” warned Ralph.
“He wouldn’t mind stones any more than if you chucked soft mud at
him. He’s awful strong.”
“Well, if I saw him coming I’d run,” went on Laddie.
“That wouldn’t do much good,” said Ralph. “That bull can run
faster than you. If you ever do see him and he’s loose, keep away
from him or get on the other side of the fence as fast as you can.
Once he nearly hooked me, but I got to the fence first. He ran right
into the fence with his head down and he bellowed like thunder.”
“Did it hurt him when he bunked into the fence?” asked Vi.
“I guess maybe he didn’t feel it any more than he’d feel a mosquito
bite,” Ralph replied. “He’s tough, our bull is.”
“Goodness! I hope he never gets out,” murmured Rose, looking
over her shoulder as if she feared, even then, the bull might be
roaming somewhere about the pasture.
But he was not in sight and soon the children were quietly driving
the cows along the road toward their barn on the farm of Ralph’s
father. In the barn the cows would be milked and some of the milk
would be sent to the cheese factory.
“Well, did you have a good time?” asked Mrs. Bunker, when her
four children arrived at Farmer Joel’s house after having gone for the
cows.
“Yes, it was fun. We had a little adventure,” said Rose, and she
told about the missing cows.
Margy and Mun Bun listened with widely opened eyes to the tale,
and when it was over, Mun Bun exclaimed:
“I wish I’d been there!”
“Why?” asked his mother.
“Oh, I would give the cows some salt and they would love me,” he
answered.
“Salt!” cried Russ. “Who ever heard of giving cows salt?”
“It would make their milk salty!” declared Laddie.
“Well, it didn’t,” said Margy. “’Cause when we went after eggs with
Farmer Joel he gave his cows some salt and when he milked them
he gave Mun Bun and me some of the milk and it wasn’t salty at all,
so there!”
“Wasn’t it, Mother?” asked Rose, who seemed to share Laddie’s
idea.
“No, of course not, child,” said Mrs. Bunker. “The farmers often
give salt to their cows, sheep and horses. Animals are very fond of a
small bit of salt. And while you were gone Farmer Joel gave his cows
some lumps of rock salt which they licked with their tongues, and
seemed very fond of.”
“Hum!” remarked Laddie. “That’s the first time I ever knew cows
liked salt.” But later when he saw how horses in the pasture followed
Adam North about when he went to “salt” them, and when the little
boy watched the sheep eagerly licking the salt in their field, then he
knew that his mother was right.
Happy days at Farmer Joel’s followed one after another. The six
little Bunkers never had such delightful times. There seemed to be
something new to do all the while. They roamed about the fields and
woods, they gathered eggs, they fed the chickens, and sometimes
they had picnics. They waded in the brook and, once or twice, fell in
and got muddy. But this was expected.
One place that the children stayed away from was the part of the
farm where Mr. Todd kept several hives of bees. The children knew
that bees stung and they did not want this to happen to them.
About a week after the Bunkers had come to stay at Farmer
Joel’s, Russ and Laddie were going to the brook to play with their
water wheel when suddenly they heard a loud buzzing, humming
sound in the air. At first they thought it was a distant aeroplane, but,
looking up, they could see none. However, over in the direction of
the bee orchard Russ saw a dark cloud in the air. The buzzing sound
seemed to come from this dark cloud.
Then Russ knew what it was—a flight of bees.
“Oh, they’re running away!” he cried. “We must tell Farmer Joel!”
He and Laddie hastened toward the house and told the news. Mr.
Todd ran out. As soon as he heard the buzzing sound and saw the
moving dark cloud he cried:
“They’re swarming! I don’t want to lose them! I must try to get
them back!” Into the house he hurried, to come out with a queer,
smoking machine in his hand. Over his head Farmer Joel wore a
broad-brimmed straw hat with a veil of mosquito netting coming
down over his shoulders.
CHAPTER XI
MUN BUN’S GARDEN

The six little Bunkers, never having been at Farmer Joel’s before
and not knowing much about bees, did not understand just what was
going to happen. In a general way the Bunker children knew that
bees made honey, but how they did it, how the insects lived in hives,
with a queen bee who ruled over her subjects almost like a real
queen—of all this the six little Bunkers knew nothing.
“What’s that thing he’s got on his head?” asked Violet, pointing to
the mosquito netting veil that was draped over Farmer Joel’s hat.
“And what’s that tin funnel full of smoke he carries?” For the machine
in the farmer’s hand was like a kitchen funnel, turned on one side,
and from the small end poured a cloud of white smoke.
“I’m going to try to get back that swarm of bees,” called Mr. Todd
as he hurried out toward the trees under which were many hives of
the honey-making insects. “That queen alone is worth fifty dollars. If
she gets away it will be a bad loss for me.”
Away he hurried, followed by a cloud of smoke, and Rose asked:
“How in the world is he going to pick out a queen bee from the
million or more that must be in the swarm?”
“I don’t know,” answered Russ.
“Let’s go out and see how he does it,” proposed Laddie, always
ready to do something. “Maybe I could think of a riddle about bees if
I went out there.”
“Most likely you’d be thinking about their stings if you went out
there,” laughed Mr. Bunker. “You children stay here where you can
watch Farmer Joel, and I’ll tell you what he is doing and how he can,
perhaps, get back his fifty dollar queen, and I’ll tell you a little about
how bees make honey.”
By this time Farmer Joel was out among his bees. The dark cloud
of the swarming hive was right over his head, moving slowly along
like some great bubble—only it was a bubble full of life. In the middle
of the swarm was the queen bee and all her court was following,
going wherever she went.
“How is he going to catch them?” asked Russ.
“He ought to have a butterfly net, or something like that,” said
Rose.
“Farmer Joel isn’t exactly going to catch the bees,” explained
Daddy Bunker. “All he can do is to follow them until the queen bee
lights on a tree branch, or some place like that. When she does, all
the other bees will cluster around her, as thickly as possible. Then, if
Farmer Joel is lucky enough to find them, he can take an empty hive,
put it on the ground under the queen bee and the bunch of worker
bees, jar them off into the hive, clap the cover on, and bring it back
to his apiary.”
“What’s an ap—an ap—ap—?” began Violet.
“An apiary means a place where bees are kept,” explained Mr.
Bunker. “It comes from the Latin word apis, which means bee. Now
while we are waiting to see what happens I’ll tell you a little about
bees and why they swarm.”
The six little Bunkers looked at Farmer Joel, with his smoking
machine and his mosquito netting hat, still following the slowly
moving swarm of bees toward the woods, and then they turned to
their father who had promised to tell them something better than a
story.
“Bees are of three kinds,” said Mr. Bunker. “There is the worker
bee, of which there are thousands in every swarm, or hive. The
drones are the father bees, and, I am sorry to say, they are a lazy lot.
They never work, and they eat lots of honey, and sometimes, when
too many drones, or father bees, get into a hive, the worker bees
sting them to death, for they can’t afford to feed too many lazy bees
that won’t work. Then, most important of all, is the queen bee.”
“How can you or Farmer Joel tell one bee from another?” inquired
Violet, and this time the other children were glad she had asked the
question, for this was something they wanted to know.
“The queen bee is larger and longer than any of the others,”
answered Mr. Bunker, “and even you, not knowing anything about
bees, could easily pick her out of hundreds of others. The drones are
a little larger than the workers, and the queer thing about the drones
is that they never sting. They have no stings and cannot harm you.
The queen can sting, but she never does, or hardly ever; for once a
bee stings, it leaves the stinger in a person or an animal, and that
means the bee dies. And it wouldn’t do to have the queen bee die.”
“What would happen if she should die?” asked Russ.
“That is taken care of by the worker bees,” said Mr. Bunker. “In the
cells, or little holes in the wax honeycomb, are many eggs that after
a while will hatch out into other bees, mostly workers or drones. The
queen bee lays the eggs that hatch into other bees. But if it should
happen that the queen should die, the worker bees at once begin to
feed to some of the half-hatched little bees a peculiar kind of food
gathered from the flowers. It is a sort of mixture of honey and juices
from the bees’ bodies. This is called royal food, royal honey or queen
bread. And when the half-hatched little bees eat this strange food
they are changed from ordinary bees into queen bees.
“But as there can be but one queen in a hive, if more hatch out all
but one are killed, and so the life in the hive goes on. The new queen
begins laying eggs, and more drones, workers and perhaps more
queens are hatched. The workers fly off to the fields to gather honey
from the flowers, and they also gather something else.”
“I know!” cried Russ. “Our teacher in school told us! They gather
yellow stuff. It is called——”
“Pollen!” exclaimed Rose. “I know that.”
“Yes,” her father answered, “the bees gather pollen, or the yellow
dust from the flowers, and by mixing this yellow dust with some
juices from their bodies they make beeswax, from which the cells are
built to hold the sweet honey juice.”

You might also like